Basic Empathy Scale
Basic Empathy Scale
d o i : 10 . 1 5 4 4 6 / rc p .v 24 n 2. 4 2 5 1 4
CÉSARMERINO-SOTO
San Martín de Porres University, Lima, Peru
MIRIANGRIMALDO-MUCHOTRIGO
Peruvian University of Applied Sciences, Lima, Peru
Unless otherwise stated, the content of this magazine is licensed under a Creative license.
Commons "recognition, non-commercial and no derivative works" Colombia 2.5, which can be consulted
license at: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/2.5/co
How to cite this article: Merino-Soto, C. & Grimaldo-Muchotrigo, M. (2015). Structural validation of
Modified Basic Empathy Scale in Adolescents: A Preliminary Study. Journal
Colombian Journal of Psychology, 24(2), 261-270. doi: 10.15446/rcp.v24n2.42514.
Correspondence related to this article should be addressed to César Merino-Soto, email: sikayax@yahoo.
com.ar. Institute of Psychology Research, University of San Martín de Porres, Av. Tomás Marsano
242, Surquillo-Lima 34, Peru.
Colombian Journal of Psychology vol. 24 no. 2 July-December 2015 ISSN 0121-5469 printed | 2344-8644 online Bogotá Colombia - pp.261-270
262 César Merino-Soto & Mirian Grimaldo-Muchotrig
Summary
A preliminary study was conducted to verify the structure of a 9-item version of the Basic Empathy Scale.
(EBE), an instrument that assesses affective and cognitive empathy. The participants were 135 adolescents between 11 and
18 yearsM=14DE1.4) of regular education, in an urban region of Metropolitan Lima. A methodology was used
structural equations to confirm the structure, and the Schmid-Leiman transformation to evaluate the bi- model
-dimensional. The results indicate that an oblique two-factor model is satisfactory for the data, the reliability-
dad is greater than .70, and the interpretation of a single score as it was previously done is not justified. They are discussed
the differences in the interpretation of the models and the assessment of the factorial methodology.
Colombian Journal of Psychology vol. 24 no. 2 July-December 2015 ISSN 0121-5469 printed | 2344-8644 online Bogotá Colombia - pp.261-270
264 César Merino-Soto & Mirian Grimaldo-Muchotrigo
a single latent dimension without subcomponents regardless of the number of items consist of
distinguishable (unidimensional), complex factors in the average inter-item correlation (Bri-
truly discriminatory among themselves (orthogonal factors- ggs & Cheek, 1986; Simms & Watson, 2007). The
factors with some degree of dependence construct validity was done through the
linear (oblique factors) and bi-factorial model, coefficientw (McDonald, 1999) for each fac-
in which a general factor is modeled alongside even regardless of the results of the
specific factors. This last model founds confirmatory factor analysis and from the re-
mention the rationale for building a score Results of the hierarchical factorial analysis (coefficient-
total and sub-scores, as in Oliva et al. (2011) hierarchical client,wh;Zinbarg,Yovel,Revelle, &
they indicate it in the construction of their benchmarks. They McDonald, 2006). These calculations were made with
used the EQS6.2 program (Bentler & Wu, 2012), the Omega program (Watkins, 2013).
and adjustment indices were chosen:c2robustc2;
Satorra & Bentler, 1994), absolute fit indices Results
mourning (RootMeanSquaredError Approximation: Regarding the evaluation of the models
RMSEA≤.05;Standardized Root Mean Square Re- (Table 1), the unidimensional model was the one that
residualSRMR≤.08) and comparative (Comparative showed the worst fit. The following models
Fit IndexCFI≥.95;Tucker-Lewis Index:TLI≥.95); they showed exceptionally good fit levels.
these indices were also based on the SB-c2. high, although the SRMR was not adequate for
The fitting function was maximum test- the orthogonal model. The results of the mode-
ability, considering that the deviations of the orthogonal ones were not better than the model
the normality of the items was not severe oblique; in this last one a correlation was detected
and would have little effect on the parameters. moderate action that influenced his better adjustment
polychoric correlations were chosen. The bi-factor model did not converge, given that
among the items to represent continuity a non-positive matrix was detected. At this point
latent among these (Holgado, Chacón, Barbero, it was preferred to proceed with the transformation of
& Vila, 2010; Morata-Ramírez & Holgado-Tello, Schmid-Leiman (SL; Schmid & Leiman, 1957),
2013). For internal consistency, calculations were made What is an exploratory approach to modeling a
the coefficient α (Cronbach, 1951) and its intervals second-order factor and first-order factors
of confidence with the Fisher method (Romano, den, within a framework of hierarchical relationships
Kromrey, & Hibbard, 2012; Romano, Kromrey, cases that also allow estimating the variance in
Owens, & Scott, 2011). An index was also used each factor (Wolf & Preising, 2005).
Table 1
Adjustment of the evaluated measurement models
.17
Unidimensional 140.902 (27) .14 .66 .74
(0.14, 0.26)
.037
Orthogonal 31.862 (27) .12 .98 .98
(0.0, 0.08)
.015
Oblique 26.789 (26) .06 .99 .99
.0, .07
Colombian Journal of Psychology vol. 24 no. 2 July-December 2015 ISSN 0121-5469 printed | 2344-8644 online Bogotá Colombia - pp.261-270
266 César Merino-Soto & Mirian Grimaldo-Muchotrigo
Table 2
Descriptive statistics and factorial results for the items
cfa Schmid–Leiman Transformation
Item M dE As Cu
F1 F2 R 2 G F1 F2 h2 G h2 F
h2total
E1 2.985 1.040 -0.249 -0.475 .718 .516 .559 -0.057 .408 .312 .170 .482
E2 2.896 1.090 -0.103 -0.530 .775 .600 .617 -0.024 .438 .381 .192 .573
E3 2.956 1.173 -0.274 -0.945 .772 .596 .609 -0.067 .446 .371 .203 .574
E6 4.074 0.892 -1.095 1.294 .496 .246 .412 .174 .231 .170 .084 .253
E4 3.756 0.881 -0.676 0.436 .668 .447 .349 .558 .062 .122 .315 .437
E5 2.881 1.168 0.148 -0.667 .574 .330 .227 .467 .007 .052 .218 .270
E7 3.800 0.925 -0.608 0.108 .753 .568 .346 .672 .023 .120 .452 .572
E8 4.163 0.772 -0.972 1.588 .748 .559 .361 .646 .042 .130 .419 .549
E9 4.178 0.842 -0.947 0.793 .669 .447 .191 .634 -0.072 .036 .407 .444
Correlation
F1 - - - - 1 - .422 - - - - -
F2 - - - - .34 1 - .820 - - - - -
α - - - - .76 .77 -
Note item reliability. G: general factor. Gcommunalities in the general factor. communality
2 2 2
F
in the specific factors.2Totalltotal communalism (sum of2Gyh2FCFA: results of the oblique model.αalpha coefficient.womega coefficient.
whhierarchical omega coefficient.
The average inter-item correlation for the empirical and parsimonious proposal of Jolliffe
EA(rii=0.462) yEC(rii.396) is found in yFarrington (2006) seems appropriate. However,
the recommended range of inter-consistency Bargo, there is not enough evidence for this
between the items, also indicating that the parsimony leads to using a single score in
Scores seem to point to mode constructs- the interpretation of the construct in children and
radically specific (Clark & Watson, 1995). teenagers.
Internal consistency through the coefficient The difference in the magnitude of the esti-
it can be considered appropriate given the small reliability formations, between the coefficient
number of items at each score, at least for α ywit suggests that if one wants an estimate
descriptive group fines; their intervals of more accurate of the reliability, it must be taken into
confidence (95%) were (.67, .82) and (.69, .83) for tell the effect of the violation of the budget-
f1 yf2, respectively. The coefficientwit was due to the tau equivalence in the items, something that has
satisfactory for each factor taken in this way has been emphasized in other works (Yang & Green,
independent, but from the hierarchical analysis 2011; Zinbarg et al., 2006). The estimation of the
The trivial loads were transformed into reliability using factor information,
0 for its calculation), its magnitude was totally Indeed, a better one must be considered.
unacceptable for the general factor (less than .46) estimation of this parameter, considering the crí-
and paraf2 (EA). current issue with the coefficient α (Yang & Green, 2011).
In the discussion of these technical aspects, it must
Discussion to emphasize two things: first, as in
The results suggest that the best inter- another study on the adaptation of a measure of
interpretation of the construct of empathy, in the BES empathy (Aristu et al., 2008), the use of correlations
abbreviated by Oliva et al. (2011), corresponds polychronic actions can allow for estimations
two moderately related areas, with less biased regarding the ordinal nature
items between moderate and strong dis- capacity of the items of the BES. Second, that the method
criminal; this variation in discrimination It allowed us to recognize how it is distributed
depends on the found factor. This clearly contrasts retained variance among the first factors
lie with the intended use of the standards for first order and second order, and it is a procedure
the total score derived by Oliva et al. (2011) and highly recommended for explo- purposes
the information from Jolliffe and Farrington (2006). If ratorios (Wolf & Preising, 2005).
a measure of empathy that can be required Our data did not allow for mode-
to be interpreted with a single score, another instruction- to create a bi-factorial structure with the method
Mentoring for the same purpose would be more appropriate. confirmatory, but a replication study
The evidence found here was confirmed from could you verify the generalization of this result
various sources (the reliability based on the mo- tado. Along with this limitation of the present study-
factorial work and the common variance among the fac- God, more heterogeneity and size are required
stores) that aim to understand that the process sample for parameter estimation
cognitive and emotional aspects of empathy work potentially less biased. Likewise, the
with a certain independence, at least when this differences in age and sex at the level of scores
is assessed by a self-report measure. and of the items, and regarding the properties is-
Along with the confirmation of a pair structure- Structural issues must be explored, because
specifically orthogonal, conceptually it indicates they can produce variations in such aspects.
since the interpretation of empathy is supported The influence of such variables (age and sex) has
better from a multidimensional framework, and that has been demonstrated in the scientific literature (e.g.,
Colombian Journal of Psychology vol. 24 no. 2 July-December 2015 ISSN 0121-5469 printed | 2344-8644 online Bogotá Colombia - pp. 261-270
268 César Merino-Soto & Mirian Grimaldo-Muchotrigo
Álvarez et al., 2010; Garaigordobil & Magan- Clark, L.A. & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validation.
to, 2011; Retuerto, 2004; Sánchez-Queija et al., Dity: Basic issues in objective scale development.
2006), which suggests assessing these differences in Psychological Assessment, 7, 309-319.
a larger and more heterogeneous sample. Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal
Finally, it could be recommended to explore structure of tests.Psychometrika, 16, 297-334. doi:
the structure of the BES Using all the items (ori- 10.1007/BF02310555
originally, 20 items), checking in this with- Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to
the replicability of the modified version individual differences in empathy.jsasCatalog of
deOliva et al. (2011) and perhaps derive another version Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.
abbreviated alternative that shows equal or better Del Barrio, M.V., Aluja, A. & García, L. (2004). Bry-
psychometric properties. But they consider- ant’s empathy index for children and adolescents:
of all the discussed aspects, the interpretation Psychometric properties in the Spanish language.
multidimensional aspect of empathy in adolescents Psychological Reports, 95, 257-262. doi: 10.2466/
seems reasonable and empirically justifiable, PR0.95.5.257-262
and from a practical perspective, the results Eisenberg, N. & Strayer, J. (1992). Empathy and its
prove to be satisfactory for use as development.Bilbao: Desclée de Brouwer.
a potential tool for applications Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation and moral
sivas, in the description of empathetic behavior development.Annual Review of Psychology, 51,
of the teenager, as the main objective or as 665-697.doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.665
secondary component of other behaviors. Eisenberg, N. (2009). Empathy-related responding:
Links with self-regulation, moral judgment, and
References moral behavior. In M. Mikulincer & P. Shaver
Álvarez, P., Carrasco, M. & Fustos, J. (2010). Relationship (Eds.), Prosocial motives, emotions, and behavior
of empathy and gender in prosocial behavior and (pp. 129-148). Washington: APA Publications.
aggressive, in adolescents of different types of this Garaigordobil, M. & García de Galdeano, P. (2006).
educational bleaching. Ibero-American Journal Empathy in children aged 10 to 12. Psychothema,
of Psychology: Science and Technology, 3(2), 27-36. 8(2), 180-186.
Aristu, A., Holgado, F., Carrasco, M. & del Barrio, M. Garaigordobil, M. & Maganto, C. (2011). Empathy and
(2008). The structure of Bryant’s Empathy Index for conflict resolution during childhood and the
Children: A cross-validation study. Spanish Journal adolescence. LatinAmerican Journal of Psychology,
of Psychology, 11(2), 670-677. 43(2), 255-266.
Bentler, R. M. & Wu, E. J. C. (2012).EQS for windows Guevara, I., Cabrera, V. & Barrera, C. (2007). Factors
(Version 6.2) [Statistical Program for Windows]. contextual and moral emotions as predictors
Encino: Multivariate Software, Inc. of psychological adjustment in adolescence. Universitas
Briggs, S.R. & Cheek, J. M. (1986). The role of factor Psychologica, 6(2), 269-283.
analysis in the development and evaluation of Hoffman,M.L.(2000).Empathyandmoraldevelopment:
personality scales. Journal of Personality, 54, 106-148. Implications for caring and justice. Cambridge:
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1986.tb00391.x University Press.
Bryant, B. (1982). An index of empathy for children Holgado, F.P., Chacón, S., Barbero, I. & Vila, E. (2010).
and adolescents. Child Development, 53, 413-425. Polychoric versus Pearson correlations in explo-
doi: 10.2307/1128984 Laboratory and confirmatory factor analysis of ordinal
variables.Quality and Quantity, 44, 153-166. doi: Sánchez-Queija, I., Oliva,A. & Parra,A. (2006). Empathy
10.1007/s11135-008-9190-y prosocialbehaviorduringadolescence.Journal
Jolliffe, D. & Farrington, D.P. (2006). Development and Social Psychology, 21(3), 259-271.
validation of the Basic Empathy Scale. Journal of Satorra, A. & Bentler, P. M. (1994). Corrections to
Adolescence, 29, 589-611. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescen- test statistics and standard errors in covariance
ce.2005.08.010 Structure analysis. EnA. von Eye & C. C. Clogg
McDonald, R.P. (1999). Test theory: A unified approach. Latent variables analysis: Applications for
Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. developmental research (pp. 399-419). Thousand
Master,V.,Frías,D. & Samper,P. (2004).The measure Oaks: Sage.
of empathy: analysis of Interpersonal Reactivity Schmid, J. & Leiman, J.N. (1957). The development of
Index.Psicothema, 16(2), 255-260. hierarchical factor solutions. Psychometrika, 22,
Morata-Ramírez, M. & Holgado-Tello, F. (2013). Construct 53-61. doi: 10.1007/BF02289209
validity of Likert scales through confirmatory factor Simms, L. J. & Watson, D. (2007). The construct valida-
analysis: A simulation study comparing different tion approach to personality scale construction. En
methods of estimation based on Pearson and poly- R.Robins, C.Fraley & R. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook
Choric correlations. International Journal of Social of research methods in personality psychology (pp.
Science Studies, 1, 54-61. doi: 10.11114/ijsss.v1i1.27 240–258). New York: Guilford.
Oliva,A.,Antolín,L.,Pertegal, M.,Ríos, M.,Parra,A., Ward, T., Keenan, T. & Hudson, S. M. (2000). Unders-
Hernando, A. & Reina, M. (2011). Instruments standing cognitive, affective, and intimacy deficits
for the evaluation of mental health and development in sexual offenders: A developmental perspective.
positive adolescent and the assets that promote it. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 5(1), 41-62. doi:
Seville: Department of Health. 10.1016/S1359-1789(98)00025-1
Retuerto, A. (2004). Differences in empathy based on Watkins, M.W. (2013). Omega [Computer software].
of the variables gender and age. Notes of Psychology, Phoenix: Ed & Psych Associates.
22(3), 323-339. Wolf, H. G. & Preising, K. (2005). Exploring item
Richaud, M. (2008). Evaluation of empathy in and higher order factor structure with the Sch-
Argentinian child population. Journal of the Institute mid–Leiman solution: Syntax codes for spssand
of Psychology Research, 11, 101-115. sas.Behavior Research Methods, 37(1), 48-58. doi:
Romano, J.L., Kromrey, J.D. & Hibbard, S.T. (2012). 10.3758/BF03206397
A Monte Carlo study of eight confidence intervals Yang,Y. & Green, S. B. (2011). Coefficient alpha:A
methods for coefficient alpha. Educational and reliability coefficient for the 21st century? Journal
PsychologicalMeasurement, 70(3), 376-393. doi: of Psychoeducational Assessment, 29, 377-392.
10.1177/0013164409355690 doi: 10.1177/0734282911406668
Romano, J.L., Kromrey, J.D. Owens, C. M. & Scott, Zinbarg, R. E., Yovel, I., Revelle, W. & McDonald, R.
H. M. (2011). Confidence interval methods for P.(2006).Estimatinggeneralizabilitytoalatent
coefficient alpha based on discrete, ordinal variable comuna a todos los indicadores de una escala:
Which one, if any, is the best? A comparison of estimators for ωh Applied
Journal of Experimental Education, 79(4), 382-403. Psychological Measurement, 30, 121-144. doi:
doi: 10.1080/00220973.2010.510859 10.1177/0146621605278814
Colombian Journal of Psychology vol. 24 no. 2 July-December 2015 ISSN 0121-5469 printed | 2344-8644 online Bogotá Colombia - pp.261-270
270 César Merino-Soto & Mirian Grimaldo-Muchotrig
Appendix
Basic Empathy Scale, nine-item version derived from the pilot study by Oliva et al. (2011)
Indicate whether the phrases listed below can define your way of being or not.
marking the chosen option with a circle.
Totally agree
Agreed
Neither agree nor in
disagreement
In disagreement
Totally in
disagreement
After being with a friend who is sad for some reason, I usually
1 2 3 4 5
feeling sad