JCM 09 00986 v2
JCM 09 00986 v2
Clinical Medicine
Article
Executive Functions and Emotion Regulation in
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and
Borderline Intellectual Disability
Elena Predescu 1 , Roxana Sipos 1, * , Cristina A. Costescu 2 , Anamaria Ciocan 3 and
Diana I. Rus 3
1 Department of Neuroscience, Discipline of Psychiatry and Pediatric Psychiatry, Iuliu Hat, ieganu University
of Medicine and Pharmacy Cluj-Napoca, Cluj-Napoca Manastur Street no. 54 C, 400660 Cluj-Napoca,
Romania; [email protected]
2 Special Education Department, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Babes-Bolyai University,
Sindicatelor Street no 7, 400029 Cluj-Napoca, Romania; [email protected]
3 Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences,
Babes-Bolyai University, Republicii Street no 37, 400015 Cluj-Napoca, Romania;
[email protected] (A.C.); [email protected] (D.I.R.)
* Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +40-723031996
Received: 26 February 2020; Accepted: 31 March 2020; Published: 1 April 2020
Abstract: The main objective of this study is to investigate the multiple relations and to determine
the differences between executive functions (EFs), emotion regulation, and behavioral and emotional
problems in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), borderline intellectual
disability (ID), and typical development (TD). The sample included 85 children aged 6 to 11 years,
42 with typical development (TD), 27 with ADHD, and 16 with borderline ID. The results emphasized
a positive correlation between adaptive emotion regulation strategies and EFs, and no significant
relations between the maladaptive emotion regulation strategies and EFs. In addition, the executive
function of planning correlated negatively with anxiety, ADHD symptoms, and conduct problems.
The performance of both clinical groups regarding EFs was significantly lower than that of the
TD group, and they differed significantly from each other only on visual attention. The presence
of oppositional-defiant and conduct problems was higher in both clinical groups than in the TD
group, and more anxiety symptoms were reported in children with ADHD. This study supports
the idea that emotion regulation, Efs, and clinical symptoms are interconnected. It also profiles the
deficits in cognitive functioning and emotion regulation in two clinical groups, thus helping future
intervention programs.
1. Introduction
One of the key concepts in developmental psychopathology is that of executive dysfunction.
Several developmental disorders have been associated with deficits in executive functions (EF),
including attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [1] and intellectual disability (ID) [2].
Usually, the EF role is considered separately for each of these disorders, and there have been attempts to
describe different types of EF problems. These attempts pointed to the need for a better understanding
of the executive functions’ normal development. Cognitive functioning and psychopathology are
closely linked to child development. The school years are characterized by cognitive skills and
self-regulation strategies refinement while, in some children, clinical symptoms emerge. A disruption
in one area of development may be accompanied by impairment in other areas [3].
Emotion dysregulation is also common in individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders,
including ADHD and ID. Emotion regulation is a component of the broader concept of self-regulation.
Diamond (2013) claims that self-regulation “refers primarily to control and regulation of one’s emotions
and overlaps substantially with inhibitory control”, an important component of executive functions
(EFs) [4,5]. Some studies highlighted that emotion dysregulation is an important risk factor for
aggressive behavior, anxiety symptoms, and eating pathology [6,7]. Also, it appears that children with
different neurodevelopmental disorders, like ADHD or ID, have poor emotion regulation skills [8].
Theories on ADHD suggest that deficits in EFs are at the core of the ADHD-syndrome and play a
key role in explaining the problems that children with ADHD confront in daily life [9,10]. Evidence
suggests that impairments in EF are related to deficits in attention, hyperactivity, impulsivity [11,12],
and other associated problems.
When it comes to intellectual disability (ID), emotion dysregulation has received little attention.
Individuals with ID may experience deficiencies in the awareness and understanding of their emotional
experience, and possibly an increased predisposition to rigidly adhere to a specific self-regulatory
response (e.g., aggression or self-harm behaviors) [13]. They tend to utilize a limited range of coping
strategies when emotionally aroused [14]. One study revealed that children with ID used more social
coping and less goal-directed strategies than typically developed (TD) children during a frustrating
task. Therefore, children with ID experienced higher levels of frustration [15].
One of the most influential taxonomies of EFs is that proposed by Miyake et al. (2000) [16–18].
They support the following three basic EFs: mental set shifting (“Shifting”), information updating
and monitoring (“Updating”) (operations of working memory), and inhibition of prepotent responses
(“Inhibition”). Diamond (2013) published an EF model in her review on EFs, enforcing the three core
EFs proposed by Miyake et al. (2000) and stating that this core EFs contribute to the higher-level EFs:
reasoning, problem-solving and planning [4].
In ADHD, clinicians and researchers reported poor inhibition [19,20] and impairment in attention
and time reproduction [20] comparative to controls. Findings regarding working memory (WM)
deficits in ADHD are inconsistent. A study that investigated which parts of WM may be affected in
ADHD found that both visuospatial short-term memory (STM) and central executive were impaired
aside from a motivational deficit [21]. On the other hand, results claiming no impairment in WM in
ADHD are also present in literature [19,20].
Research also documents a clear EFs impairment in ID. Children with ID display deficits in WM,
auditory sustained attention, visual selective attention, visual categorization, inhibition, and planning
when compared to the performance of a mentally age-matched group [22–24]. In addition, WM deficits
increased with the degree of ID [22].
The relation between emotion regulation, EFs, and behavioral problems has received little
attention. Having deficits in emotion regulation is an important risk factor for conduct problems [6].
Self-regulation and, implicitly, emotion regulation influence planning abilities [4]. Planning has been
related to performance in the social domain [25] and aggression [26].
The main objective of our study is to investigate the multiple relations and to determine the
differences between EFs, emotion regulation, and the behavioral/emotional problems in children
with ADHD, borderline ID, and typical development (TD). Within this objective, we aim to test a
mediation model, considering that the relationship between emotion regulation and conduct problems
is mediated by planning, a high-order EF. Most studies on EFs in neurodevelopmental disorders have
focused on the core EFs, but we assessed other EFs, such as visual attention, planning, and STM (as
a component of WM). Unlike EFs, emotion regulation in ADHD has been the subject of few studies.
The research on EFs and emotion regulation in children with borderline ID is scant. Moreover, we used
a multi-method approach for assessing emotion regulation (parent-reports and an observational scale),
thus making possible a more comprehensive assessment of the concept.
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 986 3 of 15
2. Experimental Section
2.1. Participants
We conducted a prospective study on 85 children aged 6 to 11 years (M = 9.12, SD = 1.41), 42 typically
developed (TD) children (M = 9.16, SD = 1.01) and 43 diagnosed with attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD) or borderline intellectual disability (ID; IQ ranging from 70 to 85; M = 9.07, SD = 1.72).
The TD children sample included 22 girls and 20 boys, recruited from a public school in Gherla. Twenty
children were first-graders and 22 were third graders. The clinical groups included 27 children
diagnosed with ADHD and 16 diagnosed with borderline ID, without comorbidities, diagnosed via
the 10th revision of the International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems
(ICD 10)-based clinical interviews. Twenty-four children were recruited from special education schools,
and 19 children were recruited from the Clinic of Pediatric Psychiatry from Cluj-Napoca.
2.2. Instruments
Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) [27]. CBCL is a valid and reliable tool used to assess children’s
emotional and behavioral problems and should be filled in by the parent/caretaker who spends the most
time with the child. It assesses various problems and child functioning over the past 6 months. CBCL
has 113 items reported on a 3-point Likert Scale. The answers are grouped into seven domain-specific
scales. This questionnaire showed good validity and reliability coefficients and is adapted for the
Romanian population.
Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC) [28]. ERC is a 24-item instrument developed to assess
the children’s emotional regulation level. It has two scales, Emotion Regulation (ER; 8 items—high
scores indicate a good capacity of emotion modulation) and Emotional Lability/Negativity (L/N;
16 items—high scores show an excessive emotional reactivity and a frequent mood change). ER assesses
the emotional expression, empathy, and emotional self-awareness, and L/N assesses the lack of
flexibility, anger dysregulation, and mood lability. Items are rated on a 4-point Likert scale (1—almost
always to 4—never). The questionnaire was translated into Romanian using the study conducted by
Molina et al. [29].
Child Adjustment Scale [30]. The Child Adjustment Scale is a 33-item measure of the child’s
socioemotional adjustment. Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1—almost never to 5—almost
always). It includes four subscales: peer relations, work habits, compliance, and emotional health.
NEPSY: A developmental neuropsychological assessment [31]. NEPSY assesses the
neuropsychological development of children aged 3 to 12 years [32]. It has good concurrent and
predictive validity test–retest reliability for the subscales. The internal reliability coefficient is r = 0.80.
NEPSY differentiates the atypical development profiles from the typical ones and is adapted and
validated for the Romanian population. The subscales used in this study were attention and executive
functioning (visual attention and tower), visuospatial processing (block construction), and memory
and learning (delayed memory for faces). The total administration time for these subscales was
approximately 30–40 min for each child.
• Visual attention domain measures visual attention speed and accuracy [16]. The children’s task is
to scan a linear array of pictures and to mark the targets as quickly and accurately as possible.
For school-aged children, we used the cats and faces targets.
• Tower subtest is a task that assesses a series of executive functions such as planning, monitoring,
problem-solving, and self-regulation [16]. The children must move the three colored balls to target
positions, on three pegs, following some rules. The task is time-limited (30–60 s), and its difficulty
varies according to the number of moves and complexity.
• Block construction is a visuospatial processing task that requires children to build constructions
using a different number of blocks. This task has a medium level of difficulty, and every subset
has a time limit of 30 to 60 s.
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 986 4 of 15
• Delayed faces memory assesses children’s memory capacity. Children are shown 16 pictures of
boys and girls, and they must memorize them. After 30 minutes, the experimenter shows the
children 16 sets of pictures, each containing three pictures, and the children’s task is to recognize
the person that they have seen before from every set of pictures. During the delayed memory
task, children must hold in mind for several minutes some visual information. We considered the
delayed memory task a measure of short-term memory (STM).
Tower construction task (Anger-eliciting task—observational scale) [33]. The children’s task
is to build a wooden 10-block tower. A picture of a tower is first presented, then the children are
instructed to build a tower exactly like the one in the picture within 2 min and 40 s in order to receive a
reward (a candy in our study). The task is impossible to solve because two blocks are slightly rounded
on one side. At the end of the assessment, the children are explained the impossibility of the task.
It is an anger-eliciting task developed to assess the anger regulation strategies using a behavioral
observation scale. Both the anger-eliciting task and the behavioral observation scale were developed
and validated by Rohlf and Krahe [33]. The observational scale includes three categories of emotion
regulation strategies: adaptive (solution orientation, substituting the anger expression, and verbalizing
the cognitive strategies), maladaptive (visual focus, verbal focus, anger expression, resignation), and
neutral strategies (ineffective help-seeking).
Raven’s progressive matrices were used to assessed children’s intellectual abilities.
A questionnaire was used to collect general information regarding children’s age, educational
level, clinical diagnosis, and other demographic data.
2.3. Procedure
We used a quasi-experimental design to analyze the associations between children’s characteristics
(TD children and children diagnosed with ADHD or borderline ID), adaptive or maladaptive emotional
regulation strategies, and executive functions. The study was carried according to the law concerning
the conduct of clinical trials, including abidance by international ethical standards foreseen in the
updated Helsinki Declaration of Human Rights. We obtained the approval of the local ethics committee
to conduct the study. Data were used respecting the regulation regarding the privacy and subject’s
identity protection, and informed consent was obtained for each participant. All questionnaires were
filled in by the parents. Children that agreed to participate in the study were assessed in a 30–40-minute
individual session. The TD children were assessed in a school laboratory and the children diagnosed
with ADHD or ID, in a playroom used for psychological assessments. The NEPSY battery tasks were
completed first (visual attention task, the learning task from the delayed memory subtest, the tower
and block construction). The anger-eliciting task was applied next. After receiving the instructions,
behaviors and the involvement level were coded using the observational scale [33]. Every child was
rewarded after completing the task.
Data were analyzed using SPSS 20. Pearson correlation was used to test the associations between
the studied variables (emotional regulation, cognitive functioning, emotional and behavioral problems)
for the entire sample, and separately for two age groups (7 to 9 and 9 to 11 years). Independent t-test
was used to compare the studied variables between the clinical groups (ADHD and borderline ID) and
the non-clinical group (TD).
3. Results
3.1. Differences in Cognitive Functioning, Emotion Regulation, and Behavioral and Emotional Problems
Across Diagnoses
Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) were computed for the cognitive functions, emotion
regulation, and behavioral and emotional problems in the typical development (TD) and clinical
groups (see Table 1). A t-test for independent samples was used to investigate the differences between
the TD children and those diagnosed with ADHD or borderline ID.
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 986 5 of 15
Table 1. Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) for cognitive functions, emotion regulation strategies,
and behavioral/emotional problems measures.
As seen in Table 1, the TD group scored better than the clinical groups on every assessed cognitive
function. Generally, the means differences were important and similar between the TD group and
the clinical groups, but for visual attention, they followed a different pattern. The mean score was
higher in the TD group (M = 8.95, SD = 1.03) than in both ADHD (M = 7.45, SD = 5.23) and borderline
ID groups (M = 3.81, SD = 2.00), but the difference was significant also between the clinical groups.
The visuospatial processing mean in TD children (M = 14.04, SD = 2.54) was higher than the means for
ADHD group (M = 9.17, SD = 3.82) and borderline ID group (M = 7.06, SD = 2.56), but the differences
did not reach significance. Regarding the emotion regulation strategies, the ADHD group reported a
higher use of maladaptive strategies (M = 3.46, SD = 2.06) than the TD group (M = 2.83, SD = 2.25)
and borderline ID group (M = 2.00, SD = 1.47). Adaptive strategies were more frequently used by the
TD group (M = 5.07, SD = 2.64) than the clinical groups which reported similar frequencies. When
analyzing the behavioral/emotional problems reported on CBCL, we noticed a higher level of affective
problems, anxiety, oppositional-defiant, and conduct problems reported for the children in the ADHD
group. The borderline ID group scored higher on ADHD and conduct problems than the children
from the TD group, but lower than the ADHD group.
TD group scored higher on cognitive functions than both ADHD and borderline ID groups,
the difference being significant for visual attention, memory, and planning. The difference was not
significant for visuospatial processing (see Table 2). When comparing the TD group with the ADHD
group, significant differences were registered for visual attention (t(64) = 1.79, p = 0.00), memory
(t(64) = 5.73, p = 0.006), and planning (t(66) = 5.11, p = 0.00). Similar results were registered for the
comparison between TD group and borderline ID group for visual attention (t(56) = 12.81, p = 0.00),
memory (t(56) = 4.73, p = 0.047), and planning (t(56) = 4.92, p = 0.001).
A t-test was used to assess the differences in emotion regulation strategies across groups.
No significant differences were registered (see Table 2). The adaptive emotional regulation strategies
mean was higher in the TD group (M = 5.07, SD = 2.64) than in both ADHD (M = 4.53, SD =
3.09) and borderline ID groups (M = 4.50, SD = 2.71), but the differences did not reach significance.
The maladaptive emotional regulation strategies mean in TD children (M = 2.83, SD = 2.25) was smaller
than the mean for ADHD group (M = 3.46, SD = 2.06) and higher than the mean for the borderline
ID group (M = 2.00, SD = 1.47), but the differences did not reach significance. Children included in
the clinical groups (ADHD and borderline ID) reported higher levels of emotional and behavioral
problems on CBCL, than the TD children (see Table 1). Significant differences were registered for
oppositional-defiant problems between TD children (M = 2.33, SD = 2.19) and the children from the
ADHD group (M = 4.29, SD = 3.67), t(67) = −2.78, p = 0.00. Similar results were obtain for the difference
between TD group and borderline ID group (M = 2.62, SD = 2.96), t(56) = −4.10, p = 0.036. Significant
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 986 6 of 15
differences were registered also for conduct problems between TD children (M = 1.78, SD = 2.64) and
children with ADHD (M = 5.66, SD = 6.33), t(67) = −3.52, p = 0.00, and between TD children and those
with borderline ID (M = 3.50, SD = 4.84), t(56) = −1.72, p = 0.00. For the ADHD problems reported by
the parents on CBCL, a significant difference t(67) = −2.89, p = 0.00 was registered between TD children
(M = 4.14, SD = 3.15) and those with ADHD (M = 6.96, SD = 4.94). The ADHD group reported more
anxiety problems (M = 3.11, SD = 2.81) than the TD group (M = 2.28, SD = 2.07), and the difference was
significant, t(67) = −1.39, p = 0.02.
t-test
TD versus ADHD TD versus Borderline ID
Visual Attention t(64) = 1.79, p = 0.00 t(56) = 12.81, p = 0.00
Memory t(64) = 5.73, p = 0.006 t(56) = 4.73, p = 0.047
Cognitive functions
Planning t(66) = 5.11, p = 0.00 t(56) = 4.92, p = 0.001
Visuospatial processing t(57) = 5.71. p = 0.09 t(56) = 9.31, p = 0.572
Maladaptive strategies t(55) = −0.95, p = 0.907 t(52) = 1.20, p = 0.205
Emotion regulation
Adaptive strategies t(55) = 0.695, p = 0.06 t(52) = 0.713, p = 0.328
Affective problems t(67) = −1.45, p = 0.648 t(56) = −0.388, p = 0.363
Behavioral/ Anxiety t(67) = −1.39, p = 0.02 t(56) = −0.781, p = 0.732
Emotional Somatic problems t(67) = 0.65, p = 0.188 t(56) = −.086, p = 0.450
problems ADHD t(67) = −2.89, p = 0.002 t(56) = −1.55, p = 0.097
Oppositional-defiant t(67) = −2.78, p = 0.00 t(56) = −0.410. p = 0.036
Conduct problems t(67) = −3.52, p = 0.00 t(56) = −1.727, p = 0.002
No significant differences were registered between the ADHD group and borderline ID group for
cognitive function visuospatial processing, memory and planning, the adaptative and maladaptive
emotion regulation strategies, and emotional and behavioral problems measured by CBCL (see
Table 3). The only significant difference between the clinical groups was registered for visual attention.
The ADHD group mean (M = 7.45, SD = 5.23) was higher than that of borderline ID group (M = 3.81,
SD = 2.00), the difference being significant t(38) = 2.65, p = 0.002.
Table 3. Differences between children with ADHD and children with borderline ID.
t-test
ADHD versus Borderline ID
Visual Attention t(38) = 2.65, p = 0.002
Memory t(38) = −0.46, p = 0.65
Cognitive functions
Planning t(40) = −0.12, p = 0.32
Visuospatial processing t(31) = 1.85. p = 0.13
Maladaptive strategies t(25) = 2.06, p = 0.09
Emotion regulation
Adaptive strategies t(25) = 0.029, p = 0.47
Affective problems t(41) = 0.71, p = 0.66
Behavioral/ Anxiety t(41) = 0.33, p = 0.27
Emotional Somatic problems t(41) = −0.57, p = 0.08
problems ADHD t(41) = 0.87, p = 0.29
Oppositional-defiant t(41) = 1.54, p = 0.25
Conduct problems t(41) = 1.17, p = 0.28
3.2. The Relation between Emotion Regulation, Cognitive Functioning, Emotional and Behavioral Problems
When considering the whole sample, we found significant correlations between the cognitive
functions assessed and the emotion regulation strategies measured by the observational scale.
The executive functions correlated positively with the adaptive emotion regulation strategies as
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 986 7 of 15
a unitary concept. The correlation between the adaptive emotion regulation strategies and visual
attention was r(69) = 0.256, p = 0.034. The correlations were also significant with delayed memory r(69)
= 0.298, p = 0.013, planning r(69) = 0.364, p = 0.002 and visuospatial processing r(69) = 0.246, p = 0.042.
The correlations were positive, meaning that a frequent use of adaptive emotion regulation strategies
is associated with better cognitive function performances.
When analyzing the specific adaptive emotion regulation strategies, verbalizing the strategies
correlated positively with visual attention r(69) = 0.383, p = 0.001, planning r(69) = 0.322, p = 0.007,
delayed memory r(69) = 0.267, p = 0.026, and visuospatial processing r(69) = 0.306, p = 0.011. Substituting
the anger, another adaptive emotion regulation strategy, correlated positively with both visual attention
r(69) = 0.279, p = 0.020 and visuospatial processing r(69) = 0.352, p = 0.003.
No significant correlations were found between the maladaptive strategies as a unitary concept
and the cognitive functions assessed. This may be explained partially by the fact that verbal focusing
correlated positively with visual attention r(69) = 0.239, p = 0.047, whereas resignation correlated
negatively with all cognitive functions assessed, ranging from −0.359 with delayed memory to −0.238
with planning, meaning that a lesser use of resignation is associated with better performances on visual
attention, delayed memory, planning, and visuospatial processing. (The correlations are presented in
Table A1).
When analyzing the relation between emotion regulation reported by the parents [30] and the
cognitive functions assessed, we found significant correlations between emotion regulation and visual
attention r(69) = 0.528, p = 0.000, delayed memory r(69) = 0.270, p = 0.025, and visuospatial processing
r(69) = 0.513, p = 0.000, but no significant correlation between emotion regulation reported by parents
and planning r(69) = 0.181, p > 0.05.
Analyzing the possible relationship between the cognitive functions assessed and
emotional/behavioral problems as measured by CBCL, planning correlated negatively with anxiety
problems r(84) = −0.235, p = 0.031, ADHD problems r(84) = −0.221, p = 0.043 and conduct problems r(84)
= −0.276, p = 0.011. Delayed memory also correlated negatively with conduct problems r(84) = −0.237,
p = 0.032.
We analyzed the associations between cognitive functions, emotion regulation strategies, and
emotional/behavioral problems for the age-based groups (7 to 9 and 9 to 11 years). For the 7 to
9 years group, a positive correlation was found between adaptive emotional regulation strategies (as
a unitary concept) and planning r(20) = 0.509, p = 0.022. Negative correlations were noted between
substituting the anger (an adaptive emotion regulation strategy) and visual attention r(20)= −0.462,
p = 0.040, and between visual attention and affective problems measured by CBCL r(20) = −0.581,
p = 0.007, meaning that, in this age-group, having poor executive function is associated with a higher
level of affective problems. For the 9 to 11 years group, delayed memory correlated with the adaptive
emotion regulation strategies r(22) = 0.595, p = 0.003. Maladaptive emotion regulation strategies (as a
unitary concept) correlated negatively with planning r(22) = −0.473, p = 0.026, meaning that, in this
age-group, having poor executive function is associated with the use of maladaptive emotion regulation
strategies. Moreover, maladaptive emotion regulation strategies correlated positively with conduct
problems measured by CBCL r(22) = 0.482, p = 0.023. We did not find any significant correlation
between the reported emotion regulation and other variables. (The correlations are presented in
Table A2).
3.3. Testing the Relationship between Maladaptive Strategies, Planning, and Conduct Problems
The model tested had the executive function of planning as a mediator in the relation between
maladaptive emotion regulation strategies and conduct problems measured by CBCL. Statistical
analysis showed a positive correlation between maladaptive emotion regulation strategies and conduct
problems r(69) = 0.245, p = 0.04. The executive function of planning, as a mediator, showed a correlation
with maladaptive emotion regulation strategies r(69) = 0.099, p = 0.41 that did not reach significance and
a significant negative correlation with conduct problems measured by CBCL r(84) = −0.276, p = 0.01.
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 986 8 of 15
Thus, the preliminary conditions for demonstrating mediation were not met, and the mediation analysis
could not be performed.
TD children, but the quality of the fixation had been very different; children with ADHD showing a
discontinuous and uncoordinated attention system [47]. In our study, the two clinical groups differed
significantly from each other only on visual attention; for memory and planning, the results were almost
similar, and for the visuospatial processing, the ADHD group performed better, but not significantly
better. A possible explanation may reside in the discontinuous and uncoordinated attention system
of the children with ADHD, making them less performant in memory and planning even if they
had superior visual attention and visuospatial processing when compared with the children with
borderline ID. Another explanation may reside in the lower motivation and higher oppositional-defiant
problems or impulsivity, which may also alter the performance. Both clinical groups performed
significantly poorer than the TD group, emphasizing that impairment in attention is not specific only
for ADHD and also indicating that this executive function deficit can be even more pronounced in
other neurodevelopmental disorders such as borderline ID. Attention deficit, which is often seen in
children with ID, significantly affect their learning and behavior [48,49]. A study on visual attention
of children with ADHD reported significantly impaired sustained attention and visual processing
speed but intact attentional selectivity, perceptual threshold, and visual short-term memory capacity,
supporting the notion of different impairment of attentional functions in children with ADHD [50].
Another goal of this study is to investigate the relations between EFs, emotion regulation, and
emotional and behavioral problems as measured by CBCL. The results on the observational scale
emphasized a positive correlation between adaptive emotion regulation strategies (as a unitary concept)
and each measured EFs: visual attention, planning, and delayed memory or STM (as a component of
WM). Further, we analyzed each adaptive emotion regulation strategy in relation to EFs. Verbalizing the
strategies positively correlated with all EFs and substituting the anger positively correlated only with
visual attention, reinforcing the common belief that adaptive emotion regulation strategies associate
with better EF performances.
The unitary concept of maladaptive emotion regulation strategies, measured by the observational
scale, did not relate to EFs. This situation is partially explained by the fact that one maladaptive
strategy correlated negatively with all EFs, whereas another one correlated positively with visual
attention. For the emotional adjustment reported by parents, we found a positive relation between
emotion regulation and both visual attention and delayed memory (STM). These findings suggest that
children with good executive functions may use both adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation
strategies, but children with poor executive functions are more prone to use maladaptive emotion
regulation strategies, such as resignation.
The examination of the relation between EFs and emotional/behavioral problems revealed that
planning correlated negatively with anxiety, ADHD symptoms, and conduct problems. These findings
are consistent with previous studies showing that a deficit in planning can be related to behavioral
problems [26]. In addition, our results indicated that planning can also be related to emotional problems.
Recent studies indicate that hyperactive children can find it more difficult to positively regulate their
emotions [51], but our results indicate more possible explanations for that, the children with ADHD
reporting higher use of maladaptive emotion regulation strategies and also a higher level of anxiety
and affective problems.
Differences regarding the relations between EFs, emotion regulation, and emotional/behavioral
problems were observed between the two age groups. The younger group (aged 7 to 9 years) showed a
positive correlation between adaptive emotion regulation strategies (as a unitary concept) and planning.
The adaptive emotion regulation strategy of substituting the anger correlated negatively with visual
attention. In the older group (aged 9 to 11 years), maladaptive emotion regulation strategies (as a
unitary concept) correlated negatively with planning and positively with conduct problems.
Moreover, we did not find support for our mediation model. The executive function of planning
did not mediate the relation between maladaptive emotion regulation strategies and conduct problems.
Even though the Diamond’s (2013) model of EFs suggests that emotion regulation influences the higher
executive functions, such as planning [4], we did not find any significant relation between maladaptive
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 986 10 of 15
emotion regulation strategies and planning. Interestingly, this relation reached significance for the 9 to
11 years age group, where maladaptive emotion regulation strategies (as a unitary concept) correlated
negatively with planning, but not in the entire sample. Our sample included children with ages
between 6 and 11 years, and this may explain the result.
This study has several limitations. The clinical sample size was relatively small, primarily
because we included only patients diagnosed with ADHD or borderline ID without other diagnosed
comorbidities. Another important limitation is that our sample had a wide range of ages. As we
noticed, there were differences between the two age groups regarding the relationship between emotion
regulation and EFs, and this fact might have influenced the findings for the entire sample. Even though
our study is a complex one and tries to cover almost all the executive functions mentioned by Diamond
(2013) [4] in her model and tries to investigate the possible connections with emotion regulation
skills and emotional and behavioral problems, for a scientific validation of our theoretical model,
a bigger sample size is needed. Therefore, future studies should consider more participants for a better
generalization of the results. Also, the investigated outcomes from our research could be measured in
various ways, which may better explain the interconnection between several factors. For example,
future studies should also take into consideration tasks for children measuring emotion regulation skills
and emotional problems. In this way, the confound variables, such as the parents’ perception, could be
better controlled. Considering all the above-mentioned limitations, we recommend interpreting the
results of this work with caution.
The study supports the idea that emotion regulation, EFs, and clinical symptoms are interconnected.
Moreover, it suggests that behavioral problems in the clinical groups may be due to poor executive
functioning and not a result of poor emotion regulation. Finally, it brings to light new aspects
regarding cognitive functioning, emotion regulation, and behavioral and emotional problems in ADHD
and borderline ID. A better understanding of each neurodevelopmental disorder can lead to the
advancement of more targeted intervention programs.
Author Contributions: Conceptualization, E.P., R.S., and C.A.C.; methodology, E.P., R.S., and C.A.C.; formal
analysis, C.A.C., A.C., and D.I.R.; writing—original draft preparation, E.P., R.S., A.C., and D.I.R.; writing—review
and editing, E.P., R.S., and C.A.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: This research received no external funding.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 986 11 of 15
Appendix A
Table A1. Correlation coefficients for the entire sample.
Measurements 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
1. Visual attention (NEPSY) 1
2. Delayed memory (NEPSY) 0.394 ** 1
3. Planning (NEPSY) 0.374 ** 0.569 ** 1
4. Visuospatial processing
0.756 ** 0.513 ** 0.546 ** 1
(NEPSY)
5. Visual focusing −0.051 0.206 0.221 0.037 1
6. Verbal focusing 0.239 * 0.110 0.080 0.232 0.098 1
7. Anger expression 0.217 0.058 0.040 0.052 −0.028 0.084 1
8. Resignation −0.276 * −0.359 ** −0.238 * −0.265 * −0.076 0.125 0.082 1
9. Solution orientation −0.115 0.087 −0.104 −0.111 0.139 −0.064 −0.255 * −0.431 ** 1
10. Substituting the anger 0.279 * 0.233 0.194 0.352 ** −0.082 0.129 −0.201 −0.141 −0.049 1
11. Verbalizing the strategies 0.383 ** 0.267 * 0.322 ** 0.306 ** 0.315 ** 0.130 0.024 −0.203 0.060 −0.019 1
12. Maladaptive strategies 0.218 0.104 0.099 0.148 0.329 ** 0.762 ** 0.622 ** 0.280 * −0.220 −0.075 0.157 1
13. Adaptive strategies 0.256 0.298 * 0.364 ** 0.246 * 0.202 0.053 −0.208 −0.533 ** 0.785 ** 0.313 ** 0.489 ** −0.115 1
14. Emotion regulation 0.528 * 0.270 * 0.181 0.513 ** 0.008 0.143 0.242 * −0.326 ** −0.106 0.282 * 0.262 * 0.183 0.175 1
15. Affective problems (CBCL) −0.030 −0.124 −0.134 −0.134 −0.016 0.155 0.046 −0.056 −0.026 −0.031 −0.030 0.114 −0.035 0.145 1
16. Anxiety (CBCL) 0.061 −0.169 −0.235 * −0.052 −0.125 0.156 −0.020 −0.070 −0.029 −0.010 −.053 0.043 −0.030 0.119 0.552 ** 1
17. Somatic problems (CBCL) −0.084 0.082 −0.047 −0.009 −0.017 −0.231 −0.076 −0.176 0.177 0.060 −0.100 −0.232 0.123 0.136 0.424 ** 0.235 * 1
18. ADHD (CBCL) −0.096 −0.207 −0.221 * −0.190 0.001 0.093 0.134 0.103 −0.016 −0.002 −0.071 0.155 −0.038 0.070 0.616 ** 0.527 ** 0.286 ** 1
19. Oppositional-defiant (CBCL_ 0.031 −0.198 −0.198 −0.081 −0.031 0.121 0.238 * −0.050 −0.025 0.050 −0.091 0.196 −0.023 0.293 * 0.617 ** 0.600 ** 0.389 ** 0.777 ** 1
20. Conduct problems (CBCL) −0.112 −0.237 * −0.276 * −0.156 −0.007 0.206 0.204 −0.023 −0.017 0.085 −0.075 0.245 * −0.007 0.299 * 0.553 ** 0.398 ** 0.303 ** 0.668 ** 0.790 ** 1
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 986 12 of 15
Table A2. Correlation coefficients for the 7–9 and 9–11-years group.
Measurements 7–9 years 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
1. Visual attentions (NEPSY) 1
2. Delayed memory (NEPSY) −0.189 1
3. Planning (NEPSY) 0.007 −0.215 1
4.Visuospatial processing (NEPSY) 0.120 0.094 −0.084 1
5. Visual focusing −0.019 0.223 0.186 0.242 1
6. Verbal focusing 0.000 0.217 −0.384 −0.016 −0.065 1
7. Anger expression 0.170 −0.011 −0.266 −0.076 0.162 0.034 1
8. Resignation 0.267 0.162 −0.008 −0.227 −0.090 0.382 0.331 1
9. Solution orientation 0.029 −0.243 0.397 −0.073 −0.252 −0.411 −0.346 −0.373 1
10. Substituting the anger −0.462 * 0.047 −0.175 0.216 −0.305 −0.162 −0.381 −0.178 0.077 1
11. Verbalizing the strategies 0.137 0.008 0.413 0.135 0.567 ** 0.234 −0.032 0.287 −0.008 −0.356 1
12. Maladaptive strategies 0.106 0.231 −0.369 −0.012 0.274 0.789 ** 0.586 ** 0.532 * −0.579 ** −0.402 0.335 1
13. Adaptive strategies −0.090 −0.222 0.509* 0.105 −0.046 −0.310 −0.472 * −0.249 0.814 ** 0.286 0.387 −0.498 * 1
14. Emotion regulation −0.019 0.021 −0.360 −0.423 −0.065 0.053 0.225 0.092 0.011 −0.196 −0.212 0.142 −0.237 1
15. Affective problems (CBCL) −0.581 ** 0.167 −0.134 −0.278 −0.138 0.205 −0.036 −0.222 0.195 −0.137 −0.096 0.077 0.048 0.165 1
16. Anxiety (CBCL) −0.368 0.305 −0.271 −0.084 −0.144 0.066 −0.169 −0.279 −0.138 −0.126 −0.424 −0.099 −0.245 0.077 0.524 * 1
17. Somatic problems (CBCL) −0.097 0.202 −0.037 −0.224 −0.250 −0.364 0.169 −0.146 0.354 −0.009 −0.483 * −0.270 0.014 0.359 0.487 * 0.409 1
18. ADHD (CBCL) −0.123 0.245 −0.065 −0.104 −0.097 −0.311 0.265 −0.117 0.229 −0.003 −0.421 −0.142 −0.015 0.005 0.386 0.465 * 0.732 ** 1
19. Oppositional-defiant (CBCL) 0.062 0.215 −0.074 −0.128 −0.068 −0.173 0.042 −0.304 0.316 0.050 −0.527 * −0.159 −0.042 0.244 0.299 0.442 0.650 ** 0.745 ** 1
20. Conduct problems (CBCL) 0.145 0.152 −0.273 −0.185 −0.220 0.012 −0.039 −0.214 −0.249 0.073 −0.467 * −0.089 −0.029 0.438 0.016 0.048 0.425 0.208 0.636 ** 1
Measurements 9–11 years 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
1. Visual attentions (NEPSY) 1
2. Delayed memory (NEPSY) 0.318 1
3. Planning (NEPSY) 0.091 0.398 1
4. Visuospatial processing
0.313 0.227 0.160 1
(NEPSY)
5. Visual focusing 0.042 0.169 0.216 0.251 1
6. Verbal focusing 0.356 −0.191 0.272 0.161 0.095 1
7. Anger expression 0.046 −0.090 −0.528 * −0.458 * −0.033 −0.084 1
8. Resignation 0.230 −0.395 −0.174 −0.058 0.292 −0.341 0.102 1
9. Solution orientation −0.029 0.475 * 0.213 −0.065 −0.120 −0.114 −0.248 −0.586 ** 1
10. Substituting the anger 0.138 0.118 0.219 −0.007 0.051 0.258 −0.221 0.145 −0.066 1
11. Verbalizing the strategies 0.245 0.244 0.010 −0.058 −0.102 −0.103 0.172 −0.287 −0.400 −0.328 1
12. Maladaptive strategies 0.324 −0.203 −0.473 * −0.099 0.349 0.733 ** 0.534 * 0.524 * −0.344 0.088 −0.040 1
13. Adaptive strategies 0.126 0.595 ** 0.312 0.024 −0.083 −0.093 −0.247 −0.581 ** 0.859 ** 0.246 0.503 ** −0.318 1
14. Emotion regulation 0.176 0.351 −0.047 0.308 0.070 −0.099 0.077 0.117 −0.043 0.111 0.247 0.010 0.017 1
15. Affective problems (CBCL) −0.051 0.008 0.049 −0.058 0.257 0.179 0.109 0.094 0.002 −0.183 0.310 0.271 −0.077 0.330 1
16. Anxiety (CBCL) 0.165 −0.020 −0.208 0.285 0.099 0.289 −0.013 0.162 −0.092 −0.255 0.324 0.246 −0.200 0.439 * 0.665 ** 1
17. Somatic problems (CBCL) 0.017 −0.006 −0.131 0.271 0.172 −0.270 −0.115 −0.106 0.058 −0.038 0.237 −0.229 −0.204 0.173 0.212 −0.021 1
18. ADHD (CBCL) 0.106 −0.087 −0.198 0.178 0.239 0.344 0.048 0.316 −0.051 −0.187 0.355 0.380 −0.158 0.470 * 0.798 ** 0.809 ** 0.197 1
19. Oppositional-defiant (CBCL) −0.032 −0.094 −0.270 0.103 0.341 0.158 0.314 0.154 −0.046 −0.135 0.246 0.407 −0.148 0.481 * 0.811 ** 0.624 ** 0.334 0.819 ** 1
20. Conduct problems (CBCL) 0.075 −0.106 −0.267 −0.012 0.199 0.247 0.395 0.168 −0.146 −0.064 0.159 0.482* −0.245 0.507* 0.732 ** 0.452* 0.173 0.634 ** 0.836 ** 1
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 986 13 of 15
References
1. Geurts, H.M.; Verté, S.; Oosterlaan, J.; Roeyers, H.; Sergeant, J.A. How specific are executive functioning
deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2004, 45, 836–854.
[CrossRef]
2. Memisevic, H.; Sinanovic, O. Executive functions as predictors of visual-motor integration in children with
intellectual disability. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2013, 117, 913–922. [CrossRef]
3. Blanken, L.M.; White, T.; Mous, S.E.; Basten, M.; Muetzel, R.L.; Jaddoe, V.W.; Wals, M.; van der
Ende, J.; Verhulst, F.C.; Tiemeier, H. Cognitive functioning in children with internalising, externalising
and dysregulation problems: A population-based study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2017, 26, 445–456.
[CrossRef]
4. Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [CrossRef]
5. Gratz, K.L.; Roemer, L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development,
factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J. Psychopathol. Behav.
Assess. 2004, 26, 41–54. [CrossRef]
6. Röll, J.; Koglin, U.; Petermann, F. Emotion Regulation and Childhood Aggression: Longitudinal Associations.
Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2012, 43, 909–923. [CrossRef]
7. McLaughlin, K.A.; Hatzenbuehler, M.L.; Mennin, D.S.; Nolen-Hoeksema, S. Emotion dysregulation and
adolescent psychopathology: A prospective study. Behav. Res. Ther. 2011, 49, 544–554. [CrossRef]
8. Walcott, C.M.; Landau, S. The relation between disinhibition and emotion regulation in boys with attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2004, 33, 772–782. [CrossRef]
9. Rapport, M.D.; Chung, K.M.; Shore, G.; Isaacs, P. A conceptual model of child psychopathology: Implications
for understanding attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and treatment efficacy. J. Clin. Child Psychol. 2001,
30, 48–58. [CrossRef]
10. Dobrean, A.; Păsărelu, C.R.; Balazsi, R.; Predescu, E. Measurement Invariance of the ADHD Rating Scale–IV
Home and School Versions Across Age, Gender, Clinical Status, and Informant. Assessment 2019. [CrossRef]
11. Burgess, G.C.; Depue, B.E.; Ruzic, L.; Willcutt, E.G.; Du, Y.P.; Banich, M.T. Attentional control activation
relates to working memory in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 2010, 67, 632–640.
[CrossRef]
12. Tillman, C.M.; Eninger, L.; Forssman, L.; Bohlin, G. The relation between working memory components and
ADHD symptoms from a developmental perspective. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2011, 36, 181–198. [CrossRef]
13. Sovner, R.; Hurley, A.D. Four factors affecting the diagnosis of psychiatric disorders in mentally retarded
persons. Psychiatr. Asp. Ment. Retard. Rev. 1986, 5, 45–49.
14. Benson, B.A.; Fuchs, C. Anger-arousing situations and coping responses of aggressive adults with intellectual
disability. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 1999, 24, 207–214. [CrossRef]
15. Jahromi, L.B.; Gulsrud, A.; Kasari, C. Emotional competence in children with Down syndrome: Negativity
and regulation. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 2008, 113, 32–43. [CrossRef]
16. Klenberg, L.; Korkman, M.; Lahti-Nuuttila, P. Differential development of attention and executive functions
in 3-to 12-year-old Finnish children. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2001, 20, 407–428. [CrossRef]
17. Jurado, M.B.; Rosselli, M. The elusive nature of executive functions: A review of our current understanding.
Neuropsychol. Rev. 2007, 17, 213–233. [CrossRef]
18. Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P.; Emerson, M.J.; Witzki, A.H.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T.D. The unity and diversity
of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis.
Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [CrossRef]
19. Pennington, B.F.; Ozonoff, S. Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. J. Child Psychol.
Psychiatry 1996, 37, 51–87. [CrossRef]
20. Kerns, K.A.; McInerney, R.J.; Wilde, N.J. Time reproduction, working memory, and behavioral inhibition in
children with ADHD. Child Neuropsychol. 2001, 7, 21–31. [CrossRef]
21. Dovis, S.; Van der Oord, S.; Wiers, R.W.; Prins, P.J. What part of working memory is not working in ADHD?
Short-term memory, the central executive and effects of reinforcement. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2013, 41,
901–917. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
22. Schuchardt, K.; Gebhardt, M.; Mäehler, C. Working memory functions in children with different degrees of
intellectual disability. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2010, 54, 346–353. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 986 14 of 15
23. Danielsson, H.; Henry, L.; Messer, D.; Rönnberg, J. Strengths and weaknesses in executive functioning in
children with intellectual disability. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2012, 33, 600–607. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
24. Costanzo, F.; Varuzza, C.; Menghini, D.; Addona, F.; Gianesini, T.; Vicari, S. Executive functions in intellectual
disability: A comparison between Williams syndrome and down syndrome. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34,
1770–1780. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
25. Clark, C.; Prior, M.; Kinsella, G. The relationship between executive function abilities, adaptive behaviour,
and academic achievement in children with externalising behaviour problems. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry
2002, 43, 785–796. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
26. Ellis, M.L.; Weiss, B.; Lochman, J.E. Executive functions in children: Associations with aggressive behavior
and appraisal processing. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2009, 37, 945–956. [CrossRef]
27. Achenbach, T.M.; Rescorla, L.A. The Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles; University of Vermont,
Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families: Burlington, VT, USA, 2001.
28. Shields, A.; Cicchetti, D. Emotion regulation among school-age children: The development and validation of
a new criterion Q-sort scale. Dev. Psychol. 1997, 33, 906. [CrossRef]
29. Molina, P.; Sala, M.N.; Zappulla, C.; Bonfigliuoli, C.; Cavioni, V.; Zanetti, M.A.; Baiocco, R.; Laghi, F.;
Pallini, S.; De Stasio, S.; et al. The Emotion Regulation Checklist Italian Translation Validation of parents and
teacher versions. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2014, 11, 624–634. [CrossRef]
30. Santrock, J.W.; Warshak, R.A. Father custody and social development in boys and girls. J. Soc. Issues 1979, 35,
112–125. [CrossRef]
31. Korkman, M.; Kirk, U.; Kemp, S. NEPSY: A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment; Psychological
Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1998.
32. Brooks, B.L.; Sherman, E.M.; Strauss, E. NEPSY-II: A developmental neuropsychological assessment. Child
Neuropsychol. 2009, 16, 80–101. [CrossRef]
33. Rohlf, H.L.; Krahé, B. Assessing anger regulation in middle childhood: Development and validation of a
behavioral observation measure. Front. Psychol. 2014, 6, 453. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
34. Goldberg, M.C.; Mostofsky, S.H.; Cutting, L.E.; Mahone, E.M.; Astor, B.C.; Denckla, M.B. Subtle executive
impairment in children with autism and children with ADHD. J. Autism. Dev. Disord. 2005, 35, 279–293.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
35. Willcutt, E.G.; Doyle, A.E.; Nigg, J.T.; Faraone, S.V.; Pennington, B.F. Validity of the executive function theory
of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biol. Psychiatry 2005, 57, 1336–1346.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
36. Van der Molen, M.J.; van Luit, J.E.H.; Jongmans, M.J.; van der Molen, M.W. Verbal working memory in
children with mild intellectual disabilities. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2007, 51, 162–169. [CrossRef]
37. Mähler, C.; Schuchardt, K. Working memory functioning in children with learning disabilities: Does
intellicence make a difference? J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2009, 53, 3–10. [CrossRef]
38. Henry, L.A. How does the severity of a learning disability affect working memory performance? Memory
2001, 9, 233–247. [CrossRef]
39. Sonuga-Barke, E.J. The dual pathway model of AD/HD: An elaboration of neuro-developmental characteristics.
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2003, 27, 593–604. [CrossRef]
40. Solanto, M.V.; Gilbert, S.N.; Raj, A.; Zhu, J.; Pope-Boyd, S.; Stepak, B.; Vail, L.; Newcorn, J.H. Neurocognitive
functioning in AD/HD, predominantly inattentive and combined subtypes. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2007,
35, 729–744. [CrossRef]
41. Spencer, T.J.; Biederman, J.; Mick, E. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Diagnosis, lifespan,
comorbidities and neurobiology. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2007, 32, 631–642. [CrossRef]
42. Biederman, J.; Spencer, T.J.; Petty, C.; Hyder, L.L.; O’Connor, K.B.; Surman, C.B.; Faraone, S.V. Longitudinal
course of deficient emotional self-regulation CBCL profile in youth with ADHD: Prospective controlled
study. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat 2012, 8, 267–276. [CrossRef]
43. Theeuwes, J.; Kramer, A.F.; Irwin, D.E. Attention on our mind: The role of spatial attention in visual working
memory. Acta Psychol. (Amst) 2011, 137, 248–251. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
44. Awh, E.; Armstrong, K.M.; Moore, T. Visual and oculomotor selection: Links, causes and implications for
spatial attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006, 10, 124–130. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
45. Pearson, D.; Sahraie, A. Oculomotor Control and the Maintenance of Spatially and Temporally Distributed
Events in Visuo-Spatial Working Memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. A 2003, 56, 1089–1111. [CrossRef]
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 986 15 of 15
46. Chun, M.M.; Nakayama, K. On the Functional Role of Implicit Visual Memory for the Adaptive Deployment
of Attention across Scenes. Vis. Cogn. 2000, 7, 65–81. [CrossRef]
47. Mohammadhasani, N.; Fabio, R.A.; Fardanesh, H.; Hatami, J. The link between visual attention and memory
in ADHD and normally developing students: Seeing is remembering? Reti Saperi Linguaggi Ital. J. Cogn. Sci.
2015, 1, 89–102. [CrossRef]
48. Deutsch, C.; Dube, W.; McIlvane, W. Attention Deficits, Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, and
Intellectual Disabilities. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2008, 14, 285–292. [CrossRef]
49. Simonoff, E. ADHD symptoms in children with mild intellectual disability. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc.
Psychiatry 2007, 46, 591–600. [CrossRef]
50. McAvinue, L.P.; Vangkilde, S.; Johnson, K.A.; Habekost, T.; Kyllingsbæk, S.; Bundesen, C.; Robertson, I.H.
A Componential Analysis of Visual Attention in Children with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 2015, 19, 882–894.
[CrossRef]
51. Shaw, P.; Stringaris, A.; Nigg, J.; Leibenluft, E. Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 2014, 171, 276–293. [CrossRef]
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).