5 DeYoung - 2010 - Personality Neuroscience and The Biology of Traits
5 DeYoung - 2010 - Personality Neuroscience and The Biology of Traits
Abstract
Personality neuroscience involves the use of neuroscience methods to study individual differences
in behavior, motivation, emotion, and cognition. Personality psychology has contributed much to
identifying the important dimensions of personality, but relatively little to understanding the bio-
logical sources of those dimensions. However, the rapidly expanding field of personality neurosci-
ence is increasingly shedding light on this topic. This article provides a survey of progress in the
use of neuroscience to study personality traits, organized using a hierarchical model of traits based
on the Big Five dimensions: Extraversion, Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and
Openness ⁄ Intellect. Evidence is reviewed for hypotheses about the biological systems involved in
each trait.
The mission of personality psychology is ‘‘to provide an integrative framework for under-
standing the whole person’’ (McAdams & Pals, 2006; p. 204), and many different meth-
ods must be brought to bear to accomplish such an ambitious goal. The field of
personality neuroscience rests on the premise that the whole person cannot be understood
without understanding the brain. In this article, I discuss the role that neuroscience can
play in personality research and review the progress of this rapidly expanding field. (For
more in depth review of the field and its influential theories see DeYoung & Gray, 2009;
and Zuckerman, 2005.)
Personality psychology’s attempt to understand the whole person calls for a broad con-
ception of personality itself, such as the one provided by McAdams and Pals (2006,
p. 212):
Personality is an individual’s unique variation on the general evolutionary design for human
nature, expressed as a developing pattern of dispositional traits, characteristic adaptations, and
integrative life stories, complexly and differentially situated in culture.
This definition describes three levels at which personality can be analyzed: traits, charac-
teristic adaptations, and life stories. Personality neuroscience has focused primarily on
traits, which are relatively stable patterns of behavior, motivation, emotion, and cognition
(Pytlik Zillig, Hemenover, & Dienstbier, 2002; Wilt & Revelle, 2009) that are not spe-
cific to a particular social milieu or culture. This is not to say that traits are evident to
the same extent or with identical manifestations in all cultures, but rather that any trait
can be observed in a subset of situations in any culture. In contrast to traits, characteristic
adaptations and life stories describe the individual’s specific responses to his or her partic-
ular life circumstances. Obviously, the latter two levels of analysis are crucial for under-
standing any individual, but their complexity renders them less amenable to study by
neuroscience, and this article focuses on the biology of traits.
Traits can be considered probabilistic descriptions of the frequency and intensity with
which individuals exhibit various behavioral, motivational, emotional, and cognitive states
(Fleeson, 2001; Fleeson & Gallagher, 2009). Individuals who are high in some trait will
experience the states associated with that trait more often and more intensely than indi-
viduals low in that trait. For example, someone high in Extraversion will be talkative,
outgoing, and excited more often than someone low in Extraversion, but even the person
low in Extraversion may experience those states occasionally. The aim of personality neu-
roscience is to understand both the biological systems that are responsible for the states
associated with traits and the parameters of those systems that cause them to function dif-
ferently in different individuals. The systems themselves are presumed to be present in
every intact human brain – hence McAdams and Pals’ (2006) reference to ‘‘the general
evolutionary design for human nature’’ – but their parameters will vary from person to
person. (For example, all people have brain systems that respond to rewards, but in differ-
ent individuals these systems will respond with different degrees of vigor to a particular
reward, and the systems’ average level of response may be associated with some personal-
ity trait.)
When considering the biological sources of personality, one must distinguish between
proximal and distal sources. The proximal sources, just described, consist of stable differ-
ences in the functioning of the neural systems that produce the states associated with
traits. The distal sources are both genetic and environmental, as indicated by the fact that
heritability estimates for personality traits are in the range of 40% to 80%, depending on
trait and method (Bouchard & McGue, 2003; Riemann, Angleitner, & Strelau, 1997).
(The heritability of a trait indicates the amount of its variation in a population due to
genetic, rather than environmental, variation.) It is important to remember that when
either genes or situations have lasting effects on traits, they must do so by changing the
brain; thus, personality differences are ‘biological’ regardless of their heritability, in the
sense that they must be proximally generated by the brain no matter whether they origi-
nated in genes or environment.
ª 2010 The Author Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/12 (2010): 1165–1180, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
Social and Personality Psychology Compass ª 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Personality Neuroscience 1167
traits can also be assessed in other ways, such as through laboratory tasks, behavioral
observation, or experience sampling, and personality should not be identified exclusively
with the variables provided by personality questionnaires. What we want to explain in
personality neuroscience is not how people answer questionnaires, but rather why they
exhibit stable patterns of behavior, motivation, emotion, and cognition.
Most studies discussed in this review begin with some psychological trait and attempt
to discover its biological basis. Another valid and useful approach is to begin with stable
individual differences in some biological parameter (such as asymmetry in the level of left
versus right cortical hemisphere activity, measured at rest by EEG) and then to attempt
to discover what psychological trait or traits are associated with that biological trait.
An important caveat for anyone exploring the literature on personality neuroscience or
contemplating entering the field as an investigator is that much inconsistency exists in the
findings to date. In neuroimaging research, inconsistency is probably due in large part to
the use very small samples, due to cost. Unfortunately, small samples often lack statistical
power to detect true effects (Yarkoni, 2009). Worse still, low power has the unfortunate
additional consequence of increasing the proportion of false positives among significant
findings (Green et al., 2008). In genetic studies, though larger samples are typically used,
power may still be an issue, and inconsistency may arise, because each trait is influenced
by many genes, each accounting for only a very small amount of the variance of the trait
(Green et al., 2008). These difficulties highlight the importance of testing reasonably
focused hypotheses, rather than simply exploring associations with biological variables in
the absence of any theory of the causal mechanisms that might underlie a given trait.
ª 2010 The Author Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/12 (2010): 1165–1180, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
Social and Personality Psychology Compass ª 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
1168 Personality Neuroscience
causes), but shared biological causation is at least a reasonable hypothesis. Individual dif-
ferences in the Big Five are strongly genetically influenced (Bouchard & McGue, 2003;
Riemann et al., 1997), and the genetic factor structure of the Big Five appears to be
invariant across European, North American, and East Asian samples (Yamagata et al.,
2006). We have good reason, therefore, to look for biological systems underlying the Big
Five.
However, the Big Five do not constitute the only level of personality traits of interest.
Traits can be arranged hierarchically, with correlated groups of more specific traits cate-
gorized together in broader traits (Markon, 2009). The existence of traits both narrower
and broader than the Big Five may indicate additional biological causes both more and
less specific than those that influence the Big Five.
At least one level of structure exists above the Big Five. Although the Big Five were
originally conceived as independent traits and the most general level of personality
description, research has shown that they are regularly intercorrelated and possess a
higher-order factor structure (DeYoung, 2006; Digman, 1997; McCrae et al., 2008). The
shared variance of Neuroticism (reversed), Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness consti-
tutes one higher-order factor or metatrait, labeled a or Stability, and the shared variance
of Extraversion and Openness ⁄ Intellect constitutes another, labeled b or Plasticity.
Additionally, multiple levels of the trait hierarchy exist below the Big Five. Each Big
Five domain comprises the shared variance of a large number of lower-level traits, often
called facets, with no consensus as to how many facets exist for each domain. Each facet
has been shown to have a unique genetic contribution, suggesting the existence of spe-
cific biological parameters that differentiate facets within a single domain (Jang, McCrae,
Angleitner, Riemann, & Livesley, 1998).
Finally, another level of personality structure appears to exist between the Big Five and
their facets. Jang et al. (2002) found that two genetic factors were necessary to account
for the shared genetic variance among facets within each of the Big Five. If the Big Five
were the next level above the facets, only one genetic factor should have been necessary
for each domain. These mid-level factors were characterized in more detail through factor
analysis of many facets for each domain, followed by examination of how the resulting
factors were correlated with over two thousand personality items (DeYoung, Quilty, &
Peterson, 2007). This analysis indicated that each of the Big Five comprises two separable
but correlated aspects, which constitute a level of personality structure between the facets
and the Big Five.
The four levels of the personality hierarchy just described are depicted in Figure 1.
(Remember that labels for personality traits are always imperfect attempts to capture what
is shared among the various characteristics encompassed by each trait.) Each level can be
considered to reflect causal influences of differing breadth. The existence of the bottom
level indicates that some causes are unique to each facet. At the second level, causes are
shared between facets within each aspect but are not shared with facets in the complemen-
tary aspect in each pair. At the third level, all traits within each of the Big Five share causes
that are not shared with traits in other Big Five domains. Finally, the existence of the meta-
traits implies that traits share causal influences even across some of the Big Five domains.
At least one caveat must be made regarding the model in Figure 1. It is necessarily an
oversimplification because personality does not have simple structure, which means that
some facets and aspects show correlations, not modeled by the hierarchy in Figure 1,
with facets and aspects in other Big Five domains. As one biologically pertinent example,
Assertiveness is negatively correlated with Politeness (which has belligerence at its low
pole), despite the fact that these traits are subsumed by two Big Five traits that are unre-
ª 2010 The Author Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/12 (2010): 1165–1180, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
Social and Personality Psychology Compass ª 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Personality Neuroscience 1169
Facets: (number and identity of facets is uncertain; any list would be partially arbitrary)
Figure 1 A hierarchical model of personality traits based on the Big Five, depicting four levels of organization.
Each level is hypothesized to represent a different set of biological factors that cause personality traits to covary.
(The negative sign by the path from Stability to Neuroticism indicates that Neuroticism is negatively associated with
Stability.).
lated (DeYoung et al., 2007). This association is likely to be of interest for personality
neuroscience because these two traits appear to be influenced in opposite directions by
testosterone (Luxen & Buunk, 2005; McIntyre et al., 2007; Netter, 2004), which may
account for their covariation. To include a trait reflecting this putative causal influence of
testosterone in Figure 1, we would need to add an additional variable outside of the
existing hierarchy (perhaps labeled ‘Competitiveness’ or ‘Dominance’), with arrows direc-
ted at Assertiveness and Politeness. To account for the full complexity of personality
structure, many such additional variables are likely to be necessary. Nonetheless, the
model in Figure 1 can effectively be used to organize most of what is currently known
about the neurobiology of traits.
ª 2010 The Author Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/12 (2010): 1165–1180, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
Social and Personality Psychology Compass ª 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
1170 Personality Neuroscience
Rather than beginning a review of personality neuroscience research with the Big Five,
however, I begin with the metatraits, Stability and Plasticity, because they are likely to
reflect biological systems with very broad impact on both brain function and personality.
Stability appears to represent a general tendency to regulate or restrain potentially disrup-
tive emotion and behavior, whereas Plasticity appears to represent a general tendency to
explore and engage with possibilities (DeYoung, 2006, 2010; DeYoung, Peterson, &
Higgins, 2002; Hirsh, DeYoung, & Peterson, 2009). Evidence is accumulating to suggest
that Stability is related to serotonin, whereas Plasticity may be related to dopamine
(DeYoung & Gray, 2009; Yamagata et al., 2006).
Serotonin and dopamine are very broadly acting neuromodulators that affect an extre-
mely wide array of brain systems. One useful feature of the theory that serotonin and
dopamine are important influences on Stability and Plasticity is that it helps to reconcile
the many different roles that have been proposed for serotonin in personality. Serotonin
typically has regulatory or inhibiting effects on mood, behavior, and cognition (Spoont,
1992) and has been associated with a variety of traits, both empirically and theoretically.
However, almost all of those traits are associated positively or negatively with Stability
and can be classified within Neuroticism, Agreeableness, or Conscientiousness. Low levels
of serotonin are associated with aggression, poor impulse control, and depression, and
drugs that boost serotonergic function are used successfully to mitigate these problems
(Carver & Miller, 2006; Spoont, 1992). As more direct evidence for the role of serotonin
in the Big Five, studies of variation in genes from the serotonin system, as well as studies
using pharmacological manipulation of the serotonin system, have found associations of
serotonergic function with Neuroticism, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness (Cools
et al., 2005; Greenberg et al., 2000; Jang et al., 2001; Lesch et al., 1996; Manuck et al.,
1998).
Plasticity appears to reflect a general exploratory tendency, with Extraversion represent-
ing a more behavioral mode of exploration and Openness ⁄ Intellect a more cognitive
mode. The role of dopamine in exploratory behavior and cognitive function is well-
established (Ashby, Isen, & Turken, 1999; Braver & Barch, 2002; Depue & Collins,
1999; Panksepp, 1998), and a growing body of evidence directly implicates dopaminergic
function in Extraversion (Depue & Collins, 1999; Wacker, Chavanon, & Stemmler,
2006; Wacker & Stemmler, 2006). Some evidence suggests that Openness ⁄ Intellect might
also be influenced by dopamine (DeYoung & Gray, 2009; DeYoung, Peterson, & Hig-
gins, 2005). The dopaminergic system has two branches that may reflect the difference
between Extraversion and Openness ⁄ Intellect. One branch innervates brain structures,
like the nucleus accumbens and amygdala, that are crucially involved in motivation, emo-
tion, and reward (Extraversion), and another branch innervates the prefrontal cortex, the
key brain region for higher cognition (Openness ⁄ Intellect).
Based on the evidence just reviewed, some parameters of the serotonergic and dopa-
minergic systems (such as those affecting rates of neurotransmitter synthesis) seem likely
to account for shared variance among the Big Five. The rest of this review will shift
to focus on biological systems that appear to be specific to each of the Big Five (and
occasionally to their distinct aspects). A great deal is known about the neurobiology of
responses to reward and punishment. As a consequence, more data is available on the
neurobiology of Extraversion and Neuroticism than of Agreeableness, Conscientious-
ness, or Openness ⁄ Intellect. Virtually every personality taxonomy has included traits
similar to Extraversion and Neuroticism. Current biological theories of Extraversion
and Neuroticism are heavily influenced by the work of Jeffrey Gray (1982); Gray &
McNaughton, 2000; Pickering & Gray, 1999), who developed a personality theory
ª 2010 The Author Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/12 (2010): 1165–1180, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
Social and Personality Psychology Compass ª 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Personality Neuroscience 1171
Extraversion
Dopamine, an important component of the BAS, plays a key role in sensitivity to
reward, driving behavior that involves approaching potential rewards (Depue & Collins,
1999). Many of the traits encompassed by Extraversion, such as assertiveness and talka-
tiveness, appear to reflect this approach behavior. Speech, for example, is often used to
attain social reward, and Extraversion is often manifested socially because many human
rewards involve social affiliation or status. Dopamine is most responsible for the drive
to achieve reward, rather than for the enjoyment of reward once it is achieved – for
‘wanting’ rather than ‘liking,’ in other words (Berridge, Robinson, & Aldridge, 2009).
In addition to traits related to wanting, Extraversion encompasses traits that reflect lik-
ing, such as positive emotionality and enjoyment of social interactions. The two aspects
of Extraversion in Figure 1 appear to represent this distinction between liking (Enthusi-
asm, which encompasses sociability and positive emotionality) and wanting (Assertive-
ness). Traits within Enthusiasm are likely to reflect the action of endogenous opioid
systems, which are involved in the positive emotions that follow attainment or con-
sumption of reward and which are particularly important in social affiliation. Pharmaco-
logical manipulation has demonstrated that opiate response to cues of affiliation is a
function of Social Closeness, a trait measure that is an excellent marker of Extraversion
and is more related to Enthusiasm than Assertiveness (Depue & Morrone-Strupinsky,
2005; Markon et al., 2005).
Neurotransmitters like dopamine and the endogenous opiates modulate specific neural
networks that constitute the brain’s reward and approach systems, and multiple neuroi-
maging studies have linked Extraversion to various structures within these systems. Func-
tional MRI studies demonstrate that Extraversion is positively associated with brain
activity at rest or in response to positive or rewarding stimuli, in the medial orbitofrontal
cortex, nucleus accumbens, amygdala, and striatum (Canli, Sivers, Whitfield, Gotlib, &
Gabrieli, 2002; Canli et al., 2001; Cohen, Young, Baek, Kessler, & Ranganath, 2005;
Deckersbach et al., 2006; Mobbs, Hagan, Azim, Menon, & Reiss, 2005). Additionally,
several structural MRI studies have found that Extraversion is associated with greater vol-
ume of medial orbitofrontal cortex, a region involved in coding the value of rewards
(DeYoung et al., 2010; Omura, Constable, & Canli, 2005; Rauch et al., 2005).
A considerable amount of EEG research has been carried out on Extraversion because
Eysenck (1967) hypothesized that Extraversion was a function of levels of cortical arousal
(which EEG is well-suited to measure). Tests of this hypothesis have been rather incon-
clusive, as both EEG and fMRI studies have found that the association between Extraver-
sion and cortical arousal is sometimes positive and sometimes negative (Matthews &
Gilliland, 1999; Zuckerman, 2005). Although Eysenck’s theory seems unlikely to be suffi-
ciently fine-grained at the neural level (as the cortex is large and carries out many differ-
ent functions), some aspects of it may be compatible with the reward sensitivity model.
ª 2010 The Author Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/12 (2010): 1165–1180, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
Social and Personality Psychology Compass ª 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
1172 Personality Neuroscience
Associations of Extraversion with cortical arousal are moderated by the type of situation
in which arousal is measured, and the reward properties of the situation may be crucial in
this moderation (Matthews & Gilliland, 1999). Pharmacological manipulation has shown
that dopamine affects the association of Extraversion with patterns of cortical arousal
(Wacker et al., 2006).
Neuroticism
Neuroticism describes the tendency to experience the negative emotions and cognitions
that accompany experiences of threat and punishment, including anxiety, depression,
anger, irritation, self-consciousness, rumination, and vulnerability. Various brain systems
associated with these reactions to threat and punishment have been linked to Neuroti-
cism. For example, neuroimaging has demonstrated that Neuroticism is associated with
brain activity at rest or in response to aversive or novel stimuli, in the amygdala, insula,
and anterior cingulate (Deckersbach et al., 2006; Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Satpute,
2005; Etkin et al., 2004; Cools et al., 2005; Haas, Omura, Constable, & Canli, 2007a;
Keightley et al., 2003; Reuter et al., 2004). Neuroticism has also been associated with
neural activity in medial prefrontal cortex that is suggestive of poor emotion regulation
(Haas, Constable, & Canli, 2008; Williams et al., 2006) and with reduced volume in that
region (DeYoung et al., 2010).
Gray and McNaughton (2000) suggested that Neuroticism is jointly determined by
sensitivities of the BIS and FFFS, with the BIS responsible for passive avoidance in situa-
tions where goals are in conflict (e.g., an approach-avoidance conflict, such as wanting to
initiate social contact but fearing rejection), and the FFFS responsible for response to
proximal threat or punishment, in which the only motivation is to avoid or eliminate the
threat. Important brain structures in the BIS are the hippocampus and amygdala, whereas
important structures in the FFFS are the amygdala, hypothalamus, and periaqueductal
gray. The FFFS overlaps with the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis that is cen-
tral to hormonal stress responses. Neuroticism has been associated with higher baseline
levels of the stress hormone cortisol, but with reduced phasic increases of cortisol in
response to specific stressors (Netter, 2004). This pattern suggests that those high in Neu-
roticism tend to be not only chronically stressed but also less able to mobilize resources
to deal with particularly stressful situations.
The neurotransmitters serotonin and norepinephrine both modulate the BIS and FFFS
(Gray & McNaughton, 2000), and Neuroticism has been associated with lower levels of
serotonergic function through various methods, including molecular genetics, PET, psy-
chopharmacological manipulation, and assays of cerebrospinal fluid (Cools et al., 2005;
Hennig, 2004; Lesch et al., 1996; Manuck et al., 1998; Schinka, Busch, & Robichaux-
Keene, 2004; Sen, Burmeister, & Ghosh, 2004; Tauscher et al., 2001). Some evidence
also links Neuroticism to higher levels of norepinephrine (Hennig, 2004; White &
Depue, 1999; Zuckerman, 2005).
Finally, EEG research has found that traits related to the Withdrawal aspect of Neurot-
icism are associated with greater activation of the right frontal lobe relative to the left
(Shackman, McMenamin, Maxwell, Greischar, & Davidson, 2009; Zuckerman, 2005).
The brain’s right hemisphere appears to be preferentially involved in emotions and moti-
vational states associated with withdrawal, whereas the left hemisphere is preferentially
involved in approach (Davidson, 2002). To associate all aspects of Neuroticism with the
right hemisphere would be oversimplistic, however. Anger-proneness is an important
component of the Volatility aspect of Neuroticism, and EEG studies have shown anger
ª 2010 The Author Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/12 (2010): 1165–1180, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
Social and Personality Psychology Compass ª 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Personality Neuroscience 1173
to be associated with greater relative left frontal lobe activation, presumably because anger
involves approach (Harmon-Jones, 2004; Harmon-Jones & Allen, 1998).
Agreeableness
All of the traits encompassed by Agreeableness appear to reflect a tendency toward altru-
ism and cooperation as opposed to antisocial and exploitative behavior. Agreeable people
are interested in and considerate of others’ needs, desires, and feelings, and they avoid
aggressing or imposing their will on others. Such behavior requires the ability to under-
stand others’ emotions, intentions, and mental states, and Agreeableness has been found
to predict tests of empathy, theory of mind, and other forms of social information pro-
cessing (Graziano, Habashi, Sheese, & Tobin, 2007; Nettle & Liddle, 2008; Wilkowski,
Robinson, & Meier, 2006). One MRI study found that Agreeableness is associated with
volume in brain regions associated with social information processing, including superior
temporal sulcus, posterior cingulate cortex, and fusiform gyrus (DeYoung et al., 2010).
Agreeableness also appears to be associated with the ability to suppress aggressive
impulses and other socially disruptive emotions (Meier, Robinson, & Wilkowski, 2006),
and an fMRI study found that Agreeableness predicted activity in left dorsolateral pre-
frontal cortex associated with emotion regulation (Haas, Omura, Constable, & Canli,
2007b). Presumably because aggression is often driven by anger, trait anger is correlated
with Agreeableness as well as with Neuroticism, and anger has sometimes been treated as
a component of low Agreeableness (Saucier, 2009). Its primary location within Neuroti-
cism, however, is consistent with the tendency of all negative emotions to covary. Reac-
tive aggression might be considered as the joint consequence of high Neuroticism and
low Agreeableness, whereas more instrumental forms of aggression may be more exclu-
sively associated with Agreeableness.
Although Agreeableness has not often been targeted explicitly by neuroscience, a num-
ber of fMRI studies have used self-report measures of empathy, which is a facet of
Agreeableness. Individual differences in empathy have been associated with neural activity
during observation and imitation of others’ actions (Gazzola, Aziz-Zadeh, & Keysers,
2006; Kaplan & Iacoboni, 2006) and during perception of others’ emotional expressions
(Chakrabarti, Bullmore, & Baron-Cohen, 2007; Schulte-Rüther, Markowitsch, Fink, &
Piefke, 2007). In these studies, empathy was found to be positively associated with activ-
ity in the medial prefrontal cortex, superior temporal sulcus, and mirror neuron system
(regions of frontal and parietal cortex that are similarly active when performing or observ-
ing the same action). Another study (Tankersley, Stowe, & Huettel, 2007) found that a
self-report measure of altruism was positively associated with activity in posterior superior
temporal sulcus, while observing performance of a task, in contrast to performing the task
oneself.
Conscientiousness
Human beings are unique as a species in their ability to govern their behavior by explicit
systems of rules and to constrain their behavior in order to pursue goals that may be
achieved only in the distant future. Conscientiousness appears to reflect variation in the
capacity for self-discipline and organization that is necessary for this form of top-down
control. Conscientiousness predicts academic and occupational success, as well as behavior
that promote health and longevity (Ozer & Benet-Martinez, 2006). It is likely to be asso-
ciated with functions of the prefrontal cortex, the brain region responsible for much of
ª 2010 The Author Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/12 (2010): 1165–1180, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
Social and Personality Psychology Compass ª 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
1174 Personality Neuroscience
the human ability to plan and follow complex rules (Bunge & Zelazo, 2006; Miller &
Cohen, 2001). A structural MRI study found that Conscientiousness was associated with
greater volume of the middle frontal gyrus in lateral prefrontal cortex (DeYoung et al.,
2010), a region involved in maintaining goal-relevant information in working memory
and in the execution of planned action based on abstract rules (Bunge & Zelazo, 2006).
As with Agreeableness, few neuroscience studies have targeted Conscientiousness
explicitly. However, many studies have targeted related constructs (reflecting low Consci-
entiousness), such as impulsivity, novelty seeking, and sensation seeking. Functional neu-
roimaging studies have found that individual differences in self-reported impulsivity are
associated with neural activity in both dorsal and ventral regions of lateral prefrontal cor-
tex (Asahi, Okamato, Akado, Yamawaki, & Yokota, 2004; Brown, Manuck, Flory, &
Hariri, 2006).
Caution is required in interpreting studies of impulsivity, novelty seeking, and sensa-
tion seeking because these traits are heterogeneous in their associations with the Big Five,
usually associated with Extraversion and sometimes with Neuroticism, as well as with
Conscientiousness (Depue & Collins, 1999; Markon et al., 2005; Whiteside & Lynam,
2001). They seem to be compound traits, influenced by multiple more basic traits. Their
compound nature appears to reflect the fact that problems of impulse control may be
exacerbated both by weakness of the top-down control systems that override impulses
(the presumed substrate of Conscientiousness) or by potentiation of the impulses them-
selves, which may be responses to cues of reward (Extraversion) or punishment (Neuroti-
cism). For example, Zuckerman (2005) noted that many studies have found traits in this
group to be associated with high levels of dopaminergic function and low levels of sero-
tonergic function. However, he argued that dopamine is associated with impulses to
approach reward, whereas low serotonin is associated with the absence of control or
restraint of those impulses. The serotonergic component is the one that appears to be
related to Conscientiousness (Manuck et al., 1998), though recall that it is also associated
with Neuroticism and Agreeableness.
Openness ⁄ Intellect
The two aspects of Openness ⁄ Intellect, not surprisingly, can be described as Intellect and
Openness to Experience, which appear to reflect engagement with abstract or intellectual
information (Intellect) and engagement with aesthetic or sensory information (Openness).
These two traits are equally central to the larger Big Five domain (DeYoung et al., 2007;
Johnson, 1994; Saucier, 1992), and both are likely to be associated with the functioning
of attention and the ability to process complex information. In keeping with this hypoth-
esis, Openness ⁄ Intellect is the only Big Five dimension consistently positively associated
with cognitive abilities such as intelligence and working memory capacity (DeYoung,
Shamosh, Green, Braver, & Gray, 2009; DeYoung et al., 2005).
Intellect is more strongly linked to intelligence and working memory than is Open-
ness (DeYoung et al., 2005, 2009). An fMRI study demonstrated that Intellect, but not
Openness, was associated with brain activity during a difficult working memory task, in
two regions of the prefrontal cortex, the left frontal pole and the posterior medial fron-
tal cortex (DeYoung et al., 2009). The frontal pole is involved in abstract integration
of multiple cognitive operations and in drawing abstract analogies (Gilbert et al., 2006;
Green, Fugelsang, Kraemer, Shamosh, & Dunbar, 2006; Ramnani & Owen, 2004).
The posterior medial frontal cortex is involved in monitoring goal-directed performance
and detecting the likelihood of error during cognitive tasks (Brown & Braver, 2005;
ª 2010 The Author Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/12 (2010): 1165–1180, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
Social and Personality Psychology Compass ª 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Personality Neuroscience 1175
Ridderinkhof, Ullsperger, Crone, & Nieuwenhuis, 2004). Neural activity in the frontal
pole was associated with self-ratings of Intellect because of variance shared between
Intellect and performance on intelligence tests, suggesting that this region is involved in
the ability component of Intellect. In posterior medial frontal cortex, however, Intellect
remained significantly related to neural activity even after controlling for intelligence,
suggesting that this region and its functions may be involved in intellectual engagement,
independently of ability. In addition to intelligence, intellectual engagement entails a
motivation to succeed at cognitive tasks, which is presumably associated with increased
monitoring of cognitive performance.
Dopamine’s involvement in Openness ⁄ Intellect is likely to be a function of dopamine’s
influence on the prefrontal cortex. One study (Harris et al., 2005) reports an association
of variation in the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene with Openness ⁄ Intellect,
and COMT provides the enzyme that is the primary mechanism for clearing dopamine
from the synapse in prefrontal cortex and thus regulates levels of dopaminergic activity in
that brain region (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2006).
ª 2010 The Author Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/12 (2010): 1165–1180, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
Social and Personality Psychology Compass ª 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
1176 Personality Neuroscience
Unfortunately, some biases will be present even in the pool of personality descriptors
drawn from natural languages. Socially salient traits are likely to be over-represented, for
example (because language is used primarily for social purposes), leading to the possibility
of over-representation of factors related to socially oriented behavior. Thus, the mere fact
that a six factor model is more replicable than the Big Five in various languages is not an
inherently sufficient reason to prefer it to the Big Five. Additional theoretical consider-
ations, such as patterns of association with biological mechanisms, are necessary, which
highlights the role that neuroscience can play in theory construction in personality psy-
chology.
The relationship between personality psychology and neuroscience should be viewed
as a two-way street. Personality psychology can help to guide neuroscience hypotheses
and to organize and synthesize neuroscience findings. Additionally, however, neurosci-
ence data may influence personality psychologists in their development of trait models, as
Eysenck (1997) suggested. Using neuroscience methods to study personality has the
potential to produce explanatory biological models for trait taxonomies that were at first
purely descriptive, and these models may help to realize the goal of a theory of personal-
ity as a system of dynamic, interacting elements that generates the ongoing flux of behav-
ior and experience (DeYoung, 2010). Neuroscience is well suited to such a theory
because each brain system interacts with many other brain systems, and neuroscience
models typically attempt to consider these interactions. A causal theory of personality
could be based on psychological constructs alone, but this would ignore the fact that the
brain is the proximal cause of behavior and experience. Neuroscience can therefore use-
fully constrain psychological theorizing. Anyone interested in the psychological functions
that underlie personality traits should be interested in the results of personality neurosci-
ence, as neuroscience can be used to test and refine our understanding of the mechanisms
involved in personality.
Short Biography
Colin DeYoung is Assistant Professor in Psychology at the University of Minnesota, in
the Personality, Individual Differences, and Behavior Genetics area. He received an A.B.
from Harvard University, completed his doctorate at the University of Toronto, and
worked as a postdoctoral fellow at Yale University before moving to the University of
Minnesota. In 2007, he won the J.S. Tanaka Dissertation Award for methodological and
substantive contributions to the field of personality psychology. His research focuses on
the structure and sources of personality, using neuroscience methods to investigate the
biological substrates of personality traits.
Endnote
* Correspondence address: Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, 75 East River Rd., Minneapolis,
MN 55455, USA. Email: [email protected]
References
Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., & Spinath, F. M. (2004). Investigating the ZKPQ-III-R: Psychometric properties,
relations to the Five-Factor Model and genetic and environmental influences on its scales and facets. In R. M.
Stelmack (Ed.), On the Psychobiology of Personality: Essays in Honor of Marvin Zuckerman (pp. 89–105). New York:
Elsevier.
ª 2010 The Author Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/12 (2010): 1165–1180, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
Social and Personality Psychology Compass ª 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Personality Neuroscience 1177
Asahi, S., Okamato, Y., Akado, G., Yamawaki, S., & Yokota, N. (2004). Negative correlation between right pre-
frontal activity during response inhibition and impulsiveness: A fMRI study. European Archive of Psychiatry and
Clinical Neuroscience, 254, 245–251.
Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence
on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550.
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of the HEXACO model of per-
sonality structure. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 150–166.
Berridge, K. C., Robinson, T. E., & Aldridge, J. W. (2009). Dissecting components of reward: ‘Liking,’ ‘wanting,’
and learning. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 9, 65–73.
Bouchard, T. J., & McGue, M. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on human psychological differences.
Journal of Neurobiology, 54, 4–45.
Braver, T. S., & Barch, D. M. (2002). A theory of cognitive control, aging cognition, and neuromodulation. Neuro-
science & Biobehavioral Review, 26, 809–817.
Brown, J. W., & Braver, T. S. (2005). Learned predictions of error likelihood in the anterior cingulate cortex.
Science, 307, 1118–1121.
Brown, S. M., Manuck, S. B., Flory, J. D., & Hariri, A. R. (2006). Neural basis of individual differences in impul-
sivity: Contributions of corticolimbic circuits for behavioral arousal and control. Emotion, 6, 239–245.
Bunge, S. A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2006). A brain-based account of the development of rule use in childhood. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 118–121.
Canli, T., Sivers, I., Whitfield, S. L., Gotlib, I. H., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Amygdala response to happy faces
as a function of Extraversion. Science, 296, 2191.
Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E., Gross, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001). An fMRI study of personality
influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 33–42.
Carver, C. S., & Miller, C. J. (2006). Relations of serotonin function to personality: Current views and a key
methodological issue. Psychiatry Research, 144, 1–15.
Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2007). Empathizing with basic emotions: Common and discrete
neural substrates. Social Neuroscience, 1, 364–384.
Cloninger, C. R. (1987). A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 573–588.
Cohen, M. X., Young, J., Baek, J.-M., Kessler, C., & Ranganath, C. (2005). Individual differences in extraversion
and dopamine genetics predict neural reward responses. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 851–861.
Cools, R., Calder, A. J., Lawrence, A. D., Clark, L., Bullmore, E., & Robbins, T. W. (2005). Individual differ-
ences in threat sensitivity predict serotonergic modulation of amygdala response to fearful faces. Psychopharmacol-
ogy, 180, 670–679.
Davidson, R. J. (2002). Anxiety and affective style: Role of prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Biological Psychiatry, 51,
68–80.
Deckersbach, T., Miller, K. K., Klibanski, A., Fischman, A., Dougherty, D. D., Blais, M. A., et al. (2006). Regio-
nal cerebral brain metabolism correlates of Neuroticism and Extraversion. Depression and Anxiety, 23, 133138.
Denissen, J. J. A., & Penke, L. (2008). Motivational individual reaction norms underlying the Five-Factor model of
personality: First steps towards a theory-based conceptual framework. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1285–
1302.
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of
incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491–569.
Depue, R. A., & Morrone-Strupinsky, J. V. (2005). A neurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: Implications
for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 313–350.
DeYoung, C. G. (2006). Higher-order factors of the Big Five in a multi-informant sample. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 91, 1138–1151.
DeYoung, C. G. (2010). Toward a theory of the Big Five. Psychological Inquiry, 21, 26–33.
DeYoung, C. G., & Gray, J. R. (2009). Personality neuroscience: Explaining individual differences in affect, behav-
ior, and cognition. In P. J. Corr & G. Matthews (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Personality Psychology (pp.
323–346). New York: Cambridge University Press.
DeYoung, C. G., Hirsh, J. B., Shane, M. S., Papademetris, X., Rajeevan, N., & Gray, J. R. (2010). Testing predic-
tions from personality neuroscience: Brain structure and the Big Five. Psychological Science, 21, 820–828.
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2002). Higher-order factors of the Big Five predict confor-
mity: Are there neuroses of health? Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 533–552.
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Sources of Openness ⁄ Intellect: Cognitive and neuro-
psychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. Journal of Personality, 73, 825–858.
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 880–896.
DeYoung, C. G., Shamosh, N. A., Green, A. E., Braver, T. S., & Gray, J. R. (2009). Intellect as distinct from Open-
ness: Differences revealed by fMRI of working memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 883–892.
ª 2010 The Author Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/12 (2010): 1165–1180, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
Social and Personality Psychology Compass ª 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
1178 Personality Neuroscience
Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology, 41,
417–440.
Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1246–1256.
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Satpute, A. B. (2005). Personality from a controlled processing perspec-
tive: An fMRI study of neuroticism, extraversion, and self-consciousness. Cognitive Affective Behavioral Neuroscience,
5, 169–181.
Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Approach-avoidance motivation in personality: Approach and avoidance tem-
peraments and goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 804–818.
Etkin, A., Klemenhagen, K. C., Dudman, J. T., Rogan, M. T., Hen, R., Kandel, E. R., et al. (2004). Individual
differences in trait anxiety predict the response of the basolateral amygdala to unconsciously processed fearful
faces. Neuron, 44, 1043–1055.
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The Biological Basis of Personality. Springfield: Thomas.
Eysenck, H. J. (1997). Personality and experimental psychology: The unification of psychology and the possibility
of a paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1224–1237.
Fleeson, W. (2001). Towards a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: Traits as density distributions
of states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 1011–1027.
Fleeson, W., & Gallagher, P. (2009). The implications of Big Five standing for the distribution of trait manifestation
in behavior: Fifteen experience-sampling studies and a meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97,
1097–1114.
Gazzola, V., Aziz-Zadeh, L., & Keysers, C. (2006). Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in
humans. Current Biology, 16, 1824–1829.
Gilbert, S. J., Spengler, S., Simons, J. S., Steele, J. D., Lawrie, S. M., Frith, C. D., et al. (2006). Functional special-
ization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10): A meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(6), 932–948.
Gray, J. A. (1982). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the Functions of the Septo–Hippocampal System.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the Functions of the Septo-
Hippocampal System (Second Edition). New York: Oxford University Press.
Graziano, W. G., Habashi, M. M., Sheese, B. E., & Tobin, R. M. (2007). Agreeableness, empathy, and helping: A
person x situation perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 583–599.
Green, A. E., Fugelsang, J. A., Kraemer, D. J., Shamosh, N. A., & Dunbar, K. N. (2006). Frontopolar cortex
mediates abstract integration in analogy. Brain Research, 1096, 125–137.
Green, A. E., Munafo, M. R., DeYoung, C. G., Fossella, J. A., Fan, J., & Gray, J. R. (2008). Using genetic data
in cognitive neuroscience: From growing pains to genuine insights. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 710–720.
Greenberg, B. D., Li, Q., Lucas, F. R., Hu, S., Sirota, L. A., Benjamin, J., et al. (2000). Association between the
serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism and personality traits in a primarily female population sample.
American Journal of Medical Genetics, 96, 202–216.
Haas, B. W., Constable, R. T., & Canli, T. (2008). Stop the sadness: Neuroticism is associated with sustained med-
ial prefrontal cortex response to emotional facial expressions. Neuroimage, 42, 385–392.
Haas, B. W., Omura, K., Constable, T., & Canli, T. (2007a). Emotional conflict and Neuroticism: Personality-
dependent activation in the amygdala and subgenual anterior cingulate. Behavioral Neuroscience, 121, 249–256.
Haas, B. W., Omura, K., Constable, T., & Canli, T. (2007b). Is automatic emotion regulation associated with
Agreeableness? A perspective using a social neuroscience approach. Psychological Science, 18, 130–132.
Haig, B. D. (2005). Exploratory factor analysis, theory generation, and scientific method. Multivariate Behavioral
Research, 40, 303–329.
Harmon-Jones, E. (2004). Contributions from research on anger and cognitive dissonance to understanding the
motivational functions of asymmetrical frontal brain activity. Biological Psychology, 67, 51–76.
Harmon-Jones, E., & Allen, J. J. B. (1998). Anger and frontal brain activity: EEG asymmetry consistent with
approach motivation despite negative affective valence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1310–1316.
Harris, S. E., Wright, A. F., Hayward, C., Starr, J. M., Whalley, L. J., & Deary, I. J. (2005). The functional
COMT polymorphism, Val158Met, is associated with logical memory and the personality trait intellect ⁄ imagina-
tion in a cohort of healthy 79 year olds. Neuroscience Letters, 385, 1–6.
Hennig, J. (2004). Personality, serotonin, and noradrenaline. In R. M. Stelmack (Ed.), On the Psychobiology of
Personality: Essays in Honor of Marvin Zuckerman (pp. 379–395). New York: Elsevier.
Hirsh, J. B., DeYoung, C. G., & Peterson, J. B. (2009). Metatraits of the Big Five differentially predict engagement
and restraint of behavior. Journal of Personality, 77, 1085–1102.
Jang, K. L., Hu, S., Livesley, W. J., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., Ando, J., et al. (2001). Covariance structure of
Neuroticism and Agreeableness: A twin and molecular genetic analysis of the role of the serotonin transporter
gene. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 295–304.
Jang, K. L., Hu, S., Livesley, W. J., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., & Vernon, P. A. (2002). Genetic and environ-
mental influences on the covariance of facets defining the domains of the five-factor model of personality. Person-
ality and Individual Differences, 33, 83–101.
ª 2010 The Author Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/12 (2010): 1165–1180, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
Social and Personality Psychology Compass ª 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Personality Neuroscience 1179
Jang, K. L., McCrae, R. R., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., & Livesley, W. J. (1998). Heritability of facet-level traits
in a cross-cultural twin sample: Support for a hierarchical model of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 74, 1556–1565.
John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five trait taxonomy:
History: measurement, and conceptual issue. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of
Personality: Theory and Research (pp. 114–158). New York: Guilford Press.
Johnson, J. A. (1994). Clarification of factor five with the help of the AB5C model. European Journal of Personality,
8, 311–334.
Kaplan, J. T., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Getting a grip on other minds: Mirror neurons, intention understanding and
cognitive empathy. Social Neuroscience, 1, 175–183.
Keightley, M. L., Seminowicz, D. A., Bagby, R. M., Costa, P. T., Fossati, P., & Mayberg, H. S. (2003). Personal-
ity influences limbic-cortical interactions during sad mood. NeuroImage, 20, 2031–2039.
Lesch, K. P., Bengel, D., Heils, A., Sabol, S. Z., Greenberg, B. D., Petri, S., et al. (1996). Association of anxi-
ety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science, 274, 1527–
1531.
Luxen, M. F., & Buunk, B. P. (2005). Second-to-fourth digit ratio related to verbal and numerical intelligence and
the Big Five. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 959–966.
Manuck, S. B., Flory, J. D., McCaffery, J. M., Matthews, K. A., Mann, J. J., & Muldoon, M. F. (1998). Aggres-
sion, impulsivity, and central nervous system serotonergic responsivity in a nonpatient sample. Neuropsychopharma-
cology, 19, 287–299.
Markon, K. E. (2009). Hierarchies in the structure of personality traits. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3,
812–826.
Markon, K. E., Krueger, R. F., & Watson, D. (2005). Delineating the structure of normal and abnormal personal-
ity: An integrative hierarchical approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 139–157.
Matthews, G., & Gilliland, K. (1999). The personality theories of H. J. Eysenck and J. A. Gray: A comparative
review. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 583–626.
McAdams, D. P., & Pals, J. L. (2006). A new Big Five: Fundamental principles for an integrative science of person-
ality. American Psychologist, 61, 204–217.
McCrae, R. R., Jang, K. L., Ando, J., Ono, Y., Yamagata, S., Riemann, R., et al. (2008). Substance and artifact in
the higher-order factors of the big five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 442–455.
McIntyre, M. H., Barrett, E. S., McDermott, R., Johnson, D. D. P., Cowden, J., & Rosen, S. P. (2007). Finger
length ratio (2D:4D) and sex differences in aggression during a simulated war game. Personality and Individual
Differences, 42, 755–764.
Meier, B. P., Robinson, M. D., & Wilkowski, B. M. (2006). Turning the other cheek: Agreeableness and the
regulation of aggressionrelated primes. Psychological Science, 17, 136–142.
Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Nichols, T., Callicott, J. H., Ding, J., Kolachana, B., Buckholtz, J., et al. (2006). Impact of
complex genetic variation in COMT on human brain function. Molecular Psychiatry, 11, 867–877.
Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neurosci-
ence, 24, 167–202.
Mobbs, D., Hagan, C. C., Azim, E., Menon, V., & Reiss, A. L. (2005). Personality predicts activity in reward and
emotional regions associated with humor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 102, 16502–16506.
Netter, P. (2004). Personality and hormones. In R. M. Stelmack (Ed.), On the Psychobiology of personality: Essays in
Honor of Marvin Zuckerman (pp. 353–377). New York: Elsevier.
Nettle, D. (2006). The evolution of personality variation in humans and other animals. American Psychologist, 61,
622–631.
Nettle, D. (2007). Personality: What Makes You the Way You Are. New York: Oxford University Press.
Nettle, D., & Liddle, B. (2008). Agreeableness is related to social-cognitive, but not social-perceptual, theory of
mind. European Journal of Personality, 22, 323–335.
Omura, K., Constable, R. T., & Canli, T. (2005). Amygdala gray matter concentration is associated with extraver-
sion and neuroticism. NeuroReport, 16, 1905–1908.
Ozer, D. J., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2006). Personality and the prediction of consequential outcomes. Annual Review
of Psychology, 57, 201–221.
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotion. New York: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Pickering, A. D., & Gray, J. A. (1999). The neuroscience of personality. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.),
Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (Second Edition) (pp. 277–299). New York: Guilford Press.
Pytlik Zillig, L. M., Hemenover, S. H., & Dienstbier, R. A. (2002). What do we assess when we assess a Big 5
trait? A content analysis of the affective, behavioral and cognitive processes represented in the Big 5 personality
inventories. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 847–858.
Ramanaiah, N. V., Rielage, J. K., & Cheng, Y. (2002). Cloninger’s temperament and character inventory and the
NEO Five–Factor Inventory. Psychological Reports, 90, 59–63.
ª 2010 The Author Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/12 (2010): 1165–1180, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
Social and Personality Psychology Compass ª 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
1180 Personality Neuroscience
Ramnani, N., & Owen, A. M. (2004). Anterior prefrontal cortex: Insights into function from anatomy and neuro-
imaging. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(3), 184–194.
Rauch, S. L., Milad, M. R., Orr, S. P., Quinn, B. T., Fischl, B., & Pitman, R. K. (2005). Orbitofrontal thickness,
retention of fear extinction, and extraversion. NeuroReport, 16, 1909–1912.
Reuter, M., Stark, R., Henning, J., Walter, B., Kirsch, P., Schienle, A., & Vaitl, D. (2004). Personality and emo-
tion: Test of Gray’s personality theory by means of an fMRI study. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 462–469.
Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal
cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443–447.
Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Strelau, J. (1997). Genetic and environmental influences on personality: A study of
twins reared together using the self- and peer report NEO-FFI scales. Journal of Personality, 65, 449–476.
Saucier, G. (1992). Openness versus intellect: Much ado about nothing? European Journal of Personality, 6, 381–386.
Saucier, G. (2009). Recurrent personality dimensions in inclusive lexical studies: Indications for a Big Six structure.
Journal of Personality, 77, 1577–1614.
Schinka, J. A., Busch, R. M., & Robichaux-Keene, N. (2004). A meta-analysis of the association between the sero-
tonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and trait anxiety. Molecular Psychiatry, 9, 197–202.
Schulte-Rüther, M., Markowitsch, H. J., Fink, G. R., & Piefke, M. (2007). Mirror neuron and theory of mind
mechanisms involved in face-to-face interactions: A functional magnetic resonance imaging approach to empathy.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1354–1372.
Sen, S., Burmeister, M., & Ghosh, D. (2004). Meta-analysis of the association between a serotonin transporter pro-
moter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and anxiety-related personality traits. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part
B (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 127B, 85–89.
Shackman, A. J., McMenamin, B. W., Maxwell, J. S., Greischar, L. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2009). Right dorsolat-
eral prefrontal cortical activity and behavioral inhibition. Psychological Science, 20, 1500–1506.
Smillie, L. D., Pickering, A. D., & Jackson, C. J. (2006). The new Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory: Implications
for personality measurement. Personality & Social Psychology Review, 10, 320–335.
Spoont, M. R. (1992). Modulatory role of serotonin in neural information processing: Implications for human
psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 330–350.
Tankersley, D., Stowe, C. J., & Huettel, S. A. (2007). Altruism is associated with an increased neural response to
agency. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 150–151.
Tauscher, J., Bagby, M. R., Javanmard, M., Christensen, B. K., Kasper, S., & Kapur, S. (2001). Inverse relationship
between serotonin 5-HT1a receptor binding and anxiety: A [11C]WAY-100635 PET investigation in healthy
volunteers. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1326–1328.
Van Egeren, L. F. (2009). A cybernetic model of global personality traits. Personality and Social Psychology Review,
13, 92–108.
Wacker, J., Chavanon, M.-L., & Stemmler, G. (2006). Investigating the dopaminergic basis of Extraversion in
humans: A multilevel approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 171–187.
Wacker, J., & Stemmler, G. (2006). Agentic extraversion modulates the cardiovascular effects of the dopamine D2
agonist bromocriptine. Psychophysiology, 43, 372–381.
White, T. L., & Depue, R. A. (1999). Differential association of traits of fear and anxiety with norepinephrine- and
dark-induced pupil reactivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 863–877.
Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, R. W. (2001). The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of
personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 669–689.
Wilkowski, B. M., Robinson, M. D., & Meier, B. P. (2006). Agreeableness and the prolonged spatial processing of
antisocial and prosocial information. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 1152–1168.
Williams, L. M., Brown, K. J., Palmer, D., Liddell, B. J., Kemp, A. H., Olivieri, G., et al. (2006). The mellow
years?: Neural basis of improving emotional stability over age. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 6422–6430.
Wilt, J., & Revelle, W. (2009). Extraversion. In M. Leary & R. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of Individual Differences in
Social Behavior (pp. 27–45). New York: Guilford.
Yamagata, S., Suzuki, A., Ando, J., Ono, Y., Kijima, N., Yoshimura, K., et al. (2006). Is the genetic structure of
human personality universal? A cross-cultural twin study from North America, Europe, and Asia. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 90, 987–998.
Yarkoni, T. (2009). Big correlations in little studies: Inflated fMRI correlations reflect low statistical power—Com-
mentary on Vul et al. (2009). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 294–298.
Zelenski, J. M., & Larsen, R. J. (1999). Susceptibility to affect: A comparison of three personality taxonomies. Jour-
nal of Personality, 67, 761–791.
Zuckerman, M. (2005). Psychobiology of Personality (Second Edition, Revised and Updated). New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D. M., Joireman, J., Teta, P., & Kraft, M. (1993). A comparison of three structural
models of personality: The Big Three, the Big Five, and the Alternative Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 65, 757–768.
ª 2010 The Author Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/12 (2010): 1165–1180, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x
Social and Personality Psychology Compass ª 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd