The document discusses repeated measures design in experimental research, where participants serve as their own controls by participating in both experimental and control conditions. It highlights the advantages of this design, such as requiring fewer participants and increasing sensitivity to detect small effects, while also addressing the potential confounding of practice effects. Techniques like block randomization and ABBA counterbalancing are presented to balance practice effects and ensure the validity of the results.
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0 ratings0% found this document useful (0 votes)
6 views
Repeated Measures Design for Bs
The document discusses repeated measures design in experimental research, where participants serve as their own controls by participating in both experimental and control conditions. It highlights the advantages of this design, such as requiring fewer participants and increasing sensitivity to detect small effects, while also addressing the potential confounding of practice effects. Techniques like block randomization and ABBA counterbalancing are presented to balance practice effects and ensure the validity of the results.
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 20
REPEATED MEASURES
DESIGN Lecture by Dr. Tehreem Arshad Key q.1
Independent measures design, also known as between-groups, is an experimental
design where different participants are used in each condition of the independent variable. This means that each condition of the experiment includes a different group of participants. This should be done by random allocation, ensuring that each participant has an equal chance of being assigned to one group. Introduction • In an independent groups design, a separate group serves as a control for the group given the experimental treatment. • In a repeated measures design, subjects serve as their own controls because they participate in both the experimental and control conditions. • In repeated measures designs, participants can undergo changes during the experiment as they are repeatedly tested. • Participants may improve with practice, for example, because they learn more about the task or because they become more relaxed in the experimental situation. • They also may get worse with practice—for example, because of fatigue or reduced motivation. These temporary changes are called practice effects. • Individual differences among participants cannot be eliminated in the random groups design, but they can be balanced by using random assignment. • Similarly, the practice effects that participants experience due to repeated testing in the repeated measures designs cannot be eliminated. Like individual differences in the random groups design, however, practice effects can be balanced, or averaged, across the conditions of a repeated measures design experiment. • When balanced across the conditions, practice effects are not confounded with the independent variable and the results of the experiment are interpretable. WHY RESEARCHERS USE REPEATED MEASURES DESIGNS • Researchers choose to use a repeated measures design in order to (1) conduct an experiment when few participants are available (2) conduct the experiment more efficiently (3) increase the sensitivity of the experiment (4) study changes in participants’ behavior over time. • First, repeated measures designs require fewer participants than an independent groups design, so these designs are ideal for situations in which only a small number of participants is available. • Researchers who do experiments with children, the elderly, or special populations such as individuals with brain injuries frequently have a small number of participants available. • Researchers choose to use repeated measures designs even when sufficient numbers of participants are available for an independent groups design. • Another important advantage of repeated measures designs is that they are generally more sensitive than an independent groups design. • The sensitivity of an experiment refers to the ability to detect the effect of the independent variable even if the effect is a small one. • Ideally, participants in a study respond similarly to an experimental manipulation. In practice, however, we know that people don’t all respond the same way. This error variation can be due to variations in the procedure each time the experiment is conducted or to individual differences among the participants. • • The increased sensitivity of repeated measures designs is especially attractive to researchers who study independent variables that have small (hard-to-see) effects on behavior. THE ROLE OF PRACTICE EFFECTS IN REPEATED MEASURES DESIGNS • Repeated measures designs cannot be confounded by individual differences variables because the same individuals participate in each condition (level) of the independent variable. • Participants’ performance in repeated measures designs may change across conditions simply because of repeated testing (not because of the independent variable); these changes are called practice effects. • Practice effects may threaten the internal validity of a repeated measures experiment when the different conditions of the independent variable are presented in the same order to all participants. Defining Practice Effects • In a repeated measures design, the characteristics of the participants cannot confound the independent variable being manipulated in the experiment. • The same participants are tested in all the conditions of a repeated measures design, so it is impossible to end up with brighter, healthier, or more motivated participants in one condition than in another condition. • Stated more formally, there can be no confounding by individual differences variables in repeated measures designs. The absence of the potential for confounding by individual differences variables is a great advantage of the repeated measures designs. • In general, practice effects should be balanced across the conditions in repeated measures designs so that practice effects “average out” across conditions. The key to conducting interpretable experiments using the repeated measures designs is learning to use appropriate techniques to balance practice effects. • The two types of repeated measures designs are the complete and the incomplete design. • The specific techniques for balancing practice effects differ for the two repeated measures designs, but the general term used to refer to these balancing techniques is counterbalancing. • In the complete design, practice effects are balanced for each participant by administering the conditions to each participant several times, using different orders each time. Each participant can thus be considered a “complete” experiment. • In the incomplete design, each condition is administered to each participant only once. The order of administering the conditions is varied across participants rather than for each participant, as is the case in the complete design. Practice effects in the incomplete design average out when the results are combined for all participants Balancing Practice Effects in the Complete Design • Practice effects are balanced in complete designs within each participant using block randomization or ABBA counterbalancing. • In block randomization, all of the conditions of the experiment (a block) are randomly ordered each time they are presented. • In ABBA counterbalancing, a random sequence of all conditions is presented, followed by the opposite of the sequence. • Participants were shown slides of photographs and were asked to rate each slide on a 7-point scale indicating the intensity of the expressed emotion. The slides were presented individually for 10 seconds, and participants were then given 35 seconds to make their rating. The critical independent variable in the experiment was the version of the photograph depicting one of the emotions (left composite, original, or right composite). Each participant rated 54 slides: 18 left composites, 18 originals, and 18 right composites. Participants’ ratings of emotional intensity were consistently higher for the left composite than for the right composite. Does this finding match your judgment that the face in pane • In general, the left hemisphere controls the right side of the body and the right hemisphere controls the left side of the body. Thus, the left composite reflects control by the right hemisphere, and the right composite reflects control by the left hemisphere. The higher ratings of emotional intensity for the left composite photographs suggest that the right hemisphere may be more heavily involved than the left hemisphere in the production of emotional expression. Block Randomization • Block randomization can also be used to order the conditions for each participant in a complete design. • In general, the number of blocks in a block randomized schedule is equal to the number of times each condition is administered, and the size of each block is equal to the number of conditions in the experiment. • The practice effects can reasonably be expected to average out over the three experimental conditions. Determining the average position of each of the three conditions in the block-randomized sequence gives a rough indication of the balancing of practice effects. • Block randomization is effective in balancing practice effects, but each condition must be repeated several times before we can expect practice effects to average out. We should not expect practice effects to be balanced after two or three blocks—any more than we would expect sample sizes of two or three in the random groups design to result in comparable groups. Fortunately, a technique is available to balance practice effects when it is not possible to administer each condition often enough for the averaging process of block randomization to work effectively • ABBA Counterbalancing In its simplest form, ABBA counterbalancing can be used to balance practice effects in the complete design with as few as two administrations of each condition. ABBA counterbalancing involves presenting the conditions in one sequence (i.e., A then B) followed by the opposite . • ABBA counterbalancing is appropriately used only when practice effects are linear. If practice effects are linear, the same amount of practice effects is added to or subtracted from performance on each successive trial. • ABBA counterbalancing balances practice effects even more effectively with larger numbers of repetitions of the cycle. Usually, however, ABBA counterbalancing is used when the number of conditions and the number of repetitions of each condition are relatively sm • ABBA counterbalancing is also ineffective when anticipation effects can occur. Anticipation effects occur when a participant develops expectations about which condition should occur next in the sequence. The participant’s response to that condition may then be influenced more by this expectation than by the actual experience of the condition itself. DATA ANALYSIS OF REPEATED MEASURES DESIGNS • Describing the Results 1. Data analysis for a complete design begins with computing a summary score (e.g., mean, median) for each participant. 2. Descriptive statistics are used to summarize performance across all participants for each condition of the independent variable. After checking the data for errors and outliers, the fi rst step in analyzing a repeated measures experiment is to summarize participants’ performance in each condition of the experiment. • Confirming what the results reveal