People's Guide Redo
People's Guide Redo
Spatial Thinking
Diana Stuart Sinton
with Sarah Bednarz, Phil Gersmehl, Robert Kolvoord,
and David Uttal
This publication of the National Council for Geographic Education (NCGE), including
all print, electronic, and web formats (including, but not limited to, images, text, and
illustrations) is protected by copyright laws of the United States and is owned or controlled
by NCGE, unless expressly noted. The materials are provided solely for the personal, non-
commercial use of purchasers.
Some of the images and graphics used in this edition are third-party submissions and/or in
the public domain. Requests to use third-party images and graphics must be made to the
owner of the images in the image credits.
To request permission for approved uses and to pay the associated fees, please contact the
National Council for Geographic Education at [email protected].
• Spatial thinking – it’s not rocket science, but just try rocket science without it
• Spatial thinking – it’s near, it’s far, it’s wherever you are
Introduction and
Background
Spatial thinking is pervasive: it is vital across a wide range of domains of
practical and scientific knowledge; yet it is underrecognized, undervalued,
underappreciated, and therefore, underinstructed.
-Learning to Think Spatially, National Research Council, 2006, p. 14
T
hroughout each day we think spatially, sometimes automatically and
intuitively and at other times with more deliberation. We navigate and
way-find, some people for their livelihood as taxi drivers, and all of us
around the rooms of our homes and the streets of our neighborhoods. We
understand things by looking at their spatial patterns, by reading X-rays or
examining slides under a microscope, or by monitoring the formation of storm
clouds in the sky. We distinguish the shape of “g” from “q” and “7” from “L” in
order to read English, or 空间 from 地方 to read Chinese. Shapes and their orientations
also help us distinguish our left from our right shoes as we get dressed. We
design structures, some people by constructing a skyscraper and others by
assembling a multi-tiered wedding cake. We engineer relationships based on
spatial proximity, such as auto mechanics adjusting the distance of electrodes at
the tip of spark plugs, or hosts setting place cards around tables at a banquet.
So, spatial thinking is not just about astronomical space (i.e., the stars and
planets and universe, outer space, the final frontier, etc.), though clearly
physicists and astronomers do think spatially. Nor is it just about geographic
space or where we are on the planet Earth. Rather, we need to recognize that we
think spatially in many dimensions and in many situations, from understanding
a precise sequence of wavelength along the electromagnetic spectrum, to
arranging furniture in a room, to modeling how ocean currents circulate.
Despite the significance of spatial thinking in our lives and the world around
us, it is a topic that is not widely understood and discussed. In 2006, the
National Research Council published a lengthy report titled Learning to Think
Spatially. The report’s authors are scientists and educators who comprised the
Committee on Support for Thinking Spatially, and they represent the fields of
11
12 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
Geography of
Life Spaces
14
GEOGRAPHY OF LIFE SPACES 15
Spatial thinking is so deeply embedded in the activities of daily life and thought
that it is difficult to disentangle and appreciate its role. We may not even
realize its role, but it is fundamental to many taken-for-granted activities,
underpinning their successful performances and sometimes accounting for their
spectacular failure.
-Learning to Think Spatially, National Research Council, 2006, p. 50
S
patial thinking is a constant and pervasive act in which we all participate,
at times automatically and intuitively and at other times very
methodically and deliberately. This can be better understood by
describing spatial thinking in three of the contexts in which it occurs: the
geographies of our life spaces, our physical and social spaces, and our
intellectual spaces.
Navigating the geography of our life spaces is the most common of all spatial
experiences that humans share. From the moment we arise in the morning we
are moving about an making plans in space, whether we are navigating our
bodies around other people simultaneously preparing breakfasts in a small
kitchen, coordinating with someone to pick him or her up at a particular corner,
knowing how long it will take to reach the school bus stop, parallel parking a
car, or emptying bags full of groceries into a refrigerator.
On a very basic level, each of these acts requires us to evaluate location,
position, or where something or someone is (Where is my body right now with
respect to the people next to me? Where is the designated corner to meet some-
one? Where is the bus stop? Where is there a parking spot near my house? Where
is an empty spot in the refrigerator?). To accomplish these tasks, we use a sense
16 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
of distance to guide us (How far away am I from this other person or from the
counter top? How much travel time must I allow to accomplish all errands and
arrive in time to pick someone up? How close am I to the adjacent cars and the
curb? Will the head of cabbage fit into the vegetable crisper bin?). We may also
have to use our sense of direction to accomplish these tasks (planning the
sequence of errands to avoid backtracking, one-way streets, or traffic jams;
turning the car’s steering wheel in the correct direction to maneuver into a
parking spot). Depending on the context, we may also employ ideas of regions
(in what part of the city or the refrigerator is the thing that I’m looking for?) or
sequences (where along this row of storefronts is the one I want?). These ideas—
distance, direction, regions, and sequence—are some of the basic spatial
concepts we use when we think.
These tasks may involve our seemingly instantaneous calculations of
perceived distances, such as knowing how far we have to move our bodies to
avoid (or achieve) contact with someone. Other times our calculations are done
with much more explicit planning, such as weighing the pros and cons of taking
different driving routes to reach a destination. We perceive both the relative
dimensions of the distances between bodies in a crowded room, or vegetables in
the crisper bin, as well as dimensions that we quantitatively measure and
calculate in larger spaces, such as the distances along a network of roads or
highways. All of these realms represent the life spaces around which we navigate
constantly.
GEOGRAPHY OF LIFE SPACES 17
Geography of Physical
and Social Spaces
Our conclusions are that similar processes underpin some aspects of spatial
thinking in all domains, whether thinking about tectonic plates, troop
movements in the Civil War, or weather.
W
hile we navigate around our daily lives, we exist within a geography of
physical space. Our climate may have distinct seasons or consistent
conditions year-round. Our house may be in an earthquake zone,
and we might secure our furniture to the walls and f loors, or we may live on a
houseboat and be acutely aware of the daily pattern of tides. Clean water might
f low easily into our homes directly through a faucet, or we may be forced to
haul it bucket-by-bucket from a distant source.
18
GEOGRAPHY OF PHYSICAL AND SOCIAL SPACES 19
HURRICANE TR ACKS
Social spaces are locations that have social attributes ascribed to them and
are used in certain social ways, like neighborhoods and playgrounds. Like their
counterparts who study physical spaces, social scientists think about social
spaces. Over time they have identified processes that create and maintain
characteristics of those features or phenomena, often beginning with theories
and hypotheses they derived from observing patterns. “Laws” of human
behavior that affect social patterns and processes may be more nuanced, less
predictable, and more dynamic than their natural or physical counterparts.
Only through our subjective, human interpretation can we describe why certain
neighborhoods develop and maintain reputations for attracting certain people,
or understand why and how children on a school playground choose to divide
their “territory,” such as 6th grade girls always hanging out at the benches near
the swing sets, year after year. The physical spaces themselves may or may not
have been designed to attract or repel any social group, but individuals or groups
can take “ownership” and the space can have an enduring association with one
group or another. We think about social spaces when we choose which bars or
nightclubs to visit on a Friday night, remember on which side of the church to
sit at a wedding, or decide in which part of town to look for a new house.
Some people are particularly aware of the dynamics of both social and
physical spaces at specific locations. For example, urban planners must think
not only of how a new east/west–running highway could impact drivers heading
into a rising or setting sun, but what types of neighborhoods will be inf luenced
by the traffic noises that are generated, what zoning regulations must be
considered, and what types of businesses could benefit or be harmed. Architects
consider not only the underlying geology, slope, and drainage characteristics for
GEOGRAPHY OF PHYSICAL AND SOCIAL SPACES 21
Physical spaces
Social spaces
Geography of
Intellectual Space
DNA HELIX
While the initial and intuitive roots of spatial thinking are grounded,
literally, in the geographies of our life and physical spaces, a major part of its
scientific and therefore educational importance lies in the process of
spatialization that creates intellectual spaces. With the advent of computers
and scientific instrumentation, we have gone from a problem-rich, data-poor
world to one that is both data-rich and problem-rich, but is currently lacking
the capacity to bring data to bear on solving problems. The solution to problems
will depend on the capacity to process, analyze, and represent the vast
quantities of data that we can gather and store.
-Learning to Think Spatially, National Research Council, 2006, p. 32
22
GEOGRAPHY OF INTELLECTUAL SPACE 23
W
e can use space to help us understand abstract information and
organize knowledge. In the geography of our intellectual space, we take
advantage of our ability to arrange information in space to help us
understand that information. That is, we think with space to help us both learn
and convey meaning to ourselves and others. When we put information, data,
or knowledge in a spatial context, and “spatialize” it, we are able to make sense
out of the information through its arrangement itself.
We think with space when we draw concept maps, construct family trees,
examine the periodic table, and make or use any number of charts, graphs, or
diagrams that describe the relationship between variables, entities, or objects. It
is their spatial arrangement that illustrates the relationship. Within a
traditionally drawn family tree, great-grandparents can be distinguished from
siblings or distant cousins based on where they are located and in proximity to
whom on the diagram (location), how far away on the page the people are from
each other and at what angle (distance and direction), and whether a person is
the first born among siblings or the baby of the family (sequences). In the
periodic table, elements are arranged both sequentially on the basis of their
24 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
atomic number (the weight of their atoms), but the rows and columns
themselves are also arranged to convey additional information about the atoms.
The far right column of the periodic table always contains the noble gasses; the
closer to the bottom of the table the row is located (as it’s depicted from top to
bottom), the heavier the elements the row contains.
Our ability to glean insights, understand more deeply, or learn more
effectively, through spatially based arrangements of complex or large or abstract
data sets can be tremendously powerful, especially in the sciences. An iconic
example of such an outcome is James Watson’s and Francis Crick’s double-helix
model of DNA. When describing the process of how he and Crick came up with
this discovery, Watson speaks specifically about the critical role of designing
and building physical models with which they “played” as they experimented
with creating alternative structures that followed the rules of chemistry.
The Processes of
Spatial Thinking
It is the links among space, representation, and reasoning that give the process
of spatial thinking its power, versatility, and applicability. Spatial thinking is
multifaceted in its operation: just as there is no single recipe for how to think
verbally or mathematically, there is no [one] single way to think spatially.
Instead, the process of spatial thinking comprises broad sets of interconnected
competencies that can be taught and learned.
-Learning to Think Spatially, National Research Council, 2006, p. 26
26
THE PROCESSES OF SPATIAL THINKING 27
W
e begin spatial thinking by “distinguishing and encoding spatial
features of the world” (Hochberg, 1978, as cited in National
Research Council, Learning to Think Spatially, 2006, p. 41).
Encoding refers to the way that a brain detects and stores the information that
it perceives. Most people get the majority of their spatial information through
the sense of sight, and when our ability to see is unimpaired, processing the
spatial information can be a smooth and seemingly instantaneous event. These
first steps of encoding are so automatic that we are unaware of them, such as
seeing an object or entity and immediately discerning—from its shape, size,
texture, color, and other attributes—what it is and what it is like. For example,
we look at a couch or sofa and we have a good sense of how it would feel if we
were to sit on it, how heavy it might be if we were to try and lift it, and how
unlikely it would be to budge if we were to bump up against it. Its behavior in
a spatial sense is regular, predictable, and static or unchanging. The regular and
predictable behavior of objects in our world helps us develop a sense of the laws
of physical science that we expect objects to follow.
That predictability of spatial behavior and patterns is critical for effective
and successful thinking in and about space. As athletes hit a tennis ball or kick
a soccer ball, they never think about the possibility of the ball itself changing its
characteristics during a game. That is, sports would be very different if a tennis
or soccer ball randomly and independently changed its size or weight during a
game. Furthermore, through study or practice we learn how to predict new
patterns when variables become altered, such as anticipating a curve ball or a
spiral pass. When driving a car, we learn how much distance is needed to apply
the brakes for a complete halt before reaching the stop sign, but we can also
make adjustments when the road is wet or icy. Engineers predict how materials
will function under different physical conditions, such as varying temperatures
or pressures. We also use prior knowledge that we have previously encoded to
understand the less predictable spatial behavior of objects, such as the
experience of standing on a rocking chair.
Frames of reference provide us with contexts for being able to understand the
spatial encoding we do. When we stand in front of different couches in a
furniture store we readily perceive their sizes and shapes, and we are then able
to mentally compare a given couch’s dimensions to the dimensions of the door-
way through which we must fit a couch in our house. In that case we are using
a static, unchanging reference frame (the size of our doorway), as well as
making comparisons between different static entities (comparing one couch to
another in the store). We process our understanding of static entities as we
determine orientation and location, assess distance, and make comparisons of
size, shapes, textures, position, and direction, among other spatial attributes.
28 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
For example, doctors examine the orientation of a spine in X-rays to evaluate for
injuries, and botanists consider the orientation of certain fungi on fallen trees
in the forest to determine the time since the trees fell.
More often we are working with entities that are changing or dynamic and
in motion, and we encode information such as the direction of an object’s
movement, the manner of its motion, and its speed and acceleration. For
example, as we think in space, we use our encoding of movement to anticipate
when two items might intersect or collide, whether it is the soccer ball we have
just kicked towards a running teammate, or the car we are driving at too fast a
speed towards a slower car in front of us. Our ability to discern the direction of
movement works across different time scales as well, so that we can envision and
predict the movement of many diverse phenomena. We can do this at
spatial scales ranging from tectonic plates and weather fronts, to bullets and
electrical currents.
So, location, direction, distance, orientation, and movement are examples of
basic spatial characteristics or concepts that we use to organize and understand
our world and how it works. Though these concepts may seem simple, they are
also nuanced and can be multidimensional. For example, if someone were to ask
you “where” you are located, valid responses could include your latitude and
longitude, or your zip code, or that you are five blocks away from the movie
theatre, or 5000 miles north of the equator, or your place in your family tree.
As we encode spatial information, we usually consider the location of items
or objects within their respective contexts. A house may be located at a single
street address, but that address ref lects its relative position along a street of
other houses, which is one street of many within a neighborhood, which lies in
a particular part of town, which is near a river that is part of a watershed that
stretches over portions of four different counties, etc. Thus, we often think of
individual locations as part of sequences or associations, perhaps arranged as a
hierarchy. Understanding where an individual object or feature (such as a single
house) fits into its contextual network gives us understanding and insight into
the object itself, such as the implications for emergency management if we know
that a house is located on a cul-de-sac. Similarly, knowing the location of a
person within an organizational chart, or an element within the periodic table,
would give us insights into expected functions or behaviors of those “objects.”
Together with location, distance is a key piece of information that spatial
thinkers consider. Calculations based on measurements of distance are what
allow us to calculate the speed of light and identify stars, land airplanes safely,
and determine the habitat ranges of endangered species.
The distance between objects or phenomena is one cue that we use to
describe their arrangement. For example, we characterize the pattern of
THE PROCESSES OF SPATIAL THINKING 29
DISEASE SPREAD/DIFFUSION
Spatial
Representations
3D CITY MAP
31
32 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
T
hus, the world is filled with stationary and moving objects and
materials, affected by spatial processes. We encode different kinds of
information about both. With this information, we create
representations of the world around us: internal ones, such as mental maps or
images that we can use to imagine fitting a couch through a door or how we
would navigate to a familiar location, and external ones, such as sketches or
maps that we create and use to communicate or communicate information to
others. Whether representations are external or internal, humans have relied on
them throughout history to understand the world and how it works.
Maps are graphic representations of the arrangement of natural and
constructed characteristics or features of places, often of our physical and social
world. We create internal, mental maps in our heads when we give directions to
someone, and we use these maps when navigating from place to place. We make
maps also as external representations of space, whether they are hand-drawn on
a piece of paper or digitally produced on a computer screen. Maps are used by
virtually everyone as a metaphor for the organization of information, whether
geographic or not. If we say “Let’s map out what we’re going to say at the
presentation,” we would generate an outline for the f low of ideas, and then we
are thinking with space.
We also use spatial models as simplified and abstract representations of
objects and phenomena, such as the model of the DNA structure that Watson
and Crick made, or the table-top structures that architects build, or a model of
the planets in our solar system that 4th graders construct. Some models are
designed to be exact and accurate, such as a detailed globe or architectural
constructions, while others are instead symbolic, such as a highly abstract
mathematical model. In any case, the representations of models help us
communicate knowledge.
The Internet has brought us useful digital tools to generate representations
that share information and communicate with and about space. With new
computer applications we can collaboratively create concept maps with
colleagues around the world, argue over diagrams illustrating football plays
from last weekend’s game, or debate the likelihood of red and blue states
changing colors in the next US Presidential election. With virtual globes—such
as Google Earth or Esri’s ArcGIS Explorer—we can see movement of oceanic
and atmospheric currents, or 3-dimensional reconstructions of shipwrecks or
buildings of ancient Rome. Through online map “mashups” people can merge
any of their own individual information (about their upcoming wedding plans,
or where they like to drink coffee) with detailed street maps or aerial
photography. As these types of simple maps proliferate and are shared around
the world, mapping is becoming part of our everyday communication. Spatial
thinking contributes to making the maps and is necessary as well to read and
interpret them.
SPATIAL REPRESENTATIONS 33
Transforming Representations
and Processes of Reasoning
34
TRANSFORMING REPRESENTATIONS AND PROCESSES OF REASONING 35
SHOE TYING
P
roducing and interpreting representations is critical because then our
minds can move on to transforming those representations. When we link
our encoded spatial knowledge with the support that representations can
provide, we have a platform for spatial reasoning: to structure problems, find
answers, and express solutions to these problems (National Research Council,
2006). Moreover, our capacity to transform or manipulate representations works
across many different scales and contexts. A classic assessment of one type of
spatial ability is a measurement of how quickly and accurately one can imagine
the rotation of a 3-dimensional object, but in the real world, very few
peoplework regularly with clusters of blocks. However, the need to imagine an
object from a different angle or perspective is more common than we think. For
example, this ability allows us to recognize familiar people even if we are
standing far behind them; we can imagine what their face would look like if
they turned around.
As another example, if someone were to ask you to provide verbal
instructions for how to tie shoelaces, in your own mind you would generate a
mental image (an internal representation) of your own shoe with its untied laces
followed by a series of sequential images of the laces at different stages of being
knotted, to analyze the steps in their proper order. If you were actually helping
a child to tie his or her shoes as you knelt in front, you would be transforming
your understanding of the steps in mirror-fashion, literally changing your
perspective. Changing perspectives and executing mirrored steps becomes more
challenging with more complicated tasks, such as tying a necktie on someone or
learning to examine dental work with a mirror. Your brain becomes accustomed
36 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
to telling your fingers to move in certain ways, and when you send it different
visual cues, it can be difficult to adjust.
The ability to imagine phenomena and processes from different
perspectives is fundamental to our ability to reason through spatial problems.
Tying someone else’s shoes or necktie are simple examples of changing
perspectives, but these capacities extend to great scientific problems and
solutions. In 1905, Einstein tried to imagine what a beam of light would look
like if he himself were riding on it, a thought-provoking question that
contributed to his understanding and developing the theory of relativity. This
spatiall grounded act of imagination, called “enacting,” is part of our thinking
in space, combining “spatial thinking with motoric thinking” (National
Research Council, 2006, p. 44). It is what actors, athletes, surgeons, and
criminals do as they mentally rehearse the sequence of their performances.
During the most advanced or complex stages of a spatial thinking
process, we are using many representations and conducting numerous transfor-
mations, sometimes simultaneously, at other times sequentially. We may need to
understand and visualize how some parts of a system fit together before we can
move on to the next phase. Such a process may require us to make inferential
leaps because we may have missing or unclear data—or a lack of encoded
information—for some of the intermediate steps.
Sometimes we must use these skills to work backwards in time. A group
of geologists may encounter an outcrop of rocks whose composition and
formation is inconsistent with everything around it, and it may take them time
to deduce what spatial processes must have occurred to lead to the current
conditions. Or detectives use the spatial arrangement of features at a crime
scene to determine where some key individuals must have been standing in
order to leave that pattern of evidence.
Sometimes we use these same skills to imagine future scenarios.
Engineers calculate how high the water level could rise behind a new dam,
taking into account the variables of water f low, topography, soils, precipitation,
land use, demand for electricity or irrigation water, and how these factors might
interact together. Doctors study digital imagery to understand a tumor they
find in someone’s body and then predict, based on its location, proximity to
other organs, and connectivity, what will happen if the tumor continues to
grow. Only later, when surgeons are able to see exactly how and where the mass
is attached, do they make the in-the-moment decisions about where to cut, and
also imagine the outcomes if alternative choices are made.
In short, representations help us to make decisions at various spatial
scales, whether it is for past, current, or future conditions. As we transform
these, in our minds or through physical or virtual models, we reason and gain
insights about the world around us and how it works.
Chapter 8
S
ingle acts of spatial thinking can be described in isolation (such as a
surgeon evaluating a tumor), and we have described different
“geographies” (life, physical, social, and intellectual) as distinctive realms
of spatial thinking. Yet in reality we are constantly and continuously moving
within and between these realms, using representations and reasoning with
spatial concepts in a very natural and automatic manner, usually without a
second thought.
Take, for example, the mental thought processes that might occur during
your drive to work. Driving itself is a thoroughly spatial process that demands
constant monitoring of your own location, often within a choreography of other
2000+ -pound objects moving at high speeds in parallel or opposing directions.
You automatically adjust your speed while coming to a sharp curve, even though
you have long forgotten the specific lessons that you had in high school about
what happens to the momentum of an object as it’s forced to change its
trajectory. Better slow down; it rained a lot last night, and there’s still water on
the roadway. Given how much it rains here every spring, you’d think they would
have put in more drainage ditches along this stretch of road.
As you pass a gas station, you note that the prices have gone up
considerably and you think of the map you saw in yesterday’s paper showing
how gas prices varied all over your region. You quickly glance down directly at
your gas gauge and see that you’ve got about a ¼ tank. That should equal about
80 miles, plenty to last you until the weekend when you’ll have more time. At
that instant, your ears perk up when you hear a siren from somewhere, but after
a couple of seconds you can tell from its sound that it’s both distant and not
moving in your direction, so you disregard the fading noise.
A sign in a f lorist’s window reminds you not to forget Mother’s Day, and
you think of calling your mother, but then you remember that she is on vacation
far away and in an earlier time zone. Plus, she’s way up in the mountains and
her cell phone probably won’t get clear reception anyway. Makes you think of
that map your carrier provided that showed their coverage areas, and you
37
38 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
wonder how that’s determined, a combination of where cell phone towers are
and where mountains block signals, and what role do satellites play? But the
tower information must change pretty quickly; seems like they’re always
putting up new ones. Right then you pass one of those towers that are designed
to look like a tree, with fake green branches to blend into the background more.
You smile as you notice that in fact the tower is not only significantly taller than
the trees around it, but it’s also meant to be some kind of pine tree or other
conifer, and they’ve stuck it in the middle of a bunch of shorter, deciduous trees.
So much for blending in.
You f lip on your turn indicator, slow down, and pull into the new breakfast
place. Used to be a small Italian restaurant but nobody ever went there, and
before that it was a bar that opened only after midnight. Now that it serves
breakfast and lunch, the lines are always long; this is an ideal location for easy
access by commuters. Today you’re in a hurry, so as you stand in the carry-out
line, your mind turns to the engineering task that your team has been
struggling with at work. You have to fit several circuit boards into a
wind-generator controller so that they do not overheat, and so far the group
hasn’t come up with any designs that solve the problem. You order a bagel and
watch the counter staff place it into the vertical holder so they can slice it safely.
Ah, a bagel balancing vertically. That bagel reminds you of one of the large
support struts that’s in your machine. What if you used that strut to align the
circuit boards vertically instead of their current horizontal arrangement? You
make a mental note to try it as soon as you get to the office.
Once back in the car, you reach over to f lip on the radio to catch the end of
the morning talk show, but it’s only sounding like fuzz and you remember that
your young son had been playing with all the dials and buttons the other day
and it seems he has lost your station. In fact, not only has he moved off your
station, but he’s toggled it over to AM radio. You fumble for a bit with the AM
tuning dial, not even really sure where to begin since it’s been ages since you’ve
listened to AM. With a quick bit of nostalgia, you recall having listened to an
AM radio show on Sunday nights when you were growing up. It had been
broadcast from a city far away from where you lived, and you remember your
parents explaining why AM signals came in so much more clearly at night time
than in the day, something about how waves at that frequency travel more
easily through the atmosphere after dark, and you make a note to explain that
to your kids some time. But for now you switch it back to FM. Oops, didn’t see
that curve coming! Better put your bagel down, keep your eyes on the road, and
let your fingers do the moving. Good thing you’re familiar enough with the
radio interface to be able to do it without looking. Upper left button is the one
you want, and finally you’ve got your morning news.
As you turn into the parking lot at your office, you begin your regular
SUMMARY: A BRIEF TIME IN THE LIFE OF A SPATIAL THINKER 39
search for an empty spot that’s on the east side of one of the few trees, knowing
that by the end of the work day the car will be cooler, having been in the shade
from the afternoon sun in the west. But then you remember that today you have
to rush out quickly at noon for a dentist’s appointment, so you go for a spot
that’s closest to your building. The only one remaining in the closest row is
reserved for your boss, and you are still hoping to move up the organizational
ladder, so you better not park there and irritate her. You find another spot
nearby. It’s a tight fit between two other cars, but you squeeze into the space
available, trying to leave as much room as possible for the driver’s door of the
car on your right side. You can’t bear to think of another scratch or ding from a
carelessly swung-open car door.
It’s 8:15 am and already a busy day in the life of a spatial thinker.
A STUDENT IN CLASS
40
THE WAY FORWARD 41
The committee does not view spatial thinking as one more piece to be added
on to an already overburdened curricular structure. Instead, spatial thinking
is seen as an integrator and a facilitator for problem solving across the curri-
culum. Spatial thinking is not an add-on but a missing link across the curri-
culum. Thus, integration and infusion of spatial thinking can help to achieve
existing curricular objectives. The idea of spatial thinking does not and should
not stand alone, but equally well, without explicit attention to it, we cannot
meet our responsibility for equipping the next generation of students for life
and work in the twenty-first century.
-Learning to Think Spatially, National Research Council, 2006, p. 241
F
ostering spatial “literacy” is one outcome sought by the committee that
authored the Learning to Think Spatially report. As they expanded their
original charge, to understand how GIS could be designed to help support
spatial thinking in children, they uncovered a broader and richer need, to better
understand the nature and character of spatial thinking itself and appreciate
how it might be supported in general.
Spatial literacy is a relatively new idea and is being defined in different
ways. The authors of Learning to Thinking Spatially suggest that spatial literacy
constitutes “proficiency in terms of spatial knowledge, spatial ways of thinking
and acting, and spatial capabilities” (National Research Council, 2006, p. 18).
A spatially literate student has the “habit of mind of thinking spatially;
practices spatial thinking in an informed way; and adopts a critical stance to
spatial thinking” (National Research Council, 2006, p. 20). Such students draw
easily and naturally from a collection of spatial concepts, ground the concepts
effectively in the spatial representations that they generate routinely, and use
these ideas and support structures as they work their way through the tasks and
problems of their lives. A related but alternative way to define spatial literacy is
the confident and competent use of maps, mapping, and spatial thinking to
address ideas, situations, and problems within daily life, society, and the world
around us.
Clearly, the discipline of geography draws significantly from within the
42 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
Fortunately, with every year these barriers are being reduced in scale and need
not be insurmountable. Web-based GIS now offers opportunities to reach new
audiences. It’s time to recognize the role that spatial thinking plays in our lives
and livelihoods. Imagine the opportunities we would create if students went
through their educational experiences able to use space as a means to structure
problems, find answers, express solutions, and carry those skills through to
their work in the world.
Chapter 10
Frequently Asked
Questions
1. What is spatial thinking?
Spatial thinking is an ability to visualize and interpret location, position,
distance, direction, relationships, movement, and change over space. These
cognitive processes take place in many different situations and at many
different scales.
We are thinking spatially when we decide where to plant different
vegetables in our garden, when we are able to walk around a familiar room in
the dark without bumping into furniture, and when we adjust our rear-view
mirrors in our car.
We are also thinking spatially and geographically when we imagine which
way we need to face to watch the sunset in summer in our backyards, and
why some mountain ranges only receive rain on one side and not the other,
and what makes oceans have low and high tides. We think spatially about
social contexts as well, when we understand how one should modify his or
her body language and physical presence when interacting with people from
different cultures, or when we observe patterns of urban growth.
Broadly defined, spatial thinking is a cognitive act in which humans are
constantly engaged. Many spatial reasoning processes, such as shifting to one
side of a path or sidewalk when someone is approaching from the other
direction, are simple ones that we do naturally, automatically, and seemingly
without conscious thought. Other forms of spatial thinking are undertaken
much more systematically and deliberately by people in their professional
careers, such as the spatial tasks involved in designing and engineering a
skyscraper, or programming a model to predict the effects of climate change.
But in either case, when these actions are reduced to their component steps,
the roles of location, position, distance, direction, relationships, movement,
and change are still central.
44
FREQUENTLY ASKED QUESTIONS 45
2. Can you give me examples of where spatial thinking, or the lack thereof,
has really mattered or made a difference in the world?
a. Stopping oil from f lowing. On April 20, 2010, an oil rig owned by the
British Petroleum Oil Company exploded, killing 11 people. The Deepwater
Horizon rig was extracting oil from a well dug almost one mile (1.6 km)
below the ocean’s surface, and over 13,000 ft (4 km) into the ocean f loor,
about 40 miles south of Louisiana (USA). For almost three months, as many
as 2 million gallons (7,570 cubic meters) of oil per day f lowed into the Gulf
of Mexico, disrupting the livelihoods of thousands of regional workers and
causing untold environmental damage to the marine and estuarine
ecosystems. Engineers worked around the clock to stop the well from f lowing
and/or contain the oil so that it could be recovered by other ships at the
surface. Each solution had to take into account the oceanic currents, the
intense pressure at that depth, the distance from the surface, and the
seasons’ tropical storm patterns. Meanwhile, additional relief wells were
being drilled down through the thousands of feet of ocean f loor to intersect
the sources of subsurface oil and permanently close off the f low to the
damaged well. The detrimental environmental and economic effects of the
disaster will be felt for many years, but as a silver lining, the crisis also forced
petroleum engineers to conduct research and development in an applied
setting and generated significant new knowledge in the field.
b. Returning to Earth from space. Some of us will recall the actual
Apollo 13 events in 1970, as well as the 1995 movie starring Tom Hanks.
The astronauts were able to keep breathing thanks to a jerry-rigged,
improvised connector between incompatible air canisters that filtered carbon
dioxide. The solution, designed by engineers on the ground during the crisis,
involved the use of duct tape. Meanwhile, the mission team in Houston was
calculating the precise angle that the ship had to be adjusted and how much
they had to move the ship forward to get into the right gravitational pull,
then Jack Swigert (played by Kevin Bacon in the movie) had to move his
hand just a precise nudge to get the module in the right place. This incident
led to several days of very intense spatial thinking, on many levels.
c. Escaping from Alcatraz. Early on the morning of June 12, 1962,
guards discovered that three men had escaped overnight from the prison
situated on the small island of Alcatraz, in the middle of San Francisco Bay.
In the six months leading up to their breakout, Frank Morris, Clarence
Anglin, and his brother John Anglin secretly constructed rudimentary tools,
including a simple periscope that they used to spy on guards and a drill from
a vacuum cleaner motor. They stitched together a rubber raft from 50 rain-
46 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
coats, sealed the seams with shoe glue heated by steam pipes, and inf lated it
with a modified concertina. They nailed together wooden paddles from
scraps of wood. Maybe leaving on that particular June night was not the best
“geographic” choice, due to the high tides and strong winds that would have
made their raft travel or swimming difficult, but they made their attempt as
soon as their preparations were complete. We can only speculate on their
success or failure, but clever spatial thinking made their escape possible.
d. Designing a prosthetic limb. Tens of thousands of people lose parts
of their bodies (hands, arms, feet, legs) each year in accidents, from land
mines, and from diseases such as diabetes. Designers of prosthetic limbs and
artificial joints study how our bones, muscles, circulation system, and nerves
fit together. They carefully examine how people move in space, to make the
devices as natural, robust, and useful as possible. Designing a leg for people
who need to return to agricultural work in a field is different from
optimizing one for speed on a race track. Dean Kamen, the person who also
designed a wheelchair that can mount curbs and raise a person to a standing
height, has created an artificial arm with the sensitivity to allow its wearer to
unlock a door or peel a banana. In Thailand, two elephants injured by land
mines now benefit from artificial limbs strong enough to hold them up.
e. Protecting air and water rights. Understanding the right to use or
own land is a fairly tangible idea, but it becomes more interestingly complex
when we consider the use or ownership of resources or material that are
above, below, next to, or moving through that land or space. We then need
laws and policies that control how pollutants that are upstream or downwind
are handled, for example. When we extract significant amounts of water at
the upper reaches of the river, there may not be any left for those at the lower
end. The space above buildings and highways can become incredibly valuable
for development, especially in urban areas. We control who can enter into air
space and who can make loud noises nearby.
f. Reconstructing an airplane. In November of 1955, a United Airlines
jet crashed into a farm in Colorado. Investigators from the FBI were able to
determine from the jagged and outward-bending hole on one part of the
wreckage that a violent explosion had occurred onboard, and they proceeded
to piece together the airplane itself within a large warehouse. Eventually,
other clues found in luggage scraps led investigators to the perpetrators. This
was the first time that an airplane had been re-created from its fragmented
parts.
FREQUENTLY ASKED QUESTIONS 47
6. On my new cell phone I have applications that can tell me where the
nearest metro station is or what good restaurants are nearby. I even read
about a new application that has “augmented reality” capabilities, and
when I point my phone’s camera in a particular direction I can be shown
things on its screen that are not actually visible otherwise. Where is the
spatial thinking in all of this?
Many of our cell phones are now location-enabled. This means that the
phone can determine where it is located on the surface of the Earth, either
through a built-in GPS receiver or by calculating its location in relation to
wifi networks and cell phone towers. The accuracy of this positioning process
varies, depending on many factors such as the strength of the signals that
phone is using. Once the phone knows where it is, it can then tell you its
proximity to other objects whose locations have also been mapped
(restaurants, metro stations, movie theatres, etc.). For example, you could
find out which restaurants are within a mile of you or a few minutes of
walking distance. You’re obviously limited to the ones whose information has
been collected by a mapping company, and you are also putting your trust in
that company’s ability to obtain and/or maintain current and accurate
information.
Now that the public has experienced the convenience of having easy access
to location information at its finger tips, we will continue to expect as much
and our world will become increasingly “geo-tagged” in the future. In fact,
geo-tagged objects do not even need to be stationary. If you and your friends
each carry a location-enabled device, some applications will let you find the
friends who are nearby at that moment (at least the ones whose cell phones
are willing to be found).
Augmented-reality applications are taking location a step further. Through
these developments, we can experience (see or hear) images or information
about a place that would otherwise not be visually apparent. For example,
when you point your location-enabled cell phone down a street, it can show
you where wi-fi spots are available, or the prices for any particular real estate
that is available. If you were walking through a historical site carrying a
location-enabled device, you could hear stories of what had happened there
or be shown historical images to compare with the current reality. Many
museums already use devices with these capabilities to allow patrons to take
a self-paced tour through exhibits.
Location-enabled technology clearly has great commercial, industrial, and
social utility. People have more opportunities than ever before to use
FREQUENTLY ASKED QUESTIONS 53
“location” as a criterion for making decisions and learning, and that may
contribute to greater spatial awareness. Any time you perceive things as part
of spatial patterns, groups, sequences, or hierarchies of different sizes, your
brain is organizing the perceptions in order to make spatial relationships
easier to remember.
7. Are there any famous spatial thinkers in history?
Some of the most famous historical spatial thinkers have been artists,
engineers, explorers, mathematicians, and scientists.
Leonardo da Vinci is a classic example. He designed, painted, engineered,
imagined, envisioned, and sketched constantly. His notebooks are filled with
insights and drawings gleaned from extensive observation of objects in
movement, such as birds in f light, that contributed to his understanding of
f luid mechanics.
Many artists who work in 3-dimensional structures are natural spatial
thinkers, relying on a mental representation of a final form that they keep in
mind during their creative process. When asked about one of his sculptures,
Michelangelo is quoted as saying, “I saw the angel in the marble and carved
until I set him free.” Those who work in 2-dimensional media, such as
painting, are just as likely to possess an aptitude for spatial perspectives.
Being able to draw or paint a 2-dimensional scene and have it appear
3-dimensional, or realistic, means understanding how our brains perceive
light and shade, foreshadowing, and perspective. For example, Escher had
great skill at manipulating arrangements and patterns to “play” with space
and create illusions of dimensions.
Early philosophers and mathematicians also relied on spatial thinking
strategies, for their own knowledge and in order to communicate to their
followers. Plato used a simple but elegant representation of a divided line to
help Socrates appreciate the differences among the metaphysical worlds he
was describing. Ptolemy was the first to propose a grid-based reference
system around the Earth, a system that later became latitude and longitude.
Albert Einstein had a great capacity for envisioning how abstract and
invisible forces or objects fit together and interact. In his development of the
theory of relativity, he famously said that it helped him to imagine what a
beam of light would look like if one were riding on it. He had a tremendous
ability to envision the spatial relationships among abstract objects.
In the absence of reliable maps, many historical explorers had to rely on
their natural sense of direction as they journeyed. Navigators from Europe,
54 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
China, and Scandinavia managed to find their way to and from their home
ports, multiple times, without the benefit of digital technologies. Together
with his colleague William Clark, Meriwether Lewis planned a route
westward from St. Louis across unknown territory and reached the Pacific
Ocean. In the early 20th century, Ernest Shackleton used simple tools to
keep his bearings while he and his crew were stuck in Antarctic ice for over a
year.
Marie Tharp was a geologist who mapped the oceanic f loors using data
collected by research ships in the 1950s and 1960s. She had insights into the
formation of the Mid-Atlantic Ridge before many others and knew that the
data had to be seen to be understood (and that the visualization of the
patterns would contribute to our knowledge of plate tectonics). Ms. Tharp’s
valuable contributions to our understanding of underwater geologic
formations and plate tectonics is profiled in the National Research Council’s
Learning to Think Spatially (2006).
Why aren’t there more women who are easily identified as “famous”
spatial thinkers? There are many reasons that address these inequalities. One
is that women are under-represented and less-celebrated in our history,
period. For example, it wasn’t until 2009 that the Women Airforce Service
Pilots (WASPs), women with prodigious abilities to think spatially, were
awarded the Congressional Gold Medal for their role in World War II.
8. I hear people say they are visual learners, and we know that a picture
is worth 1000 words. Sometimes visual and spatial learners are lumped
together as one group. What’s the difference between visual learning and
spatial learning?
When people say they are visual learners, they can mean that they prefer
to see information written rather than hearing it spoken, or that they prefer
to see a picture or image rather than see information in text form. Seeing a
picture or a graph (or any other external representation) can convey a story
more rapidly than a written paragraph. Details can be gleaned effectively and
quickly, as compared to a written description; hence the well-known phrase
about a picture being worth many words.
Sometimes, when people say that they are visual learners, they are
differentiating themselves from people who learn aurally (through the spoken
word) or kinesthetically (by moving their bodies). For example, some people
can listen to a lecture and understand someone else’s verbal explanations
about a topic. Learning through this method requires people to actively use
FREQUENTLY ASKED QUESTIONS 55
their working memory, as they must remember what had just been said as
they move on to the new. For this reason, many people find that some kind
of visual aid (in the form of a table, chart, figure, or graph) is a useful aid to
organize the information being shared. When people see a graphic
representation of the information, whether it is someone’s name or phone
number or an equation explaining a scientific model, their brain attaches
meaning to that object and it is stored in a different part of the brain. Later,
they may find it easier to recall the internal representation (of that name,
number, or equation) than if they had only heard the information spoken.
This dual-coding of text and images is one simple model of how our memory
works to learn information, but it is only one of many.
In reality, unless one is visually impaired, everyone is a visual learner, as
that is the mode by which we receive the majority of the information that our
brains are encoding. In this book we talked about the role of internal and
external representations in the spatial thinking process. Our mind is
constantly generating internal representations that help us understand
spatially based structures and organizational frameworks. We also create
2-dimensional and 3-dimensional external representations of spatially and
geographically based information, like organizational charts, road maps, or
models of chemical bonds. External models are particularly helpful in the
learning process because they allow our working memory to focus on the
content or function of the representations, rather than on the structure or
framework itself. Though we may be calling it visual learning, it is actually
the spatial arrangement of the information through which we are learning
the material. The arrangement itself is the helpful device and support,
whether it’s a concept map or a work-f low or an x-y chart that shows the
relationship between two variables. In this manner, it is spatial thinking that
is helping us extract meaning from the representation, one which we happen
to be using our vision to perceive.
9. There’s a guy at work who stands really close to me when he talks. He
invades my “personal” space and makes me uncomfortable. Is he just a
lousy spatial thinker?
There is actually a whole field of study, called “proxemics,” about the
distances at which people physically interact. An anthropologist named
Edward T. Hall first coined the term in the 1960s, and his research suggested
the ranges of distance most common for different interaction types: intimate,
personal, social, and public. Since his work, many have studied how these
distances vary considerably by culture, gender, age, location, time of day,
56 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
rats continued making the same direction of turns. This led the researchers
to believe that the rats were indeed following their own cognitive, internal
memories of direction in space.
Some animals use climate, season, and food availability to geographically
adjust their home and territorial ranges. Birds follow landmarks, the sun, the
stars, and magnetic fields to navigate across thousands of miles in search of
food and a mate. Homing pigeons and sea turtles are among some of the
animals who may be able to sense spatial differences in magnetic fields as
they navigate within their ranges. The ability of birds to navigate to specific
destinations is so reliable that for many centuries, carrier pigeons were used
to transport messages during wartime and were successful at these endeavors
up to 95% of the time.
Observing the spatial interactions and movements of animals has helped
us understand physical patterns. For example, we have learned much from
the aerodynamics of bird f locks in f light, the f luid mechanics of schools of
fish, and the locomotion of other animals on land.
11. If spatial thinking is so pervasive, is it ref lected in our language?
There are multiple ways in which spatial thinking is a component of our
written and oral language. At a very fundamental level, it is the shapes and
patterns of letters themselves that allow us to differentiate letters and words
when we read. When children are taught to write the shapes of letters in a
standard form, they must follow the spatial rules of placing shapes above and
below a line, or facing their letters to the right or the left.
In all languages there are examples of metaphors that are built on a
common understanding of spatial concepts. English is full of them. There are
metaphors of orientation that fall into a number of different categories. For
example, happy is up and sad is down: feeling on top of the world; that
boosted my spirits; seeing her gives me a lift; he’s really low these days; my
spirits sank; she hit rock bottom. Notions of perception and understanding
often follow wide and narrow patterns: step back to see the big picture;
far-sighted; take the larger view; tunnel vision; narrow-minded; short-sighted.
Organizational structure is hierarchical: low man on the totem pole; top dog;
moving up the ladder; glass ceiling; back of the pack. Attitudes and actions
often use motion: take the high road; tread on thin ice; breaking new ground;
getting to the bottom (of a mystery). For an in-depth discussion of spatial
metaphors in language, see Lakoff and Johnson’s Metaphors We Live By
(University of Chicago Press, 1980).
58 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
Even in sign language, the nature of signs themselves ref lects similar
spatial metaphors. Signs with upward movement often indicate more, good,
and success. Downward motions are part of failing and losing.
An interesting area of research is how certain words are imbued with
spatial or directional context. That is, some people automatically associate
“north” with “up” and “south” with “down.” We see this in our language
with statements such as “the Dow Jones is heading south.” Sometimes, people
are even confused by rivers that naturally f low northward, such as the Nile in
Africa or the Willamette in Oregon (USA). Marketing researchers have found
that people believe “that it will take longer to travel north than south, that it
will cost more to ship to a northern than to a southern location, and that a
moving company will charge more for northward than for southward move-
ment. Furthermore, people have greater intention to visit stores advertised to
be south (versus north) of a reference point, especially when ease of travel is
important” (Nelson & Simmons, 2009, p. 715).
12. Is there evidence that our ancestors were accomplished spatial
thinkers?
Throughout human history, people were using spatial thinking strategies
in their daily lives as they travelled, worshipped, hunted, and sought safety,
food, and shelter. Their survival would have depended heavily on thinking in
and about physical space.
Humans have long relied on physical and environmental cues to help them
navigate around their respective landscapes, including snow drifts, sand
dunes, vegetation patterns, or ocean waves, depending on their situation.
Within each one of these contexts, those cues would have provided
information about location, distances, and directions that would have aided
navigation, migration, and movement. For example, to be successful at
celestial navigation, one must be able to extract meaning from the patterns of
stars, to estimate distances between individual stars or the stars and the
horizon, and to calculate directions based on these inputs. Early explorers
such as the Vikings may have also used patterns of light refraction coming
through crystals to be able to discern the sun’s location during cloudy days.
Polynesians and other island-dwelling people also used the shapes of clouds,
the color of the ocean water, and the directions from which waves were
traveling to keep track of their location. Their famous “stick chart” maps
could show not only the distance between islands, but the regional patterns
of ocean currents as well.
FREQUENTLY ASKED QUESTIONS 59
The island inhabitants of Rapa Nui (also known as Easter Island) were
thinking in and about space when they engineered a system of felled logs to
move stones weighing tens of thousands of pounds. The statues carved from
the stones could also be moved from one location to another, without tipping
or breaking, using a system of ropes and sheer muscle strength. Plus, some of
the Rapa Nui statues are oriented to face the location where the sun sets
during the equinox. This type of earth-sun spatial knowledge is also evident
at other ancient sites throughout the world, such as Angkor Wat in
Cambodia, Chichen Itza in Mexico, Machu Pichu in Peru, Karnak in Egypt,
and several Native American sites. Native populations across almost all
continents used their skills at spatial thinking both to design and construct
their sacred structures.
Thousands of years ago, engineers built simple and elegant aqueducts in
the Roman Empire, for example, that demonstrated remarkable
understanding of hydrology. Some of these structures carried water over 1000
miles with an average gradient of just inches per mile, enough gradient to
f low but not enough to achieve velocities that would erode the surfaces.
In other cases, mere survival also depended on being able to construct
simple tools out of simple materials. Designing and assembling a bow and
arrow, an adze, or a trap for small animals requires active and accurate
spatial thinking.
13. Is there spatial thinking prevalent in the video games that children
(and adults) play? Will all the hours that my children spend playing video
games make them better spatial thinkers?
Video and computer games are filled with spatial activities: driving,
navigating, seeking, hiding, shooting, balancing, arranging, and
maneuvering. Researchers are regularly investigating how these virtual,
digital escapades translate into our spatial realities.
For example, we know that regularly playing Tetris, which requires
rotating and shifting a series of shaped blocks to fit into a continuous field of
other blocks, improves people’s mental rotation skills. Playing
action-oriented games, including popular first-person shooting games,
improves a variety of spatial skills. Practicing a first-person shooter game
leads to substantial improvements in how well people are able to pay
attention to visual-spatial patterns. After they practiced playing the game,
both men and women became more observant of a greater portion of a
computer screen and could better identify what had happened in different
60 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
maker to query, measure, and analyze the data, rather than draw conclusions
based on visual interpretation alone. Perhaps the map maker will be inspired
to gather additional data sets to explore further. GIS has vast potential for
use in many different formal and informal educational settings, though
thoughtful guidance to learning with this tool is necessary.
To date, we know little about the connections between the use of GIS and
the areas of spatial cognition that many psychologists study, such as mental
rotation, for example. Most often, psychologists prefer to test their subjects
in lab settings, and with simple maps, so that it is easier to control for other
variables and single out the effects of the “treatment” they might be
researching. Realistic use of GIS, on the other hand, would have many
factors affecting one’s spatial cognitive abilities concurrently.
15. Education is supposed to help us become “critical thinkers.” Are there
connections between critical and spatial thinking?
Critical thinking means balancing evidence, considering multiple
perspectives, and evaluating ideas systematically. Critical thinkers have the
habit of mind to study a situation or problem from all angles, draw upon
information from multiple and diverse frames of reference, and reach a
reasoned conclusion that ref lects their best judgment.
Effective critical thinking may involve strategies that are inherently
spatial. Our descriptions of critical thinking use spatial vocabulary:
perspectives, angles, frames of reference. The metaphors we use for the act of
critical thinking are often spatial too: step back to see the big picture, look at
it from another angle, get to the bottom of it. While critical thinkers weigh
evidence and consider alternatives, they may create spatial arrangements of
information, such as a f low chart or a concept map, to help them understand
a sequence of events or the relationships among the variables.
Another connection between critical and spatial thinking concerns the use
of representations themselves. To wade through the plethora of data and
information being generated by the world today, more and more people are
relying on graphical representations, including tables, charts, diagrams,
figures, and maps. Think of all the popular media (newspapers, magazines,
and television news shows) that now employ these visual representations of
information. Many of these have spatial dimensions: trend lines, clusters,
sequences, and patterns that need to be interpreted to be able to “read” the
image and extract meaning.
Graphicacy is the ability to understand and present information in the
FREQUENTLY ASKED QUESTIONS 63
form of sketches, images, diagrams, maps, plans, charts, graphs and other
non-textual formats. Strong graphicacy skills mean that one can competently
and confidently interpret information that comes in a graphical format. To
the list of other “critical” competencies within education (reading, writing,
speaking and thinking), we must add critical viewing skills. Visual images
can be powerfully compelling, and critical thinkers must be able to extract
and assess the evidence when it is presented in these formats. Plus, they need
to be able to generate their own representations of information as they
evaluate factors. In this sense we find a further connection between spatial
and critical thinking.
16. How is spatial thinking being addressed in formal school settings? Is it
part of any curricula?
Students are often called upon to use spatial thinking skills during their
school years. Young children build with blocks, learn about the phases of the
moon and what generates changing seasons, and are taught about the
geographical patterns of rivers, mountains, and cities around the world. They
are encouraged to learn their street address and the relative location of their
home to school and other key locations in the community. As students
progress through the grades, they learn about different places and events
through social studies, and about natural phenomena in science at increasing
levels of sophistication, and using a growing range of observational and
recording skills. Thus, there are opportunities for school content to engage
spatial thinking strategies, but these activities are more likely to be isolated
experiences. The development of spatial thinking is not an explicit goal of
any school curriculum as literacy and numeracy are; spatial thinking
strategies are not part of any regular collection of instructional goals nor
learning outcomes. As the committee [that wrote Learning to Think Spatially]
said, “Spatial thinking is…under-recognized, undervalued, underapprecia-
ted, and therefore, under-instructed” (National Research Council, 2006, p.
15).
In the absence of spatial thinking being a recognized portion of our
educational content, it is accordingly not explicitly assessed. Many suggest
that this has led to a “gap” in our educational achievement. Students with
strong spatial skills will have their talents go unrecognized and therefore
unsupported, or not be challenged to do more. With all prioritization going
to verbal and mathematical content, those with spatial aptitudes and interests
are side-lined. The largest outcome of this oversight may be an unnecessary
limiting of those who might otherwise choose science, technology,
64 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
A Note for
Geography Educators
Dear Teachers and Professors of Geography:
The focus of this volume has been on space at all scales: from the nano to the
astro-physical. Though people most frequently function and interact at a human
scale (that of their personal spaces, their local environment, and the surface of
the earth) they are capable of thinking across all spatial scales. This is because
the cognitive processes of spatial thinking (of visualizing and interpreting loca-
tion, position, distances, directions, relationships, movement, and changes) can
be applied and are relevant at all scales, though especially at scales that are very
small and very vast, we must rely on digital tools to help us with the visualization
and interpretation.
The domain of Geography, as it is typically taught and understood as
a disciplinary subject, focuses on an environmental and earth-scale. This
small book was not written for spatial thinking only at that scale, though it does
include it. Understanding geographic processes, interpreting geographic patterns
seen directly or through representations, and analyzing human/environment
interactions, the fundamental activities within Geography, are all practices that
rely on the act of spatial thinking.
The recently updated Geography for Life: National Geography Standards,
Second Edition, identifies what every geographically informed person should
know and be able to do, ideally by the time he/she graduates from twelfth grade.
Geography for Life, as it is commonly referred to, is a consensus document produ-
ced in collaboration by representatives of the four main geography organizations
in the United States: the American Geographical Society, the Association of
American Geographers, the National Council for Geographic Education, and the
National Geographic Society. It informs state and local geography curriculum
standards.
Geography for Life is broken into three main parts: perspectives, content
knowledge and skills. Geographers have unique perspectives to frame their
understanding of the world. The spatial perspective is one of these. This
perspective asks, “Where is it? Why is it there?” and is concerned with spatial
patterns of both human and physical phenomena. The 18 geographic knowledge
66
A NOTE FOR GEOGRAPHY EDUCATORS 67
2. G eogr aphy Standard 12: The processes, patterns, and functions of human
settlement. Geography for Life, p. 70
12th G r ade: Urban Forms and Functions: Urban models are used to ana-
lyze the growth and form of urban regions. Therefore, the student is able to
explain and compare the growth and structure of cities using different urban
models, as exemplified by being able to:
a. Identify and explain contemporary urban conditions that may not
be addressed in urban models (e.g., homelessness, squatter settlements,
transitions in ethnic neighborhoods, low-income public housing, gentri-
fication).
CHAPTER 10
1. What is spatial thinking?
National Research Council. (2006). Learning to Think Spatially: GIS as a Support System in the K-12
Curriculum. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. Retrieved from http://www.nap.edu/open-
book.php?record_id=11019
University of California at Santa Barbara. (2008). Spatial thinking. Convergence. Retrieved from
http://convergence.ucsb.edu/article/spatial-thinking
2. Can you give me examples of where spatial thinking, or the lack thereof, has really mattered or made a
difference in the world?
a. Stopping oil from flowing
Deepwater Horizon response and restoration. U.S. Department of the Interior. Retrieved from
http://www.doi.gov/deepwaterhorizon/index.cfm
NOAA Deepwater Horizon/BP Oil spill archive. Office of Response and Restoration, National Oceanic and
Atmospheric Administration. Retrieved from http://response.restoration.noaa.gov/deepwaterhorizon
b. Returning to Earth from space
Easton, P. (2005, April 17). Group honors engineers who saved Apollo 13. Houston Chronicle. Retrieved from
http://www.chron.com/disp/story.mpl/space/3139248.html
c. Escaping from Alcatraz
A byte out of history. (2007, June 8). Federal Bureau of Investigation. Retrieved from
http://www.fbi.gov/news/stories/2007/june/alcatraz_060807
Sullivan, S. (2009, September 22). Mystery still swirls around Alcatraz escape. National Public Radio.
Retrieved from http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=112857262
The great escape from Alcatraz. Ocean View Publishing. Retrieved from http://www.alcatrazhistory.com/
alcesc1.htm
d. Designing a prosthetic limb
70
REFERENCES AND BIBLIOGRAPHY 71
Adee, S. (2008, February 1). Dean Kamen’s Luke Arm prosthesis readies for clinical trials. IEEE Spectrum.
Retrieved from http://spectrum.ieee.org/biomedical/bionics/dean-kamens-luke-arm-prosthesis-readies-for-
clinical-trials
Longman, J. (2007, May 15). An amputee sprinter: Is he disabled or too-abled? The New York Times.
Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2007/05/15/sports/othersports/15runner.html
Motala: The Thai elephant. Retrieved from http://animom.tripod.com/motala.html
See Amy run. Retrieved from http://www.seeamyrun.com/
e. Protecting air and water rights
Airport noise report. Retrieved from http://www.airportnoisereport.com/
American Electric Power settles $4.6B pollution suit. (2007, October 9). National Public Radio. Retrieved
from http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=15117556
Blanford, N. (2007, Sept. 10). Why did Israeli planes enter Syria? Time. Retrieved from
http://www.time.com/time/world/article/0,8599,1660477,00.html
Cody, E. (2006, January 12). A stand against China’s pollution tide. The Washington Post. Retrieved from
http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/article/2006/01/11/AR2006011102246.html
El Dahan, M. (2009, July 27). Egypt says historic Nile river rights not negotiable. Reuters. Retrieved from
http://www.reuters.com/article/idUSTRE56Q3LZ20090727
Kansas’ largest utility settles Clean Air lawsuit for $500 million. (2010, Janary 25). Environment News
Service. Retrieved from http://www.ens-newswire.com/ens/jan2010/2010-01-25-091.html
Nile river politics: Who receives water? (2000, August 10). Global Policy Forum. Retrieved from
http://www.globalpolicy.org/component/content/article/198/40151.html
Pollution turns river red in central China. (2008, February 27). The New York Times. Retrieved from
http://www.nytimes.com/2008/02/27/world/asia/27iht-china.1.10466115.html
Ross, C. (2009, August 20). State may absorb $30m on project built over Pike. The Boston Globe. Retrieved
from http://www.boston.com/realestate/news/articles/2009/08/20/state_may_absorb_30m_on_pike_air_
rights_project/
Russian aircraft have skirted British airspace 18 times in two years. (2009, July 10). The Telegraph. Retrieved
from http://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/newstopics/politics/defence/5788832/Russian-aircraft-have-skirted-
British-airspace-18-times-in-two-years.html
f. Reconstructing an airplane
A byte out of history: The case of the mysterious mid-air explosion. (2005, December 9). Federal Bureau of
Investigation. Retrieved from http://www.fbi.gov/news/stories/2005/december
Van Natta Jr., D. (1997, May 11). Reconstructing T.W.A 800. The New York Times. Retrieved from
http://www.nytimes.com/1997/05/11/magazine/reconstructing-twa-800.html?pagewanted=1
g. Striking the intended target
Myers, S. L. (2000, April 17). Chinese embassy bombing: A wide net of blame. The New York Times.
Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2000/04/17/world/chinese-embassy-bombing-a-wide-net-of-blame.
html
h. Making strategic decisions in war
Napoleon’s invasion of Russia. Geographia. Retrieved from http://www.geographia.com/russia/rushis05.htm
72 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
The malleability of spatial skills: A meta-analysis of training studies. Spatial Intelligence and Learning Cen-
ter. Retrieved from http://www.silccenter.org/index.php/resources/showcase/155-showcase-june-2012-the-
malleability-of-spatial-skills-a-meta-analysis-of-training-studies
4. How does spatial thinking correlate with the right-brain, left-brain divisions? You know, the way that
our right brain controls our creative side and parallel processing, and our left-brain does the sequential
ordering and more logical and rational work? What are brain scientists saying about spatial thinking?
Brain changes seen in cabbies that take ‘The Knowledge’. (2011, December 8). BBC. Retrieved from
http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/health-16086233
Carey, B. (2009, July 22). The curious case of Phineas Gage, refocused. The New York Times. Retrieved from
http://tierneylab.blogs.nytimes.com/2009/07/22/the-curious-case-of-phineas-gage-refocused/
Gersmehl, P. J. (2011). Wormholes in the Common Core: Spatial reasoning, literacy and mathematics educa-
tion. Anekumene, 1(2). Retrieved from http://www.anekumene.com/index.php/revista/article/view/26/25
Gersmehl, P., & Gersmehl, C. (2007). Spatial thinking by young children: Neurologic evidence for early
development and ‘educability’. Journal of Geography, 106, 181–191.
Jabr, F. (2011, December 8). Cache Cab: Taxi drivers’ brains grow to navigate London’s streets. Scientific
American. Retrieved from http://www.scientificamerican.com/article.cfm?id=london-taxi-memory
5. Our car has a GPS unit, and we use it to find places with which we’re unfamiliar, whether that’s a par-
ticular address in our city or driving to places far away while we’re on vacation. Are we thinking spatially?
Charette, R. (2008, March 19). GPS says: Turn left, turn right, drive off cliff. IEEE Spectrum. Retrieved from
http://spectrum.ieee.org/riskfactor/computing/it/gps_says_turn_left_turn_right
GPS puts truck driver under a bridge too short. (2009, July 9). KATU.com. Retrieved from
http://www.katu.com/news/weird/50360517.html
Hutchingson, A. (2009, November). Global impositioning systems. The Walrus. Retrieved from
http://www.walrusmagazine.ca/articles/2009.11-health-global-impositioning-systems/
Huth, J. (2013). The Lost Art of Finding Our Way. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Sat Nav sends mini-bus into river. (2008, April 23). Metro (UK). Retrieved from
http://www.metro.co.uk/weird/144292-sat-nav-sends-mini-bus-into-river
The last word: This is your brain on GPS. (2009, November 15). The Week. Retrieved from
http://www.theweek.com/article/index/102501/The_last_word_This_is_your_brain_on_GPS
Wilson, K. A. C. (2009, December 29). Cheery-voiced GPS units lead drivers into danger. The Oregonian.
Retrieved from http://www.oregonlive.com/news/index.ssf/2009/12/search_and_rescue_experts_say.html
6. On my new cell phone I have applications that can tell me where the nearest metro station is or what good
restaurants are nearby. I even read about a new application that has “augmented reality” capabilities, and
when I point my phone’s camera in a particular direction I can be shown things on its screen that are not
actually visible otherwise. Wow. Where is the spatial thinking in all of this?
Geospatial revolution. Penn State Public Broadcasting. Retrieved from http://geospatialrevolution.psu.edu/
10 amazing augmented reality iPhone apps. Mashable. Retrieved from
http://mashable.com/2009/12/05/augmented-reality-iphone/
7. Are there any famous spatial thinkers in history?
Analogy of the divided line. Retrieved from
http://www.princeton.edu/~achaney/tmve/wiki100k/docs/Analogy_of_the_divided_line.html
CSISS Classics. Center for Spatially Integrated Social Sciences. Retrieved from http://www.csiss.org/classics/
Ernest H. Shackleton. Antarctic explorers. Retrieved from http://www.south-pole.com/p0000097.htm
Leonardo. National Museum of Science and Technology. Retrieved from
74 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
http://www.museoscienza.org/english/leonardo/
Remembered: Marie Tharp, pioneering mapmaker of the ocean floor. Columbia News. Retrieved from
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/news/06/08/tharp.html
Rosenberg, M. Ptolemy. What is Geography. Retrieved from
http://geography.about.com/od/historyofgeography/a/ptolemy.htm
Sicinski, A. Visual thinking: The path to genius? Visual Thinking Magic. Retrieved from
http://www.visualthinkingmagic.com/path-to-genius
With resolute and thorough planning: Captain Meriwether Lewis’ preparations for
the journey to the Pacific ocean. Corps of Discovery: Lewis and Clark. Retrieved from
http://www.history.army.mil/LC/The%20Mission/planning_and_preparation.htm
8. I hear people say they are visual learners, and we know that “a picture is worth 1000 words.” Sometimes
visual and spatial learners are lumped together as one group. What’s the difference between visual learning
and spatial learning?
Developing your visual awareness. Visual Thinking Magic. Retrieved from
http://www.visualthinkingmagic.com/
Thompson, C. (2010, September 27). Clive Thompson on the power of visual thinking. Wired Magazine.
Retrieved from http://www.wired.com/magazine/2010/09/st_thompson_visual/
Visual thinking strategies. Retrieved from http://www.vtshome.org/
9. There’s a guy at work who stands really close to me when he talks. He invades my “personal” space, and it
makes me uncomfortable. Is he just a lousy spatial thinker?
Cohen, S. (2009, July 13). Proxemics – the psychology of positioning and personal space. My career manager:
Reach tour career potential. Retrieved from http://mycareermanager.blogspot.com/2009/07/proxemics-
psychology-of-positioning-use.html
Harrison, B., Ishii, H., & Chignell, M. H. (n.d). An empirical study on orientation of
shared workspaces and interpersonal spaces in video-mediated collaboration. Retrieved from
http://www.dgp.utoronto.ca/OTP/papers/video.mediated.collaboration/ishii.html
Sheppard, M. (1996, July). Proxemics. Retrieved from http://www.cs.unm.edu/~sheppard/proxemics.htm
10. Do we have evidence that animals, apart from humans, demonstrate the capacity for spatial thinking?
Bird migration facts. Zoological Society of Milwaukee. Retrieved from
http://www.zoosociety.org/Conservation/BWB-ASF/Library/BirdMigrationFacts.php
Jensen, R. (2006). Behaviorism, latent learning, and cognitive maps: Needed revisions in introductory
psychology textbooks. The Behavior Analyst, 29, 187–209.
Montgomery, J. C., & Walker, M. M. (2001). Orientation and navigation in Elamsobranchs: Which way
forward? Environmental Biology of Fishes, 60, 109–116.
Pigeons and World War One. History Learning Site. Retrieved from
http://www.historylearningsite.co.uk/pigeons_and_world_war_one.htm
Raver, A. (1994, December 11). CUTTINGS; Now it can be told: All about squirrels and nuts. The New York
Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/1994/12/11/nyregion/cuttings-now-it-can-be-told-all-about-
squirrels-and-nuts.html
Wolf behavior 101. Retrieved from http://www.wolfcenter.org/wolf-behavior.aspx
11. If spatial thinking is so pervasive, is it reflected in our language?
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (2003). Metaphors We Live By. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Original
edition, 1980.
Lehrer, J. (2009, November 20). The reading brain. Science Blogs. Retrieved from
http://scienceblogs.com/cortex/2009/11/20/the-reading-brain-1/
REFERENCES AND BIBLIOGRAPHY 75
Nelson, L. D., & Simmons, J. P. (2009). On southbound ease and northbound fees: Literal consequences
of the metaphoric link between vertical position and cardinal direction. Journal of Marketing Research, 46,
715–724.
Rivers that flow north. World Atlas. Retrieved from http://www.worldatlas.com/aatlas/infopage/riversno.htm
Rosenberg, M. Rivers flowing north. About Geography. Retrieved from
http://geography.about.com/od/learnabouttheearth/a/northrivers.htm
Sanger-Katz, M. (2010, March/April). This side up. Yale Alumni Magazine. Retrieved from
http://www.yalealumnimagazine.com/articles/2783
Spatial metaphor in sign language poetry. Retrieved from
http://www.bristol.ac.uk/education/research/sites/micsl/downloads/research-materials/spatialmetaphorandi-
dentity.pdf
Sutton-Spence, R. Spatial metaphor and expressions of identity in sign language poetry. Retrieved from
http://www.metaphorik.de/19/sutton-spence.pdf
Why we use spatial metaphors, like high and low, for emotions – and how they can help you feel better.
(2010, March 30). Science Codex. Retrieved from http://www.sciencecodex.com/to_remember_the_good_
times_reach_for_the_sky
12. Is there evidence that our ancestors were spatial thinkers?
Clark, L. (2000, February 15). Polynesia’s genius navigators. NOVA. Retrieved from
http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/nova/ancient/polynesia-genius-navigators.html
Desert survival: Navigation. Lawrence of Arabia. PBS. Retrieved from
http://www.pbs.org/lawrenceofarabia/revolt/navigation.html
Lai, L. (2012, June 22). The riddle of the statues of Easter Island, solved? ABC News. Retrieved from
http://abcnews.go.com/blogs/headlines/2012/06/the-riddle-of-the-statues-of-easter-island-solved
Marchant, J. (2011, January 31). Did Vikings navigate by polarized light? Retrieved from
http://www.nature.com/news/2011/110131/full/news.2011.58.html
Odenwald, S. Technology through time, Issue #72: Ancient astronomical alignments. NASA.
Retrieved from http://sunearthday.nasa.gov/2011/articles/ttt_72.php
Prehistoric tools. About.Com Archaeology. Retrieved from
http://archaeology.about.com/od/tooltypes/Prehistoric_Tools.htm
Roman aqueducts. Roman History. Retrieved from http://www.unrv.com/culture/roman-aqueducts.php
Taylor, R. (2012, March/April). How a Roman aqueduct works. Archaeology Archive, 65(2). Retrieved from
http://archive.archaeology.org/1203/features/how_a_roman_aqueduct_works.html
13. Is there spatial thinking prevalent in the video games that children (and adults) play? Will all the hours
that my children spend playing video games make them better spatial thinkers?
Dewar, G. (2011–2012). Improving spatial skills in children and teens: Evidence-based activities and tips.
Parenting Science. Retrieved from http://www.parentingscience.com/spatial-skills.html
Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2007). Action video game experience alters the spatial resolution of vision.
Psychological Science, 18, 88–94. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01853.x
Play your way to the top. (2008, December 18). The Age/Digital Life. Retrieved from
http://www.theage.com.au/digital-life/play-your-way-to-the-top-20090615-ccgj.html#ixzz25ZGWPpOH
Playing video games reduces sex differences in spatial skills. (2007, October 26). Science Daily.
Retrieved from http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2007/10/071024145626.htm
Schmidt, M. E., & Vandewater, E. A. (2008, Spring). Media and attention, cognition, and school achieve-
ment. Children and Electronic Media, 18(1). Retrieved from http://futureofchildren.org/futureofchildren/
publications/journals/article/index.xml?journalid=32&articleid=57§ionid=256
Spatial intelligence and learning center. Retrieved from http://spatiallearning.org/
76 THE PEOPLE’S GUIDE TO SPATIAL THINKING
Spatial reasoning. National Institute for Women in Trades, Technology, and Science. Retrieved from
http://www.iwitts.org/proven-practices/retention-sub-topics/spatial-reasoning
Yates, M. (2009, September 3). What are the benefits of Tetris? BBC News. Retrieved from
http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/uk_news/magazine/8233850.stm
14. Can you describe the connections between using geographic information systems (GIS) and spatial
thinking?
Lee, J. W., & Bednarz, B. (2009). Effect of GIS learning on spatial thinking. Journal of Geography in Higher
Education, 33, 183–198. Retrieved from http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/03098260802276714
National Research Council. (2006). Learning to Think Spatially: GIS as a Support System in the K-12 Cur-
riculum. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. Retrieved from http://www.nap.edu/openbook.
php?record_id=11019
Schöning, J., Hecht, B., Raubal, M., Krüger, A., Marsh, M., & Rohs, M. (2008). Improving interac-
tion with virtual globes through spatial thinking: Helping users ask “why?”. In Proceedings of the 13th
International Conference on Intelligent User Interfaces (IUI ’08). ACM, New York, NY (pp. 129–138).
doi:10.1145/1378773.1378790
Sinton, D., & Lund, J. (2007). Understanding place: GIS and mapping across the curriculum. Redlands, CA:
Esri Press. Retrieved from http://esripress.esri.com/display/index.cfm?fuseaction=display&websiteID=108&
moduleID=0
15. Education is supposed to help us become “critical thinkers.” Are there connections between critical and
spatial thinking?
Goodchild, M., & Janelle, D. (2010). Toward critical spatial thinking in the social sciences and humanities.
GeoJournal, 75, 3–13. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2863328/
Goodchild, M. R. (2006, Fall). The fourth R? Rethinking GIS education. ArcNews. Retrieved from
http://www.esri.com/news/arcnews/fall06articles/the-fourth-r.html
Graphicacy. Innovation Design in Education. Retrieved from
http://theasideblog.blogspot.com/p/graphicacy.html
Literacy, numeracy, graphicacy. YouTube video. Retrieved from
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=7ZvsZtUfQQs
16. How is spatial thinking being addressed in formal school settings? Is it part of any curricula?
Hill, C., Corbett, C., St. Rose, A. (2010, March 21). Why so few? Women in science, technology, engineer-
ing, and math. American Association of University Women. Retrieved from http://www.aauw.org/research/
why-so-few/
National Council for Geographic Education. (2012). Geography for Life: National Geography Standards (2nd
ed.). Washington, DC: NCGE. Retrieved from http://www.ncge.org/geography-for-life
National Research Council. (2006). Learning to Think Spatially: GIS as a Support System in the K-12 Cur-
riculum. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. Retrieved from http://www.nap.edu/openbook.
php?record_id=11019
Wai, J. (2012, March 11). Why don’t we value spatial intelligence? Finding the next Einstein. Psychology
Today. Retrieved from http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/finding-the-next-einstein/201203/why-dont-
we-value-spatial-intelligence
Wai, J. Three reasons why schools neglect spatial intelligence. Finding the next Einstein. Psychology Today.
Retrieved from http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/finding-the-next-einstein/201208/three-reasons-why-
schools-neglect-spatial-intelligence
17. I’ve never heard of spatial thinking before. Is this really just something that a few academics are talking
about?
Learning Spatially at the University of Redlands. Retrieved from http://www.spatial.redlands.edu/lens/
REFERENCES AND BIBLIOGRAPHY 77
Spatial@UCSB, Center for Spatial Studies, University of California at Santa Barbara. Retrieved from
http://www.spatial.ucsb.edu/
Spatial Intelligence and Learning Center. Retrieved from http://spatiallearning.org/
What is geoliteracy? National Geographic. Retrieved from
http://education.nationalgeographic.com/education/geoliteracy/?ar_a=1
CHAPTER 11
National Council for Geographic Education. (2012). Geography for Life: National Geography Standards (2nd
ed.). Washington, DC: NCGE. Retrieved from http://www.ncge.org/geography-for-life
PHOTO CREDITS
Page 14: Liverpool street station at rush hour, Keith Gentry/Shutterstock.com
Page 15: (left) Cars parked on a NYC street, Roman Striga/Shutterstock.com; (right) Ballroom dance in
motion, Richard Goldberg/Shutterstock.com
Page 16: Conductor before military band, Kosarev Alexander/Shutterstock.com
Page 17: Rube Goldberg Diagram, Artwork Copyright© and TM Rube Goldberg Inc. All Rights Reserved.
RUBE GOLDBERG© is a registered trademark of Rube Goldberg Inc. All materials used with permission.
rubegoldberg.com
Page 18: Time zone map, Olinchuk/Shutterstock.com
Page 19: (left) Old sun clock dial, vintage sundial, Successo/Shutterstock.com; (right) The Western Wall,
Jerusalem, Kim Briers /Shutterstock.com
Page 20: Hurricane tracks over historic time, http://www.commerce.gov/blog/2012/08/13/noaa-provides-easy-
access-historical-hurricane-tracks
Page 21: (top) US property. No trespassing, AdStock RF/Shutterstock.com; (middle) Tectonic plates,
subduction zone, Daulon/Shutterstock.com; (bottom) City road interchange, Maria Skaldina/Shutterstock.
com
Page 22: DNA Helix, Shutterstock.com
Page 23: Minard diagram of Napoleon’s march, http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:Minard.png
Page 24: (left) Car dashboard, Cowardlion/Shutterstock.com; (right) Handmade cross-stitch, Mycola/
Shutterstock.com;
Page 25: (top) Periodic Table of the Elements, concept w/Shutterstock.com; (bottom left) A corporate hi-
erarchy structure chart, John T Takai/Shutterstock.com; (bottom right) Hand playing chess, Randy Lewis/
Shutterstock.com
Page 26: Shapes of continents to hypothesize drift and plate tectonics, a. http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Conti-
nental_drift b. http://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/thumb/1/10/Snider-Pellegrini_Wegener_fos-
sil_map.svg/2000px-Snider-Pellegrini_Wegener_fossil_map.svg.png
Page 29: (left) ‘Blood drips’/Shutterstock.com; (right) Passenger plane and blue sky with clouds, Sergey Nivens/
Shutterstock.com
Page 30: Disease spread/diffusion, http:/commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File%3ABubonic_plague_map_2.png
Page 31: 3D city, view from above, Martan/Shutterstock.com
Page 33: (left) ‘Male tourist asking female driver about direction’ - DmitriMaruta/Shutterstock.com; (right)
‘Man drawing a game strategy’ - Viorel Sima/Shutterstock.com
Page 34: ‘DaVinci Crossbow’ - http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:DaVinci_Crossbow.JPG
Page 35: ‘Mother teaching little unhappy girl to tie her shoes’ - Ilike/Shutterstock.com
Page 39: ‘Business woman walking outside her office’ - 41/Shutterstock.com
Page 40: ‘Girl with map at Reichstag building’- Jan Kranendonk/Shutterstock.com
COVER
‘Boy building a sand castle at a beautiful tropical beach’ - Kamira/Shutterstock.com
‘Mind Puzzle: Find the correct missing part! Answer: C’ - VOOK/Shutterstock.com
‘Girl pointing on map of United States’ - iofoto/Shutterstock.com
‘Hand and blueprint - engineer working on blue print concept’ - Semisatch/Shutterstock.com
‘Electronic engineer solving problems’ - Dariush M/Shutterstock.com
About the Authors
Diana Stuart Sinton is a geographer who works for the University Consortium
for Geographic Information Science. She likes to teach, read, write, and talk
about GIS, geography education, and spatial thinking. Since the late 1990s,
she has pursued these activities under direction and support from the National
Science Foundation, the National Endowment for the Humanities, Esri, the W.
M. Keck Foundation, the Andrew W. Mellon Foundation, the European Union,
and several universities, including the University of Redlands. You can learn
more about her professional experiences at dianamaps.com and teachGIS.org.
78
ABOUT THE AUTHORS 79
AP Human Geography
By Jody Smothers Marcello
AP Human Geography: Engaging Students in Constructing an Understand of Human
Geography is designed in three facets important to success in AP Human Geography.
1) Synthesis: Due to the nature of geography, the disciplinary mind in geography
naturally demands synthesis. At its most basic level, geography synthesizes concepts
from the human and physical worlds. 2) Inquiry: Geography is a field of inquiry. The
lessons chosen for inclusion in this publication engage students in the inquiry process
and our organized so as to give teachers a way to manage this process within the APHG
classroom and course. And 3) Spatial Data: Students need more practice reading
maps. This does not mean learning place locations, but being able to critically interpret
the information presented. Students who were able to read and understand the maps
generally got the highest scores. Available at www.ncge.org