_OceanofPDF.com_1___1__L2__How_to_learn_a_language_effici_-_Steven_Tran
_OceanofPDF.com_1___1__L2__How_to_learn_a_language_effici_-_Steven_Tran
Steven Tran
Copyright © 2020 by Steven Tran
All rights reserved.
2nd edition
Visit www.psylingo.com/blog for more quirky science studies
Contents
Receptive and productive knowledge
Second language acquisition
Intentional and incidental language learning
Involvement Load Hypothesis
Reading for language acquisition
Intensive reading and extensive reading
Repeated Reading
Language acquisition and television shows
Word pair learning
To listen is to attend
Say no more
Downloading chunk
Mindless games
Breaking the language barrier
Bibliography
Introduction
Ask a friend or family member to write 1 + 1 = 2. What did it look like?
Did they write the numerals? Or did they write out the words: “one plus one
equals two”? The unsurprising outcome would most likely be 1 + 1 = 2.
Despite the constant exposure to letters more often than numerals, we tend
to lean towards writing the latter, particularly in this case. Now imagine that
1 + 1 = 2 is your target language. How can you consciously create a system
to unconsciously see foreign languages similar to this example?
While studying international business and Mandarin Chinese, I
developed an interest in finding quirky language studies to help boost my
learning efficiency. If you are like me, you will have had your fair share of
opinions from other language learners. Some ideas sound good, while
others can be difficult to apply. I always saw myself as the typical language
learner, interested in language learning but lacking in talent. However, to
compensate for the lack of aptitude, I turned towards science for more
effective language learning.
The good news is that studies that observe language learning are not a
new subject. For decades, different fields of science have examined the art
of language learning, especially in the field of psychology. Unfortunately,
studies have yet to find a concrete answer for the perfect study plan. The
diverse investigations presented in this book are a collection of language
learning studies to demonstrate strong evidence in favour of which tasks are
potentially the most efficient. Each study finds a connection with the others
that lends support to a possible route for acquiring different language skills.
Contrary to popular belief, listening, speaking, writing, and reading may not
be as distinctly divided as they may seem.
The collection of this research is not merely to demonstrate that language
self study is an achievable objective, but also to introduce psycholinguistics
to the world of language learners. I hope it will persuade you to take an
interest in the research and effort these academics have spent so much time
on while also helping you enjoy the learning process.
It must be stated that despite the continuous efforts of these researchers,
their studies only demonstrate correlation, not causation. Many factors can
affect how the methods in these studies produce results in a real situation.
Since the studies discussed in this book mostly involve English as a foreign
language or use participants that have different levels of language abilities,
they do have limitations, not only in the experiments but also for each self-
studying individual.
Nevertheless, an objective viewpoint is an important tool. Hopefully,
these studies can shed light on some new quirky ideas for your next
language learning routine and add fun to your studies.
Happy Learning.
Chapter 1
Studies suggest that attaining all nine areas in both receptive and
productive knowledge would indicate a strong depth of vocabulary. But
before you decide to jump into 18 different tasks to target each aspect, it is
necessary to mention there is no need to go to such extreme lengths. Later
we will go through different studies which demonstrate that one task can
cover several aspects and applying these tasks can reduce the number of
impractical tasks in your routine.
The point in discussing the differences in how children and adults learn a
language is to call attention to the learning method children use and how
adults can apply a similar method. That is, they can address receptive tasks
before productive tasks and apply different task types for both receptive and
productive skills. Children begin through listening and monitoring, whereas
adults read second language (L2) texts with key ideas explained to them in
their first language (L1). The reason this needs to be addressed is the order
of knowledge that is exposed first. Receptive knowledge appears to come
first in the learning order, and at a later stage, productive tasks are
introduced.
The order of learning has long revealed the imbalance between the size
of receptive and productive word knowledge. This is not necessarily
abnormal, as indicated in a study by Stuart Webb3. In one of his studies, he
examined the relationship between receptive and productive English
vocabulary size in 83 native Japanese speakers, with over half of the
participants majoring in English. In order to examine the difference in size
for their receptive and productive vocabulary, the participants were tested
with translation tasks. Their test results showed a difference in size, with
receptive scoring higher than productive. These results show that the size of
high-frequency words in receptive and productive knowledge is similar; but
as the size of vocabulary increases, the balance is gradually broken. This
could be related to the lack of use for the low-frequency vocabulary. For
example, a university student will most likely use low-frequency
vocabulary in their assignments more often than a nine-year-old child
would for describing his day with his family.
Another important point in Stuart Webb’s research is the potential to
estimate a person’s productive vocabulary size by the size of their receptive
vocabulary. The test results indicated partial productive knowledge of
orthography. It is possible that the higher the listening and reading skills,
the greater the ability to speak or write. In other words, receptive
knowledge could be the building block of productive knowledge.
The previous research employed mostly university students that majored
in English. In another size comparison study by Dongkwang Shin, Yuah
Chon and Heejin Kim4, the researchers wanted to investigate the English
vocabulary level of the Korean National Curriculum, as they believed there
was a mismatch between the required vocabulary size for the Korean
College Scholastic Ability Test and the education delivered to high school
students based in South Korea. The English section of the Korean College
Scholastic Ability Test (CSAT) is famous for being challenging even for
native English speakers.
The researcher's analysis of 402 seventeen-year-old students reveals a
receptive English vocabulary size of 6,000 and a productive English
vocabulary size of 2,400 on average. The difference in size displays a
sizable imbalance between the two skills. The results imply that the
students have a high comprehension level for reading but a low-level ability
to express themselves. When these numbers are taken into account and
compared to a native speaker, they are equivalent to a native German-
speaking child in grade one or a child that is roughly five to six years old5.
Given the importance of larger receptive skills than productive skills,
how much productive knowledge affects spoken ability? Those reading may
be curious how to learn to speak a language as soon as possible. Sadly,
there is no definite answer; it mostly depends on how much effort and time
you spend on learning. Before you decide to burn this book after hearing
that cliché, it needs to be clarified that although effort does play a large role,
within that role lies an effective method. That being said, researchers have
shown a connection between vocabulary knowledge and speaking skills by
testing creative tasks.
Takumi Uchihara and Kazuya Saito investigated the relationship between
productive vocabulary knowledge and speaking fluency of second language
learners6. The chosen participants were 21 females and 18 males. All
participants had six years of experience in learning English. At the
beginning of the procedure, the participants were tested on their productive
vocabulary knowledge. In the next phase, they were tasked with verbally
explaining what they saw in seven different pictures, each with three
keywords as hints. The researchers needed to analyse the fluency of the
participants, so five native speakers were invited to participate in the
research as examiners. After the post-test, the researchers concluded that
productive knowledge had a significant correlation with L2 speaking
fluency. However, they also pointed out that productive knowledge had
little relation to comprehensibility or accent and that productive tasks may
not be the most effective task type for comprehension development. In
addition to these results, their research suggests that strong productive
vocabulary knowledge could lead to speaking with fewer pauses and
quicker speed.
Understanding the importance of strong productive knowledge should
lead to discovering a task that can provide the necessary skills to speak in
the target language, as it is better to have one good task than to waste
several hours on dozens of mediocre tasks. Common sense would lead most
learners to believe developing spoken fluency requires frequent dialogue
creation through spoken tasks. However, this might not be entirely true, as
shown by another study that probes into productive vocabulary knowledge.
If speaking and writing are in the category of productive skills, would
developing speaking skills lead to an increase in writing skills and vice
versa? While the question of whether or not developing speaking skills will
lead to greater writing skills is still unclear, the reverse process has been
observed. A study by Ehsan Namaziandost and Fariba Rahimi Esfahani
found that developing writing skills could lead to better speaking skills7. In
their study, they set out to discover if writing practices can have a
significant effect on improving EFL learners' speaking skills. There were 50
participants invited for the study. A fair analysis was required, so the
researchers created two groups. First, an experimental group was given
extensive writing tasks, which included writing 150 words on a topic and
writing short stories based on pictures. The second group was a control
group that was given a typical language learning class. In addition to the
experimental group's writing tasks, they were also instructed to read the
written material out loud.
The experimental group focused on extensive writing performed
significantly well in both the speaking and writing tasks. These findings not
only demonstrate that writing skills compliment speaking skills, but also
potentially suggest that developing one productive skill can increase both
skills in the same category. A notable outcome from this study is the
possibility of practicing speaking skills without a partner.
During my time at university studying Mandarin Chinese, my classmates
and I had to spend our third year in Hangzhou in the north-western region
of China. Most of my time with my peers from England was spent
observing how they studied. Two of the best students had excellent
speaking skills, yet they would spend most of their study time writing. One
of them would spend days writing characters, even filling rows and
columns with a single character. To say the least, I was impressed at the
sight of that book. The other classmate did not have such an extreme habit,
but one thing they had in common was their writing tasks. They would
spend more time writing sentences than any of the other classmates. Could
that writing task have contributed to their ability to speak so well?
Let's take an example of a popular task: flashcards. Imagine someone is
learning French and they decide to only use flashcards to learn. They then
decide to pick the most commonly used words in that language and make
flashcards. If the learner has chosen to study from L1 to L2 they will mainly
be developing three aspects of receptive knowledge: the recognition of
written form, meaning, and possibly roots and suffixes. This type of task
will lack in recognising collocations, grammar function, and the other four
receptive knowledge tasks. On the other hand, the writing tasks from the
previous study addressed most, if not all, of the aspects of productive
knowledge. The writing tasks demonstrated more return than the flashcard
task.
The emphasis on productive tasks and how well they produce results for
speaking is not merely a lure to entice you to the potential of writing for
developing speaking skills, but to demonstrate the importance of both
receptive and productive skills. The two skills will find ways to develop
reading, listening, writing and speaking in their respective fields.
Summary
Vocabulary knowledge can be broken into both receptive and productive
knowledge. Each has its own rules; and the more aspects you can master,
the greater the depth of your vocabulary. As such, you need to ask yourself
if your task helps develop any of the nine areas of receptive and productive
knowledge. Grab a pen and paper to draw a spider diagram with a list of
tasks and point them towards the nine aspects of knowledge. The task that
covers the most knowledge may be the most effective for learning
vocabulary.
● There is a definite distinction between receptive and productive
knowledge.
● There are a total of nine aspects of knowledge that represent the
depth of a word, which can be divided into receptive and productive
knowledge.
● The number of vocabulary items known receptively will be greater
than those known productively.
● Receptive and productive tasks can complement each other, and each
one can simultaneously develop some aspects of the other.
Chapter 2
By listening and repeating this sentence while changing the words, the
learner is expected to not only learn the vocabulary, but also create a habit
of spontaneously producing the sentence structure. Another unique feature
of this method is the practice of connectionism and chunking, which will be
seen later in the book.
In the case of the direct method, total immersion is expected in the
classroom. Teachers use the target language throughout the entire class
while the students are expected to focus on guessing the meaning. This
approach cannot be applied in self-study, as it requires a teacher to provide
a visual action along with L2 audio. The focal point of this approach is the
student’s ability to guess the meaning. The act of continuously hearing and
seeing the same or similar actions will help the student process the
language’s system. The student should eventually be able to point to certain
repetitive rules.
In the early 1980s, Tracy Terrell introduced the natural approach, which
sought to provide natural and comprehensible input in the target language.
Students are provided with an abundant number of comprehensible inputs
from the teacher. While receiving target language input, the students are not
expected to speak in the target language and are encouraged to speak when
they feel like they are ready. This is different from the direct method as it
does not involve encouraging or forcing the students to reply with L2. The
natural approach aims to gradually develop the students' confidence in
using the language.
The natural approach has mostly been developed in relations to Stephen
Krashen’s language acquisition theory. It can also be proposed that the
natural approach is a modified version of Krashen’s theory for classroom
use. Although the natural approach is an interesting subject, it is not the
natural approach which this book is interested in, but rather Krashen’s input
hypothesis.
In 1981, Stephen Krashen generated a hypothetical proposal that has
proven popular for the past few decades. He proposed that students need to
be exposed to comprehensible input to acquire a language. Enough input
would then lead to the development of speaking in the target language1.
Therefore, acquiring a language involves the need to understand input in
order to effectively feed output. His work has introduced five hypotheses:
the acquisition-learning distinction, the natural order hypothesis, the
monitor hypothesis, the input hypothesis, and the affective hypothesis.
Although his work on these five aspects is important to realise his thoughts,
this book deals with language self-study; therefore, the most important
aspect of his theory for us is the input hypothesis, which states that
comprehensible input provides language acquisition.
Comprehensible input is language that the learner is able to understand.
Gaining comprehensible input involves acquiring information that is not too
challenging for the learner. Not surprisingly, the input theory is all about
input: the student acquires their target language when they receive
understandable messages. Adults often communicate with children using
simplified sentence structures and simple vocabulary. This helps children
grasp the meaning of the presented word. Comprehensible input can be
acquired from people, books or television shows. The final result is to
promote natural acquisition and the opportunity to speak the language, as
opposed to forming a habit of talking from forced speaking tasks.
When I was young, I would often use the expression 'worser' to describe
a situation that had a stronger meaning than worse. It felt similar to words
such as colder or warmer. At the time, I was not the only child in the school
to use this expression. It was something that spread around the playground.
It was only later that I had learnt to correct this mistake and understand that
not every adjective can add the ending 'er' to increase its degree. I don't
remember exactly where I heard it from, but even now it feels natural to say
it. Out of curiosity, I asked my eight-year-old nephew if he thinks 'worser' is
a real word. He replied, 'No, because I've never heard it before'.
This story is a fine example for depicting the potential of Stephen's
Krashen's language acquisition hypothesis. When I was eight years old, the
internet was still new. YouTube and Netflix were non-existent. I was
acquiring English from the people that were close to me. The unfortunate
result was picking up the expression 'worser' from the other children at
school. Nowadays, children spend more time sitting in front of a tablet
screen than playing with their peers. The results are clear in my nephew's
example. Children in the 21st century have been acquiring input from
various online sources created by adults. This form of input has reduced the
likelihood of acquiring the wrong vocabulary; but it is also a double-edged
sword, since my nephew would often unnecessarily use the word 'like'
several times in one sentence.
A self-studying language learner will have few resources to learn from -
namely, books and the internet. If Stephen Krashen's hypothesis on
comprehensible input is correct, then students can acquire a language
through reading graded books or watching television shows. A professional
classroom environment or moving abroad would be unnecessary. Yet
students that have spent time abroad have consistently shown to have
gained more language ability after traveling as compared to their peers who
have not.
In a study by Taeko Oya, Emmanuel Manalo and Jackie Greenwood, a
correlation was discovered between the duration of time spent studying
abroad in an English-speaking country and the oral skills of native Japanese
speaking students studying English2. The study offers evidence of increased
language skills while studying abroad. The fascinating feature of this
research is the effects seen with other forms of learning; the study was not
exclusively focused on studying abroad. The main objective of the research
was to study the influence of language contact and speaking performance.
Other contact conditions included studying English at professional schools,
the duration of English contact outside of school environments with friends
and members of the household, and the duration of contact with tasks such
as reading English books, watching television, and listening to music. The
overall results indicated there was a relationship between speaking
performance and the duration of language contact.
Another possible explanation that can clarify the outcome of this study is
the duration and location of comprehensible input. Although the researchers
discussed the student’s need to produce more advanced output during their
stay in another country, which could have led to their increased speaking
ability, it can also be assumed that input had assisted in the improvement of
their overall skills, since output requires input. This hypothesis would
suggest total immersion in another country may be the best, but not the
only, method for input.
No matter which scenario they reviewed, the researchers believed there
was a connection between vocabulary knowledge and the duration of
contact. The researchers’ results showed speaking English at home in the
participant's native country with another person, not even necessarily a
native English speaker, had demonstrated positive results. This can also be
explained with comprehensible input: since both participants may not have
the high-level language skills of a native, they would be expected to
communicate on a level that is understandable for the both of them.
Consequently, there is no reason to believe you have to study abroad to gain
comprehensible input.
While searching for a relationship between contact and language skills,
the researchers discovered an interesting outcome from watching television
shows. The results indicated that the time spent watching television had
negatively affected the student’s verb accuracy. It would appear that the
longer the students spent watching, the more likely they were to make
mistakes with verbs. A reasonable explanation for this is the replication of
real dialogues. Television shows often use local dialects which contain
hyphenated or shortened verbs.
A hypothesis, however, is still a hypothesis; and as Stephen Krashen has
outlined, his proposal has yet to be recognised as concrete fact. This may
lead many readers to believe discussing his work up until now was a misuse
of time. Fortunately, researchers have taken an interest in his hypothesis and
formed academic studies based on it.
In one of these academic experiments, Robert Hamilton, while at Meiji
University in Japan, sought to understand how effective a new prototype
bilingual Japanese-English picture book (a comprehensible input material)
would be for natural language acquisition3. The experiment involved two
native Japanese speaking mothers, considered to have either moderate or
excellent English skills, assigned to read the book to their children for a
month and record their reactions.
The researcher was curious to understand whether or not Stephen
Krashen's proposed hypothesis could be utilized to help children that have
yet to acquire reading skills to learn English vocabulary. The parents of the
two test subjects were assigned to expose the pictures and passages in
Japanese and English translation to their children through reading. The pre-
test and post-test on the children's language ability suggested that the
children did acquire new English words, such as friend, help, and
goodnight.
Despite the success of this research, it has limitations and does not show
the effects on adult learners. The participants were children and considered
still in their 'critical period' for learning and, unlike adults, could creatively
attach meaning to new words. Nevertheless, this study shows the potential
of Stephen Krashen's hypothesis.
In another study, in contrast to the last example, 220 non-English-major
students in China were invited to participate. In this study by Han Xiaohui,
the participants were examined to investigate the effects of comprehensible
input on incidental English vocabulary meaning recognition4. The research
involved reading passages that were either exposed in the original form or
several different modified forms. One of these forms included an elaborated
input form which was intended to offer simpler interpretation of the
vocabulary. The control group was assigned to the original passage while
the other experimental groups were assigned to the modified group. After
the post-test, the researchers concluded that all forms of comprehensible
input had scored higher than the control group and that the modified groups
had achieved better understanding of the target vocabulary.
It is important to recognise from the results of this experiment that
comprehensible input is significant and potentially promotes recognition of
vocabulary. If the context is not easy to understand, there will be no
advancement in learning. Stephen Krashen created the i + 1 idea to explain
that context must be understandable for language acquisition, yet it must
also be slightly more difficult to advance.
If two tasks were compared: a cloze task (“fill in the blank”) and a task
that required the reader to read a comprehensible passage, the latter would
prove to be more effective for comprehension acquisition as it provides
more clear information.
Now that we have established some connection between comprehensible
input and language acquisition. We need to identify how comprehensible
input can be more effective than memorisation drills. It is quite common for
language learners to suggest memorising phrases and building a reflex for
using these phrases; but when a comparison is made between a task that
involves memorisation and natural acquisition, which is superior? Between
the audio-lingual approach and the natural approach, which learning
method is more effective?
Professors at the University of Isfahan wanted to look for the most
effective second language acquisition method for improving English
communication. Two methods were used for comparison in their research:
the audio-lingual method and the natural approach5. In this study, 40 Iranian
participants were chosen to attend either an audio-lingual class or a natural
approach class. Both classes were allocated to tasks in their corresponding
group. For example, the audio-lingual class was provided material to listen
to and repeat the grammatical structure, while the natural approach group
had a teacher supply them with L2 input. The students in the natural
approach were not expected to speak the target language until they felt
comfortable in doing so. The post-test results show the natural approach
outperformed the audio-lingual method.
Through a questionnaire, the students in the natural approach stated that
they enjoyed the natural approach since the 'silent period' made them feel
more comfortable. An arguable opinion about this outcome is that
comprehensible input had enabled the students to absorb a moderate
number of sentences and vocabulary, and as a result, they felt confident of
their output sentences. They may have judged their sentences to be correct,
since those sentences resembled what they had heard. If this conjecture is
correct, then acquiring a large amount of comprehensible input would allow
the learner to develop a feeling that their output is correct, ultimately
developing their confidence to speak.
You may be wondering why we are examining research on a classroom
language learning program and not focusing on a type of comprehensible
input task. Part of this answer is the importance of comfort, as mentioned in
the previous paragraph. The aforementioned research exposed an advantage
to acquiring language through comprehensible input: the method provides
adequate time to develop the confidence to speak.
A drawback to the natural approach is time. Depending on the person,
comprehensible input can take many months or even a year before the
learner utters a sentence. In the study that employed a bilingual picture
book, the parents had voiced their scepticism in teaching their children with
comprehensible input, and the fear comes from the lack of action the learner
needs to learn the language. Everyone wants to see results as soon as
possible, and our belief that the more you work the more you earn has
rendered us incapable of understanding 'less is more'. It is important to
acknowledge that using comprehensible input to acquire a language
requires time and patience. Even dust, if piled, can become a mountain.
Summary
There are many different second language learning methods, but as a self-
study learner, these methods are difficult to access without a teacher. The
best option would be to pick the best feature from these methods, such as
Stephen Krashen's input theory, and apply them to your routines.
There are many more vocabulary learning tasks, which is one of the
reasons why vocabulary learning is interesting and fun. You can choose to
try them all to prevent yourself from succumbing to boredom from
countless vocabulary learning sessions; yet if you are only interested in
finding the fastest route for language learning, then I highly recommend
you search for the most effective tasks.
You may have noticed that the tasks were categorized as either receptive
or productive in brackets. The difference between receptive and productive
knowledge was discussed in the first chapter with the intention of helping
readers understand the tasks in this chapter. As you can see, as well as
dividing each task into intentional and incidental, they can be further
classified as receptive and/or productive.
Now you know that a task can be more complicated than you thought.
Imagine placing Post-it Notes all over your furniture with the L2
vocabulary written on each sheet. What skills would you be building up? It
is, in fact, an intentional task that raises your receptive skills. You may
believe seeing these notes every day can help you develop your vocabulary
and provide you with the skills to spontaneously use these words in
conversation but, contrary to popular belief, it is only practising your ability
to recognise meaning.
Before we look at how to find an effective task, we have to discuss the
difference between intentional and incidental. I have given definitions, but
now we need to examine which is better for learning vocabulary. The quick
answer is that incidental learning is better than intentional. The advantages
of incidental learning outweigh those of intentional learning, which will be
reviewed as we discuss each task further. Although this is a quick answer,
the real answer is not that simple.
A direct distinction between intentional and incidental vocabulary
learning has been explored by various researchers. One of these researchers,
Jameel Ahmad, wanted to examine the difference between the two based on
the ‘learner’s ability to understand, retain and use new words’2. In his
research, 20 students were chosen and divided into two groups: an
intentional type question group and an incidental type question group. The
scores from his test indicated group B, assigned to the incidental type
questions, had outperformed group A. This outcome can potentially be
explained by using the relationships between vocabulary. Words are usually
strung together with other vocabulary to provide meaning, syntax, and
morphology; and incidental tasks can deliver the opportunity to see all of
these aspects.
Similarly, while at the University of Arkansas3, Amirreza Karami and
Freddie A. Bowles wanted to explore the best strategy to encourage longer
retention of vocabulary. In contrast to Jameel Ahmad’s research, this study
added a combined group. The researchers decided to create three
experimental groups: incidental learning, intentional learning and a
combined group (a combination of both types) to compete against three
control groups. The strict standards required for this study reduced the
number of participants from 100 to 78 English as a foreign language (EFL)
students. The design of the experiment was simple: each group was
assigned to complete tasks that conformed to the learning procedures
assigned to them, while the control group had various vocabulary
instructions that differed from the experimental group.
After eight weeks of administering the tasks, the post-test scores showed
the three experimental groups had different outcomes, and the scores
suggested incidental learning is better than intentional. When the results of
the combined learning were compared to the other two tasks, incidental
learning ranked second. Consequently, the researchers concluded that
combined learning was the most effective method, followed by incidental
learning. Intentional learning was found to be the least effective. The results
also indicated all three experimental groups had outperformed the control
group. Similar to Jameel Ahmad's findings, intentional tasks provided more
opportunity to learn several aspects of receptive knowledge and, when
combined with the intentional strategy to purposely target specific words,
the final result was longer retention of vocabulary. The combined strategy
had placed more involvement load to target each word.
Repeated Reading
The role of repeated reading can differ from both extensive and intensive
reading. It is not necessarily the type of text that makes them different, but
how the text is used. Typically, repeated reading aims to create meaning
from the text in the first trial, and then, in the subsequent repeated sessions,
to transfer meaning into language form. One example of language form is
morphology, such as the difference between run and running. This approach
to reading delivers a different set of benefits, as demonstrated in the
following studies. The expected benefits are increased reading fluency and
comprehension development.
The majority of studies discussing the benefits of the frequency of
occurrence were based on extensive reading. Repeated reading, on the other
hand, does not contain elements of the frequency of exposure, since the
previous studies suggest exposure in different contexts is the major factor in
this phenomenon. Repeated reading differs by exposing the same context
over several trials and does not present any new text. Looking carefully at
the relationship between repeated reading and intensive reading, they are
not exactly that far apart; in fact, repeated reading may even be considered
a form of intensive reading, since the first time the learner is exposed to the
text it may be difficult for them.
Before looking further into the relationship between repeated reading and
reading fluency and comprehension, we have to look at assisted and
unassisted repeated reading, since the ensuing studies have incorporated
assisted reading into repeated reading tasks. The purpose of adapting audio
to the reading material can be associated with the difficulty of reading
unknown material. At the beginning of language acquisition and learning,
there is a difficult phase in which the learner may find it challenging to read
the text, especially in languages that use logograms. Audio assistance can
support the learner's reading speed and understanding of the text.
Reading an unfamiliar text in a logographic language is daunting for
many people. The reading material can become an unpleasant chore and
may result in the learner ignoring specific words. When this point is taken
into account, assisted repeated reading can be viewed as the best strategy
for vocabulary acquisition. A study by Stuart Webb and Anna Ching-
Shyang Chang demonstrated the difference between assisted and unassisted
repeated reading; the study lends support in favour of using of the former1.
The authors found evidence that both assisted and unassisted repeated
reading had naturally increased vocabulary in the 82 participants; however,
the assisted group delivered higher results.
In another experiment by Etsuo Taguchi et al., the researchers developed
a study aimed at understanding the effects of assisted repeated reading and
extensive reading in developing reading fluency2. They aimed to find
answers to the following questions: (1) Is repeated reading an effective task
for developing fluency? (2) Is repeated reading just as effective as extensive
reading at developing and improving reading fluency and comprehension?
(3) How do beginner learners feel about using repeated reading?
To find the answers, the researchers invited 20 students to participate.
The participants were divided into two groups: extensive reading and
repeated reading. They were then grouped by their TOEFL test scores and
had their words per minute rate measured. These results were used to
compare their results for the post-test.
The result of the experiment was in favour of repeated reading. In
conclusion to their findings, they believe the experiment answered two of
their questions. Repeated reading is as effective as extensive reading in
developing reading fluency for beginners; however, the comprehension
score was less favourable. Even though both tasks had shown improvement
in language comprehension, the researchers stated that extensive reading
slightly outperformed repeated reading in the comprehension test.
Before the results were analysed, the researchers had expected the
automaticity theory would support the repeated reading group. This theory
dictates that the brain provides better resource allocation when there is less
task load. Each repetition should have reduced the need to look at
previously observed information and allowed the mind to freely notice
other unique features of the text that were not noticeable in the previous
trials, resulting in the transformation of meaning to language form.
Intrigued by the failed outcome of comprehension development in the
previous study, Greta Gorsuch and Etsuo Taguchi created another
specifically targeting repeated reading in relations to developing reading
fluency and reading comprehension3. In this study, native speakers of
Vietnamese studying English were invited. The experimental group was
assigned to carry out a repeated reading task five times, with the addition of
every second and third session including assisted reading.
The participants were tasked with repeating segmented text from three
books five times. The experiment continued for eleven weeks, and at the
end of the procedure, a post-test was administered to measure their reading
fluency and comprehension. Again, the results showed improvement in
reading fluency, and these new skills could be seen on unpractised texts.
Comprehension test scores were similar to the previous experiment, with
improvement in the experimental group. The results were more clearly
visible when compared to the control group. Despite the similarity in
reading speed for both groups, the scores on the post-test indicated that the
experimental group was able to understand the text faster than the control
group. This may be supportive evidence for the previous study, which
demonstrated reading while listening developed faster word recognition.
In addition to the main goal, the researchers also wanted to discover why
comprehension score for this experiment was more successful than the
previous experiment. They believe it may have been due to the
experimental group's expectation of a post-test. The experimental group
read slower during the repeated sessions to grasp as much information as
they could in order to prepare for the post-test. This outcome could
potentially imply that fast reading and slow reading both support different
outcomes. Faster reading might accommodate reading fluency
development, whereas reading slowly might encourage comprehension
development.
In reality, the results of the last two studies have yet to provide strong
evidence for developing comprehension. Let us take another look at a study
by Anna Ching-Shyang Chang and Sonia Millet4. They cooperated on a
study including students from different majors, such as accounting and
finance. Similar to the previous two studies, the researchers looked at
understanding the reading fluency and comprehension gains from repeated
reading.
The participants were divided into two groups: repeated and non-repeated
reading. The repeated reading group followed the same routine from the
previous study by repeating the material five times. The non-repeated group
was instructed to read the passages once.
The results demonstrated a positive outcome for the repeated reading
group. This difference was evident when comparing the pre-test with the
post-test scores. Both groups had scored similar results in the pre-test; yet
by the end of the post-test, the difference was significant. The scores
indicated that the repeated reading had led to a greater increase in reading
rate (words per second) than the non-repeated group. The reading
comprehension results were also satisfactory. The experimental group
achieved higher comprehension scores than the group that only read the text
once. The researchers concluded that these skills can be transferred to
passages that have not been practised.
Discussing the results of reading tasks is to present the possible gains of
comprehensive input from reading; and as the research suggests, repeated
reading not only provides input but also improves comprehension. The
results of the studies offer a reason to consider adding repeated reading into
your learning routine. A downside to repeated reading is the length of time
required. In these studies, the participants read the material five times, with
different text material each session, and the procedure continued for several
months. Despite this disadvantage, the advantages proposed by the
researchers may still prove it is worth it.
Improved reading fluency and comprehension are the only advantages of
repeated reading that have been thoroughly studied. In this next study, Sujin
Kim from Sunchon National University investigated the advantages of
repeated reading. Dissimilar to the previous studies, the focus was how
repeated reading affected speaking fluency and writing5. The repeated
reading approach was also different when compared with the previous
studies. The participants were only required to read texts three times a week
for two months, and the reading material was changed every week.
After analysing the post-test, the writing and speaking test scores showed
a positive correlation between repeated reading and improved speaking and
writing abilities. The pre-test mean scores for speaking and writing were 9.7
and 7.667 respectively; those of the post-test were 17.5 and 10.333. When
the scores for speaking and writing are compared, speaking scored the
highest. This could indicate reading aloud had a positive effect on the task
and, again, a receptive task had stimulated growth in productive skills.
If the excellent results from reading tasks have yet to persuade you to add
this method into your routine, then you should look into the other
advantages it can provide, such as building receptive vocabulary, realising
the most commonly used grammar, learning vocabulary by frequency of
occurrence, and in some instances developing certain aspects of productive
knowledge. The studies discussed have also shown strong support for
reading with audio; in these cases, the participants had increased their
listening skills.
Employing television shows to develop language skills not only boasts
amazing popularity, but it can also be seen as an effective method.
Unfortunately, the research demonstrated in this chapter showed it may
have a few flaws. The learner would need prior knowledge of vocabulary,
and it may require more encounters before acquiring vocabulary
knowledge.
To listen is to attend
Say no more
Downloading chunk
Mindless games
This book contains a total of fifty-seven studies. Now that we have gone
through all of them, what do they mean? If you do not use them, they can
mean nothing at all; or they can become invaluable tools towards your goal
of breaking the language barrier. The researchers have dedicated so much
time to understanding the psychology and acquisition of languages, and to
not exploit these results would be devastating. The idea that ‘correlation
does not imply causation’ is true in research; however, correlation is more
concrete than subjective influence.
This book was written to provide an objective viewpoint for learning a
language. As such, it is an attempt to search for answers that are close to the
truth. When the research is organised, a certain pattern is visible.
This chapter is an organised layout of the research with my individual
views on how learning could be ordered. I hope to use the information
demonstrated throughout this book to create a possible sequence. Whether
you find this intriguing or outrageous, the intended purpose is to create a
route for those that do not have one. You can either follow the sequence or
apply the previously discussed research into your personal routine. This
table suggests how the research should be split and followed according to the
learner’s stages.
Absolute beginner
At the absolute beginner stage, if the learner has yet to make contact with the
target language, they should spend time listening to audio in the target
language. This can be done while doing chores or any other activity, as long
as there is audio of the target language playing. At this stage, you are not
learning vocabulary or practising listening comprehension, but simply
building your understanding of the target language’s phonetics. This task
should be done at least two hours a day for four to seven days.
Beginner
Assuming you have either completed the absolute beginner stage or spent
time immersed in media in the target language, the beginner stage will begin
by practising shadowing. The main focus is to practise following along with
the audio and producing similar sounds. The task requires you to imitate
short beginner sentences as closely as possible for 15 minutes a day for
seven days.
Intermediate
The next stage is split into two sections, lower and upper intermediate, as
they have similar yet different objectives. The intermediate stage is
dedicated to developing receptive knowledge with comprehensible input.
Reading narrative dialogue texts will provide you with the most frequently
used 3000 spoken word-families as well as the most commonly used
grammar. It is different from the expository text and descriptive text, as these
contain words and grammar regularly found in textbooks. For example, in
English text, passive verbs are prevalent in written text but rare in spoken
language.
The aim of dividing this stage is to propose beginning with dialogues in
intensive reading before using repeated reading. Staying ahead of dialogues
before repeated reading will allow you to review grammar, read translations,
use word pair tests, and provide the necessary rest to decode difficult texts.
Producing sentences is not the purpose of this stage, but according to the
studies that have been discussed, you can expect these receptive tasks to
develop certain aspects of productive knowledge.
The lower intermediate stage introduces intensive reading to develop
receptive knowledge and the opportunity to review grammar. Other reasons
for intensive reading include learning vocabulary and grammar through the
frequency of occurrence and learning pronunciation. You may have noticed,
despite discussing shadowing in the previous chapters, it has not been placed
in the intermediate or advanced stages; this is to save time and incorporate
shadowing into the reading session.
The intensive reading will require two sessions for every dialogue. Session
one involves reading aloud and reviewing with translation for
comprehensible input. Following this task is a word-pair task, writing the
unknown L2 words or chunks in a column to prepare a word-pair test after
the second session. The second session would then involve reading aloud
and shadowing audio. After completing the two sessions, return to the L2 to
L1 receptive word-pair test and complete the list. The combination of
intentional and incidental tasks will support each other for vocabulary
retention.
The upper intermediate level will continue with intensive reading towards
more advanced dialogues or graded books. In addition to this sub-stage, a
read-aloud repeated reading task will be included for developing
comprehension and reading fluency. The task should include five repetitions,
with every second and third repetition employing assisted reading. Dialogues
should be repeated to turn meaning into language form. Repeated reading is
a persistent task that will cause fatigue, so I would suggest using around five
dialogues, especially for advanced dialogues.
Reading quickly will improve your reading fluency more than
comprehension, but the intermediate stage is aimed at developing
comprehension and learning the most common vocabulary so I recommend
learners take their time. The results will allow you to comfortably read
graded material in the advanced stage.
Because this sequence relies on reading to acquire vocabulary, it is natural to
look back and forth at translation, word lists, dictionaries as well as grammar
notes. This may appear exhausting, but the act of need and search, as stated
in the involvement load hypothesis, should assist in memorising the items.
It would be advisable to achieve receptive knowledge of 3000 word-families
using beginner to advanced dialogues before moving on to the advanced
stage. A reason this number was chosen is to provide 95% comprehension of
television shows and to achieve relative ease reading graded books, which
are tasks for the next stage. Sound easy? 3000 words is the suggested aim,
but encountering these words 10 or more times is the objective. Reaching
this number can be difficult as words increase and frequency decreases.
Relying on acquiring vocabulary through the frequency of occurrence would
be detrimental to the speed of learning. The addition of a receptive condition
word-pair test is not only to respect the results from earlier studies, which
demonstrated combination tasks are more effective than single tasks, but is
also added as a supplemental task to practice the low-frequency vocabulary.
Advanced
The final stage introduces more tasks to continue receptive vocabulary
growth and develop the aspects of productive knowledge for the 3000 word-
families. At this stage, your reading skills should be adequate for general
comprehension.
The reading task will now focus on extensive assisted reading in narrative
graded books, descriptive textbooks, and expository textbooks, starting from
beginner books and advancing to more difficult texts. It may be reasonable
to believe at this stage you will no longer need to start with beginner
textbooks; however, due to the difference between written text and spoken
text, new vocabulary and grammar will often appear. Another reason is to
allow more time to improve the productive knowledge of the 3000 words.
Unlike reading in the previous stage, extensive assisted reading has the
purpose of leisurely reading. There is no requirement to read aloud unless
you feel the need to practice shadowing. Incorporating assisted reading is to
provide listening comprehension development and understanding of the
phonetic system of languages that use logograms.
Two productive methods are advisable. First, you will need to return to the
dialogue narrative text used in the intermediate stages. Assuming you have
developed the receptive knowledge of the vocabulary used in these texts,
your next step is to increase their productive knowledge. This will also be a
valuable opportunity to improve the grammar found in these texts using
grammar drills.
Occasionally, a new word and/or grammar topic will appear in the advanced
stage. These words should not be practised with productive tasks, as our
focus is on the depth of the previous vocabulary in the intermediate stage;
instead, this stage will rely on watching television and reading for exposure
of low-frequency vocabulary.
The first method involves using the productive condition (L1-L2) word-pair
test for the 3000 words. Chunking can also be used to save time. It is highly
recommended to use short spaced repetition, 10-minute breaks, and to
practice the productive condition. After the word-pairing test, the next step is
to write a sentence with each word or use all the target words to write a
paragraph. While it is important to develop productive knowledge as soon as
possible, quality cannot be sacrificed. The most desirable condition is to
focus on five to ten words per session and allow adequate time for extensive
reading. Incorporating writing and reading out loud would be beneficial for
improving speaking fluency.
This writing task engages the learner to create sentences, who may also want
to find a teacher to correct their work. It is not a terrible problem if you
cannot find an authority to correct you, since making mistakes is common in
our daily lives, and the goal of writing sentences is not to memorise the
sentence but to install the ability to spontaneously create the target word.
Write what you want, as long as the target word is present. Other approaches
to sentence writing are either to recall and write the original sentence or to
use the original sentence with a few adjustments, such as replacing the
pronoun, noun, verb or adjective.
The second method supports software or mobile app usage to create a cloze
sentence: simply enter the sentence and replace the target word with a blank
space. This task can also be hand-written. In addition to using software or
mobile apps, long spaced repetition can be incorporated into the routine. The
advantage of using cloze is the opportunity to practice chunking. Set
phrases, idioms and collocations can be replaced with a blank space, as
opposed to single words. The discussion with exploiting the involvement
load hypothesis was to suggest the most profitable tasks, and the writing
tasks demonstrated superior scores to those of the cloze task which had only
scored 2 on the degree of involvement. To supplement the cloze task’s lack
of involvement, it would be beneficial to add an extra task: translation from
L1 to L2. Method two can offer study time during your commute to work or
school.
The previous studies have demonstrated that training in grammar chunks can
increase the complexity of your sentences and should also be a part of the
advanced routine. First, memorise an example sentence with the target
grammar chunk. This will be the base of your knowledge and will be used
for future reconstruction of similar sentences. The next task would either
involve finding a language partner for a question and answer task, replying
with sentences based on the grammar chunk, or writing a question and
answer with five different sentences using the same chunk while also
reading aloud. Employing long spaced repetition would also be a worthwhile
task, since the example sentence must be memorised. You can write the
grammar structure on one side of a flashcard and the sentence on the other
side. It is unnecessary to write a description of how the grammar structure
works, as we are aiming to achieve ‘less is more’.
Both methods only contain writing productive tasks, since many learners
find it difficult to speak. Following the results of the studies, it can be
presumed that writing can also lead to developing speaking skills. However,
intonation, tones, and speaking fluency will rely on shadowing during the
reading task for development. The idea of writing to develop speaking skills
is also my empirical viewpoint. After observing both introverted and
extroverted language learners, I have concluded that extroverts may find it
more useful to use real speaking practice. In this case, negotiation interaction
would provide an interesting opportunity for learners. Practice by
memorising target words before engaging in conversation, and try to use as
many of those words as possible during the conversation.
Although the productive tasks are important, it is also important to continue
reading and acquiring receptive vocabulary knowledge. In addition to
understanding grammar, the overall goal is to achieve knowledge of 6000
receptive vocabulary items and a depth of 3000 productive vocabulary items.
I have long been adamant that long spaced repetition should not be used for
single words, as the substantial investment only yields little return. Rather
than reviewing words through spaced repetition, it would be more
appropriate to invest time in reading and encountering the words through
context.
Finally, watching television can be either a task or a leisurely pastime. In the
case of using it as a task, then it should be left until the advanced stage. It is
important to mention that subtitles or captions are an effective method for
practising low-frequency vocabulary. It was stated previously that word
frequency decreases as the word list grows and that watching television with
subtitles and captions is a complementary task that should be used with
reading to counter the low frequency. It would be advisable to focus on one
television subgenre before moving on to another. Some general advice
would be to find a subgenre that has the least number of vocabulary items. If
the show is designed for children, a good recommendation would be to
watch with visual and audio to improve listening comprehension.
Even if you skip the absolute beginner and beginner stages, it is still
necessary to complete the intermediate stage, because these achievements
will provide building blocks for the advanced stage. As you may have
noticed, arranging the studies into these stages resembles how a baby begins
to acquire a language up until their early stages of school. This was not a
planned outcome, but as the sequence suggests, we may have to take a
similar direction as children.
This book was written for learners that enjoy self-studying, and it is thanks
to the researchers that new learning concepts and theories are appearing,
which in turn help us acquire a language more effectively.
Interested in more?
Visit www.psylingo.com/blog
Bibliography
Receptive and productive knowledge
[1] Clahsen, H., & Muysken, P. (1986) The availability of universal
grammar to adult and child learners – a study of the acquisition of German
word order, Volume 2 (2) 93-119.
[2] Nation, I.S.P. (2001) Learning Vocabulary in another Language.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
[3] Webb, S. (2008) Receptive and Productive Vocabulary Sizes of L2
Learners. Cambridge University Press. 79-95. DOI:
10.1017/S0262263108080042
[4] Shin, D, K., & Chon, Y, V., & Kim H, J. (2011) Receptive and
productive vocabulary sizes of high school learners: What next for the basic
word list? English Teaching, 66 (3) 123-148.
[5] Segbers, J., & Schroeder, S. (2017) How many words do children
know? A corpus-based estimation of children’s total vocabulary size.
Language Testing. Volume 34 (3) 297-320. DIO:
10.1177/0265532216641152.
[6] Uchihara, T., & Kazyua, Saito. (2016) Exploring the relationship
between productive vocabulary knowledge and second language oral
ability. The Language Learning Journal, ISSN 0957-1736.
[7] Namaziandost, E., & Esfahani, F, R. (2018) The impact of writing
practices on enhancing productive skills among pre-intermediate EFL
learners. Asean Journal of Teaching and Learning in Higher Education.
Volume 10, No. 1. ISSN: 1985-5826.
Repeated reading
[1] Webb, S., & Chang, A, C, S. (2012) Vocabulary Learning through
Assisted and Unassisted Repeated Reading. The Canadian Modern
Language Review. 68 (3). 267-290. DOI:10.3138/cmlr.1204.1.
[2] Taguchi, E., Takayasu-Maass, M., & Gorsuch, G. (2004) Developing
reading Fluency in EFL: How assisted repeated reading and extensive
reading affect fluency development. Reading in a foreign Language.
Volume 16 (2). 70- 96. ISBN 1539-0578
[3] Gorsuch, G., & Taguchi, E. (2008) Repeated reading for developing
reading fluency and reading comprehension: The case of EFL learners in
Vietnam. Elsevier Ltd. 36. 253-278. DOI:10.1016/j.system.2007.09.009.
[4] Chang, A, C, S., & Millet, S. (2013) Improving reading rates and
comprehension through timed repeated reading. Reading in a Foreign
Language. Volume 25 (2). 126-148. ISSN 1539-0578
[5] Kim, S. (2017) The effect of Repeated Reading on Speaking and
Writing of Adult EFL Learners. Studies in English Language & Literature.
Volume 43, 4. 205-229. http://dx.doi.or/10.21559/aellk.2017.43.4.11.
Word pairing
[1] Webb, S. (2009) The Effects of Receptive and Productive learning for
Word Pairs on Vocabulary Knowledge. RELC Journal. Volume 40 (3). 360-
376. DOI: 10.117/0033688209343854.
[2] Erten, I, H., & Tekin, M. (2008) Effects on vocabulary. Acquisition of
presenting new words in semantic sets versus semantically unrelated sets.
Elsevier Ltd. 36. 407-422. DOI:10.1016/j.system.2008.02.005.
To listen is to attend
[1] Kurkel, J., & Hämäläinen, J., & Leppänen, P, H, T., & Shu, H., &
Astikainen, P. (2019) Passive exposure to speech sounds modifies change
detection brain responses in adults. Neurolmage. 188. 208-216.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.12.010.
[2] Bozorgian, H. (2012) The Relationship between Listening and Other
Language Skills in International English Language Testing System. Theory
and Practice in Language Studies. Vol. 2. No. 4. 657-663.
doi:10.4304/tpls.2.4.657-663.
Say no more
[1] Koizumi, R., & In’nami, Y. (2013) Vocabulary Knowledge and
Speaking Proficiency among Second Language Learners from Novice to
Intermediate Levels. Journal of Language Teaching and Research. Vol. 4,
No. 5, 900-913. DOI: 10.4304/jltr.4.5.900-913.
[2] Hsieh, K, T., & Dong, D, H., & Wang, L, Y. (2013) A PRELIMINARY
STUDY OF APPLYING SHADOWING TECHNIQUE TO ENGLISH
INTONATION INSTRUNCTION. Taiwan Journal of Linguistics. Vol 11, 2.
43-66. DOI: 10.6519/TJL.2013.11(2).2.
[3] Nashta, F, J., & Rahimy, R. (2018) An Investigation of the Effectiveness
of Dialogue Shadowing Technique (DST) on Iranian Intermediate EFL
Learners’ Conversation Ability. International Journal of Research in
English Education. Volume 3, Number 2. 35-47.
[4] Yavari, F., & Shafiee, S. (2019) Effects of Shadowing and Tracking on
Intermediate EFL Learners’ Oral Fluency. Internation Journal of
Instruction. Volume 12 (1) 869-884.
[5] Shahraki, S, H., & Kassaian, Z. (2011) Effects of learner interaction,
receptive and productive learning on vocabulary acquisition: An Iranian
case. Elsevier Ltd. Procedia – Social and Behavioural Sciences. Volume 14.
2165-2171. DOI: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.04.073.
Downloading chunk
[1] Miller, G, A (1956) The magical number seven, plus or minus two:
Some limits on our capacity for processing information. The Psychological
Review, 63 (2) 81-97.
[2] Ellis, N, C. (2001) Construnction, chunking and connectionism: The
emergence of second language structure. Handbook of Second Language
acquisition. Oxford: Blackwell.
[3] Hebb, D, O. (1961) Disctinctive deatures of learning in the higher
animal. In J. F. Delafresnaye (Ed.) Brain mechanisms and learning. 37-46.
Oxford: Blackwell
[4] Xu, F. (2016) Short-term Working Memory and Chunking in SLA.
Theory and Practice in Language Studies. Vol 6, No 1. PP 119-126.
DOI:http://dx.doi.ord/10.17507/tpls.0601.16.
[5] Taguchi, N. (2008) Building Language Blocks in L2 Japanese: Chunk
Learning and the Development of Complexity and Fluency in Spoken
Production. Foreign Language Annals. Volume 41 (1) 130-154
[6] Karpicke, J, D., & Blunt, J, R. (2011) Retrieval Practice Produces More
Learning than Elaborative Studying with Concept Mapping. Science.
Volume 331. 772-774. DOI:10.1126/science.1199327.
[7] Reder, L, M., & Liu, X, N, L., & Keinath, A., & Popov, V. (2016)
Building knowledge requires bricks, not sand: The critical role of familiar
constituents in learning. Psychonomic Society, inc. 23. 271-277. DOI:
10.3758/s13423-015-0998-1.
Mindless games
[1] Karpicke, J, D., & Bauernschmidt, A. (2011) Spaced Retrieval:
Absolute Spacing Enhances Learning Regardless of Relative Spacing.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition.
Volume 37 (5) 1250-1257.
[2] Kelley, P., & Whatson, Terry. (2013) Making long-term memories in
minutes: a spaced learning pattern from memory research in education.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. Volume 7 Article 589. 1-9. DOI:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00589.
[3] Wagner, U., Gais, S., & Haider, H., & Verleger, R., & Born, J. (2004)
Sleep inspires insight. Nature Publishing Group. Volume 427. 352-354.
[4] Carpenter, S, K., & Mueller, F, E. (2013) The effects of interleaving
versus blocking on foreign language pronunciation learning. Mem cogn. 41.
671-682. DOI: 10.3758/s13421-012-0291-4.
Table of Contents
Receptive and productive knowledge
Second language acquisition
Intentional and incidental language learning
Involvement Load Hypothesis
Reading for language acquisition
Intensive reading and extensive reading
Repeated Reading
Language acquisition and television shows
Word pair learning
To listen is to attend
Say no more
Downloading chunk
Mindless games
Breaking the language barrier
Bibliography