100% found this document useful (1 vote)
79 views11 pages

Grasping The Nettle The Importance of Pe

The document discusses the challenges learners face in listening comprehension, particularly with fast spontaneous speech, and identifies four main problems in current teaching methodologies. It proposes solutions including engaging in classroom research, enhancing teacher training to better address the features of fast speech, and emphasizing the importance of the post-listening phase to improve learners' perception skills. The author argues that teachers should prepare students for the challenges of real-life listening situations rather than relying solely on easier, scripted materials.

Uploaded by

Thutra Dinh
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
100% found this document useful (1 vote)
79 views11 pages

Grasping The Nettle The Importance of Pe

The document discusses the challenges learners face in listening comprehension, particularly with fast spontaneous speech, and identifies four main problems in current teaching methodologies. It proposes solutions including engaging in classroom research, enhancing teacher training to better address the features of fast speech, and emphasizing the importance of the post-listening phase to improve learners' perception skills. The author argues that teachers should prepare students for the challenges of real-life listening situations rather than relying solely on easier, scripted materials.

Uploaded by

Thutra Dinh
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 11

Grasping the nettle: The importance of perception work

in listening comprehension

Richard T. Cauldwell
English for International Students Unit
Department of English
The University of Birmingham
Edgbaston
Birmingham
B15 2TT

[email protected]
5th November 2000

1
Abstract

A common complaint from learners on first visiting an English-speaking


country is that their listening skills cannot cope with fast spontaneous
speech. Four inadequacies in the teaching of listening lead to this
complaint: we rely too much on first language research findings; we
neglect perception; we give learners easy and enjoyable, rather than
challenging tasks; we use listening activities to serve other language-
learning goals. I propose four things: that teachers themselves engage in
classroom research in second language listening; that teachers should be
provided with the skills of observing and explaining the features of fast
speech; that teachers should be prepared for students to be challenged
(even frustrated) in the early parts of a listening lesson; that the post-
listening phase should be expanded to include aural and oral ‘handling’
of crucial fast extracts from recordings to improve students’ perception
skills. [140 words]

2
Introduction

Learners, teachers, teacher trainers and university researchers have been


stung by casual contact with the nettle of fast spontaneous speech, and
have tended to avoid further contact. The legacy of this avoidance
includes four problems for the effective teaching of listening. I shall first
describe the four problems; I then suggest ways in which we can improve
our teaching of listening. In doing so, I shall make reference to the
standard listening comprehension class, with four phases: warm-up,
while listening, post-listening, and follow-up.
Problems

Listening comprehension methodology of the last two decades has been


characterised by systematic avoidance of the painful fact that fast
spontaneous speech is difficult for learners. We avoid confronting this
fact in four ways: we place too much faith in first language research; we
rely on, but refuse to develop, learners’ perception skills; we focus on
what learners can manage, rather than on what they have to master; and
we favour follow-up activities such as discussions and writing tasks
rather than teaching listening.
Problem 1: Too much faith in first language research

Twelve years ago, Anderson and Lynch (1988: 21) noted that there was
very little research into listening in a second language. Because of this
gap in research, applied linguists, textbook writers, and teacher trainers
have gone to research in first language listening for guidance. As a result,
listening comprehension exercises are greatly (and in my view
inappropriately) influenced by what is known about successful first
language listening.

First language research has established that successful listening is


characterised by:
• listening for a purpose
• making predictions based on contextual information
• making guesses when things aren't clear
• inferring what is meant where necessary
• not listening ('straining') for every word
(adapted from Brown 1990: 148)

Teacher trainers and textbook writers have made appropriate use of some
of these findings, and inappropriate use of others. In particular they have
3
taken the last of these points ('they don't listen for every word') and have
made it an article of faith. They advocate 'top-down' activities and urge
the avoidance of any activity which could be characterised as 'bottom-
up'. Of course, we should be careful about this particular issue: we don't
want learners to strain so much to hear every word that they cannot
understand anything. In my view though, it is a mistake to abandon, as
we have, bottom-up activities which introduce learners to the essential
characteristics of speech.

From first language research comes the teacher’s standard advice in a


listening lesson: 'You won't be able to understand every word, and you
don't need to'. I find this explanation illogical: the ‘reasoning’ goes
something like this:
1. non-natives don't understand
2. natives understand without paying attention to every word
3. therefore, in order to understand, non-natives should not try to pay attention
to every word

The first statement describes the problem which all listening classes
address in some way; the second is a research finding; the third is the
false deduction. It is not reasonable to deduce from the first two
statements that ‘improvement in listening skills follows from not trying
to pay attention to every word’. In acting (as we do) on this illogical
deduction, we confuse goals and methodology: we require learners to
simulate the goal of native listener behaviour instead of teaching learners
how to acquire progressively native-like abilities in perception and
understanding. We have made the mistake of allowing the goal to
become the method: we should recognise that the skill of understanding
without attending to every word is a goal to be reached, not a means of
getting there.

Adopting the goal-as-method procedure conveniently allows us to ignore


the fact that native speaker listeners have great advantages over non-
natives particularly in terms of perceptual ability, it allows us to avoid
grasping the nettle of fast speech. Activities which encourage bottom-up
processing, which target learners perceptual abilities, have become taboo.
Problem 2: Too much hope in listening out for ‘stresses’

Listening exercises are also characterised by the hope which often


appears in the following words of encouragement: 'Just listen to the
stresses, they'll be in the most important words, then you'll understand'.

4
There are three problems with this view: first, very often, 'important'
words such as negatives are often unstressed, and so-called 'unimportant'
grammatical words such as prepositions and pronouns are stressed;
second, research indicates that it is difficult to pick out stressed words in
a language which is not your own (c.f. Roach, 1982); third, the concept
of stress is loosely defined and fails to distinguish between word-level
stress, and stresses associated with higher order phenomena such as tone
units.
Problem 3: Too much help

Although many listening comprehension recordings boast that they are


'natural', few of them are truly so. Many (though not all) are scripted and
artificially slow. The reasons for this can be found in statements such as
the following from Penny Ur:
Students may learn best from listening to speech which, while not entirely
authentic, is an approximation to the real thing, and is planned to take into
account the learners' level of ability and particular difficulties. (Ur, 1984: 23)

I myself find nothing wrong in what Penny Ur says here but I would
argue that listening comprehension materials are often over-charitable in
leaning towards 'the learners' level of ability' and not taking account of
the level of ability required to understand spontaneous fast speech. The
gap between the learners' level and the target level (fast spontaneous
speech) is a gap that we as teachers and materials writers must help
learners bridge. But we cannot help them bridge this gap if we continue
with our charitable focus on what learners can manage at their current
level.

In recent years, listening materials in main course textbooks at upper-


intermediate and advanced levels have featured spontaneous speech, and
this move is a good one. However, the methodology (crudely, give the
answers, and move on) has remained much the same, and teachers are not
trained to explain what the features of fast spontaneous speech are.

We have to help learners cope with speech which is above their current
level, and to arrive at a description of 'above current level', we need a
description of the topmost level - a description of the features of
'difficult' (fast spontaneous) speech. We need such a description for use
in teaching so that we can have an equal focus on both where our
learners are, and where they have to get to: this description should form
part of teacher training – it should be part of every teacher’s tool-kit.

5
Problem 4: Rushing to the follow-up

We offer too little help in the post-listening phase. My impression is (and


this is backed up by research by Field 1998) that of the four phases of a
listening lesson it is the post-listening phase which has the least amount
of time devoted to it. The first – warm-up – phase (with contextualisation
and personalisation) and the fourth – follow-up – phase (often a
discussion or writing task) have the most time devoted to them. It is at
this point that avoidance is at its most obvious worst, and the reasons for
it can be found in the standard training of communicative language
teachers.

Our training predisposes us to obey a communicative imperative which


demands rapid movement to the next activity to keep the variety, interest,
and motivation high: we are anxious to see and hear learners enjoying
social interaction in English. We prefer this high level of social ‘buzz’ to
staying with and helping learners through the difficulties of a recording:
when there might be silent private struggles to perceive and understand
the acoustic blur of speech.
Suggestions

In the description of problems a number of themes emerged: research


into L2 listening, teacher training, grasping the nettle, and methodology.
Suggestion 1: Research into L2 listening in the classroom

Fry1 (personal communication) advises, where circumstances permit,


allowing learners to control the tape-recorder so that they can work on,
and re-hear, those passages of the recording that they have problems
with. Fry’s experience is with classes of adult learners of English: he
divides the class up into small groups and, after having done the warm up
phase and set the listening task, he gives each group a tape-recorder, and
the tape, and leaves it to the group to control the tape-recorder. He
reports being very surprised at what they found easy, and what they find
difficult in listening.

My experience of working with learners with computer controlled access


to recordings (reported in part in Cauldwell, 1996) is also one in which I
learned a great deal about their powers and weaknesses in perception and

1
John Fry, of The British Council, Hong Kong

6
understanding. It brought home to me the fact that their difficulties lay in
what were for me ‘surprising’ places.

So there are two benefits to allowing learners to control of the tape


recorder: they can focus on their own needs; and for the teachers it
amounts to research into second language listening – teachers discover
where gaps in understanding and perception lie.
Suggestion 2: A fast speech phonology

Teachers should be trained in ‘observing’ speech, and particularly the


authentic speech that now is a feature of many listening comprehension
and general textbooks. This training does not currently take place. The
training they get is in the area of fixed position phonology for the
teaching of pronunciation. This training is typically concerned with the
articulation of minimal pairs of consonants and vowels so that teachers
can explain to learners how they can improve their pronunciation.

But these current approaches to ‘phonology for pronunciation’ do not


give adequate preparation for dealing with the features of authentic fast
speech, not even in the areas where they might be thought to do so:
elision, assimilation, sentence stress, and intonation. The ‘rules of
speech’ presented in such materials are derived from introspection
concerning how decontextualised written sentences might be read aloud.
These ‘rules of speech’ are inadequate to account for what happens in
fast spontaneous speech.

There is therefore a need for a ‘fast speech phonology’ which prepares


teachers to observe and explain the variability of fast speech. A major
element of this training would be to encourage teachers to rid their minds
of the expectations and rules they have inherited from fixed position
phonology. As for what else might be included, Field (1998: 13) suggests
features such as 'hesitations, stuttering, false starts, and long, loosely
structured sentences'. To this list one can add all the features of speech
described in Brazil (1994; 1997) - prominences, tone units of different
sizes, tones, pitch height. One can also add the differences between
citation and running forms of words, turn taking, accent, voice quality,
and the effects of speed on speech.
Suggestion 3: Grasping the nettle

Learners will claim that fast speech is too difficult for them: and teachers
will naturally feel tempted to give them easier, slower, scripted materials
that they feel comfortable with. If this solution is adopted however,

7
learners will be under-prepared to cope with the fast spontaneous speech
that will come their way when they meet native speakers of English.

It is necessary to allow learners to feel challenged, and it may be


necessary for them to feel frustrated by the demands of the listening task.
I took a survey of one class of seven advanced learners of English
(teachers of English from Japan) at the moment when they were deeply
immersed in a difficult recording, and attempting to answer questions
relating to the recording. I asked them to score their feelings on a five
point scale with ‘A’ as ‘happy’ and ‘E’ ‘unhappy’. Some time later, after
doing the post-listening exercises I asked them to make judgments on the
same scale. The results are shown in Table 1.
Table 1 Survey of learners’ feelings before and after post-listening
activities
Happy Unhappy
A B C D E
Before 0 0 1 1 5
After 3 3 1 0 0
Table 1 shows that there was a major shift in feeling between the end of
the while listening phase and the end of the post-listening phase: learners
moved from being broadly ‘unhappy’ to broadly ‘happy’. (The means by
which this change was brought about will be described in the next
section.) Here, it is important to note that it is vital for teachers to be
prepared for periods of learner frustration, and to have the
methodological training and knowledge base to help learners through
periods of discomfort and frustration to increasingly sophisticated levels
of perception and understanding. If the goal is to help learners become
better listeners, it is vital that they learn to be comfortable handling fast
speech..
Suggestion 4: The Post-Listening phase: the importance of handling speech

What is involved in ‘handling’ fast speech? When we invite learners to


do a reading task, we ask them to inspect sequences of words of varying
sizes (paragraphs, clauses, phrases) for evidence to help them complete
the tasks we have set. The same should be true for listening tasks: we
should ask learners to inspect sequences of words (in speech units of
different sizes) for answers to the tasks. However, there are important
differences between reading and listening tasks: with the written
language perception is not an issue, the words occur and remain for
inspection on the page; with the spoken language the words are not
available for inspection in the same way, they are available only for
8
inspection in the short-term memory of the learners, and here perception
is an issue. Perception – particularly the ability to hold sounds in short
term memory long enough to inspect them for meaning – is a skill that is
a pre-requisite for understanding.

One feature of any post-listening phase, therefore, is to give learners the


experience of handling sequences of speech while inspecting them for
clues to understanding. It is therefore necessary for the learners to re-hear
and spend time (this may be private, or in discussion with a partner what
they hear) with the crucial answer-bearing moments of a recording, and
this must be done before the learners see the written transcript, so that the
ears are doing the work, not the eyes.

It is vital therefore that the points chosen to be the focus of the listening
task should be both central to the ‘meaning’ of the recording, and
challenging in terms of perception. One way of doing this is to select
those parts of the recording which are both using software such as
‘Motormouth’ (Cauldwell & Batchelor, 1999) and ‘meaningful’.

At some stage (after an appropriate amount of ‘ear-handling’) learners


should see the written transcript so that they can get feedback on the
accuracy or waywardness of their perceptions. This is the point in the
listening class when we have the opportunity of actually teaching
listening (which Field 1998 argues for): we can help the learners bridge
the gap between the known and the unknown, but paradoxically it is the
part of a listening comprehension class that is most often omitted, or to
which least time is devoted.

Then comes the second vital stage in handling speech, the one that made
my learners turn from being ‘unhappy’ to being ‘happy’. This stage
involves the learners imitating short, fast, challenging extracts of the
recording at the same time and the same speed as the speaker. The
teacher chooses an extract and first asks learners to look at a written
version and to say it repeatedly to themselves, gradually increasing the
speed at which they say it. The teacher then plays the selected extract
repeatedly (by skilful use of the rewind button) and the learners try to
imitate as accurately as possible the features of the original.

Such extracts should not be long: the longest sequence of words I use for
such work lasts just over two seconds and is spoken at 408 words per
minute, with two prominent syllables in the places indicated by upper-
case letters:
this is ONE i’m going to be looking at in slightly more DEtail in fact
9
My (advanced level) learners find it an exciting challenge to handle
speech in this way, to be able to match native speaker speeds, and I
believe it is important to give learners at all levels practice of handling
fast speech in the two ways outlined in this section: handling by ear –
repeated listening to the fastest meaning-bearing extracts; and handling
by speaking – imitating the features of the fastest extracts. It is important
to refrain from looking at written versions of the extract too early (ear-
handling should precede eye-handling), but it is equally important to
inspect written versions of the extracts at some stage.
Conclusion.

There was a time when listening comprehension did involve perception


exercises (Field 1998), but they have generally disappeared, a fact that
Brown describes as 'a quite extraordinary case of throwing the baby out
with the bath water' (1990: 145). The emphasis in recent years has been
to view listening as an activity which serves other goals. For example
White (1987, cited in Anderson & Lynch 1988: 66) found teachers
valued listening materials for reasons such as ‘good for starting
discussions’, ‘amusing’ and ‘consolidates language’. Nowhere in White’s
list of reasons is there recognition of the characteristics unique to fast
speech, or of the necessity that listening activities should have listening
goals.

The major suggestions therefore, are that we should expand the post-
listening phase, and we should abandon the follow-up phase where this
takes the focus away from improving listening. In order to do this we
need to provide teachers with the skills of observing and explaining the
features of fast speech, and provide them with a methodology which
helps their learners become comfortable handling and understanding fast
speech. Our ignorance of the features of fast speech, our confusion of
goals with methodology, have resulted in our avoiding the nettle of fast
speech. We need to be bold and grasp this nettle to help our learners
become better listeners.
References
Anderson, A. and Lynch, T. (1988) Listening. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Brown, G. (1990) Listening to Spoken English. [Second Edition]. Harlow: Longman.
Brazil, D. (1994) Pronunciation for Advanced Learners of English. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Brazil, D. (1997) The Communicative Value of Intonation in English. [Second
Edition]. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cauldwell, R.T. (1996) ‘Direct Encounters with Fast Speech on CD Audio to Teach
Listening’. System 24/4: 521-528

10
Cauldwell, R.T. and Batchelor, T. (1999) ‘Mr. Motormouth’. [Multimedia Toolbook
software under development]. Birmingham: The University of Birmingham.
Field, J. (1998) ‘The Changing Face of Listening’. English Teaching Professional 6:
12-14.
Roach, P. (1982) On the Distinction between ‘Stress-timed’ and ‘Syllable-timed’
languages. In D. Crystal (ed.) Linguistic controversies, Essays in linguistic
theory and practice. (73-79). London: Edward Arnold.
Ur, P. (1984) Teaching Listening Comprehension. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
White, G. (1987) ‘The Teaching of Listening Comprehension to Learners of English
as a Foreign Language: A Survey’. Unpublished M. Litt. Dissertation, The
University of Edinburgh.

11

You might also like