Schreibman Ingersoll
Schreibman Ingersoll
The application of behavioral techniques to autism, like the diagnosis itself, has a
relatively short history. However the progress in the development and refinement of
behavioral treatment has been swift and impressive. Prior to the mid 1960’s,
psychodynamic therapy was standard in the treatment of children with autism, reflecting
the now antiquated theory that the cause of autism was psychogenic. The monumental
failure of the psychodynamic approach in the treatment of autism opened the door for the
emerging field of applied behavior analysis. Applied behavior analysis grew out of the
field of the experimental analysis of behavior, in which the general laws of learning
derived from work with animal populations were applied to socially significant
behaviors.
Behavioral procedures do not cure the disorder, but they have been shown to be
extremely effective in substantially improving the lives of people with autism and those
around them. The objectives of this chapter are: to describe current behavioral
intervention techniques for the treatment of autism and to discuss recent trends and
Current behavioral interventions can trace their roots to studies conducted in the
early 1960’s. Prior to this time, it was commonly believed that children with autism
could not learn. Ferster and DeMyer (1961; 1962) were among the first to demonstrate
that children with autism could indeed learn and would do so if the systematic application
these children could learn new, albeit non-functional, behaviors under conditions where
2
correct answers were followed by contingent applications of reinforcement. Later, other
These early studies were mostly concerned with addressing isolated behaviors.
Lovaas and his colleagues (e.g., Lovaas, 1977; Lovaas, Berberich, Perloff, & Schaeffer,
1966; Lovaas, Freitag, Gold, & Kassorla, 1965; Lovaas, Koegel, Simmons, & Long,
interventions that addressed a wide range of behaviors in children with autism. Such
in language, social, play, and academic skills. As the field of applied behavior analysis
has evolved, so too have the intervention techniques designed for use with these children.
training, are widely used in early intervention programs for children with autism. These
procedures, described by Lovaas (1987), are very similar to the original operant
techniques described in the 1960’s. More recent elaborations have included increased
focus on non-academic skills such as play and peer interaction, decreased use of
aversives, and more varied delivery of reinforcement. Programs using this procedure
share the following basic components: 1) the learning environment is highly structured;
3
2) target behaviors are broken down into a series of discrete sub-skills, and taught
successively; 3) teaching episodes are initiated by the adult; 4) teaching materials are
selected by the adult and rarely varied within a task; 5) the child’s production of the
unrelated to the target response; and 7) the child receives reinforcement only for correct
Discrete trial training has been credited with success in teaching children a variety
of important behaviors (e.g., Baer, Peterson, & Sherman, 1967; Lovaas, Berberich,
Perloff, & Schaeffer, 1966; Metz, 1965; Schroeder & Baer, 1972). In addition, this
approach has been credited with impressive gains in children with otherwise poor
prognoses (e.g., Lovaas, 1987) and in accelerated skill acquisition (Miranda-Linne &
Melin, 1992). One oft cited study found that 47 % of preschool-aged children with
autism who had received intensive (40 hours per week) discrete trial intervention for two
received less intensive intervention (Lovaas, 1987). Although this study has been
criticized for several methodological flaws (e.g., lack of random assignment, selecting
Despite these impressive findings, the highly structured behavioral approach has
been criticized on a number of grounds. First, the adult-directed nature of the instruction
and the fact that the target behavior is brought under tight stimulus control has been
shown to compromise the spontaneous use of the behavior (Carr, 1981). Second, the
highly structured teaching environment (Lovaas, 1977) and use of artificial reinforcers
4
(Koegel, O'Dell, & Koegel, 1987) has been shown to prevent generalization to the natural
environment (e.g., Spradlin & Siegel, 1982). Finally, the highly structured teaching
Structured operant teaching techniques have been modified over the years to
address some of the shortcomings noted above. The resulting behavioral interventions
are more naturalistic and child-centered. The first naturalistic behavioral treatment was
designed by Hart and Risley (1968) to teach the use of descriptive adjectives to
generalization and spontaneous use of skills by teaching them in the context of ongoing
classroom activities.
intervention techniques, including incidental teaching (Hart & Risley, 1968; McGee,
Krantz, Mason, & McClannahan, 1983), mand-model (Rogers-Warren & Warren, 1980),
time delay (Halle, Marshall, & Spradlin, 1979), milieu teaching (Alpert & Kaiser, 1992),
interrupted behavior chains (Hunt & Goetz, 1988), and the natural language
paradigm/pivotal response training (PRT) (Koegel, O’Dell, & Koegel, 1987; Koegel,
Schreibman, Good, Cerniglia, Murphy, & Koegel, 1989). Although the specific
academic backgrounds, these approaches are similar in that they all share the following
5
within ongoing interactions between the child and the adult; 3) the child initiates the
selected by the child and varied often; 5) the child’s production of the target behavior is
explicitly prompted; 6) a direct relationship exists between the child’s response and the
reinforcer; 7) the child is reinforced for attempts to respond (Delprato, 2001; Kaiser,
mand-model, and time delay are all specific prompting procedures for increasing
language that are implemented once the child has expressed interest in an item or activity
(Miranda & Iacono, 1988). In the incidental teaching approach, the adult waits for the
child to initiate a request for a desired item or activity. The adult then prompts an
elaborated response. The mand-model approach was developed for children who do not
readily initiate (Rogers-Warren & Warren, 1980). In this procedure, the adult waits for
the child to indicate interest in an item and then places an instruction for a particular
behavior. If the child does not respond, the adult models the correct response for the
child to imitate. The time delay procedure was designed to transfer the child’s response
from a verbal cue given by the adult to the environment (Halle et al., 1979). In this
procedure, the adult waits for the child to indicate interest in an item and then approaches
the child with an expectant look. If the child does not respond within 15 seconds, the
adult uses a model or mand-model procedure. For all of these techniques, reinforcement
is the delivery of the desired item or activity. Milieu teaching includes all three of these
procedures as well as a direct model procedure in which the adult models language and
6
this child is expected to imitate the adult’s model to receive access to a desired item or
context for requesting items or assistance. This procedure is typically used with children
who have very limited initiations and are minimally motivated to communicate (Hunt &
Goetz, 1988). The child is allowed to begin an activity and then the adult prevents the
child from completing the next step of the routine. The adult then uses mand-model or
time delay to prompt the child to request the next step in the sequence. In this procedure,
reinforcement is the ability to complete the next step in the behavior chain. This
procedure differs from milieu teaching in that the instruction is presented after the child
begins the activity rather than before (Miranda & Iacono, 1988).
response-reinforcer relationship. PRT does not define the specific types of prompts to
use; however, implementation of the procedure usually involves the same prompting
strategies as those used in milieu teaching and interrupted behavior chains. In contrast to
the other procedures which have focused almost exclusively on increasing verbal and
non-verbal communication, PRT has been adapted to teach a variety of skills including
symbolic (Stahmer, 1995) and sociodramatic play (Thorpe, Stahmer, & Schreibman,
validated the position that naturalistic strategies lead to more generalized and
7
spontaneous use of skills (Charlop-Christy & Carpenter, 2000; Delprato, 2001; McGee,
Krantz, & McClannahan, 1985; Miranda-Linne & Melin, 1992). In addition, studies have
found that parents who have been trained to implement these techniques exhibit more
positive affect while teaching their children (Schreibman et al., 1991) and both the
parents and children exhibit more happiness and interest and less stress during family
interactions (Koegel, Bimbela, & Schreibman, 1996) than families in which the parents
have been trained to implement highly structured behavioral techniques. These findings
suggest that the naturalistic approach is more enjoyable for parents and children and leads
One question that has not yet been answered is whether intensive naturalistic
interventions lead to similar or superior intellectual improvement as that which has been
often inclusion-based, and targets functional behaviors (e.g., McGee, Daly, & Jacobs,
1994), whereas intensive structured intervention is typically conducted in the home and
often targets more cognitive behaviors, thus confounding any sort of direct comparison.
principles have been utilized with children with autism and other severe communication
handicaps. These strategies include sign language (e.g., Carr, Binkoff, Kologinsky, &
8
Eddy, 1978), picture boards and picture point systems (Mirenda & Santogrossi, 1985;
Reichle & Brown, 1986), and picture exchange systems (Bondy & Frost, 1994).
Sign language has historically been the preferred method of treatment for non-
verbal children. However, sign language is symbolic and requires the ability to imitate;
therefore, many non-verbal children fail to acquire it. In addition, as the majority of the
population is unfamiliar with signs, children using this system are often unable to
children with autism who fail to acquire verbal or signed language. These systems (e.g.,
picture boards, picture exchange) seem to be easier to acquire than sign language
(Anderson, 2002) and are readily recognizable to individuals that have not been trained in
the system, thus increasing the number of people with whom the children can
most widely used iconic systems for non-verbal children (Bondy & Frost, 1994). This
system requires that the child exchange a picture for a desired item or activity. Although
the research on the effectiveness of PECS is limited, it has found wide acceptance in
technique is being conducted and findings suggest it is efficacious and that acquisition of
PECS leads to increases in vocal speech (Charlop-Christy, Carpenter, Le, LeBlanc, &
Kellet, 2002).
is most effective for increasing communication skills in non-verbal children with autism.
One study compared acquisition rates of modified signs and PECS pictures in non-verbal
9
preschoolers with autism. Six children were trained to request desired items via each
system. Results indicated that all six participants acquired pictures more rapidly and
demonstrated better generalization to novel stimuli with PECS than with sign. However,
the children had more spontaneous initiations and were more likely to pair verbalizations
with sign (Anderson, 2002). In addition, this study found that children indicated a clear
preference for one system over another, suggesting the importance of individualizing
verbal, school-age children who have already shown the inability to acquire spoken
communication system for non-verbal toddler and preschool-age children who are just
beginning intervention (i.e., Bondy & Frost, 1994; McGee, Morrier, & Daly, 1999).
PECS, sign). Also we need to examine which of these systems may promote vocal
area of research.
Self-Management
Although many behavioral treatment strategies are effective at teaching new skills, the
10
presence of a treatment provider is usually necessary to maintain these behavioral gains.
procedures have been adapted for use with individuals with autism and developmental
delay. They typically involve teaching the individual to identify appropriate and
inappropriate behavior, record his or her own behavior using stickers or other material,
and to reward him or herself after exhibiting the appropriate behavior or refraining from
the inappropriate behavior. The presence of the therapist is gradually faded so that the
Eventually the self-management materials are also faded so that the individual is able to
demonstrate self control completely independently (Koegel, Koegel, & Parks, 1990).
individuals with autism. They have been used to decrease inappropriate behavior such as
self-stimulation (e.g., Mancina, Tankersly, Kamps, Kravits, & Parrett, 2000; Koegel &
Koegel, 1990) and perseverative play (Newman, Reinecke, & Meinberg, 2000) as well as
to increase appropriate behavior such as toy play (Stahmer & Schreibman, 1992), social
initiations (Koegel, Koegel, Hurley & Frea, 1992) and independent interactions with
11
limited or no language are less likely to benefit from this approach. Accordingly,
adaptations to the typical self-management procedure have been developed for use with
individual steps of a task in a book format. This procedure involves teaching the child to
complete the step associated with each picture, turn the page to view the next picture, and
reinforce him or herself after the task is complete. This strategy has been shown to
Video Instruction
individuals has emerged. Video technology has some intrinsic appeal as an instructional
tool for this population. First, although not well documented in the literature, people
have often suggested that children with autism are visual learners and typically excel in
treatment modalities that rely on visual stimuli (e.g., Campbell, Lison, Borsook, Hoover,
& Arnold, 1995; Charlop & Milstein, 1989; Schreibman, Whalen, & Stahmer, 2000;
Pierce & Schreibman, 1994). Second, motivation may be enhanced because most
children (including children with autism) typically enjoy watching videos. Third,
videotapes can be replayed repeatedly without any variation, which might enhance
learning in a population that benefits from predictability. Finally, the use of video does
Video modeling has been the most well researched form of video instruction for
children with autism. Modeling research has shown that these children can learn new
12
Schreibman, & Tryon, 1983). Video modeling presents target behaviors in video format
and has been shown to improve various skills in individuals with autism, including
conversational speech (Charlop & Milstein, 1989, Sherer, et al., 2001), verbal responding
(Buggey, Tombs, Gardener, & Cervetti, 1999), helping behaviors (Reeve, 2001),
purchasing skills (Haring, Kennedy, Adams, & Pitts-Conway, 1987) and daily living
skills (Shipley-Benamou, Lutzker, & Taubman, 2002). This medium has also been
play actions, duration of play, and play-related statements (Schwandt, Pieropan, Glesne,
In addition to modeling, other forms of video technology have recently been used
These include procedures such as priming (Schreibman, Whalen, & Stahmer, 2000),
Research examining the effect of the type of model used in video based
instruction has been equivocal. One study compared the effectiveness of using the target
model). This study found that although neither technique proved to be clearly superior,
some individual children responded better to one or the other (Sherer et al., 2001). In
contrast, a study comparing the use of video to in vivo instruction suggested that video
modeling promotes faster acquisition and better generalization of new behaviors than in
vivo modeling (Charlop-Christy, Le, & Freeman, 2000). In addition, these authors
argued that video modeling is more cost effective because it does not require the use of a
13
live model (Charlop-Christy et al., 2000). Although this is only a preliminary study, it
warranted.
One cautionary note related to the use of video instruction (and other similar
Just because a child may learn certain skills better with one type of intervention does not
mean that the intervention is better overall. One reason that children with autism may
learn well from video is because video instruction can eliminate the social demands that
in vivo instruction requires. However, like all children, children with autism are required
to function in a social world and must be able to learn from their social environment.
Therefore, it is suggested that the use of video instruction be balanced with the use of in
TREATMENTS
of behavioral technology as it is applied to people with autism. The earlier work in the
area has laid the foundation for the current state-of-the-art and for what will follow. The
fact that we now view some of our early efforts as relatively “simplistic” and are now
much more effective and efficient in providing treatment should in no way be considered
as criticism of these earlier efforts. Quite the contrary. We are not moving away from
our early efforts; rather we are moving beyond them. Simply put, we would not be where
14
we are now, if we have not been there first. Our future directions and challenges are
One of the continued challenges in the treatment of children with autism is the
generalization of treatment gains across environments and over time (e.g., Lovaas et al.,
1973). The use of naturalistic behavioral teaching techniques and training family
et al.,1985; Koegel, Schreibman, Britten, Burke, & O’Neill, 1982). Despite these efforts,
generalization remains an obstacle in the treatment of many children with autism. Future
studies which focus on improving the generalization and maintenance of skills in children
Individualization of Treatment
reflected in the creation of a child’s IEP (individualized education plan). In this sense it
is used to emphasize the need for individualized educational goals. However, the field is
just beginning to recognize that there is no one treatment best suited to meet these goals.
meaningless because no treatment can boast substantial success in more than 50-70% of
children. This variability in child response calls for the individualization of treatment
strategies. Treatment variables and how they interact with child, family, and service
provider characteristics are also crucial elements in the individualized treatment equation.
15
will be most effective and efficient with a particular child and a particular family
particular intervention is just beginning (e.g., Anderson, 2002; Ingersoll, Schreibman, &
Stahmer, 2001) but is hugely important. For example, one study has identified a
behavioral profile of children who are likely to respond well to PRT and a separate
profile of children who are likely to have a poor response to this treatment (Sherer &
Schreibman, in press). These findings can help determine which children are
appropriate candidates for PRT as well as provoke additional research to determine which
alternative treatment options are most appropriate for children who meet the profile of a
subsequent study also found that the profile predicted outcome for PRT but not for
another behavioral intervention, Discrete Trial Training, thus suggesting the profile is
indeed predictive of a specific treatment and not just response to treatment in general. As
a field it is important to continue to fine-tune this line of research, thus deriving the most
characteristics of the child’s caregivers. The field is just beginning to examine how
family variables interact with treatment effectiveness. The effects of ethnicity, culture,
marital status, parental attitudes, parental age, level of education, socioeconomic status,
and other factors all may affect how treatment is best delivered and the ultimate
effectiveness of the treatment. For example, research has shown that children of more
responsive and educated parents are more likely to benefit from prelinguistic milieu
16
teaching, while children of less responsive and educated mothers benefit more from
responsive small group instruction (Yoder & Warren, 1998). Also, training in self-
independence is important (Schreibman & Koegel, 1996). However, certain cultures may
not place a strong emphasis on child independence; thus, the parents may not choose to
use self-management with their child or may use it ineffectively. Another example is the
effect of stress. It has been shown that parents of children with autism report being under
high levels of stress (e.g., Koegel, Schreibman, Loos, & Derlich-Wilhelm, 1992).
Perhaps parents who are under a good deal of stress at a point in time would be poor
candidates to implement training with their child; a clinician may then be the treatment
provider of choice. At a later time, if the stress is reduced, these parents could perhaps
It is also important to recognize teacher variables that predict the ability to learn
and implement different intervention techniques. Given that the majority of intervention
for children with autism is provided by teachers and paraprofessionals who may not have
identify the best ways to teach them and discern which techniques they are likely to use.
service providers have come to recognize that multiple behavioral methodologies have a
place within a comprehensive treatment program. Although there are still strong
researchers and service providers have broadened their view of teaching methodology;
17
however, despite the incorporation of multiple behavioral methodologies, change is still
slow. Ideally we will develop a set of formulas that will allow us to evaluate a child and
based upon research results be able to prescribe the best treatment for this child at this
time. Continued assessment would then allow us to alter the child’s treatment regimen in
response to the child’s changing needs. We are still in the initial stages of being able to
match treatment procedures with specific children but continued research in this area will
Integration of Disciplines
learning and change behavior, the field has come to recognize that autism is a complex
disorder, the cause of which cannot be explained by learning theory alone. To this end,
the field has gravitated towards other disciplines to help in the development of more
(Hemmeter & Kaiser, 1994), Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching (Warren, Yoder, Gazdag &
Another recent trend has been the use of behavioral techniques to target deficits in
18
literature. Many such autistic deficits, such as reciprocal imitation (Ingersoll &
Schreibman, 2001; 2002), joint attention (Whalen & Schreibman, in press), symbolic
play (Stahmer, 1995), and theory of mind (Garfinkle, 2000) which were originally
autism. For example, Whalen (2001) used a behavioral methodology to teach young
children with autism to make joint attention initiations and found increases in language
despite the fact that it was not directly targeted. Similarly, Ingersoll (2003) found
increases in language, play, and joint attention after targeting reciprocal imitation.
framework is exciting, and can only enhance the effectiveness of our interventions.
Future behavioral research will need to draw on the rapidly developing field of
neuroscience. For example, more recent imaging techniques have confirmed the
behaviorally (Lovaas, Schreibman, Koegel, & Rehm, 1971), which are likely involved in
the development of a variety of autistic behaviors. In addition, recent work has suggested
that face processing is impaired in autism and may lead to some of the abnormal social
behavior observed in this population (Schultz et al., 2000). Newer behavioral research
focused on targeting deficits identified by the field of neuroscience may lead to exciting
new gains in child outcome. Additionally, imaging studies which examine functional
19
changes in neurological systems that are a result of behavioral therapy will help to
identify whether our treatment contributes to brain reorganization. Such studies are
currently underway.
As a field, we should also collaborate with treatment approaches that are not
considered behavioral, but nonetheless, share some common elements with behavioral
interventions. For example, the TEACCH model or structured teaching (Lord, Bristol, &
Schopler, 1993) uses many forms of visual supports, such as picture schedules, to help
individuals with autism navigate their world. Although the use of picture schedules is not
inherently behavioral, many behavioral interventions have incorporated their use. For
example, pictorial self-management combines the use of a picture schedule with self-
reinforcement. Similarly, social stories (Gray & Garand, 1993), which provide a brief
priming, which has been shown to be an effective behavioral strategy with children with
autism (Schreibman, Whalen, & Stahmer, 2000; Zanolli, Daggett, & Adams, 1996).
Another intervention approach commonly used with children with autism is the
developmental approach. This approach, which includes floor time (Greenspan &
Wieder, 1998), is focused on building emotional reciprocity. One key component to floor
time, following the child’s lead, is also central to naturalistic behavioral strategies.
Another central component, opening and closing circles of communication, shares many
interventions than naturalistic and structured behavioral interventions are to each other
20
(Prizant, Wetherby, & Rydell, 2000). Similarly, sensory integrative therapy is also
focused on following the child’s lead and gradually increasing the level of demands to
help the child increase his or her fine motor, play, language, and sensory processing skills
is the focus on data collection and validation Despite their common use and likely
benefit, most non-behavioral interventions have not been empirically validated leading
many behaviorists to doubt their efficacy. In recent years, the TEACCH model and
social stories have undergone empirical study (e.g, Norris & Datillo, 1999; Ozonoff &
Cathcart, 1998) while developmental and sensory integrative interventions have lagged
behind. Future research which examines the efficacy of these types of interventions and
their overlap with behaviorally-based interventions will undoubtedly improve both fields
specific behavioral techniques and general principles of applied behavior analysis among
21
professionals in other disciplines, as well as the public at large. In fact, some
professionals have challenged the use of applied behavior analysis for children with
autism because they are uncomfortable with the highly structured, adult-directed
techniques used in discrete trial training, without recognizing that discrete trial training is
only one of many behavioral techniques (e.g., Greenspan & Wieder, 1998). (In reality,
the term “applied behavior analysis” refers to a specific research methodology – not a
disciplines in a clear manner that can highlight the similarities between disciplines and
encourage collaboration.
exclusively by specialists in clinic settings, recognizing that children need to learn within
the context of their daily lives (e.g., as provided by naturalistic teaching strategies).
importance of family participation in treatment. Research has shown that parents can be
trained to implement behavioral procedures with their children with autism and that this
training leads to increases in a variety of skills (e.g., Alpert & Kaiser, 1992; Hemmeter &
Kaiser, 1994). In addition, research has shown that parent training leads to more durable
There has also been an increase in the use of siblings (Celiberti & Harris, 1993)
and peers as intervention agents (e.g., McGee, Almeida, Sulzer-Azaroff, & Feldman,
1992; Pierce & Schreibman, 1995; 1997). Research suggests that typical children as
22
young as two years old can be trained to use behavioral strategies with peers with autism
have been shown to increase children with autism’s social interaction (McGee et al.,
1992), language (Pierce & Schreibman, 1997), and joint attention skills (Ingersoll &
Given the variety of treatment providers and intervention settings in any one
receive intervention in multiple settings throughout their lifetime. Parents and teachers
may not be using the same techniques, which in some cases can lead to a breakdown in
behavior. For example, many non-verbal children in are on PECS in their classrooms;
however, many of their families are not familiar with the system, do not have the
materials in the home, or do not know which pictures their child can use. In this
situation, the lack of collaboration between the home and the school may lead to missed
Continuum of Services
Much of the early behavioral research was conducted with older children,
adolescents, and adults with autism and other severe disabilities. With the recognition of
the importance of early intervention, there has been a tremendous increase in research
devoted to treatments for young children with autism. This focus is necessary and
exciting has likely yielded better long-term outcomes for individuals with autism in
general. However, despite better outcomes with the provision of early intervention
23
services, the prognosis for adults with autism is still very poor. Therefore, it is necessary
that research on treatment options for older individuals with autism grow in step with the
Dissemination
Another extremely important issue is: How can behavioral researchers rapidly
disseminate our findings so that they are widely applied. Like most scientific disciplines,
parents, teachers, and other frontline treatment providers are unlikely to be among the
audience of the journals. We need to specifically target outlets that reach these
personnel. Thus, publication of our work in media outlets and the popular press might be
programs, largely because they have been described in excellent detail in several
commercially-available manuals (e.g., Leaf & McEachin, 1999; Maurice, Green, & Luce,
1996). Similarly, PECS, which also has a comprehensive training manual (Frost &
Bondy, 2000), has received wide-acceptance in school programs across the country.
Discrete Trial Training has enjoyed a level of success and advocacy far beyond its
all children with autism). Why is this the case? Probably because of the Lovaas (1987)
study reporting that 47% of very young children provided with 40 hours a week of this
treatment achieved “normal” functioning. Despite cautions about the methodology of the
study and the fact that the study participants might not be representative of the wide
24
range of children with autism, parents understandably applauded the treatment as a
potential “cure” and demanded the treatment for their children. (Interestingly, however,
few people seem to notice that 53% did NOT achieve normal functioning.) The Internet,
autism organizations, etc. all provided a ready vehicle for the dissemination of this very
hopeful information from the Lovaas study to parents so eager for an answer to their
received less widespread acceptance, and remain primarily within an academic setting.
Several possibilities for this difference in rate of dissemination arise. First, it is possible
that there are less well-known, well-detailed manuals for these techniques. Second, it is
possible that more naturalistic techniques, self-management, and video technology are
more complex and less user-friendly for families and educators without a background in
behavioral techniques. It is important for us to determine these barriers so that they can
be overcome.
and therapists providing intervention to individuals with autism in the community do not
opposed to those provided in academic research settings and research training procedures
25
CONCLUSIONS
interventions designed to treat the disorder. One of the strengths of the field of behavior
modification is the reliance on data collection to validate our procedures and monitor
progress. As our own field has grown, we have become more open to differing treatment
other fields. This expanding view of behavior analysis can only serve to improve
interventions for individuals with autism. It is arguably the case that behavioral
intervention is the most thoroughly studied treatment modality in the field. While one
can cite some methodological shortcomings in some of the studies (e.g., small Ns, lack of
random assignment) it is the case that these are endemic not only to research on
behavioral interventions but of autism treatment in general. The necessity for continued
research on effective treatments for this population is crucial and doubtless behavioral
interventions will be in the forefront of this research for some time to come.
26
References
of sign language and the picture exchange communication system. (Doctoral dissertation,
4269-B.
Buggey, T., Toombs, K., Gardener, P., & Cervetti, M. (1999). Training
Campbell, J. O., Lison, C. A., Borsook, T. K., & Hoover, J. A., & Arnold, P. H.
27
Carr, E.G. (1981). Sign language. In O.I. Lovaas, A. Ackerman, D. Alexander,
P. Firestone, M. Perkins, & A.L. Egel (Eds.). The me book: Teaching manual for
Carr, E. G., Binkoff, J. A., Kologinsky, E., & Eddy, M. (1978). Acquisition of
children with autism: A focus on skills to enhance play. Behavior Therapy, 24, 573-599.
conversational speech using video modeling. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 22,
275-285.
Charlop-Christy, M. H., Carpenter, M., Le, L., LeBlanc, L. A., & Kellet, K.
(2002). Using the picture exchange communication system (PECS) with children with
28
Charlop-Christy, M. H., & Carpenter, M. H. (2000). Modified incidental teaching
sessions: A procedure for parents to increase spontaneous speech in their children with
modeling with in vivo modeling for teaching children with autism. Journal of Autism &
Press, G. A., Murakami, J. W., Lincoln, A. J., James, H. E., & Saitoh, O. (1994). A new
Implications for brain function. (pp. 101-137). Hillsdale, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Inc.
intervention for young children with autism. Journal of Autism & Developmental
13, 312-345.
Ferster, C. B., & Demyer, M. K. (1962). A method for the experimental analysis
29
(PECS) Training Manual, 2nd Edition. Newark, DE. Pyramid Products, Inc.
young children with autism: A model for praxis in early childhood special education. U
Washington, US.
students with autism with accurate social information. Focus on Autistic Behavior, 8, 1-
10.
Greenspan, S. & Wieder, S. (1998). The child with special needs: Encouraging
Halle, J. W., Marshall, A. M., & Spradlin, J. E. (1979). Time delay: A technique
30
Hunt, P., & Goetz, L. (1988). Teaching spontaneous communication in natural
treatment approach: Effects on imitation, language, play, and social behaviors (Doctoral
imitation training on imitative and spontaneous play in children with autism. Poster
presented at the annual meeting for the International Meeting for Autism Research,
Orlando, FL
toddlers with autism: Effects of contingent imitation training. Paper presented at the
imitation in children with autism using naturalistic teaching strategies. Paper presented at
the annual meeting of the International Meeting for Autism Research, San Diego, CA.
Ingersoll, B., Schreibman, L., & Stahmer, A. (2001). Brief report: Differential
treatment outcomes for children with Autistic Spectrum Disorder based on level of peer
31
Kaiser, A. P., Ostrosky, M. M., & Alpert, C. L. (1992). Training teachers to use
Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 18, 188-199.
Kaiser, A. P., Yoder, P. J., & Keetz, A. (1992). Evaluating milieu teaching. In S.
F. Warren & J. E. Reichle (Eds.), Causes and effects in communication and language
Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Hurley, C., & Frea, W. D. (1992). Improving social
skills and disruptive behavior in children with autism through self-management. Journal
Koegel, R. L., Bimbela, A., & Schreibman, L. (1996). Collateral effects of parent
training on family interactions. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 26, 347-
359.
Koegel, R. L., Koegel, L. K., & Parks, D. R. (1990). How to teach self
management skills to people with severe disabilities: A training manual. Santa Barbara:
University of California.
teaching paradigm for nonverbal autistic children. Journal of Autism & Developmental
32
Koegel, R. L., Schreibman, L., Britten, K. R., Burke, J. C. & O'Neill, R. E.
Rincover, & A. L. Egel (Eds.), Educating and understanding autistic children. (pp. 260-
Koegel, R.L., Schreibman, L., Good, A., Cerniglia, L., Murphy, C., & Koegel, L.
(1989). How to teach pivotal behaviors to children with autism: A training manual.
Consistent stress profiles in mothers of children with autism. Journal of Autism &
strategies and a curriculum for intensive behavioral treatment of autism. New York:
DRL Books.
Lord, C., Bristol, M. M., & Schopler, E. (1993). Early intervention for children
& et al. (Eds.), Preschool issues in autism. (pp. 199-221). New York, NY, US: Plenum
Press.
33
Lovaas, O. I. (1977). The autistic child: Language development through behavior
Lovaas, O.I., Berberich, J.P., Perloff, B.F., & Schaeffer, B. (1966). Acquisition
Lovaas, O. I., Freitag, G., Gold, V. J., & Kassorla, I. C. (1965). Experimental
Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R., Simmons, J. Q., & Long, J. S. (1973). Some
Lovaas, O. I., Schreibman, L., Koegel, R., & Rehm, R. (1971). Selective
Mancina, C., Tankersley, M., Kamps, D., Kravits, T., & Parrett, J. (2000).
34
Matsuoka, K., & Kobayashi, S. (2000). Understanding of other people's intentions
Maurice, C., Green, G., & Luce, S. C. (Eds.). (1996). Behavioral intervention for
young children with autism: A manual for parents and professionals. Austin, TX, US:
PRO-ED, Inc.
McGee, G., Daly, T., & Jacobs, H. (1994). The Walden Preschool. In Harris, S. &
Handleman, J., (Eds.), Preschool Education Programs for Children with Autism (pp. 127-
McGee, G. G., Morrier, M. J., & Daly, T. (1999). An incidental teaching approach
to early intervention for toddlers with autism. Journal of the Association for Persons with
35
Mesibov, G. B. (1993). Treatment outcome is encouraging. American Journal on
150.
varied responding in three students with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 15, 145-151.
Norris, C., & Dattilo, J. (1999). Evaluating effects of a social story intervention
on a young girl with autism. Focus on Autism & Other Developmental Disabilities, 14,
180-186.
36
Ozonoff, S., & Cathcart, K. (1998). Effectiveness of a home program intervention
for young children with autism. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 28, 25-
32.
Pierce, K., & Schreibman, L. (1997). Multiple peer use of pivotal response
training social behaviors of classmates with autism: Results from trained and untrained
Pierce, K. L., & Schreibman, L. (1994). Teaching daily living skills to children
intervention issues for children with autism spectrum disorders. In A. M. Wetherby & B.
37
Reichle, J., & Brown, L. (1986). Teaching the use of a multipage direct selection
communication board to an adult with autism. Journal of the Association for Persons with
Schreibman, L. (1988). Autism. Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Sage Publications, Inc.
delivered pivotal response training for children with autistic disorder. In E. D. Hibbs & P.
S. Jensen (Eds.), Psychosocial treatments for child and adolescent disorders: Empirically
based strategies for clinical practice. (pp. 525-552). Washington, DC, US: American
Psychological Association.
treatment protocols for children with autism. Paper presented at the 1st annual meeting of
Schreibman, L., Whalen, C., & Stahmer, A. C. (2000). The use of video priming
38
Schroeder, G. L., & Baer, D. M. (1972). Effects of concurrent and serial training
301.
Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, R. K., Anderson, A. W., Volkmar,
F., Skudlarski, P., Lacadie, C., Cohen, D. J., & Gore, J. C. (2000). Abnormal ventral
temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and
Schwandt, W. L., Pieropan, K., Glesne, H., Lundahl, A., Foley, D., & Larsson, E.
V. (2002, May). Using video modeling to teach generalized toy play. Paper presented at
the annual meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Toronto, Canada.
7, 205-220.
Sherer, M. R., & Schreibman, L. (in press). Individual behavioral profiles and
predictors of treatment effectiveness for children with autism. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology.
Sherer, M., Pierce, K. L., Paredes, S., Kisacky, K. L., Ingersoll, B., &
Schreibman, L. (2001). Enhancing conversation skills in children with autism via video
140-158.
39
Shipley-Benamou, R., Lutzker, J. R., & Taubman, M. (2002). Teaching daily
living skills to children with autism through instructional video modeling. Journal of
Spradlin, J. E., & Siegel, G. M. (1982). Language training in natural and clinical
using pivotal response training. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 25, 123-
141.
Thiemann, K. S., & Goldstein, H. (2001). Social stories, written text cues, and
Warren, S. F., Yoder, P. J., Gazdag, G. E., & Kim, K. (1993). Facilitating
40
Whalen, C. M. (2001). Joint attention training for children with autism and the
collateral effects on language, play, imitation, and social behaviors (Doctoral dissertation,
6122-B.
Whalen, C. & Schreibman, L. (in press). Joint attention training for children with autism
using behaviour modification procedures. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry.
Zanolli, K., Daggett, J., & Adams, T. (1996). Teaching preschool age autistic
children to make spontaneous initiations to peers using priming. Journal of Autism &
41