(Ebook) Equine Behavior: A Guide For Veterinarians and Equine Scientists by Paul Mcgreevy Isbn 9780702026348, 0702026344
(Ebook) Equine Behavior: A Guide For Veterinarians and Equine Scientists by Paul Mcgreevy Isbn 9780702026348, 0702026344
https://ebooknice.com/product/domestic-animal-behavior-for-
veterinarians-and-animal-scientists-6th-edition-11092606
ebooknice.com
https://ebooknice.com/product/equine-endoscopy-and-arthroscopy-for-
the-equine-practitioner-equine-made-easy-series-54931172
ebooknice.com
https://ebooknice.com/product/broodmare-reproduction-for-the-equine-
practitioner-book-cd-for-the-equine-practitioner-equine-made-easy-
series-54931184
ebooknice.com
https://ebooknice.com/product/applied-equine-nutrition-and-training-
equine-nutrition-and-training-conference-4660762
ebooknice.com
(Ebook) Equine Dentistry: A Practical Guide by Patricia Pence ISBN
9780683304039, 9780102030402, 0102030405, 0683304038
https://ebooknice.com/product/equine-dentistry-a-practical-
guide-1357082
ebooknice.com
https://ebooknice.com/product/equine-facilitated-psychotherapy-and-
learning-the-human-equine-relational-development-herd-approach-6614674
ebooknice.com
https://ebooknice.com/product/equine-facilitated-psychotherapy-and-
learning-the-human-equine-relational-development-herd-
approach-55811072
ebooknice.com
https://ebooknice.com/product/horse-safe-a-complete-guide-to-equine-
safety-2138098
ebooknice.com
https://ebooknice.com/product/applied-equine-nutrition-and-training-
equine-nutrition-and-training-conference-enutraco-2015-5397464
ebooknice.com
For Sandro and Mary
For Saunders:
ISBN 0702026344
Notice
Veterinary medical knowledge is constantly changing.
Standard safety precautions must be followed, but as new
research and clinical experience broaden our knowledge,
changes in treatment and drug therapy may become necessary
or appropriate. Readers are advised to check the most current
product information provided by the manufacturer of each drug
to be administered to verify the recommended dose, the method
and duration of administration, and contraindications. It is the
responsibility of the practitioner, relying on experience and
knowledge of the patient, to determine dosages and the best
treatment for each individual patient. Neither the Publisher nor
the author assumes any liability for any injury and/or damage to
persons or property arising from this publication.
The Publisher
The
Publisher’s
policy is to use
paper manufactured
from sustainable forests
Printed in China
Guest contributors
The behavior of horses has intrigued horse-lovers, been subject to scientific study, and Dr Paul
since ancient times. The domestication of the horse McGreevy has been one of a handful of veterinari-
and subsequent modern use for recreational pur- ans and scientists who have undertaken pioneer-
poses has led to close observations of and fascina- ing research in this field. This book is an important
tion with horse behavior. Whether or not they milestone in the progress towards better under-
realize it, those who get the best out of their horses standing of the effect of various equine man-
are students of equine behavior. Many opinions agement practices and their impact on horse
exist in relation to what are regarded as normal performance and wellbeing. Dr McGreevy’s book
and abnormal behaviors by horse owners and distills key scientific knowledge on horse behavior
trainers. Most recently, this has culminated in a from over 1000 separate sources and covers the
renaissance for the ‘horse whisperer’, who has salient ethological characteristics of free-ranging
supposedly mythical powers to commune with horses, the impact of various management prac-
horses in their very souls. Undoubtedly, there are tices, as well as the significance of abnormal
individuals who have rare gifts at an intuitive behaviors. In this latter area Dr McGreevy has
level, in understanding horses and their behavior. undertaken groundbreaking research with
Popular interest in the skills of such individuals Professor Christine Nicol on stereotypies, and this
has accompanied demand for more humane material will be of particular interest to those
training and handling techniques. Concurrently, interested in applying equine science.
the scientific study of equine behavior has been Dr McGreevy has provided an important guide
developing, and it is important to bring this to the for veterinarians and scientists and is to be con-
attention of the horse industry. gratulated for drawing together a veritable trove
For veterinarians and equine scientists, under- of scientific data on equine behavior that will be of
standing the key aspects of horse behavior is criti- benefit to the international horse industry.
cal to evaluation of health and welfare. It is only in
relatively recent times that horse behavior has Professor Reuben Rose
Preface
Equine Behavior: A Guide for Veterinarians and Equine growing sensitivity to what horses have been
Scientists has been an enlightening labor of love for telling us for a very long time. The influence of
me and I am honored to be commissioned to write science has helped to prompt fresh thinking
it. Destiny may have had a hand in my selection, about the relationship between horses and train-
since legend has it that Ireland’s McGreevy clan ers. Science has also shown that horses learn in
were keen horsemen and, indeed, were notorious the same ways as other animals, in accordance
for having stolen St Patrick’s horses. For their with the principles of learning theory. This means
alleged crime the Church forbade them to become that we are better placed to do the right thing
priests. I doubt if that would have troubled them, by our horses and get the best out of them. This
as long as they had horses with which to work. For volume coins the term equitation science not as an
me the study of equine behavior is the most fasci- attempt to undermine the art and skill of out-
nating occupation in the world. Along with other standing riders, but rather to demystify equi-
enthusiasts, I delight in watching horses at work tation and to allow more people to achieve better
and play, and comparing notes on our observa- results and reduce wastage.
tions. The aim of this book is to be a companion for As a veterinarian and a qualified riding
all horse watchers. instructor I had two related audiences in mind:
Horse lovers are often tempted to conjure my colleagues in the veterinary profession and
interpretations of how horses may process infor- the veterinary students who will soon join us,
mation and to imagine that horses share many of and equine scientists and graduates in equine
our value systems. On occasion, I have been crit- studies, who are assuming their place in the
icized for failing to consider the rich emotional industry hierarchy. Veterinary professionals are
qualities horses may well possess. Suffice it to say uniquely placed to benefit from the book’s scien-
that when we have a scientific way of measuring tific examination of those rare and poorly defined
these qualities – and that time may not be too far commodities, horse-sense and horsemanship.
away – I will be its strongest advocate, but until Owners naturally look to veterinarians for advice
then I make no apology for confining myself to on the physical health of their horses, but this
what we sometimes call the facts. book is designed to empower veterinarians to
The past three decades have witnessed a ter- become, also, a source of enlightened views on
rific explosion in equine ethology – the science of the mental health of their patients. Similarly,
horse behavior – so this is an exciting time for equine scientists can use the book to support
horse welfare. This book examines the truth scholarly approaches to enhance the manage-
behind modern trends and ancient traditions, by ment of horses. I hope this volume is written in a
bringing together the latest cutting-edge research style that makes it accessible to a very wide audi-
and best practice from around the world. The ence and that it helps the industry as a whole to
increased availability of data is matched by a work with these two stakeholders in designing
XII PREFACE
optimal scientific responses to behavior prob- technology means that, more than ever before, we
lems and in developing management and train- can disseminate our observations, management
ing systems that avoid their appearance. innovations, and empirical findings and training
Although this is a book that can be browsed, successes through scientific abstracts, Internet
I strongly encourage you to digest some of its discussion forums and email chat lists. Although
chapters in sequence. Specifically, the chapter on horse enthusiasts are notoriously traditional and
social behavior should be read before those on resistant to change, there is a pleasing uptake in
the behavior of the mare and stallion. Similarly, these methods of exchanging information and an
the chapter on learning contains material that acceptance of new approaches.
facilitates understanding of the chapters on train- I hope you enjoy Equine Behavior: A Guide for
ing, handling and miscellaneous problems. Veterinarians and Equine Scientists and that it
The future for our relationship with horses improves your understanding of horses and
seems bright. As we appreciate how to com- inspires you to find out more. I warmly invite
municate with them sensitively and consistently, you to submit suggestions (through my website
misunderstanding and misinterpretation by both www.animalbehaviour.net) on how to improve
parties will diminish. These advances are likely future editions of this text. Finally, on behalf of
to be matched by greater sharing of knowledge my ancient and far-flung clan, I hope that St
among practitioners. Gone are the dark days Patrick might regard my work as some kind of
when shamans locked themselves in stables to atonement.
weave their magic on equine subjects and
emerged with impressive results but no intention Paul McGreevy
of divulging the techniques used. Information 2004
Acknowledgments
Horses deserve the unique place they hold in Townsend-Sweeting from the University of
the hearts and minds of humans, so it should be Bristol. A number of undergraduate students
no surprise that horse behaviour as a research studying both veterinary science and agricultural
domain is populated by some extremely well- science made valuable contributions to early
motivated and talented people, who have demon- research for the book. They include Hilary Bugg,
strated their commitment to the species and their Russell Bush, Elvira Currie, Bradley Dong, Jayne
great kindness to me by providing invaluable Foster, Tanya Grassi, Lesley Hawson, Tammy Hunt,
feedback on various chapters of this book. They Kate Ireland, Lyndel Kruusamagi, Sally Leigo,
include Cheri Asa, Francis Burton, Hilary Therese McGillion, Joe McGrath, Shawn McGrath,
Clayton, Amy Coffman, Sharon Crowell-Davis, Sarah Mitchell, Kate Montgomery, Alex Nash,
Nancy Diehl, Machteld van Dierendonck, Brian Katherine O’Toole, Claire Ruting, Denbigh Simond,
Farrow, Jane French, Debbie Goodwin, Caroline Cathy Stimson and Joseph Toohey.
Hahn, Alison Harman, Kathe Houpt, Kevin The staff at the University of Sydney Library
Keay, Cecilia Lindberg, Andrew McLean, Finola have been characteristically helpful, efficient and
McConaghy, Sue McDonnell, Christine Nicol, professional in supplying research materials. In
Kim Ng, Sarah Ralston, Ken Sedgers, Amanda particular I would like to thank Joanne Bottcher,
Warren-Smith and Elaine Watson. One colleague, Ross Coleman, Su Hanfling and Alison Turner.
Daniel Mills, distinguished himself by going way My extraordinary friends have been both
beyond the call of duty and reading the entire supportive of my endeavours and tolerant of the
draft manuscript. I am tremendously grateful to extent to which writing has consumed my life
him for his advice and encouragement. over the past few years. Among the most helpful
Other veterinary and academic colleagues and supportive I wish to acknowledge Chris
who have provided helpful comments on specific Coffey, Nick Copping, Lucy Hall, Claudia Jones,
topics include George Abrams, Ian Bidstrup, Emma Lawrence, Bryan McMahon, Alastair
Heather Clegg, Sheila Crispin, Janet Eley, David Paterson, Julia Perry, Jarther Taylor and Ben
Evans, Elsa Flint, Merran Govendir, Josie Hall, Scandrett-Smith. While Michael Jervis-Chaston
Lee Morris, Chris Murphy, Justine O’Brien, Trank gave the most refreshingly blunt advice on all the
Odberg, Mellisa Offord, Lesley Rogers, David early chapters, Lynn Cole voluntarily cast an edi-
Todd, Claire Weekes, Elsa Willis, Bonnie Williams torial eye over every last sentence and this has
and Carol Wyldes. Others who have helped me made the transition from manuscript to proofs
obtain particular illustrations include Victor smoother than any author deserves. I have learned
Cabello, Natasha Ellis, Caroline Foote, Luke a great deal in the process and will remain forever
Henderson, Geoffrey Hutson, Peter Knight, grateful. Meanwhile, Sandro Nocentini happily
Peter Read, Peter Thomson, and Clive Wynne gave up an untold number of weekends to format
and, most notably, John Conibear and Tracey illustrations and draw samples for the book.
XIV ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Finally I would like to thank the outstanding McConville, Deni McKenzie, Daniel Naprous,
horse riders, trainers and enthusiasts who Gerard Naprous, Megan Roberts, Jo-Anne
have given their time and expertise to help with Rooker, Katie Rourke, Sigrid Schaerf, Daren
photography for the book. These include Terry Taylor and David Todd.
Cooper, Valmai Furlong, Scott Glover, Brenna Special thanks to Richard Tibbitts of Antbits
Harrison, Greg Hogan, Les Holmes, Portland Illustration and Maggie Raynor for all their hard
Jones, Victoria Kendall-Hawk, John Leckie, Dave work in the illustration of this title.
CHAPTER CONTENTS
1
Evolution and classification 1
Evolutionary background 1
Classification of equids 2 Introduction
Changing roles 4
Domestication 4
Current status 7
The horse’s future 11
Stable management 13
Traditional stable management 13
Modern stable management 13
Management practices 14
Behavior 16
Normal behavior in stabled horses 16
Abnormal behavior in stabled horses 21
Stereotypic behaviors 23
Introduction to evaluating behavior problems 28 EVOLUTION AND CLASSIFICATION
References 31
Because the evolution of the horse has been dealt
with many times elsewhere, I shall spare the
reader detailed descriptions of three-toed forest-
dwelling dog-like creatures. Suffice it to say that
we have been left with a most remarkable animal
that can exploit impoverished grazing niches by
foraging on very poor fibrous material and digest-
ing it quicker than ruminants. Various anatomical,
physiological and behavioral features of the
modern horse mean that it is useful to humans in
many ways (Fig. 1.1).
EVOLUTIONARY BACKGROUND
A plains feeder that does not disperse and defend
territories individually or in pairs as foragers of
richer resources tend to, the horse has a long nose
that allows it to graze while maintaining surveil-
lance above the sward. As an animal without
horns or antlers it relies largely on caution, speed
and agility as its chief means of self-preservation.
A social herbivore that capitalizes on companions
for added safety, mutual comfort and enhanced
detection of food, this is a creature that is likely to
feel insecure when isolated.
To digest material ruminants generally discard,
the horse needs a large fermentation chamber, the
cecum. Obliged to carry such a voluminous diges-
tive vat, the successful horse must therefore have
tremendous muscular power to shift its necessary
and considerable bulk from rest to top speed in
the event of danger, so it has developed the ability
1
2 EQUINE BEHAVIOR
CLASSIFICATION OF EQUIDS
Prior to the beginning of domestication, in the
late Pleistocene, long-term geographic isolation
of equid populations occurred.1 This led to the
distinct species that exist today (Table 1.1). True
horses (Equus caballus) occupied the Eurasian low-
(a) lands north of the great mountain ranges, while
the asses occupied the arid zones of Asia. Crosses
with asses, zebras and onagers are possible but
the hybrids are normally sterile.
Diploid chromosome
Common name Species number
stimuli. The least reactive equids are the so-called single fusion mutation in the haploid state (32)
‘cold-bloods’, the heavy horses and solid ponies. could have brought about their differences. They
In between the two groups are the ‘warmbloods’. could be part of a single species exhibiting
These can be described as crosses between hot and chromosomal dimorphism as occurs in the Asian
cold bloods and are exemplified by European per- ass (Equus hemionus).3 This then is the single-
formance horse breeds such as the Hannoverian. dichotomy theory that currently prevails over less-
fashionable claims of multiple lineage.
Subsequent isolation of the two gene pools
Equus caballus and Equus could perhaps have arisen because of capricious
przewalskii selectivity on the part of Caballus stallions (see
Although mitochondrial deoxyribonucleic acid Ch. 11) and the retreat of Przewalski herds into
(DNA) studies suggest a divergence occurred as regions of the steppe uninhabited by humans.4
long ago as a quarter of a million years, it seems While outnumbered by the similarities between
that the most likely link between modern horse the two species, the differences are fascinating.
(E. caballus) and its more numerous and diverse For example, the striking bistability of hock joints
predecessors is the pony-shaped Mongolian Wild that allows a very rapid switch from one extreme
Horse, Przewalski’s Horse (E. przewalskii). Apart position to the other is much more marked in
from Caspian ponies that appear to be polymor- domestic horses than in Przewalski horses and,
phic in the diploid number (some having 64 chro- for that matter, zebras.5
mosomes while others have 65),2 E. caballus has 64 Przewalski horses became extinct in the wild in
chromosomes. Equus przewalskii, on the other hand, the 1950s.6 However, from a nucleus of 11 founda-
has 66 chromosomes in the diploid state. tion animals, they survive today in captivity and
There is some debate as to how closely the first in successfully re-introduced wild populations,
ancestors of E. caballus were related to E. przewal- e.g. in Mongolia.7,8 The survival story of the
skii. Cross-breeding between E. caballus and Przewalski horse is an extraordinary one, and we
E. przewalskii does produce fertile offspring, and are indeed fortunate to be able to study them in a
there are similarities in their serum proteins variety of contexts. It is fascinating that we now
and their blood groups. Geneticists explain that a have data to refute the notion that Przewalski
4 EQUINE BEHAVIOR
horses are socially more aggressive than domestic remains elsewhere dating from periods since
horses.9 However, studies of their behavior should 9000 years ago are rare for this reason.13
be treated with some caution since they have Interestingly, it seems the ‘true horse’
emerged from a shallow gene pool that has been (E. caballus) survived in Eurasia but with patchy
filtered through 20 generations of captivity. There- distribution14 despite, or perhaps even because
fore before assuming that present-day Przewalski of, this predation.
horses behave as true wild horses, we should bear Archaeological evidence of horse domestica-
in mind that some would regard them as survivors tion dates from 4000 BC (late Neolithic or Bronze
of the first stages of domestication. Age) in the Eurasian Steppes of the Ukraine.15
In this text, I have selected examples of feral Ancient middens used as dumping grounds for
horse behavior rather than Przewalski horse the bony remains of human meals, most notably
behavior as benchmarks for what is normal. in a place called Dereivka on the steppes north of
Discussions of such free-ranging behavior appear the Black Sea, have proved an especially rich
in each of the subsequent chapters. source of domestication data. This is where river-
valley agricultural economies evolved, relying on
the use of stone enclosures to trap large numbers
of game. The numbers involved in a draft often
CHANGING ROLES exceeded the immediate demand for consump-
tion and prompted the maintenance of surviving
Although initially horses found themselves in the herd members, to be slaughtered at a later stage.
human domain as a food source, their subsequent From studies of the bone remains found in
roles are nearly as varied as those of the other most middens, the first pastoral species seems to have
domesticated species, the dog, and include the been bovine. A colder climatic shift that affected
provision of power, leisure and companionship. this region around 3500 BC is thought to have
favored equids rather than other domestic or
wild herbivores because, being large and long-
DOMESTICATION legged, they were better able to forage in snow.
French and Spanish cave paintings from around The prevalence of horses in Ukrainian communi-
15 000 years ago, depicting hunting for food and ties saw a dramatic rise at this time, with horse
hides, represent the earliest record of human use bones comprising 74% of animal bones at
of horses.10 Dereivka.16 Evidence of domestication at this site
While early horses would eventually provide includes this dramatic rise and the predominance
their keepers with unprecedented mobility and of colt skeletons. Analysis of the dental remains
power, the herding of horses probably had its ori- of horses in these sites suggests that humans con-
gins in the consumption of horseflesh.11 Hunters sumed more males than females and that the age
favor meat, such as that from horses, that has a range of these animals did not show a normal
high glycogen content. As a source of dietary distribution.
sugar, this facilitates endurance, which is impor- Some authors12,17 maintain that the relatively
tant for members of hunting cultures. Horses high number of colts indicates domestication
were regularly consumed in favor of other large rather than hunting which would be more likely
herbivores. In the Palaeolithic, our ancestors to harvest older animals, more often mares. Other
hunted horses by driving them over cliffs and workers suggest that the vast majority of horses at
into traps and pitfalls. They continued to do so this time were hunted, by stalking (Fig. 1.2).11
for thousands of years to the extent that they had Perhaps the age distribution of horse teeth most
a pivotal effect on population numbers.12 It is closely matches what one would expect as rem-
thought that, along with climatic changes, this nants of such activity, but it is worth remembering
exploitation contributed to the extinction of wild that assessing age by dentition is an imprecise
horses in North and South America. Wild horse science.18
INTRODUCTION 5
20
Archaeological findings
18 Dereivka data
Mortality (percentage of population)
Theoretical models
16 Natural attrition
Carnivorous husbandry
14
Stalking
12
10
6
Figure 1.3 Artifact from what is believed to be an early bit
4 fashioned from antlers.19 The rope would have been held in
place in the mouth by the cheek-pieces. (Reproduced by
2 permission of David Anthony, Dorcas Brown and Antiquity
0
Publications Ltd, from The Origins of Horseback Riding in
Antiquity 65:22–38.)
0 5 10 15 20
Age (years)
Figure 1.2 Graph of age distribution of remains of 151 heavy-set animals,15 a description strikingly rem-
horses found at Dereivka, Ukraine. In free-ranging horses iniscent of the Przewalski horse. When the skull
death occurs most commonly in the very young and very old of a male equid was removed from a ritual burial
(natural attrition model). In populations raised for meat site shared by two dogs and anthropomorphic
(carnivorous husbandry), the curve is skewed by preferential
figurines, it aroused considerable excitement in
consumption of 2- to 3-year-olds. Meanwhile selective hunting
of prime-age adults (stalking) produces a different pattern, archaeological circles. Dating from the Copper
with a sharp central peak. The age distribution of horse teeth Age, about 4300 BC, the ritualized nature of the
found at Dereivka most closely matches the stalking model. burial was suggestive of profound domestica-
(Reproduced by permission of Marsha Levine and Antiquity tion. Interestingly, in the same grave, excavations
Publications Ltd, from Levine M, The Problems of Horse
revealed antler tines that seemed to have been
Domestication in Antiquity 64:727–740.)
crafted into the cheek-pieces of a crude bit
(Fig. 1.3). By facilitating the comparison of the
The issue here seems to pivot on whether there teeth of modern domesticated and feral horses
is any reason for members of a domesticated pop- with those of the so-called Dereivka cult stallion,
ulation to be selected for consumption at 5–8 electronmicroscopic analysis of anterior premo-
years of age. A more traditional farming approach lar tooth wear has revealed bevels and fractures
would be to harvest the surplus while they were consistent with the use of a bit.19 Even though
younger and their flesh was reasonably tender. the first bit was not as robust and therefore as
Although one might also ask why hunters would damaging as modern hardware, the damage
target animals in a wild group that were undoubt- incurred was regarded as being consistent with
edly in their prime and therefore most fleet of 300 hours of bridled control. However, before
foot, it seems likely that they would be more making such comparisons, it is worth considering
abundant than adolescents or elderly horses. ways in which the evidence may have been
The reported preponderance of what we skewed. Freshly domesticated horses may have
regard as male type canine teeth seems to be cru- been very headstrong and early equitation unre-
cial here. Could it be that small canines as found fined. Notwithstanding this minor point, the
in many modern mares were erroneously identi- Dereivka stallion is regarded as the first ridden
fied as the teeth of stallions? horse and is thought to have predated the inven-
Horse remains at Dereivka have been charac- tion of the wheel by at least 500 years.20 The
terized as being from large-headed, short and domestication of horses on the steppes may have
6 EQUINE BEHAVIOR
been repeated in other places. Recent mitochon- than most on humans, not least in terms of the
drial DNA evidence21 indicates that a large num- dispersion of culture and language.13,20 Beginning
ber of founders was recruited over an extended with the migration of humans across the deep
time period. This ‘multiple origins’ scenario steppes of the Ukraine that could not support
implies that horses may have been independently large human populations prior to the emergence
captured from diverse wild populations and then of riding, horses facilitated the spread of human
bred in captivity as the wild populations disap- genes. Because riders move two or three times
peared.21 This suggests that what spread from faster and farther than pedestrians, exploitable ter-
that equine cradle was not necessarily the horses ritories expanded sixfold. At the same time grass-
themselves but the innovation and technology of land subsistence became more predictable, reliable
taming them. and productive, and decisive military advantages
Although it seems likely that meat production were secured over more static neighbors.20 As a
was the first reason for the domestication of consequence, human social groups grew tenfold,
horses, their prevailing impact was linked to their conflicts over resources escalated and patterns of
use in riding and haulage. When handling an trade and theft expanded in range and variety.
orphan foal, early horse farmers would have While riding was established as the prime means
appreciated the horse’s ability to pull and the fact of hunting, exploring and herding, the additional
that control of the body depended on control use of horses for milk and blood is likely to have
of the head. A natural progression would have peaked at the same time. One should not overlook
seen the development of harnesses for traction the usefulness of horses in the haulage of fallen
and headcollars for control. It may be that too game, firewood, ploughs, produce and physically
much emphasis has been placed on the signifi- challenged members of the group. Such was the
cance of bit wearing as the necessary step that importance of the horse in the early dispersal of
took the use of horses beyond the dining table. Copper Age humans that horses became one of
It is implied that horses could not have been con- the chief tradable items.
trolled from behind by anything less than a bit. The emergence of equestrian cultures is also
This is something of a moot point since it pre- likely to have brought with it an increase in hit-
cludes riding with bitless bridles. Equally, horses and-run warfare (Fig. 1.4). This suggestion is
could have been used for non-riding purposes borne out by the effective use of the horse by
such as traction of non-wheeled haulage devices, American Indians in the 1800s which resulted in
or fallen prey for that matter. the unprecedented rise in conflicts and accumu-
Early farmers had to have some means of pre- lation of wealth leading to the tribal supremacy
venting the usual migratory dispersal of horses but of the Sioux, Comanche and Apache.24
men on foot, even if they were accompanied by Interestingly it was not until approximately
trained dogs, would be no match for galloping 2000 BC that horses arrived in the Middle East and
horses.22 Only when riding was attempted could displaced asses and ass-onager hybrids as the
the herding of horses become efficient. Further- favored draft animals for battle carts from which
more, while using horses is a matter of taming the arrows could be fired. A similar displacement took
individuals as required, breeding them is far more place in mid 19th century Africa when colonists’
technically demanding.23 The difficulty of prevent- demand for the social status inferred by using
ing domestic mares from dispersing in search of apparently superior riding animals prompted
stallions whenever they were in estrus may have the importation of often poorly adapted horses
been the reason why domestication of the horse while the local equid, the quagga (Equus quagga),
took such a long time. For these reasons, horses was hunted to extinction by English and Boer
were domesticated after cattle, pigs, sheep and farmers.25
goats, which were all easier to herd and contain. A new wave of horse-mediated military success
Although the horse was one of the last mam- emerged when the horse’s speed and maneuver-
mals to be domesticated, it has had more impact ability was fully appreciated. In the first place,
INTRODUCTION 7
The overarching fields in which horses are now dogs, and very few populations are inbred.
used include recreational and social purposes, Notable exceptions are the horses of the Namib
breeding, sport and competition and to some Desert, Namibia40 and Blue Arabians from the
extent meat production.36 Huge reductions in the USA.41 In-hand showing as a sole means of select-
use of horses in military, agricultural and trans- ing breeding animals removes many performance
portation activities have been matched, in devel- indicators from the selection process in breeding
oped countries, by increased equine numbers for systems and is therefore short-sighted.
sport and leisure.37–39 While donkeys are still used by the working
Prevalent modern breeds are often used in classes in many parts of the world, the horse is now
more than one activity (Table 1.2), and this may a status symbol in Western cultures, a luxury item
help to account for the retention of some diver- or an emotional focus. Interestingly, this has done
sity. Happily, horse breed societies have tended nothing to guarantee its welfare, since many own-
to avoid the sort of intense line-breeding that ers of luxury items fail to budget for their mainte-
characterizes the bloodlines of many purebred nance. Despite the emergence of equine insurance
Table 1.2 Some generalizations about the attributes and uses of modern breeds and types
Dressage Controlled and • Classic good looks and • Responsive • Draft/TB crosses
yet demonstrably impressive gaits help to • Calm • Warmbloods including
powerful execution keep the judge’s eye on • Classically correct, free, Hannoverians, Dutch
of set maneuvers the horse regular paces Warmbloods,
• Powerful limbs • Confident Lipizzangers,
• Forward-sloping femur44 Oldenburgs
• Neck set on high
• Short neck42
• Long humerus43
• Long upright phalanges43
• Small hip angle43
• Flexible and supple
• ⬎14.2 hands high
(FEI height rule)
Meat Slaughter for human Not applicable Not applicable • European heavy
production consumption and breeds
pet food; notably: • Small pony breeds
• Spent working • Feral horses
animals
• Poor young-stock
specimens of
all breeds
• Some purpose-bred
animals
Carriage Dressage and • Strong hind quarters • Tough • Welsh ponies and
driving cross-country driving • Relatively short, strong legs • Compliant Welsh cobs
of teams, pairs capable of scrambling, • Tolerant of other horses • Friesians
and singles scurrying and turning in close proximity • Gelderlanders
• Sure footed • Cleveland Bays
• Agile • Dales
• High hock action • Fells
• Fjords
• Shetland ponies
• Standardbreds
FEI, Fédération Equestre Internationale; TB, thoroughbred.
INTRODUCTION 11
modern ‘horse whisperers’.58–61 Unfortunately, for including cardiac output and maximal oxygen
some exponents the emphasis seems to be to consumption.
placed on shows and the introduction of jargon While there may be initiatives to identify the
rather than on education. It is hoped that etholo- genes of horses that cope best with intensive man-
gists will continue to demystify the language of agement and do so without displaying stereo-
showmen and shamans so that communication typies, it is hoped that the technology can be
with horses can be enhanced wholesale. harnessed to improve horse welfare rather than
The practice of identifying and logging the simply to select for tolerance. The worldwide web
performance of non-racing sport horses has the seems an appropriate forum for information on
potential to enhance the quality and, it is hoped, the identification and treatment of common
the depth of the gene pool. For that reason it is ailments and the showcasing of best practice in
particularly sad to note the disappearance of the stable management.
British Horse Database. Although grazing land will become harder to
Similarly the introduction of novel reproduc- come by, stabling is likely to evolve to meet more
tive technologies such as artificial insemination closely the behavioral needs of horses. We may
should allow horse breeders to capitalize on yet identify key elements of established manage-
genetic material from overseas in the same way ment protocols that can have a deleterious effect
that shuttle stallions, flown in from the Northern on our horses.64,65 Although these days it is com-
hemisphere, have liberated Thoroughbred breed- mon for owners and trainers to regard the risk
ers in the Southern hemisphere. of injury in sport horses as being great enough to
Since we know that some unwelcome behav- outweigh the benefits of being turned out,36 new
iors follow a familial pattern of inheritance, it evidence may emerge to fortify the case for
may be that there will emerge a drive to breed increased opportunities for spontaneous exercise
from animals that can cope with the stressors of and social contact.
intensive management. Geneticists may also help The welfare of riding-school horses is likely to
to improve fertility, one of the most pressing improve as technological advances make it easier
problems in Thoroughbred circles. Globally, the to detect those of a novice rider’s signals that are
annual reproduction rate for Thoroughbreds is particularly confusing and unhelpful. In France,
approximately 50%.37 This compares poorly with an electronic riding machine has been developed
other single-offspring species such as cattle, in with all of the paces of a horse (Fig. 1.7). Currently
which 85% is normal.37 This disturbing trend is it has a predictable action that allows the rider
certainly not helped by a rise in the rate of twin- to learn, for instance, how to rise at the trot and
ning, an unfit gene that is passed from one gener- even jump a fence.66 With sensors to detect and
ation to the next almost exclusively by veterinary respond to the rider’s signals, the simulator
intervention. reduces the time required for novices to learn to
It seems unlikely that, since it has been the sin- ride.67 It is not difficult to see how this technology
gle breeding objective for the past two centuries, may advance. It could be programmed to behave
speed on the track can undergo much improve- less predictably, to be more or less responsive
ment. Mirroring the current focus of human exer- or to demonstrate typical equine conflict behav-
cise physiologists, the role of lactic acid clearance iors in response to confusing commands.68 One
as a limiting factor in racehorse performance con- could even speculate that the welfare of those
tinues to attract considerable attention.62 It may dependable riding-school horses that always
be that selection of genotypes capable of acceler- carry novice riders will be of less concern in the
ated lactic acid metabolism may reduce winning future. Currently balance is acquired over weeks
times in the longer classic races such as the of crude education, for example, staying on
St Leger and the Oaks.63,37 Markers of speed may board by pulling on the reins and therefore the
be identified by genome mapping studies but mouth. The numbing effect this has on the horse’s
these are likely to combine a number of features, mouth reflects the discomfort it must endure. This
INTRODUCTION 13
TRADITIONAL STABLE
MANAGEMENT
The horses sent over to the British Isles by the
Roman conquerors would have included a vari-
ety of heavy, ‘cold-blooded’ and less weather-
tolerant, ‘hot blooded’ animals. Archaeological
evidence69 suggests that they were small by mod-
ern standards. Evidence of how Roman horses
were housed exists but is rather cryptic, with rem-
nants often being limited to urine and manure
staining on chalk beds and teeth in feeding chan-
nels. However, a site at Hod Hill in Dorset70
appears to have been particularly well studied.
Two types of stable division were noted, and the
pattern of chalk-compression and the extent to
Figure 1.7 Persival, the Ecole Nationale d’Equitation’s which hooves had mixed chalk with manure gave
mechanical horse simulator. the investigators an impression of how the ani-
mals were secured within the building. In one
type of compartment, measuring 3.6 m ⫻ 3.3 m,
inelegant and inhumane use of sentient beings in
three horses might have been tethered to the wall,
human education may disappear with the grow-
their dung falling into a 2-meter channel to the
ing use of mechanical alternatives.
rear. This channel also permitted access by
Digitization of the interaction of elite riders with
grooms. The other compartment appeared to have
their mounts may provide for a quantum improve-
accommodated six horses that again were tethered
ment in the teaching of equitation. Measurement
facing the wall. Measuring 3.6 m ⫻ 5.5 m, this
of tensions and pressures applied by the handler’s
house comprised a central dunging passage that
hands, seat and legs can be correlated to observed
was communal to both rows of horses.
behavior of the animal and provide an indication
Information regarding accommodation for
of the magnitude of stimulation required to be
military horses since that time is limited. Smith71
applied by the handler to modify the behavior of
indicates that the British Army billeted horses in
the animal. Logging devices of this sort will also
lines and did not have stables until a program of
provide teachers with some quantitative means of
building barracks and stables began in 1792. Of
assessing the handling of an animal under the
these stables little is known except that they were
control of a student.
not ventilated and therefore precipitated consid-
erable losses from glanders and other respiratory
diseases.71 Coach horses from the same era were
confined to tie-stalls rather than individual boxes
STABLE MANAGEMENT that were the reserve of saddle horses.
Humans can control horses most effectively by
stabling them. This imposes limits on the extent
MODERN STABLE MANAGEMENT
to which horses can meet their behavioral needs
and can have profound effects on welfare. Equine Just as domestic horses are put to a number of
behavior undergoes considerable modification uses, so they are housed for a number of reasons
14 EQUINE BEHAVIOR
should be at least 4 m ⫻ 4 m and preferably which horses are housed individually, under one
4 m ⫻ 5 m. Ensminger82 states that except for foal- roof, in pens made largely from bars and grilles),
ing mares and for stallions, there is no advantage which facilitates communication between neigh-
in having box stalls larger than 3.6 m square. Evans boring horses, is increasing in popularity. Studies
et al83 note that the popular size is 3.6 m ⫻ 3.6 m on the effect of increasing visual contact with
and that 3 m ⫻ 3 m is adequate for young horses conspecifics on weaving86,87 (see Ch. 5) suggest
but suggests that the more time the horse spends that whether a stable, rather than a tie-stall, is
in the stall, the larger the stall should be. There are acceptable depends on its walls more than its
several differences between the housing of horses size in that isolation is more of a problem than
compared with that of other farm animals. Horses confinement.79
usually have individual living areas, and a much Modern stable management brings with it a
greater labor input per animal is present in stables number of disadvantages for both owners and
than in farm-animal housing. A survey of racing horses. For the owner, the disadvantages of keep-
stables in the Southwest of England found that ing their animals in stables rather than at grass
floor space varied between 8.76 sq m and 21.8 sq m revolve around time and money. Time commit-
with a median of 12.1 sq m per horse.84 This com- ments in stable management include bedding
pares favorably with the floor space provided for and feeding as well as having to exercise the con-
other agricultural species per unit of body size.85 fined horse on a more regular basis than its graz-
While box designs have been suggested by ing equivalent in order to maintain a given level
agricultural engineers and by horse-lore, there of fitness. The disadvantages of stabling from the
appear to be no recommendations in management horse’s perspective are considered later.
texts about the amount of time that the occupants
of these quarters should spend within them on
a daily basis. There are a number of reasons for
protracted confinement, including inclement
weather, locomotory illness and isolation of
BEHAVIOR
contagious pathogens. Episodes of enforced con-
When practitioners are asked to comment on
finement are popularly noted as being contempo-
abnormal or unwelcome behaviors, they must first
raneous with the onset of stereotypies. Given that
of all appreciate the range of normal behaviors.
Evans83 advocates the provision of more space for
animals that are turned out less regularly, it could
be argued that the design of all stables should
meet the needs of the worst-case scenario that
NORMAL BEHAVIOR IN STABLED
precipitates withdrawal of the daily turn-out
HORSES
period. Domestication affects behavior,88 not least because
The traditional layout of a stable-yard it limits the amount of space in which stock is able
includes the quadrangular enclosure of a central to range. This limitation can be effective from day
lawned area by boxes whose doors and windows one of life, since many foals, especially valued
face inwards. Walkways in many yards pass close Thoroughbreds, are born indoors.89 Interestingly it
to these portals and expose stabled horses to the has been suggested that myopia may be a conse-
unpredictable and arousing movement of humans, quence for horses that spend too much time in
feed-buckets and conspecifics. Therefore, it has stables90 (this is discussed further in Ch. 2).
been suggested that regimented geometrical The changes in equine behavior associated
accommodation of this sort makes relaxation with confinement and limited choice merit par-
difficult for the occupants.58 ticular consideration. From the horse’s perspec-
Despite similar problems with the distracting tive, the ways in which stabling can compromise
effects of activity in the passageways and others feeding, social and kinetic behavior and indeed
to do with air hygiene, barn-style housing (in health are considered below.
INTRODUCTION 17
10% 10%
60% 57%
40%
15%
10%
3% 5%
65%
15%
47%
(c) (d)
5%
30%
25%
Figure 1.9 Time budgets for horses in a variety of environments: (a) free-
ranging Camargue horses, averaged over a year; (b) eight horses housed as
40% a group with ad libitum hay and straw; (c) three horses in individual stables
given ad libitum hay and straw and able to see and touch each other;
(d) horses fed restricted fiber in stables where they cannot touch each
other and only see each other over stable doors; (e) six crib-biters fed ad
libitum hay and able to see and touch each other. NB: Crib-biting accounts
Eat Stand Lie Other for 20% of these horses’ ‘other’ behaviors. (Charts (a)–(d) reproduced
with permission of M Kiley-Worthington from The Behaviour of Horses in
(e) Relation to Management and Training.)
(d) (e)
Figure 1.10 a–d Irish Draft Horse undoing the bolt of his stable door. (e) After a chain was secured across the threshold to
prevent his escape, he moved his hay so that he could watch activities on the yard while eating.
are of particular importance when tie-stalls are escape. Until we have costed out the value of
considered, with recent evidence to suggest that resources or the absence of threats to safety and
they do not sufficiently cater for horses’ needs to comfort, it behoves us to consider these imposi-
perform social, recumbent resting and allogroom- tions from the horses’ perspective. For example,
ing behaviors.110 while stallions and occasionally geldings create
Stables of all sizes when designed for individual stud-piles of feces in the stable, eliminative behav-
horses have the potential to conflict with many of ior in the stable differs markedly from that in the
their occupants’ survival instincts.10 While impos- paddock, since the occupants cannot easily avoid
ing the vulnerability of isolation and an unhelpful contact with their urinary and fecal waste. Since
concentration of excretory products that may attract fresh layers of bedding regularly prompt urination
predation, they prevent detection of predators and in male equids, some consider this a form of
20 EQUINE BEHAVIOR
marking.58 So, the extent to which bedding is man- Space is certainly restricted when horses are
aged to meet human rather than equine needs housed, particularly in the case of tie-stalls. The
bears considerable merit but it is but one feature of extent to which stabled horses can see con-
the stabled horse’s world that represents a removal specifics is restricted. In a traditional loosebox,
of choice. In many ways horses attempt to meet this can often be achieved only by looking over
their behavioral needs despite the shortcomings the stable door, while many tie-stalls restrict visi-
of intensive environments but may fail to do so bility further by having tall dividers between
because of management routines. neighboring pens. This view is a contentious one.
Marsden114 maintains that the behavior of horses
kept in tie-stalls is similar to that when they are
kept at pasture. This was based on the fact that, in
Kinetic behavior tie-stalls, horses showed no significant increase
Among the prevailing features that arise in inten- in the time they spent performing abnormal
sively managed horses is restricted space. The behavior. The proximity of neighboring horses
physical limitations imposed on a horse by sta- was thought to facilitate social behavior more
bling mean that kinetic behavior is more difficult than is the case in individual looseboxes. It may
to perform than it is at pasture. For example, it has be that the small sample size (n ⫽ 4) could have
been estimated that an average sized horse needs resulted in the inadvertent selection of horses
a 6 m span to roll from one side to another, so that were especially unreactive and not predis-
many stabled horses are unable to perform this posed to perform stereotypies. This work did,
most basic of maintenance behaviors. The relative however, indicate that stalled horses spent signif-
lack of space may either prevent the stabled horse icantly less time lying down and moving, with
from choosing to roll or precipitate casting. significantly more time standing in stalls than at
Confinement within looseboxes is common and is pasture. Subsequent studies of pregnant mares in
an important limitation on the amount of exercise tie-stalls have shown their time budgets to be
that a horse can take through choice. After peri- similar to those of free-range horses.115 It would
ods of confinement, horses show post-inhibitory be interesting to see whether the mares’ gravid
rebound111 that may explain why unwanted state influenced this outcome.
behaviors during training occur more frequently
among stabled horses than those at pasture.112
Health
It has been suggested49 that voluntary kinesis
indicates that, rather than being simply a substra- The continual proximity to feces and urine may
tum for most behavior, locomotory activity has a be aversive to a stable’s occupant and may also
motivation of its own. However, the possibility of have an impact on health since humidity and air-
motivation for spontaneous locomotion is a con- borne pathogen viability may rise, especially if
tentious area because there is often difficulty in air-changes per hour are insufficient.84 An unnat-
eliminating motivation to perform other behav- ural foraging posture, e.g. eating from a haynet
iors for which locomotion is a prerequisite, e.g. for extended periods rather than from the
intrinsic exploration.113 ground, may compromise the function of the cil-
Although unacquainted horses from different iary escalator in the respiratory tract116 and pre-
yards may be mixed on a national and interna- cipitate pulmonary disease. Rhabdomyolysis117
tional scale for equestrian competition, isolation and chronic degenerative joint disease118 are
is often practiced when new horses arrive on a often exacerbated by the physical restriction that
yard, in a bid to control the spread of pathogens. accompanies extended periods of stabling. A sta-
Should such animals perform an unwelcome ble with insufficient or uncomfortable bedding
behavior pattern, they will often remain in isola- may limit the willingness of its occupant to lie
tion, since stereotypies are traditionally regarded down.119 This is thought to be associated with a
as contagious. foreshortening of the horse’s working life.120
INTRODUCTION 21
correctly identified the primary target. For exam- these horses may make them generally more reac-
ple, horses that eat their bedding material are tive when being handled in the stable.58 Similarly
thought by some to be performing a redirected inappropriate levels of stimulation are thought
behavior that meets their physiological needs for to cause psychopathologies such as hyperphagia
dietary fibre.96 Others regard this as a form of nervosa49 and polydipsia nervosa.134
coprophagia if the bedding is soiled, but since the
remedies advocated involve increasing the provi-
sion of roughage (which redirects the behavior Learned behavior
back to its evolutionary target), the distinction is
Learning in companion animals is often not
descriptive rather than functional. Coprophagia
recognized and is therefore regularly misinter-
is regarded as an adaptive behavior in foals127–129
preted. This often involves so-called superstitious
but as being an ethopathy in older individuals.49
learning whereby a response is acquired as a result
Another common example of redirected behav-
of its accidental link with a reinforcer.135 Misinter-
ior in equine husbandry systems is wood-chewing
pretation arises because the handlers are unaware
which is regarded as an ingestive behavior that
of the cues that their presence or actions represent
would be more readily directed toward grass or
to the animal. Chapter 4 is designed to allow read-
other palatable fiber were it available.96 However,
ers to become conversant with learning theory and
it is known that horses at pasture will chew
to understand why the term superstitious learning
wood,130–132 which implies that our understanding
is virtually redundant, given that animals are
of the causes of this behavior is incomplete. It is
learning all the time. Recognizing ways in which
possible that the ingestion of small quantities of
animals learn associations unanticipated by their
bark may be adaptive as a means of acquiring
handlers is a cornerstone of behavior therapy.
micronutrients.96
A bizarre example is a case of psychogenic colic
Both redirected and displacement behaviors are
that was associated with a demand for human
subject to the rules of operant conditioning (see
company.136
Ch. 4) and may be reinforced to become estab-
Another example is learned aggression to
lished unwelcome responses. For example, many
humans that can evolve as a result of inappropri-
horses paw at the ground or at the stable door prior
ate associations between agonistic posturing and
to being fed. Although this may be a response that
the unwitting delivery of a food reward. Thus,
would be appropriate for a free-ranging horse
arrival of the handler prompts hunger-motivated
that is being kept from its forage by a temporary
head-threatening behavior that is reinforced by
barrier such as snow, some would see the target-
the consequent acquisition of food as the human
ing of the door as a redirection of the behavior.
retreats. This agonistic behavior escalates when
If the horse is fed during or shortly after pawing
punished directly by the handler since his even-
the door it will be more likely to paw the door in
tual retreat becomes a more valued goal.137 Other
future.
learned responses, including conflict behaviors
that arise during horse–human interactions, are
Over-stimulation or under- considered in Chapter 13.
stimulation of behavioral systems
While lack of stimulation is often cited as a cause
Physical problems
of anomalous behavior,46,133 including apathy, the
opposite also merits consideration in the ontogeny Problems that arise as a direct consequence of
of unusual behavioral strategies linked to housing. dimensions of the stable, such as ‘refusal to lie
Kiley-Worthington78 indicates that too much noise, down’ have been blamed on some deficiency on
excitement or exercise can cause over-stimulation, the part of the occupants rather than the design-
as can the presence of too many horses and humans ers of the accommodation. Certainly, the area
to mix with socially. Raised levels of arousal in available influences resting behavior.138 Other
INTRODUCTION 23
examples of physical problems include habitu- ancient sites of worship in Syria have shown
ally getting cast, catching hips on the doorway signs of wear that have been likened to the ero-
(often exacerbated by learned fear that prompts sion one finds in crib-biters.141 The use of incisor
faster, though not necessarily better judged, exits wear to identify palaeolithic horses as crib-biters
through doorways) and getting feet caught in is not without its critics.142 The current preva-
haynets. lence of crib-biting in donkeys is negligible and,
while it is possible that crib-biting was more
common in the past and has been selected out of
STEREOTYPIC BEHAVIORS the population to some extent, it seems strange
Stereotypic behavior is characterized by being that three of the five donkeys in this site had this
repetitive, relatively invariant and apparently stereotypy. The use of incisor erosion as a means
functionless.139 Stereotypies are heterogeneous in of confirming that the bearer is or was a crib-biter
their causes and their forms. Caged tigers that remains controversial because pre-purchase evi-
repeatedly pace up and down in their enclosures dence of erosion is cited in instances of litigation
raise public concern about welfare in zoos. On a when the vendor fails to declare the behavior.
less exotic scale, similar behaviors are performed This link between crib-biting and domestica-
by horses and ponies. Historically known in tion is made in the belief that stereotyped behav-
horse-lore as ‘stable vices’ and given specific ior occurs only in animals that have experienced
descriptive labels such as box-walking (stall- captivity. Furthermore, the assumption that this
walking in the USA), weaving, wind-sucking and wear is invariably the result of an oral stereotypy
crib-biting, these seemingly irreversible behavior is not without hazards since sandy soils and
changes tended to cause more embarrassment particular species of grass may produce similar
than concern. Despite this, questions regarding erosion of enamel on the labial surface and the
‘stereotypies’ (as these behaviors are more cor- tables of the incisors.
rectly described) have been fielded by behavior- As well as crib-biting and a number of the
ists and vets for decades. other so-called stable vices, weaving is described
Most lay authors on the subject tend to use the in a number of antique texts on equine hus-
blanket term ‘boredom’ to explain how these bandry.143–145
behaviors arise, and the remainder imply that it is
the fault of the horses themselves. However, the
days of dismissive attitudes to behavioral anom-
Characteristics of stereotypic
alies in horses and ponies would appear to be
behaviors
numbered. Therefore, while there are still those Stereotypic behaviors are not recognized in free-
who regard the ‘private life’ of their stabled horses living feral horses and are not purely a product of
as being unimportant as long as it does not cause domestication since they are also reported in cap-
poor performance, others have begun to question tive examples of wild equids such as the onager
the merits of traditional stable management that mountain zebra146 and Przewalski horses.147 In
push the horse beyond its limits of adaptation. the horse, these behaviors have therefore been
linked to a number of management practices.
A number of equine stereotypies have been
Historical accounts of stereotypic identified,78 including:
behavior
Stereotypic behavior is not described in the • chewing
earliest text on horsemanship and stable manage- • lip-licking
ment140 but archaeologists have used erosion on • licking environment
the incisors of equine skulls as an indicator of • wood-chewing
crib-biting and therefore domestication. Donkey • crib-biting
teeth gathered from what are thought to be • wind-sucking
24 EQUINE BEHAVIOR
Table 1.3 Reported prevalence (%) of stereotypies and wood-chewing from studies published
between 1993 and 1998151 (reproduced with permission)
Standard
Mean deviation Median Minimum Maximum
are extremely variable, with reports of both inability to suppress non-functional behavior
decreases167,168 and increases.163 While it is possi- (Daniel Mills, personal communication 2000).
ble that weaving may also be opioid mediated, While this may have repercussions for the train-
further work with larger numbers of weavers ability of stereotypic horses, it should be borne in
would be required before there can be a clearer mind when equine scientists select subjects for
understanding of the mechanisms involved.163 studies of learning.
A further suggestion is that a given stereotypy
may retain a function within the motivational
system from which it is derived.78 Thus, an oral Management factors and abnormal
stereotypy such as crib-biting may provide a route behaviors in stabled horses
to normal feeding and digestive activity within
It is clear that the prevalence of crib-biting and
an environment that severely limits normal for-
weaving is greater in Thoroughbreds than in
age intake (e.g. an intensive training program
other breeds.171,172 The prevalence of box-
characterized by the provision of high concen-
walking, wind-sucking/crib-biting and weaving
trate : minimal roughage diets).
in UK Thoroughbred populations has been esti-
Persevering with non-functional behaviors or
mated at 1.1%, 4.2% and 2.8%, respectively
previously trained but currently unrewarded
(n ⫽ 1033)173 and in Italy at 2.5%, 2.4% and 2.5%
responses is a characteristic of stereotypic ani-
(n ⫽ 1035).174 Data from 4468 UK Thoroughbred
mals.169,170 Preliminary reports from a pilot study
horses in training showed the total prevalence of
involving two stereotypic and two normal horses
all three of these stereotypies to be 10.8%, similar
suggest that they may have developed a general
to the prevalence of lameness.175 Estimates of the
combined prevalence of all stereotypies are more
difficult; for example, when lip-licking, pawing,
tail-swishing, head-tossing, head-nodding and
box-kicking were included, the prevalence on
some yards reached 26%.78 However, it remains
difficult to dissect the effects of management and
breed because Thoroughbreds are generally
raced and therefore managed intensively.
In other species, cage-design,176 isolation-
rearing177 and food-deprivation178 have been impli-
cated as proximate causes of stereotypic behavior.
Arousal, generated by frustrated motivation, is a
possible shared underlying cause,179 although oth-
ers emphasize the possible heterogeneity of the
cause and effect of different stereotypies.139 Despite
much work on farm and laboratory species, the
proximate causes of stereotypic and redirected
behavior in the horse have yet to be verified. How-
ever, the possible causal factors associated with
oral and locomotory stereotypies are discussed in
Chapters 8 and 5, respectively.
Figure 1.11 Horse crib-biting on metal substrate. Although
horses prefer to grasp wood rather than metal (perhaps
because it cushions their teeth), they will crib-bite on metal if
no other substrate is available. This helps to demonstrate a The role of genes and environment
considerable behavioral need to perform the stereotypy.
(Reproduced by permission of the University of Bristol, Although they have failed to control for variabil-
Department of Clinical Veterinary Science.) ity in management factors, studies into the role
26 EQUINE BEHAVIOR
that heritability plays in stereotypy frequency including factors associated with weaning, social
have added weight to the view that certain fam- contact, crowding, feeding, housing and/or train-
ily groups are more likely than others to demon- ing practices.78,182
strate stereotypic behavior.94,174,180 Meanwhile a Some authors120,149 emphasize the importance
growing branch of the literature identifies the of mimicry rather than environmental deficits,
importance of management factors that might believing that exposure to a stereotypic neighbor
frustrate motivation in the horse.101,151 In horses may increase the likelihood of stereotypy devel-
bred for Flat racing, these include the amount opment or performance. Such social influences,
and type of forage, the bedding type, the number known in voles,183 may also affect stereotypy lev-
of horses on the yard and the amount of commu- els in horses,184 despite no current indication that
nication that is possible between neighboring horses can learn by observation.185–187 Even if
horses.101 A parallel study in dressage and event- observational learning is not involved, we should
ing horses demonstrated that the amount of time not rule out the possibility that having a stereo-
spent in the stable correlated with the likelihood typic neighbor may increase the arousal of
of stereotypies being reported.181 observing horses and therefore predispose them
Although feeding practices tend to have a to developing stereotypic behavior.
greater effect than housing practices on the An exhaustive study of abnormal behavior on
incidence of abnormal behavior,114 a single Thoroughbred studs gathered data on more than
causative factor is rarely to blame. For example, 11 000 horses, which suggested that, as a result of
wood-chewing increased when high-protein emancipation, the prevalence of stereotypies
rations were fed, because of a concomitant reduc- tends to rise with age (Fig. 1.12).175 Therefore, it
tion in the total fiber content,95 and when exercise seems likely that few horses are ever ‘cured’ of
was withdrawn.131 Many authors have suggested these behaviors. This implies that once these
other possible causes of anomalous behaviors, behavioral anomalies are established, they persist
to an extent, despite attempts to improve any
potentially causative deficiencies in the horse’s
8 management (Fig. 1.13). The continual reinforce-
Crib-biting/wind-sucking
ment of stereotypic behaviors contributes to their
Percentage performing abnormal behavior
Box-walking
resistance to therapy. Having said that, in one
Weaving
6 Wood-chewing
0
Foal 1 2 3 Adult
Age (years)
Figure 1.13 Two horses crib-biting at pasture. Since they
Figure 1.12 The percentage of Thoroughbreds reported as have one another’s company and optimal foraging
showing stereotypic and redirected behaviors from a sample opportunities they provide a useful example of emancipation.
population of 1023 foals, 746 yearlings, 1001 2-year-olds, (Reproduced by permission of the University of Bristol,
711 3-year-olds and 6250 adults. Department of Clinical Veterinary Science.)
INTRODUCTION 27
study effective treatment of crib-biting was offered can become emancipated from their initiating
as a possible reason (along with euthanasia) for causes.195 Therefore, while stereotypies may arise
the largest percentage prevalence in crib-biting in response to adverse management, they may per-
rates being found in foals.188 sist in more enriched environments. Despite this,
Fundamentally the possibility of emancipa- owners of stereotypic horses often feel responsible
tion means that it is better to implement environ- for their charges’ behavioral anomalies. The behav-
mental enrichment prophylactically rather than iors are therefore unwelcome not least because
therapeutically. Notwithstanding its effects in they are a potential source of embarrassment to
maintaining normal behavior in youngstock,189 it owners.
is also likely to increase learning ability190 while at Stereotypic horses are considered undesirable
the same time promoting musculoskeletal health.191 for many reasons. Generally diminished per-
The process of emancipation also means that formance in stereotypers has been identified by
stereotypic foals are better research subjects than some authors,78,196 while health problems associ-
mature stereotypers. Studies on cohorts of foals ated with these behaviors have been highlighted
from neonates to 4-year-olds have yielded fasci- by others.102,119,197–199 Furthermore, it has been
nating results. For example, time-related patterns suggested that stereotypic horses in tie-stalls
for the development of oral behaviors differ from spend less time interacting with neighboring
those of locomotory behaviors.188 The results horses.115 Prevention of crib-biting, involving
of these and other recent studies on oral and physical or surgical approaches, is commonly
locomotory stereotypies are discussed in more attempted,1,119,200–204 with considerable variation
detail in Chapters 8 and 5, respectively. in rates of success. Certainly surgery is falling out
The future holds tremendous promise for of favor as the causes of stereotypies become
sympathetic and productive understanding of better understood.
equine stereotypies and the responsibility we Although there is no empirical evidence that
must take for their emergence and effective observational learning can occur in horses, the
management. belief that behaviors can be learnt or copied affects
the management of stereotypic horses. Physical
prevention of the behaviors is the prevalent
Management of stereotypies response, while isolation of affected horses from
Detailed therapeutic approaches to individual other horses is also common (Table 1.4). If these
stereotypic behaviors will be considered in later behaviors are not as functionless as assumed, e.g.
parts of this book, but it is worth noting some they constitute a coping response to a suboptimal
overarching principles that apply to many of environment, the common practice of preventing
these ethopathies. stereotypic behavior may be of welfare signifi-
Of all of the anomalous behavior patterns in cance. Conversely, managing stereotypic animals
horses, stereotypies seem to have proved most so that they are subjected to minimal frustration,
insoluble to horse owners. Work on other species e.g. by feeding and exercising before other horses
by different groups154,159,192–194 often appears con- on the yard, is very helpful.
tradictory and has highlighted heterogeneity in The need to prevent stereotypies for aesthetic
stereotypies.152 Since the study of equine stereo- and occasional health reasons has prompted
typies is in its infancy and because information searches for permanent cures. A remedy that is
from other species can be applied only with cau- effective for every crib-biter remains elusive (but
tion, it is perhaps not surprising that hippologists readers are directed to Ch. 8 for a discussion of
have found vices so mysterious and frustrating current humane approaches). The resourceful-
and have met with so little success when attempt- ness of horses in satisfying their motivation to
ing to modify these behaviors.120 perform this behavior seems to overwhelm
As the data demonstrating increasing pre- humans’ ability to physically prevent established
valence with age suggest, stereotypic behaviors forms of the behavior.
28 EQUINE BEHAVIOR
Environmental enrichment, often mentioned Care should be taken to translate horsey jargon
in the context of captive exotic species, is no less when taking a history. Various terms may be used
important in the horse. Turning stabled horses out to describe the same behavior; for example,
to pasture offers a dramatic illustration of the depending on the observer, a horse may be vari-
effects of enrichment. However, for some owners ously described as sharp, keen, fizzy or flighty.8,207
this is not an option. While many owners reach A survey of horses described by their owners as
for the blanket term ‘boredom’ to describe the ‘head-shakers’ identified some that were simply
problems faced by the stabled horse, some are nodding their heads.208 Having said that, there is
beginning to explore ways to increase complexity evidence that even among a large group of ama-
of stables.206 For example, using a chain in place of teurs the use of a human personality inventory
the stable door is a surprisingly effective means of achieves high inter-correlations in the ranking of
increasing the visual stimuli that enter at stable horses, with especially strong agreement being
(see Fig. 1.10e). The key with any environmental achieved for neuroticism and extraversion.209 The
enrichment is to ensure that the intervention is reliability of these assessments increases the legiti-
relevant and unlikely to provide a focus for sim- macy of everyday use of psychological terms to
ple redirection of abnormal behaviors, an out- describe animal behavior.209 Additionally it is
come that is quite unsatisfactory. By studying encouraging to note that when confined to the use
this book, and most notably Chapters 5 and 8, of objective descriptive definitions, riders score
readers will be well placed to design enrichment horses’ temperaments with significant agreement
strategies that are likely to meet the behavioral and that their scores correlate significantly with
needs of all horses, especially those that exhibit objective measures from temperament tests.210
under-stimulation and frustrated motivation. Although clients tend to be impressed by
shortcuts in history-taking, experience shows
this tempting path to be more hazardous than
INTRODUCTION TO EVALUATING worthwhile. Acquiring only half the salient facts
BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS means one is half as likely to emerge with a cor-
Many behaviors labeled abnormal are simply nor- rect diagnosis and successful therapy.
mal but unwelcome. It is the veterinarian’s job to More than dog and cat owners, horse owners
distinguish normal and learned behaviors from tend to seek advice from each other before calling
those with organic origins. Owners and grooms out the veterinarian, so it is often the case that
generally know their horses better than the con- horse-lore has been applied unsuccessfully in the
sulting clinician and therefore it is important to past. At the end of a complete case history, one
keep one’s counsel until a thorough case history should have all the benefit of hindsight and
has been taken. therefore make intelligent suggestions based on
INTRODUCTION 29
suggests that a visual appraisal may help to indi- unwanted behaviors, it pays to clarify the specifics
cate chronic pain, with the affected horse typically of any behavior that seems to have departed from
having a loose lip but clenched masseter muscles. the norm. For example, if the animal is described
Meanwhile, there may be a facial expression sug- as aggressive, considerably more information is
gestive of ‘concern’, with decreased mobility of the necessary before the motivation to demonstrate
eyes, puckering of the eyelids, backward inclina- agonistic behaviors can be understood. One
tion of the ears211 and dilation of the nostrils.49 As should establish whether the horse is aggressive to
social herbivores, horses might not be expected to other horses, other domestic species, humans or
demonstrate signs of pain more overt than teeth- all of the above. Aggression toward specific targets
grinding, since to do so may single them out for merits further questioning. For example, aggres-
predation. However, it is proposed that when sion toward humans can be investigated further
vocalization accompanies pain, it generally takes with questions such as:
the form of a squeal if the pain is of somatic origin
and a grunt if visceral.211 While reduced locomo- • Under what circumstances is the horse
tion and postural attempts to shift weight away aggressive?
from the seat of pain can be spotted from a dis- • Does the horse avoid personnel or is it
tance, guarding of muscle groups may be appreci- actively aggressive?
ated during palpation. • Does the horse target particular individuals
Although the role of neuropathic pain is now or types of people (male, female, large, small)
recognized in the etiology of head-shaking (see or all people?
Ch. 3),213 it remains unclear why some horses
head-shake most when being ridden in certain The unwanted behavior and the way it has
ways, e.g. with a flexed poll. Sometimes the role evolved to its current form need to be clearly
of pain in emergent unwelcome behavior is not defined (Box 1.3). Given the apparent inconsisten-
revealed without analgesics. While one horse cies in the terminology and interpretation of
that is slow to leave from and quick to return to equine behavior,215 practitioners are commended
its yard may have become overly bonded to a to equine ethograms designed to establish stan-
companion, another may refuse to leave the yard dard nomenclature for equid behavior.216 Because
until placed on a course of anti-inflammatory of the possibility of inconsistent labeling of
drugs. The latter would be one of those horses behaviors,175,213 direct observation or video mate-
that have learned that being ridden, especially on rial is certainly preferable to an oral account of the
resilient surfaces, is reliably painful and therefore behavior. Problems in the ridden horse can be
should be avoided. fully appreciated only if the patient is ridden sad-
In behavioral terms, donkeys are not small dled and unsaddled, preferably on more than one
horses. Behavioral peculiarities in the donkey are occasion by different riders.
sometimes of clinical significance. Compared with
horses, donkeys give fewer overt signs of pain, Box 1.3 The unwanted behavior
tending instead to lie down or stand still with their
heads lowered. Sweating and hyperpnoea are • Description of what specifically happens
more common responses to abdominal pain than • Where does the behavior occur?
rolling and flank-kicking (Jane French, personal • When does the behavior occur?
• When was it first detected?
communication). Even during intestinal strangu- • What was the owner’s reaction?
lation or rupture, donkeys rarely lie down and roll • What advice has the owner received to date?
but instead show little more than inappetance, a • What problems has the owner encountered with any
attempts at correction?
mildly elevated heart rate and a vague reluctance • Has there been any change in the frequency or
to move.214 appearance of the problem?
Because behavior modification relies on being • What will be done with the horse if its behavior does not
improve?
able to redirect motivation that has prompted
INTRODUCTION 31
With a clear picture of the ontogeny and moti- 8. King SRB. Home range and habitat use of free-ranging
Przewalski horses at Hustai National Park, Mongolia.
vation of the unwelcome behavior, the develop- Appl Anim Behav Sci 2002; 78(2–4):103–113.
ment of an effective, tailored strategy to remove 9. Christensen JW, Zharkikh T, Ladewig J, Yasinetskaya N.
the reinforcement of the behavior and redirect the Social behaviour in stallion groups (Equus przewalskii
and Equus caballus) kept under natural and domestic
horse’s motivation is likely to emerge.217–220 This is conditions. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2002; 76(1):11–20.
behavior modification. Although a number of case 10. Goodwin D. The importance of ethology in under-
studies are presented in this book, readers may standing the behaviour of the horse. The role of the
horse in Europe. Equine Vet J Suppl 1999; 15–19.
find more exhaustive accounts of behavior modifi- 11. Levine MA. Social evolution and horse domestication.
cation elsewhere (for a series of 60 cases, see Why 1993; Oxbow Monograph 33:135–141.
does my horse … ?50). If applied with consistency, 12. Bokonyi S. Evolution of domesticated animals. London:
Longman; 1984.
the results of behavior modification programs are 13. Clutton-Brock J. Horse power: a history of the horse
rarely immediate in the horse, because of its innate and donkey in human societies. Cambridge, MA:
resistance to extinction78 but they are usually Harvard University Press; 1992.
14. Forsten A. Horse diversity through the ages. Biol Rev
impressive. There have often been years of incon- 1989; 64:279–304.
sistent and inappropriate handling which leave 15. Bibikova VI. Studies on ancient domestic horses in
habits that must be surmounted before the slate is Eastern Europe. Bjulleten Moscovskogo Obshchestva
Ispytatelei Prirody, Otdel Biologicheskii 1967;
clean enough for fresh lessons to be learned. It is 22:106–118.
therefore worth reminding owners that, because 16. Telegin DY, ed. Dereivka: a settlement and cemetery of
horses have excellent memories, ‘quick fixes’ are Copper Age horse keepers on the middle Dneiper
[Russian]. Pyatkovsky VK, transl; Mallory JP, ed.
generally a rarity in equine behavior therapy. Oxford: British Archaeological Reports International
Some apparently expeditious solutions may mask Series; 287; 1986.
pain, so they should be used with tremendous 17. Bibikova VI. A study of the earliest domestic horses of
Eastern Europe, parts 1 and 2, reprinted in ‘Dereivka: a
caution and only after the role of discomfort has settlement and cemetery of Copper Age horse keepers
been assessed. on the middle Dneiper’. Ed: Telegin DY. 1986. Oxford:
In the following chapters, we will explore the British Archaeological Reports International Series
1986; 287:135–162.
variety of innate and learned behaviors seen in 18. Richardson JD, Cripps PJ, Hillyer MH, et al.
horses. Given this information, the success of An evaluation of the accuracy of ageing horses by
behavior therapy depends largely on the versatil- their dentition: a matter of experience. Vet Rec 1995;
137(4):88–90.
ity of the practitioner and the compliance of the 19. Anthony DW, Brown DR. The origins of horseback
owner. riding. Antiquity 1991; 65:22–38.
20. Anthony D, Telegin DY, Brown D. The origin of
horseback riding. Scientific American 1991;
12:44–48.
REFERENCES 21. Vila C, Leonard JA, Gotherstrom A, et al. Widespread
origins of domestic horse lineages. Science 2001;
1. Waring GH. Horse behaviour: the behavioural traits and 291(5503):412.
adaptations of domestic and wild horses including 22. Anthony DW. The domestication of the horse. In:
ponies. Park Ridge, NJ: Noyes; 1983. Meadow RH, Oerpmann HP, eds. Equids in the ancient
2. Hatami-Monagazah H, Pandit RV. A cytogenetic study world, vol II. Wiesbaden: Rechart Verlag; 1991:
of the Caspian pony. Journal of Reproduction and 250–277.
Fertility 1979; 57:331–333. 23. Levine MA. Domestication, breed diversification and
3. Ryder OA. The chromosomes of a rare equine: early history of the horse. Havemeyer Workshop on
Equus hemionus kulan. Zoonooz 1977; 50:15. Horse Behavior and Welfare, Holar, Iceland 2002;
4. Budiansky S. The nature of horses – exploring equine 64–69.
evolution, intelligence and behavior. New York: Free 24. Ewers JC. The horse in Blackfoot Indian culture.
Press; 1997. Bureau of American Ethnology Bulletin 1955; 159.
5. Alexander RMcN, Trestik CL. Bistable properties of the 25. Lawrence EA. Horses in society: In: Rowan AN, ed.
hock joint of horses. J Zool 1989; 218:383–391. Animals and people sharing the world. Hanover, NH:
6. Mohr E. The Asiatic wild horse. London: Allen; 1971. University Press of New England; 1988:95–115.
7. Boyd L, Bandi N. Reintroduction of Takhi, Equus 26. McMiken DF. Ancient origins of horsemanship. Equine
ferus przewalskii, to Hustai National Park, Mongolia: Vet J 1990; 22:73–78.
time budget and synchrony of activity pre- and 27. Schulman AR. Egyptian representations of horsemen
post-release, Appl Anim Behav Sci 2002; and riding in the new kingdom. J Near East Studies
78(2–4):87–102. 1957; 16:263–271.
32 EQUINE BEHAVIOR
28. Moorey PRS. Pictorial evidence for the history of horse 51. Lagoni L, Butler C, Hetts S. The human–animal bond
riding in Iraq before the Kassite period. Iraq 1970; and grief. Philadelphia: WB Saunders; 1994.
32:36–50. 52. Digard J-P. Research in the social science of horses:
29. Hancar F. Das pferd in prähistorisches and frühen why? And how? The role of the horse in Europe.
historisches Zeit. Weiner Beitrage zur Kulturgeschichte Equine Vet J Suppl 1999; 28:56–57.
and Linguistik, 11. Vienna: Harold; 1955. 53. Brown D. Personality and gender influences on human
30. Azzaroli A. An early history of horsemanship. Leiden: relationships with horses and dogs. In: Anderson RK,
EJ Brill; 1985. Hart BL, Hart LA, eds. The pet connection: its influence
31. Cohen E. Animals in medieval perceptions, the image on our health and quality of life. Minneapolis: Center to
of ubiquitous other. In: Manning A, Serpell J, eds. Study Human–Animal Relationships and Environments,
Animals and human society, changing perspectives. University of Minnesota; 1984:216–223.
London: Routledge; 1994. 54. Burch MR, Bustad LK, Duncan SL. The role of pets in
32. Probst GF. The Kikkuli text on the training of horses therapeutic programmes. In: Robinson IH, ed. The
(ca. 1350 BC). Lexington: University of Kentucky, King Waltham book of human–animal interaction: benefits
Library Press; 1977. and responsibilities of pet ownership. Oxford:
33. Anderson JK. Ancient Greek horsemanship. Berkeley: Pergamon/Elsevier; 1995:55–69.
University of California Press; 1961. 55. Brackenridge SS, Shoemaker RS. The human-horse
34. Cohen C, Sivan D. The Ugaritic hippiaric texts: a bond and client bereavement in equine practice.
critical edition. New Haven, CT: American Oriental Equine Practice 1996; 18:19–22.
Society; 1983. 56. Rollin BE. Equine welfare and emerging social ethics.
35. Hyland A. Equus, the horse in the Roman world. Animal Welfare Forum: Equine Welfare. J Am Vet Med
London: Batsford; 1990. Assoc 2000; 216(8):1234–1237.
36. Endenburg N. Perceptions and attitudes toward horses 57. Ewbank R. Contribution of ethology to clinical
in European societies. The role of the horse in Europe. interpretation of the horse’s welfare. Equine Vet J 1985;
Equine Vet J Suppl 1999; 28:38–41. 17:1, 2–3.
37. Cunningham P. The genetics of Thoroughbred horses. 58. Rees L. The horse’s mind. London: Stanley Paul; 1984.
Scientific American 1991; 5:56–62. 59. Dorrance T. True unity. Tuscarora, NB: Give-it-a-go
38. Mellor DJ, Love S, Reeves MJ, et al. A demographic Enterprises; 1987.
approach to equine disease in the northern UK through 60. Parelli P. Natural horsemanship. Colorado Springs, CO:
a sentinel practice network. Epidemiol Sante Anim Western Horseman; 1995.
1997; 31–32. 61. Hunt R. Think Harmony with Horses. Fresno, CA:
39. Robinson IH. The human-horse relationship: how much Pioneer; 1995.
do we know? The role of the horse in Europe. Equine 62. Fregin GF, Thomas DP. Cardiovascular response to
Vet J Suppl 1999; 28:42–45. exercise: a review. In: Snow DH, Persson SGB, Rose
40. Goergen R. Life on the edge. New Scientist 2001; RJ, eds. Equine exercise physiology. Cambridge:
2305:30–33. Burlington; 1983.
41. Cothran EG, van Dyk E, van der Merwe FJ. Genetic 63. Gaffney B, Cunningham PE. Estimation of genetic trend
variation in the feral horses of the Namib Desert, in racing performance of Thoroughbred horses. Nature
Namibia. J S Afr Vet Med Assoc 2001; 72(1):18–22. 1988; 322(6166):722–724.
42. Clayton HM. Performance in equestrian sports. In: 64. Gable AA. Metabolic bone disease: problems of
Back W, Clayton HM, eds. Equine locomotion. London: terminology. Equine Vet J 1988; 20:4–6.
WB Saunders; 2001. 65. Jeffcott LB. Osteochondroses in the horse – searching
43. Holstrom M, Magnusson LE, Philipsson J. Variation in for the key to pathogeneses. Equine Vet J 1991;
conformation of Swedish warmblood horses and 23:331–338.
conformational characteristics of elite sport horses. 66. Galloux P, Richard N, Dronka T, et al. Analysis of
Equine Vet J 1990; 22:186–193. equine gait using three dimensional accelerometers
44. Holstrom M. The effects of conformation. In: Back W, fixed on the saddle. Equine Vet J Suppl 1994;
Clayton HM, eds. Equine Locomotion. London: WB 17:44–47.
Saunders; 2001. 67. Arabian AK, Clayton HM. Modeling of locomotion. In:
45. Hayes KN. Temperament tip-offs. Horse and Rider Back W, Clayton HM, eds. Equine locomotion. London:
1998; 37:46–51. WB Saunders; 2001: 365–371.
46. Magnusson LE. Studies on the conformation and the 68. Herr HM, McMahon TA. A galloping horse model. Int J
related traits of standardbred trotters in Sweden. PhD Robotics Res 2001; 20(1):26–37.
thesis, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, 69. Walker RE. Roman veterinary medicine. In: Animals in
Skara, 1985. Roman life and art. London: Thames & Hudson; 1973.
47. Sellet LC, Albert WW, Groppel JL. Forelimb kinematics 70. Toynbee J. Animals in Roman life and art, p 343.
of the Standardbred pacing gait. Proc Equine Nutr London: Thames & Hudson; 1973.
Physiol 1981; 210–215. 71. Smith F. The early history of veterinary literature, vol 3,
48. Akhatova IA. A system for breeding horses for dairy p 17. Reprinted London: JA Allen; 1976.
purposes. Zootekhniya 1995; 10:13–15. 72. Holcombe SF, Jackson C, Gerber V, et al. Stabling is
49. Fraser AF. The behaviour of the horse. London: CAB associated with airway inflammation in young Arabian
International; 1992. horses. Equine Vet J 2001; 33(3):244–249.
50. McGreevy PD. Why does my horse …? London: 73. Hintz HF. Hay racks vs feeding hay on the stall floor.
Souvenir Press; 1996. Equine Practice 1997; 19:5–6.
INTRODUCTION 33
74. Townson J, Dodd VA, Brophy PO. A survey and 93. Duncan IJH. An ethological approach to the welfare of
assessment of racehorse stables in Ireland. Irish Vet J farm animals. In: Proceedings 19th International
1995; 48:364–372. Ethological Conference, Toulouse, France,
75. Murray MJ, Eichorn ES. Effects of intermittent feed 1985:295–305.
deprivation, intermittent feed deprivation with ranitidine 94. Marsden MD. An investigation of the heredity of
administration, and stall confinement with ad libitum susceptibility of stereotypic behaviour pattern –
access to hay on gastric ulceration in horses. Am J Vet stable vices—in the horse. Equine Vet J 1995;
Res 1996; 11:1599–1603. 27(6):415.
76. Harris PA. Review of equine feeding and stable 95. Ralston SL, van den Brock G, Baile CA. Feed intake
management practices in the UK concentrating on the patterns and associated blood glucose free fatty acid
last decade of the 20th century. The role of the horse in and insulin changes in ponies. J Anim Sci 1979;
Europe. Equine Vet J Suppl 1999; 46–54. 49:838–845.
77. Mills DS, Eckley S, Cooper JJ. Thoroughbred bedding 96. Ralston SL. Feeding behaviour. Vet Clin North Am
preferences, associated behaviour differences and their Equine Pract: Behav 1986; 2:609–621.
implications for equine welfare. Anim Sci 2000; 97. McBane S. Behaviour problems in horses, p 304.
70(1):95–106. London: David & Charles; 1987.
78. Kiley-Worthington M. The behaviour of horses: in 98. Ruckebusch Y. The hypnogram as an index of
relation to management and training. London: JA Allen; adaptation of farm animals to changes in their
1987. environment. Appl Anim Ethol 1975; 2:3–8.
79. Houpt KA. Equine welfare. In: Recent advances in 99. Dallaire A. Rest behaviour. Vet Clin North Am Equine
companion animal behavior problems. Ithaca, NY: Pract: Behav 1986; 2:591–607.
International Veterinary Information Services; 2001. 100. Dallaire A, Ruckebusch Y. Sleep and wakefulness in
A0810. 1201. the housed pony under different dietary conditions.
80. Hinton MH. On the watering of horses – a review. Can J Compar Med 1974; 38:65–71.
Equine Vet J 1978; 10:27–31. 101. McGreevy PD, Cripps PJ, French NP, et al.
81. UFAW (Scott WN, ed). The care and management of Management factors associated with stereotypic and
farm animals. London: Baillière Tindall; 1978. redirected behaviour in the Thoroughbred horse.
82. Ensminger ME. Horses and horsemanship. Danville, IL: Equine Vet J 1995; 27:86–91.
Interstate; 1969. 102. Summerhays RS. The problem horse, p 93. London:
83. Evans JW, Borton A, Hintz HF, van Vleck LD. The JA Allen; 1975.
horse. San Francisco: WH Freeman; 1969. 103. Houpt KA, Hintz HF, Pagan JD. The responses of
84. Jones RD, McGreevy PD, Robertson A, et al. Survey of mares and their foals to brief separation. J Anim Sci
the designs of racehorse stables in the South-west of 1981; 53:129.
England. Equine Vet J 1987; 19:454–457. 104. Houpt KA, Hintz HF, Butler WR. A preliminary study of
85. Sainsbury DWB, Sainsbury P. Livestock health and two methods of weaning foals. Appl Anim Behav Sci
housing, 2nd edn. London: Baillière Tindall; 1979. 1984; 12:177–1881.
86. Cooper JJ, McDonald L, Mills DS. The effect of 105. McCall CA, Potter GD, Kreider JL. Locomotor, vocal
increasing visual horizons on stereotypic weaving: and other behavioural responses to varying methods
implications for the social housing of stabled horses. of weaning foals. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1985;
Appl Anim Behav Sci 2000; 69:67–83. 14:27–35.
87. Mills DS, Davenport K. The effect of a neighbouring 106. Mal ME, Friend TH, Lay DC, et al. Physiological
conspecific versus the use of a mirror for the control of responses of mares to short-term confinement and
stereotypic weaving behaviour in the stabled horse. isolation. J Equine Vet Sci 1991; 11:96–102.
Anim Sci 2002; 74:95–101. 107. Keiper RR. Social structure. Vet Clin North Am Equine
88. Ratner SC, Boice R. Effects of domestication on Pract: Behav 1986; 2:465–484.
behaviour. In: Hafez ESE, ed. The behaviour of 108. Feh C, de Mazieres J. Grooming at a preferred site
domestic animals. London: Baillière Tindall; reduces heart rate in horses. Anim Behav 1993;
1975:3–19. 46:1191–1194.
89. Rossdale PD. Modern stud management in relation to 109. Dougherty DM, Lewis P. Generalization of a tactile
the oestrus cycle and fertility of Thoroughbred mares. stimulus in horses. J Exp Anal Behav 1993;
Equine Vet J 1968; 2:65–72. 59:521–528.
90. Harman AM, Moore S, Hoskins R, Keller P. Horse vision 110. Zeitler-Feicht MH, Buschmann S. [Are standing
and an explanation of the visual behaviour originally stalls for horses acceptable today with regard to
explained by the ‘ramp retina’. Equine Vet J 1999; animal welfare?] [German] Pferdeheilkunde 2002;
31(5):384–390. 18(5):431.
91. Wiepkema PR. Umwelt and animal welfare. In: Baxter 111. Houpt K, Houpt TR, Johnson JL, et al. The effect of
SH, Baxter MR, MacCormack JAC, eds. Farm animal exercise deprivation on the behaviour and physiology
housing and welfare. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff; of straight-stall confined pregnant mares. Anim
1983:45–51. Welfare 2001; 10(3):257–267.
92. Dawkins MS. The current status of preference tests in 112. Rivera E, Benjamin S, Nielsen B, et al. Behavioral
the assessment of animal welfare. In: Baxter SH, and physiological responses of horses to initial
Baxter MR, MacCormack JAC, eds. Farm animal training: the comparison between pastured versus
housing and welfare. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff; stalled horses. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2002;
1983: 20–26. 78(2–4):235–252.
34 EQUINE BEHAVIOR
113. Nicol CJ. Behavioural responses of laying hens 135. Lieberman DA. Learning: behavior and cognition.
following a period of spatial restriction. Anim Behav Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole; 1993.
1987; 35:1709–1719. 136. Murray MJ, Crowell-Davis S. Psychogenic colic in a
114. Marsden MD. Feeding practices have greater effect horse. J Am Vet Med Assoc 1985; 186:381–383.
than housing practices on the behaviour and welfare 137. Beaver B. Aggressive behaviour problems. Vet Clin
of the horse. Livestock Environment IV, 4th North Am Equine Pract: Behav 1986; 2:609–621.
International Symposium of the American Society of 138. Zeitler-Feicht MH, Prantner V. Recumbence resting
Agricultural Engineers, University of Warwick, behaviour of horses in loose housing systems with
Coventry, 1993:314–318. open yards. Arch Tierzucht 2000; 43(4):327–335.
115. Flannigan G, Stookey JM. Day-time time budgets of 139. Mason GJ. Stereotypies: a critical review. Anim
pregnant mares housed in tie stalls: a comparison of Behav 1991; 41:1015–1037.
draft versus light mares. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2002; 140. Xenopohon. The art of horsemanship (400 BC).
78(2–4):125–143. Morgan MH, transl. London: JA Allen; 1999.
116. Racklyeft DJ, Love DN. Influence of head posture on 141. Clutton-Brock J, Davies S. More donkeys from Tell
the respiratory tract of healthy horses. Aust Vet J Brak. Iraq 1993; 55:209–221.
1990; 67:402–405. 142. Bahn PG. Crib-biting: tethered horses in the
117. Rose RJ. The TG Hungerford Vade Mecum series for Palaeolithic? World Archaeology 1980; 12:
domestic animals. Number 1: Horses, p 24. Sydney, 212–217.
Australia: Post-graduate Committee in Veterinary 143. Lawson A. The modern farrier, p 47. London:
Science; 1983. Newcastle, Mackenzie & Dent; 1828.
118. Richardson DW. Degenerative joint disease. 144. Youatt W. The horse, p 448–451. London: Charles
In: Robinson NE, ed. Current therapy in equine Knight; 1846.
medicine, Philadelphia: WB Saunders; 145. Lupton JI. Evils of modern stables. In: Mayhew’s
1992:137–140. illustrated horse management. London: WH Allen;
119. Schramm U. The trouble with horses, p 124. Bury 1884.
St Edmunds, Suffolk: JA Allen; 1988. 146. Holzapfel M. Uber Bewegungsstereotypien bei
120. Hayes MH. Veterinary notes for horse-owners. gehalten Sangetieren; 2. Mitteilung: das Weben der
London: Stanley Paul; 1968. pferde. Z Tierpsychol 1939; 60–72.
121. Fraser A, Manolson F. Fraser’s horse book. London: 147. Boyd LE. Behaviour problems of equids in zoos.
Pan; 1979. Vet Clin North Am Equine Pract: Behav 1986;
122. Farm Animal Welfare Council second report on 2:653–664.
priorities for research and development in farm animal 148. Meyer-Holzapfel M. Abnormal behavior in zoo
welfare. MAFF, Tolworth; 1993. animals. In: Fox MW, eds. Abnormal behavior
123. Webster AJF. Animal welfare: a cool eye towards in animals. Philadelphia: WB Saunders; 1968:
Eden. London: Blackwell Science; 1994. 476–503.
124. Summerhays RS. The problem horse. London: 149. Sambraus HH, Rappold D. Crib-biting and wind-
JA Allen, 1959. sucking in horses. Pferdeheilkunde 1991; 7:211–216.
125. Hamilton S. The vice squad: conquering your horse’s 150. Mills DS, Alston RD, Rogers V, Longford NT. Factors
behavioural quirks. Equus 1980; 32:68–71. associated with the prevalence of stereotypic
126. Fraser AF, Broom DM. Farm animal behaviour and behaviour amongst thoroughbred horses passing
welfare. London: Baillière Tindall, 1990. through auctioneer sales. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2002;
127. Francis-Smith K, Wood-Gush DGM. Coprophagia as 78 (2–4):115–124.
seen in Thoroughbred foals. Equine Vet J 1977; 151. Nicol CJ. Stereotypies and their relation to
9:155–157. management. In: Harris PA, Gomarsall GM, Davidson
128. Crowell-Davis SL, Houpt KA. Coprophagia by foals: HPB, Green RE, eds. Proceedings of the BEVA
effects of age and possible functions. Equine Vet J specialist days on behaviour and nutrition,
1985; 17:17–19. 1999:11–14.
129. Crowell-Davis SL, Caudle AB. Coprophagy by foals: 152. Mason GJ. Forms of stereotypic behaviour. In:
recognition of maternal faeces. Appl Anim Behav Sci Lawrence AB, Rushen J, eds. Stereotypic animal
1989; 24:267–272. behaviour: fundamentals and applications to welfare.
130. Crowell-Davis SL, Houpt KA, Carnevale JM. Feeding London: CAB International; 1993.
and drinking behaviour of mares and foals with free 153. Broom DM. Animal welfare: concepts and
access to pasture and water. J Anim Sci 1985; measurements. J Anim Sci 1991; 69:4167–4175.
60:883–889. 154. Cooper JJ, Nicol CJ. Stereotypic behaviour affects
131. Krzak WE, Gonyou HW, Lawrence LM. Wood-chewing environmental preference in bank voles,
by stabled horses: diurnal pattern and effects of Clethrionomys glareolus. Anim Behav 1991;
exercise. J Anim Sci (USA) 1991; 69:1053–1058. 41:971–977.
132. McCall CA. Wood-chewing by horses. Equine Practice 155. Fentress JC. Dynamic boundaries of patterned
1993; 15:35–36. behaviour: interaction and self-organization.
133. Bush K. The problem horse – an owner’s guide, In: Bateson PPG, Hinde RA, eds. Growing points in
p 160. Marlborough, Wilts, UK: Crowood Press; 1992. ethology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press;
134. Buntain BJ, Coffmann JR. Polyuria and polydipsia in a 1976:135–167.
horse induced by psychogenic salt consumption. 156. Levine MA, Weinberg J, Ursin H. Definition of the
Equine Vet J 1981; 13:266–268. coping process and statement of the problem.
INTRODUCTION 35
In: Ursin H, Baade E, Levine S, eds. Psychobiology 171. Luescher UA, McKeown DB, Dean H. A cross-
of stress: a study of coping men. New York: Academic sectional study on compulsive behaviour (stable vices)
Press; 1978:3–21. in horses. Equine Vet J Suppl 1998; 27:14–18.
157. McBride G. Adaptation and welfare at the man–animal 172. Redbo I, Redbo-Tortensson P, Ödberg FO, et al.
interface. In: Wodzickka M, Edey TN, Lynch JJ, eds. Factors affecting behavioural disturbances in race-
Reviews in rural science IV. Armidale, NSW: University horses. Anim Sci 1998; 66:475-481.
of New England; 1980. 173. Prince D. Stable vices. In: McBane S, ed. Behaviour
158. Wood-Gush DGM, Stolba A, Miller C. Exploration in problems in horses. London: David & Charles; 1987.
farm animals and animal husbandry. In: Archer J, 174. Vecchiotti G, Galanti R. Evidence of heredity of
Birke LIA, eds. Exploration in animals and humans. cribbing, weaving and stall-walking in Thoroughbred
London: Van Nostrand Reinhold; 1983:198–209. horses. Livestock Production Sci 1986; 14:91–95.
159. Kennes D, de Rycke PH. Influences of performance of 175. McGreevy PD. The functional significance of
stereotypies on plasma corticosterone and leucocyte stereotypies in the stabled horse. PhD thesis,
levels in the bank vole (Clethrionomys glareolus). University of Bristol, UK; 1995.
In: Proceedings of the International Congress on 176. Ödberg FO. The jumping stereotypy in the bank vole
Applied Ethology in Farm Animals, 1988:238–240. (Clethrionomys glareolus). Biol Behav 1986;
160. Terlouw EMC, Lawrence AB, Ladewig J, et al. 11:130–143.
Relationship between plasma cortisol and stereotypic 177. Morgan MJ. Effects of post-weaning environment on
activities in pigs. Behav Processes 1991; 25:133–153. learning in the rat. Anim Behav 1973; 21:429–442.
161. Würbel H, Stauffacher M. Prevention of stereotypy in 178. Appleby MC, Lawrence AB. Food restriction as a
laboratory mice: effects on stress physiology and cause of stereotypic behaviour in tethered gilts.
behaviour. Physiol Behav 1996; 59(6):1163–1170. J Anim Production 1987; 45:103–110.
162. McGreevy PD, Nicol CJ. Behavioural and 179. Duncan IJH, Rushen J, Lawrence AB. Conclusions
physiological consequences associated with the and implications for animal welfare. In: Lawrence AB,
short-term prevention of crib-biting in horses. Physiol Rushen J, eds. Stereotypic animal behaviour:
Behav 1998; 65(1):15–23. fundamentals and applications to welfare. London:
163. McBride SD, Cuddeford D. The putative welfare- CAB International; 1993.
reducing effects of preventing equine stereotypic 180. Hosoda T. On the heritability of susceptibility to
behaviour. Anim Welfare 2001; 10(2):173–189. windsucking in horses. Jpn J Zootechnical Sci 1950;
164. Lebelt D, Zanella AJ, Unshelm J. Physiological 21:25–28.
correlates associated with cribbing behaviour in 181. McGreevy PD, French NP, Nicol CJ. The prevalence of
horses: changes in thermal threshold, heart rate, abnormal behaviours in dressage, eventing and
plasma beta-endorphin and serotonin. Equine Vet J endurance horses in relation to stabling. Vet Rec
Suppl 1998; 27:21–27. 1995; 137:36–37.
165. Minero M, Canali E, Ferrante V, Verga M, Ödberg FO. 182. Luescher UA, McKeown DB, Halip J. Reviewing the
Heart rate and behavioural responses of crib-biting causes of obsessive-compulsive disorders in horses.
horses to two acute stressors. Vet Rec 1999; Equine Pract Vet Med 1991; 5(91):527–530.
145(15):430–433. 183. Cooper JJ, Nicol CJ. Neighbour effects on the
166. Gillham SR, Dodman NH, Shuster L, et al. The effect development of locomotor stereotypies in bank voles
of diet on cribbing behaviour and plasma ß-endorphin Clethrionomys glareolus. Anim Behav 1994; 47:
in horses. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1994; 41:147–153. 214–216.
167. Dodman NH, Shuster L, Court MH, Dixon R. 184. Houpt KA, McDonnell SM. Equine stereotypies. The
Investigation into the use of narcotic antagonists in Compendium of Continuing Education for the
the treatment of a stereotypic behavior pattern (crib- Practicing Veterinarian 1993; 15:1265–1272.
biting) in the horse. Am J Vet Res 1987; 48:2,311–319. 185. Baer KL, Potter GD, Friend TH, Beaver BV.
168. McBride S. A comparison of physical and Observation effects on learning in horses. Appl Anim
pharmacological treatments for stereotypic behaviour Ethol 1983; 11:123–129.
in the horse. In: Duncan IJH, Widowski TM, Haley DB, 186. Baker AEM, Crawford BH. Observational learning in
eds. Proceedings of the 30th International Congress horses. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1986; 15:7–13.
for the International Society of Applied Ethology. 187. Clarke J, Nicol CJ, Jones R, McGreevy PD. Effects of
Guelph, Canada: CSAW;1996. observational learning on food selection in horses.
169. Garner JP, Mason GJ, Broadbent H. Cage stereotypy Appl Anim Behav Sci 1996; 50:177–184.
in Blue tits (Parus caeruleus) and Marsh tits (Parus 188. Waters AJ, Nicol CJ, French NP. Factors influencing
palustris) is associated with a deficit in the inhibition the development of stereotypic and redirected
of non-functional behaviour. In: Veissier I, Boissy A, behaviours in young horses: findings of a four year
eds. Proceedings of the 32nd International Congress prospective epidemiological study. Equine Vet J 2002;
of the ISAE. Clermont-Ferrand, France: INRA; 34(6):572–579.
1998:56. 189. Heleski CR, Shelle AC, Nielsen BD, Zanella AJ.
170. Garner JP, Meehan CL, Mench JA. Stereotypic parrots Influence of housing on weanling horse behavior and
fail the same psychiatric task as stereotypic autists subsequent welfare. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2002;
and schizophrenics. In: Garner JP, Mench JA, 78(2–4):297–308.
Heekin S, eds. Proceedings of the 35th International 190. Rosenzweig MR, Bennett EL. Psychobiology of
Congress of the ISAE. Davis, CA: Center For Animal plasticity: effects of training and experience on brain
Welfare, University of California at Davis; 2001. and behaviour. Behav Brain Res 1996; 78:57–65.
36 EQUINE BEHAVIOR
191. Bell RA, Nielsen BD, Waite K, et al. Daily access to 207. Mills DS. Personality and individual differences in the
pasture turnout prevents loss of mineral in the third horse, their significance, use and measurement.
metacarpus of Arabian weanlings. J Anim Sci 2001; Equine Vet J Suppl 1998; 27:10–13.
79(5):1142–1150. 208. Taylor K, Cook S, Mills DS. A case-controlled study
192. Keiper RR. Causal factors of stereotypies in caged investigating health, management and behavioural
birds. Anim Behav 1969; 17:114–117. features of horses commonly described as
193. Dantzer R, Mormede P. De-arousal properties of headshakers. Havemeyer Workshop on Horse
stereotypic behaviour: evidence from pituitary-adrenal Behavior and Welfare, 2002: 16–20.
correlates in pigs. Appl Anim Ethol 1983; 10:233–244. 209. Morris PH, Gale A, Duffy K. Can judges agree on the
194. Cronin GM, Wiepkema PR, Van Ree JM. Endogenous personality of horses? Personality and Individual
opioids are involved in abnormal stereotyped Differences 2002; 33(1):67–81.
behaviour in tethered sows. Neuropeptides 1985; 210. Visser EK, van Reenen CG, Rundgren M, et al.
6:527. Responses of horses in behavioural tests correlate
195. Cooper JJ, Ödberg FO. The emancipation of with temperament assessed by riders. Equine
stereotypies with age. In: Appleby MC, Horrell RI, Vet J 2003; 35(2):176–183.
Petherick JC, Rutter SM, eds. Applied Animal 211. Casey R. Recognising the importance of pain in the
behaviour: past, present and future. Hertfordshire: diagnosis of equine behaviour problems. In: Harris PA,
UFAW; 1991:142. Gomarsall GM, Davidson HPB, Green RE, eds.
196. Straiton EC. The TV vet horse book, p 193. Suffolk: Proceedings of the BEVA Specialist Days on
Farming Press; 1973. Behaviour and Nutrition, 1999:25–28.
197. Steele DG. El Cribbing (pica) y la aerofagia en los pura 212. Houpt KA. Domestic animal behaviour for
sangre. Ganaderia 1960; 18:74–78. veterinarians and animal scientists, 3rd edn. London:
198. Karlander S, Mansson J, Tufvesson G. Buccostomy as Manson; 1998.
a method of treatment for aerophagia (wind-sucking) 213. Taylor KD, Cook S, Mills DS. A case-controlled study
in the horse. Nordisk Veterinär Medicin 1965; investigating health, management and behavioural
17:455–458. features of horses commonly described as head-
199. Hachten W. Cribbing treatment. The Equine Athlete shakers. Ippologia 2001; 12:29–37.
1995; 8:20–21. 214. Taylor TS, Matthews NS. Mammoth asses – selected
200. Baker GJ, Kear-Colwell J. Aerophagia (windsucking) behavioural considerations for the veterinarian. Appl
and aversion therapy in the horse. Proc Am Assoc Anim Behav Sci 1998. 60(2–3):283–289.
Equine Practitioners 1974; 20:127–130. 215. Houpt KA, Rudman R. Foreword to special issue on
201. Hamm D. A new surgical procedure to control crib- equine behaviour. Appl Anim Behav Sci
biting. Proc Am Assoc Equine Practitioners 1977; 2002;78(2–4):83–85.
23:301–302. 216. McDonnell SM. The equid ethogram: a practical field
202. Firth EC. Bilateral ventral accessory neurectomy in guide to horse behavior. Lexington, KY: The Blood
windsucking horses. Vet Rec 1980; 106:30–32. Horse; 2003.
203. Hakansson A, Franzen P, Petersson H. Comparison of 217. McCall CA. A review of learning behavior in horses
two surgical methods for treatment of crib-biting in and its application in horse training. J Anim Sci 1990;
horses. Equine Vet J 1992; 24:494–496. 68:75–81.
204. Broom DM, Kennnedy MJ. Stereotypies in horses: 218. Mills DS. Applying learning theory to the management
their relation to welfare and causation. Equine Vet Ed of the horse: the difference between getting it right
1993; 5:151–154. and getting it wrong. Equine Vet J Suppl 1998;
205. McBride SD, Long L. Management of horses showing 27:44–48.
stereotypic behaviour, owner perception and the 219. Cooper JJ. Comparative learning theory and its
implications for welfare. Vet Rec 2001; application in the training of horses. Equine Vet J
148(26):799–802. Suppl 1998; 27:39–43.
206. Goodwin D, Davidson HPB, Harris P. Foraging 220. Barakat C, McGreevy PD. How cribbing takes hold.
enrichment for stabled horses: effects on behaviour Equus 2002; 297:34–43.
and selection. Equine Vet J 2002; 34(7):686–691.
CHAPTER CONTENTS
2
Vision 37
Acuity 38
Depth perception 42
Stimulus visibility 42
Perception
Accommodation 43
Color vision 44
Foal vision 45
Problems with vision 46
Chemoreception 46
Smell 46
Taste 48
Hearing 49
Touch 50
VISION
(a) (b)
Figure 2.4 (a) The visual field in front of a horse when allowed to carry its head naturally. (b) The blind area in front of a horse
when over-bent. ((b) Reproduced with permission of the Captive Animals Protection Society.)
(d)
Figure 2.5 The visual field of one eye of a horse. Horse and human – (a) rear view, (b) front view – are standing looking out
over Perth from a viewpoint in Kings Park. So in front of them they see Perth and to the back of them is a man taking a photo
of Perth. The human view (c) is of Perth itself and not much more, since our vision is so frontal. Also we see just the very middle in
high acuity. The horse (d) by contrast, sees Perth and everything else simultaneously right back to the man taking the photograph.
(Reproduced with permission of Alison Harman.)
PERCEPTION 41
(a)
(b) (c)
(e)
42 EQUINE BEHAVIOR
lateral vision to get a better look at jumps as they horizon, enabling scanning and depth perception.
get up close. Perhaps this is why blinkers have In this position monocular lateral vision is com-
found little favor in show-jumping circles, the promised. However, when the head is lowered
traditional source of a multiplicity of gadgets. and the binocular vision is directed at the area
Blinkers are most effective in preventing shying directly in front of the head, the lateral monocular
and have been favored by carriage drivers because fields afford good lateral horizon vision.
they make the horses less likely to attempt to turn Effective use of the binocular field is required
in the shafts or bolt. It is also suggested that blink- when a horse attempts to discern an object that is
ers can render a horse more responsive to voice close and low. The horse is best able to use its
commands used to increase speed because they binocular field of view by arching the neck and
prevent it from seeing when the driver is not carry- rotating the head. It can focus on the object by
ing or about to use a whip (Les Holmes, personal simultaneous rotation of the eye downward to
communication 2002). Blinkers on racing animals optimize orientation of the visual streak (see p. 43).
bring rather different benefits – especially, it seems,
when used for the first time. It has been suggested
that, if it is a generally low-ranking animal, a race-
horse that sees another horse approaching from
STIMULUS VISIBILITY
behind is more likely to defer to the challenger Factors that affect the visibility of a stimulus for a
if not wearing blinkers. This assumes that more horse include size of the object, contrast and envir-
dominant horses are more motivated to assume onmental illumination. When a moving horse spots
the lead in a galloping herd, a hypothesis that has something underfoot, it not only looks at the stim-
yet to be tested. ulus, but is also likely to change speed.12 The level
of arousal plays a part in the recognition of stimuli,
an outcome that may be influenced by the horse’s
age and training because recognition of distant
DEPTH PERCEPTION stimuli on the ground is facilitated by carrying the
It was long believed that animals with laterally head at a lower angle. Saslow12 found that younger
placed eyes and extensive monocular visual fields animals tend to carry their heads higher and there-
did not have stereopsis – the ability to see in stereo fore may not notice stimuli as readily as older
and perceive depth. However, recent studies10 horses, especially those that have been trained not
have demonstrated that the horse’s binocular field to carry their neck straight and head high.
is an arc of approximately 60° in front of the head, Saslow12 also found that horses were able to
affording good stereopsis and thresholds of depth discern stimuli better in overcast rather than bright
detection comparable to those of cats and pigeons. sunny conditions. This suggests that the equine
These findings indicate that larger interocular dis- rod-dominated eye may not find bright conditions
tances, as found in the horse, may provide useful as favorable as dull conditions. The high propor-
depth judgment. This may have arisen because tion of rods to cones (generally 20:1)13 gives the
the horse has evolved to make judgments over a horse excellent night vision but insufficient to
range of several meters, whereas ground-feeding make horses innately fearless of areas that are
birds such as pigeons, with an extremely small poorly lit. As we will see in Chapter 4, horses will
interocular distance, have to focus at a distance of work to keep a stable illuminated, and this helps
only a few centimeters. Horses also use monocular to explain some of the aversiveness of small dark
depth cues to judge distance.4 This makes sense spaces, including trailers (floats in the USA) (Fig.
because they spend so much of their day with their 2.7). A reflective layer of cells behind the retina, the
heads to the ground, a position that makes tapetum, enhances this. Acting like a mirror, the
stereopsis redundant. tapetum reflects light back on to the retina,
Harman et al11 suggest that when a horse lifts its enabling further light to be gathered. The down-
head the binocular area of vision is directed at the side of this arrangement is that the image is
Exploring the Variety of Random
Documents with Different Content
T/243088 Webb, F.
G/2367 Webb, J. G.
S/10901 Webb, J. A.
G/9076 Webb, L.
L/8856 Webb, P.
G/18821 Webb, P. J.
G/26568 Webb, S. C.
G/10344 Webb, T. W.
G/7070 Webb, W. H.
G/21207 Webber, H. G.
G/13181 Webster, J.
T/203970 Webster, W.
G/13259 Weeding, J.
T/203563 Weeks, G.
G/21288 Weeks, G.
G/5207 Weeks, R. G.
L/8171 Weeks, T. E.
S/272 Welch, W.
T/243305 Welford, W. S.
G/15708 Wellband, N. K.
G/1354 Wellard, G. J.
L/7998 Weller, A. A.
T/203968 Weller, J. E.
L/9498 Wells, A.
G/5747 Wells, E. G.
S/889 Wells, F. H.
G/9685 Wells, F.
L/9611 Wells, F. C.
G/188 Wells, G. F.
G/18977 Wells, J. W.
L/8074 Wells, S. J.
G/1395 Welsh, W. F.
G/356 Welton, F. C.
L/8122 Wenban, F. H.
G/6366 Wenham, J.
G/8118 Wenman, A. H.
G/4129 Wenman, H.
T/3071 West, B. G.
G/29256 West, C. W.
G/70 West, E. F.
G/25652 West, F. W.
G/5613 Westbrook, W.
G/17392 Westlake, H. G. S.
L/5748 Weston, F. J.
G/7068 Whale, R. F.
G/26642 Wheatley, A.
L/10276 Wheatley, A. J.
T/206156 Wheatley, H.
G/3334 Wheeler, C.
G/8574 Wheeler, F. G.
G/392 Wheeler, I. A.
L/10209 Whenham, H. S.
L/8620 Whiddett, G.
G/6843 Whitby, G. E.
G/8624 White, E. E.
L/7918 White, G. T.
G/20008 White, J. A.
G/13260 White, J.
G/6889 White, J. F.
G/17491 White, L. V.
G/21833 White, L. S.
G/20177 White, N. E.
G/2559 White, P.
G/17471 White, R. W.
S/245 White, R. H.
G/1987 White, W. E.
G/5454 White, W.
L/9440 White, W. D.
L/9522 White, W. J.
S/10367 White, W. T.
T/1167 Whitehead, A.
G/11100 Whitehead, J.
G/19030 Whitehead, L.
G/11669 Whiteley, G.
G/22145 Whiteman, W.
G/12940 Whiting, C. A.
L/8811 Whittingham, C. E.
G/404 Whittingham, G. H.
G/14889 Whittington, G. W.
G/3962 Whittington, H.
L/9347 Whyatt, S. A.
S/334 Whybrow, W. J.
T/2103 Wickens, F.
L/10039 Wicker, H.
T/1160 Wickham, T. H.
G/20183 Wiffen, E.
G/2286 Wiffen, T.
G/2900 Wigg, G. A.
G/14557 Wigg, W.
T/204112 Wilber, T.
G/13959 Wilbraham, W. J.
G/8005 Wilcox, A. H.
G/12863 Wiles, A.
G/5721 Wiles, H.
G/2841 Wiles, L. W.
G/18677 Wiles, P. H.
G/1774 Wilkins, H.
L/8100 Wilkins, J.
G/6523 Wilkinson, E. W.
G/1124 Wilkinson, W. J.
G/308 Willard, J.
G/9030 Willard, M. W.
G/1551 Willard, R.
G/2227 Willett, H.
L/9323 Willey, C. F.
G/25565 Williams, A. J.
T/1422 Williams, B. J.
L/10316 Williams, C.
G/1087 Williams, C. D.
G/4178 Williams, E.
G/12719 Williams, E. A.
L/8974 Williams, E. C.
G/405 Williams, E. H.
G/21199 Williams, F. A.
G/20054 Williams, G.
T/206029 Williams, G.
S/249 Williams, H. E.
G/14256 Williams, J.
L/9961 Williams, J.
L/6049 Williams, J. A.
G/6504 Williams, P.
G/1107 Williams, T.
L/9690 Williams, T. D.
G/6834 Williams, W.
G/17477 Williams, W. C.
S/8322 Williams, W. G.
G/26567 Williams, W. S.
G/1664 Williamson, H.
G/40 Willis, H.
L/10511 Willis, J.
G/4676 Willis, T.
G/23944 Willis, T.
G/3955 Willis, W.
G/8917 Willmore, G. H.
G/18871 Willmott, F.
G/17407 Willoughby, L. J.
G/9020 Wilshire, C. E.
G/17 Wilson, A.
G/3445 Wilson, A.
G/1084 Wilson, A. E.
G/23943 Wilson, F. W.
G/29262 Wilson, H.
G/303 Wilson, H.
G/9888 Wilson, H.
G/6660 Wilson, H.
L/10605 Wilson, H. P.
G/5506 Wilson, H. W.
S/10777 Wilson, J. C.
G/17385 Wilson, R.
G/5671 Wilson, R. H.
G/4820 Wilson, S. H.
G/14565 Wilson, T.
G/11866 Wilson, W.
S/10848 Wiltshire, H. W.
L/10070 Winch, J.
G/13289 Winch, O. E.
G/1355 Winchcombe, A. J.
G/5247 Winchester, W. S.
G/5867 Winder, C.
G/7565 Winkley, J. R.
G/9558 Winson, L.
G/6372 Winter, A. E.
G/7505 Winter, W.
G/12573 Winterbottom, F.
G/6522 Winterbottom, R.
G/17676 Winterflood, W.
G/21187 Wisbey, L.
L/8345 Wisdom, W.
L/8239 Wise, W.
G/13611 Witch, P. C.
G/22700 Wollett, E. E.
G/13307 Wood, A. E.
L/9065 Wood, A. H.
L/9869 Wood, C. E.
G/585 Wood, D.
G/5064 Wood, F.
G/9388 Wood, H.
G/729 Wood, H.
G/1957 Wood, H. J.
G/20839 Wood, H. T.
G/3718 Wood, J.
G/26440 Wood, L. A. G.
G/8581 Wood, R. G.
L/9182 Wood, R.
G/5541 Wood, T.
S/341 Wood, W. C.
G/8852 Wood, W. F.
G/3940 Wood, W. H.
G/14135 Wood, W. H.
S/10588 Wood, W. J.
G/48 Woodcock, E.
T/1132 Woodcock, N.
G/18926 Woodfield, W. C.
G/1086 Woodhouse, R. W.
G/10135 Woodland, E.
G/15718 Woodland, H.
G/4086 Woodley, F.
G/13180 Woodman, W. T.
G/323 Woods, E.
G/6788 Woodward, A.
S/216 Woodward, A. J.
S/10736 Woodward, B. J.
G/14952 Woodward, F. J.
G/1921 Woodward, H.
G/5606 Woodward, H. H.
L/8516 Woodwards, F. J.
G/15719 Woolf, J. W.
L/10385 Woolgar, C. M.
L/9956 Woollett, H.
T/1411 Woollett, T.
G/8044 Woolley, W. H.
L/10502 Woolven, S.
G/14494 Woplin, J.
G/20033 Worley, B. E.
G/1701 Wormald, S. O.
G/17470 Worman, A. A.
G/987 Wormley, H.
T/240190 Worsley, C.
G/4302 Worster, D. E.
L/9071 Wotherspoon, A. W.
G/13383 Wraight, A. B.
S/352 Wraight, G. A.
L/6991 Wraight, W. G.
L/10046 Wraith, J. A.
T/203618 Wratten, A.
G/5178 Wrench, G.
G/1065 Wretham, A.
S/10637 Wright, A.
L/7568 Wright, A. E.
G/14580 Wright, E.
G/20021 Wright, E. V.
S/379 Wright, F. G.
G/4661 Wright, F. W.
L/7680 Wright, G. T.
G/20908 Wright, H. C.
S/420 Wright, H. H.
G/29257 Wright, J.
G/3204 Wright, J. G.
L/9101 Wright, R. L.
T/20061 Wright, T.
S/10468 Wright, W. G.
G/1790 Wright, W. H.
G/3355 Wyatt, G. E.
G/2849 Wyatt, W.
G/1435 Wyborn, C. G.
G/8930 Wyeth, J. T.
G/7223 Wyett, H.
G/13354 Yates, A. A.
G/14031 Yates, A.
T/241546 Yates, J.
G/10373 York, A.
G/6088 Young, C. T.
G/3056 Young, F.
S/87 Young, F. G.
T/2427 Young, F. G.
G/6635 Young, H.
G/8160 Young, J.
T/201043 Young, S. W. T.
G/14287 Youngman, G.
G/24721 Zealey, G.
APPENDIX III
REWARDS
All Ranks
(The ranks shown are those held at the time of award)
V.C.
Cotter, W. R., L.-Corpl., 6707.
K.C.B.
Bainbridge, Major-General E. G. T., C.B.
Lynden-Bell, Major-General Sir A. L., K.C.M.G., C.B.
C.B.
Hill, Lt.-Colonel H. C. de la M.
Lynden-Bell, Major-General A. L., C.M.G.
McDouall, Br.-General R., C.M.G., D.S.O.
K.C.M.G.
Lynden-Bell, Major-General A. L., C.B., C.M.G.
C.M.G.
Finch Hatton, Lt.-Colonel E. H., D.S.O.
Hulke, Lt.-Colonel L. I. B.
McDouall, Colonel R., D.S.O.
Porter, Br.-General C. ’L., D.S.O.
Vyvyan, Colonel Sir C. B., Bart., C.B.
C.H.
Perrott, Colonel Sir H. C., Bart., C.B.
C.B.E.
Findlay, Colonel H.
McDouall, Lt.-Colonel R., C.B., C.M.G., D.S.O.
D.S.O.
Barnard, Major W. G. F.
Beevor, Lt.-Colonel M.
Body, Captain J.
Chapman, Major G. A. E.
Crookenden, Major J.
Friend, Lt.-Colonel R. S. I.
Green, Lt.-Colonel H. W.
Groves-Raines, Captain R. G. D.
Kirkpatrick, Lt.-Colonel H. F.
Sebastian, Captain E. G.
Smeltzer, Lt.-Colonel A. S., M.C.
Stronge, Lt.-Colonel H. C. T., M.C.
Studd, Lt.-Colonel F. C. R.
Thewles, Lt.-Colonel H. A.
Trevor, Major W. H.
Whitmarsh, Captain A. J.
CLASP TO D.S.O.
Body, Lt.-Colonel J., D.S.O.
O.B.E.
Body, Lt.-Colonel J., D.S.O.
Booth, Major W. H., D.S.O.
Dixon, Captain G. S.
Eaton, Lt.-Colonel W. A.
Raikes, Lieut. W. O.
Ternan, Major H. A. B.
Trueman, Lt.-Colonel A. P. H.
Ward, Captain H. E.
M.B.E.
Barber, Lieut. L. W.
Beale, Captain G. S.
Corney, Lieut. A.
Cree, Captain H. F.
Filmer, Captain W. G. H.
Silverwood-Cope, Captain A. L.
Thomson, Captain A. B.
Watson, Captain F. W.
Wilkins, Captain D. A.
Wilson, Lieut. C. E.
M.C.
Allen, Captain J. F. W.
Anderson, Captain D. K.
Anderson, 2nd Lieut. L.
Asprey, 2nd Lieut. P. R.
Aylward, Lieut. J. A. S.
Campbell, Captain D. S.
Caney, 2nd Lieut. C.
Carles, Captain C. W.
Carter, Captain E. A.
Cattley, Captain C. F.
Causton, Captain L. P.
Chapman, Lieut. C. M. B.
Chapman, Lieut. E. R.
Chater, 2nd Lieut. E. C.
Chilvers, 2nd Lieut. J. E.
(Christopherson, Rev. N. C.)
Church, 2nd Lieut. G. W.
Clapperton, Captain T.
Clarke, Lieut. A. H.
Clouting, 2nd Lieut. C. E.
Cockeram, 2nd Lieut. P. A.
Connell, Lieut. W. C.
Corrall, Captain W. R.
Cotching, 2nd Lieut. E. G.
Emery, Major T. S.
Hale, Lieut. F. W.
Hall, 2nd Lieut. E. Foster.
Hamilton, Lieut. G. F.
Hanmer, 2nd Lieut. A. J.
Hardy, Captain H. S.
Harrison, No. 8798 C.S.M. A.
Hatfield, Captain C. E.
Haughton, Lieut. M. G.
Hawkins, No. 2948 R.S.M. A.
Hayfield, 2nd Lieut. C. D.
Hendin, 2nd Lieut. D. W.
Hicks, 2nd Lieut. P.
Holder, Lieut. F. D.
Hollis, 2nd Lieut. C. F. G.
Howcroft, Lieut. G. J.
Howgrave-Graham, Captain A. H.
Hudson, 2nd Lieut. F. N.
Hughes, 2nd Lieut. J. H.
Hunter, Captain H.
Jacobs, Lieut. B.
Jacobs, Lieut. I. A.
Jeffrey, No. S/191 S.-M. W.
Jessel, Lieut. G.
Johnston, Captain W. T.
Jones, 2nd Lieut. G. M.
Jones, 2nd Lieut. H. L.
Kenchington, Captain A. G.
Keown, Lieut. R. W.
Kidd, Lieut. L. G. M.
Laverton, Captain W. R. C.
Lawrence, 2nd Lieut. W. B.
Lee, Captain G.
Liles, 2nd Lieut. R. W.
Lilley, 2nd Lieut. A. A.
Lindley, Lieut. G.
Lister, 2nd Lieut. D. S.
Lucas, Captain L. W.
McCallum, Lieut. A.
Macfadyen, Lieut. W. A.
Marchant, Captain F. O.
Marchant, 2nd Lieut. S. H. S.
Marshall, Lieut. F. A. J. E.
Mason-Springgay, 2nd Lieut. W. H.
Mathias, 2nd Lieut. C. A. S.
Maxted, No. 141 C.S.M. G. W.
Milles, Lieut. H. L.
Mitchell, Lieut. H. V.
Morley, 2nd Lieut. H. L.
Morrell, Captain F. A.
Morse, 2nd Lieut. E. V.
Moss, Lieut. V. Newton.
Nicholas, Captain W. L. J.
Nicholson, 2nd Lieut. A. C. L.
Page, Captain J. C.
Pannell, 2nd Lieut. H.
Parnis, Lieut. W. H.
Peake, Captain W.
Peckham, Lieut. G. H.
Piper, Lieut. J. D.
Pritchard, No. 635 C.S.M. W. J.
Prothero, Captain L. E. A.
Sandilands, Captain R. B.
Sankey, 2nd Lieut. C. M.
Scarlett, Captain Hon. P. G.
Shafto, 2nd Lieut. J. S. H.
Shaw-Lawrence, 2nd Lieut. L. E.
Sherwill, 2nd Lieut. A. J.
Smeltzer, 2nd Lieut. A. S.
Spence, Captain F. A.
Stainforth, Captain A. G. C.
Stevens, 2nd Lieut. E. A. M.
Stevens, 2nd Lieut. G. E. W.
Stevens, Lieut. H. F.
Stevens, 2nd Lieut. W. T.
Strauss, Captain B. L.
Stronge, Captain H. C. T.
CLASP TO M.C.
Black, Captain C. K., M.C.
Our website is not just a platform for buying books, but a bridge
connecting readers to the timeless values of culture and wisdom. With
an elegant, user-friendly interface and an intelligent search system,
we are committed to providing a quick and convenient shopping
experience. Additionally, our special promotions and home delivery
services ensure that you save time and fully enjoy the joy of reading.
ebooknice.com