0% found this document useful (0 votes)
10 views40 pages

Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger. A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry.

This systematic review and meta-analysis investigates the relationship between screen time and behavior problems in children aged 12 years or younger, analyzing 87 studies with over 159,000 participants. The findings indicate a weak but significant correlation between increased screen time and both externalizing (e.g., aggression) and internalizing (e.g., anxiety) behavior issues, with variations attributed to demographic and methodological factors. The results highlight the need for further research to clarify these associations and address methodological discrepancies in existing studies.

Uploaded by

Juliana Costa
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
10 views40 pages

Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger. A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry.

This systematic review and meta-analysis investigates the relationship between screen time and behavior problems in children aged 12 years or younger, analyzing 87 studies with over 159,000 participants. The findings indicate a weak but significant correlation between increased screen time and both externalizing (e.g., aggression) and internalizing (e.g., anxiety) behavior issues, with variations attributed to demographic and methodological factors. The results highlight the need for further research to clarify these associations and address methodological discrepancies in existing studies.

Uploaded by

Juliana Costa
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 40

24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does
not imply endorsement of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes
of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

JAMA Psychiatry. 2022 Mar 16;79(5):393–405. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2022.0155

Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing


Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger
A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis

Rachel Eirich 1,2, Brae Anne McArthur 1,2, Ciana Anhorn 1, Claire McGuinness 1, Dimitri A Christakis 3,4,
Sheri Madigan 1,2,✉

Author information Article notes Copyright and License information


PMCID: PMC8928099 PMID: 35293954

This systematic review and meta-analysis examines the duration of screen time among children
12 years or younger and its association with externalizing and internalizing behavior problems.

Key Points

Question

Is there an association between screen time and children’s internalizing and externalizing
behavior problems in the extant screen time literature?

Findings

In this systematic review and meta-analysis of 87 studies (98 independent samples) including
159 425 children 12 years or younger, greater duration of screen time was weakly but

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 1/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

significantly correlated with externalizing (eg, aggression, inattention) and internalizing (eg,
anxiety, depression) behavior problems. Results varied as a function of demographic (eg, sex)
and methodological factors (eg, informant and measurement method).

Meaning

The findings showed weak but significant correlations between screen time and children’s
behavior problems and suggest that methodological variability may have contributed to mixed
findings in the literature.

Abstract

Importance

Currently, there is a lack of consensus in the literature on the association between screen time
(eg, television, video games) and children’s behavior problems.

Objective

To assess the association between the duration of screen time and externalizing and
internalizing behavior problems among children 12 years or younger.

Data Sources

For this systematic review and meta-analysis, MEDLINE, Embase, and PsycINFO databases
were searched for articles published from January 1960 to May 2021. Reference lists were
manually searched for additional studies.

Study Selection

Included studies measured screen time (ie, duration) and externalizing or internalizing
behavior problems in children 12 years or younger, were observational or experimental (with
baseline data), were available in English, and had data that could be transformed into an effect
size. Studies conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic were excluded. Of 25 196 nonduplicate
articles identified and screened for inclusion, 595 met the selection criteria.

Data Extraction and Synthesis


https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 2/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

The study followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses
(PRISMA) reporting guideline. Extracted variables were child age, sex, and socioeconomic
status; informants and measurement type for screen time and behavior problems; study
publication year; and study design and quality. Data were extracted by 2 independent coders
and were pooled using a random-effects model.

Main Outcomes and Measures

The primary outcome was the association of screen time duration with externalizing (eg,
aggression, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms) and internalizing (eg,
depression, anxiety) behaviors or diagnoses.

Results

Of the 595 full-text articles assessed for eligibility, 87 studies met all inclusion criteria,
comprising 98 independent samples and 159 425 participants (mean [SD] age, 6.07 [2.89] years;
83 246 [51.30%] male). Increased duration of screen time had a small but significant
correlation with more externalizing problems (90 samples; r, 0.11; 95% CI, 0.10-0.12) and
internalizing problems (43 samples; r, 0.07; 95% CI, 0.05-0.08) in children. Several
methodological moderators explained between-study heterogeneity. There was evidence of
significant between study heterogeneity (I2 = 87.80).

Conclusions and Relevance

This systematic review and meta-analysis found small but significant correlations between
screen time and children’s behavior problems. Methodological differences across studies likely
contributed to the mixed findings in the literature.

Introduction

The effects of screen time on children’s mental health has been rigorously debated.1,2 Some
literature suggests that screen time may be associated with risk of poor mental health
outcomes by displacing sleep and physical activities as well as social exchanges and learning
opportunities known to foster well-being.3,4 Screen media may also impede self-regulation
strategies and increase arousal levels owing to fast-paced and intense audiovisual effects, which
may be associated with inattention and aggressive behavior.5,6 However, it has been argued that
concern with regard to screen time and its effect on child mental health is not empirically
justified owing to conflicting research results and methodological shortcomings.7,8,9,10

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 3/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

Meta-analyses can address discrepancies in the literature by providing greater statistical


precision through pooled results from multiple individual studies11 and can detect whether
variations in study findings are explained by moderators. In the screen time literature,
methodological differences have likely contributed to the conflicting findings: effect sizes may
differ based on the statistical analyses conducted,12 how variables are measured,13 and
whether data are cross-sectional or longtudinal.7 Effect sizes may also vary as a function of
child sex (eg, sex differences in the associations of screen time14,15 with behavior
problems),16,17,18 child age (eg, association of increased screen use with increasing age among
children),15,19 and socioeconomic risk (eg, association of increased screen use with behavior
problems in the context of low socioeconomic status).20,21 Identifying when and for whom
correlations are stronger or weaker may inform the ongoing screen time debate and help
develop targeted interventions for children most at risk for potential behavior problems
associated with screen time.

Owing to substantial research to date on duration of screen time and behavior problems, it
appeared timely to meta-analytically summarize this body of research. Given that the context
and nature of screen use changes from childhood to adolescence from mostly parent-mediated
and television-centric to less parent-mediated and weighted toward social media,19,22,23 this
systematic review and meta-analysis focused on children 12 years or younger. Because of their
different mechanisms,24,25 we conducted 2 separate meta-analyses for externalizing (eg,
aggression, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms) and internalizing (eg, anxiety,
depression) behavior problems.25 In addition, we sought to identify sources of between-study
heterogeneity.

Methods

Search Strategy

In this systematic review and meta-analysis, searches were conducted in the MEDLINE,
Embase, and PsycINFO databases by a science librarian for articles published from January
1960 to May 2021. The concepts of screen time, internalizing and externalizing behavior, and
children were captured by searching database-specific subject headings and text word fields
(eTable 1 in the Supplement). Synonymous terms were combined with the Boolean “OR” and
then combined with the Boolean “AND.” The concept of children (12 years or younger) was
searched using the “age limits” database functions and via text word search. In all databases,
truncation symbols were used in text word searches to capture variations in phrasing and
spelling. No language limits were applied. Reference lists in included studies and review articles
were manually searched for additional studies. This study followed the Preferred Reporting
Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA ) reporting guideline.

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 4/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

Study Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

Studies were included if (1) the age range of included children was 12 years or less, (2) screen
time duration was measured (hours and/or minutes), (3) behavior problems (ie, internalizing,
externalizing) were measured, (4) the studies were observational or experimental (with
baseline data), (5) statistical data were available, and (6) the article was available in English.
Because child screen time and mental distress have increased during the COVID-19
pandemic,26,27 we excluded studies conducted during the pandemic. Inclusion and exclusion
criteria are detailed in eTable 2 in the Supplement. Studies were assessed for inclusion by 2
coders (R.E. and C.A.). Any study deemed to meet inclusion criteria by either coder was
considered for full-text review.

Data Extraction

Study Quality Assessment

Each study was evaluated by 2 independent coders (C.A. and C.M.) for quality based on an
adapted 16-item quality assessment tool28 (eTable 3 in the Supplement), with each item coded
as 0 (no) or 1 (yes) (eTable 4 in the Supplement). Intercoder agreement for the overall quality
score was good (intraclass correlation coefficient, 0.75). Discrepancies were resolved via
consensus.

Moderating Variables

Moderators extracted were (1) child sex (percentage of males in the study); (2) child age at
outcome measurement (in months); (3) screen time informant (child or parent); (4) screen time
measurement method (ie, activity log, questionnaire, or interview); (5) type of internalizing
(anxiety, depression, or somatization) or externalizing (aggression or attention
deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms) behavior problems; (6) informant (child, parent,
clinician or coder, peers, teacher, or combination); (7) measurement method (diagnostic and
structural interview, observer report, or questionnaire); (8) whether informants differed for
screen time and behavior problems; (9) clinical sample (diagnosis of a preexisting behavior
problem [yes or no]); (10) publication year; (11) study design (cross-sectional, longitudinal
with baseline outcome adjustment, or longitudinal with no baseline adjustment); (12)
sociodemographic risk (less than 80% vs 80% or more of the sample had at least 1 of the
following: low income, low caregiver educational level, or an adolescent parent); and (13)
geographic location. eTable 5 in the Supplement shows the data extraction document used. All
included studies were independently coded by 2 trained coders (C.A. and C.M.), with excellent
reliability29 for continuous moderators (intraclass correlation coefficient, 0.80-1.00). The mean
https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 5/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

percentage of agreement for categorical moderators was 92.5% (range, 85%-100%).


Discrepancies were resolved via consensus.

Data Synthesis

When multiple studies conducted analyses on the same sample, the study with the largest
sample size and most comprehensive information was selected. Adjusted statistics were
selected over unadjusted statistics whenever available. Global measures of internalizing or
externalizing symptoms and of screen time were selected over discrete symptoms (eg,
depression, anxiety) or screen types (eg, tablet, video games). Similar to the methods of other
meta-analyses,30,31,32 when a single study measured screen time and/or behavior problems at
multiple time points, effect sizes with the largest temporal distance between measures were
selected. When studies reported nonsignificant findings without any corresponding statistic or
P value, a P value of .50 was entered.33 Studies reporting β coefficients between –0.50 and 0.50
were imputed to correlations (r).34 Effect sizes were pooled when a single effect size among
many in a study could not be selected based on the aforementioned criteria. In addition, if a
study provided effect sizes from multiple discrete samples with different population
parameters (eg, different cohorts), these samples were entered into the meta-analysis
separately.

Statistical Analysis

Pooled effect size estimates and moderator analyses were conducted using Comprehensive
Meta-Analysis, version 3.0 (Biostat).35 Effect sizes were transformed into correlations (r) with
95% CIs using random-effects modeling.36 Correlations were interpreted as small (0.1),
moderate (0.2), or large (0.3) based on conventional standards.37

Q and I2 statistics were used to assess between-study heterogeneity. Random-effect meta-


regressions and subgroup comparisons were conducted to assess continuous and categorical
moderators, respectively. Subgroup comparisons were only conducted when there were at least
3 samples (ie, ≥3 cells) available for each categorical comparison.35 Inspection of funnel plots
and the Egger test were used to estimate publication bias.38 A 2-tailed P value of .05 was used
for the primary analyses, and owing to the number of moderator analyses conducted, a
conservative 2-sided α value of .01 was used to assess the significance of moderators.39

Results

The search strategy revealed 25 196 nonduplicate abstracts to be reviewed for determination of
meeting inclusion criteria. A total of 595 full-text articles were assessed for eligibility, and 87
https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 6/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

studies (with 98 unique samples) were included in the meta-analysis (Figure 1).

Figure 1. PRISMA Flow Diagram.

Open in a new tab

aInternalizing and externalizing categories were not mutually exclusive.

Study Characteristics

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 7/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

The 98 samples consisted of 159 425 participants. Sample sizes in the included studies ranged
from 15 to 15 291 participants (median, 532.5 participants). The mean (SD) age of participants
when screen time was assessed was 6.07 (2.89) years (range, 0.5-11.0 years), and the mean (SD)
age when behavior problems were assessed was 7.16 (2.70) years (range, 1.3-12.0 years). A total
of 83 246 participants (51.25%) were male (median percentage of males per study, 51.31%
[range, 36.60% to 78.70%]). Of the 98 total samples, 44 (44.9%) were in North America, 24
(24.5%) in Europe, 1 (1.0%) in Africa, 14 (14.3%) in Asia, 7 (7.1%) in Australia or New Zealand,
5 (5.1%) in the Middle East, and 2 (2.0%) in South America; 1 sample (1.0%) was multisite
(detailed study characteristics are shown in eTable 6 in the Supplement).

Screen Time and Externalizing Behavior Problems

Pooled Effect Sizes

In the 90 samples from 80 studies (124 027 children), the correlation was small but significant
(r, 0.11; 95% CI, 0.10-0.12) (Figure
2).16,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,
74,75,76,77,78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92,93,94,95,96,97,98,99,100,101,102,103,104,105,106,
107,108,109,110,111,112,113,114,115,116,117,118 The Egger test result provided evidence that
studies with smaller sample sizes had more extreme effect sizes, and the funnel plot showed
asymmetry (eFigure 1 in the Supplement), indicating possible publication bias and/or small-
study effects. There was evidence of significant between-study heterogeneity (Q, 729.78; P
< .001; I2 = 87.80); therefore, moderators were explored (Table 1).

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 8/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

Figure 2. Meta-analysis of the Correlation of Screen Time With Externalizing Problems.

Open in a new tab

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 9/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

Markers indicate estimates, with the size of the marker indicating weight; horizontal
lines represent 95% CIs; diamonds represent pooled estimates, with the outer points
indicating 95% CIs.

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 10/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

Table 1. Moderator Analyses for the Correlation Between Screen Time and Behavior
Problems.

Variable Samples, r (95% CI) Q Contrast


No. P value a

Externalizing behavior
Screen type
Video games or computer 9 0.13 (0.08 to 0.18)b 0.32 .57
Television 42 0.11 (0.09 to 0.14)b
Externalizing behavior type
Aggression 21 0.17 (0.13 to 0.20)b 11.11 .001
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity 25 0.09 (0.06 to 0.12)b
disorder symptoms
Sociodemographic risk
No 87 0.11 (0.09 to 0.12)b 0.04 .84
Yes 3 0.12 (0.03 to 0.21)c
Study design
Cross-sectional 50 0.13 (0.11 to 0.15)b 23.55 <.001
Longitudinal without control 21 0.11 (0.08 to 0.14)b
for baseline outcome
Longitudinal with control for 19 0.06 (0.04 to 0.08)b
baseline outcome
Screen time measurement
method
Activity log 9 0.12 (0.08 to 0.17)b 2.81 .25
Interview 7 0.15 (0.09 to 0.21)b
Questionnaire 74 0.10 (0.09 to 0.12)b
Screen time informant
Child 19 0.15 (0.11 to 0.18)b 5.71 .02
Parent 69 0.10 (0.08 to 0.12)b
Behavior measurement
method
https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 11/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

Variable Samples, r (95% CI) Q Contrast


No. P value a

Diagnosis and/or structured 8 0.12 (0.07 to 0.17)b 10.04 .007


interview
Observer report 8 0.20 (0.14 to 0.26)b
Questionnaire 74 0.10 (0.08 to 0.12)b
Behavior informant
Child 4 0.08 (0.00 to 0.15)c 15.39 .009
Clinician or coder 6 0.16 (0.08 0.23)b
Peers 8 0.20 (0.15 to 0.26)b
Teacher 7 0.07 (0.02 to 0.13)d
Parent 57 0.10 (0.08 to 0.12)b
Combination 8 0.09 (0.04 to 0.14)d
Different informants for screen
time and behavior
Yes 35 0.13 (0.09 to 0.16)b 1.56 .21
No 55 0.10 (0.08 to 0.12)b
Geographic location
Asia 13 0.12 (0.08 to 0.16)b 25.17 <.001
Australia and New Zealand 7 0.05 (−0.01 to 0.10)
Europe and UK 22 0.08 (0.06 to 0.11)b
Middle East 5 0.23 (0.17 to 0.29)b
North America 39 0.12 (0.10 to 0.15)b
Clinical sample
Yes 4 0.13 (0.03 to 0.23)c 0.16 .69
No 86 0.11 (0.09 to 0.12)b
Internalizing behavior
Screen type
Video games or computer 4 0.02 (−0.07 to 0.12) 1.58 .21
Television 8 0.09 (0.04 to 0.15)d
Study design
Cross-sectional 29 0.08 (0.05 to 0.10)b 2.76 .25
https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 12/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

Variable Samples, r (95% CI) Q Contrast


No. P value a

Longitudinal without control 5 0.07 (0.01 to 0.08)b


for baseline outcome
Longitudinal with control for 9 0.03 (−0.02 to 0.08)
baseline outcome
Screen time informant
Child 10 0.09 (0.04 to 0.13)b 1.43 .23
Parent 32 0.06 (0.03 to 0.08)b
Behavior measurement
method
Questionnaire 40 0.06 (0.04 to 0.08)b 0.94 .33
Diagnosis and/or structured 3 0.10 (0.03 to 0.17)d
interview
Different informants for screen
time and behavior
Yes 9 0.08 (0.05 to 0.10)b 14.86 <.001
No 34 0.01 (−0.02 to 0.03)
Geographic location
Asia 9 0.08 (0.06 to 0.10)b 1.75 .42
Europe 12 0.07 (0.03 to 0.11)d
North America 16 0.05 (0.02 to 0.09)d
Behavior informant
Child 7 0.10 (0.05 to 0.14)b 1.20 .27
Parent 33 0.07 (0.04 to 0.09)b

Open in a new tab

a
Contrast P values represent the P values for the comparison of effect sizes between
categorical moderators.
b
P < .001 for the effect size of this categorical moderating factor alone.
c
P < .05 for the effect size of this categorical moderating factor alone.
d
P < .01 for the effect size of this categorical moderating factor alone.

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 13/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

Moderator Analyses

Meta-regression analyses of the 90 samples showed that the effect size for the association
between screen time and externalizing problems increased as the percentage of males in studies
increased (b, 0.007 [SE, 0.002]; z, 3.23; P = .001). Effect sizes also decreased as the study
publication year increased (b, −0.003 [SE, 0.001]; z, –3.98; P < .001). The effect size for the
association between screen time and externalizing problems decreased as study quality
increased (b, −0.017 [SE, 0.004]; z, –4.30; P < .001) (Table 2).

Table 2. Meta-regression Analyses of the Association Between Screen Time and


Externalizing and Internalizing Problems.

Continuous moderator Samples, β (SE) z Score P value


No.
Externalizing behavior
Male 90 0.007 (0.002) 3.23 .001
Age at externalizing assessment 90 0.000 (0.000) 0.10 .92
Publication year 90 −0.003 (0.001) −3.98 <.001
Study quality 90 −0.017 (0.004) −4.30 <.001
Internalizing behavior
Male 43 0.002 (0.003) 0.52 .60
Age at internalizing assessment 43 0.000 (0.000) −0.37 .71
Publication year 43 −0.001 (0.002) −0.44 .66
Study quality 43 −0.003 (0.006) −0.49 .62

Open in a new tab

Correlations between screen time and externalizing problems were stronger in studies
examining aggression (21 samples; r, 0.17; 95% CI, 0.13-0.20) compared with those examining
attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms (25 samples; r, 0.09; 95% CI, 0.06-0.11).
Correlations were weaker in longitudinal studies that controlled for baseline externalizing
problems (19 samples; r, 0.06; 95% CI, 0.04-0.08) compared with cross-sectional studies (50
samples; r, 0.13; 95% CI, 0.11-0.15) and longitudinal studies without baseline control (21

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 14/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

samples; r, 0.11; 95% CI, 0.08-0.14). Studies conducted in the Middle East had stronger
associations between screen time and externalizing problems (5 samples; r, 0.23; 95% CI, 0.17-
0.29) compared with those conducted in all other geographic locations. Stronger correlations
were also found in studies using observer reports (8 samples; r, 0.20; 95% CI, 0.14-0.26)
compared with those using questionnaires (74 samples; r, 0.10; 95% CI, 0.08-0.12). In addition,
studies using peers to assess externalizing problems had stronger correlations (8 samples; r,
0.20; 95% CI, 0.15-0.26) compared with those that used child report (4 samples; r, 0.08; 95% CI,
0.00-0.15), parent report (57 samples; r, 0.10; 95% CI, 0.08-0.12), teacher report (7 samples; r,
0.07; 95% CI, 0.02-0.13), and a combination of informants (8 samples; r, 0.09; 95% CI, 0.04-0.14).

Screen Time and Internalizing Behavior Problems

Pooled Effect Sizes

In 43 samples from 40 studies (99 603 children), the effect size between child screen time and
internalizing problems was weak but significant (r, 0.07; 95% CI, 0.05-0.08) (Figure
3).16,42,43,45,50,51,52,56,60,66,69,73,74,76,78,81,83,84,87,88,92,94,95,98,101,105,106,107,110,113,114,115
,116,119,120,121,122,123,124,125 The funnel plot showed some asymmetry (eFigure 2 in the

Supplement), indicating possible publication bias; however, the Egger test result did not suggest
that smaller sample sizes had more extreme effect sizes. The Q statistic was significant (Q,
285.10; P < .001; I2, 85.27), and moderator analyses were conducted to explain between-study
heterogeneity (Table 1). Only 1 moderator analysis had significant results: studies that used
different informants to measure internalizing problems and screen time had a stronger
correlation (9 samples; r, 0.08; 95% CI, 0.05-0.10) compared with those with the same
informant across measures (34 samples; r, 0.01; 95% CI, –0.02 to 0.03).

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 15/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

Figure 3. Meta-analysis of the Correlation of Screen Time With Internalizing Problems.

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 16/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 17/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

Open in a new tab

Markers indicate estimates, with the size of the marker indicating weight; horizontal
lines represent 95% CIs; diamonds represent pooled estimates, with the outer points
indicating 95% CIs.

Discussion

The increasing rate of screen time in early childhood has engendered concern among clinicians,
policy makers, and parents regarding its possible effects on children’s mental health. This study
found small but significant correlations between screen time and children’s internalizing and
externalizing behavior problems. The magnitude of these correlations is comparable with that
found in other meta-analyses on the association between screen time and child language
skills126 and academic performance.127 Moreover, effect sizes derived in this study are similar
to those in other meta-analyses examining associations between various family and child
factors (eg, language skills128 and socioeconomic status129) and internalizing and externalizing
problems. Although the effect sizes found in this study were small, the consequences of screen
time at a population level are likely meaningful,37,130 particularly because a recent meta-
https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 18/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

analysis131 suggested that 75% of children younger than 2 years and 64% of children aged 2 to
5 years exceed screen time guidelines.

Effect sizes were larger for the association between screen time and externalizing problems
compared with internalizing problems given nonoverlapping 95% CIs. Future studies testing
which discrete mechanisms underlie the association between screen time and externalizing vs
internalizing problems are warranted. For example, it is possible that the content viewed (eg,
inappropriate or violent) underlies the association between screen time and externalizing
problems, whereas indirect effects such as social withdrawal or sleep disruption may underlie
the association between screen time and internalizing problems. From a methodological
perspective, externalizing problems may be easier for reporters to observe than internalizing
problems owing to their outward display, leading to poorer sensitivity for identifying
internalizing problems.132

We found stronger correlations between screen time and externalizing problems in boys
compared with girls. This finding is consistent with that of previous research in which boys had
higher screen use14,133 and more externalizing problems17 than did girls. Externalizing
behaviors, such as aggression, may be more readily modeled by boys through sex-stereotypic
socialization.134 In addition, we found that studies measuring aggression had larger effect sizes
than those measuring attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. While viewing screen
media, children may be exposed to inappropriate content, aggression, and violence.40,135,136
Consistent with social learning theory,137 children may become desensitized after repeated
exposures and model aggressive or violent content toward others.6,40,136 Moreover, as screen
time becomes more normalized, it is possible that aggressive behavior within some screen
programming does as well. Screen use guidelines138 suggest that parents should monitor screen
time, ensure the content their children are viewing is age-appropriate, limit exposure to violent
content, communicate with their children about inappropriate on-screen content, and model
healthy device habits.139

Correlations of screen time with externalizing behavior problems were stronger in the Middle
East compared with any other geographic location; however, sample sizes in studies from the
Middle East were small, and 95% CIs were wide, limiting the conclusions that can be made. A
dose-response relationship between screen time and externalizing problems41,42,140 may
explain the stronger correlations found in countries in the Middle East because early-onset
screen use has been increasing at a faster rate in these countries compared with other
countries.141,142 More research is needed to understand how cultural differences (eg, different
guidelines and beliefs about screen time)143 contribute to associations between screen time
and behavior problems.

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 19/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

The mixed findings in the literature on screen time and behavior problems may largely be the
result of methodological variability.7 Studies that used more objective methods (eg, observer
report) and reporters (eg, peers) of children’s externalizing problems had stronger correlations
than those using child, parent, or teacher reports. In addition, studies that used different
informants for screen time and internalizing problems had stronger correlations than those
using the same informant across measures. Inconsistency between informants for children’s
behavior problems is common,144 but informant discrepancies may indicate the contexts and
ways in which behavior problems are perceived by the child and others.145 For example,
aggression may be noticed more readily by peers owing to children having more opportunities
to engage with peers aggressively (eg, bullying).

Variability exists both across (ie, aggression, oppositional behavior, and hyperactivity) and
within (eg, demanding, hits others) the broad construct of externalizing behavior problems.
However, historically, externalizing problems have been examined as a composite.25,146 This
study’s finding that screen time was more strongly correlated with aggression suggests that the
use of an externalizing composite may not capture inherent nuances in the association between
screen time and behavior problems. Furthermore, as children move into adolescence, attention
problems and aggression are often measured as separate constructs.147 Future research should
consider disaggregating externalizing behaviors, presenting associations in their
subcomponents (eg, aggression, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms), and
examining which items within the subcomponents are correlated most strongly with screen
time.

Given that methodological variability is critical for understanding patterns of associations in


this body of research, several methodological implications are suggested for future research. To
the extent possible, future research should use multi-informant (eg, peers, child), multimethod
(eg, passive sensing apps and questionnaires), and longitudinal approaches to measure the
association of screen time with child outcomes. Of importance, self-report screen time
measurement may be biased.148 None of the studies identified in our meta-analysis used
objective measures (eg, sensing apps) to evaluate screen time, presenting an opportunity for
future research to use novel screen-time measurement methods.

Two characteristics of publications were identified as significant moderators of associations


between screen time and externalizing problems: study quality and publication year.
Associations found between screen time and externalizing problems decreased as study quality
increased. Poorer quality studies often have more random error and inflated effects owing to
less methodological rigor (eg, a single informant, lack of statistical controls). Study quality is
also closely connected to study year, with larger effect sizes found in older, discovery studies
typically owing to more reliance on significance testing (P value thresholds), inadequate power,
and measurement error.149 Thus, effect sizes also decreased in newer studies, which is
https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 20/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

consistent with the notion of a decline effect (ie, diminishing effect sizes over time).150,151 The
accessibility and use of screens have increased over time.152 As screens become normalized in
childhood and contemporary culture, the risks associated with their use may become less
consequential for children’s behavior problems. Furthermore, parents today may be more
informed and better at monitoring screen time and content owing to awareness of screen time
guidelines.131

Limitations

This study has several limitations. First and of most importance, effect sizes are correlational,
not causal. Causal reductionism should be avoided because behavior problems are associated
with a complex combination of genetic and environmental factors. Additional research that
addresses this complexity is needed (eg, genetically informative designs).153,154,155
Directionality cannot be inferred; screens may be used as a tool to placate or negotiate with
children.156,157 Second, although there was significant heterogeneity for internalizing
problems, only 1 of the moderator analyses had significant results. Other important but
unexamined moderators, such as genetic susceptibility,155 sleep,43 or language
development,126 may have a significant role in associations. Third, with rapid shifts in
technology, generational cohorts may not use devices comparably.7 Mobile phones were
originally used almost exclusively for calling, and few children had their own; now,
smartphones are used to access web pages, play games, stream videos, and socialize, and 69% of
US children have their own smartphone by 12 years of age.22 Although we excluded studies
conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic, a cohort of children are growing up during the
pandemic. Screen time has increased significantly during the pandemic, and this increase may
affect child development.26,158,159 In addition, not all screen time is equal in terms of content,
subject, and formal features; some screen time may be more consequential than other screen
time. Our findings only apply to screen time broadly in terms of duration or quantity of use.
More nuanced aspects of screen time, such as screen content (eg, social media, violent video
games), context (eg, coviewing vs passive viewing), and quality (eg, educational vs
entertainment), should be examined. Future studies should also ensure that screen time is not
measured as a single construct across multiple devices and should instead focus on the function
of screen use (eg, entertainment, socializing, or education) to examine the way screens are
being used without being device-specific.

Conclusions

The association between screen time and children’s mental health has garnered marked
attention from academic, health, and public sectors. This systematic review and meta-analysis
found that screen time was weakly but significantly correlated with children’s internalizing and

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 21/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

externalizing behaviors. Our study identified several important methodological moderators of


the association between screen time and behavior problems, such as the type of informants and
the measures of screen time used, that highlight the lack of harmony in the screen time
literature. It is essential for researchers to use more rigorous methods and approaches to
analyzing screen time data16,160 and to further explore the various facets of screen use (ie,
content and context) associated with children’s mental health in a rapidly evolving digital
world.

Supplement.

eTable 1. Search Strategy

eTable 2. Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

eTable 3. Study Quality Evaluation Coding Criteria

eTable 4. Study Quality Scoring for Each Study Included in the Meta-analysis

eTable 5. Coding System of Study Variables

eTable 6. Study and Sample Characteristics

eFigure 1. Funnel Plot Depicting Standard Error by Fisher Z of Included Studies in Meta-
analysis of Screen Time and Externalizing Problems

eFigure 2. Funnel Plot Depicting Standard Error by Fisher Z of Included Studies in Meta-
analysis of Screen Time and Internalizing Problems

eReferences

Click here for additional data file. (681KB, pdf)

References

1. Browne D, Thompson DA, Madigan S. Digital media use in children: clinical vs scientific
responsibilities. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(2):111-112. doi:
https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 22/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.4559 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

2. Odgers CL, Jensen MR. Annual research review: adolescent mental health in the digital
age: facts, fears, and future directions. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2020;61(3):336-348.
doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13190 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

3. Christakis DA, Gilkerson J, Richards JA, et al. Audible television and decreased adult
words, infant vocalizations, and conversational turns: a population-based study. Arch
Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(6):554-558. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.61 [
DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

4. Christakis DA. Interactive media use at younger than the age of 2 years: time to rethink
the American Academy of Pediatrics guideline? JAMA Pediatr. 2014;168(5):399-400. doi:
10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.5081 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

5. Linebarger DL, Barr R, Lapierre MA, Piotrowski JT. Associations between parenting,
media use, cumulative risk, and children’s executive functioning. J Dev Behav Pediatr.
2014;35(6):367-377. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000069 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

6. Nikkelen SW, Valkenburg PM, Huizinga M, Bushman BJ. Media use and ADHD-related
behaviors in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. Dev Psychol. 2014;50(9):2228-
2241. doi: 10.1037/a0037318 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

7. Kaye L, Orben A, Ellis D, Hunter S, Houghton S.. The conceptual and methodological
mayhem of “screen time.” Int J Env Res Public Health. 2020;17(10):3661. doi:
10.3390/ijerph17103661 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

8. Blum-Ross A, Livingstone S. The trouble with “screen time” rules. In: Mascheroni G,
Jorge A, Ponte C (Eds.). Digital Parenting: The Challenges for Families in the Digital Age.
International Clearinghouse on Children, Youth, and the Media; 2018:179-187.

9. Ferguson CJ, Beresin E. Social science’s curious war with pop culture and how it was
lost: the media violence debate and the risks it holds for social science. Prev Med.
2017;99:69-76. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.02.009 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

10. Elson M, Ferguson CJ, Gregerson M, et al. . Do policy statements on media effects
faithfully represent the science? Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. 2019;2(1):12-25. doi:
10.1177/2515245918811301 [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

11. Cohn LD, Becker BJ. How meta-analysis increases statistical power. Psychol Methods.
2003;8(3):243-253. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.8.3.243 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 23/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

12. McBee MT, Brand RJ, Dixon WE Jr. Challenging the link between early childhood
television exposure and later attention problems: a multiverse approach. Psychol Sci.
2021;32(4):496-518. doi: 10.1177/0956797620971650 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

13. Stiglic N, Viner RM. Effects of screentime on the health and well-being of children and
adolescents: a systematic review of reviews. BMJ Open. 2019;9(1):e023191. doi:
10.1136/bmjopen-2018-023191 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

14. de Jong E, Visscher TLS, HiraSing RA, Heymans MW, Seidell JC, Renders CM.
Association between TV viewing, computer use and overweight, determinants and
competing activities of screen time in 4- to 13-year-old children. Int J Obes (Lond).
2013;37(1):47-53. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2011.244 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

15. Rideout V, Hamel E.. The Media Family: Electronic Media in the Lives of Infants,
Toddlers, Preschoolers and Their Parents. Kaiser Family Foundation;2006. [Google
Scholar ]

16. Neville RD, McArthur BA, Eirich R, Lakes KD, Madigan S. Bidirectional associations
between screen time and children's externalizing and internalizing behaviors. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry. 2021;62(12):1475-1484. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13425 [DOI ] [PubMed]

17. Zahn-Waxler C, Shirtcliff EA, Marceau K. Disorders of childhood and adolescence:


gender and psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2008;4:275-303. doi:
10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091358 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

18. Bado P, Schafer J, Simioni AR, et al. Screen time and psychopathology: investigating
directionality using cross-lagged panel models. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. Published
online November 1, 2020. doi: 10.1007/s00787-020-01675-5 [DOI ] [PMC free article]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

19. Rideout V, Robb MB. The common sense census: media use by kids age zero to eight.
Common Sense Media; 2020. Accessed January 20, 2021.
https://www.commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/uploads/research/2020_zero
_to_eight_census_final_web.pdf [Google Scholar ]

20. Carson V, Spence JC, Cutumisu N, Cargill L. Association between neighborhood


socioeconomic status and screen time among pre-school children: a cross-sectional
study. BMC Public Health. 2010;10:367. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-10-367 [DOI ] [PMC
free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

21. Tandon PS, Zhou C, Sallis JF, Cain KL, Frank LD, Saelens BE. Home environment
relationships with children’s physical activity, sedentary time, and screen time by
https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 24/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

socioeconomic status. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9:88. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-9-
88 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

22. Rideout V, Robb MB. The common sense census: media use by tweens and teens.
Common Sense Media; 2019. Accessed January 10, 2021.
https://www.commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/uploads/research/2019-
census-8-to-18-full-report-updated.pdf

23. Ramirez ER, Norman GJ, Rosenberg DE, et al. Adolescent screen time and rules to
limit screen time in the home. J Adolesc Health. 2011;48(4):379-385. doi:
10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.07.013 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

24. Marceau K, Laurent HK, Neiderhiser JM, et al. Combined influences of genes, prenatal
environment, cortisol, and parenting on the development of children’s internalizing
versus externalizing problems. Behav Genet. 2015;45(3):268-282. doi: 10.1007/s10519-
014-9689-z [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

25. Achenbach TM. The classification of children’s psychiatric symptoms: a factor-


analytic study. Psychol Monogr. 1966;80(7):1-37. doi: 10.1037/h0093906 [DOI ]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

26. McArthur BA, Racine N, Browne D, McDonald S, Tough S, Madigan S. Recreational


screen time before and during COVID-19 in school-aged children. Acta Paediatr.
Published online June 15, 2021. doi: 10.1111/apa.15966 [DOI ] [PMC free article]
[PubMed]

27. Racine N, McArthur BA, Cooke JE, Eirich R, Zhu J, Madigan S. Global prevalence of
depressive and anxiety symptoms in children and adolescents during COVID-19: a meta-
analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2021;175(11):1142-1150. doi:
10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.2482 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

28. National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. Study quality assessment tools: quality
assessment tool for observational cohort and cross-sectional studies. National Institutes
of Health. Accessed February 10, 2021. https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-
quality-assessment-tools

29. Cicchetti DV. Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and
standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychol Assess. 1994;6(4):284–290.
[Google Scholar ]

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 25/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

30. Caldwell DM, Davies SR, Hetrick SE, et al. School-based interventions to prevent
anxiety and depression in children and young people: a systematic review and network
meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. 2019;6(12):1011-1020. doi: 10.1016/S2215-
0366(19)30403-1 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

31. Brumariu LE, Madigan S, Giuseppone KR, Movahed Abtahi M, Kerns KA. The Security
Scale as a measure of attachment: meta-analytic evidence of validity. Attach Hum Dev.
2018;20(6):600-625. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1433217 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

32. Witzel TC, Eshun-Wilson I, Jamil MS, et al. Comparing the effects of HIV self-testing to
standard HIV testing for key populations: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC
Med. 2020;18(1):381. doi: 10.1186/s12916-020-01835-z [DOI ] [PMC free article]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

33. Rosenthal R. Writing meta-analytic reviews. Psychol Bull. 1995;118(2):183-192. doi:


10.1037/0033-2909.118.2.183 [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

34. Peterson RA, Brown SP. On the use of beta coefficients in meta-analysis. J Appl
Psychol. 2005;90(1):175-181. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.175 [DOI ] [PubMed] [
Google Scholar ]

35. Borenstein M, Hedges L, Higgins JPT, Rothstein HR. Comprehensive Meta-Analysis


Version 3.0. Biostat; 2013. [Google Scholar ]

36. DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials.


1986;7(3):177-188. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(86)90046-2 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

37. Funder DC, Ozer DJ. Evaluating effect size in psychological research: sense and
nonsense. Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. 2019;2(2):156-168. doi:
10.1177/2515245919847202 [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

38. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a


simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629-634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629 [
DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

39. Borenstein M. Introduction to Meta-Analysis. John Wiley & Sons; 2009. doi:
10.1002/9780470743386 [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

40. Huesmann LR. The impact of electronic media violence: scientific theory and
research. J Adolesc Health. 2007;41(6)(suppl 1):S6-S13. doi:

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 26/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.09.005 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google


Scholar ]

41. Christakis DA, Zimmerman FJ, DiGiuseppe DL, McCarty CA. Early television exposure
and subsequent attentional problems in children. Pediatrics. 2004;113(4):708-713. doi:
10.1542/peds.113.4.708 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

42. Liu M, Wu L, Yao S. Dose-response association of screen time-based sedentary


behaviour in children and adolescents and depression: a meta-analysis of observational
studies. Br J Sports Med. 2016;50(20):1252-1258. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2015-095084 [
DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

43. Guerrero MD, Barnes JD, Chaput J-P, Tremblay MS. Screen time and problem
behaviors in children: exploring the mediating role of sleep duration. Int J Behav Nutr
Phys Act. 2019;16(1):105. doi: 10.1186/s12966-019-0862-x [DOI ] [PMC free article]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

44. Al-Ali NM, Yaghy HS, Shattnawi KK, Al-Shdayfat NM. Parents’ knowledge and beliefs
about the impact of exposure to media violence on children’s aggression. Issues Ment
Health Nurs. 2018;39(7):592-599. doi: 10.1080/01612840.2017.1422201 [DOI ]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

45. Allen MS, Vella SA. Screen-based sedentary behaviour and psychosocial well-being in
childhood: cross-sectional and longitudinal associations. Ment Health Phys Act.
2015;9:41-47. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2015.10.002 [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

46. Amawi SO, Subki AH, Khatib HA, et al. Use of electronic entertainment and
communication devices among a Saudi pediatric population: cross-sectional study.
Interact J Med Res. 2018;7(2):e13. doi: 10.2196/ijmr.9103 [DOI ] [PMC free article]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

47. Barlett ND, Gentile DA, Barlett CP, Eisenmann JC, Walsh DA. Sleep as a mediator of
screen time effects on US children’s health outcomes. J Child Media. 2012;6(1):37-50.
doi: 10.1080/17482798.2011.633404 [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

48. Byun Y-H, Ha M, Kwon H-J, et al. Mobile phone use, blood lead levels, and attention
deficit hyperactivity symptoms in children: a longitudinal study. PLoS One.
2013;8(3):e59742. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0059742 [DOI ] [PMC free article]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

49. Cao H, Yan S, Gu C, et al. Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder


symptoms and their associations with sleep schedules and sleep-related problems

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 27/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

among preschoolers in mainland China. BMC Pediatr. 2018;18(1):70. doi:


10.1186/s12887-018-1022-1 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

50. Chonchaiya W, Sirachairat C, Vijakkhana N, Wilaisakditipakorn T, Pruksananonda C.


Elevated background TV exposure over time increases behavioural scores of 18-month-
old toddlers. Acta Paediatr. 2015;104(10):1039-1046. doi: 10.1111/apa.13067 [DOI ]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

51. Christodoulou G, Majmundar A, Chou C-P, Pentz MA. Anhedonia, screen time, and
substance use in early adolescents: a longitudinal mediation analysis. J Adolesc.
2020;78(C):24-32. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.11.007 [DOI ] [PMC free article]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

52. Coker TR, Elliott MN, Schwebel DC, et al. Media violence exposure and physical
aggression in fifth-grade children. Acad Pediatr. 2015;15(1):82-88. doi:
10.1016/j.acap.2014.09.008 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

53. Corkin MT, Peterson ER, Henderson AME, Waldie KE, Reese E, Morton SMB. Preschool
screen media exposure, executive functions and symptoms of inattention/hyperactivity. J
Appl Dev Psychol. 2021;73:101237. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2020.101237 [DOI ] [Google
Scholar ]

54. Davison LL. Media Viewing and the Neurological Development of ADHD: Voluntary
and Involuntary Processing. Dissertation. Claremont Graduate University; 2004. [Google
Scholar ]

55. Ebenegger V, Marques-Vidal P-M, Munsch S, et al. Relationship of


hyperactivity/inattention with adiposity and lifestyle characteristics in preschool
children. J Child Neurol. 2012;27(7):852-858. doi: 10.1177/0883073811428009 [DOI ]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

56. Erdogan A, Kiran S, Aydogan G, et al. . Behavioral correlates of television viewing time
in a Turkish sample of preschool children. Neurol Psychiatr Brain Res. 2006;13(4):225-
230. [Google Scholar ]

57. Froiland JM, Davison ML. Home literacy, television viewing, fidgeting and ADHD in
young children. Educational Psychology. 2016;36(8):1337-1353. doi:
10.1080/01443410.2014.963031 [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

58. Gentile DA, Berch ON, Choo H, Khoo A, Walsh DA. Bedroom media: one risk factor for
development. Dev Psychol. 2017;53(12):2340-2355. doi: 10.1037/dev0000399 [DOI ]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 28/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

59. Gialamas A, Haag DG, Mittinty MN, Lynch J. Which time investments in the first 5
years of life matter most for children’s language and behavioural outcomes at school
entry? Int J Epidemiol. 2020;49(2):548-558. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyz192 [DOI ] [PubMed]
[Google Scholar ]

60. Guxens M, Vermeulen R, Steenkamer I, et al. Radiofrequency electromagnetic fields,


screen time, and emotional and behavioural problems in 5-year-old children. Int J Hyg
Environ Health. 2019;222(2):188-194. doi: 10.1016/j.ijheh.2018.09.006 [DOI ]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

61. Hastings EC, Karas TL, Winsler A, Way E, Madigan A, Tyler S. Young children’s
video/computer game use: relations with school performance and behavior. Issues Ment
Health Nurs. 2009;30(10):638-649. doi: 10.1080/01612840903050414 [DOI ] [PMC
free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

62. Hefner HL. Risks of Television Viewing Among Children With and Without the ADHD
Diagnosis. Dissertation. Walden University; 2013. [Google Scholar ]

63. Hill MM, Gangi D, Miller M, Rafi SM, Ozonoff S. Screen time in 36-month-olds at
increased likelihood for ASD and ADHD. Infant Behav Dev. 2020;61:101484. [DOI ] [PMC
free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

64. Holton KF, Nigg JT. The association of lifestyle factors and ADHD in children. J Atten
Disord. 2020;24(11):1511-1520. doi: 10.1177/1087054716646452 [DOI ] [PMC free
article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

65. Hong GCC, Conduit R, Wong J, Di Benedetto M, Lee E. Diet, physical activity, and
screen time to sleep better: multiple mediation analysis of lifestyle factors in school-aged
children with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Atten Disord.
Published July 14, 2020. [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

66. Hosokawa R, Katsura T. Association between mobile technology use and child
adjustment in early elementary school age. PLoS One. 2018;13(7):e0199959. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0199959 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

67. Huesmann LR, Eron LD. Cognitive processes and the persistence of aggressive
behavior. Aggress Behav. 1984;10(3):243-251. doi: [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

68. Jia S, Wang L, Shi Y, Li P. Family risk factors associated with aggressive behavior in
Chinese preschool children. J Pediatr Nurs. 2016;31(6):e367-e374. doi:
10.1016/j.pedn.2016.08.001 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 29/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

69. Kahn M, Schnabel O, Gradisar M, et al. Sleep, screen time and behaviour problems in
preschool children: an actigraphy study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. Published online
October 1, 2020. [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

70. Kuhhirt M, Klein M. Parental education, television exposure, and children's early
cognitive, language and behavioral development. Soc Sci Res. 2020;86:102391. [DOI ]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

71. Linebarger DL. Contextualizing video game play: the moderating effects of cumulative
risk and parenting styles on the relations among video game exposure and problem
behaviors. Psychol Pop Media Cult. 2015;4(4):375-396. doi: 10.1037/ppm0000069 [
DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

72. Levelink B, van der Vlegel M, Mommers M, et al. The longitudinal relationship
between screen time, sleep and a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in
childhood. J Atten Disord. 2020. [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

73. Lin H-P, Chen K-L, Chou W, et al. Prolonged touch screen device usage is associated
with emotional and behavioral problems, but not language delay, in toddlers. Infant
Behav Dev. 2020;58:101424. [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

74. Lobel A, Engels RCME, Stone LL, Burk WJ, Granic I. Video gaming and children’s
psychosocial wellbeing: a longitudinal study. J Youth Adolesc. 2017;46(4):884-897. doi:
10.1007/s10964-017-0646-z [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

75. Manganello JA, Taylor CA. Television exposure as a risk factor for aggressive behavior
among 3-year-old children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(11):1037-1045. doi:
10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.193 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

76. Martin A, Razza R, Brooks-Gunn J. Specifying the links between household chaos and
preschool children’s development. Early Child Dev Care. 2012;182(10):1247-1263. doi:
10.1080/03004430.2011.605522 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

77. Martins N, Wilson BJ. Social aggression on television and its relationship to children’s
aggression in the classroom. Hum Commun Res. 2012;38(1):48-71. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-
2958.2011.01417.x [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

78. McNeill J, Howard SJ, Vella SA, Cliff DP. Longitudinal associations of electronic
application use and media program viewing with cognitive and psychosocial
development in preschoolers. Acad Pediatr. 2019;19(5):520-528. doi:
10.1016/j.acap.2019.02.010 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 30/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

79. Miller CJ, Marks DJ, Miller SR, et al. Brief report: television viewing and risk for
attention problems in preschool children. J Pediatr Psychol. 2007;32(4):448-452. doi:
10.1093/jpepsy/jsl035 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

80. Miller LE, Grabell A, Thomas A, Bermann E, Graham-Bermann SA. The associations
between community violence, television violence, intimate partner violence, parent–
child aggression, and aggression in sibling relationships of a sample of preschoolers.
Psychol Violence. 2012;2(2):165-178. doi: 10.1037/a0027254 [DOI ] [Google
Scholar ]

81. Mistry KB, Minkovitz CS, Strobino DM, Borzekowski DLG. Children’s television
exposure and behavioral and social outcomes at 5.5 years: does timing of exposure
matter? Pediatrics. 2007;120(4):762-769. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-3573 [DOI ]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

82. Moyer VS. The Role of User Motivations in Moderating the Relation Between Video
Game Playing and Children's Adjustment. Dissertation. Bowling Green State University;
2008. [Google Scholar ]

83. Mundy LK, Canterford L, Olds T, Allen NB, Patton GC. The association between
electronic media and emotional and behavioral problems in late childhood. Acad Pediatr.
2017;17(6):620-624. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2016.12.014 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

84. Niiranen J, Kiviruusu O, Vornanen R, Saarenpä ä -Heikkilä O, Paavonen EJ. High-dose


electronic media use in five-year-olds and its association with their psychosocial
symptoms: a cohort study. BMJ Open. 2021;11(3):e040848. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-
2020-040848 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

85. Nikkelen SWC, Vossen HGM, Valkenburg PM. Children’s television viewing and ADHD-
related behaviors: evidence from The Netherlands. J Child Media. 2015;9(4):399-418.
doi: 10.1080/17482798.2015.1088872 [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

86. Obel C, Henriksen TB, Dalsgaard S, et al. Does children’s watching of television cause
attention problems? retesting the hypothesis in a Danish cohort. Pediatrics.
2004;114(5):1372-1373. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0954 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

87. Ö zmert E, Toyran M, Yurdakö k K. Behavioral correlates of television viewing in


primary school children evaluated by the child behavior checklist. Arch Pediatr Adolesc
Med. 2002;156(9):910-914. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.156.9.910 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 31/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

88. Parkes A, Sweeting H, Wight D, Henderson M. Do television and electronic games


predict children’s psychosocial adjustment? Longitudinal research using the UK
Millennium Cohort Study. Arch Dis Child. 2013;98(5):341-348. doi:
10.1136/archdischild-2011-301508 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

89. Patel C. Co-occurrence and Non-overlap Among Behavioral and Health-Related Problems
in Preschool Children. Dissertation. University of South Carolina; 2019.

90. Paulus FW, Sinzig J, Mayer H, Weber M, von Gontard A. Computer gaming disorder
and ADHD in young children—a population-based study. Int J Ment Health Addict.
2018;16(5):1193-1207. doi: 10.1007/s11469-017-9841-0 [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

91. Peralta GP, Forns J, García de la Hera M, et al. Sleeping, TV, cognitively stimulating
activities, physical activity, and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptom
incidence in children: a prospective study. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2018;39(3):192-199. doi:
10.1097/DBP.0000000000000539 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

92. Poulain T, Vogel M, Neef M, et al. Reciprocal associations between electronic media
use and behavioral difficulties in preschoolers. Int J Environ Res Public Health.
2018;15(4):814. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15040814 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [
Google Scholar ]

93. Reynolds JE. An Assessment of Variables Associated With Television Viewing and Their
Influence on Aggressive Behavior of Third Grade Children. Dissertation. Southern Illinois
University. ProQuest Dissertations; 1978. [Google Scholar ]

94. Rosen LD, Lim AF, Felt J, et al. Media and technology use predicts ill-being among
children, preteens and teenagers independent of the negative health impacts of exercise
and eating habits. Comput Human Behav. 2014;35:364-375. doi:
10.1016/j.chb.2014.01.036 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

95. Sanders W, Parent J, Forehand R, Sullivan ADW, Jones DJ. Parental perceptions of
technology and technology-focused parenting: associations with youth screen time. J
Appl Dev Psychol. 2016;44:28-38. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2016.02.005 [DOI ] [PMC free
article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

96. Schaefer WG. The Effects of Television Viewing on the Academic Performance of
Elementary School Children With Attention Deficit Disorder. Dissertation. Western
Michigan University; 1991. [Google Scholar ]

97. Schmiedeler S, Niklas F, Schneider W. Symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity


disorder (ADHD) and home learning environment (HLE): findings from a longitudinal

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 32/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

study. Eur J Psychol Educ. 2014;29(3):467-482. doi: 10.1007/s10212-013-0208-z [


DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

98. Sé guin D, Klimek V. Just five more minutes please: electronic media use, sleep and
behaviour in young children. Early Child Dev Care. 2016;186(6):981-1000. doi:
10.1080/03004430.2015.1071528 [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

99. Sheehan PW. Age trends and the correlates of children’s television viewing. Aust J
Psychol. 1983;35(3):417-431. doi: 10.1080/00049538308258753 [DOI ] [Google
Scholar ]

100. Shmukler D. A descriptive analysis of television viewing in South African


Preschoolers and its relationship to their spontaneous play. S Afr J Psychol.
1981;11(3):106-110. doi: 10.1177/008124638101100303 [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

101. Stenseng F, Hygen BW, Wichstrøm L. Time spent gaming and psychiatric symptoms
in childhood: cross-sectional associations and longitudinal effects. Eur Child Adolesc
Psychiatry. 2020;29(6):839-847. doi: 10.1007/s00787-019-01398-2 [DOI ] [PubMed] [
Google Scholar ]

102. Stevens T, Mulsow M. There is no meaningful relationship between television


exposure and symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics.
2006;117(3):665-672. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0863 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

103. Sugawara M, Matsumoto S, Murohashi H, Sakai A, Isshiki N.. Trajectories of early


television contact in Japan: relationship with preschoolers’ externalizing problems. J
Child Media. 2015;9(4):453-471. doi: 10.1080/17482798.2015.1089298 [DOI ] [
Google Scholar ]

104. Swing EL, Gentile DA, Anderson CA, Walsh DA. Television and video game exposure
and the development of attention problems. Pediatrics. 2010;126(2):214-221. doi:
10.1542/peds.2009-1508 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

105. Tamana SK, Ezeugwu V, Chikuma J, et al. ; CHILD study Investigators . Screen-time is
associated with inattention problems in preschoolers: results from the CHILD birth
cohort study. PLoS One. 2019;14(4):e0213995. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0213995 [
DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

106. Tansriratanawong S, Louthrenoo O, Chonchaiya W, Charnsil C. Screen viewing time


and externalising problems in pre-school children in Northern Thailand. J Child Adolesc
Ment Health. 2017;29(3):245-252. doi: 10.2989/17280583.2017.1409226 [DOI ]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]
https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 33/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

107. Teramoto S, Soeda A, Hayashi Y, Saito K, Urashima M. Problematic behaviours of 3-


year-old children in Japan: relationship with socioeconomic and family backgrounds.
Early Hum Dev. 2005;81(6):563-569. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.12.011 [DOI ]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

108. Verlinden M, Tiemeier H, Hudziak JJ, et al. Television viewing and externalizing
problems in preschool children: the Generation R Study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med.
2012;166(10):919-925. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2012.653 [DOI ] [PubMed] [
Google Scholar ]

109. Viemerö V, Paajanen S. The role of fantasies and dreams in the TV viewing—
aggression relationship. Aggress Behav. 1992;18(2):109-116. doi: [DOI ] [Google
Scholar ]

110. Wan MW, Fitch-Bunce C, Heron K, Lester E. Infant screen media usage and social-
emotional functioning. Infant Behav Dev. 2021;62:101509. doi:
10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101509 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

111. Wimbarti S. Children’s Aggression in Indonesia: The Effects of Culture, Familial


Factors, Peers, TV Violence Viewing, and Temperament. Dissertation. University of
Southern California; 2002. [Google Scholar ]

112. Woodfield DL. Mass Media Viewing Habits and Toleration of Real-life Aggression.
Dissertation. In: ProQuest Dissertations; 1987. [Google Scholar ]

113. Wu X, Kirk SFL, Ohinmaa A, Veugelers P. Health behaviours, body weight and self-
esteem among grade five students in Canada. Springerplus. 2016;5(1):1099. doi:
10.1186/s40064-016-2744-x [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

114. Wu X, Tao S, Rutayisire E, Chen Y, Huang K, Tao F. The relationship between screen
time, nighttime sleep duration, and behavioural problems in preschool children in China.
Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;26(5):541-548. doi: 10.1007/s00787-016-0912-8 [
DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

115. Xie G, Deng Q, Cao J, Chang Q. Digital screen time and its effect on preschoolers’
behavior in China: results from a cross-sectional study. Ital J Pediatr. 2020;46(1):9. doi:
10.1186/s13052-020-0776-x [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

116. Yousef S, Eapen V, Zoubeidi T, Mabrouk A. Behavioral correlation with television


watching and videogame playing among children in the United Arab Emirates. Int J
Psychiatry Clin Pract. 2014;18(3):203-207. doi: 10.3109/13651501.2013.874442 [
DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 34/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

117. Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA. Associations between content types of early media
exposure and subsequent attentional problems. Pediatrics. 2007;120(5):986-992. doi:
10.1542/peds.2006-3322 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

118. Zimmerman FJ, Glew GM, Christakis DA, Katon W. Early cognitive stimulation,
emotional support, and television watching as predictors of subsequent bullying among
grade-school children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2005;159(4):384-388. doi:
10.1001/archpedi.159.4.384 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

119. Brubaker MM. Exploring the Relationship Between Social Support and Social
Behavior: Moderating Effects of Video Game Use. Dissertation. Fielding Graduate
University. ProQuest Dissertations Publishing; 2020. [Google Scholar ]

120. Cooper NS. The Identification of Psychological and Social Correlates of internet Use in
Children and Teenagers. Dissertation. Alliant International University; 2004. [Google
Scholar ]

121. Dunkeld C, Wright ML, Banerjee RA, Easterbrook MJ, Slade L. Television exposure,
consumer culture values, and lower well-being among preadolescent children: the
mediating role of consumer-focused coping strategies. Br J Soc Psychol. 2020;59(1):26-
48. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12325 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

122. Lam MW, Burgoon E, Jin C. 5.37 Associations of screen time and depression in
children in a US nationally representative sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry.
2019;58 (10)(suppl):S257. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.08.351 [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

123. Lin S-Y, Eaton NR, Schleider JL. Unpacking associations between mood symptoms
and screen time in preadolescents: a network analysis. J Abnorm Child Psychol.
2020;48(12):1635-1647. doi: 10.1007/s10802-020-00703-x [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

124. Zhang J, Liu M-W, Yu H-J, et al. Associations of health-risk behaviors with mental
health among Chinese children. Psychol Health Med. Published December 9, 2020. [DOI ]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

125. Zhu X, Haegele JA, Healy S. Movement and mental health: behavioral correlates of
anxiety and depression among children of 6-17 years old in the US. Ment Health Phys
Act. 2019;16:60-65. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2019.04.002 [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

126. Madigan S, McArthur BA, Anhorn C, Eirich R, Christakis DA. Associations between
screen use and child language skills: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA
Pediatr. 2020;174(7):665-675. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.0327 [DOI ] [PMC
free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]
https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 35/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

127. Adelantado-Renau M, Moliner-Urdiales D, Cavero-Redondo I, Beltran-Valls MR,


Martínez-Vizcaíno V, Á lvarez-Bueno C. Association between screen media use and
academic performance among children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-
analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(11):1058-1067. doi:
10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.3176 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

128. Hentges RF, Devereux C, Graham SA, Madigan S. Child language difficulties and
internalizing and externalizing symptoms: a meta-analysis. Child Dev. 2021;92(4):e691-
e715. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13540 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

129. Peverill M, Dirks MA, Narvaja T, Herts KL, Comer JS, McLaughlin KA. Socioeconomic
status and child psychopathology in the United States: a meta-analysis of population-
based studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2021;83:101933. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101933 [
DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

130. Rutledge T, Loh C. Effect sizes and statistical testing in the determination of clinical
significance in behavioral medicine research. Ann Behav Med. 2004;27(2):138-145. doi:
10.1207/s15324796abm2702_9 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

131. McArthur BA, Volkova V, Tomopoulos S, Madigan S. Global prevalence of meeting


screen time guidelines among children 5 years and younger: a systematic review and
meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. Published online February 14, 2022. doi:
10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.6386 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

132. Groh AM, Roisman GI, van Ijzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Fearon RP.
The significance of insecure and disorganized attachment for children’s internalizing
symptoms: a meta-analytic study. Child Dev. 2012;83(2):591-610. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-
8624.2011.01711.x [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

133. Hoyos Cillero I, Jago R. Sociodemographic and home environment predictors of


screen viewing among Spanish school children. J Public Health (Oxf). 2011;33(3):392-
402. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdq087 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

134. Mesman J, Bongers IL, Koot HM. Preschool developmental pathways to


preadolescent internalizing and externalizing problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry.
2001;42(5):679-689. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00763 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

135. Tomopoulos S, Dreyer BP, Valdez P, et al. Media content and externalizing behaviors
in Latino toddlers. Ambul Pediatr. 2007;7(3):232-238. doi: 10.1016/j.ambp.2007.02.004
https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 36/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

[DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

136. Funk JB, Baldacci HB, Pasold T, Baumgardner J. Violence exposure in real-life, video
games, television, movies, and the internet: is there desensitization? J Adolesc.
2004;27(1):23-39. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.10.005 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

137. Bandura A. Social Learning Theory. Prentice-Hall; 1977. [Google Scholar ]

138. World Health Organization. Guidelines on Physical Activity, Sedentary Behaviour and
Sleep for Children Under 5 Years of Age. World Health Organization; 2019. Accessed
February 9, 2022. https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/311664 [PubMed]

139. Madigan S, Racine N, Tough S. Prevalence of preschoolers meeting vs exceeding


screen time guidelines. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(1):93-95. doi:
10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.4495 [DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

140. Sigman A. Time for a view on screen time. Arch Dis Child. 2012;97(11):935-942.
doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2012-302196 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

141. Harvey DL, Milton K, Jones AP, Atkin AJ. International trends in screen-based
behaviours from 2012 to 2019. Prev Med. 2022;154:106909. doi:
10.1016/j.ypmed.2021.106909 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

142. Paulo MS, Nauman J, Abdulle A, et al. Results from the United Arab Emirates’ 2018
report card on physical activity for children and youth. J Phys Act Health.
2018;15(S2):S419-S421. doi: 10.1123/jpah.2018-0543 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

143. Ernest JM, Causey C, Newton AB, Sharkins K, Summerlin J, Albaiz N. Extending the
global dialogue about media, technology, screen time, and young children. Childhood
Education. 2014;90(3):182-191. doi: 10.1080/00094056.2014.910046 [DOI ] [Google
Scholar ]

144. De Los Reyes A, Kazdin AE. Informant discrepancies in the assessment of childhood
psychopathology: a critical review, theoretical framework, and recommendations for
further study. Psychol Bull. 2005;131(4):483-509. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.483 [
DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

145. De Los Reyes A. Introduction to the special section: more than measurement error:
discovering meaning behind informant discrepancies in clinical assessments of children

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 37/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

and adolescents. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2011;40(1):1-9. doi:


10.1080/15374416.2011.533405 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

146. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock CS. The classification of child psychopathology: a review
and analysis of empirical efforts. Psychol Bull. 1978;85(6):1275-1301. doi:
10.1037/0033-2909.85.6.1275 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

147. Greenbaum PE, Dedrick RF. Hierarchical confirmatory factor analysis of the Child
Behavior Checklist/4-18. Psychol Assess. 1998;10(2):149-155. doi: 10.1037/1040-
3590.10.2.149 [DOI ] [Google Scholar ]

148. Radesky JS, Weeks HM, Ball R, et al. Young children’s use of smartphones and
tablets. Pediatrics. 2020;146(1):e20193518. doi: 10.1542/peds.2019-3518 [DOI ]
[PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

149. Ioannidis JP. Why most discovered true associations are inflated. Epidemiology.
2008;19(5):640-648. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e31818131e7 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google
Scholar ]

150. Schooler J. Unpublished results hide the decline effect. Nature.


2011;470(7335):437. [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

151. de Bruin A, Della Sala S. The decline effect: how initially strong results tend to
decrease over time. Cortex. 2015;73:375-377. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.025 [DOI ]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

152. Chen W, Adler JL. Assessment of screen exposure in young children, 1997 to 2014.
JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(4):391-393. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.5546 [DOI ]
[PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

153. van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Differential susceptibility


experiments: going beyond correlational evidence: comment on beyond mental health,
differential susceptibility articles. Dev Psychol. 2012;48(3):769-774. doi:
10.1037/a0027536 [DOI ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

154. Sudan M, Olsen J, Arah OA, Obel C, Kheifets L. Prospective cohort analysis of
cellphone use and emotional and behavioural difficulties in children. J Epidemiol
Community Health. 2016;70(12):1207-1213. doi: 10.1136/jech-2016-207419 [DOI ]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

155. Christakis DA. The challenges of defining and studying “digital addiction” in
children. JAMA. 2019;321(23):2277-2278. doi: 10.1001/jama.2019.4690 [DOI ]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 38/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

156. Shah RR, Fahey NM, Soni AV, Phatak AG, Nimbalkar SM. Screen time usage among
preschoolers aged 2-6 in rural Western India: a cross-sectional study. J Family Med Prim
Care. 2019;8(6):1999-2002. doi: 10.4103/jfmpc.jfmpc_206_19 [DOI ] [PMC free article]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

157. Tang L, Darlington G, Ma DWL, Haines J; Guelph Family Health Study . Mothers’ and
fathers’ media parenting practices associated with young children’s screen-time: a cross-
sectional study. BMC Obes. 2018;5(1):37. doi: 10.1186/s40608-018-0214-4 [DOI ]
[PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

158. Korhonen L. The good, the bad and the ugly of children´s screen time during the
COVID-19 pandemic. Acta Paediatr. 2021;110(10):2671-2672. doi: 10.1111/apa.16012 [
DOI ] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

159. Xiang M, Zhang Z, Kuwahara K. Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on children and


adolescents’ lifestyle behavior larger than expected. Prog Cardiovasc Dis.
2020;63(4):531-532. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2020.04.013 [DOI ] [PMC free article]
[PubMed] [Google Scholar ]

160. Hamaker EL, Kuiper RM, Grasman RPPP. A critique of the cross-lagged panel model.
Psychol Methods. 2015;20(1):102-116. doi: 10.1037/a0038889 [DOI ] [PubMed] [
Google Scholar ]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in
this article.

Supplementary Materials

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 39/40
24/11/24, 20:13 Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systemat…

Supplement.

eTable 1. Search Strategy

eTable 2. Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

eTable 3. Study Quality Evaluation Coding Criteria

eTable 4. Study Quality Scoring for Each Study Included in the Meta-analysis

eTable 5. Coding System of Study Variables

eTable 6. Study and Sample Characteristics

eFigure 1. Funnel Plot Depicting Standard Error by Fisher Z of Included Studies in Meta-
analysis of Screen Time and Externalizing Problems

eFigure 2. Funnel Plot Depicting Standard Error by Fisher Z of Included Studies in Meta-
analysis of Screen Time and Internalizing Problems

eReferences

Click here for additional data file. (681KB, pdf)

Articles from JAMA Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of American Medical Association

https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8928099/ 40/40

You might also like