Excerpt
Excerpt
chapter one
1.1 Introduction
This chapter is a brief introduction to the theory, terminology and tech-
niques used in the scientific study of bird songs. Although everyone may
assume that they do know what a bird song is, how does it differ from the
other sounds that birds make? There are calls, notes, syllables and phrases
to consider – and what are repertoires? Before we start using these words,
it is just as well to define them and become acquainted with a terminology
which can be confusing. Only then can we move on to consider the role of
song in the lives of birds and to review the many studies that have
attempted to shed some light upon it. Animal communication is a rapidly
expanding field, and at this early stage it is also useful to consider some of
the recent theoretical background. For example, what is Ôcommunication’
and how do we know it has occurred? What is Ôinformation’ and who
benefits from sending and receiving it? What are Ôsignals’ and why is
sound transmission particularly effective? Finally, recording, analysing
and experimenting with sounds is a highly technical field, currently being
revolutionised by the use of computers at various points. We will also
attempt to give the reader just a brief introduction to some of the more
important techniques and any recent developments. But first, let us set the
current study of bird song in its historical context.
1.2 History
Clearly, from references to it in music and in literature, particularly
poetry, people have been interested in bird song for a very long
time. But its detailed scientific study is a comparatively recent
phenomenon. One reason for this is that making a permanent record of
it, as we now do routinely on tapes or discs, was not easy until half a
century or so ago. Song can be so rapid and complicated that only with
such a permanent record that could be slowed down, repeated and ana-
lysed in various ways, is it possible to make a serious study of many aspects
of it.
Some interesting work was done before that time. The Hon. Daines
Barrington wrote a letter to the President of the Royal Society in 1773
recounting a variety of observations he had made. He established the
existence of song learning, for example, because he heard the song of
a wren emanating from a house he was passing and, knowing how difficult
such birds were to keep in captivity, knocked on the door out of curiosity,
only to discover that the singer was a captive goldfinch. Presumably this
bird had been exposed to wren song at some stage and had picked it up.
At around the same time, in 1789, the great English parson and naturalist
Gilbert White described how birds previously known as willow wrens
could be separated by their songs into three separate species. These we
now call the willow warbler, the wood warbler and the chiff-chaff. Those
with a good ear were also able to detect that birds had repertoires of songs
and study the way these were strung together into sequences, as Craig
(1943) did with eastern wood pewee song, or that song could vary from
place to place, as found by Marler (1952) for the chaffinches singing in
different glens in the Scottish highlands.
The depth of such studies was severely limited, not just by lack of
the possibility of recording, except latterly on wax drums, but most
importantly by the lack of analytical equipment. The real revolution
came with the invention of the sound spectrograph, first used to provide
a visual representation of song by Thorpe in 1954. Such equipment was
not cheap, and therefore its use was somewhat restricted, but it still led
to a huge growth in studies of song. Today, equivalent visualisations of
song, together with many other forms of analysis, can be carried out
using a variety of computer packages at a fraction of the cost. The detailed
study of bird song is within the scope and budget of many laboratories and
even amateurs: as a result the subject is advancing with great strides.
Thanks to these very powerful techniques, there are now few areas of
animal behaviour research that have not been illuminated by studies of
bird song.
back a tape recording of a male great tit song to another male, we may
cause the second male to respond by approaching the speaker and dis-
playing aggressively. As the song appears to have modified his behaviour,
we are entitled to conclude that communication has occurred. This is a
somewhat restricted definition of communication, as it relies upon a
behavioural response and thus excludes passive signal detection by the
receiver. For example, if we repeated the experiment on another great tit
and obtained no response, it may be that the great tit had heard the song
but decided for some reason not to respond. Such behaviour may often
occur, so we must temper our definition with caution and also be sure to
carry out a number of experiments before we draw any firm conclusions.
Although it may be relatively easy to demonstrate that communication
takes place, it is much more difficult to suggest why it has evolved. Early
theories emphasised the benefits that might accrue to both the sender and
the receiver (e.g. Smith 1977) and saw communication as a sharing of
information between individuals to their mutual advantage. Modern ethol-
ogists are much more inclined to view communication as the outcome of
conflict rather than cooperation between sender and receiver.
Krebs & Dawkins (1984) examined how different kinds of signals may
result from the benefits that accrue to senders and receivers. Sometimes,
cooperation rather than conflict is involved, and they suggest that a system
which benefits both receiver and sender would give rise to the evolution of
relatively quiet, inconspicuous signals. For example, a great tit may give
an alarm call to warn its fledglings that a sparrowhawk is approaching. The
call should be loud enough to reach the fledglings but not loud enough to
reach the hawk and give away the position of the caller. To be able to hear
the call, the fledglings should develop sensitive hearing, and so a coevolu-
tionary process will lead to the production of calls that are Ôcost-minimiz-
ing conspiratorial whispers’. The fascinating story of the evolution of
alarm calls, and their possible detection by predators, will be discussed
in detail later in the book.
But where the interests of sender and receiver conflict, as in a territorial
dispute, there will be a different kind of coevolution. Here, Krebs &
Dawkins (1984) suggest that there is an evolutionary arms race between
Ômanipulation’ by the sender and Ôsales resistance’ by the receiver. The
male great tit this time sings as long and loud as he can manage, simply to
force his message across. As we will see in later chapters, loudness and
repetition are a particular feature of the songs of males when defending
song as a key example. The latter point out that reliability requires there to
be a correlation between some aspect of the signal and some attribute of
the signaler that the receiver benefits from knowing about. Hence the
receiver benefits from assessing the signal rather than ignoring it. Deceit
requires not only that the correlation between signal and attribute be
broken, but that the signaler benefits from that breakdown. Searcy &
Nowicki (2005) present a detailed review of this complex area and discuss
how opinions about the prevalence of reliability and deception in animal
communication have changed over the years.
The apparent coevolutionary arms race between senders and receivers
involves many different aspects of communication, which we will be
considering throughout this book. It is important to emphasise that we
will not just consider the signal (song) itself, but how it is transmitted
through the environment, how it is perceived by receivers and, in partic-
ular, how males and females react to both natural and experimental signals.
switch to another. Each version is called a song type, and the male chaf-
finch is said to have a repertoire of song types (see Fig. 1.1, and the more
detailed discussion of repertoires in Chapter 8).
Moving our analysis to a more detailed level, we can also see that each
chaffinch song consists of a number of distinct sections. These are called
phrases, and each phrase consists of a series of units which occur together
in a particular pattern. Sometimes, the units in a phrase are all different, as
in the end phrase shown in Fig. 1.1. The units themselves are usually
referred to as syllables. Syllables can be very simple or quite complex in
their structure. When complex, they are constructed from several of the
smallest building blocks of all, called elements or notes (but the latter is
usually avoided because of its musical connotations). One definition of an
element is simply a continuous line on a sonagram, as illustrated in Fig. 1.1.
Songs, syllables and elements can also be defined by the time intervals
which separate them, intersong intervals are the longest, and so on down-
wards. Because of the great variety of form and structure in songs, indi-
vidual workers often use and define their own terms, which may be slightly
different from those given above. These can only serve as a general guide
for this book, for there can be linguistic as well as technical problems with
definitions. For example, in German there are two different words for
Ôsong’: Ôgesang’ means the song of a particular species, whereas Ôstrophe’
means a particular delivery of a song. This last word is now often adopted
in English to refer to a single rendition.
10