Karen C.H Zhoc (2018) Trí tuệ cảm xúc (EI) và học tập tự do
Karen C.H Zhoc (2018) Trí tuệ cảm xúc (EI) và học tập tự do
© 2018 by Karen C. H. Zhoc, Tony S. H. Chung and Ronnel B. King is licensed under CC BY-
ref=chooser-v1
Citation:
Zhoc, K. C., Chung, T. S., & King, R. B. (2018). Emotional intelligence (EI) and self‐directed learning:
Examining their relation and contribution to better student learning outcomes in higher education. British
Link:
https://doi.org/10.1002/berj.3472
British Educational Research Journal
Vol. 44, No. 6, December 2018, pp. 982–1004
DOI: 10.1002/berj.3472
Introduction
With technological advancement, information and massive data can be relayed
instantly, accelerating the growth of knowledge and change in society. In response to
the rapid pace of change in modern society, self-directed learning has been deemed
an important educational aim in many countries, such as Hong Kong, Taiwan,
Korea, Japan and Thailand (OECD, 2000; Mok et al., 2007). In the context of
higher education, self-directed learning has also been fostered greatly. In addition to
its relevance in meeting the needs of society, self-directed learning is highly valued in
higher education because its emphasis on personal autonomy, personal responsibility
and personal growth embodies the core values of higher education (Wilcox, 1996).
Hence, other than self-directed learning, other similar concepts such as autonomous
learning and lifelong learning are emphasised and designated as generic graduate
attributes in many universities all over the world (Chemers et al., 2001; Macaskill &
Denovan, 2013).
In Hong Kong, similar to many other countries, an important mission of higher
education is to nurture globally competitive students equipped with the capacity for
lifelong learning, so as to cope with the challenges of a more dynamic future (Univer-
sity Grants Committee, 2010). In relation to this, there was a revamp of the university
system in Hong Kong. More specifically, since September 2012, universities have
moved from a 3-year university system favouring early specialisation within a disci-
pline to a 4-year system that allows disciplinary study to be grounded in a broader
knowledge base that encourages critical thinking and self-directed learning (Curry,
2012). The new 4-year undergraduate curriculum not only provides more diverse
learning experiences for students—such as academic exchange, internship and com-
munity service—but also, more importantly, provides an inclusion of the common
core courses, which encourage inquiry-based learning.
While self-directed learning has received extensive attention from educators and
researchers, there is a paucity of research that investigates factors that are facilitative
of self-directed learning, especially in the higher education context (Song & Hill,
2007; Macaskill & Denovan, 2013). The present study aims to fill this gap by examin-
ing the role of emotional intelligence (EI) in self-directed learning in higher educa-
tion. Indeed, the process of becoming a self-directed learner is a painful one, as
students have to experience negativity, confusion, frustration and dissatisfaction with
their learning experience when they enter a new learning experience (Lunky-Child
et al., 2001). While some may react to negative emotions by choosing to withdraw,
there are others who are able to override them and respond assertively, to invest more
effort in solving the problem. That said, EI is likely to play an influential role in the
process of self-directed learning. The aim of the present study was to examine the
effects of EI on self-directed learning and how EI and self-directed learning con-
tribute to key learning outcomes in higher education, including academic achieve-
ment, generic learning outcomes (i.e. social, cognitive and self-growth outcomes) and
students’ satisfaction with their university experience.
Emotional intelligence
Emotional intelligence refers to individuals’ ability to appraise, express and regulate
emotions in the self and others and to make use of these emotions for problem solving
(Salovey & Mayer, 1990). According to Salovey and Mayer (1990), EI is conceptu-
alised as encompassing three major categories of emotional abilities. The first dimen-
sion involves the appraisal and expression of emotions in the self and others (i.e. the
ability to accurately perceive and express the emotions of the self and others, enabling
socially adaptive behaviours). The second dimension involves the regulation of emo-
tions in the self and others (i.e. the ability to control and modify the emotional states
of the self and others, so as to meet particular goals). The third dimension refers to
the utilisation of emotions in adaptive ways (i.e. the ability to make use of one’s own
emotions for solving problems). Although the model was later refined by Mayer and
Salovey (1997), the two models are similar in their basic components of EI (Schutte
et al., 2009).
While there are different theoretical conceptualisations of EI in the field, Salovey
and Mayer’s (1990, 1997) ability model of EI is one of the most widely accepted
models in the field, with the most research generated in peer-reviewed journals
(Zeidner et al., 2002; Fernandez-Berrocal & Extremera, 2006; Fernandez-Berrocal
et al., 2012).
Self-directed learning
the locus of control and the responsibility for learning lie in the hands of the learner)
(Pierson, 1996). Meanwhile, for self-regulated learning, although it appears highly
similar to self-directed learning, there is a distinctive difference between the two (Pil-
ling-Cormick & Garrison, 2007; Loyens et al., 2008). As explained by Jossberger
et al. (2010), self-directed learning is situated at the macro level, which concerns the
planning of the learning trajectory as a whole, since the self-directed learner is always
the one to decide what is to be learned and how best to accomplish it. In contrast,
self-regulated learning is a more micro-level process, concerning the processes within
task execution, whereby the learner is involved in monitoring, regulating and control-
ling their own cognition, motivation and behaviour in the learning process. Self-direc-
ted learning, therefore, is a broader concept than self-regulated learning. As to
lifelong learning, self-directed learning is indeed a prerequisite for this (Greveson &
Spencer, 2005). As explained by Candy (1991), self-directed learning and lifelong
learning have a reciprocal relationship. Given that self-directed learning is the princi-
pal activity in the independent pursuit of learning, lifelong learning is ‘equipping peo-
ple with skills and competencies to continue their own “self-education” beyond the
end of formal schooling’ (Candy, 1991, p. 15). Self-directed learning, therefore, can
be considered both a means and an end to lifelong learning.
students’ GPA and public examination results of the Hong Kong Advanced Level
Examination (HKALE) and the Hong Kong Diploma of Secondary Education
(HKDSE). Indeed, the experience and opportunity to engage in self-directed learning
activities were found to enhance students’ confidence, intrinsic motivation to learn,
critical thinking, quality of understanding as well as retention and recall (Fry, 1972;
Lunky-Child et al., 2001; Jennings, 2007; Smedly, 2007).
The analysis by McCombs and Whisler reveals the significance of two elements in
driving autonomous or self-directed learning: (i) self-regulation and control of affect;
and (ii) generation of positive affect and motivation, which are both core components
of EI.
information in the process to guide thinking and action, are facilitative of self-reflection
and self-monitoring, which are important for self-regulation (Lambert & McCombs,
1998; Kanfer & McCombs, 2000). When an individual is more aware of positive or
negative emotions through the processes of self-reflection and self-monitoring, atten-
tion is turned towards the self before taking action. Hence, self-regulation has a self-
referenced origin as an individual evaluates value to self, based on feedback from the
emotions, that facilitates action to regulate oneself in achieving goals (Salovey, 1992).
academic performance. For example, studies have shown the significant association
between EI and academic performance, such as GPA and examination results
(O’Connor & Little, 2003; Fabio & Palazzeschi, 2009; Hogan et al., 2010). Students
who were academically successful were found to have significantly higher EI than the
unsuccessful students (Parker et al., 2004, 2005). Indeed, EI was found to predict
academic performance after controlling for general mental abilities and personality
traits (Song et al., 2010). In a study conducted by Sanchez-Ruiz et al. (2013), they
even found that EI was able to predict academic performance over and above cogni-
tive ability and established personality traits. Above all, based on a meta-analytic
study that involved 8,700 participants, EI was found to have modest to moderate
validity in predicting academic performance (Perera & DiGiacomo, 2013).
for social outcomes (e.g. communication skills, leadership and teamwork), interac-
tions with peers and teachers, as well as participation in educational out-of-class expe-
riences (e.g. work experience, residential hall living experience and meaningful
leadership activities) are all significant (Astin, 1993; Kuh, 1995; Pascarella & Teren-
zini, 2005). As to the self-growth outcomes (such as time management and critical
self-reflection), peer interactions, voluntary work, community service and experience
of serving in student organisations are all positively related (Kuh, 1995; Astin et al.,
1999). To facilitate self-growth, critical reflection is an essential element, which is
motivated and prompted by the intensity of emotions experienced. Hence, individu-
als with higher EI are more aware of emotions in the self and others, facilitating more
critical reflections and thereby fostering self-growth (Clarke, 2010).
self-directed learning both directly influence the various student learning outcomes,
EI also influences self-directed learning, which in turn enhances the different student
learning outcomes. That said, self-directed learning would be partially mediating the
relationship between EI and the various student learning outcomes.
The study is significant as it provides empirical evidence on the value of EI and
self-directed learning in promoting student success. Given that EI has been shown to
be modifiable through training or intervention (Di Fabio & Kenny, 2011; Vesely
et al., 2014), the study has practical implications on how to enhance the key learning
outcomes in higher education, including students’ academic performance, generic
outcomes and their satisfaction with the university experience. It can also help shed
light on how best to improve the quality of undergraduate education, especially since
the early experiences in higher education are critical in establishing values, attitudes
and approaches to learning that will promote success for and beyond tertiary educa-
tion (McInnis & James, 1995; Trautwein & Bosse, 2017).
Apart from the above, the study contributes to expand our understanding of self-
directed learning studied in the eastern context (a large body of research on self-direc-
ted learning has been conducted mainly in the western context, which casts doubt on
the applicability of this concept in the eastern context) (Kramsch & Sullivan, 1996;
Chan et al., 2002). In fact, students in Hong Kong are generally perceived to be
dependent, reticent and passive (Pierson, 1996; Chan, 2001). Before the introduc-
tion of the New Senior Secondary Curriculum (NSSC) in 2009, the old curriculum
was criticised as being largely examination oriented, emphasising passive learning. As
a result, students under the old curriculum were used to learning in a teacher-centred
approach, with teachers having great control over the learning process. As such, stu-
dents tended to see knowledge as something to be transmitted by the teachers, rather
than as something to be explored and discovered on their own. Their passivity in
learning could also be reflected in their preference for memorisation and regurgitation
of information, an adaptive strategy for coping with assessment demands. To address
this problem, the NSSC was developed based on the guiding principle of developing
‘students’ overall capacities for self-directed, lifelong learning’ (Education and Man-
power Bureau, 2005, p. 20). In addition to increasing students’ competence in critical
thinking and independent learning, its ultimate purpose was to nurture students to
become self-directed lifelong learners. In other words, self-directed learning is a
timely issue that is greatly applicable in the local context.
Method
Participants
A total of 560 first-year undergraduate students from 10 faculties of a university in
Hong Kong responded to the survey at two time points of the study. Among them,
there were 213 males (38.0%) and 346 females (61.8%) [1 unreported (0.2%)].
Their ages ranged from 17 to 25 (M = 18.35, SD = 1.10). All of the respondents in
the study were Chinese. The academic disciplines of the participants are listed in
Table 1.
Procedures
The study adopted a prospective longitudinal design. Participants were invited to fill
in a paper survey during the registration period, which included measures of EI and
self-directed learning, as well as items on the background information of students
(e.g. gender, age and faculty). To prevent self-presentation, students were told that
the aims of the survey were to better understand their learning attitudes and personal
emotional experiences, instead of measuring their EI and self-directedness in learn-
ing. Then, they were followed up via email to answer an online survey at the end of
the first year, so as to tap students’ GPA and their satisfaction with the university
experience, as well as their achievement of the various generic outcomes. Participa-
tion in the study was entirely voluntary.
Measures
Emotional intelligence. EI was measured via the 33-item Emotional Intelligence Scale
(EIS) (Schutte et al., 1998), developed on the basis of Salovey and Mayer’s (1990)
model of EI. The scale has six key dimensions, including: (i) appraisal of emotions in
the self (AES) (e.g. ‘I am aware of my emotions as I experience them’); (ii) appraisal of
emotions in others (AEO) (e.g. ‘I know what other people are feeling just by looking at
them’); (iii) emotional regulation of the self (ERS) (e.g. ‘I seek out activities that make
me happy’); (iv) emotional expression (EE) (e.g. ‘I know when to speak about my per-
sonal problems to others’); (v) emotional regulation of others (ERO) (e.g. ‘I help other
people feel better when they are down’); and (vi) utilisation of emotions in problem solv-
ing (UEPS) (e.g. ‘I use good moods to help myself keep trying in the face of obstacles’)
(Zhoc et al., 2017). The internal consistency of the scale was reported by Zhoc et al.
(2017) to range from 0.85 to 0.93.
Faculty n (%)
Architecture 30 (5.4%)
Arts 70 (12.5%)
Business & Economics 96 (17.1%)
Dentistry 10 (1.8%)
Education 31 (5.5%)
Engineering 64 (11.4%)
Law 25 (4.5%)
Medicine 88 (15.7%)
Science 92 (16.4%)
Social Sciences 52 (9.3%)
Missing 2 (0.4%)
Total 560 (100%)
scale developed by Lounsbury and Gibson (2006). Respondents have to indicate their
level of agreement on a five-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly
agree). A higher total score on the SDLS would imply a higher level of self-directed-
ness in learning. Examples of the items include: ‘If there is something I don’t understand
in a class, I always find a way to learn it on my own’, ‘I am very motivated to learn on my
own without having to rely on other people’, and so on. The internal consistency of the
scale was reported to range from 0.84 to 0.87 in the college samples (Lounsbury
et al., 2009).
Student learning outcomes. In the follow-up survey, there was a 15-item Student
Learning Outcomes Scale (SLOS) that was designed to tap students’ self-
evaluation on the achievement of the generic learning outcomes. The scale was
devised with reference to the educational aims and institutional learning outcomes
for the undergraduate programme of the sampled university, which could be cate-
gorised broadly into the cognitive, social and self-growth outcomes of students.
The details of items in each category are listed in Table 2. The confirmatory fac-
tor analyses (CFA) results supported the three-factor model of the scale (see
Table 4 later for more details). Besides, the Cronbach alpha coefficients for the
social, cognitive and self-growth outcomes in this study were 0.87, 0.83 and 0.83,
respectively, indicating that all three factors were internally consistent. Apart from
the above, there were two items that gauged students’ end-of-year GPA and their
satisfaction with the university experience (‘overall evaluation of the first-year uni-
versity experience’).
Data-analytic plan. Structural equation modelling (SEM) (AMOS 23.0) was per-
formed to conduct measurement and structural analyses. Prior to testing the struc-
tural model, CFA were performed to validate the factor structure of the three
measurement scales used in the study. In addition to the conventional v2 test that was
used to evaluate whether the data deviate from the model significantly, a number of
Table 2. Scale measuring students’ self-evaluation on the achievement of the cognitive, social and
self-growth outcomes at the end of the academic year
fit statistics were also employed to assess the model fit [e.g. comparative fit index
(CFI), goodness-of-fit index (GFI), normed fit index (NFI), non-normed fit index
(NNFI) and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA)]. According to Hu
and Bentler (1999), values of CFI, NFI and NNFI should be over 0.95 so as to be
recognised as a good fit, although 0.90 is considered by many researchers as an
acceptable cutoff (Bentler & Bonett, 1980). As to the RMSEA, a value of 0.05 or less
indicates a close model fit (Browne & Cudeck, 1993), while a value below 0.08 is still
considered acceptable (MacCallum et al., 1996).
The purpose of performing SEM was to verify our hypothesised model that illus-
trates the structural relationships among EI, self-directed learning and the various
student learning outcomes as a whole, including students’ GPA, generic outcomes
and their satisfaction with the university experience. Since EI is a latent factor in the
model, multiple regression analyses were employed to further tease out whether and
how the six factors of EI are significant in influencing self-directed learning and the
achievement of generic outcomes.
Results
Measurement models
Confirmatory factor analyses were performed to test the measurement models of the
EIS, SDLS and SLOS. All the fit statistics indicated a good fit of the data to the mea-
surement models tested (see Table 4).
Structural model: Relationships among EI, self-directed learning and student learning outcomes
The results of the SEM supported that the model was tenable [v2(174) = 348.03,
p < 0.001, GFI = 0.94, CFI = 0.95, RMSEA = 0.04, NFI = 0.91, NNFI = 0.94]
(see Figure 1). As expected, EI had quite a substantial association with self-directed
learning (b = 0.62, p < 0.005) and was able to explain a total of 38% of its variance.
Self-directed learning, in turn, was positively related to both the GPA (b = 0.15,
p < 0.005) and generic learning outcomes (b = 0.14, p < 0.005). While EI was posi-
tively associated with generic learning outcomes (b = 0.20, p < 0.005), it was found
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. EI –
2. Self-directed learning 0.46** –
3. GPA 0.02 0.12** –
4. UES 0.13** 0.14** 0.25** –
5. Generic learning out- 0.31** 0.26** 0.15** 0.51** –
comes
6. Cognitive outcomes 0.28** 0.25** 0.15** 0.53** 0.92** –
7. Social outcomes 0.30** 0.21** 0.08 0.46** 0.87** 0.73** –
8. Self-growth outcomes 0.27** 0.24** 0.18** 0.48** 0.89** 0.77** 0.66** –
to have non-significant association with GPA. Furthermore, the results showed that
both the GPA (b = 0.17, p < 0.005) and generic learning outcomes (b = 0.49,
p < 0.005) were positively linked with students’ satisfaction with their university
experience.
Overall, the model was able to explain 5% of the variance in GPA, 9% of the vari-
ance in generic learning outcomes and 30% of the variance in students’ satisfaction
with their university experience, respectively.
Direct, indirect and total effects of EI and self-directed learning on student learning outcomes
To better delineate the relationships among the variables, the direct, indirect and
total effects of EI and self-directed learning on the various student learning outcomes
are presented in Table 5. It is worth noting that EI was found to have a significant
total effect on both the generic learning outcomes (b = 0.29, p < 0.005) and
.05
Generic .17
LOs GPA
.49 .30 .17
Figure 1. Model illustrating the relationships among EI, self-directed learning and different
student learning outcomes
Note: AEO = appraisal of emotions in others; AES = appraisal of emotions in the self;
CogOut = cognitive outcomes; EE = emotional expression; ERO = emotional regulation of others;
ERS = emotional regulation of the self; Generic LOs = generic learning outcomes; SDL = self-
directed learning; SGOut = self-growth outcomes; SocOut = social outcomes; UEPS = utilisation
of emotions in problem solving; UES = satisfaction with the university experience. [Colour figure
can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
students’ satisfaction with the university experience (b = 0.14, p < 0.005), but not on
the GPA. Having said that, based on 1,000 bootstrap samples, we found that through
self-directed learning, EI had significant indirect effects on students’ GPA (b = 0.14,
p < 0.05), generic learning outcomes (b = 0.09, p < 0.05) and satisfaction with the
university experience (b = 0.14, p < 0.005).
Multiple regression analyses examining how the different emotional abilities of EI influence
students’ self-directed learning
The multiple regression analyses (see Table 6) clearly delineate that the three emo-
tional abilities of EI (i.e. ERS, ERO and AES) were able to explain 26% of the vari-
ance in self-directed learning [adjusted R2 = 0.26, F(3, 556) = 65.55, p < 0.001].
Among the three emotional abilities of EI, ERS was the most influential (b = 0.29,
p < 0.001), explaining the largest proportion of the variance in self-directed learning
(adjusted R2 = 0.21). This was followed by ERO (b = 0.21, p < 0.001) and
Table 5. Direct, indirect and total effects of EI and self-directed learning on generic learning
outcomes and satisfaction with the university experience
Table 6. Multiple regression analyses examining how the different emotional abilities of EI
influence students’ self-directed learning and generic learning outcomes
Self-directed learning
ERS 0.29 0.00** 0.21
ERO 0.21 0.00** 0.04 0.26 65.55 0.00**
AES 0.13 0.00** 0.01
Generic learning outcomes
UEPS 0.21 0.00** 0.08 0.10 31.54 0.00**
ERO 0.17 0.00** 0.02
Discussion
The aim of the present study was to examine the effects of EI on self-directed learning
and how EI and self-directed learning contribute to key learning outcomes in higher
education, including academic achievement, generic learning outcomes (i.e., social,
cognitive and self-growth outcomes) and students’ satisfaction with their university
experience. There are several findings that are worth noting.
Firstly, the study expands the extant literature by affirming the role of EI on
self-directed learning, which in turn is shown to enhance both the GPA and gen-
eric learning outcomes, including cognitive, social and self-growth outcomes.
Apart from concurring with the results of previous studies (Buvoltz et al., 2008;
Muller, 2008) in showing the significant association between EI and self-directed
learning, the present study, as a step further, finds that there are three emotional
abilities that are significant in driving the occurrence of self-directed learning:
ERS, AES and ERO.
As expected, emotional regulation of the self is the most crucial for self-directed
learning. Given that self-directed learners are goal oriented, it is critical for them to be
self-disciplined so as to realise their self-defined learning goals (Kirwan et al., 2014).
Indeed, in the process of achieving learning goals, self-regulation of emotions is
important so as to override different temptations and short-term attractions that may
lead to distraction and procrastination in goal-directed behaviour. Apart from this, as
mentioned before, the process of self-directed learning is painful as students have to
experience negativity, confusion, frustration and dissatisfaction with their learning
experience when they enter a new learning experience. The ability to regulate nega-
tive emotions is therefore critical, to sustain study effort and persist in the face of set-
backs in the course of pursuing goals.
As to the appraisal of emotions in the self, it is an important process facilitating
self-reflection, which in turn is critical for self-directed learning. Given that emotions
are signals providing information to an individual on what is valuable and meaningful,
an individual who is well aware of their own emotions can make good use of this emo-
tional information to guide thinking and action. For example, in the process of goal
setting for self-directed learning, the desirability of the goal is a function of the posi-
tive and negative emotional consequences of goal pursuit and achievement or failure
in achieving the goal. Similarly, in the process of monitoring and evaluating progress
during self-directed learning, individuals will use feedback from their emotions to
determine actions for further self-regulation (or not) in achieving the intended goals
(Salovey, 1992). That said, the appraisal of emotions in the self is critical for
self-directed learning.
Meanwhile, for the emotional regulation of others, it is facilitative of intellectual
exchanges with others in the highly interactive learning environment in higher educa-
tion. Although self-directed learners are self-reliant in learning, intellectual exchanges
with teachers and peers are critical parts of the learning process that not only nurture
cognitive growth and intrinsic motivation, helping individuals to delve into the sub-
ject matter, but may also inspire a solution to the problems encountered during learn-
ing. In short, self-directed learning is fostered by helpful interactions and
relationships with other people, who can offer advice and insight that facilitate indi-
viduals to progress in the course of achieving the learning goals. This explains why
emotional regulation of others is significant for self-directed learning.
Secondly, the study provides more evidence on the relationship between self-direc-
ted learning and student learning outcomes. Specifically, it shows that self-directed
learning positively affects not only the GPA, but also students’ development of gen-
eric outcomes, including social, cognitive and self-growth outcomes. In other words,
individuals with a higher level of self-directedness tend to gain more in both academic
and non-academic terms. This is understandable considering that, in the process of
learning, self-directed learners tend to take the initiative and are self-regulated in
learning, which leads them to exhibit behaviours (such as taking a deep learning
approach, setting higher learning goals and having more discussion and collaboration
with faculty and peers) that help them to achieve more in both academic and non-aca-
demic terms. It is worth noting that both the GPA and the generic learning outcomes
are influential on students’ satisfaction with the university experience, with the latter
having a much greater weight than the former, indicating that university students’
focus is no longer on academic performance only, but on the extent of achieving per-
sonal development such as leadership skills, problem-solving and communication
skills, as well as analytical and creative thinking, and so on. In other words, the find-
ings in this study correspond to those of Lounsbury et al. (2009), in which self-direc-
ted learners were found to be more optimistic, conscientious, self-actualised and
open to new experiences. They not only achieved higher academic performance, but
also had higher college satisfaction.
Thirdly, EI is found to pose a significant direct impact on the generic student learn-
ing outcomes. Further analyses show that UEPS and ERO are influential in students’
achievement of generic outcomes, which are largely acquired through the processes of
interacting with peers and teachers as well as participating in educational out-of-class
activities (such as community work, internship and serving in student organisations).
For UEPS and ERO, they are both significant in social interactions. For example, the
expression of positive emotions, the suppression of negative communication with
others, the control of emotional responses to private problems are all important in
enhancing interpersonal relationships (Salovey & Mayer, 1990). The achievement of
the generic learning outcomes, in turn, can facilitate academic performance. After all,
academic learning involves a lot of cognitive activities, which requires having different
cognitive skills, such as problem-solving skills and viewing things from a global per-
spective. Besides, social skills may also facilitate academic performance through qual-
ity interaction with teaching staff and peers, as cognitive abilities can be enhanced
through active discussion of ideas, debating points of view and critically reviewing
work with peers or teachers (Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000).
Last but not least, while it was found in many studies that there was a direct rela-
tionship between EI and academic performance, this study shows that the relation-
ship between the two is mediated by students’ self-directedness in learning.
Moreover, EI may differentially predict success by major subject area, with social and
emotional skills more crucial for liberal arts than for sciences (Parker et al., 2009).
Indeed, there were concurring results reported by Zhoc et al. (2017), who found that
EI was positively associated with students’ GPA in the faculties of business and social
sciences only, not with all students as a whole. Since subjects in both faculties are
people oriented, it is likely that a good knowledge and understanding of the emotions
of the self and others could contribute to better academic performance in studies
which are related to an understanding of human behaviour (e.g. business, psychology,
sociology and political science).
The study is significant as it not only establishes the affirmative relationship
between EI and self-directed learning, but also teases out the specific emotional abili-
ties that are influential on self-directed learning and generic learning outcomes. Given
that EI is shown to be modifiable through training or intervention programmes (Di
Fabio & Kenny, 2011; Vesely et al., 2014), a practical implication of this study is
that relevant interventions may be incorporated in the university orientation pro-
gramme so as to lay a better psychological foundation that fosters students’ optimal
functioning in the university. In addition, an inclusion of EI as part of the standard
college curriculum could lead to many potential personal, social and societal benefits
(Vandervoort, 2006).
Despite the positive findings, there are several limitations in this study that should
be noted. Firstly, the study sample was restricted to one university in Hong Kong.
Since the sample was not representative of higher education as a whole, the study
should be replicated in other higher institutions so as to ensure the generalisability of
the results. Besides, the study made use of self-reported GPA. Although there is a
high correlation between self-reported and actual college GPA (r = 0.90), the use of
actual academic achievement from school records could further ensure the reliability
of the data (Kuncel et al., 2005). Moreover, self-report measures are used to assess
EI, self-directed learning and the generic learning outcomes, which may be subject to
response biases and cannot accurately reflect actual behaviours (Mega et al., 2014).
Behavioural measures may be needed as well in future studies.
To conclude, students who are more emotionally intelligent have higher self-direct-
edness, which leads them to achieve more not only in academic terms, but also in per-
sonal development including social, cognitive and self-growth aspects. As a result,
they are also more satisfied with their overall university experience. To facilitate self-
directed learning, there are three crucial emotional abilities of students that should be
strengthened: ERS, AES and ERO.
References
Astin, A. W. (1993) What matters in college: Four critical years revisited (San Francisco, CA, Jossey-
Bass).
Astin, A. W., Sax, L. J. & Avalos, J. (1999) Long-term effects of volunteerism during the undergrad-
uate years, The Review of Higher Education, 22(2), 187–202.
Barrie, S. C. (2007) A conceptual framework for the teaching and learning of generic graduate
attributes, Studies in Higher Education, 32(4), 439–458. https://doi.org/10.1080/0307507070
1476100
Bentler, P. M. & Bonett, D. G. (1980) Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of
covariance structures, Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588–606. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.
88.3.588
Bowden, J., Hart, G., King, B., Trigwell, K. & Watts, O. (2000) Generic capabilities of ATN uni-
versity graduates. Available online at: http:/www.clt.uts.edu.au/ATN.grad.cap.project.in-
dex.html (accessed 1 June 2018).
Bridgstock, R. (2009) The graduate attributes we’ve overlooked: Enhancing graduate employability
through career management skills, Higher Education Research and Development, 28(1), 31–44.
https://doi.org/10.1080/07294360802444347
Brockett, R. G. (1983) Self-directed learning and the hard-to-reach adult, Lifelong Learning: The
Adult Years, 6(8), 16–18.
Browne, M. W. & Cudeck, R. (1993) Alternative ways of assessing model fit, in: K. A. Bollen & J.
S. Long (Eds) Testing structural equation models (Newbury, CA, Sage), 132–162.
Buvoltz, K. A., College, H., Powell, F. J., University, R., Solan, A. M., University, D. et al. (2008)
Exploring emotional intelligence, learner autonomy and retention in an accelerated undergrad-
uate degree completion program, New Horizons in Adult Education and Human Resource Devel-
opment, 22(3/4), 26–43. https://doi.org/10.1002/nha3.10315
Candy, P. C. (1991) Self-direction for lifelong learning: A comprehensive guide to theory and practice
(San Francisco, CA, Jossey-Bass).
Chan, V. (2001) Learning autonomously: The learners’ perspectives, Journal of Further and Higher
Education, 25(3), 285–300. https://doi.org/10.1080/03098770120077649
Chan, V., Spratt, M. & Humphreys, G. H. (2002) Autonomous language learning: Hong Kong ter-
tiary students’ attitudes and behaviours, Evaluation and Research in Education, 16(1), 1–18.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09500790208667003
Chemers, M. M., Hu, L. & Garcia, B. F. (2001) Academic self-efficacy and first-year college stu-
dent performance and adjustment, Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 55–64. https://doi.org/
10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.55
Chene, A. (1983) The concept of autonomy in adult education: A philosophical discussion, Adult
Education Quarterly, 34(1), 38–47. https://doi.org/10.1177/0001848183034001004
Clarke, N. (2010) Emotional intelligence and learning in teams, Journal of Workplace Learning, 22
(3), 125–145. https://doi.org/10.1108/13665621011028594
Cockrell, K. S., Caplow, J. A. H. & Donaldson, J. F. (2000) A context for learning: Collaborative
groups in the problem-based learning environment, The Review of Higher Education, 23, 347–
363. https://doi.org/10.1353/rhe.2000.0008
Corno, L. (1992) Encouraging students to take responsibility for learning and performance, Ele-
mentary School Journal, 93, 69–83.
Curry, J. M. (2012) Cultural challenges in Hong Kong to the implementation of effective general
education, Teaching in Higher Education, 17(2), 223–230. https://doi.org/10.1080/13562517.
2012.658565
DeSteno, D. & Salovey, P. (1997) Structural dynamism in the concept of self: A flexible model for
a malleable concept, Review of General Psychology, 1(4), 389–409. https://doi.org/10.1037/
1089-2680.1.4.389
Di Fabio, A. & Kenny, M. E. (2011) Promoting emotional intelligence and career decision making
among Italian high school students, Journal of Career Assessment, 19(1), 21–34. https://doi.org/
10.1177/1069072710382530
Education and Manpower Bureau. (2005) The New Academic Structure for Senior Secondary
Education and Higher Education - Action Plan for Investing in the Future of Hong Kong.
Available online at: http://334.edb.hkedcity.net/doc/eng/report_e.pdf (accessed 31 August
2018).
Ekman, P. (1992) An argument for basic emotions, Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169–200.
Engelberg, E. & Sjoberg, L. (2004) Emotional intelligence, affect intensity, and social adjustment,
Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 533–542. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.024
Fabio, A. D. & Palazzeschi, L. (2009) An in-depth look at scholastic success: Fluid intelligence,
personality traits or emotional intelligence? Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 581–585.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.12.012
Fernandez-Berrocal, P. & Extremera, P. (2006) Emotional intelligence: A theoretical and empirical
review of its first 15 years of history. Psicothema, 18(Suppl.), 7–12.
Fernandez-Berrocal, P., Cabello, R., Castillo, R. & Extremera, N. (2012) Gender differences in
emotional intelligence: The mediating effect of age, Behavioral Psychology, 20(1), 77–89.
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C. & Paris, A. H. (2004) Student engagement: Potential of the con-
cept, state of the evidence, Review of Educational Research, 74(1), 59–109. https://doi.org/10.
3102/00346543074001059
Fry, J. (1972) Interactive relationship between inquisitiveness and student control of instruction,
Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 459–465. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0033237
Furr, R. M. & Funder, D. C. (1998) A multimodal analysis of personal negativity, Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology., 74, 1580–1591. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1580
Grayson, P. A. & Meilman, P. W. (1999) Beating the college blues (New York, Checkmark).
Green, D. P. & Salovey, P. (1999) In what sense are positive and negative affect independent? A
reply to Tellegen, Watson, and Clark, Psychological Science, 10(4), 304–306. https://doi.org/10.
1111/1467-9280.00158
Greveson, G. C. & Spencer, J. A. (2005) Self-directed learning – The importance of concepts and
contexts, Medical Education, 39, 348–349.
Guthrie, J. T. & Wigfield, A. (2000). Engagement and motivation in reading, in: M. Kamil & P.
Mosenthal (Eds) Handbook of reading research (vol. 3) (Mahwah, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum), 403–
422.
Hiemstra, R. (2000). Self-directed learning: The personal responsibility model, in: G. A. Straka
(Ed.) Conceptions of self-directed learning: Theoretical and conceptual considerations (Munster,
Waxmann), 93–108.
Hogan, M. J., Parker, J. D. A., Wiener, J., Watters, C., Wood, L. M. & Oke, A. (2010) Academic
success in adolescence: Relationships among verbal IQ, social support and emotional intelli-
gence, Australian Journal of Psychology, 62(1), 30–41. https://doi.org/10.1080/
00049530903312881
Hu, L. T. & Bentler, P. M. (1999) Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:
Conventional criteria versus new alternatives, Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
Huang, C. (2012) Discriminant and criterion-related validity of achievement goals in predicting
academic achievement: A meta-analysis, Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 48–73.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026223
Jennings, S. F. (2007) Personal development plans and self-directed learning for health care profes-
sionals: Are they evidence based? Postgraduate Medical Journal, 83, 518–524. https://doi.org/10.
1136/pgmj.2006.053066
Jossberger, H., Brand-Gruwell, S., Boshuizen, H. & Van de Wiel, M. (2010) The challenge of self-
directed and self-regulated learning in vocational education: A theoretical analysis and synthe-
sis of requirements, Journal of Vocational Education and Training, 62(4), 415–440. https://doi.
org/10.1080/13636820.2010.523479
Kanfer, R. & McCombs, B. L. (2000) Motivation: Applying current theory to critical issues in train-
ing, in: S. Tobias & D. T. Fletcher (Eds) Handbook of training (New York, Macmillan), 85–
108.
Kirwan, J. R., Lounsbury, J. W. & Gibson, L. W. (2014) An examination of learner self-direction in
relation to the big five and narrow personality traits, Sage Open, 4(2), 2158244014534857.
Kramsch, C. & Sullivan, P. (1996) Appropriate pedagogy, ELT Journal, 50(3), 199–212. https://
doi.org/10.1093/elt/50.3.199
Knowles, M. (1975) Self-directed learning: A guide for learners and teachers (New York, Association
Press).
Kuh, G. D. (1995) The other curriculum: Out-of-class experiences associated with student learning
and personal development, Journal of Higher Education, 66(2), 123–155.
Kuh, G. D., Kinzie, J., Buckley, J. A., Bridges, B. K. & Hayek, J. C. (2006). What matters to stu-
dent success: A review of the literature. Available online at: https://nces.ed.gov/npec/pdf/kuh_
team_report.pdf (accessed 31 August 2018).
Kuncel, N. R., Crede, M. & Thomas, L. L. (2005) The validity of self-reported grade point aver-
ages, class ranks, and test scores: A meta-analysis and review of the literature, Review of Educa-
tional Research, 75(1), 63–82. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543075001063
Lambert, N. & McCombs, B. L. (1998) How students learn: Reforming schools through learned-centered
education (Washington, D.C., APA Books). https://doi.org/10.1037/10258-000
Linnenbrink, E. A. (2007) The role of affect in student learning: A multi-dimensional approach to
considering the interaction of affect, motivation and engagement, in: P. A. Schutz & R. Pekrun
(Eds) Emotion in education (San Diego, CA, Elsevier Academic), 107–124. https://doi.org/10.
1016/b978-012372545-5/50008-3
Lopes, P. N., Salovey, P. & Straus, R. (2003) Emotional intelligence, personality and the perceived
quality of social relationships, Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 641–658. https://doi.
org/10.1016/s0191-8869(02)00242-8
Lopes, P. N., Brackett, M. A., Nezlek, J. B., Schutz, A., Sellin, I. & Salovey, P. (2004) Emotional
intelligence and social interaction, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(8), 1018–1034.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167204264762
Lounsbury, J. W., Saudargas, R. A., Gibson, L. W. & Leong, F. T. (2005) An investigation of Big
Five and narrow personality traits in relation to general and domain-specific life satisfaction of
college students, Research in Higher Education, 46(6), 707–729.
Lounsbury, J. W. & Gibson, L. W. (2006) Personal style inventory: A personality measurement system
for work and school settings (Knoxville, TN, Resource Associates).
Lounsbury, J. W., Levy, J. J., Park, S. H., Gibson, L. W. & Smith, R. (2009) An investigation of the
construct validity of the personality trait of self-directed learning, Learning and Individual Differ-
ences, 19, 411–418. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2009.03.001
Loyens, S. M. M., Magda, J. & Rikers, M. J. P. (2008) Self-directed learning in problem-based
learning and its relationships with self-regulated learning, Educational Psychology Review, 20(4),
411–427. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-008-9082-7
Lunky-Child, O. I., Crooks, D., Ellis, P. J., Ofosu, C., O’Mara, L. & Rideout, E. (2001) Self-direc-
ted learning: Faculty and student perceptions, Journal of Nursing Education, 40(3), 116–123.
Macaskill, A. & Denovan, A. (2013) Developing autonomous learning in first-year university stu-
dents using perspectives from positive psychology. Studies in Higher Education, 38(1), 124–142.
https://doi.org/10.1080/03075079.2011.566325
Macaskill, A. & Taylor, E. (2010) The development of a brief measure of learner autonomy in uni-
versity students, Studies in Higher Education, 35(3), 351–359. https://doi.org/10.1080/
03075070903502703
MacCallum, R. C., Browne, W. M. & Sugawara, H. M. (1996) Power analysis and determination
of sample size for covariance structure modeling, Psychological Methods, 1, 130–149. https://doi.
org/10.3102/00028312039003727
Mayer, J. D. & Salovey, P. (1993) The intelligence of emotional intelligence, Intelligence, 17(4),
433–442. https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(93)90010-3
Mayer, J. D. & Salovey, P. (1997) What is emotional intelligence? in: P. Salovey & D. Sluyter (Eds)
Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Implications for educators (New York, Basic
Books), 3–31.
McCombs, B. L. & Whisler, J. S. (1989) The role of affective variables in autonomous learning,
Educational Psychologist, 24(3), 277–306. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep2403_4
McInnis, C. & James, R. (1995) First year on campus: Diversity in the initial experiences of Australian
undergraduates (Canberra, Australian Government Publishing Service).
Mega, C., Ronconi, L. & De Beni, R. (2014) What makes a good student? How emotions, self-
regulated learning, and motivation contribute to academic achievement, Journal of Educational
Psychology, 106(1), 121–131. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033546
Mok, M. M. C., Cheng, Y. C., Leung, S. O., Shan, P. W. J., Moore, P. & Kennedy, K. (2007) Self-
directed learning as a key approach to effectiveness of education: A comparison among main-
land China, Hong Kong, Macau and Taiwan, in: T. Townsend (Ed.) International handbook of
school effectiveness and improvement (London, Springer).
Muller, K. E. (2008) Self-directed learning and emotional intelligence: Interrelationships between
the two constructs, change and problem solving, International Journal of Self-Directed Learning,
5(2), 11–22.
NSSE. (2005). Student engagement: Exploring different dimensions of student engagement.
Bloomington, IN. Available online at: http://nsse.indiana.edu/pdf/NSSE2005_annual_report.
pdf (accessed 31 August 2018).
O’Connor, R. M. & Little, I. S. (2003) Revisiting the predictive validity of emotional intelligence:
Self-report versus ability-based measures, Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 1893–1902.
https://doi.org/10.1016/s0191-8869(03)00038-2
Oddi, L. F. (1987) Perspectives on self-directed learning, Adult Education Quarterly, 38(1), 21–31.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0001848187038001003
OECD (2000) Motivating students for lifelong learning (Paris, OECD).
O’Shea, E. (2003) Self-directed learning in nurse education: A review of the literature, Journal of
Advanced Nursing, 43(1), 62–70. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2648.2003.02673.x
Parker, J. D. A., Summerfeldt, L. J., Hogan, M. J. & Majeski, S. A. (2004) Emotional intelligence
and academic success: Examining the transition from high school to university, Personality and
Individual Differences, 36, 163–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0191-8869(03)00076-x
Parker, J. D. A., Duffy, J., Wood, L. M., Bond, B. J. & Hogan, M. J. (2005) Academic achievement
and emotional intelligence: Predicting the successful transition from high school to university,
Journal of First Year Experience and Students in Transition, 17, 67–78.
Parker, J. D. A., Saklofske, D. H., Wood, L. M. & Collin, T. (2009) The role of emotional intelli-
gence in education, in: C. Stough, D. H. Saklofske & J. D. A. Parker (Eds) Assessing emotional
intelligence: Theory, research and applications (London, Springer), 239–252.
Pascarella, E. T. & Terenzini, P. T. (2005) How college affects students: A third decade of research (San
Francisco, CA, Jossey-Bass).
Perera, H. N. & DiGiacomo, M. (2013) The relationship of trait emotional intelligence with aca-
demic performance: A meta-analytic review, Learning and Individual Differences, 28, 20–33.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.08.002
Pierson, H. (1996) Learner culture and learner autonomy in Hong Kong Chinese context, in: R.
Pemberton, E. Li, W. Or & H. D. Pierson (Eds) Taking control: Autonomy in language learning
(Hong Kong, Hong Kong University Press), 49–58.
Pilling-Cormick, J. & Garrison, D. R. (2007) Self-directed and self-regulated learning: Conceptual
links, Canadian Journal of University Continuing Education, 33(2), 13–33.
Pintrich, P. R. & De Groot, E. (1990) Motivated and self-regulated learning components of aca-
demic performance, Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33–40. https://doi.org/10.1037/
0022-0663.82.1.33
Rager, K. B. (2009) I feel, therefore, I learn: The role of emotion in self-directed learning, New
Horizons in Adult Education and Human Resource Development, 23(2), 22–33. https://doi.org/10.
1002/nha3.10336
Saklofske, D. H., Austin, E. J., Beaton, L. & Osborne, S. E. (2012) Relationships of personality,
affect, emotional intelligence and coping with student stress and academic success: Different
patterns of association for stress and success, Learning and Individual Differences, 22, 251–251.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.02.010
Salovey, P. (1992) Mood induced self-focused attention, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
62(4), 699–707. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.62.4.699
Salovey, P. & Mayer, J. D. (1990) Emotional intelligence, Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 9,
185–211. https://doi.org/10.2190/dugg-p24e-52wk-6cdg
Salovey, P., Bedell, B. T., Detweiller, J. B. & Mayer, J. D. (1999) Coping intelligently: Emotional
intelligence and the coping process, in: C. R. Synder (Ed.) Coping: The psychology of what works
(New York, Oxford University Press).
Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J., Mavroveli, S. & Poullis, J. (2013) Trait emotional intelligence and its links to
university performance: An examination, Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 658–662.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.013
Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., Golden, C. J. et al.
(1998) Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence, Personality and
Individual Differences, 25, 167–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0191-8869(98)00001-4
Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M. & Bhullar, N. (2009) The Assessing Emotions Scale, in: C. Stough,
D. H. Saklofske & J. D. A. Parker (Eds) The assessment of emotional intelligence (New York,
Springer), 119–135. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-88370-0
Seifert, T. L. (2004) Understanding student motivation, Educational Research, 46, 137–149.
https://doi.org/10.1080/0013188042000222421
Smedly, A. (2007) The self-directed learning readiness of first year of bachelor of nursing students,
Journal of Research Nurse, 12, 373–382. https://doi.org/10.1177/1744987107077532
Smith, C. (2016) Self-directed learning: A toolkit for practitioners in a changing higher education
context, Innovations in Practice, 10(1), 15–26.
Song, L. & Hill, J. R. (2007) A conceptual model for understanding self-directed learning in online
environments, Journal of Interactive Online Learning, 6(1), 27–42.
Song, L. J., Huang, G. H., Peng, K. Z., Law, K. S., Wong, C. S. & Chen, Z. (2010) The differential
effects of general mental ability and emotional intelligence on academic performance and social
interaction, Intelligence, 38, 137–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2009.09.003
Strauss, L. C. & Volkwein, J. F. (2002) Comparing student performance and growth in 2- and 4-
year institutions, Research in Higher Education, 43(2), 133–161. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:
1014495823183