Week 8 20240402053320
Week 8 20240402053320
In Piaget’s theory, the sensorimotor stage occurs first, and is defined as the
period when infants “think” by means of their senses and motor actions. As every
new parent will attest, infants continually touch, manipulate, look, listen to, and
even bite and chew objects. According to Piaget, these actions allow children to
learn about the world and are crucial to their early cognitive development.
The infant’s actions allow the child to represent (i.e., construct simple
concepts of) objects and events. A toy animal may be just a confusing array of
sensations at first, but by looking, feeling, and manipulating it repeatedly, the child
gradually organizes her sensations and actions into a stable concept: toy animal.
The representation acquires a permanence lacking in the individual experiences of
the object, which are constantly changing. Because the representation is stable, the
child “knows,” or at least believes, that toy animal exists even if the actual toy
animal is temporarily out of sight. Piaget called this sense of stability object
permanence, a belief that objects exist whether or not they are actually present.
Object permanence is a major achievement of sensorimotor development, and
marks a qualitative transformation in how older infants (~24 months) think about
experience compared to younger infants (~6 months).
In the preoperational stage, children use their new ability to represent objects
in a wide variety of activities, but they do not yet do it in ways that are organized or
fully logical. One of the most obvious examples of this kind of cognition is dramatic
play, or the improvised make-believe of preschool children. If you have ever had
responsibility for children of this age, you have likely witnessed such play.
The other new feature of thinking that develops during the concrete
operational stage is the child’s ability to decenter, or focus on more than one
feature of a problem at a time. There are hints of decentration in preschool
children’s dramatic play, which requires being aware on two levels at once—
knowing that a banana can be both a banana and a “telephone.” But the
decentration of the concrete operational stage is more deliberate and conscious
than preschoolers’ make-believe. Now the child can attend to two things at once
quite purposefully. Suppose you give students a sheet with an assortment of
subtraction problems on it, and ask them to do this: “Find all of the problems that
involve two-digit subtraction and that involve borrowing from the next column.
Circle and solve only those problems.” Following these instructions is quite possible
for a concrete operational student (as long as they have been listening!) because
the student can attend to the two subtasks simultaneously—finding the two-digit
problems and identifying which actually involve borrowing. (Whether the student
actually knows how to “borrow” however, is a separate question.)
In the last of the Piagetian stages, the child becomes able to reason not only
about tangible objects and events, but also about hypothetical or abstract ones.
Hence, it has the name formal operational stage—the period when the individual
can “operate” on “forms” or representations. With students at this level, the
teacher can pose hypothetical (or contrary-to-fact) problems: “What if the world had
never discovered oil?” or “What if the first European explorers had settled first in
California instead of on the East Coast of the United States?” To answer such
questions, students must use hypothetical reasoning, meaning that they must
manipulate ideas that vary in several ways at once, and do so entirely in their
minds.
The young person is not allowed to solve this problem by trial-and-error with
the materials themselves, but must mentally reason a way to the solution. To do so
systematically, he or she must imagine varying each factor separately, while also
imagining the other factors that are held constant. This kind of thinking requires
facility at manipulating mental representations of the relevant objects and actions—
precisely the skill that defines formal operations.
2. Lev Vygotsky
The work of Lev Vygotsky (1934, 1978) has become the foundation of much
research and theory in cognitive development over the past several decades,
particularly what has become known as sociocultural theory. Vygotsky’s theory
comprises concepts such as culture-specific tools, private speech, and the zone of
proximal development. Vygotsky believed cognitive development is influenced by
cultural and social factors. He emphasized the role of social interaction in the
development of mental abilities e.g., speech and reasoning in children. Vygotsky
strongly believed that community plays a central role in the process of “making
meaning.” Cognitive development is a socially mediated process in which children
acquire cultural values, beliefs, and problem-solving strategies through
collaborative dialogues with more knowledgeable members of society. The more
knowledgeable other (MKO) is someone who has a higher level of ability or greater
understanding than the learner regarding a particular task, process, or concept.
The MKO can be a teacher, parent, coach, or even a peer who provides
guidance and modeling to enable the child to learn skills within their zone of
proximal development (the gap between what a child can do independently and
what they can achieve with guidance).
These operations occur separately. Indeed, before age two, a child employs
words socially; they possess no internal language.
Effects Of Culture
Vygotsky claimed that infants are born with the basic abilities for intellectual
development called “elementary mental functions” (Piaget focuses on motor
reflexes and sensory abilities). These develop throughout the first two years of life
due to direct environmental contact.
o Attention
o Sensation
o Perception
o Memory
Vygotsky, therefore, sees cognitive functions, even those carried out alone,
as affected by the beliefs, values, and tools of intellectual adaptation of the culture
in which a person develops and, therefore, socio-culturally determined.
Like Piaget, Vygotsky believes that young children are curious and actively
involved in their own learning and discovering and developing
new understandings/schema.
Shaffer (1996) gives the example of a young girl given her first jigsaw. Alone,
she performs poorly in attempting to solve the puzzle. The father then sits with her
and describes or demonstrates some basic strategies, such as finding all the
corner/edge pieces, and provides a couple of pieces for the child to put together
herself, and offers encouragement when she does so.
As the child becomes more competent, the father allows the child to work
more independently. According to Vygotsky, this social interaction involving
cooperative or collaborative dialogue promotes cognitive development.
Although the implication is that the MKO is a teacher or an older adult, this is
not necessarily the case. Often, a child’s peers or an adult’s children may be the
individuals with more knowledge or experience.
For example, who is more likely to know more about the newest teenage
music groups, how to win at the most recent PlayStation game, or how to correctly
perform the newest dance craze – a child or their parents?
In fact, the MKO need not be a person at all. To support employees in their
learning process, some companies are now using electronic performance support
systems.
Electronic tutors have also been used in educational settings to facilitate and
guide students through learning. The key to MKOs is that they must have (or be
programmed with) more knowledge about the topic being learned than the learner
does.
This important concept relates to the difference between what a child can
achieve independently and what a child can achieve with guidance and
encouragement from a skilled partner.
The zone of proximal development is the gap between the level of actual
development, what the child can do on his own, and the level of potential
development, what a child can do with the assistance of more advanced and
competent individuals.
The first is intersubjectivity, where two individuals who might have different
understandings of a task, arrive at a shared understanding by adjusting to the
perspective of the other.
The second feature is referred to as scaffolding. Adults may begin with direct
instruction, but as children’s mastery of a task increases, so the adult tends to
withdraw their own contributions in recognition of the child’s increasing success.
For example, the child could not solve the jigsaw puzzle (in the example
above) by itself and would have taken a long time to do so (if at all), but was able to
solve it following interaction with the father, and has developed competence at this
skill that will be applied to future jigsaws.
ZPD is the zone where instruction is the most beneficial, as it is when the task
is just beyond the individual’s capabilities. To learn, we must be presented with
tasks just out of our ability range. Challenging tasks promote maximum cognitive
growth.
Vygotsky (1978) sees the Zone of Proximal Development as the area where
the most sensitive instruction or guidance should be given – allowing the child to
develop skills they will then use on their own – developing higher mental functions.
Freund (1990) conducted a study in which children had to decide which items
of furniture should be placed in particular areas of a doll’s house.
Some children were allowed to play with their mother in a similar situation
before they attempted it alone (zone of proximal development) while others were
allowed to work on this by themselves (Piaget’s discovery learning).
Freund found that those who had previously worked with their mother (ZPD)
showed the greatest improvement compared with their first attempt at the task.
The conclusion is that guided learning within the ZPD led to greater
understanding/performance than working alone (discovery learning).
For Vygotsky, thought and language are initially separate systems from the
beginning of life, merging at around three years of age.
When this happens, children’s monologues are internalized to become inner speech.
The internalization of language is important as it drives cognitive development.
Private Speech
Vygotsky (1987) was the first psychologist to document the importance of
private speech.
He considered private speech as the transition point between social and inner
speech, the moment in development where language and thought unite to
constitute verbal thinking.
Private speech is overt, audible, and observable, often seen in children who
talk to themselves while problem-solving.
Vygotsky sees “private speech” as a means for children to plan activities and
strategies, aiding their development. Private speech is the use of language for self-
regulation of behavior.
Vygotsky (1987) notes that private speech does not merely accompany a
child’s activity but acts as a tool the developing child uses to facilitate cognitive
processes, such as overcoming task obstacles, and enhancing imagination, thinking,
and conscious awareness.
Children use private speech most often during intermediate difficulty tasks
because they attempt to self-regulate by verbally planning and organizing their
thoughts (Winsler et al., 2007).
Berk (1986) provided empirical support for the notion of private speech. She
found that most private speech exhibited by children serves to describe or guide
the child’s actions.
Berk also discovered that children engaged in private speech more often
when working alone on challenging tasks and when their teacher was not
immediately available to help them.
Furthermore, Berk also found that private speech develops similarly in all
children regardless of cultural background.
There is also evidence (Behrend et al., 1992) that those children who
displayed the characteristic whispering and lip movements associated with private
speech when faced with a difficult task were generally more attentive and
successful than their ‘quieter’ classmates.
Children’s use of private speech diminishes as they grow older and follows a
curvilinear trend. This is due to changes in ontogenetic development whereby
children can internalize language (through inner speech) to self-regulate their
behavior (Vygotsky, 1987).
For example, research has shown that children’s private speech usually
peaks at 3–4 years of age, decreases at 6–7, and gradually fades out to be mostly
internalized by age 10 (Diaz, 1992).
Vygotsky proposed that private speech diminishes and disappears with age not
because it becomes socialized, as Piaget suggested, but because it goes
underground to constitute inner speech or verbal thought” (Frauenglass & Diaz,
1985).
Educational Implications
Every function in the child’s cultural development appears twice: first, on the
social level, and later, on the individual level; first, between people
(interpsychological) and then inside the child (intrapsychological).
Rather than having a teacher impose their understanding onto students for
future recitation, the teacher should co-create meaning with students in a manner
that allows learners to take ownership (Hausfather, 1996).
For instance, a student and teacher might start a task with varying levels of
expertise and understanding. As they adapt to each other’s perspective, the teacher
must articulate their insights in a way that the student can comprehend, leading the
student to a fuller understanding of the task or concept.
The student can then internalize the task’s operational aspect (“how to do it”)
into their inner speech or private dialogue. Vygotsky referred to this reciprocal
understanding and adjustment process as intersubjectivity.”
ZPD
Because Vygotsky asserts that cognitive change occurs within the zone of
proximal development, instruction would be designed to reach a developmental
level just above the student’s current developmental level.
Reciprocal Teaching
The teacher and students alternated turns leading small group discussions on
a reading. After modeling four reading strategies, students began to assume the
teaching role.
Scaffolding
A crucial element in this process is the use of what later became known as
scaffolding; the way in which the teacher provides students with frameworks and
experiences which encourage them to extend their existing schemata and
incorporate new skills, competencies, and understandings.
Scaffolding describes the conditions that support the child’s learning, to move
from what they already know to new knowledge and abilities.
Scaffolding requires the teacher to allow students to extend their current
skills and knowledge.
As the adult withdraws their help, the child assumes more of the strategic
planning and eventually gains competence to master similar problems without a
teacher’s aid or a more knowledgeable peer.
2. Maintaining the child’s interest in the task e.g., avoiding distraction and
providing clear instructions on how to start the task.
5. Demonstrating the task: showing the child how to do the task in simple,
clear steps.
As the child progresses through the ZPD, the necessary scaffolding level
declines from 5 to 1.
In addition, the teacher must look for discrepancies between students” efforts
and the solution, control for frustration and risk, and model an idealized version of
the act (Hausfather, 1996).
However, the studies described above offer empirical evidence that learning
based on the social development theory facilitates cognitive development over
other instructional strategies.
The structure of our schools does not reflect the rapid changes our society is
experiencing. The introduction and integration of computer technology in society
has tremendously increased the opportunities for social interaction.
Computer technology is a cultural tool that students can use to meditate and
internalize their learning. Recent research suggests changing the learning contexts
with technology is a powerful learning activity (Crawford, 1996).
Critical Evaluation
Vygotsky’s work has not received the same level of intense scrutiny that
Piaget’s has, partly due to the time-consuming process of translating Vygotsky’s
work from Russian.
Perhaps the main criticism of Vygotsky’s work concerns the assumption that
it is relevant to all cultures. Rogoff (1990) dismisses the idea that Vygotsky’s ideas
are culturally universal and instead states that scaffolding- heavily dependent on
verbal instruction – may not be equally useful in all cultures for all types of learning.
There is much emphasis on social interaction and culture, but many other
aspects of development are neglected, such as the importance of emotional factors,
e.g., the joys of success and the disappointments and frustration of failure act as
motivation for learning.
development.
Unlike Piaget, who emphasized universal cognitive change (i.e., all children would
go through the same sequence of cognitive development regardless of their cultural
experiences), Vygotsky leads us to expect variable development depending on
cultural diversity.
1. Vygotsky states the importance of cultural and social context for learning.
Cognitive development stems from social interactions from guided learning
within the zone of proximal development as children and their partners co-
construct knowledge.
2. For Vygotsky, the environment in which children grow up will influence how
they think and what they think about.
The importance of scaffolding and language may differ for all cultures.
Rogoff (1990) emphasizes the importance of observation and practice in
pre-industrial societies (e.g., learning to use a canoe among Micronesian
Islanders).
According to Piaget, language depends on thought for its development (i.e., thought
comes before language). For Vygotsky, thought and language are initially separate
systems from the beginning of life, merging at around three years of age, producing
verbal thought (inner speech). In Piaget’s theory, egocentric (or private) speech
gradually disappears as children develop truly social speech, in which they monitor
and adapt what they say to others.
Vygotsky disagreed with this view, arguing that as language helps children to think
about and control their behavior, it is an important foundation for complex cognitive
skills. As children age, this self-directed speech becomes silent (or private) speech,
referring to the inner dialogues we have with ourselves as we plan and carry out
activities.
development.