08 Anne Runehov
08 Anne Runehov
Anne L. C. RUNEHOV *
* Department
of Systematic Theology,
Copenhagen University, Denmark
Abstract
We cannot disregard that the neuroscientific
research on religious phenomena such as religious
experiences and rituals for example, has increased
significantly the last years. Neuroscientists claim that
neuroscience contributes considerably in the process of
understanding religious experiences, because
neuroscience is able to measure brain activity during
religious experiences by way of brain‐imaging
technologies. No doubt, those results of neuroscientific
research on religious experiences are an important
supplement to the understanding of some types of
religious experiences. However, some conclusions drawn
from neuroscientific research on religious experiences are
arguable. For example, one such conclusion is that
religious experiences are actually nothing but neural
activity, i.e. there is nothing ‘religious’ to the experiences
at all. Another such conclusion is that a person’s religious
experiences actually derive from some ultimate reality,
meaning that religious experiences are real. It is the latter
assertion that will be analyzed in the present paper. The
question is asked whether neuroscience alone is able to
affirm that religious experiences are real or whether there
are, besides neuroscientific issues, also cultural‐religious
assumptions that underlie this conclusion.
Introduction
The study I choose as a passage to the analysis is an
empirical neuroscientific study titled “The Measurement of
Cerebral Blood Flow during the Complex Cognitive Task of
Meditation using HMPAO‐Spect Imaging”, by Andrew
Newberg, Abass Alavi, Michael Baime, Michael Pourdehnad,
141
RUNEHOV ‐ Neuroscientific Explanations of Religious Experience are Not
free from Cultural Aspects
The Study
The participants in the neuroscientific study
performed by Newberg et al. on religious experiences
obtained by meditation were male and female Tibetan
Buddhist meditators and Franciscan nuns between the ages
of 38 and 52. All persons had more than 15 years of
practice of meditation or prayer. They practiced at least
one hour per day and at least five days a week.
Furthermore, all of them had participated in several
retreats lasting a minimum of three‐months and in one
yearly retreat of one month. The control group existed of
nine healthy persons who did not perform any meditation.
They were brain‐scanned once during 20 minutes.
However, even if both the participants who meditated and
those who did not were SPECT‐scanned at baseline, only
the meditating persons were also SPECT‐scanned in a later
phase of the experiment.
The task of the participants began with the
instruction to put themselves in their preferred position
for meditation or prayer. They were allowed to surround
themselves with objects that they generally use when they
142
Journal for Interdisciplinary Research on Religion and Science,
No. 2, January 2008
143
RUNEHOV ‐ Neuroscientific Explanations of Religious Experience are Not
free from Cultural Aspects
144
Journal for Interdisciplinary Research on Religion and Science,
No. 2, January 2008
Discussion
The results show whether the hypotheses were
confirmed or not and they show which brain structures
and functions that underwent increased or decreased
neural activity at the most intensive moment of meditation
and prayer. However, they do not tell us anything about the
phenomenon religious experience itself, about the quality
of the experiences, about the core characteristics of the
religions to which the meditators belonged, nor about the
reality status of the experiences. Furthermore, they do not
tell us anything about the experiencers themselves or
about how they interpret their religious experiencers.
Nevertheless, Newberg et al. emphasize neuroscience’s
weight when they present their neuroscientific view on the
reality of religious experiences, as follows.
Firstly, Newberg et al. equal all human experiences,
neuroscientifically seen, and they mean that everything we
experience we experience in a second‐hand‐way, i.e. all our
experiences are mediated by our brain. They write:
If God exists, for example, and if He appeared to you in some
incarnation, you would have no way of experiencing His presence,
except as part of a neurologically generated rendition of reality.
You would need auditory processing to hear His voice, visual
processing to see His face, and cognitive processing to make sense
of His message. Even if He spoke to you mystically, without words,
you would need cognitive functions to comprehend His meaning,
and input from the brain’s emotional centers to fill you with
145
RUNEHOV ‐ Neuroscientific Explanations of Religious Experience are Not
free from Cultural Aspects
146
Journal for Interdisciplinary Research on Religion and Science,
No. 2, January 2008
147
RUNEHOV ‐ Neuroscientific Explanations of Religious Experience are Not
free from Cultural Aspects
148
Journal for Interdisciplinary Research on Religion and Science,
No. 2, January 2008
Cultural Implications
Let us return to Newberg et al.’s first inference based
on their studies and ask what arguments that could
underlie their claim that all experiences are identical seen
from a neuroscientific point of view. To discover these we
have to dig deeper into Newberg et al.’s reasoning
concerning the equality of experiences, especially religious
experiences. They maintain that there cannot be two
different experiences of Absolute Unitary Being as such.
However, this claim does not derive from the results of the
149
RUNEHOV ‐ Neuroscientific Explanations of Religious Experience are Not
free from Cultural Aspects
150
Journal for Interdisciplinary Research on Religion and Science,
No. 2, January 2008
151
RUNEHOV ‐ Neuroscientific Explanations of Religious Experience are Not
free from Cultural Aspects
152
Journal for Interdisciplinary Research on Religion and Science,
No. 2, January 2008
153
RUNEHOV ‐ Neuroscientific Explanations of Religious Experience are Not
free from Cultural Aspects
Conclusion
Even though Newberg et al. agree that one has to take
into account both the material and the spiritual side of the
world when studying religious experiences, they would
profit from further considering the strong impact of the
culture‐religious implications of religious experiences. If
neuroscientists want to study the spiritual side of the
world, they also need to study its cultural side more
seriously. Nevertheless, this does not exclude that the
experience Absolute Unitary Being, everything else being
the same, is as Newberg et al. describe it. Neither does it
exclude that when somebody says that she experienced
Jesus, that she really did so. However, these queries belong
to another debate.
154
Journal for Interdisciplinary Research on Religion and Science,
No. 2, January 2008
References
155
RUNEHOV ‐ Neuroscientific Explanations of Religious Experience are Not
free from Cultural Aspects
156