5702 LN13 Synambig
5702 LN13 Synambig
Syntactic Ambiguity
Contents
13.1 Syntactic Ambiguity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
13.2 Scope Ambiguity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2
13.3 Local Ambiguity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2
13.4 Catastrophic Local Ambiguity [Bever, 1970] . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3
13.5 Garden Path Model [Frazier, 1979] . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
13.6 Problems with Garden Path Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5
13.7 Problems for Competition in Constraint-based Models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6
13.8 Surprisal [Hale, 2001, Levy, 2008] . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
Ambiguity occurs in syntax too, even though it must propagate for several words:
• (headline) ‘Teacher strikes idle kids’
V V
N V-aN N V-aN
A-aN N
N V-aN N V-aN
We can also have scope ambiguity with same categories (more common). . .
1
13.2 Scope Ambiguity
N A-aN
A-aN R-aN
R-aN-bN N
N A-aN
R-aN-bN N
N A-aN
N V-aN N V-aN
V-aN-bN N V-aN-bN N
N N N A-aN
N A-aN N N
... depict V.M. and child on hobbyhorse ... depict V.M. and child on hobbyhorse
2
• ‘While Susan was dressing the baby ... ’ (prior to end of sentence)
V
R-aN V
R-aN-bV V
N V-aN
V-aN-b(A-aN) A-aN
A-aN-bN N
R-aN V
R-aN-bV V N V-aN
N V-aN
V-aN-b(A-aN) A-aN
V N V-aN
N V-aN N A-aN
the horse raced past the barn ...fell?! the horse raced past the barn fell
3
‘Raced ...’ must be reanalyzed as a passive (reduced relative) modifier.
Often readers don’t even see this second possible analysis. Perhaps it comes too late.
The Garden Path model also ignores lexical preferences for subcategories.
The Garden Path model makes parse decisions using heuristics (rules of thumb):
• late closure: the parser prefers to keep constituents open, attach low
• minimal attachment: the parser prefers to build simpler structure with fewer nodes
(but you have to providently define verb phrases to have fewer nodes)
• main assertion: the parser prefers to modify the main assertion
4
13.6 Problems with Garden Path Model
[Gorrell, 1991]: Mitchell’s sentences had suggestive segmentations, lacked punctuation.
[Trueswell et al., 1993]: eye-tracking experiments show rapid lexical subcategory effects
• stimuli: sentences read with eye-tracking headset
(a) ‘The student forgot the solution was in the book.’ (prefer noun phrase complement)
(b) ‘The student hoped the solution was in the book.’ (prefer sentential complement)
• measure: eye-tracking fixation positions and durations
• results: large delay at ‘in’ for verbs preferring nominal (a)
[Kjelgaard & Speer, 1999]: word-naming experiments show rapid prosody effects
• stimuli: manipulated speech; visual request to pronounce ‘is’ after ‘house’
5
(a) cooperative: ‘When Roger leaves – the house is dark.’ (‘–’ = pause)
(b) cooperative: ‘When Roger leaves the house – it’s dark.’
(c) uncooperative: ‘When Roger leaves the house – is dark.’
(d) uncooperative: ‘When Roger leaves – the house it’s dark.’
• measure: duration of word-naming from onset of request
• results: delay for uncooperative sentences (c,d)
6
• readers choose a single analysis randomly, based on multiple interactive factors;
• readers stick with it, then reanalyze if wrong;
• processing is interactive, but not parallel (i.e. propagated along multi-word sequences).
N V-aN N V-aN
D N-aD R-aN-bN N
D N-aD
the horse raced past the barn the horse raced past the barn fell
7
grammatical rules lexical rules 12
z }| { z }| { z }| {
(First tree: 1 · .5 · .05 · 1 · .5 · .5 · .001 · .001 · .1 · .5 · .001 = . 000000000000 3125.)
(Second: 1| · .1 · .5{z
· .1 · 1 · .5 .5 · .001 · .0001 {z
}·| · .1 · .5 · .001 · .001
} = . 000000000000
| {z } 00000625.)
grammatical rules lexical rules 12
The probability of the observed words is the sum of the probabilities of possible trees:
. 000000000000
| {z } 3125 + . 000000000000
| {z } 00000625 = . 000000000000
| {z } 31250625.
12 12 12
PRACTICE: Using the above rules, what’s the probability of ‘the horse raced,’ and ‘the horse fell’:
V V
N V-aN N V-aN
PRACTICE: What share of activation (probability) remains, above, when ‘raced’ is encountered?
(You may limit your consideration to just the above two trees.)
(The probability before ‘raced’ is the sum of both trees; the probability after just contains the first.)
References
[Bever, 1970] Bever, T. G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structure. In J. R. Hayes
(Ed.), Cognition and the Development of Language (pp. 279–362). New York: Wiley.
[Frazier, 1979] Frazier, L. (1979). On comprehending sentences: syntactic parsing strategies.
PhD thesis, University of Connecticut.
[Gorrell, 1991] Gorrell, P. (1991). Subcategorization and sentence processing. In R. Berwick, S.
Abney, & C. Tenny (Eds.), Principle-based parsing, Studies in linguistics and philosophy ; 44.
Dordrecht: Kluwer.
8
[Hale, 2001] Hale, J. (2001). A probabilistic earley parser as a psycholinguistic model. In Pro-
ceedings of the second meeting of the North American chapter of the Association for Computa-
tional Linguistics (pp. 159–166). Pittsburgh, PA.
[Kjelgaard & Speer, 1999] Kjelgaard, M. M. & Speer, S. R. (1999). Prosodic facilitation and
interference in the resolution of temporary syntactic closure ambiguity. Journal of Memory and
Language, 40(2), 153–194.
[Levy, 2008] Levy, R. (2008). Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition, 106(3),
1126–1177.
[Mitchell, 1987] Mitchell, D. C. (1987). Lexical guidance in human parsing: Locus and process-
ing characteristics. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance XII: The Psychology of
Reading (pp. 601–618). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
[Tanenhaus et al., 1995] Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy,
J. C. E. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehen-
sion. Science, 268, 1632–1634.
[Trueswell et al., 1994] Trueswell, J., Tanenhaus, M., & Garnsey, S. (1994). Semantic influences
on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of
Memory and Language, 33(3), 285 – 318.
[Trueswell et al., 1993] Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Kello, C. (1993). Verb-specific
constraints in sentence processing: Separating effects of lexical preference form garden-paths.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 19(3), 528–553.
[van Gompel et al., 2001] van Gompel, R. P. G., Pickering, M. J., & Traxler, M. J. (2001). Reanal-
ysis in sentence processing. evidence against current constraint-based and two-stage models.
Journal of Memory and Language, (pp. 225–258).