Preview-9781351042772 A38969147
Preview-9781351042772 A38969147
and Phonolog y
Intended for the absolute beginner, Introducing Phonetics and Phonology requires
no previous background in linguistics, phonetics or phonology. Starting with a
grounding in phonetics and phonological theory, the book provides a base from
which more advanced treatments may be approached.
It begins with an examination of the foundations of articulatory and acoustic
phonetics, moves on to the basic principles of phonology and ends with an outline
of some further issues within contemporary phonology. Varieties of English,
particularly Received Pronunciation and General American, form the focus of
consideration, but aspects of the phonetics and phonology of other languages
are discussed as well. This new edition includes revised exercises and examples;
additional coverage of typology, autosegmental phonology and articulatory and
acoustic phonetics; broader coverage of varieties that now features Australian
English; and an extended Chapter 7 that includes more information on the
relationship between phonetics and phonology.
Introducing Phonetics and Phonology, 4th Edition remains the essential
introduction for any students studying this topic for the first time.
4th Edition
List of tables ix
List of figures x
Preface to the first edition xii
Preface to the second edition xiv
Preface to the third edition xv
Preface to the fourth edition xvi
The International Phonetic Alphabet xvii
1 Introduction 1
1.1 Phonetics and phonology 1
1.2 The generative enterprise 3
Further reading 6
3 Consonants 19
3.1 Stops 20
3.2 Affricates 27
3.3 Fricatives 28
3.4 Nasals 31
3.5 Liquids 32
vi Contents
3.6 Glides 35
3.7 An inventory of English consonants 37
Further reading 38
Exercises 38
4 Vowels 40
4.1 Vowel classification 40
4.2 The vowel space and Cardinal Vowels 41
4.3 Further classifications 43
4.4 The vowels of English 44
4.5 Some vowel systems of English 53
Further reading 57
Exercises 57
5 Acoustic phonetics 59
5.1 Fundamentals 59
5.2 Speech sounds 64
5.3 Cross-linguistic values 74
Further reading 75
Exercises 75
7 Features 95
7.1 Segmental composition 95
7.2 Phonetic versus phonological features 96
7.3 Charting the features 98
7.4 Conclusion 114
Further reading 115
Exercises 117
Glossary 247
References 254
Subject index 257
Varieties of English index 262
Language index 263
Tables
This textbook is intended for the absolute beginner who has no previous knowl-
edge of either linguistics in general or phonetics and phonology in particular.
The aim of the text is to serve as an introduction first to the speech sounds of
human languages – that is phonetics – and second to the basic notions behind
the organisation of the sound systems of human languages – that is phonology.
It is not intended to be a complete guide to phonetics nor a handbook of current
phonological theory. Rather, its purpose is to enable the reader to approach more
advanced treatments of both topics. As such, it is primarily intended for students
beginning degrees in linguistics and/or English language.
The book consists of two parts. After looking briefly at phonetics and phonol-
ogy and their place in the study of language, Chapters 2 through 6 examine the
foundations of articulatory and acoustic phonetics. Chapters 7 through 12 deal
with the basic principles of phonology. The final chapter of the book is intended as
a pointer towards some further issues within contemporary phonology. While the
treatment does not espouse any specific theoretical model, the general framework
of the book is that of generative phonology and in the main the treatment deals
with areas where there is some consensus among practising phonologists.
The primary source of data considered in the book is from varieties of English,
particularly Received Pronunciation and General American. At the same time,
however, aspects of the phonetics and phonology of other languages are also dis-
cussed. While a number of these languages may be unfamiliar to the reader, their
inclusion is both justifiable and important. In the first place, English does not
exemplify the full range of phonological processes that need to be considered
and exemplified. Second, the principles of phonology discussed in the book are
relevant to all human languages, not just English.
At the end of each chapter there is a short section suggesting further readings.
With very few exceptions the suggested readings are secondary sources, typically
intermediate and advanced textbooks. Primary literature has generally not been
referred to since the intended readership is the beginning student.
Exercises are included at the end of Chapters 2 through 12. These are intended
to consolidate the concepts introduced in each chapter and to afford the stu-
dent the opportunity to apply the principles discussed. While no answers are
Preface to the first edition xiii
provided, the data from a number of the exercises are given fuller accounts in
later chapters.
As with any project of this sort, thanks are due to to a number of colleagues,
friends and students. In particular we’d like to thank Michael Mackert for his
comments and critique. A number of other people have also given us the benefit of
their comments and suggestions, including Maggie Tallerman, Lesley Davenport,
Roger Maylor and Ian Turner. None of these people is to be blamed, individually
or collectively, for any remaining shortcomings. Thanks also to generations of
students at the universities of Durham, Delaware, Odense and Swarthmore Col-
lege, without whom none of this would have been necessary!
Mike Davenport & S. J. Hannahs
Durham, March 1998
Preface to the second edition
Whilst maintaining the basic structure and order of presentation of the first edi-
tion, we have added a new chapter on the syllable, stress, tone and intonation,
as well as adding or expanding sections in existing chapters, including a section
on recent developments in phonological theory. We have also made numerous
minor changes and corrections. We have been helped in this endeavour by many
colleagues, students, reviewers and critics. For his specialist help on the anatomy
of the vocal tract we’d like to express our thanks to James Cantrell. For help,
encouragement, and apposite (and otherwise!) criticism we’d also like to thank (in
alphabetical order): Loren Billings, Maria Cosy, David Deterding, Laura J. Downing,
Jan van Eijk, Andras Kertesz, Thomas Klein, Ken Lodge, Annalisa Zanola
Macola, Donna Jo Napoli, Kathy Riley, Jürg Strässler, Maggie Tallerman, Larry
Trask, and anonymous reviewers for Hodder Arnold. We’d also like to acknowl-
edge the help (and considerable patience) of staff at Hodder Arnold: Eva Marti-
nez, Lesley Riddle, Lucy Schiavone and Christina Wipf Perry. We apologise to
anyone we’ve left out (and to anyone who didn’t want to be included). None of
these people can be assumed to agree with (all of) our assumptions or conclusions;
nor (unfortunately) can they be held responsible for any remaining infelicities.
Mike Davenport & S. J. Hannahs
Durham & Newcastle, December 2004
Preface to the third edition
We are gratified and flattered that this book has maintained its popularity. We’ve
tried in this edition to correct further errors and to update and expand the con-
tent in the light of recent developments in phonological theory. We have added a
chapter on Optimality Theory and drawn explicit parallels between derivational
analysis and optimality accounts. We have also included a glossary of terms.
In preparing this edition, we’ve had the benefit of various comments from col-
leagues and students over the past few years. These include, in no particular order,
Mais Sulaiman, Tina Fry, Clare Wright, Magda Sztencel, Gosia Krzek, Paksiri
Tongsen, Yousef Elramli, Mohana Dass Ramasamy, Alex Leung, Robert Bell, Ali-
son Pennell. We would also like to thank staff at Hodder Education, in particular
Bianca Knights and Liz Wilson. We’d also like to thank Caroline McPherson for
her invaluable assistance in preparing the glossary. We apologise to any helpful
souls we have missed out, or any we have included against their will.
Mike Davenport & S. J. Hannahs
Durham & Newcastle, June 2010
Preface to the fourth edition
While this new edition has been updated in the light of recent theoretical research,
particularly in the later chapters, it remains what we originally set out to write:
an introduction to phonetics and phonology for students with no background in
phonetics, phonology or linguistics in general. And we’re pleased that it continues
to be useful for such students, despite the technical nature of the field.
Over the years since the publication of the last edition we have continued to
benefit from the comments of students and colleagues who have used the text-
book, including Danielle Turton, Míša Hejná, Twana Hamid, Zana Abdulkareem,
Adeline Charlton, Becky Musa, Tamader Hwaidi and numerous others. To them
and to the generations of undergraduate students, thanks.
Mike Davenport & S.J. Hannahs
Durham, January 2020
The International Phonetic Alphabet
Chapter 1
Introduction
This book is about the sounds we use when we speak (as opposed to the sounds
we make when we’re doing other things). It’s also about the various kinds of rela-
tionships that exist between the sounds we use. That is, it’s about ‘phonetics’ – the
physical description of the actual sounds used in human languages – and it’s about
‘phonology’ – the way the sounds we use are organised into patterns and systems.
As speakers of a particular language (English, say, or Hindi or Gaelic or Mohawk)
we obviously ‘know’ about the phonetics and phonology of our language, since
we use our language all the time, and unless we are tired or not concentrating (or
drunk), we do so without making errors. Furthermore, we always recognise when
someone else (for example a non-native speaker) pronounces something incor-
rectly. But, equally obviously, this knowledge is not something we are conscious
of; we can’t usually express the knowledge we have of our language. One of the
aims of this book is to examine some ways in which we can begin to express what
native speakers know about the sound system of their language.
DOI: 10.4324/9781351042789-1
2 Introduction
systems for each individual language; for example: how the sounds can be com-
bined, the relations between them and how they affect each other.
Consider the word ‘tlip’. Most native speakers of English would agree that this
is clearly not a word of their language, but why not? We might think that there is
a phonetic reason for this, for instance that it’s ‘impossible to pronounce’. If we
found that there are no human languages with words beginning ‘tl . . .’, we might
take that as evidence for claiming that the combination of ‘t’ followed by ‘l’ at
the beginning of a word is impossible. Unfortunately for such a claim, there are
human languages that happily combine ‘tl’ at the beginnings of words, e.g. Tlingit
(spoken in Alaska), Navajo (spoken in Southwestern USA), Welsh (spoken in
Wales); indeed, the language name Tlingit itself begins with this sequence. So,
if ‘tl . . .’ is phonetically possible, why doesn’t English allow it? The reason is
clearly not phonetic. It must therefore be a consequence of the way speech sounds
are organised in English which doesn’t permit ‘tl . . .’ to occur initially. Note that
this sequence can occur in the middle of a word, e.g. ‘atlas’. So the reason English
doesn’t have words beginning with ‘tl . . .’ has nothing to do with the phonetics,
since the combination is perfectly possible for a human being to pronounce, but
it has to do with the systematic organisation of speech sounds in English, that is
the phonology.
We noted that phonetics and phonology deal with many of the same things. In
another very real sense, however, phonetics and phonology are only accidentally
related. Most human languages use the voice and vocal apparatus as their primary
means of expression. Yet there are fully fledged human languages which use a
different means of expression, or ‘modality’. Sign languages – for example Brit-
ish Sign Language, American Sign Language, Sign Language of the Netherlands
and many others – primarily involve the use of manual rather than vocal ges-
tures. Since these sign languages use modalities other than speaking and hearing
to encode and decode human language, we need to keep phonetics – the surface
manifestation of spoken language – separate from phonology – the abstract sys-
tem organising the surface sounds and gestures. If we take this division seriously,
and we have to on the evidence of sign language, we need to be careful to distin-
guish systematically between phonetics and phonology.
A couple of words are in order here about the terms ‘generative’ and ‘gram-
mar’. To take the second word first, ‘grammar’ is here used as a technical term.
Outside linguistics, ‘grammar’ is used in a variety of different ways, often being
concerned only with certain aspects of a language, such as the endings on nouns
and verbs in a language like German. In generative linguistics, its meaning is
something like ‘the complete description of a language’, that is, what the sounds
are and how they combine, what the words are and how they combine, what the
meanings of the words are etc. The term ‘generative’ also has a specific meaning
in linguistics. It does not mean ‘concerning production or creation’; rather, adapt-
ing a usage from mathematics, it means ‘specifying as allowable or not within
the language’. A generative grammar consists of a set of formal statements which
delimit all and only all the possible structures that are part of the language in ques-
tion. That is, like a native speaker, the generative grammar must recognise those
things which are allowable in the language and also those things which are not
(hence the rather odd ‘all and only all’ in the preceding sentence).
The basic aim of a generative theory of linguistics is to represent in a formal way
the tacit knowledge native speakers have of their language. This knowledge is termed
native speaker competence – the idealised unconscious knowledge a speaker has
of the organisation of his or her language. Competence can be distinguished from
performance – the actual use of language. Performance is of less interest to genera-
tive linguists since all sorts of external, non-linguistic factors are involved when we
actually use language – factors like how tired we are, how sober we are, who we
are talking to, where we are doing the talking, what we are trying to achieve with
what we are saying etc. All these things affect the way we speak, but they are largely
irrelevant to our knowledge of how our language is structured and so are at best only
peripheral to the core generative aim of characterising native speaker competence.
So what exactly are the kinds of things that we ‘know’ about our language? That
is, what sorts of things must a generative grammar account for? One important
thing we know about languages is that they do indeed have structure; speaking
a language involves much more than randomly combining bits of that language.
If we take the English words ‘the’, ‘a’, ‘dog’, ‘cat’ and ‘chased’, native speakers
know which combinations are permissible (the term is grammatical) and which
are not (ungrammatical); so ‘the dog chased a cat’ or ‘the cat chased a dog’ are
fine, but *‘the cat dog a chased’ or *‘a chased dog cat the’ are not (an aster-
isk before an example indicates that the example is judged to be ungrammatical
by native speakers). So one of the things we know about our language is how
to combine words together to form larger constructions like sentences. We also
know about relationships that hold between words in such sentences; we know,
for example, that in ‘the dog chased a cat’ the words ‘the’ and ‘dog’ form a unit
and are more closely related than say ‘dog’ and ‘chased’ in the same sentence.
This type of knowledge is known as syntactic knowledge and is the concern of
that part of the grammar known as the syntax.
We also know about the internal make-up of words. In English a word like
‘happy’ can have its meaning changed by adding the element ‘un’ at the beginning,
Introduction 5
giving ‘unhappy’. Or it could have its function in the sentence changed by adding
‘ly’ to the end, giving ‘happily’. Indeed, it could have both at once, giving ‘unhap-
pily’, and again, native speakers ‘know’ this and can recognise ungrammatical
structures like *‘lyhappyun’ or *‘happyunly’. In the same way, speakers recog-
nise that adding ‘s’ to a word like ‘dog’ or ‘cat’ indicates that we are referring to
more than one, and they know that this plural marker must be added at the end of
the word, not the beginning. This type of knowledge about how words are formed
is known as morphology and is the concern of the morphological component of
the grammar.
The grammar must also account for our knowledge about the meanings of
words, how these meanings are related and how they can be combined to allow
sentences to be interpreted. This is the concern of the semantics.
Finally, as we have seen in this chapter, we as native speakers have knowledge
about the sounds of our language and how they are organised, that is, phonologi-
cal knowledge. This is the concern of the phonological component of the gram-
mar (and, of course, of this book).
So a full generative grammar must represent all of these areas of native speaker
knowledge (syntactic, morphological, semantic and phonological). In each of
these areas there are two types of knowledge native speakers have: that which is
predictable and that which is not. A generative grammar must therefore be able
to characterise both these sorts of knowledge. As an example, it is not predictable
that the word in English for a domesticated feline quadruped is ‘cat’; the relation-
ship between the animal and the sequence of sounds we use to name it is arbi-
trary (if it wasn’t arbitrary then presumably all languages would have the same
sequence of sounds for the animal). On the other hand, once we know what the
sounds are, it is predictable that the first sound will be accompanied by the outrush
of air known as aspiration that we discussed above, whereas the last sound will
not. Our model of grammar must also make this distinction between the arbitrary
and the predictable. This is done by putting all the arbitrary information in a part
of the grammar known as the lexicon (which functions rather like a dictionary).
The predictable facts are then expressed by formal statements known as rules or
constraints, which act on the information stored in the lexicon.
So, to return to our feline quadruped, the lexicon would contain all the arbitrary
facts about this word, including information on its syntactic class (that it is a
noun), on its meaning (a domesticated feline quadruped!) and on its pronunciation
(a ‘c’ sound followed by an ‘a’ sound followed by a ‘t’ sound). This information,
known as a lexical entry, is then available to be acted upon by the various sets of
statements in the components of the grammar. So the syntax might put the word
in the noun slot in a structure like ‘the big NOUN’, the phonology would specify
the actual pronunciation of each of the three sounds in the word, the semantics
link the word to its meaning etc. In this way, the grammar as a whole serves to
‘generate’ or specify allowable surface structures that the lexical entries can be
part of, and can thus make judgements about what is or is not part of the language,
in exactly the same way that a native speaker can. If faced with a structure like
6 Introduction
*‘the very cat dog’ the syntactic component of the grammar would reject this as
ungrammatical because the word ‘cat’ (a noun) is occupying an adjective slot,
not a noun slot; if faced with a pronunciation which involves the first sound of
‘cat’ being accompanied by a ‘glottal stop’ (see Section 3.1.5), the phonological
component would similarly reject this as ungrammatical, since this is not a char-
acteristic of such sounds at the beginning of words in English. The components
of the grammar thus serve to mediate between, or link, the two levels of structure:
(1) the underlying, mental elements of the language (that is, linguistic structures
in the speaker’s mind which the speaker is not consciously aware of) and (2) the
surface, physical realisations of these elements (that is, the actual sounds made by
the speaker when uttering a word).
The nature of the organisation of the phonological component of a generative
grammar is the concern of the second part of this book, Chapters 7 to 13. To begin
with, however, we concentrate in Chapters 2 to 6 on the description, classification
and physical characteristics of speech sounds, that is, phonetics.
Further reading
For general introductions to generative linguistic theory, including phonetics and
phonology, see for example Fromkin, Rodman and Hyams (2014), Frommer and
Finegan (2014), Akmajian, Farmer, Bickmore, Demers and Harnish (2017),
Kuiper and Allan (2017), Yule (2016).
Chapter 2
Introduction to articulatory
phonetics
The medium through which most of us experience language most of the time
is sound; for all non-deaf language users, the first exposure to language is
through sound, and in non-literate, hearing societies it is typically the only
medium. Humans have a variety of ways of producing sounds, not all of
which are relevant to language (for example: coughing, burping etc.). How
sound is used in language, that is, speech sounds, is the focus of this book,
and one obvious place to start out is to look at the physical processes involved
in the production of speech sounds by speakers – the study of articulatory
phonetics.
This chapter examines the major aspects of speech production:
• the airstream mechanism – where the air used in speech starts from and which
direction it is travelling in
• the state of the vocal cords – whether or not the vocal cords are vibrating,
which determines voicing
• the state of the velum – whether it is raised or lowered, which determines
whether a sound is oral or nasal
• the place and manner of articulation – the horizontal and vertical positions of
the tongue and lips.
DOI: 10.4324/9781351042789-2
8 Introduction to articulatory phonetics
2.1 Overview
Speech sounds are created by modifying the volume and direction of a flow of
air using various parts of the human respiratory system. We need to consider the
state of these parts in order to be able to describe and classify the sounds of human
languages. Figure 2.1 illustrates the parts of anatomy we need to examine.
the vocal cords) – velaric and glottalic airstreams, respectively. This gives a pos-
sible six airstream mechanisms:
However, as can be seen from the list, two of the possible types – pulmonic
ingressive and velaric egressive – are not found in any human language (it is
unclear why this is so).
Having established the starting point of the airflow and the direction it is travel-
ling in, we can then look at what happens to it as it moves over the other organs
involved in speech sound production. For what follows, we will assume a pul-
monic egressive airstream mechanism; sounds produced with other airstream
types will be discussed in later sections.