Ej 1358674
Ej 1358674
Developing students’ skills in solving mathematical problems and supporting ARTICLE DETAILS
creative mathematical thinking have been important topics of Finnish National Core
Curricula 2004 and 2014. To foster these skills, students should be provided with LUMAT Special Issue
Vol 10 No 2 (2022), 111–146
rich, meaningful problem-solving tasks already in primary school. Teachers have a
crucial role in equipping students with a variety of tools for solving diverse Pages: 36
mathematical problems. This can be challenging if the instruction is based solely on References: 60
tasks presented in mathematics textbooks. The aim of this study was to map
Correspondence:
whether a teaching approach, which focuses on teaching general heuristics for [email protected]
mathematical problem-solving by providing visual tools called Problem-solving
Keys, would improve students’ performance in tasks and skills in justifying their https://doi.org/10.31129/
reasoning. To map students' problem-solving skills and strategies, data from 25 fifth LUMAT.10.2.1696
graders’ pre-tests and post-tests with non-routine mathematical tasks were
analysed. The results indicate that the teaching approach, which emphasized
finding different approaches to solve mathematical problems had the potential for
improving students’ performance in a problem-solving test and skills, but also in
explaining their thinking in tasks. The findings of this research suggest that teachers
could support the development of problem-solving strategies by fostering
classroom discussions and using for example a visual heuristics tool called Problem-
solving Keys.
1 Introduction
During the primary school years, students develop their understanding of concept of
numbers and fluency in arithmetic skills (FNBE, 2016, p. 307). Learning
mathematical procedures is important, but it is also crucial to equip students with
strong problem-solving, reasoning, and thinking skills (e.g. Lester, 2003; Pehkonen
et al., 2013) to give tools for functioning in a complex, unpredictable future.
Mathematical problem-solving requires skills to apply variety of different solution
strategies and models (Leppäaho, 2018, p. 374). It is not uncommon that while
students may excel on routine exercises (those that they have already seen and
practiced), they fail to solve problems that differ from those they have previously
encountered (OECD, 2014).
112
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
113
LUMAT
2 Theoretical framework
114
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
4. Looking back and checking if the answer makes sense. (Polya, 1973, 5–6.)
115
LUMAT
Proportional reasoning requires skills to convey the same relationship for example
in producing or comparing ratios or finding a missing value. Abilities to reason
proportionally are a marker of a move towards more developed forms of reasoning
and form a foundation for example for algebra. Previous research indicates that
students are capable of solving proportional word problems already during their early
years of primary school (e.g. Tourniaire, 1986; Van Dooren et al., 2005; Vanluydt et
al., 2019).
Understanding ratio and proportion requires the ability to reason with
multiplicative relationships and distinguish them from relationships, which are
additive in nature (Van Dooren et al., 2010; Son, 2013). In an additive approach, the
student operates with an invariant difference between two values, whereas a
multiplicative approach requires an understanding of an invariant ratio between two
values (Van Dooren et al., 2010). Even if some proportional reasoning tasks can be
solved by additive approaches, also in those situations students need to understand
the co-varying situation of given values. Building-up or scaling-down by skip-
counting until the anticipated value is reached represents one of the strategies, which
116
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
often bases in additive reasoning. These types of solution methods can be supported
as steps towards multiplicative and proportional strategies.
Reasoning is an integral part of mathematical problem-solving and skills reach
beyond solving routine problems. Reasoning requires logical and systematic thinking,
being a process, which requires making conclusions on how to achieve certain goals;
these conclusions guide problem-solving and decision-making behaviour (Leighton,
2004; Grønmo et al., 2013). Students make notions on patterns and regularities and
use that information on making decisions on problem-solving approaches. Reasoning
involves skills to make conjectures, logical deductions based on assumptions and
rules, and abilities to justify results. (Grønmo et al., 2013, p. 27.) Teachers can help
students to develop these skills by presenting mathematical problems linked to
unfamiliar contexts and providing opportunities to solve open-ended or multi-step
problems (e.g. Grønmo et al., 2013). This has not been typically encouraged in school
culture (e.g. Pehkonen et al., 2013). Close-ended textbook examples do not necessarily
support students’ skills to apply the learned procedures or algorithms outside the
school context, and the applications to real-world situations can seem rare to them.
117
LUMAT
This report suggests that any student would benefit from getting familiar with a range
of generalisable problem-solving approaches instead of just learning a variety of
algorithms fit for certain types of mathematical problems.
Research was carried out in a large urban school in Northern Finland with a class of
25 fifth graders (12 boys and 13 girls). In the beginning of academic year 2015-2016,
students’ skills and strategies were mapped by a pre-test with proportional reasoning
problems. At that time, students’ mean age was 11 years and 2 months (range from 10
years and 9 months to 11 years and 7 months). During the autumn semester, the class
got familiar with a range of generalisable heuristics, which were used in solving a
variety of mathematical problems. Participating class followed the guidelines of
mathematics education outlined in the Finnish National Core Curriculum. Students
had attended five years of elementary school, but not received any formal instructions
in solving proportional reasoning tasks, which were the main domain for assessing
the development of mathematical problem-solving skills in this research. The class-
teacher had a degree as a Master of Education and had been teaching for 10 years in
primary and secondary schools. She was working on her Doctoral research on
mathematical problem-solving, and the study described in this report was carried out
of an interest towards developing students’ problem-solving skills.
At the beginning of the fifth school year, it appeared that many students seemed
to struggle in mathematical tasks and especially in explaining their problem-solving
processes in written form. Students were introduced to concrete tools called Problem-
solving Keys, which were modified from Strategy Keys based on work by Herold-
Blasius (2021). The aim was to provide students with a visual reminder of heuristics
for mathematical problem-solving tasks. Similar heuristics were outlined also in the
Singaporean Mathematics Syllabus 2013 and used as a reference when classifying and
modifying the Keys for teaching purposes in Finland.
Fifth graders had three mathematics lessons every week. Mathematics textbooks
were used, but in addition to those, during the autumn semester the class spent on a
weekly basis on average one mathematics lesson on working with mathematical tasks
in a practical context and learning a variety of general heuristics for problem-solving.
Out-of-the-textbook problems were solved during the spring semester, too, but
learning heuristics was not the focus anymore. Post-test data was collected at the end
of the fifth grade in 2016 by using the same test than in the beginning of the school
118
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
year. At that time, the mean age of students was 12 years (range from 11 years and 7
months to 12 years and 5 months).
The central idea of study in a real-life context was to teach mathematics and general
heuristics through solving a variety of out-of-the-textbook problems. Mathematical
problems were sourced for example from everyday situations, children’s literacy, and
local, national and international news. In addition to that, students created word
problems for their peers and learned to solve them in various ways. Problems were
often integrated into other subjects, such as Environmental Studies and other Science
themes, Finnish as a mother tongue and Arts and Crafts.
Exploration of mathematical problems followed a framework with different
phases of problem-solving (Stein et al., 2008; OECD, 2014, p. 31): first, the task was
presented by the teacher to the students (a launch phase), then students worked on
problems either in small groups or individually (an exploration phase, planning and
executing) and finally the outcomes were shared and discussed (a summarising and
reflecting phase). In practice, the process was not a linear, step-by-step progressing
path, but rather a flexible model for moving between different phases. Quite often
discussing and sharing the ideas led to returning to the exploration phase and
assessing the problem-solving approaches from new perspectives. Polya’s
(1945/1973) four step model was followed especially during the exploration and
summarising phases. Problems were solved in collaborative settings always when it
was possible: this enabled discussion and made the importance of justifying thinking
more visible.
Heuristics or general techniques for solving mathematical problems were
introduced to students by using a visual tool called Problem-solving Keys, which are
based on for example Polya’s (1945/1973) and Bruder and Collet’s (2011) heuristics,
and the same ideas were outlined in Singaporean Mathematics Curriculum 2013.
These heuristics were modified into a concrete tool by Herold-Blasius and Rott (2016)
and named as Strategy Keys. They describe these tools as “door openers” for a
problem-solving process and reminders of general heuristics that students have
learned (Herold-Blasius & Rott, 2016; Herold-Blasius, 2021).
Keys were modified for teaching experiment purposes, translated in Finnish, and
renamed as Problem-solving Keys. Keys that were used in this study were chosen
based on their generalisability, transferability and fit for the mathematics curriculum
119
LUMAT
for this age group. The guidance progressed by introducing one or two keys
(heuristics) at the time, linking them in a variety of out-of-the-textbook problems. As
the new heuristic was introduced and practised, the key linked to that particular
heuristic was added to a student’s personal “Problem-solving key chain”. Each key
was linked to a mathematical problem, which was often open-ended, or at least had
multiple different solution paths to choose from. The problem was chosen so that the
heuristic in that Key worked well in solving a particular problem: for example,
Gravett’s The Rabbit Problem (2009) was based in Fibonacci’s approach and used
when practising the problem solving by using a table. Literature often offers an
excellent context to bring abstract and complicated concepts closer to real-world
situations. The following Figure 1 shortly illustrates the keys which were chosen as a
focus area in this study, and some prompts, which were presented in guiding the
learning processes.
In addition to the keys described in Figure 1, students had three additional keys,
which were called “When I’m stuck” -keys:
120
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
These ideas often enabled either using other heuristics or continuing with other
steps in the process. Key called “Solve part of the problem” turned out to be well used.
Breaking the problem down into more approachable steps and solving even a small
part of the problem opened new insights on how to proceed in tasks. The notion is not
new: Duncker (1945, p. 8) linked “reformulation of the original problem” as one of the
important characteristics in the problem-solving process and referred to this
reformulation as a step or phase on a path towards solution. As Kilpatrick (2016, p.
45) points out, it might be easier to solve the problem if it is broken into smaller pieces
or modified into another form.
It is important that teachers value attempts for intuitive problem-solving methods
and will be able to guide the student forward. Children often use everyday logic and
apply that also to mathematical problems. They can be invited to justify their thinking
and invent proofs for their ideas. Later students should learn about mathematical
proof and formalities. They need to recognise that there is a difference between a
guess, a conjecture, and a proven assertion. It is important to encourage students to
wonder why things are as they are and guide them in providing a logical chain of
reasons as the explanation. (Goldenberg et al., 2003, p. 24.) An educated guess differs
from a random guess by its metacognitive aspects. True mathematical problem
solving is challenging, but at the same time rewarding for both students and the
teacher, as Schoenfeld (1992, p. 354) points out.
Students benefit from having opportunities to explain their thinking not only by
using mathematical language, but also pictorial and natural language: possibilities to
draw and write during the problem-solving process may strengthen the
understanding of mathematical concepts and contribute to mathematical thinking
skills (Joutsenlahti & Kulju, 2017). Open-ended problems or planted error tasks are
excellent domains for developing students’ skills in negotiating and articulating their
mathematical ideas to others. According to D’Ambrosio and Prevost (2008, p. 276)
“all contributions should be valued and respected“. By assessing students’ solution
methods, also the self-generated ones, teachers can correct the ones which are
mathematically acceptable, or guide students forward in partially constructed
explanations. Classroom discussions provide crucial information on students’
understanding on topic and problem-solving processes. Effective teaching includes
121
LUMAT
listening to students’ ideas and explanations and using that information as a guide in
making decisions on instruction (e.g. Lester, 2013; Ivars et al. 2020; Shaughnessy et
al., 2021). These views were at the centre point of the study, because a variety of out-
of-the-textbook problems enabled interesting mathematical discussions in the
classroom. Conversations were emphasized as important steps in learning problem-
solving. Students were advised and expected to show their thinking in tasks by writing
down the calculations or drawing the stages in solving the problem in a
mathematically understandable way. That can be surprisingly difficult even for the
10-12-year-old students, who have already attended several mathematics lessons per
week for multiple years.
122
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
1. Mum is making lemon tea. She mixes tea and sugar in a jug. Comparing ratios
Lemon tea in mixtures
Which one tastes the most sweet? Choose. (qualitative
1 glass of tea and 1 spoon of sugar comparison),
4 glasses of tea and 4 spoons of sugar adapted from
1 glass of tea and 3 spoons of sugar Noelting (1980)
and Kaput and
2. Which one tastes the most sweet? Choose. West (1994)
Lemon tea 1 glass of tea and 2 spoons of sugar
2 glasses of tea and 2 spoons of sugar
2 glasses of tea and 1 spoon of sugar
8. Paint- Green paint is made by mixing two buckets of blue paint and three Comparing ratios
mixture buckets of yellow paint. The painter needs to get more paint. How (quantitative
many buckets of blue and yellow paint does he need to get the comparison),
exactly same shade of green? Choose one option. similarity,
3 buckets of blue and 4 buckets of yellow paint adapted from
4 buckets of blue and 6 buckets of yellow paint Tourniaire
5 buckets of blue and 6 buckets of yellow paint (1986)
6 buckets of blue and 8 buckets of yellow paint
Tasks 6A, 6B, 7 and 9 required proportional reasoning with ratios, inverse
proportionality or similarity of mixtures. Students were explicitly asked to record
their thinking in these tasks and explain their problem-solving processes by
mathematical, pictorial and/or natural language in written form.
123
LUMAT
Table 2. Tasks used in assessing strategies: Proportional reasoning tasks with ratios, inverse proportionality
or similarity of mixtures
6A. Students are building geometric shapes. They make two similar Determining a
Rectangles rectangles and triangles by using short and long sticks. missing value with
continuous ratio-
How many sticks do they need in x? preserving,
geometric
similarity, idea
adapted from Mr.
Tall and Mr. Small
tasks by Karplus et
al. (1974), Lamon
6B. How many sticks do they need in x? (1993) and
Triangles research by Son
(2013)
7. Six painters paint a house in three days. If they all work at the same Inverse
Painters speed, how many painters would be needed to paint the same proportionality,
house in one day? item was created
for this research
9. Paint- Orange paint is made by mixing four buckets of yellow paint and one Comparing ratios
mixture bucket of red paint. To get exactly the same shade of orange, how (quantitative
many buckets of red would the painter need to mix to six buckets of comparison),
yellow? similarity, adapted
from Tourniaire
(1986)
124
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
First, students’ overall test performance in tasks 1-9 was assessed by awarding points
on correct and erroneous answers, but also on intermediate steps towards correct
explanation. In multiple choice items 1-5 and 8, students received 1 point for a correct
answer and 0 points for an erroneous one. In item 3, students were expected to choose
both options A and B to gain 1 point, and 0,5 points were given, if they chose either A
or B. Maximum points for multiple choice questions were 6. In items 6A, 6B, 7 and 9
students were expected to explain their thinking, and their answers were serving as a
base for building a framework for correct and erroneous strategies from intuitive to
more sophisticated ones. Maximum points for these items were 2 points for each. The
in-between marks were the following:
With this grading, it was possible to gain a maximum of 8 points in items 6A, 6B,
7 and 9. This approach was close to rating used in school mathematics tests for this
age group, and took also the partially correct answers into account. Numerical scores
were used as indicators of overall performance and possible development between
pre- and post-tests. Maximum points for the whole test were 14.
Exploring and mapping the strategies that students used in task began by
dismantling the data (students’ responses in items 6A, 6B, 7 and 9). This was done on
a detailed level by creating codes based on how students justified their thinking and
explained it by using numbers, drawings or written explanations. Coding was
concluded with Grounded Theory methods, which provide systematic, yet flexible
guidelines for collecting and analysing data (Charmaz, 2014; Birks & Mills, 2015;
Chun Tie et al., 2019). Written explanations were worked through in three phases of
analysis (Charmaz, 2014), and the framework for coding was created by classifying
similar responses to sub-categories (focused coding phase) and core categories
125
LUMAT
(theoretical coding phase). This scheme was used as the analytical tool to assess the
strategies that students used in solving tasks and on the other hand, as an indicator
on whether the teaching approach provoked a shift from intuitive to more
sophisticated heuristics linked to proportional reasoning.
Table 3 illustrates students’ correct answers in Task 6A, and how they were
grouped as sub-categories during the focused coding phase.
Table 3. Dismantling the data during the initial coding phase and sub-categories in focused coding phase:
example from correct approaches in Task 6A
Initial coding phase: observable behaviour Focused coding phase: sub-category
Student understands that the long side on the Demonstration of relative thinking between given
second rectangle is “three times longer” than the quantities but failing to provide mathematically
corresponding side on the first rectangle. understandable explanations.
Demonstrates thinking by addition: 20+20+20=60
and 15+15+15=45, but cannot clearly explain
where ”three times” comes from.
Student understands that 20 long sticks = 60 short
sticks by comparing corresponding parts but
cannot explain how he/she gets x=45.
Student calculates the ratio between the sides of Demonstration of relative thinking between
the first rectangle and applies the same logic to quantities e.g. by using ratio as a unit in
another picture. calculations, but not necessarily able to create
Student calculates that on the first rectangle the generalisable formulas.
vertical side is ¾ of the horizontal side and applies
the logic to the second rectangle to determine x
(for example by deducting ¼ from 60).
Student understands that long sticks are three
times longer than short sticks, and is able to utilize
the knowledge to solve missing value x.
Student works with both rectangles simultaneously Use of formal operations based on ratio or use of a
by using the ratio 3:4 to solve the missing value certain algorithm, such as cross-multiplication or
(ability to form generalisable calculations). “rule of three”.
Student uses a formula, such as a cross-
multiplication algorithm, “rule of three” or
equivalent to solve the task.
Consistency for the coding scheme was ensured by comparing the original data in
several phases of the coding by student to another student, student’s answer to
anticipated strategy and strategy by strategy. This involved repeated visits to original
answers to ensure that they were understood and interpreted correctly. Final scheme
for coding can be found in Appendix 1.
126
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
5 Results
Tasks 1-5 and 8 were multiple choice mixture tasks, and even though the student’s
choice of option could give some indication on solution strategy as well, this report
focuses on analysis and classification of solution approaches in tasks 6A, 6B, 7 and 9.
Students’ performance in tests provided insights on whether the teaching
approach, which focused on mathematical problem-solving, improved students’
general skills in solving also proportional reasoning tasks. In the beginning of fifth
grade, the mean for total score in the proportional reasoning test was 6,1 points (SD
2,5 p.). Boys (N=12) performed better than girls, their mean being 6,5 points (SD 2,1
p., minimum 3,5 p. and maximum 11 p.), whereas girls (N=12, one being absent) had
a mean of 5,5 points (SD 2,9 p., minimum 0,5 p. and maximum 12 p.).
After getting familiar with a variety of different heuristics (but not explicitly
algorithms) for solving mathematical problems, the post-test in June indicated
positive results: the mean score of students had risen to 8,9 points (SD 3,6 p.). It was
interesting to notice that this time girls performed better than boys. Female students’
mean had risen from pre-tests’ 5,5 points to 9,3 points (SD 3,3 p., min. 5 p. and max.
13,5 p.). Male students also improved their performance: in the pre-test they had a
mean of 6,5 points and in the post-test 8,4 points (SD 4 p., min. 0,5 p., max. 13,5 p.).
Development of total points is illustrated in Figure 2.
127
LUMAT
1
Student number 24 was absent during pre-test tasks 7-9 and the total points are not calculated. In the post-test,
student 1 did not answer any of the questions 6-9, which affected the final score. Student 11 left several tasks
unanswered, or it was not possible to determine the answer.
128
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
were designed to get gradually more challenging, but it seems that the difficulty of
items in this test did not match students’ skills and therefore the interpretations on
students’ performance need to be addressed with reservations. Tasks 1-5 were too easy
for fifth graders, and on the other hand the success rate in tasks 6A, 6B and 9 was 20-
28%. With this setting, the difficulty of task 7 was fairly ideal (success rate 58%) and
task 8 was almost too difficult. If the results of the post-test would be considered, too
difficult items would appear to be more ideal also in tasks, which required skills to
explain reasoning.
Results indicate that there was no significant improvement in how students
performed in multiple-choice mixture tasks in pre- and post-tests. High success rates
suggest that tasks 1-5 were easy for fifth graders at the first place. Development of
skills is visible in more difficult tasks 6, 7, 8 and 9, which are discussed in more detail
in Chapter 5.2. These results indicate that the teaching approach with a focus on
problem-solving may have had a positive influence on students’ abilities to solve tasks,
which require proportional reasoning skills.
Mathematical problems were usually not presented in a written form in a similar
way as typical word problems in mathematics books. Students did not get any extra
training in solving word problems and therefore the development of skills cannot be
explained by them getting more fluent in solving mathematical problems presented
in written form. A table describing students’ ability to solve tasks correctly can be
accessed in Appendix 2 and will be discussed task by task in the next sub-chapter.
One of the aims of the study was to find out if teaching approach, which offered tools
for heuristics, improved students’ skills in explaining their thinking in mathematical
tasks. In addition to getting familiar with problem-solving techniques, the practical
aim for the intervention was to build up students’ mathematical self-confidence so
that they would become active in describing their problem-solving processes.
Questions 6A, 6B, 7 and 9 were assessed as indicators, if students were able to give an
understandable explanation on how they processed the task. Informal techniques and
strategies provided an insight on how students understood problem-solving concepts
and were able to progress even in an unfamiliar type of a problem. Explanations and
heuristics were also assessed to see if there were differences in students’ use of correct
and erroneous strategies between the pre-test and the post-test.
129
LUMAT
In the beginning of fifth grade, students had major difficulties in describing their
problem-solving path and often left the explanation completely out. Teaching
approach, which encouraged students to describe their thinking even with partially
complete explanations and solving problems one step after another, seemed to have a
positive impact on their performance during the later phases of the academic year.
130
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
During the pre-test, the most common erroneous strategy was additive reasoning
(16%, N=4). It was typical to focus on dimensions within one rectangle, for example
reasoning that because the difference between the sides of the first figure was five (20-
15=5), the same difference applies for the second figure (60-5=55). In some cases,
students calculated the perimeter of the first rectangle and tried to apply or modify
the logic to find the missing value in the second rectangle. In both examples students
failed to understand the relational nature of the task: if 20 long sticks equal the length
of 60 short sticks, the same ratio should be maintained with 15 long sticks and x short
sticks. According to the previous research, students often rely on additive strategies
also in multiplicative situations (e.g. Tourniaire & Pulos, 1985; Nunes & Bryant, 1996;
Van Dooren et al., 2010; Son, 2013). Still, it is not clear how students choose their
preferations between additive and multiplicative relations (Vanluydt et al., 2019).
Both approaches can be characterised as intuitive in nature, yet it is difficult to
verbally describe reasoning; the given explanations are not necessarily in line with
students’ actual solution processes (Degrande et al., 2020).
Distinguishing multiplicative missing value problems from additive ones is
challenging for students. Additive thinking is emphasized during the first years of
school and the transition towards multiplicative ideas is not always straightforward.
On the other hand, additive reasoning could support the development of
multiplicative reasoning. Yet, the shift from additive to multiplicative thinking
requires a qualitative change in thinking (e.g. Nunes & Bryant, 1996). In the beginning
of the fifth grade, only one student approached the problem via multiplicative
reasoning but ended up in an erroneous end-result. After the intervention, one fifth
(N=5) of students turned into this approach. Even though these solution attempts
were erroneous, they could be interpreted as a shift towards understanding the
relative nature of the task. A more detailed description on the range of strategies that
students used can be accessed in Appendix 3.
Development of solution approaches and possible shifts between the strategies
was visualised as individual students’ performance in tasks. In Figure 4, explanation
categories are presented in an order, which suggests a hierarchy from erroneous and
intuitive ones to more sophisticated and generalisable strategies. Light green area
marks correct approaches. Opaque fill-ins in pre- and post-test markers indicate that
the student was able to solve the task correctly.
131
LUMAT
Correct solution approaches were rare in the pre-test, even though the task was
relatively easy. In the end of the fifth school year the frequency for correct strategies
had increased and students were able to approach the task by correct ratio or unit
factor approach.
132
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
post-test 76% of students (N=19) answered the question and less than a quarter did
not (N=6). 48% (N=12) of students were able to provide a correct answer. The
majority, 16 students out of 19, tried to explain their thinking in a written form. Three
students answered the question but did not provide any insights on the solution
process. In the pre-test only one student was able to choose a correct strategy, but in
the post-test the number increased to seven students (37% of 19 students answering
this question). This was quite an interesting finding, because students had not
encountered any similar mathematical problems during the academic year.
In the pre-test, only one student was able to explain thinking by demonstrating
mathematically correct reasoning. During the intervention the variety of correct
solution strategies increased. Students came to conclusions by additive reasoning or
more sophisticated multiplicative reasoning, and there were also some examples of
abilities to create correct, generalisable formulas to solve these types of problems.
None of the students solved the task by using ratio or unit factor.
Students often relied on erroneous intuitive strategies, such as trying to solve the
problem by random calculations on given numbers or basing the problem-solving
process on visual observations on given pictures, and not mathematically valid
concepts. The range of observable strategies in this task can be accessed in Appendix
3. Figure 5 illustrates the changes in used strategies that individual students had
between from the pre-test and to the post-test.
133
LUMAT
As one can see from Figure 5, task 6B was more difficult than 6A for this student
group. After the teaching experiment, successful students chose usually correct
additive or multiplicative reasoning, but many students left the explanation out still
during the post-test.
134
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
It is likely that the wording and the number structure of this task also guided the
choice of erroneous approaches: students often relied on erroneous multiplicative
reasoning, which was the most common erroneous strategy. To map the real
understanding of inverse proportionality, the task could be worded for example by
“Two painters paint the house in three days. If they all work at the same speed, how
many painters would be needed to paint the same house in two days?” In this case,
multiplying two by three would not result in a correct answer.
135
LUMAT
17% of students (N=4) had a correct answer in pre-test, but clearly struggled in
providing explanations on their reasoning processes: 12 out of 17 students, who
answered the question, left the explanation out. Only one student was able to choose
the correct strategy in this task, the other four relied on erroneous approaches. Post-
test results indicated significant improvement. 52% of students (N=13) ended up with
a correct answer, and 10 out of 23 students answering this question also described
their thinking with a correct strategy.
When having a closer look on strategies that students used, in the pre-test only
one student was able to provide an explanation while solving the problem correctly,
turning into a building-up strategy. Development of skills was visible in the post-test:
more students were able to not only explain their correct problem-solving process, but
also use a more sophisticated strategy by working with the ratio. Even though students
did not necessarily have skills to explain thinking with mathematically valid
expressions, they became more confident in using different strategies. Figure 7
illustrates the ratio approach, in which student proceeds one step at the time. In this
example, the student correctly reasons that because there is one red paint bucket in
every four yellow paint buckets, you need to add 1,5 buckets of red to six buckets of
yellow.
During the pre-test, only one of the students was able to provide a correct
explanation for the task, working with building-up strategy. In the end of the school
year there were indications on improved skills of explaining thinking also visible: 32%
(N=8) utilised either building-up or scaling-down strategy, ratio or unit factor
approach (the most common) or even correct formal operations with generalisable
formulas. In the post-test, five students (20%) were able to work through the task by
expressing that for every two buckets of yellow you need 0,5 buckets of red paint.
54% (N=13) of students gave an erroneous answer in the pre-test. Three students
relied on multiplicative reasoning but failed to understand the relative nature of the
136
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
task. They multiplied the given amounts of yellow paint, 6x4=24 or only stated
“Calculated by multiplication”, without providing a more detailed explanation.
During the post-test the most common erroneous strategy was additive reasoning.
24% (N=6) of students chose that strategy. They often based their reasoning on the
idea that “you need three more yellow than red”, focusing on the difference between
the given numbers in the original paint mixture and ignoring the need to maintain the
same relationship for the second paint. More detailed frequencies for the strategies
visible in task 9 can be accessed in Appendix 4.
Assessing and classifying students’ strategies was not always straightforward. For
example, student could state that multiplication was needed, but on the other hand,
relied on additive reasoning when providing an answer: “Because in the beginning
you needed three more yellow buckets than red buckets, so you just need to multiply
it”, providing three as an answer.
137
LUMAT
Figure 9. Development of strategies of three students scoring the lowest points in pre-test.
These three students tended to leave the answers completely out in the beginning
of the fifth grade. By the end of the fifth grade, frequencies for solving the tasks
correctly increased. With some individuals the difference was remarkable: for
example, student 7 got correct answers in the post-test, but would still have needed a
bit of support in explaining thinking (see Figure 9). After the problem-based teaching
period, students were more willing to engage in attempts to solve mathematical
problems, even though the strategies might not have been valid. With a correctly
138
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
timed intervention the teacher has a change to support the shift from erroneous
strategies to correct ones.
If lacking the skills to explain thinking with mathematically correct processes,
students often started to explore the dimensions between the given values by
implementing intuitive methods. Consider the explanation in Figure 10 that student
22 gave in Task 6B: the answer was correct 40 sticks, and in this case, the student
seemed to calculate the solution by exploring the given values and their relationships
within the first triangle. This student calculated the difference between the
hypotenuse and opposite side is multiplied by two to get the adjacent side.
Figure 10. It was not uncommon that the answers were correct,
but not necessarily based on generalisable ideas.
In these kinds of examples, which are very common in primary school, students
would benefit from opportunities to discuss their ideas with the teacher or with a peer;
what is the purpose of short and long sticks in this task, and how should that
information guide the solution process? If the strategy works with the given values,
can that be generalised to all triangles with a 90-degree angle? How about all types of
triangles?
Findings of the study indicated that especially students with lower points
benefited from exploring different problems and heuristics to approach them.
Students with high points in the pre-test were able to develop their skills in explaining
their ideas and on the other hand, to move towards more advanced strategies (see
Figure 11).
139
LUMAT
Figure 11. Development of strategies of four students scoring the highest points in pre-test.
6 Discussion
140
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
141
LUMAT
142
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
Acknowledgements
Strategy Keys as concrete tools were first introduced to the author by Raja Herold-
Blasius in 2015 at the Joint Conference of ProMath and the GDM Working Group of
Problem Solving in Halle, Germany. Thank you for the idea.
References
Baxter, G. P. & Junker, B. (2001). Designing Cognitive-Developmental Assessments: A Case Study
in Proportional Reasoning. In National Council for Measurement in Education. Washington.
Birks, M. & Mills, J. (2015). Grounded theory: a practical guide (2nd Ed.). Sage.
Bruder, R. & Collet, C. (2011). Problemlösen lernen im Mathematikunterricht. Cornelsen Verlag.
Charmaz, K. (2014). Constructing grounded theory (2nd Ed). Sage.
Christou, C. & Philippou, G. (2002). Mapping and development of intuitive proportional
thinking. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 20(3), 321-336.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0732-3123(02)00077-9
Chun Tie, Y., Birks, M. & Francis, K. (2019). Grounded theory research: A design framework for
novice researchers. SAGE Open Medicine, 7. https://doi.org/10.1177/2050312118822927
D'Ambrosio, B. S. & Prevost, F. J. (2008). Highlighting the humanistic dimensions of mathematics
activity through classroom discourse. In P. C. Elliott. & C. M. E. Garnett (Eds.), Getting into
the mathematics conversation: valuing communication in mathematics classrooms:
readings from NCMT's school-based journals (pp. 273-277). National Council of Teachers of
Mathematics.
Degrande, T., Verschaffel, L. & Van Dooren, W. (2020). To add or to multiply in open problems?
Unraveling children’s relational preference using a mixed-method approach. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 104(3), 405-430. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-020-09966-z
Duncker, K. (1945). On problem-solving. Psychological Monographs, 58(5), i-113.
Finnish National Board of Education. (2004). Perusopetuksen opetussuunnitelman perusteet
2004. Opetushallitus.
Finnish National Board of Education. (2016). National Core Curriculum for Basic Education
2014. Opetushallitus.
Fuchs, L. S. & Fuchs, D. (2003). Enhancing the mathematical problem solving of students with
mathematics disabilities. In H. L. Swanson, K. Harris & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of
learning disabilities (pp. 306-322). Guilford Press.
Fujimura, N. (2001). Facilitating Children's Proportional Reasoning: A Model of Reasoning
Processes and Effects of Intervention on Strategy Change. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 93(3), 589-603. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.93.3.589
143
LUMAT
Gallagher Landi, M. A. (2001). Helping Students with Learning Disabilities Make Sense of Word
Problems. Intervention in School and Clinic, 37(1), 13-18.
https://doi.org/10.1177/105345120103700103
Goldenberg, E. P., Shteingold, N. & Feurzeig, N. (2003). Mathematical habits of mind of young
children. In F. K. J. Lester (Eds.), Teaching mathematics through problem solving:
Prekindergarten-Grade 6 (pp. 15-29). National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Gravett, E. (2009). The Rabbit Problem. MacMillan Children's Books.
Grønmo, L. S., Lindquist, M., Arora, A. & Mullis, I. V. S. (2013). TIMSS 2015 Mathematics
Framework. In I. V. S. Mullis & M. O. Martin (Eds.), TIMSS 2015 assessment
frameworks (pp. 11-27). TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center, Lynch School of
Education, Boston College and International Association for the Evaluation of Educational
Achievement (IEA).
Hart, K. (1984). Ratio and proportion. In K. Hart, M. Brown, D. Kerslake, D. Küchemann & G.
Ruddock (Eds.), Chelsea Diagnostic Mathematics Test. Teacher's guide (pp. 93-100). NFER-
Nelson.
Herold-Blasius, R. (2021). Problemlösen mit Strategieschlüsseln. Eine explorative Studie zur
Unterstützung von Problembearbeitungsprozessen bei Dritt- und Viertklässlern. Springer
Spektrum. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-32292-2
Herold-Blasius, R. & Rott, B. (2016). Using strategy keys as tool to influence strategy behaviour. A
qualitative study. In T. Fritzlar, D. Assmus, K. Bräuning, A. Kuzle, & B. Rott (Eds.), Problem
solving in mathematics education (Vol. 6, pp. 137–147). VTM.
Hiebert, J. (2003). Signposts for teaching mathematics through problem solving. In F. K. J. Lester
(Eds.), Teaching mathematics through problem solving: Prekindergarten-Grade 6 (pp. 53-
61). National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Ivars, P., Fernández, C. & Llinares, S. (2020). A Learning Trajectory as a Scaffold for Pre-service
Teachers’ Noticing of Students’ Mathematical Understanding. International Journal of
Science and Mathematics Education, 18(3), 529-548. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-019-
09973-4
Joutsenlahti, J. & Kulju, P. (2017). Multimodal Languaging as a Pedagogical Model—A Case Study
of the Concept of Division in School Mathematics. Education Sciences, 7(1),
9. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci7010009
Kaitera, S. (2021). Mathematical problem-solving keys. Library of Open Educational Resources.
https://aoe.fi/#/materiaali/1685
Kaput, J. J. & West, M. M. (1994). Missing-value proportional reasoning problems: Factors
affecting informal reasoning patterns. In G. Harel & J. Confrey (Eds.), The Development of
Multiplicative Reasoning in the Learning of Mathematics (pp. 235–287). State University of
New York Press.
Karplus, E. F., Karplus, R. & Wollman, W. (1974). Intellectual Development Beyond Elementary
School IV: Ratio, The Influence of Cognitive Style. School Science and Mathematics, 74(6),
476-482. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1949-8594.1974.tb08937
Karplus, R., Pulos, S. & Stage, E. K. (1983). Early Adolescents' Proportional Reasoning on 'Rate'
Problems. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 14(3), 219-233.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00410539
Kilpatrick, J. (2016). Reformulating: Approaching Mathematical Problem Solving as Inquiry. In P.
Felmer, E. Pehkonen & J. Kilpatrick (Eds.), Posing and Solving Mathematical Problems:
Advances and New Perspectives (pp. 69-82). Springer.
Lamon, S. (1993). Ratio and proportion: Children's cognitive and metacognitive processes. In T. P.
Carpenter, E. Fennema & T. A. Romberg (Eds.), Rational numbers: An integration of
research (pp. 131-156). Lawrence Erbaum Associates.
144
KAITERA & HARMOINEN (2022)
Lamon, S. (2012). Teaching Fractions and Ratios for Understanding. Essential Content
Knowledge and Instructional Strategies for Teachers (3rd Ed.). Routledge.
Lamon, S. J. (2007). Rational Numbers and Proportional Reasoning. Toward a Theoretical
Framework for Research. In F. Lester (Eds.), Second handbook of research on mathematics
teaching and learning (pp. 629-668). Information Age Publishing.
Langrall, C. W. & Swafford, J. (2000). Three balloons for two dollars: Developing proportional
reasoning. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 6, 254–261.
Leighton, J. P. (2004). Defining and describing reason. In J. P. Leighton & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.),
The nature of reasoning (pp. 3–11). Cambridge University Press.
Lesh, R., Post, T. & Behr, M. (1988). Proportional Reasoning. In J. Hiebert & M. Behr
(Eds.), Number concepts and operations in the middle grades (pp. 93-118). Lawrence
Erlbaum & National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Lester, F. K. (2003). Preface. In F. Lester (Eds.), Teaching mathematics through problem solving:
Prekindergarten-Grade 6 (pp. ix-xvi). National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Lester, F. K. J. (2013). Thoughts about research on Mathematical problem-solving instruction. The
Mathematics Enthusiast, 10 (1-2), 245-278. https://doi.org/10.54870/1551-3440.1267
Leppäaho, H. (2018). Ongelmanratkaisun opettamisesta. In J. Joutsenlahti, H. Silfverberg, & P.
Räsänen (Eds.), Matematiikan opetus ja oppiminen (pp. 368–393). Niilo Mäki Instituutti.
Ministry of Education Singapore. (2012). Mathematics Syllabus 2013. Primary One to Six.
https://www.moe.gov.sg/-
/media/files/primary/mathematics_syllabus_primary_1_to_6.pdf?la=en&hash=B401E761C
0BFC490279883CCE4826924CD455F97
Misailidou, C. & Williams, J. (2003). Diagnostic assessment of children's proportional
reasoning. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 22, 335-368. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0732-
3123(03)00025-7
Näveri, L., Pehkonen, E., Ahtee, M., Hannula, M. S., Laine, A. & Heinilä, L. (2011). Finnish
elementary teachers’ espoused beliefs on mathematical problem solving. In MAVI-17
Conference, Bochum, Germany. (pp. 161-171).
Niemi, H. & Nevgi, A. (2014). Research studies and active learning promoting professional
competences in Finnish teacher education. Teaching and Teacher Education, 43, 131-
142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2014.07.006
Noelting, G. (1980). The Development of Proportional Reasoning and the Ratio Concept Part I -
Differentiation of Stages. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 11(2), 217-
253. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00304357
Nunes, T. & Bryant, P. (1996). Children doing mathematics. Wiley.
OECD. (2014). PISA 2012 Results: Creative Problem Solving. Students' skills in tackling real-life
problems. (Volume V). OECD Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264208070-en
Pehkonen, E., Näveri, L. & Laine, A. (2013). On Teaching Problem Solving in School
Mathematics. Center for Educational Policy Studies Journal, 3(4), 9-23.
Polya, G. (1945). How to solve it: a new aspect of mathematical method. Princeton University
Press.
Polya, G. (1973). How to solve it: A new aspect of mathematical method (2nd. edition). Princeton
University Press.
Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (2014). Smart technology for self-organizing processes. Smart
Learning Environments, 1(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40561-014-0001-8
Schoenfeld, A. H. (1985). Mathematical problem solving. Academic Press.
145
LUMAT
146