Compet
Compet
What Is Linguistics?
Figure 1.1
A competence model
Figure 1.2
A performance model
Figure 1.3
An acquisition model
requests, making assertions, and so on. And there is nothing that can be
expressed in one language that cannot be expressed in any other. Obvi-
ously, one language may have terms not found in another language, but
it is always possible to invent new terms to express what we mean: any-
thing we can imagine or think, we can express in any human language.
Turning to more abstract properties, even the formal structures of
language are similar: all languages have sentences made up of smaller
phrasal units, these units in turn being made up of words, which are
themselves made up of sequences of sounds. All of these features of hu-
man language are so obvious to us that we may fail to see how surprising
it is that languages share them. When linguists use the term language, or
natural human language, they are revealing their belief that at the abstract
level, beneath the surface variation, languages are remarkably similar in
form and function and conform to certain universal principles.
In relation to what we have just said about universal principles, we
should observe once again that most of the illustrative examples in this
book are drawn from the English language. This should not mislead you
into supposing that what we say is relevant only to English. We will be
introducing fundamental concepts of linguistics, and we believe that these
have to be applicable to all languages. We have chosen English examples
so that you can continually check our factual claims and decide whether
they are empirically well founded. Linguistics, perhaps more than any
other science, provides an opportunity for the student to participate in
the research process. Especially in chapter 5, ‘‘Syntax,’’ you will be able
to assess the accuracy of the evidence that bears on hypothesis formation,
and after having followed the argumentation in the chapter, you will be in
a position to carry out similar reasoning processes in the exercises at the
end.
Finally, we o¤er a brief observation about the general nature of linguis-
tics. To many linguists the ultimate aim of linguistics is not simply to un-
derstand how language itself is structured and how it functions. We hope
that as we come to understand more about human language, we will cor-
respondingly understand more about the processes of human thought. In
this view the study of language is ultimately the study of the human mind.
This goal is perhaps best expressed by Noam Chomsky in his book
Reflections on Language (1975, 3–4):
Why study language? There are many possible answers, and by focusing on some
I do not, of course, mean to disparage others or question their legitimacy. One
may, for example, simply be fascinated by the elements of language in themselves
and want to discover their order and arrangement, their origin in history or in the
11 What Is Linguistics?
individual, or the ways in which they are used in thought, in science or in art, or in
normal social interchange. One reason for studying language—and for me per-
sonally the most compelling reason—is that it is tempting to regard language, in
the traditional phrase, as ‘‘a mirror of mind.’’ I do not mean by this simply that
the concepts expressed and distinctions developed in normal language use give us
insight into the patterns of thought and the world of ‘‘common sense’’ constructed
by the human mind. More intriguing, to me at least, is the possibility that by
studying language we may discover abstract principles that govern its structure
and use, principles that are universal by biological necessity and not mere histori-
cal accident, that derive from mental characteristics of the species. A human lan-
guage is a system of remarkable complexity. To come to know a human language
would be an extraordinary intellectual achievement for a creature not specifically
designed to accomplish this task. A normal child acquires this knowledge on rela-
tively slight exposure and without specific training. He can then quite e¤ortlessly
make use of an intricate structure of specific rules and guiding principles to con-
vey his thoughts and feelings to others, arousing in them novel ideas and subtle
perceptions and judgments. For the conscious mind, not specifically designed for
the purpose, it remains a distant goal to reconstruct and comprehend what the
child has done intuitively and with minimal e¤ort. Thus language is a mirror of
mind in a deep and significant sense. It is a product of human intelligence, created
anew in each individual by operations that lie far beyond the reach of will or
consciousness.
Bibliography