Grow & LeBlanc (2013)
Grow & LeBlanc (2013)
net/publication/273065328
CITATIONS READS
8 205
2 authors, including:
Laura L Grow
California State University, Fresno
17 PUBLICATIONS 202 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All in-text references underlined in blue are linked to publications on ResearchGate, Available from: Laura L Grow
letting you access and read them immediately. Retrieved on: 13 August 2016
Teaching Receptive Language Skills:
Recommendations for Instructors
Laura Grow
University of British Columbia
Linda LeBlanc
Trumpet Behavioral Health
Keywords: autism, developmental disabilities, early intervention, instructional strategies, listener behavior,
receptive language
56 RECOMMENDATIONS FOR TEACHING RECEPTIVE LANGUAGE Behavior Analysis in Practice, 6(1), 56-75
Receptive language refers to responding appropriately to another person’s spoken language.
Most curricula dedicate a proportion of early intervention to developing receptive
language skills. The specific terms used to refer to the receptive language programs
and the recommendations for teaching such skills vary considerably across the early
intervention curricula. The present paper will provide a conceptual analysis of the desired
controlling variables for different receptive language programs, teaching recommendations,
a brief review of the literature to substantiate the teaching recommendations, and a
discussion of the potential negative effects of deviating from the recommendations.
Young children quickly learn to respond to the spoken bias (e.g., select the option on the right most of the time
language of other people (i.e., receptive language) as regardless of the task) that may hinder learning and
they begin to orient to their own name and familiar lead to problem behavior (Green, 2001). The resulting
voices, follow simple instructions, and identify a wide problems with stimulus control and escape-maintained
range of stimuli and events in their environment (Hart problem behavior could interfere with acquisition in
& Risley, 1995; Lovaas, 1977). Hundreds of everyday other skill areas as well. It is critically important to use
interactions with caregivers readily produce receptive optimal procedures for teaching receptive language skills
language skills as children learn to select pictures in a from the very beginning of EIBI programming to en-
book when an adult says the name of an item or to find sure that effective patterns of responding are established.
the right color during reading and play activities. When
Common Terms and Conceptualization
basic receptive language skills are not readily acquired, a
child misses many important learning opportunities re- Most EIBI curricula and conceptual models refer to
sulting in delays in overall development and subsequent the overall repertoire of responding to another’s spoken
acquisition of spoken language (Drash & Tudor, 1993; language as receptive language (Leaf & McEachin, 1999;
Hart & Risley, 1992; Lovaas, 1977). Lovaas, 2003, Sundberg & Partington, 1998), although
Learners with developmental disabilities require the term listener behavior is also used (Barbera, 2007;
a carefully engineered environment to learn how to Skinner, 1957). The first receptive skills targeted during
respond effectively to the language of others (e.g., fol- early intervention involve responding to basic instruc-
low instructions, identify objects by name, orient when tions (e.g., “Come here” and “Clap your hands”), one’s
called) (Drash & Tudor, 1993; Lovaas, 1977). Thus, own name, and the names of common items. Later tar-
many of the initial programs in early intensive behav- gets in the receptive language domain include following
ioral intervention (EIBI) curricula are designed to teach multiple-step instructions, writing letters and numbers
learners to respond to their name or simple directions upon request, and identifying items based on abstract
followed by training in literally hundreds of other recep- features (e.g., relative size).
tive language targets (e.g., identifying objects by name, Table 1 displays a sampling of receptive language
feature, function; Lovaas, 2003; Smith, 2001; Sundberg programs recommended by several well-recognized
& Partington, 1998). If the instructional procedures for early intervention curricula (Barbera, 2007; Leaf &
teaching receptive language skills are not optimal, sev- McEachin, 1999, Lovaas, 2003, Sundberg & Par-
eral problems may emerge that can slow the rate of skill tington, 1998). The terms for the different programs
acquisition in EIBI (Schilmoeller, Schilmoeller, Etzel, & vary somewhat across curricula, but similar targets are
LeBlanc, 1979). For example, simple errors in arranging displayed across rows while the programs are generally
instructional materials can inadvertently establish a side arranged from basic to advanced skills in a top-bottom
fashion. Regardless of the specific name of the program, Examples of programs requiring an auditory-visual con-
it is important to conceptualize the skills based on the ditional discrimination are identifying items based on
type of discrimination required and the desired stimu- their name, basic features (e.g., red, has a tail), compara-
lus control. Receptive language programs that involve tive features (e.g., bigger, higher), classes (e.g., foods,
simple discriminations can be described as a 3-term toys), and functions (e.g., draw with, clean with). The
contingency: (1) an auditory instruction (discriminative reader may want to use Appendix A, a list of technical
stimulus), (2) a particular nonvocal response (behavior), terms and definitions used in the paper, as a reference.
and (3) the delivery of reinforcers, usually in the form Recent literature reviews and experimental labora-
of praise and tangible items (consequence). Educational tory studies provide findings that should inform the
programs that teach simple discriminations include instructional procedures used to teach receptive lan-
teaching a learner to respond to their name, follow basic guage in school and clinic settings (e.g., Green, 2001;
instructions, write letters, numbers, and shapes, and to Grow, Carr, Kodak, Jostad, & Kisamore, 2011; Gutier-
follow many other action-specific requests. Programs rez et al., 2009; Holcombe, Wolery, & Snyder, 1994;
that involve an auditory conditional discrimination Rodgers & Iwata, 1991). The purpose of the paper is to
can be described as a 4-term contingency: (1) an ar- provide five overarching best practice recommendations
ray of comparison stimuli (discriminative stimuli and for receptive language instruction. Each general recom-
incorrect comparison stimuli) with (2) a corresponding mendation is accompanied by a conceptual analysis, the
auditory instruction that occasions the (3) selection of specific practice parameters, a review of the experimen-
the appropriate picture/object from the array (behavior) tal literature, and descriptions of common problematic
that is followed by (4) a reinforcer (consequence). The response patterns that may emerge from deviating from
important difference between the two types of discrimi- the practice recommendations.
nations is that the auditory instruction (i.e., the sample
stimulus) establishes one of the comparison stimuli as Recommendation 1:
the correct one (i.e., discriminative stimulus) and the Require an Observing Response
others as distracters (i.e., S-deltas) at that particular An observing response is emitted by a learner before
moment. Each of the comparison stimuli functions as or during a training trial and results in sensory contact
the discriminative stimulus or distracter on a given trial. with the discriminative stimulus. A differential observ-
If these performance patterns develop, instructors et al., 1979). If those particular targets were arbitrary
should conduct an analysis of the potential source(s) of (e.g., “Touch your feet,” “Find the hippopotamus”), the
stimulus control that might have been established based instructor might simply alter the programming to teach
on the program components (e.g., prompting strate- new targets optimally. If the targets were critical for
gies, arrangement of stimuli) and develop strategies to long-term functioning (e.g., “Stop,” recognizing their
remediate the faulty stimulus control (see Table 2). For own name), the stimulus control must be systematically
example, an instructor may suspect that inadvertent reprogrammed by reintroducing the stimuli under op-
small movements are evoking correct responses dur- timal programming conditions, using errorless learning
ing a receptive instructions program resulting in false procedures and eliminating the erroneous stimulus fea-
mastery. To test whether this faulty stimulus control tures that are currently controlling responding. For ex-
is a problem, a different instructor could conduct the ample, an instructor selects two similar targets in terms
teaching session and accuracy across instructors could of the antecedent stimulus (e.g., “Stand up,” “Stomp
be compared. Alternatively, if the instructor has been feet”), and the learner develops faulty stimulus control.
arranging the visual stimuli in front of the learner and During trials, the learner is responding before the in-
suspects that some placement cue could be occasioning structor presents the entire auditory instruction and en-
correct selection responses, the instructor could switch gages in “switching” responses (i.e., rapidly alternating
to preparing the stimuli out of sight of the learner. If the between stomping feet and standing up and down). The
learner is tracking the correct stimulus by observing the instructor could pick one of the targets and include it in
instructor prepare the stimuli, changes should occur in a new training set with other stimuli that have dissimilar
the accuracy of responding. auditory instructions. A progressive prompt delay could
If there are specific targets that have already been be used to promote accurate responding during trials. If
inadvertently trained under faulty sources of stimu- the learner engages in vocal verbal behavior, the instruc-
lus control, the instructor should decide whether the tor might consider prompting the learner to engage in
specific targets are worthy of targeting again as the echoic behavior following the auditory instruction to
effort required to eliminate faulty stimulus control and increase the saliency of the auditory instruction and
program new control may be substantial (Schilmoeller promote observing.
Comparison stimuli: the visual array of stimuli presented during auditory-visual and visual-
visual conditional discrimination training. The function of the comparison stimuli (i.e., a
discriminative stimulus or S-delta) is altered by the presentation of the auditory or visual
sample stimulus.
Counterbalancing: a method of arranging the antecedent stimuli such that each of the
stimuli are presented proportionally across sessions. The presentation of the auditory
instruction should be counterbalanced across sessions in which simple discriminations
are taught. The presentation of the auditory instruction and the comparison array should
be counterbalanced for auditory-visual conditional discrimination training.
Differential observing response: the learner engages in a unique observing response for each
sample stimulus.
Distracter stimulus: a stimulus in the array of comparison stimuli that functions as the
incorrect response (i.e., S-delta) during a given trial.
Faulty stimulus control: learner responses that are evoked by irrelevant or a restricted range of
antecedent stimuli.
Generalized imitation repertoire: the learner can reliably engage in imitation behavior when
presented with novel, untrained imitation tasks.
Mass trials: the instructor teaches one relation in isolation within a teaching session.
Observing response: a learner response that results in sensory contact with the sample
stimulus.
Response prompts: the instructor supplements the behavior of a learner by proving gestural,
model, or physical prompts to increase the likelihood of a correct response.
Sample stimulus: the first part of the contingency for conditional discrimination training.
The sample stimulus alters the function of the comparison stimuli.
Sequential method: teaching a set of relations as simple discriminations before targeting the
relations as conditional discriminations.
Stimulus control: the presence or absence of antecedent stimuli influences the emission of
behavior.
Stimulus overselectivity: a type of faulty stimulus control in which responses are evoked by
only a subset of features in a multi-component antecedent stimulus.