Introduction to Linguistics Study Guide
Introduction to Linguistics Study Guide
Anna Gartsman
Recommended Citation
Hughes, Laura E. and Gartsman, Anna, "Introduction to linguistics study guide" (2007). Honors Junior/Senior Projects. Paper 23.
http://hdl.handle.net/2047/d10006552
2
Phonetics
Phonetics is the study of speech sounds.
Lessons
The International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA)
Overview & Tips
IPA Example Chart: Consonants
IPA Example Chart: Vowels
IPA Transcription Exercises
Natural Classes
Overview, Examples & Tips
Consonant Chart
Vowel Chart
Exercises
Features
Overview, Examples & Tips
Example Problems
List of Distinctive Features (Consonants)
List of Distinctive Features (Vowels)
Distinctive Feature Chart (Consonants)
Distinctive Feature Chart (Vowels)
Exercises
3
The International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA)
Objective: Transcribe English speech sounds in the international phonetic alphabet.
What is the international phonetic alphabet (IPA), and why do we need one?
The International Phonetic Alphabet is a way of transcribing speech as it’s pronounced, which
means it can be used to transcribe any language (so long as there are symbols for the sounds
spoken in that language). Because there is a one-to-one correspondence between symbols and
sounds in IPA, there are no problems with weird, ambiguous spelling, silent letters, etc. such as
we often find in written English.
In phonetics and phonology, we use IPA to represent data sets of sounds from different
languages. Using words written in IPA, we can focus on the sounds of a language without having
to learn the writing systems of different languages—many of which don’t have any writing
system at all, or have one that isn’t based on sounds (such as Chinese).
You can use your textbook or handouts or the charts provided here to learn the IPA symbols
used for English consonants and vowels. Since many languages have sounds we don’t use in
English, you may be introduced to more symbols in later problem sets; but these symbols are all
you need to start transcribing English speech.
Transcription Tips:
• Sounds Not Spelling When transcribing words into the IPA, focus on the sounds, not the
way that they’re spelled. Say the word aloud.
• Normal (Fast) Speech Be careful not to say the word too slowly and carefully, because
that may change some of the sounds. The idea is to transcribe the way you usually
pronounce the word in normal speech.
• IPA Symbols Are Not the Same As Letters An IPA symbol may look like an English
letter, but represent a different sound than that letter normally does. This is especially true
of vowels. Note, for example, that [e] is the vowel sound in “say” or “weigh”, not in
“bed” (that would be []).
• Ignore Silent Letters Many English words have silent letters, the “e” at the end of
“cape,” for example. Remember, the difference between “cap” and “cape” doesn’t have
to do with the “e”—it’s a different vowel between [k] and [p] (that is, [kæp] vs. [kep]).
• Your Pronunciation May Vary Even within English, people with different dialects may
pronounce words differently. (Again, this is especially true of vowels!) There may be
more than one way to transcribe a word, but there is only one way to transcribe the word
the way you say it.
• Use Your Friends If you’re worried that your accent or dialect is too non-standard, or if
you can’t tell what sound you’re saying, ask someone else to say the word. If you’re
worried about “priming” them to pronounce the word the way you do, write down the
word and have them read it to you.
The charts on the following pages give examples of the English consonants and vowels. We
transcribed the words into IPA using our own dialect of Standard American English; your
mileage may vary. (The symbols in parentheses show alternative symbols for the same sound.)
4
Phonetic Alphabet Chart: Consonants of English
5
Phonetic Alphabet Chart: Vowels of English
i superhero suprhiro
invisibility nvzbli
e cape kep
incredible nkrdbl
æ Batman bæmæn
Buffy bfi
ability bli
a Robin rabn
u Superman suprmæn
Doctor Octopus daktr aktps
o Rogue ro
law l
aj ( a ) kryptonite krptnajt
aw ( a ) powers pawrz
j ( ) Superboy suprbj
6
IPA Transcription Exercises
I. Consonants
1. Write the IPA symbol for the first sound in these words:
sniffles ___ bronchitis ___
cold ___ pneumonia ___
flu ___ death ___
2. Write the IPA symbol for the last sound in these words:
rash ___ concussion ___
lacerations ___ hemorrhage ___
fracture ___ death ___
3. Write the IPA symbol for the highlighted sound in these words:
alcoholism ___ hemophilia ___
diabetes ___ cancer ___
hypothyroidism ___ chronic death... syndrome___
II. Vowels
1. Write the IPA symbol for the first sound in these words (just the first sound of the first
word is fine if it’s a phrase):
obesity ___ amnesia ___
epilepsy ___ aphasia ___
autism ___ attention deficit disorder___
2. Write the IPA symbol for the last sound in these words (just the last sound of the last
word is fine if it’s a phrase):
dystrophy ___ sciatica ___
polio ___ punched in the jaw ___
3. Write the IPA symbol for the highlighted sound in these words:
borderline ___ schizoid ___
bipolar ___ paranoid ___
obsessive compulsive ___ antisocial ___
7
2. Transcribe the following into IPA:
Do no harm.
A few days with a cooperative test subject could be worth months of theoretical research.
maj hajpolajsimi z nat mædnd. aj wz dstktli tld æt aj hæd bld r prablm
V. Going Beyond
1. If you know another language, what (if any) sounds in that language cannot be represented
using the symbols you now know?
2. Here’s a passage from another dialect of English. Try transcribing into standard orthography.
Can you guess what kind of dialect this is? ( is a new vowel sound. To tell what it sounds
like, try saying the word “nurse” without the “r”.)
fst rul v dntks: sprd dinz pa, j no! bt rojlz a bsst w,
“a ju rojl fæmli? a ju rojl mmb? wl n ju kn mæri mi kz j sem din pul,
nd a aj kjuz wl o dawn tojlt.” fæntæstk! ts waj e a no krezi rojlz,
e dst kajnd v, “hlo! hlo, wt d ju du? o, j plm! wt an z æt?” i
8
Natural Classes
Objective: Define a natural class. Given a natural class, identify the sounds that belong to it.
Given a set of consonants or vowels, identify what class, if any, they belong to.
Examples
• The set of sounds {p, t, k} is in a natural class of “voiceless stops”.
• The set of sounds {n, , } do not comprise a natural class because there is no natural
class that will include all these sounds and only these sounds. One could say that they are
voiced stops, and this would be correct, except that would also include /d/, /b/, and /m/,
which are not part of the set.
• The set of sounds {p, b, m, n} do not comprise a natural class because while /p/, /b/, and
/m/ are all bilabial stops, /n/ is not, and the natural class needs to include all the sounds in
the given set.
Tips:
• Look at the charts (next page). When analyzing whether a set of consonants comprises
a natural class, a good place to start is to look at the IPA chart and see if the sounds all fit
into a particular row (manner of articulation) or column (place of articulation). For
example, {m, n, } are all nasals, and {k, , } are all velar sounds.
• Don’t forget about voicing. Voiceless sounds are shaded in the consonant chart
provided. All vowels are voiced.
• Look at combinations. If there is no clear natural class in the manner or place of
articulation, try looking at a combination of the two. For example, {f,v} are labiodental
fricatives – “labiodental” has to be mentioned in order to exclude other fricatives, such as
// and //.
• Each sound is a natural class. For example, {p} is the only member of the natural class
voiceless bilabial stops.
• Vowels have a different set of descriptors than consonants. Vowels are distinguished
from each other by frontness (front, central, or back), height (high, mid, or low),
tenseness (tense or lax), and roundness (round or non-round). Check the chart for details.
All vowels are voiced.
9
IPA English Consonant Chart: Manner of Articulation, Place of Articulation, Voicing*
Place of Articulation
Bilabial Labio- Inter- Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
dental dental
Stop p t k
b d
Manner of Articulation
Nasal m n
Fricative f s h
v z
Affricate t
d
Approximant w r j
Lateral l
Approximant
* Shaded sounds are voiceless
,
Low
æ a
* Shaded sounds are tense
**Bold sounds are round
10
Natural Class Exercises
I. List all the sounds in each class.
a. Voiceless f. Mid
b. Fricative g. Round
c. Velar h. Low back non-round
d. Alveolar stop i. High tense
e. Velar nasal
II. Determine whether the following sets of sounds are in a natural class. If so, what is the class?
a. {f, , s, , h} e. {u, , o}
b. {z, t, d} f. {æ, a, }
c. {p, b, m, w} g. {i, , }
d. {b, d, , } h. {, , }
11
Features
Objective: Describe speech sounds in terms of their features.
Contents:
Overview
Consonant Feature Chart
Vowel Feature Chart
Example Problems
Exercises
Distinctive features are features which differentiate sounds from each other. Whether or not a
feature is distinctive depends on the set of sounds that one is describing. For example, if one
wants to differentiate [t] from [d], there is only one distinctive feature: [t] is {–voice}, and [d] is
{+voice}. These two sounds have all other features in common, so they are not distinctive
features in this case.
Another aspect of distinctive features is redundancy – there are instances where one feature is a
subset of another feature (for example, all {+nasal} sounds are also {+consonantal}). The idea is
to use the narrowest feature possible: use {+nasal} instead of {+consonantal} to describe [n],
because the feature {+nasal} is narrower in that it describes fewer sounds, and is therefore more
descriptive. Once {+nasal} is used, there is no need to indicate that the sound is also
{+consonantal} – that would be redundant, since all {+nasal} sounds are also {+consonantal}.
Describing sounds in features is similar to describing sounds in the terms of a natural class, but it
is not identical. It is helpful to use the natural classes as a guide, but not all natural classes
correspond to features (for example, “voiceless” is a natural class, but not a feature: that would
be indicated by {-voice}).
12
List of Distinctive Features (Consonants)
Feature - Feature Included Sounds Definition
consonantal nonconsonantal Consonants (not vowels, also Consonants.
not /w/ or /j/).
sonorant obstruent Nasals, approximants. Equal air pressure inside &
outside the vocal tract.
syllabic nonsyllabic Vowels and sometimes Can be the nucleus of a syllable.
approximants
voice voiceless Vowels, voiced consonants Vocal chord vibration
continuant stop Fricatives, approximants. Continuous passage of air
(Not nasals!) through the mouth.
nasal oral {m, n, } Air passes through the nose.
sibilant nonsibilant {s, z, , , t, d} Jet of air through narrow
passage toward obstacle (teeth)
lateral rhotic {l} but not {r} Air flows along sides of the
tongue
labial non-labial {p, b, m, f, v, w} Use of lips
alveolar non-alveolar {t, d, n, s, z, l, r} Use of alveolar ridge
palatal non-palatal {, , t, d, j} Use of hard palate
velar non-velar {k, , } Use of velum
anterior posterior Labials, dentals, alveolars Produced forward of the
alveolar ridge.
coronal noncoronal Dentals, alveolars, palatals. Use of tip and/or blade of the
tongue.
13
Distinctive Feature Chart (Consonants)
Feature p b t d k m n f v s z h t d r l w j
consonantal + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + - -
sonorant - - - - - - - + + + - - - - - - - - - - - + + + +
syllabic - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - +/- +/- +/- +/-
voice - + - + - + - + + + - + - + - + - + - - + + + + +
continuant - - - - - - - - - - + + + + + + + + + - - + + + +
nasal - - - - - - - + + + - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -
sibilant - - - - - - - - - - - - - - + + + + - + + - - - -
lateral - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - + - -
labial + + - - - - - + - - + + - - - - - - - - - - - + -
alveolar - - + + - - - - + - - - - - + + - - - - - + + - -
palatal - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - + + - + + - - - +
velar - - - - + + - - - + - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -
anterior + + + + - - - + + - + + + + + + - - - - - + + + -
coronal - - + + - - - - + - - - + + + + + + - + + + + - -
14
Example Problems:
2. What list of features describe(s) this/these sound(s)? Make sure there are no redundant
features, and the sound is described uniquely (no other sound is described by this list of features).
a. [f]
The place feature is a good place to start; [f] is {+labial}. We need to distinguish
it from the other {+labial} sounds [p], [b], [m], [v], and [w]. Adding
{+continuant} gets rid of the stops [p], [b], and [m], so we only need to deal with
[v] and [w]. Both are {+voice} while [f] is {-voice}, so there’s our third feature.
Our complete list is {+labial, +continuant, -voice}.
b. [r]
[r] is {+alveolar}. We need to distinguish it from the other alveolar sounds, [t],
[d], [n], [s], [z] and [l]. A nice way to get rid of everything except [l] is to add
{+sonorant}. Now we just have to distinguish between [r] and [l]. The only
difference is that [l] is {+lateral}, so we add that [r] is {-lateral}. The complete list
is {+alveolar, +sonorant, -lateral}.
Another way we could have gone instead of using {+sonorant} is to get of
[t], [d] and [n] by adding {+continuant} to our list of features. Now we need to
deal with [s], [z], and [l]. [s] and [z] are {+sibilant}, so we add {-sibilant} to our
list. Now we still need to distinguish between [r] and [l], so we add{-lateral}. Our
complete list is {+alveolar, +continuant, -sibilant, -lateral}. This is also correct,
but less efficient, because it requires four features instead of three.
15
c. {k, }
[k] and [] are both {+velar}; the only other {+velar} sound is []. To get rid of
[] we just have to say {-nasal}. Hooray, the features are {+velar, -nasal}.
d. {u, }
You can start by saying that they are {+high}. Now you only need to differential
them from {i, I}. Either {+back} or {+rounded} will do this. Since there are no
high, back, non-round vowels and no high, rounded, non-back vowels, either way
will work equally well. So there are two correct answers: {+high, +back} or
{+high, +round}.
3. What are the distinctive features that differentiate these two sounds?
a. {, }
This is one of those many pairs where the only difference is that [] is {+voice},
and [] is {-voice}. So the feature involved here is {+/- voice}.
b. {s, }
The difference between these is place; [s] is alveolar and [] is palatal. There are
features for both, so you can either say that the difference is {+/- alveolar} or {+/-
palatal}.
c. {, }
If you look at the feature chart, you can see that the only difference between these
two is that [] is {+round}. So {+/- round} is the relevant feature.
4. What are the distinctive features that differentiate these two sets of sounds?
a. {t, d} & {s, z}
To show the difference between these two sets, we need to describe each set
individually. {t, d} are {-continuant, +alveolar, -nasal}. {s, z} are {+continuant,
+alveolar, -sonorant}. The distinctive feature is {+/- continuant}: both sets are
+alveolar, and even though it is unnecessary to state this for {t, d}, both sets are
also {-sonorant}.
` b. {i, } & {u, }
{i, } are {-back, +high}. {u, } are {+back, +high}. The distinctive feature is
{+/- back}, since both sets of vowels are {+high}.
16
Exercises
17
18
Phonology
A phoneme is a distinct speech sound in a language.
Lessons
Minimal Pairs
Overview
Example Problems
Exercises
Phonemes and Allophones
Definitions
Explanation
Summary
Complementary & Contrastive Distribution
Definitions
Explanation
Summary
Environments
Overview
Step-by-Step Example
Example Problems
Exercises
Determining the Underlying Phoneme
Overview
Example Problems
Exercises
Rules
Overview
Example Problems
Reference: Rule-writing Conventions
Exercises
Analyzing Data Sets
Overview
Example Problems
Exercises
Phonological Processes
Overview
Example Problems
Exercises
19
Minimal Pairs
Objective: To identify minimal pairs.
Minimal pairs are pairs of words with different meanings and exactly one sound difference. For
example, “cat” and “bat” are minimal pairs because only the first sound is different ([k] vs [b]).
However, “cat” and “flat” are not minimal pairs, because there are two sound differences: ([k] vs
[f] and [l].)
The reason we look for minimal pairs is to identify a contrast between two sounds. If two
different sounds, placed in the same exact environment, produce different words with different
meanings, then those sounds really are different phonemes. See the “Phonemes vs. Allophones”
lesson in this section for more information.
Remember, the restrictions on minimal pairs are:
Tips
• If the words are in English, they will probably be given to you in standard orthography.
Remember, DO NOT RELY ON SPELLING! Write the words in IPA. Then you just
have to compare the symbols. For example, in standard orthography it looks like
“rewind” and “resigned” must have more than one sound change, but if you write them in
IPA (rwajnd, rzajnd) you will see that they are actually minimal pairs (Again, your
dialect may vary.)
• If the words are in another language, you will be given the transcription and a gloss (what
the word means). So, you don’t have to worry about trying to do a transcription into IPA.
Example Problems
b. cab, cash
IPA transcriptions: kæb, kæ
These are minimal pairs. You’re comparing [b] and [].
20
2. Consider this data set from another language, and determine whether the given pairs are
minimal pairs or not.
a. Tagalog: kahon “box”
kaon “to fetch”
These two words have different meanings and one sound change (between “h” and “”).
They are a minimal pair.
Exercises
2. Consider this data set from Thai, and determine whether the given pairs are minimal pairs
or not.
a. ba (“sheet”), pa (“to go”)
b. bryy (“extremely fast”), myy (“hand”)
c. pa (“to go”), pa (“danger”)
21
Phonemes vs. Allophones
Objective: To distinguish phonemes from allophones.
Definitions
So far, we’ve been describing speech sounds. Now we’re going to distinguish between two types
of speech sounds: phonemes and allophones.
Phoneme: A speech sound that is distinct from other sounds in the language. Changing a
phoneme changes the meaning of a word.
Explanation
There is a lot of variation in the way that sounds are produced. For example, someone with a
very high voice saying [k] creates quite a different sound wave than someone with a very low
voice saying [k]. Yet we still recognize the two different sounds as being the same; they’re the
same phoneme. There’s any number of individual or random variations that affect the quality of
the sound, but which don’t affect us as listeners and perceivers of language. We’re wired to
process phonemes, and string them into meaningful words in our minds, without noticing
unimportant differences in how they sound from one speaker or moment to the next. What
differences count as unimportant? Well, it depends on the language.
22
spa, spa, spa”). (If it doesn’t, you may not have this difference with your particular dialect or
accent. Don’t worry—someone in your class will have it, and you don’t need to have the
difference to understand what it is.) The two kinds of [p] are really different sounds: one has an
additional puff of air, which is called aspiration. Aspirated [p] like you find at the beginning of a
word is written as [p], whereas the unaspirated [p] like you find in “spot” is written just as
regular old [p]. You can find aspiration with every voiceless stop in English (so, [t] and [k] also).
The candle trick also works with “stop” and “top,” or “cab” and “scab,” for example.)
Now, in English, these are not different phonemes. If you really try, you can get aspiration on
a voiceless stop after [s] (try saying [stap]) or produce an unaspirated word-initial voiceless stop
(try saying [tap])—it sounds weird to native speakers, but it doesn’t change the meaning of the
word.
There are some languages in which aspiration is a phonemic difference, though. For example,
in the minimal pair exercise above, the Thai words pa (“to go”) and pa (“danger”) constitute a
minimal pair which contrasts aspirated and unaspirated [p]. Since the meaning of the word
changes depending on which [p] is used, aspiration is a phonemic difference in Thai.
Psycholinguistic Research
How can phonemic distinction different cross-linguistically? Aren’t sounds like [p] and [b]
fundamentally different? In some ways, the distinctions we draw between different sounds are
arbitrary. Take a pair like [r] and [l]. By slowing moving your tongue forward and back, you can
switch continuously between the two sounds. They can be thought of not so much as two
separate, concrete entities, but as ends of a continuum. Voiced/voiceless pairs are also like that.
Suppose someone is making a bilabial stop followed by [a] (so, “pa” or “ba”). Because of the
vowel, the speaker will have to start voicing at some point. Whether the listener hears a [p] or a
[b] depends on the voice onset time. If voicing starts early, the listener will hear “ba”; if voicing
starts late, they will hear “pa”. But if the voicing starts at some in-between time, they have to
make a judgment call.
The extent to which various sounds seem distinct varies from language to language. It
may sound like [r] and [l] are very different because we are used to them being different;
however, people who speak languages that don’t treat [r] and [l] as different phonemes find that
they sound very similar. In contrast, it may seem to native English speakers that [p] and [p]
sound very similar, but to speakers of Thai, they sound entirely different.
Summary
• Even though there may be systematically different ways of pronouncing some sound (that
is, different allophones of some phoneme), speakers of a language recognize the
difference between different phonemes and usually fail to notice the difference between
different allophones.
• Whether a pair of sounds represents two different phonemes or allophone variants of the
same phoneme depends on the language.
23
Complementary and Contrastive Distribution
Objective: Distinguish between contrastive and complementary distribution. Know which one
indicates phonemes, and which indicates allophones.
Definitions
Environment: For these simple examples, the environment is simply the sounds before
and after the ones we are looking at.
Contrastive Distribution: Sounds that are in contrastive distribution can be found in the
same environment. The sounds constrast and therefore they are different phonemes.
Explanation
We know that different phonemes can appear in the same surrounding word and create different
meanings (minimal pairs). We also know that when there are different allophone variants of a
phoneme, the environment determines which one appears (for example, in standard English,
[pH] is always pronounced at the beginning of a word, never [p]). We can use this knowledge to
determine whether the difference between a pair of sounds is phonemic or allophonic in a given
language. If the two sounds can appear in the same environment (that is, with the same
surrounding sounds), then they are in constrastive distribution and they are different phonemes.
On the other hand, if the two sounds always appear in different environments—if there’s a
systematic way to tell when to use one sound and when to use the other—then they’re in
complementary distribution and they’re allophones of the same phoneme.
Summary
• Phonemes are found in contrastive distribution (same environment)
• Allophones are found in complementary distribution (different environments)
24
Environments
Objective: Make charts showing the environments to compare two sounds, and make a decision
about complementary vs. contrastive distribution. If complementary, decide which is the
underlying phoneme.
Overview
In order to determine if a pair of sounds represents different phonemes or different allophones,
we need to know whether they are in contrastive or complementary distribution. And in order to
determine that distribution, we need to carefully look at the environments in which the sounds
are found. If the sounds can be found in the same environment—if both sounds could come
before and after the same surrounding sounds—then they’re in contrastive distribution and
they’re phonemes. But if we can find some systematic difference in the environments, then we’re
looking at complementary distribution (allophones).
If there are minimal pairs that contrast the sounds you’re looking at, you know right away that
the sounds are different phonemes. If there aren’t, though, you need to carefully look at the
sounds before and after. One way to clarify the problem is to draw charts of the immediate
environments of each sound.
Representing Environment
The environment of a sound means the sounds that come immediately before and after it. By
convention, we write this as [preceding sound]_[following sound]. For example, the environment
of [k] in the word [bækt] would be written s_.
Environment-Writing Tips:
• Word boundaries—the beginning or end of a word—are represented by the symbol #.
The environment of the [k] in [luk] (the name “Luke”) would be written u_#.
• A diphthong counts as one sound, so the environment for [k] in [skaj] is s_aj.
• Notice that a single word can have the letter of interest multiple times. For example,
[luk skajwkr] has three separate environments for [k]:
u_# s_aj _
2. Compare each side of one list to the corresponding side of the other list, looking for overlap.
Overlap means the same sounds appear in the same position (before or after the sound of
interest) on both lists.
If there is overlap on both sides, we can conclude the sounds are in contrastive distribution
because they could, theoretically, appear in the exact same environment. There’s no
difference between the environments in which one sound occurs vs. the other sound, so the
sounds themselves contrast. In other words, they’re two different phonemes.
25
If there is no overlap on one or both of the sides (i.e. there is no over lap on either side OR
either the right side or left side has no overlap, although the other may certainly have
overlap), we can conclude the sounds are in complementary distribution, because they occur
in systematically different environments. In other words, they are two allophone versions of
the same underlying phoneme. Later sections will show how to tell which is the underlying
phoneme and what rule governs the use of the allophone.
Example Problems
1. Consider this data from Tongan, a Polynesian language. Are [s] and [t] different
phonemes, or allophones of the same phoneme?
First off, we can check for minimal pairs, since that’s the easiest way to tell that the two sounds
are phonemes. There are none, so we can’t conclude anything yet. We need to figure out if the
sounds are in complementary or contrastive distribution.
s t
#_i #_a
i_i o_u
o_i o_o
o_i o_o
e_i #_o
#_i o_o
a_i a_a
i_i e_e
Note that since we care about variability, there’s really no point in listing all the repeated
environments in each list. So we could write the list like this:
s t
#_i #_a
i_i o_u
o_i o_o
e_i #_o
a_i a_a
e_e
26
In the rest of the examples we present, we won’t write repeat environments. For the purposes of
comparing the lists, it doesn’t matter whether you do or not. Just make sure, if you get rid of
them, that you do it separately for each list—so if the same environment is in both lists, you
definitely want to leave that.
Step 2. Compare each side of one list to that side of the other list, looking for overlap.
Let’s look at the lists we made in the previous step. First, we will compare the left-hand side of
each environment. (We re-wrote the lists one side at a time for this purpose, but you can do this
visually in a number of ways--covering up the side you’re not looking at, circling the side you
are looking at, etc.)
There’s definitely overlap between these two lists: we see [o], [e], [a] and word boundaries (#)
on both lists.
What does this mean? Well, if we find overlap on the right-hand side as well, then the sounds
are in contrastive distribution (and are phonemes). However, if there is no overlap on the right-
hand side, then we can conclude that they are in complementary distribution, and it’s the sound
on the right-hand side that causes the change from one allophone to the other. So let’s check out
the right-hand side.
s t
i a
i u
i o
i o
i a
e
Absolutely no overlap. The list for [s] consists only of [i], and there are no [i]s on the list for [t].
Since we found no overlap on one side, we can conclude that the two sounds are in
complementary distribution. They’re allophones of the same phoneme.
27
2. Using the same data set from the previous problem (reprinted below), are [m] and [t]
different phonemes, or allophones of the same phoneme?
First, we check for minimal pairs, since that’s the easiest way to tell that the two sounds are
phonemes. There are none, so we can’t conclude anything yet. We need to figure out if the
sounds are in complementary or contrastive distribution.
t m
#_a #_o
o_u i_o
o_o o_o
#_o #_i
a_a
e_e
Step 2. Compare each side of one list to that side of the other list, looking for overlap.
There’s overlap: [o] and word boundaries (#) occur on both lists.
What does this mean? Well, if we find overlap on the right-hand side as well, then the sounds
are in contrastive distribution (and are phonemes). However, if there is no overlap on the right-
hand side, then we can conclude that they are in complementary distribution, and it’s the sound
on the right-hand side that causes the change from one allophone to the other. So let’s check out
the right-hand side.
28
Right-hand side comparison
t m
a o
u o
o o
o i
a
e
There’s overlap on this side, too—[o] occurs on both lists. Since we see overlap on both sides,
the sounds are in contrastive distribution, and we can conclude that [t] and [m] are different
phonemes in this language.
Exercises
1. Consider the following data from English (use the IPA transcriptions, not the standard
orthography glosses.) According to this data, are [] and [] allophones of the same phoneme, or
different phonemes?
2. Consider the following data from English. According to this data, are [n] and [] allophones of
the same phoneme, or different phonemes?
3. Consider the following data from Ganda. According to this data, are [l] and [r] allophones of
the same phoneme, or different phonemes? iv
29
wulira ‘hear’ effirimbi ‘whistle’
beera ‘help’ emmeeri ‘ship’
jjukira ‘remember’ eraddu ‘lightning’
eryato ‘canoe’ wawaabira ‘accuse’
omuliro ‘fire’ laira ‘command’
Going Beyond
In English, are [n] and [] allophones of the same phoneme, or different phonemes? Give data to
support your answer. Does your answer support or conflict with your answer to exercise #2? If
there is a conflict, how might you explain it?
30
Determining the Underlying Phoneme
Objective: Given two sounds in complementary distribution and their environments, determine
which sound is the underlying phoneme.
Overview
The previous section, “Environments,” described how to decide if two sounds are in contrastive
(phonemic) or complementary (allophonic) distribution. If we find that two sounds are different
phonemes, then that’s about all we can say about them, but if they are two allophones of the
same phoneme, there are still some questions to resolve. One important question is: which is the
underlying phoneme? The basic answer to that question is the sound that is in wider distribution.
Wider distribution means the sound could occur in more different environments.
To determine distribution, we look at the environment lists—specifically, the side with no
overlap. Which list is more variable?
Example Problems
1. This is the right-hand (no-overlap) side of the environments in example #1 (Tongan data) of
the previous section (“Environments”). Which sound is in wider distribution?
s t
i a
i u
i o
i o
i a
e
This is a very clear-cut case. [t] can occur before all sorts of vowels, and [s] can only occur
before [i]. Therefore, [t] is in wider distribution and is the underlying phoneme.
2. Compare the following environments. Which sound is in wider distribution, [s] or [z]?
s z
#_ _
f_ a_
t_ _
h_ i_
p_ o_
m_
In this case, even though [z] looks like it appears with more sounds, all of these sounds can be
combined in one category, namely “voiced” sounds (remember that all vowels are voiced). There
is no way to group the environment of the [s] sound (because there is no way to group the word
31
edge with a sound), so [s] is the underlying phoneme, and [z] is the allophone of [s] that occurs
in voiced environments.
Exercises
For exercises 2 and 3 of the previous section (“Environments”), state which sound is the
underlying phoneme. They are reproduced below for convenience:
1. Consider the following data from English. According to this data, which sound of [n] and []
is the underlying phoneme?
2. Consider the following data from Ganda. According to this data, which sound of [l] and [r] is
the underlying phoneme?
Going Beyond
1. Explain why we didn’t ask for the underlying phoneme of the first exercise from that section.
2. Consider the Going Beyond question of the previous section (“Environments”): “In English,
are [n] and [] allophones of the same phoneme, or different phonemes? Give data to support
your answer. Does your answer support or conflict with your answer to exercise #2? If there is a
conflict, how might you explain it?” Does considering the answer to the question provide you
with more data which supports or contradicts the decision to label either [n] or [] as the
underlying phoneme?
32
Rules
Overview
When we find that a pair of sounds constitutes allophones of the same phoneme, the next
question is “When do we use which sound?” The general rule is that you start with the basic
phoneme, and it turns into a specific allophone version of that phoneme in certain situations. For
example, you might say, “/p/ becomes [p] at the beginning of a word,” or more generally,
“voiceless stops become aspirated at the beginning of a word.” Formally, these rules would be
written as follows:
Symbol Notation:
/p/ [p] / #_
/p/ becomes [p] when it follows a word boundary (i.e. at the beginning of a word)
Feature Notation:
[-voice, -continuant] [+aspirated] / #_
You can see from the examples that the rules take this form:
where the arrow ( ) means “becomes” and the slash (/) means “where/when”. So some
underlying phoneme becomes some specific allophone when some environment occurs. Your job
when writing a rule is to figure out:
• What’s the underlying phoneme and what’s the allophone, and
• What environment triggers the use of the allophone
Feature Notation
Often, it makes more sense to use feature notation than symbol notation for rules. This allows
you to see precisely what is changing. For example, if /k/ is showing up as [g] in a certain
environment, you know right away that the change is in voicing, since [g] is simply the voiced
version of [k].
33
Example Problems
1. In the “Environments” section, example problem #1, we looked at data from Tongan, and
decided that [s] and [t] were in complementary distribution. Here are the environments:
s t
#_i #_a
i_i o_u
o_i o_o
e_i #_o
a_i a_a
e_e
Write a rule in symbol notation which describes when each allophone occurs.
Remember that in “Determining the Underlying Phoneme,” example problem #1, we decided
that [t] was the underlying phoneme since it occurs in more variable environments (before [a],
[u], [o], or [e], while [s] only occurs before [i]). Since [t] is the underlying phoneme, the rule will
look like this:
What environment do we want to put there? Well, we want to say when [s] occurs. Looking at
the data, it’s pretty clear that [s] only occurs before [i]. So that’s what we write.
/t/ [s] / _i
How would we write the rule for this example in feature notation? We’ll have to consult our old
phonetics charts to figure out the relevant features of [t], [s], and [i].
Note that when we write the rule, we only have to include those features of allophone [s] that are
different from the features of the underlying phoneme /t/.
2. The English plural “-s” can be pronounced as either [s] or [z] depending on the context. Here’s
some data:
34
Write the rule (in feature notation) for when [s] appears and when [z] appears.
s z
t_# v_#
k_# n_#
_# l_#
i_#
We’re concerned with the sound on the left (obviously, since the right side of both lists is
composed entirely of word boundaries.) Because [z] occurs with both vowels and consonants and
[s] occurs only with consonants, [z] appears to be in wider distribution. Therefore we could write
the rule in symbol notation as follows:
Now it’s just a matter of converting the rule to feature notation. First of all, what feature, if any,
is common to {t, k, } but not {v, n, l, i}? Consulting our chart, we see that {t, k, } are all
unvoiced. Interestingly enough, voicing also accounts for the difference between [z] and [s].
Exercises
Consider the exercises in the previous section (“Determining the Underlying Phoneme”). Can
you write the rule for these exercises? Remember to write in both symbol and feature notation.
1. Consider the following data from English. According to this data, what is the rule that
predicts the occurrence of the [n] vs. the [] sound?
35
2. Consider the following data from Ganda. According to this data, what is the rule that predicts
the occurrence of the [r] vs. the [l] sound?
Going Beyond
1. In example problem 2, when we developed a rule for plural pronunciation, we decided [z] was
the basic underlying phoneme. Given what the rule turned out to be, what’s an alternate
explanation for why [z] is in wider distribution than [s]? Could [z] and [s] actually be equally
ranked (neither one more “underlying” than the other, or you can’t tell)?
36
Rule-writing Conventions
Symbols To Know
/X/ X is a phoneme (underlying sound)
[Y] Y is an allophone (actual pronunciation in this environment)
-> “becomes”; “shows up as”
/ “when”; “where”; “in the environment of”
_ placeholder for the allophone, showing its location in the environment
C consonants
V vowels
Ø null/nothing
# word boundary (can be used for the beginning or end of a word)
$ syllable boundary
37
Analyzing Data Sets for Phonemic and Allophonic Distinctions
Objective: Given a data set, identify whether a pair of sounds represent different phonemes or
allophones of the same phoneme in that language. If different allophones, write the rule.
Step-by-Step Guide
Each one of these steps is described in detail a previous section.
Question:
Consider the following data from Daga. Are [s] and [t] allophones of the same phoneme or of
separate phonemes?
jamosivin ‘I’m licking’ simura ‘whisper’
jamotain ‘they will lick’ otu ‘little’
asi ‘grunt’ topen ‘hit’
anet ‘we should go’ use ‘there’
senao ‘shout’ tave ‘old’
urase ‘hole’ siuran ‘salt’
sinao ‘drum’ tuian ‘I kill’
wagat ‘holiday’ v
s t
o_i o_a
a_i e_#
#_e a_#
a_e o_u
#_i #_o
u_e #_a
#_u
38
ALLOPHONES ONLY: Step 4: Write the rule
We need to fill in a rule of the form
/underlying phoneme/ [allophone version] / environment
Right away, we can see that [t] is in wider distribution because while [s] only comes before
certain vowels, [t] comes before certain vowels and word boundaries.
Symbol Notation
/t/ [s] / _ [i, e]
“/t/ becomes [s] before [i] or [e]”
For many rules, especially those with multiple sounds, it will make more sense to write at least
part of the rule in feature notation. What’s another way we can say “[i] or [e]”? Consult your
vowel chart. What do they have in common (that [a], [o], and [u] do not have in common)?
We can also put the whole rule into feature notation by describing [t] and [s] in terms of their
features. First we must uniquely describe [t], then we can describe [s] in terms only of the
feature(s) that make it distinct from [t].
(Note that because we are working with another language, there may be additional
sounds/features to take into account. These will usually be provided for you if they are relevant.
For the purposes of this problem, we will assume the features we developed for English are
sufficient.)
39
Another Example Question
Consider the following data set from Tongan. Are [m] and [t] different phonemes, or allophones
of the same phoneme (if they are allophones, provide the rule)?
t e_e
#_a m
o_u #_o
o_o i_o
#_o o_o
a_a #_i
Exercises
1. Consider the following data from English. According to this data, are [n] and [] phonemes or
allophones (if allophones, provide the rule in symbol and feature notations):
40
2. Consider the following data from English (use the IPA transcriptions, not the standard
orthography glosses.) According to this data, are [] and [] allophones of the same phoneme, or
different phonemes (if allophones, provide the rule in symbol and feature notations)?
bo “both” ej “they”
oz “those” lo “loathe”
rær “rather” s “this”
n “thin” mæ “math”
farz “fathers”
k “think”
3. Consider the following data from Ganda. According to this data, are [r] and [l] phonemes or
allophones (if allophones, provide the rule in symbol and feature notations):
kola ‘do’ beera ‘help’
lwana ‘fight’ jjukira ‘remember’
buulira ‘tell’ eryato ‘canoe’
lya ‘eat’ omuliro ‘fire’
luula ‘sit’ effirimbi ‘whistle’
omuole ‘bride’ emmeeri ‘ship’
lumonde ‘sweet potato’ eraddu ‘lightning’
eddwaliro ‘hospital’ wawaabira ‘accuse’
oluanda ‘Ganda language’ laira ‘command’
olulimi ‘tongue’
wulira ‘hear’
4. Consider this data from Tongan, a Polynesian language. Are [s] and [t] different phonemes or
allophones of the same phoneme (if allophones, provide the rule in symbol and feature
notations)?
tauhi “to take care”
sisi “garland”
motu “island”
mosimosi “to drizzle”
motomoto “unripe”
fesi “to break”
sino “body”
totonu “correct”
pasi “to clap”
fata “shelf”
movete “to come apart”
misi “to dream” vii
41
Going Beyond
You may run into a problem at a certain point in this data set.
Consider this data from Tojolabal. Are [t] and [t] separate phonemes or allophones of the same
phoneme? If allophones, what rule specifies their distribution?
Note: The ejective /t’/ is a separate phoneme and therefore will be considered as just
another sound, not relevant to solving this problem. viii (Also, remember that /t/ is one sound!)
42
Phonological Processes
Objective: Given a rule, determine what, if any, phonological process is at work.
Therefore, in an assimilation rule, the surface sound (allophone) and environment will have some
feature in common which the underlying sound lacks.
In a dissimilation rule, the underlying phoneme and the environment will share some feature
which the surface sound lacks.
Ø -> X / Y _ Z
“X is inserted between Y and Z”
Examples
1. Consider the following words in English (pronounce them together as you would in normal
speech):
“fast task”
“camp fire”
“first grade”
“second semester”
43
You may notice that some consonants are dropped (some may not be, depending on dialect). So,
a lot of people pronounce these words in the following manner:
What happens here is that the medial consonant in a cluster ([t] or [d] in this case) gets deleted in
surface pronunciation. This is the process of deletion. The rule for this would be:
/t, d/ Ø / C _ C
What happens to the English words when they are borrowed into Spanish? It looks as if they are
made to comply with the Spanish standard rules of pronunciation (which require an initial “e” []
before a sC cluster). So, an “e” is inserted to comply with Spanish pronunciation. This is the
process of epenthesis. The rule for this would be:
Ø []/ #_sC
3. Consider the following words from English (pay attention to the vowels):
jab jam
sag sang
dad Dan
You may notice that the vowel [æ] in the first column of words is slightly different from the
same vowel [æ] in the second column. The [æ] in the second column is nasalized. The reason for
the nasalization becomes clear when we look at the environments. The environment on the left
side is the same, so the difference must be on the right. In the first column, the sounds on the
right are all voiced oral stops; in the second column, the sounds are all nasal stops. So, the vowel
assimilates to its environment (becomes nasalized when it’s next to a nasal sound):
/æ/ [+ nasal] / _ [+nasal] C
44
4. Consider the following data from Kitharaka ix ( is a voiced velar fricative):
What happens when a [ke] or [ka] prefix is added to a word that starts with a voiceless sound?
The [k] in the prefix becomes voiced, so that it is different from its environment. Notice here that
the vowel between the two consonants does not seem to play a role. This is sometimes the case
with phonological data sets – sometimes, the environment that triggers a change is not the
immediate environment. So, in this case of dissimilation, the rule is:
/k/ [+ voice] / _ V [-voice]C
Here, it is the voicing that is important, so we ignore, in partial feature notation, the change from
–continuant to +continuant that also occurs.
Exercises
1. Consider the following data from a dialect of English sometimes spoken in Eastern
Massachusetts (“:” next to a vowel indicates a long vowel); what phonological process could
account for the transformation from underlying to surface form? Provide the rule:
2. Consider the following data from English; what differences in pronunciation do you find
among the basic word and the word with an attached suffix? Pay specific attention to the last
sound in the root word. What phonological process could account for the transformation?
Provide the rule:
45
3. Consider the following data from English; keep in mind that all of these words have the same
underlying prefix: “con-”. What phonological process could account for the transformation
from underlying to surface form of the prefix “con-”? Provide the rule:
conjoin commerce
contraction compose
contemplate comply
conspire combine
configure
conclude
4. Consider the following data from English; keep in mind that all of these words have the same
underlying suffix: “-al”. What phonological process could account for the transformation
from underlying to surface form of the suffix “-al”? Provide the rule:
Noun Adjective Noun Adjective
person personal pole polar
region regional circle circular
autumn autumnal single singular
cause causal uvula uvular
46
Morphology
A morpheme is the smallest unit of meaning.
Lessons
47
Basic Lexical Categories (Noun, Verb, Adjective, Adverb)
For morphology and syntax, it is vital to be familiar with these lexical categories. (In syntax, we
also discuss prepositions, determiners, qualifiers, quantifiers, auxiliaries, and complementizers—
for that discussion, see “Lexical Categories” in the Syntax section.)
Nouns Typically defined as “person, place, or thing.” Feelings and ideas are also nouns.
Examples: John, genius, Atlantis, space ship, computer, regret, sadness, loyalty, milk, noun
48
Morphology Terms and Definitions
Objectives: To separate English words into the morphemes that compose them. To identify the
root and the affixes. To identify whether an English morpheme is free or bound, and if bound,
whether inflectional or derivational.
Morpheme
A morpheme is the smallest unit in language which holds meaning.
• A basic word like “cat” is a morpheme – the individual parts of the word (the sounds /k/,
/æ/, /t/) don’t have any meaning of their own.
• A word like “unsinkable” can be broken down into three morphemes: “un-“ which means
“not”; “sink” which means, well, “sink”; and “-able” which means “able to be...”
Bound morphemes cannot stand alone as their own word but, instead, join to other words.
English Examples: un-, -ing, -able, re-, de-, -ist, -er, -s (plural)
Inflectional morphemes denote number, gender, tense, or case. (They don’t really change
the meaning of the root word, except to make it plural, past tense, etc.)
49
Roots
Every word has a single root—a free morpheme onto which other, bound morphemes may be
attached.
• Exception: Compound words (like “pancake” or “blackboard”) have two roots (free
morphemes).
• Bound Roots: There are certain words which clearly have morphology, but which don’t
have a free root. (For example, “antagonist” and “antagonize” have the “-ize” and “-ist”
suffixes as we know them, and they are certainly related in meaning, so it seems
senseless to consider them separate, monomorphemic words; but the problem with
separating them into “antagon” and “-ist” or “antagon” and “-ize” is that “antagon”
cannot stand alone.) In our example problems, every word should have a free root, but if
your teacher gives problems like this, and you need to find out how they are to be
handled in your class.
Affixes
An affix is any morpheme that is added onto a root—in other words, any bound morpheme is an
affix. In English, we have two kinds of affixes:
Infixes: Inserted in the middle of the root. In English we actually do have one infix, but only one:
forms of the word “fucking,” as in “abso-fucking-lutely!”
• Note: Don’t make the mistake of identifying an affix as an infix just because it is neither
at the beginning nor the end of the word. In “activation” (act+ive+ate+tion), “-ate” is
NOT an infix—it’s just the first suffix. Remember, infixes only occur when the root is
actually broken into two pieces to surround the morpheme.
Circumfixes: Single morphemes that are broken into two parts and placed on either side of a root.
There is no example of this in English.
• Note: Don’t make the mistake of identifying a pair of different morphemes as a
circumfix. A word like “unreliable” (un+rely+able) may have a root surrounded by
morphemes on either side, but “un...able” is not a circumfix—they’re two different
morphemes which can be separated, mixed, and matched.
50
Subcategorization Frames
Objective: Write subcategorization frames for various derivational morphemes.
Overview
A subcategorization frame tells you what lexical category a morpheme takes, and what lexical
category it produces.
For example, Take the suffix “-ize,” found in words like “prioritize,” “energize,” and
“ionize.” All of these are verbs, so we know that the suffix “-ize” takes words and turns them
into verbs. What kind of words does it take? Well, look at the roots this suffix is acting on:
“Priority,” “energy,” and “ion” are all nouns. From this evidence, we see that “-ize” takes nouns
and turns them into verbs. So the subcategorization frame looks like this:
-ize: N -> V
Step-by-Step
Suppose we’re trying to find the subcategorization frame for the suffix –tion.
1. Brainstorm some words that use this morpheme. I came up with “creation,” “action”,
“retention,” and “station.”
2. Break down these examples, removing the morpheme. Here, we realize we need to throw out
“station” since it seems to be a free morpheme on its own, not “sta + tion”. (What’s a sta?) The
rest look like this:
3. Identify the lexical categories of the words, with and without the morpheme, and compare.
“Create,” “act” and “retain” are all verbs; “creation”, “action” and “retention” are all nouns. So –
tion takes a verb, and turns it into a noun. The subcategorization frame looks like this:
Tips
• Some morphemes have more than one meaning (and therefore, more than one
subcategorization frame). Much like words like “bat” have multiple meanings, many
morphemes look and sound the same, but are really different, and may have different
subcategorization frames. There are, for example, two kinds of “un-” and two kinds of “-
er” (see example problems).
• Roots might change spelling (or even pronunciation) when a morpheme is added.
You might have to do a little thinking to realize that, for example, the root of “detention”
is “detain.”
• Be aware of the meaning of your morpheme (and choose evidence accordingly). For
example, “underwear” can’t be used as evidence for the subcategorization frame of “un-”
51
as that is not the same “un-” that mans “not”: “underwear” doesn’t mean “not derwear.”
Similarly, although there is a word “bell,” “rebel” isn’t good evidence for “re-” since “re-
“ means “again” and “rebel” doesn’t mean “bell again.”
Example Problems
3. Give subcategorization frames for the following English morphemes. Support your answer
with evidence (words using that morpheme). Note: Some of these are intentionally tricky.
a. un-
Think of words that start with “un-”. First off, you have adjectives like “unhappy” or
“unambitious.” Then you have verbs like “undo” or “unwrap.” Notice that the adjectives are
all of the form un+adjective, and the verbs are all of the form un+verb.
Like many morphemes, “un-” actually has two subcategorization frames.
(There are some words, like “under,” which do start with “un,” but if you think about it,
that’s not the morpheme “un-”. You can’t split up “under” into “un” and “der” – “der”
doesn’t mean anything on its own. “Under” is a single, free morpheme.)
b. re-
What words start with “re”? I can think of verbs like “reheat” and “remodel.”
I can also think of a handful of other verbs, like “revere,” “rescue,” and “repair,” but I
won’t use these because I don’t think they are using the morpheme “re.” You can tell
“revere” and “rescue” are monomorphemic (they have one morpheme) because they can’t be
broken down: there’s no word “vere” or “scue.”
There is a word “pair,” but think about it: “re” means “again.” “Repair” doesn’t mean
“pair again.” Actually, the word “pair” and the word “repair” have nothing to do with each
other; it’s just a coincidental similarity. “Repair” is monomorphemic too!
So, we’ll just go with those “do again” verbs: “reheat” meaning “heat again,” and
“remodel” meaning “model again”. We know our subcategorization frame looks like this so
far: “re: ?? -> V.”
What are those words that “re” is attaching to? “Heat” and “model” could be nouns or
verbs. Hmm. More evidence might clarify this. What are some other “re-” verbs? “Redesign”
(also could be noun or verb); “recreate” (ooh, that’s a verb); “relocate” (another verb!) Looks
like “re-” attaches to verbs.
c. –er
“-er” can either go at the end of adjectives, like in “happier” or “smarter”—this is the
inflectional comparative. It can also be used to make words like “painter” or “writer”, to turn
a verb X into a noun meaning “person who does X”.
52
There is no subcategorization frame for inflectional morphemes, since they don’t change
the meaning or lexical category of the root. So the only subcategorization frame we need to
worry about is the one for the derivational, “person-who-does” “-er”.
1. Give subcategorization frames for the following English morphemes, giving your evidence.
(If you cheat and use a chart of common morphemes, the angels will weep for you.)
a. –s
b. –ify
c. -ly
53
Common Derivational Morphemes Chart
54
Morphology Trees
Overview: Draw morphology trees.
Overview
Morphology trees are diagrams used to illustrate:
(1) what are the individual morphemes in a given word,
(2) what order the affixes were added to the root,
(3) what each affix is doing to the word—that is, the subcategorization frame of the
morpheme, and the lexical category of each word along the way.
Step-by-Step
Suppose we’re trying to do a morphology tree for the word “activation.”
1. Divide the word into all its component morphemes. For “activation”, that’s:
2. Give the lexical category of the root, and the subcategorization frames of the morphemes. (If
you can’t do them all now… you may be able to fill them in later.) The root is “act,” which
is a verb. So we’ll label “act” with its lexical category, and write the subcategorization
frames off to the side.
V
act + ive + ate + tion
3. Connect the root with first morpheme that’s attached, and label with the lexical category of
the new word. We only have one option for which morpheme is attached first, because there
is only one morpheme next to the root! (For more complicated cases, see “How do I
tell…?”) The new word, “active,” is an adjective.
Adj
V
act + ive + ate + tion
“Activate” is a verb:
55
V
Adj
V
act + ive + ate + tion
Adj
V
act + ive + ate + tion
Subcategorization frames:
-ive: V->Adj
-ate: Adj->V
-tion: V->N
Generally, the morphemes have to connect in order: in the above example, “activation” is not
formed by combining “act” and “-ate” first, for instance. However, what happens when there are
two possible morphemes that can connect next?
Let’s take, for example, the word “unbelievable”. This word is composed of three morphemes:
un + believe + able. We know that believe is a verb, so we can start making the tree:
un: V V
un: Adj Adj
able: V Adj (“doable”, “imaginable”)
V
un + believe + able
However, here we run into a problem. Does “un-” connect first, or does “-able”? There is a
shortcut that can be used here because one of these morphemes connects to the root to make a
word, and one of them does not. That is, “believable” is a word, but “unbelieve” is not.
Therefore, we know that “-able” connects first.
56
However, it sometimes happens that both options create valid words. In that case, we need to
look at the subcategorization frames. Let’s take the word “rewriteable” as an example. It has the
following structure:
V
re + write + able
We know that “write” is a verb, but “rewrite” and “writeable” are both words, so the shortcut
from above doesn’t work here. We need to look at the subcategorization frames: “re-” takes a
verb and produces a verb; “-able” takes a verb and makes an adjective. We know that
“rewritable” is an adjective, so the end result has to be an adjective. The only frame that
produces an adjective is “-able”, so we know that “-able” has to connect last. Alternately, we can
try to connect “-able” first, and see that “writeable” is an adjective, but “re-” can’t attach to
adjectives, so we are unable to attach the “re-” morpheme. So, the tree for “rewritable” looks like
this:
Adj
V
re + write + able
Example Problems
1. Make morphology trees for the following words:
a. revitalize
First, we break the word into its component morphemes: re + vital + ize. (Although –al is
sometimes a separate morpheme, “vital” cannot be broken down further: “vit” alone is
meaningless.) Vital is an adjective, so we can go ahead and label that.
Now we need to decide what morpheme is added first: “re-” or “–ize”. I’m not sure if I’ve
ever heard the word “vitalize,” but “revital” is definitely not a word. It wouldn’t even work with
the subcategorization frames: “re-” takes verbs, and “vital” is an adjective. On the other hand, “-
ize” takes adjectives, and makes them into verbs. So we know “-ize” needs to be added first,
forming a verb (“vitalize”) and then “re-” gets added onto that. The whole word remains a verb.
The tree looks like this:
Adj
re + vital + ize
57
re-: V -> V
-ize: Adj -> V
b. indefensible
First, break the word into its component morphemes: in + defend + ible. (“-ible” is a variant of “-
able”.) The tricky thing is that this breakdown relies on the intuition that the root is “defend” and
not “defense”. Either one is possible given the spelling (the changing of /d/ to [s] could be one of
those phonological changes that sometimes occur as a result of adding morphology). I chose the
verb “defend” rather than the noun “defense” because the meaning of the word is “not able to be
defended”.
Now, choose which morpheme to add first. “Indefend” (or “indefense”) is not a word, but
“defensible” is. Furthermore, “-able” or “-ible” take a verb and turn it into an adjective, and “in-”
must be added to an adjective. So we know “-ible” gets added first, then “in-” is added to the
adjective “defensible.”
Adj
Adj
V
in + defend + ible
c. furtive
Although this word looks like it has morphology (the “-ive”), it really can’t be broken down any
more than it already is—“furt” cannot stand on its own. Since it is monomorphemic, there’s
really no tree—all you can do is label the word with its lexical category and call it a day.
Adj
furtive
d. misunderstandings
The morpheme breakdown:
“Understand” looks like two free morphemes (a compound, perhaps) but since the meaning of
“understand” doesn’t have anything do with the meanings of “under” or “stand”, we’ll treat it
like a single, non-compound, free root. Furthermore, we know that “understand” is a verb, so we
can go ahead and label that right away.
58
Now, what order do we add the morphemes in? Recall that “-s” is inflectional (the plural
morpheme for nouns) and that inflectional morphemes always get added last. So we know that
much.
Next question: Is this “-ing” the inflectional “-ing” or the derivational “-ing”? If it’s
inflectional, then the whole word “misunderstanding” will be a verb (since inflectional “-ing”
always works on verbs). If it’s derivational, then the whole word will be a noun (since
derivational “-ing” turns verbs into nouns). Actually, the word “misunderstanding” could go
either way (a noun in sentences like “It was all a big misunderstanding” and a verb in sentences
like “She was misunderstanding everything I said.”) But the word “misunderstandings” must be
a noun, since only nouns can be plural (in other words, “-s” is N -> N). So in this case,
“misunderstanding” is a noun and “-ing” is derivational.
So we know that “misunderstandings” is going to be a noun, and “misunderstanding” is
also going to be a noun. Now we just need to decide what to add to the root first: the “-ing” (V-
>N) or the “mis-” (V->V). Both take verbs, so either one could work on “understand”. But if we
add “-ing” first, we’ll get a noun, and “mis-” can’t work on a noun. On the other hand, if we add
“mis-” first, we’ll still have a verb, and “-ing” can work on it. So the only thing we can do is add
“mis-” first and then add “-ing.”
To sum up: we start with “understand”, a verb; add “mis-” and we have “misunderstand”,
still a verb; add “-ing” and we have “misunderstanding”, a noun; and finally, add the inflectional
plural “-s” and we have “misunderstandings”, a noun.
V
mis + understand + ing + s
e. unlockable
The morpheme breakdown:
un + lock + able
Here, it is crucial to realize that “unlockable” has two meanings. One meaning is “not able to be
locked”, as in “The lock is broken, so this door is unlockable”. The other meaning is “able to be
unlocked” as in “That door has a flimsy lock, it’s unlockable with a credit card”. Each meaning
has a different tree structure, but both trees have a lot in common: both versions of the word are
adjectives, both have “lock” as the root (and we know “lock” has to be a verb and not a noun
because “-able” does not attach to nouns, and neither does “un-”). The difference is in the
attachment of the morphemes. One of the meanings corresponds to “un-” attaching first, the
other meaning corresponds to “-able” attaching first.
59
Let’s draw the tree with “un-” attaching first:
Adj
V
un + lock + able
Which meaning does this correspond to? We make the word “unlock” first, and then attach “-
able”. “-able” attaches to verb X, and makes an adjective that means “able to be Xed”, so this
structure means “able to be unlocked”, which is the second meaning discussed above.
Adj
Adj
V
un + lock + able
In this structure, we make “lockable” first, and then attach “un-” which, when attached to an
adjective X, means “not X”. So, this structure makes “not lockable”, which is the first meaning
discussed above.
Exercises
Make morphology trees for the following words:
a. unmentionable
b. revelations
c. undeniably
d. untieable
e. deactivating
60
Zero Derivation
Overview
You know that “-ed” and “-ing” are inflectional morphemes which assign tense to verbs, as in “I
waited for you all day, but you never arrived!” But what about, “This is the most anticipated film
of the year”? What lexical category do you assign to “anticipated” in this context? How can you
explain this?
One way to explain the adjectival “anticipated” in “the most anticipated film of the year” is to
suggest that this is a new, derivational “-ed” which transforms verbs into adjectives. But there is
an alternative explanation, which is that a new, non-pronounced (or null, written as Ø) morpheme
is being added to the word. Ø can take any subcategorization frame you please.
Adj
V
anticipate + ed + Ø (as in “the most anticipated film of the year”)
Adding a null morpheme, or zero derivation, is the favored explanation because the practice of
using a word in a different part of speech than usual is so widespread in English. Consider the
following three sentences, where the word “baby” changes its lexical category without any
attachment of morphology:
• He held the baby awkwardly. (noun)
• The baby mice were adorable. (adj)
• Her mother likes to baby her. (verb)
Example Problems
1. Show how to derive the adjective and noun versions of “-ing” words, as in “the
slumbering giant” or “Swimming is my favorite activity.”
Adj
V
slumber + ing + Ø (as in “the slumbering giant”)
61
N
V
swim + ing + Ø (as in “Swimming is my favorite activity”)
-ing: inflectional verb tense marker
Ø: V N
2. Show how to derive the past tense verb “monkeyed” as in “The children monkeyed
around” from the noun “monkey.”
N
monkey + Ø + ed
Ø: N V
-ed: inflectional verb tense marker
Notice that, unlike in the previous examples, we added the Ø morpheme before the other
morphology. How do you tell what order to add the morphemes? Use the same skills you know
from previous morphology problems: pay attention to the subcategorization frames and the
meanings of the words.
Ø can take and produce any lexical category, but the other morphemes still have their
usual constraints. As an inflectional morpheme, “-ed” takes and produces verbs. So it would be
impossible to add “-ed” to a noun like “monkey” without first turning “monkey” into a verb
using Ø.
This makes intuitive sense if you think about the meaning of the words. “Monkeyed” is
the past tense of the verb “to monkey”, derived from the noun “monkey.” (Interesting,
synonymous phrases with “to monkey around”—like “to fool around” and “to horse around”—
also depend on zero derivation from nouns!)
62
3. Show the derivation of the word “heatedly”.
If you assume that “heat” is a verb, you can add “-ed” to make it past tense, and then zero-derive
it into an adjective in order to add “-ly”, which turns adjectives into adverbs.
Adv
Adj
V
heat + ed + Ø + ly
You could also start out by assuming “heat” is a noun, in which case this word would have two
instances of zero derivation.
Adv
Adj
N
heat + Ø1 + ed + Ø2 + ly
Is “heat” actually a noun or a verb? It’s unclear which originated first, so you could go either
way.
Tips
• The null morpheme Ø can take and produce any lexical category. Assign it whatever
subcategorization frame you need to get through the problem.
• Ø can be added after inflectional morphology. Normally, inflectional morphology is
added last (after all derivational morphology), but Ø may be added after inflectional
morphology (and then more morphology may be added after that).
63
• The original or underlying lexical category of a given word isn’t always clear.
Remember the word “heat” from the examples--it might originally be a noun or a verb. If
you can manage to consult one, an etymology dictionary may resolve the question. When
in doubt, you may zero-derive or just assume that the word originally has the lexical
category that would make life easiest for you; as always, your mileage (and your
professor’s) may vary.
Exercises
1. Business jargon provides a rich source of zero derived words. Provide tree structures for
the following words:
a. “actioning” as in, “I’ll talk to Todd in accounting and see about actioning that for
you.”
b. “table” as in “I think we should table this discussion for now.”
2. Provide tree structures for the first two words in the sentence “Verbing weirds language.”
64
Morphology in Other Languages
We can divide languages into the following groups according to how they handle morphology:
khi tôi dên nhà ban tôi, chúng tôi bát dâu làm bài.
Agglutinative: A sequence of morphemes are added in a specific order, and each morpheme
represents one unit of meaning. Agglutinating languages tend to be very regular (with some
exceptions, e.g. Georgian).
xi
Example: Turkish
Turkish adds morphemes (mainly suffixes) to words, in a specific order, to indicate
person, number, aspect, and other features of language:
ev house
evin your house
evinde at your house
Evindeyim I am at your house
65
xii
MFL 1
What appear to be the distinctions that are identified in the English glosses? First, there are three
different roots (“see”, “eat”, “write”). There are several tenses (present, past, future), and
pronouns that sometimes differentiate between genders (“I”, “He”, “You”, “She”, “We”, “They
(fem)”, “They (masc)”). There are also direct objects (“her”, “them (fem, masc, inanimate)”,
“it”). So, let’s find all the corresponding morphemes.
To do this, we need to analyze the MFL phrases that differ in only one element (do you recall
“minimal pairs” from phonetics/phonology? The principle is the same). So, for example, the only
difference between “I ate” and “I ate them (fem)” is the presence of the direct object “them
(fem)”. So, we can compare the MFL phrases “tixoki” and “tixokir”. The only difference
between them is the final “r”, so we can assume that the direct object “them (fem) is indicated by
the morpheme “r”.
Now, we can try to find the root word for “eat”. Look at “I ate” and then “She eats”. There are
two differences between these phrases: one difference is the pronoun, and the other is the tense
of the verb. So, when we compare the two phrases, the root is what they have in common:
“tixoki” ad “tixoa” have “tixo” in common, so that is probably the root.
Next, let’s look at the first two examples. There, the root is probably “kura” (for “see”) because
that is what those two phrases have in common. Further, we can compare “he saw her” with “I
ate”, since we know both of the roots. “kuratix” and “tixoki” have one thing in common (if the
roots are disregarded), namely the “i”. Since both of these phrases are in the past tense, and there
is no other similarity, we can conclude that “i” indicates past tense.
66
We can continue in this way until all the distinctions are identified, and we can also judge what
order they are added to the words:
I k
You (sg) l
He t
She
We m
They (masc) f
They (fem) n
Present tense a
Past tense i
Future tense o
Her x
It
Them (masc) z
Them (fem) r
Them (inan.)
Negation æ
See kura
Write ela
Eat tixo
67
The order of the suffixes (all the affixes attach after the root) is as follows:
root + subject + tense + object + negation (mood)
Tips
• You can start at any point. You can start examining these puzzles from comparing
almost any two phrases. If the first two you choose don’t seem to give you any
information, pick another two.
• Don’t judge by English. Just because English has or does not have a particular feature,
does not tell you anything about the language that you are analyzing. For example, as
above, English does not differentiate between masculine and feminine “they” (e.g. a
group of men vs. a group of women) but this language does.
• Sometimes similarities are misleading. In these puzzles, it is often possible to make a
false assumption. The key is to keep track of all assumptions you make about a specific
morpheme, and to be able to go back and modify the assignments if you run into a
problem upon further analysis.
• Sometimes, a particular feature is not indicated by a morpheme. If you cannot find a
morpheme that corresponds to a specific feature, that feature is probably indicated by the
lack of a morpheme, or the null (ø) morpheme.
68
Exercise:
MFL 2
Find the following morphemes in the data set below:
Subjects Objects Tense Aspect Roots
I present progressive (-ing) like
we us past perfect (have done) draw
you (sg) future wear
you (pl) you (pl)
he him
she her
they them
it
69
Word Formation Processes
New words are created all the time. Here are some of the processes that account for the coining
of new words. The “<” symbol means “comes from”.
Process Examples
adding morphology A new word is created by adding undead < un- + dead
morphology to an existing word. slayage < slay + -age
embiggen < en- + big + -en
backformation A new word is created when people take pea < pease – -s
a word that appears to have been created by adding resurrect < resurrection – -tion
morphology, and try to get at the root by removing the
morphology. Happens due to a misanalysis of the origin
of the original word.
compounding Two words are combined, retaining the weblog < web + log
entirety of both words.
blending Two words are combined, and some smog < smoke + fog
intermediate portions of both words are dropped. Bennifer < Ben + Jennifer
clipping A word is shortened. lab < laboratory
blog < weblog
initialism (or alphabetism) A sequence of initials used as ATM < automatic teller machine
a word (the individual letters are pronounced). ID < identification
acronym A sequence of initials used as a word (sounded NASA < National Aeronautics and
out phonetically rather than saying individual letters). Space Administrations
laser < Light Amplification by
Stimulated Emission of Radiation
generification People come to use a brand name to Kleenex (meaning any tissue) <
describe any similar product. Kleenex (meaning Kleenex brand
tissue)
lexical shift (or zero derivation) Words that used to be to Xerox (V) < (a) Xerox (N)
one lexical cateogory are used in another, without adding to blog/blogging (V) < (a) blog (N)
morphology. The phrase “zero derivation” refers to the
idea that a null (unpronounced) derivational morpheme is
being added to the root to change its lexical category.
If you like this, you’ll love “Processes of Semantic Change” in the section on Semantics.
70
Syntax
Syntax is the study of sentence structure.
Lessons
Constituency
Overview
Constituency Tests
Example Problems
Exercises
Lexical Categories
Overview
Lexical Categories Quick Reference
Example Problems
Exercises
English Phrase Structure & Syntax Trees
Overview
English Phrase Structure Rules
Abstract Phrase Structure Rule
Key Points about Phrase Structure Rules
Tree Drawing Overview
Examples
Example Problems
Tips
Exercises
Ambiguous Sentences
Overview
Example Problems
Exercises
Complex Sentences
Overview
Example Problems
Exercises
Transformations
Overview
Example Problems
Tips
Exercises
71
Constituency
Objective: Decide whether an indicated group of words forms a constituent using the tests of
substitution, question/answer, cleft, and pseudocleft.
Overview
A constituent is a word or group of words in a sentence that functions as a unit. That is, it can be
taken out of the sentence, or moved, or replaced with other words, and these actions do not
change the grammaticality of the sentence. These constituents are also called “phrases” (See:
Syntax: Phrases).
Constituency tests are operations that can be performed on a group of words to see if it is a
constituent. There are many of these tests; we will cover four of them. Generally, if two of the
tests are passed, the group of words is a constituent. If three of the tests fail, then it is not a
constituent.
Constituency Tests
1. Replacement/Substitution: replace the group of words with another word or phrase. For
example:
a. The pretty girl put the bowl on the counter.
The pretty girl can be replaced with “she”: She put the bowl on the counter. The
pretty girl passes this constituency test. Pronoun-substitution is generally used for
a noun and any adjectives that describe it (noun phrases).
b. The pretty girl put the bowl on the counter.
On the counter can be replaced with “there”: She put the bowl there. On the
counter passes this constituency test. “There” is generally used to substitute for a
location (prepositional phrases).
c. The pretty girl put the bowl on the counter.
Put the bowl on the counter can be replaced with “did so”: The pretty girl put the
bowl on the counter. The ugly girl did so too. Put the bowl on the counter passes
this constituency test. “Did so” is generally used to substitute for actions (verb
phrases).
2. Question/Answer: form a question from the given sentence such that the group of words
being tested is the answer. For example:
a. The pretty girl put the bowl on the counter.
The pretty girl should be the answer. The question would be: Who put the bowl
on the counter? The pretty girl.
b. The pretty girl put the bowl on the counter.
On the counter should be the answer. The question can be: Where did the pretty
girl put the bowl? On the counter.
c. The pretty girl put the bowl on the counter.
Put the bowl on the counter should be the answer. The question can be: What did
the pretty girl do? Put the bowl on the counter.
3. Cleft: the cleft construction moves the words being tested into a different location. This
construction follows the structure: “It is (/was/will be) possible constituent that (/who)
rest of sentence.” For example:
72
a. The pretty girl put the bowl on the counter.
It was the pretty girl who put the bowl on the counter.
b. The pretty girl put the bowl on the counter.
It was on the counter that the pretty girl put the bowl.
c. The pretty girl put the bowl on the counter.
It was put the bowl on the counter that the pretty girl did. (Note that this sentence
is not acceptable to everyone; constituency tests are somewhat subjective. If you
are unsure about a particular test with a particular example, use the other tests to
provide further evidence.)
4. Pseudocleft: the pseudocleft construction is similar to the cleft in that it splits up the
words being tested from the rest of the sentence. However, where the cleft construction
moves the possible constituent to the front of the sentence, the pseudocleft moves it to the
back of the sentence: “The thing that (/The person who/The place where/The reason
why/What) rest of sentence is (/was/will be) possible constituent.
a. The pretty girl put the bowl on the counter.
The person who put the bowl on the counter was the pretty girl.
b. The pretty girl put the bowl on the counter.
The place where the pretty girl put the bowl was on the counter.
c. The pretty girl put the bowl on the counter.
What the pretty girl did was put the bowl on the counter. (Note that this is more
acceptable than the same example with clefts – some movements seem more
natural than others. This is why it is important to do several tests for each
example).
Example Problems
1. Decide if the underlined portion of the following sentence is a constituent:
John solved many mathematical equations for Meredith.
If John is a constituent, then at least two of the constituency tests need to produce
grammatical results:
Substitution: He solved many mathematical equations for Meredith.
Q/A: Who solved many mathematical equations for Meredith? John.
Cleft: It was John who solved many mathematical equations for Meredith.
Pseudocleft: The person who solved many mathematical equations for Meredith was John.
Since John passes all of the constituency tests, it is a constituent.
73
of the tests produce a grammatical result, we can say that many mathematical equations is a
constituent in this sentence.
Exercises
Decide if the underlined portion of the following sentences is a constituent (show the
constituency tests used):
1. Meredith mocked John’s solutions in the science lab.
2. Meredith mocked John’s solutions in the science lab.
3. Meredith mocked John’s solutions in the science lab.
4. Meredith mocked John’s solutions in the science lab.
5. Meredith mocked John’s solutions in the science lab.
6. Meredith mocked John’s solutions in the science lab.
7. Meredith mocked John’s solutions in the science lab.
74
Lexical Categories
Objective: Given a sentence, identify any given word’s lexical category.
Overview
Lexical categories are often referred to as “parts of speech”. Lexical categories are divided into
two main groups: function words and content words.
Content Words: Generally, content words nouns, adjectives, and verbs – they are words that
contain meaning in and of themselves.
Function Words: Function words, which include prepositions, determiners, and conjunctions, are
used for grammatical structure and do not have inherent meaning. For example, “dancing” is a
content word, whereas “with” is a function word.
Words can be different parts of speech in different sentences, depending on context. For
example, both of the sentences below include the word “dancing”. However, in one sentence,
“dancing” is a verb, and in the other, it’s a noun:
a) John is dancing with Meredith. (verb)
b) John likes dancing. (noun)
Similarly, “her” has different lexical categories in the following sentences:
a) He likes her. (noun)
b) Her books are on the table. (determiner)
Generally speaking, one can use the following rules to identify lexical categories. The rules for
content words (nouns, verbs, adjectives, and adverbs) are described in the Morphology: Basic
Lexical Categories section. These are not hard and fast rules; there are always exceptions. A non-
exhaustive list of function words and their categories is in the Lexical Categories Quick
Reference (later this section), but it is important to understand that some words can be in any of
several lexical categories depending on the sentence; it’s better to look at the role the word is
playing in the sentence than what word it specifically is.
Determiners: The most common determiners are “the”, “a”, and “an”; however, there are others
including possessive pronouns (his, her, their) and identifiers like this, that, and those. One
simple test: a word is acting as a determiner if it can be replaced by “the” or “a”/“an”.
Some examples:
• A book is on the table: Both “a” and “the” are determiners.
• Her book is on that table: Both “her” and “that” are determiners.
• Our book is on his table: Both “our” and “his” are determiners.
• He gave his book to her: Only “his” is a determiner in this case because it’s the only word
that can be replaced by “the” or “a”/“an”. That is, “He gave the book to her” is an
acceptable sentence, but “He gave his book to the” is not, even though “her” was an
acceptable determiner a previous example.
75
Modals/Auxiliary Verbs: Auxiliary verbs are also called “helping verbs”. In sentences where
there are two verbs immediately next to each other, one of the verbs is a main verb and the other
is an auxiliary. For example, “I am eating now” contains two verbs: “am” and “eating”. “Eating”
is the main verb; “am” is an auxiliary that helps determine the tense of the sentence. Auxiliary
verbs can sometimes be main verbs. For example, in “I am here,” “am” is the main verb.
Likewise, in “I have seen a cat,” “have” is an auxiliary, whereas in “I have a cat,” “have” is the
main verb.
Modal verbs are verbs that can never function as the main verb. There are nine of them in
English: can/could, may/might, shall/should, will/would, and must. For example, in the sentence,
“I could use a drink,” “could” is the modal verb, and “use” is the main verb.
Conjunctions: Conjunctions connect two words or phrases of the same lexical category. For the
purposes of this guide, the only conjunctions are “and”, “but”, and “or”. For instance, “and”
connects two nouns in the first sentence, two verbs in the second, and two whole sentences in the
third:
• John and Meredith like to explore. (John and Meredith are connected by the conjunction)
• John flies planes and hates bugs. (“flies planes” and “hates bugs” are verb phrases
connected by the conjunction)
• John sets them up and Meredith knocks them down (each side of the conjunction
comprises an entire sentence)
“Meredith is stalking him” is a complete, embedded sentence. Note that this use of “that” is
different from the previously discussed uses of “that” as a noun or a determiner. In this sentence,
it is not possible to substitute another noun in place of “that”, so it does not function as a noun. It
is not possible to substitute “the” in place of “that,” so it is not acting as a determiner.
It is conceivable to imagine a reading of the sentence where “that” is a determiner, but only
if there is more than one Meredith, and the point of the sentences is that “that Meredith” is
stalking John, and not “this other Meredith”. However, this reading of the sentence is not the one
that first comes to mind, so it is probable that “that” is a complementizer in this sentence.
Qualifiers: Qualifiers modify adjectives and adverbs. Common examples include rather, very,
and quite. For example, in the following statement, “rather” modifies the adjective “incinerating”
and “very” modifies the adverb “quickly”:
Meredith’s rather incinerating glare made John catch on very quickly.
Quantifiers: Quantifiers are “number” words such as: one, two, fifteen, 300, etc. They are also
expressions that refer to number, such as: many, few, some, much, etc.
76
Prepositions: Common prepositions include: to, from, with, of, by, for, in, into, on, under,
about, near, around, etc. Prepositions are notoriously difficult to define, but, in general, they are
relational indicators: words which show the relationship between things (in space or in some
other way, e.g. “with”, “of”). Often, if the word doesn’t fit into any of the other categories, it is
probably a preposition.
Note that prepositions belong to a wider class, called adpositions. We call them
“prepositions,” with the suffix “pre-” meaning “before” because, in English, these words occur
before the noun that they modify. In other languages, such as Japanese, they can occur after (e.g.
“on the desk” in English vs. “the desk on” in Japanese). In Japanese the word corresponding to
“on” would be called a postposition.
Example Problems
Label all the words in the following sentences with the appropriate lexical category labels.
N V N
John likes Meredith.
N V D N
John likes the scientist.
N V D Adj N P N
John likes the pretty scientist from Canada.
77
4. The scientists in the lab like that John thinks highly of them.
Here, there are a few new categories. “That”, in this case, is a complementizer, because it
introduces a new sentence (“John thinks highly of them”). Any sentence that has two
different verbs (as opposed to a set of auxiliary + main verb) must contain a
complementizer. “Highly” is an adverb. Like many adverbs, it ends in “-ly” and describes
a verb (“thinks”). So:
D N P D N V C N V Adv P N
The scientists in the lab like that John thinks highly of them.
D N V C N V Adj
That scientist believes that he is smarter.
N Mod V C N V N Adv
Everyone will see that John likes Meredith eventually.
Exercises:
Label the words in the following sentences with appropriate lexical category labels.
1. Meredith loves chocolate.
2. Meredith loves chocolate truffles.
3. Meredith loves chocolate truffles with real milk chocolate.
4. Meredith loves chocolate truffles that John brought to the city.
5. All scientists adore John and his shiny brain.
6. Scientists from Canada detest the Antarctic winter with a fiery passion.
78
English Phrase Structure & Syntax Trees
Objective: Use phrase structure rules to draw syntax trees. Know the parts of a syntax tree and be
able to describe and understand them.
Overview
There are language-specific rules of syntax that govern sentence structure. This section examines
one way of looking at English syntax, with an internally consistent set of rules that is not
complete (or entirely correct) but that shows, conceptually, how the sentence structure rules are
used to create syntactic trees. Keep in mind that these rules are provided as an example of simple
rules we might use to describe English; the ones used in your class might vary in terms of
sophistication and/or labeling conventions.
Recall that in the section on constituency tests, we described units of the sentence which
naturally go together, or “constituents.” Now, we’ll call constituents “phrases.” We’ll have a
special kind of phrase for every kind of content word: nouns phrases, verb phrases, adjective
phrases, and adverb phrases, as well as prepositional phrases.
CP C IP
IP NP I VP
NP (Det) (Quant) (AdjP) N {PP, CP}
VP V {PP, NP, CP, AdvP}
AdjP (Qual) Adj
AdvP (Qual) Adv
PP P NP
P In general, “P” means “phrase” (except in the case of Prepositional Phrases, in which case the
first P means, funnily enough, “Prepositional.”) The other letters correspond to lexical categories
(the same abbreviations we used in the previous section, “Lexical Categories.”)
I means “inflectional”; a sentence is known as an inflectional phrase. Auxiliaries and modals are
considered inflectionals, as well as any tense information (more on this later).
Arrows The phrase at the left of the arrow is the one “on top”, which contains the elements to
the right of the arrow.
Brackets { } The elements included in { } can occur in any order and are also optional.
WARNING: These rules apply to English syntax only. Other languages have their own rules for
word and phrase order.
79
Phrase Structure Rules, Abstracted
As you may notice from above, there are some regularities in the way phrase structure rules are
formed. For one thing, every phrase has at least one non-optional element, which the phrase is
named after: a noun phrase always has a noun, a verb phrase always has a verb, etc. That non-
optional namesake element is called the head. Like every person, phrase has one head.
Beyond that, all the phrase structure rules are basically structured as follows:
Some Kind of Phrase (XP) (Some Optional Stuff) X (Some more optional stuff)
where X is the head of the X-phrase (whatever X is). We call the stuff before the head the
specifier, and the stuff after the head the complement. Thus the general phrase structure rule is
XP Spec X Comp
Remember that even this seemingly basic rule only applies to English—Japanese, for example,
puts the specifier after the head.
As noted above, the specific rules from your class may differ; the important thing is that you
follow the rules that you develop (whatever those rules are) so that your structures are consistent.
Node: Any place on the tree which is labeled (e.g. N, NP, AdjP). The point of tree-drawing is to
show how the nodes attach to each other.
80
Branch: A line on the tree showing relationships/attachments between nodes.
Mother: The node directly above/on top of another node (only when directly connected with a
branch) is its parent.
Daughter: The node directly below/under another node (only when directly connected with a
branch) is its daughter.
Immediately dominates: A node immediately dominates any node which it contains/is on top of,
as long as it is connected by one branch.
Dominates: A node dominates any node which it contains/is on top of, as long as it is connected
by some series of branches (it need not be a direct, one-branch connection, but you may not
include branches which lead to a higher level).
Examples
Here’s a sample tree which demonstrates the vocabulary we’ve talked about so far. “Example
Problems” (next page) gives more detail on how to draw these trees for yourself.
IP
NP1 VP
I
+past NP2
N V D N
John kissed the scientist.
You can see how the phrase structure rules relate to the tree:
IP NP I VP The IP contains an NP, I, and VP
NP (Det) (Quant) (AdjP) N {PP, CP} NP1 contains its only mandatory member, N.
NP2 also contains an optional determiner.
VP V {PP, NP, CP, AdvP} The VP contains a V and an NP, acceptable in the rule
• All of the labeled areas where branches can connect (IP, NP1, I, VP, N, V, NP2, D, N) are
nodes.
• IP is the mother of NP1, I, and VP. IP is not considered the mother of N (either one), V,
D, or NP2 because there is no direct connection between them.
• V and NP2 are daughters of VP. Can you find some more mother-daughter relationships?
• NP1, I, and VP are sisters. Can you find some more sister relationships?
• VP immediately dominates V and NP2. IP dominates both VP and V (actually, IP
dominates everything), but only immediately dominates VP.
• NP1 is on a higher level than, say, D, it does not dominate D. There is no way to trace
down branches from NP1 to D without going up a level first.
81
Note that non-optional elements must be included in a tree regardless of whether a corresponding
word is actually present in the sentence. Notably, “I” must always be included regardless of
whether there is an auxiliary or modal verb that actually holds that position. The “I” is
considered the head of the IP, and indicates the tense of the sentence. If there s no auxiliary or
modal, the “I” is labeled with the tense of the sentence (e.g. +present, +past, +future).
In other cases where a position is indicated with no word holding that position, the general
rule of thumb is to assume that the position is there in the mind, but the corresponding word is
dropped in speech. In your tree, you will include the position but write a null symbol (Ø) where
the corresponding word should be. This happens most commonly with CPs (complementizer
phrases; see “Complex Sentences”). Sometimes, an embedded sentence is introduced with no
complementizer, such as in the sentence “I heard John likes Meredith” (instead of “I heard that
John likes Meredith”).
Example Problems
Using the structure rules above, draw the following trees:
N V N
John likes Meredith.
Now, we have to extend all the elements that are heads to their appropriate phrases (so N goes to
NP, V goes to VP). It’s a good strategy to find the subject N (in this case, John) and the main V
and extend them higher than the rest of the sentence, since they will eventually connect to the
main, top-level phrase, that is, the IP:
NP VP
NP
N V N
John likes Meredith.
There are two steps left, which could be done in either order.
- By the rule, IP NP I VP, we see that our top-level phrase, the IP, needs an NP and a VP.
Handily, we have one. Connect the subject NP and the VP to the IP. Make sure to place the I
under the IP, between the NP and VP, as it is required. Since no word corresponds to it, simply
note the tense information (the sentence is in present tense.)
82
IP
NP VP
I
+present NP
N V N
John likes Meredith.
- Connect the object NP (Meredith) to the VP (likes). Generally, the object always connects to
the VP (since it describes/belongs with/has to do with the verb), but in case there were any
doubts, you can see that there’s no other place for it to go. Crossing lines of the tree is not
allowed, so only two nodes are open: the VP and the IP. Both are allowed to have an NP inside
them (see the rules: VP V {PP, NP, CP, AdvP} and IP NP I VP), but our IP already has its
NP, and the rule does not allow for an additional NP after the VP. So, the VP is the only place
our NP can actually go.
IP
NP VP
I
+present NP
N V N
John likes Meredith.
The procedure is similar here to the example above. The only special circumstance to note is
that, by our current rules, determiners do not get their own phrase – they attach directly to the
noun phrase of the noun they modify. In this case “the” attaches to the “scientist” NP:
NP VP
NP
N V D N
John likes the scientist.
83
Once this is complete, the tree is the same as above:
IP
NP VP
I
+present NP
N V D N
John likes the scientist.
This is a little more complicated, as it contains a conjunction. First, we label the words:
N V N Conj Adj N
John likes Meredith and turkey sandwiches
Note that “turkey” in this case is an Adjective because it describes the kind of sandwich that
John likes. As before, we now find the subject N (John) and the V (likes) and draw them higher
than the other phrases:
NP VP
NP NP
AP
N V N Conj Adj N
John likes Meredith and turkey sandwiches
Notice that the adjective phrase modifies the noun that follows it (what kind of sandwiches?
turkey sandwiches!). So, the AdjP connects to the NP “sandwiches”. Also, now we can see that
the conjunction combines two noun phrases (“Meredith” and “turkey sandwiches”) so we can
combine them into a new NP. As before, the subject NP and the VP go to IP:
IP
NP VP
I NP
+present
NP NP
AP
N V N Conj Adj N
John likes Meredith and turkey sandwiches
84
Note that in the case of conjunctions is the only case, according to our rules, that a XP phrase can
dominate itself (i.e. normally, NP is not allowed to contain an NP). Now, for the final tree, we
connect the object NP to the VP and we are done:
IP
NP VP
I NP
+present
NP NP
AP
N V N Conj Adj N
John likes Meredith and turkey sandwiches
D Adj N V D Adj N P N
The military commander likes the pretty scientist from Canada
The new element here is the prepositional phrase; the key with these is to remember that a
prepositional phrase always takes the noun phrase immediately to the right of the preposition.
We find the subject N (commander) and the V (likes), and draw the extensions as before:
NP VP
NP
PP
AP AP NP
D Adj N V D Adj N P N
The military commander likes the pretty scientist from Canada
We know that the main NP and the VP will go to IP; we know that “the pretty scientist” is an
object of the verb “likes”, so will attach to the VP. However, the PP has two places it could go –
either to the NP next to it, or to the VP a little further away. The question is, what does “from
Canada” modify – the scientist, or the liking? It’s pretty clear from this example, that “from
Canada” is talking about the scientist, so it will connect to the NP:
85
IP
NP I VP
+present
NP
PP
AP AP NP
D Adj N V D Adj N P N
The military commander likes the pretty scientist from Canada
Tips
• Find the subject N and the main V, and immediately place them as the head of their
respective NP and VP. These will go directly to IP. You can draw them higher than the
other elements.
• Generally, Adjectives will modify something, so the associated AP needs to be drawn
lower than the phrase containing whatever it is they modify (usually an NP).
• Follow the rules – the rules are very limiting, and can help you if you’re not sure which
words to put into the same phrase (e.g. in “the pretty scientist”, “the” is part of the NP the
head of which is “scientist” – if you’re not sure about this, the rules will tell you because
they don’t allow determiners in adjective phrases).
• The entire tree has to be connected; the only node that doesn’t have a mother is the main
IP.
• Everything else has one (and only one) mother. A node can have (but doesn’t have to have)
many daughters, but can only have one mother.
Exercises
Using the structure rules, draw the following trees:
1. Meredith loves chocolate.
2. Meredith loves chocolate truffles.
3. Meredith loves chocolate truffles with real milk chocolate.
4. Meredith loves cookies and milk.
86
Ambiguous Sentences
Objective: Distinguish multiple readings of ambiguous sentences, and draw the distinct tree that
corresponds with each reading.
Overview
This section deals with sentences that are ambiguous: that is, they could have more than one
meaning. For example, the sentence “Meredith hit the scientist with the brick” has two meanings:
the first meaning is that Meredith hit the scientist using the brick to do the hitting; the second
meaning is that Meredith hit the scientist who had the brick (as opposed to the scientist who had
the hammer).
The difference can be expressed using syntactic trees by attachment of the phrase that is
responsible for the ambiguity. In this case, it is the phrase “with the brick”, which is either
referring to the scientist or to the act of hitting, will attach to either “the scientist” noun phrase or
to the “hit” verb phrase.
(Note: There are other kinds of ambiguities which we won’t talk about here; the most
common are semantic ambiguities, which are caused by the multiple meanings of a single word.
For example, “John went to the bank” could mean “John went to the financial institution” or
“John went to the riverside.” There is no way to show this disambiguation using syntax trees, so
before you try, make sure it’s the structure, and not a word, that’s responsible for the ambiguity!)
Example Problems
Determine the two meanings in these ambiguous sentences and provide the meanings with
corresponding trees.
IP
NP VP
I
[past] PP
NP NP
N V D N P D N
John teased the scientist with the device.
87
The attachment of the PP is based on meaning. Since our meaning is that John is using the device
as a tool to tease the scientist, then the device is an instrument (See Semantics: Theta Roles) of
the teasing. That means it attaches to the verb phrase:
IP
NP VP
I
[past] PP
NP NP
N V D N P D N
John teased the scientist with the device.
The second meaning, that John teased the scientist who had the device, has an almost identical
tree. The only difference is the attachment of the PP, which now explains which scientist John
teased as opposed to explaining how John accomplished the teasing:
IP
NP VP
I NP
[past] PP
NP
N V D N P D N
John teased the scientist with the device
88
Meaning 1: John sold the device to the scientist who is currently in Canada:
IP
NP VP
I PP
[past]
NP
PP
NP NP
N V D N P D N P N
John sold the device to the scientist in Canada.
Note that “to the scientist” attaches directly to the verb phrase; it is not part of the other noun
phrase “the device”. This is because “to the scientist” refers to the selling; it doesn’t say anything
about the device. In another sentence, as in: “John sold the device with the shiny light in
Canada”, “with the shiny light” would be describing the device and would therefore attach to
“the device” NP.
Meaning 2: John sold the device to the scientist, and the sale took place in Canada:
IP
NP VP
I PP PP
[past]
NP
NP NP
N V D N P D N P N
John sold the device to the scientist in Canada.
Note that the PP in question (in Canada) attaches directly to the VP.
89
3. Meredith drinks coffee with extra milk and sugar.
Here, the ambiguity is of a slightly different sort: the question is whether Meredith drinks coffee
with extra “milk and sugar” (that is, if there’s extra milk AND extra sugar) or if Meredith drinks
coffee with “extra milk” and sugar. So, the question is in whether or not the conjunction
dominates the word “extra”.
Meaning 1: Meredith drinks coffee with extra milk and extra sugar.
IP
NP VP
I
[pres] NP
PP
NP
AdjP NP NP
N V N P Adj N Conj N
Meredith drinks coffee with extra milk and sugar.
Note that with this reading, the adjective “extra” is modifying both milk and sugar, since it
attaches at the level of the conjunction.
Meaning 2: Meredith drinks coffee with extra milk and a normal amount of sugar.
IP
NP VP
I
[pres] NP
PP
NP
NP NP
AdjP
N V N P Adj N Conj N
Meredith drinks coffee with extra milk and sugar.
Note that here, the adjective phrase “extra” only modifies “milk”, because it attaches to “milk”
before the conjunction unites the milk NP and the sugar NP.
90
Exercises
Determine the meanings in these ambiguous sentences and provide the meanings with
corresponding trees.
1. John shot the marine with the gun.
2. Meredith likes chocolate truffles and cake.
3. John burned the picture of Elizabeth with her ex-boyfriend. (three possible meanings here,
one requires treating “with” as a conjunction)
91
Complex Sentences
Objective: Draw trees with embedded sentences/complementizer phrases (CPs).
Overview
Some sentences contain embedded sentences (e.g. “John believes that Meredith likes him.”). In
these sentences, there is the main sentence (“John believes ___”) and the embedded sentence
(“Meredith likes him”) which are joined by the complementizer (in this case, “that”).
The way to handle these complex sentences is to determine which is the upper-level
sentences and which is the embedded sentence. Then, draw the tree for the embedded sentence
first. It will be a complete tree (with an IP). Then you can work on the rest of tree, and figure out
where the embedded sentence attaches in the main sentence.
One hint that you have an embedded sentence: if you have a sentence with more than one
verb (excluding aux and modal verbs) and the verbs are not connected with a conjunction, then
there’s going to be embedded sentence(s). There will be only one VP, and therefore only one
main verb, per IP.
Example Problems
Construct the trees for the following complex sentences:
IP
NP VP
CP
I
[pres] IP
NP VP
I
[pres] NP
N V C N V N
John believes that Meredith likes him.
Use the same logic to figure out where CP attaches as you would use for any other phrase.
• Look at the phrase structure rules. From our rules from “English Phrase Structure Rules &
Syntax Trees”, we see a CP could go in either a noun phrase or a verb phrase:
NP (Det) (Quant) (AdjP) N {PP, CP}
VP V {PP, NP, CP, AdvP}
• Use the meaning of the sentence to help determine the place of attachment. In this case,
“that Meredith likes him” is talking about what John believes, so it attaches to the
“believe” VP.
Remember that even the embedded IP must have an I.
92
2. The rumor that Meredith likes John annoys Elizabeth.
This one is a little tricky because it’s harder to find the main sentence. The subject of the main
sentence is “rumor”, and what does the rumor do? It annoys Elizabeth. So, the main sentence is
“the rumor annoys Elizabeth”. The embedded sentence is “Meredith likes John.” The CP “that
Meredith likes John” is talking about the rumor, so will end up attached to the “rumor” NP.
IP
NP VP
I
CP [pres]
IP NP
NP VP
I
[pres] NP
D N C N V N V N
The rumor that Meredith likes John annoys Elizabeth
NP VP
I
[pres] CP
IP
NP VP
I
[pres] CP
IP
NP VP
I
[pres] NP
N V C N V C N V N
John believes that Meredith believes that John loves Elizabeth
93
The English language allows for an infinite construction of this type (another example of
recursion!) It’s always possible to embed the sentence in another sentence. It’s important to think
of every CP as just another constituent in order to not become confused about the attachments of
the various phrases.
IP
NP VP
CP
I
[pres] IP
NP VP
I
[pres] NP
N V C N V N
John believes Ø Meredith likes him.
Exercises:
Construct the trees for the following complex sentences:
1. John insists that Meredith brought the cake without citrus to the party.
2. The announcement that Meredith went to the party raised some eyebrows.
3. John played the game because Meredith insisted on his participation.
4. John thinks Meredith sang the song about love for him.
5. John heard that Meredith ate the cake yesterday. (This sentence is ambiguous; give both
meanings/trees)
94
Transformations
Objective: Draw trees for yes/no and wh-word questions.
Overview
Think about how we phrase questions in English (e.g. “What has John done?”). The question
form is related to the declarative statement form, and you can form a declarative version of any
sentence (“John has done what”). We assume that the declarative version is the basic version you
start with in your brain (the deep or underlying structure), and the version you actually say—the
question form—is the result of a transformation, moving words around in a specific, rule-based
way. This section covers the transformations that occur in English sentences to turn them from
declarative statements to question statements.
Now that we’re talking about questions, we’ll start drawing the main IP of all our
sentences as embedded within an overarching CP. The transformations rely upon having those
extra nodes to work with. (The examples will explain further.) We’ll also start including a non-
specific Specifier (Spec) position in a CP. Recall that “specifier” just means “stuff before the
head” (for English, at least).
The two transformations we cover are Inversion (movement of the I to the C position of
the main CP, as in yes/no questions such as, “Can John love Meredith?” from “John can love
Meredith”) and Wh- Movement (movement of a wh-word, such as who, what, where, when, to
the Specifier position of the main CP, as in “What has John done?” from “John has done what”).
Example Problems
Draw the underlying and surface structure of the following sentences:
Underlying structure:
CP
Spec C
IP
NP I VP
NP
N mod V N
John can love Meredith
95
The surface structure (or the version of the question that you actually pronounce) is very, very
similar, so the easiest thing to do is to re-draw the entire tree from above and then make the
changes with a little erasing. The changes are as follows:
- Figure out what word moves, and where it moves to. The only term that moves here is “can,”
and it moves to the beginning of the sentence. We put in the C position in the main CP. So, write
“Can” under C, and erase it from where it used to be (under mod).
- Replace the moved term with a trace (t). In this case, write t where “can” used to be, under
mod.
- Attach the trace to the word’s new location with an arrow, and label the arrow to describe the
kind of movement. Here, draw an arrow from the t to “Can.” Since we’re doing an inversion, we
write “inversion.”
Surface structure:
CP
Spec C
IP
NP I VP
NP
N mod V N
Canx John tx love Meredith
inversion
CP
Spec C
IP
NP I VP
NP
N mod V
John has done what
96
In the surface structure, we have to show both movements (of “has” and of “what”). The “has”
movement is just an inversion, like we did before. “What” moves to the Specifier position of the
CP, and this is called “wh-movement” because we are moving a wh-question word.
Because there are two traces, we label each trace with a little letter, called a coindex
(typically x, y, z) to distinguish between them. We then label the corresponding word with the
same coindex, so it’s clear which trace goes with which word.
Surface structure:
CP
Spec C
IP
NP I VP
NP
N mod V
Whaty hasx John tx done ty
inversion
wh- movement
Tips
• Underlying structure: make sure you draw the empty CP
• Underlying structure: label the wh- word, if it exists, with the correct category (what = NP,
when = AdvP; where = PP, why = CP, who = NP).
• Surface structure: draw arrows to label movement
• Surface structure: make sure to co-index the traces and the words that moved
• Surface structure: label the arrows with appropriate transformation types (i.e. inversion or
wh-movement)
• Surface structure: the movement from the I position always goes to C; the movement of the
wh- word always goes to Spec.
Exercises
Draw the underlying and surface structure of the following sentences:
1. Does Meredith love chocolate truffles?
2. Who did John sell the device to?
3. Where did John sell the device?
4. When will Meredith yell at the scientists?
97
98
Semantics
Semantics is the study of meaning.
Lessons
Connotation & Denotation
Overview
Definitions & Examples
Processes of Semantic Change
Theta roles
Overview
Definitions & Examples
Exercises
Tautologies, Contradictions, and Contingencies
Overview
Definitions & Examples
Sense & Reference
Overview
Definitions & Examples
Exercises
Presupposition & Entailment
Definitions
Tests for Presupposition & Entailment
Example Problems
Exercises
99
Connotation & Denotation
Overview
When talking about the meanings of words, it is important to distinguish between the basic,
literal definition, such as one might find in the dictionary (denotation), and the feelings or
associations evoked by a particular word choice (connotation).
(WARNING: “Connotation” and “denotation” have two meanings in linguistics. The
other meaning has to do with the meaning of the words used to describe an object (connotation)
vs. the actual object referred to (denotation); we will use “sense” and “reference”, respectively,
to describe this distinction, in the section entitled “Sense & Reference.” Be aware of which kind
of connotation/denotation your professor is using, and adjust your use of the words accordingly.)
For example, “woman,” “lady”, “dame”, “broad”, and “chick” all mean (denote) “adult female,”
but they have different connotations: “woman” is fairly neutral; “lady” might be associated with
politeness and gentility, or with advanced age; “dame” and “broad” are both fairly outdated
slang, and nowadays probably have strongest associations with the time period they come from;
“chick” would probably be considered derogatory or at least highly informal.
We can talk about connotations as being positive, negative, or neutral depending on the kind
of feelings they evoke. Depending on how you feel about them, you might describe the same
animal as “vermin” (negative), “rats” (neutral/possibly negative), or “fuzzy-wuzzy little
creatures” (positive).
100
Processes of Semantic Change
Here are some of the processes by which word meanings can change.
extension (or metaphorical extension) A word gains an additional meaning when it is used in a
new context.
• the use of “mouse” to mean the computer pointing device in addition to the rodent
• the word “illuminate”, or, similarly, the phrase “to shed light on” originally meant “to
light up”, but has been extended in metaphor to “to clarify” in terms of understanding,
not just physical environment
metonymy A concrete symbol or part is used to represent a larger or more abstract whole.
• “crown” for “king” or “monarchy” (“I am loyal to the crown of England!”)
• “wheels” for “car”
• “the steak,” used by a waiter to describe a patron who ordered a steak
101
Theta roles
Objective: To identify the roles that noun phrases serve in a given sentence.
Overview
A theta-role of a noun phrase is its semantic role in a sentence. The semantic role is important; it
is not the same as the syntactic roles of subject/object/etc positions. For instance, in the example,
“John loves Meredith,” “John” is the subject of the sentence (in the syntactic sense), and is also
the Agent/Experiencer (in the semantic sense) as he is the one who is doing the action. However,
in the passive version of that sentence, “Meredith is loved by John,” “Meredith” becomes the
subject but “John” is still the Agent/Experiencer.
Keep in mind that you may only use a subset of the given roles, or there may be extra ones that
are not covered here. For example, one can consider Theme/Patient to be one theta role (any
recipient of the action) because Patient can be seen as a subset of Theme.
Examples: Label all the underlined phrases in the following sentences with the appropriate theta
roles.
1. Meredith held a giant bug.
Meredith is the Agent, because “holding” is a deliberate action.
A giant bug is the theme, because it is the recipient of the holding action but is not
changed by it.
2. John saw Meredith hold a giant bug from behind the pillar.
John is the experiencer, because “seeing” is not a deliberate action.
From behind the pillar is the source from which John performed his action.
102
3. Carson stabbed the giant bug with sharp scissors to get at its insides.
Carson is the agent, because “stabbing” is a deliberate action.
The giant bug is the patient because it is the recipient of the action, and was changed by
that action.
Sharp scissors is the instrument that is used in the stabbing action.
To get at its insides is the goal of the action.
Exercises: Label all the underlined phrases in the following sentences with the appropriate theta
roles.
1. The explosion took out most of the western pier in the city.
2. Meredith activated the weapon from a distance using a special device.
3. John picked up the shiny device from the closet in the office with the tongs
103
Tautologies, Contradictions, and Contingencies
Objective: To identify statements as tautologies, contradictions, or contingencies.
Overview
In semantics we care not only about the meanings of individual words, but the meanings of
propositions. Without knowing any facts about the world we live in beyond the meanings of the
words used, we can automatically identify certain propositions as true (tautologies) and certain
propositions as false (contradictions).
Tautology: A statement that must, necessarily, be true, regardless of the actual conditions of the
world. Examples:
• My brother is male
• A dog is a dog
Here are some examples of true statements which are not tautologies:
• Buckingham Palace has guards
• Some people buy Ivory soap
Contradiction: A statement that must, necessarily, be false, regardless of the actual conditions of
the world. Examples:
• My brother is female
• A dog is not a dog
Here are some examples of false statements which are not contradictions:
• All humans hate ice cream
• The Canadian flag depicts a man holding an orange and weeping uncontrollably
Contingency: A statement which might be true or false, depending on the reality of the world.
All of the examples above of statements that are not tautologies or contradictions are
contingencies. In fact, most statements you hear every day are contingencies, since these are the
only kind of statements which provide useful information. It’s generally a waste of time to state a
tautology (since obviously they are true) or a contradiction (since obviously they are false).
Exercises
Decide if the following statements are tautologies, contradictions, or contingencies:
1. All bachelors are not married.
2. The red cake is green.
3. The cakes are red.
4. Every man loves a woman.
5. John loves Meredith.
6. Meredith is a woman.
104
Sense & Reference
Objective: Distinguish between the sense and reference of a word or phrase.
Overview
Different noun phrases may refer to the same actual object. For example, at the time of this
writing, the president of the United States is George W. Bush. So, the phrase “the president of
the United States” refers to the same individual as does “George W. Bush.” If we’re talking
about John, who is a pilot with dark hair and who is dating Meredith, the phrases “John” and
“Meredith’s boyfriend” and “the dark-haired pilot” refer to the same person.
But with a little thought we can see that these different ways of describing the same person (or
object, or entity) are not identical. One way to tell is that the sentence “John is John” is clearly a
tautology, but the sentences “John is Meredith’s boyfriend” or “the dark-haired pilot is John” are
contingencies, even though they are also of the form “{phrase referring to John} is {phrase
referring to John}.” So, there must be more to the meaning of a descriptive noun phrase than the
actual object that it refers to. Thus the distinction between sense and reference.
Note that the referent of some sense can change over time. For example, “the president of the
United States” refers to George W. Bush in 2007, but Franklin Delano Roosevelt in 1934. The
phrase “Meredith’s boyfriend” might refer to John now, but it could be different tomorrow when
John finds out that Meredith has been secretly writing love notes to Sam. And so on.
Exercises
1. Identify the sense and reference of the following phrases.
a. Prime Minister of Canada
b. the last team to win the World Series
c. my (the exercise-doer’s) favorite fruit
2. Consider the statements “Meredith’s boyfriend is not Meredith’s boyfriend” and “John is
not Meredith’s boyfriend.”
a. What is the referent of each of the underlined phrases?
b. What kind of statements are these (tautology, contradiction, or contingency)? If
they are different, why?
105
Presupposition & Entailment
Objective: To use the appropriate test (negation or contradiction) on a pair of sentences, A and B,
to determine whether A presupposes B, A entails B, or both (mutual entailment).
Definitions
Entailment: If statement A entails statement B, then whenever A is true, B is also true. “John
shot the monster” entails “The monster was shot.”
• If A entails B, then “A and not B” is a contradiction.
• Mutual Entailment: If A and B entail each other, then B true whenever A is true, AND A
is true whenever B is true. This generally happens with statements that mean the same
thing.
• Note: Statement A can strongly imply statement B without requiring that B be true. Such
a statement is not an entailment; it is an implicature. For example, “John shot Meredith”
can imply “Meredith is dead” but does not require it – John could have shot Meredith
with a BB gun, for example. Alternately, “John killed Meredith” requires “Meredith is
dead”. Implicatures are discussed further in the Pragmatics section.
Negation Test for Presupposition: Since presupposition is constant under negation, you can test
whether A presupposes B by negating A (saying “It’s not true that A” or a statement which
means the same thing).
• If B is still true when “not A” is true, then A presupposes B.
• If B is untrue when “not A” is true or you can’t tell whether B is true or not anymore,
then A does not presuppose B.
Contradiction Test for Entailment: We know that if A entails B, then “A and not B” is a
contradiction. Therefore, to test for entailment, we can negate sentence B (“It’s not true that B”)
and put the two sentences together.
• If “A and not B” must be false, then A entails B.
• If “A and not B” could be true (it’s definitely true or you don’t know whether it’s true or
not), then A does not entail B.
• If A entails B (that is, “A and not B” is a contradiction), then you can test for mutual
entailment. Flip the order of the sentences (mark A as B and B as A) and do the test
again.
106
Example Problems
We negate 1A to get: John was not glad that Meredith came to his party.
1B is still true: Meredith came to John’s party, whether or not John was glad of it. Therefore, 1A
presupposes 1B.
We negate 2A to get: It is not the case that John and Meredith went to Carson’s party. Note here
that the negation “John and Meredith did not go to Carson’s party” is incorrect, because does not
allow for the possibility that John went to Carson’s party and Meredith did not, which is still a
possible negation of 2A.
So, given that “It is not the case that John and Meredith went to Carson’s party,” the truth of 2B
is unknown: It’s possible that Meredith went and John did not, or it’s possible that John went and
Meredith did not, or maybe both of them didn’t go. Because we don’t have the information to
decide whether Meredith went to Carson’s party, 2A entails (and does not presuppose) 2B.
We negate 3A to get: Not everyone finds John attractive. Note here that the negation of
“everyone” is “not everyone”; “no one” is the negation of “someone”.
Given the negation of 3A, the truth of 3B is unknown. It’s possible that Meredith finds John
attractive, or it’s possible that Meredith is one of the people who does not find John attractive.
Since the truth of 3B is unknown, 3A entails 3B.
We negate 5A to get: Carson does not understand John’s feeling for Meredith.
107
5B is now not necessarily true; it’s possible that someone else understands John’s feelings for
Meredith, but it’s also possible that Carson was the only person who understood John’s feelings
for Meredith. So, the truth of 5B is now unknown, so 5A entails 5B.
Mutual Entailment. In the following sets of statements, does A entail B, does B entail A, does
both A entail B and B entail A (mutual entailment), or does neither entail the other?
When testing for entailment, we have to use the contradiction test where we combine sentence A
with the negation of sentence B. If the combined sentence is a contradiction, then A entails B.
We then repeat the test for B entailing A.
We negate 1B to get: Meredith is not shorter than John; then we combine A and B to get: John
is taller than Meredith AND Meredith is not shorter than John, which is a contradiction, so A
entails B.
Now, we switch the sentences and negate 1A: John is not taller than Meredith; then we combine
A and B to get: Meredith is shorter than John AND John is not taller than Meredith, which is
also a contradiction, so B entails A.
Since A entails B and B entails A, this is a case of mutual entailment.
We negate 2B to get: Meredith is not the same height as John; then we combine A and B to get:
John is taller than Meredith AND Meredith is not the same height as John, which is not a
contradiction, so A does not entail B.
Now, we switch the sentences and negate 2A: John is not taller than Meredith; then we combine
A and B to get: Meredith is the same height as John AND John is not taller than Meredith,
which is also not a contradiction, so B does not entail A.
Since A does not entail B and B does not entail A, there is no entailment in any direction.
We negate 3B to get: Meredith does not find John attractive; then we combine A and B to get:
Everyone finds John attractive AND Meredith does not find John attractive, which is a
contradiction, so A entails B.
Now, we switch the sentences and negate 3A: Not everyone finds John attractive; then we
combine A and B to get: Meredith finds John attractive AND Not everyone finds John attractive,
which is not a contradiction, so B does not entail A.
Since A entails B and B does not entail A, this is a case of one-way entailment (A B only).
108
We negate 4B to get: No one loves Meredith like John loves Meredith; then we combine A and B
to get: Not everyone loves Meredith like John loves Meredith AND No one loves Meredith like
John loves Meredith, which is not a contradiction, so A does not entail B.
Now, we switch the sentences and negate 4A: Everyone loves Meredith like John loves Meredith;
then we combine A and B to get: Someone loves Meredith like John loves Meredith AND
Everyone loves Meredith like John loves Meredith, which is not a contradiction, so B does not
entail A.
Since A does not entail B and B does not entail A, there is no entailment in any direction.
We negate 5B to get: No one understands John’s feelings for Meredith; then we combine A and
B to get: Carson understands John’s feelings for Meredith AND No one understands John’s
feelings for Meredith, which is a contradiction, so A entails B.
Now, we switch the sentences and negate 5A: Carson does not understand John’s feelings for
Meredith; then we combine A and B to get: Someone understands John’s feelings for Meredith
AND Carson does not understand John’s feelings for Meredith, which is not a contradiction, so
B does not entail A.
Since A entails B and B does not entail A, this is a case of one-way entailment (A B only).
Exercises
1A. John did not realize that Meredith was working in Antarctica.
1B. Meredith was in Antarctica.
Mutual Entailment: In the following sets of statements, does A entail B, does B entail A, does
both A entail B and B entail A (mutual entailment), or does neither entail the other?
109
2A. John’s charming personality makes everyone like him.
2B. Meredith likes John.
110
Pragmatics
Pragmatics is the study of language at the discourse level; or, how language is used.
Lessons
Grice’s Cooperative Principle, Maxims of Conversation & Conversational Implicature
The Cooperative Principle
The Maxims of Conversation
Maxim Violations
Natural Language vs. Logical Language
More on Conversational Implicature
Criticisms of the Maxims
Exercises
Speech Acts
Definitions
Searle’s Classification of Illocutionary Acts
Felicity Conditions
Exercises
Brown & Levinson’s Politeness Theory
About Face
Politeness Strategies
Example Problems
Exercises
111
Grice’s Cooperative Principle, Maxims of Conversation & Conversational
Implicature
Objective: Given a short dialogue which makes use of the maxims, identify the maxim in play,
and explain your answer. If applicable, explain the implication created.
Make your conversational contribution such as is required, at the stage at which it occurs,
by the accepted purpose or direction of the talk exchange in which you are engaged. xiii
In other words, we as speakers try to contribute meaningful, productive utterances to further the
conversation. It then follows that, as listeners, we assume that our conversational partners are
doing the same.
You can think of reasons why someone might be uncooperative in conversation (maybe
they’re being interrogated for information they don’t want to give up; maybe they hate the
person they’re talking to; maybe they’re just crazy) but in the vast majority of conversations, it’s
safe to assume that both participants are trying to be cooperative.
This assumption (that the cooperative principle holds, and the people we’re speaking to
are trying to cooperate) explains two things:
(i) why speech errors are often ignored (or even go unnoticed) in conversation. As long as
the meaning the speaker is trying to get across is clear, the listener usually gives them the benefit
of the doubt and focuses on the meaning.
(ii) why we can find meaning in statements which, on the surface, seem ridiculous, untrue
or unrelated (i.e. metaphors, sarcasm, overstatement, understatement, etc.) Rather than assuming
that our conversational partner is lying, crazy, or speaking at random, we assume they’re trying
to get across some meaning, and we can figure out what that meaning is.
Quantity
• Make your contribution as informative as required. (Don’t say too much or
too little.)
• Make the strongest statement you can.
Quality
• Do not say what you believe to be false.
• Do not say that for which you lack adequate evidence.
Relation
• Be relevant. (Stay on topic.)
Manner
• Avoid obscurity of expression.
• Avoid ambiguity.
112
• Be brief (avoid unnecessary prolixity).
• Be orderly. xiv
The simplest way to think of Grice’s maxims is general rules we follow in conversation.
However, that’s not entirely accurate. The interesting thing about these “rules” is that often, we
don’t follow them.
Maxim Violations
There are several ways/reasons a speaker might break one of the rules:
2. Signaling a violation (minor violation). A person might essentially come out and tell
you they are violating a maxim and why.
Examples.
3. Maxim clash. A speaker might violate one maxim in order to preserve another.
Example.
Examples.
113
Maxim Violated: Quantity; Elizabeth didn’t give as much information as John wanted
(Meredith’s exact location), but instead gave a weaker statement (giving two possible options).
Implication: Elizabeth doesn’t know which of the two places Meredith is.
114
and the natural language statement
All we know about the logical statements is that at least one of the propositions is true. But we
know more than that about the natural language statement: we know (or, we have a good reason
to assume) that the speaker doesn’t know which of the two places Meredith is, or they would say
so. According to the maxim of quantity, a person shouldn’t give a weaker statement when a
stronger one is available. But logical language has no such rules about cooperation. If we used
strictly logical language, the weaker statement would be equally acceptable, as long as it was
true. (This natural/logical language distinction also explains why we get so mad when people do
smartass things like say “Yes” to the question “Can you pass the salt?” We then fix them with a
withering glare and say “You know what I mean.”)
Some people have argued that natural language is inferior to logical language because it is
less precise and more ambiguous, but using the maxims and the idea of implicature, Grice argued
that natural language, while different, is just as good. Indeed, more often than not, listeners do
know what the speaker means, even if it’s not explicitly stated.
It looks like this is a quantity violation (too much information), but it could also be
argued that it is a violation of relation (since the extra information Meredith volunteers is
largely irrelevant to the question John asked). It could further be argued that this lengthy
tirade violates manner, since it’s unnecessarily prolix (wordy). It’s also probable that
Meredith is exaggerating about the level of seriousness of the systems failures and about
killing that guy, so a quality violation is also likely.
115
Exercises
Each problem presents a short dialogue. You must identify which a maxim is being used or
violated. You may be asked to figure out the implication, or it may be given to you.
3. JOHN: We just have to fly real close to the corona of the sun!
MEREDITH: You’re lucky you’re pretty.
What is Meredith implying?
What maxim creates that implication, and why?
116
Speech Acts
Objective: Given a speech act, give the type of illocutionary force (according to Searle’s
classification). Tell whether the speech act is explicit or nonexplicit. Given a failed speech act,
identify the felicity condition(s) not met.
Definitions
John Austin (1955) introduced the idea of speech acts, utterances which have some effect
beyond simply stating information. For example, if you say “I’m sorry for accidentally killing
your cat,” you’re not just saying something—you’re apologizing. If you say “I dub thee Sir
Mopes-a-lot,” you may (under the right circumstances) be giving someone a new name.
Speech act: A speech event or utterance. Each speech act is comprised of:
• Locutionary act: The act of saying something.
• Illocutionary act (or illocutionary force): What the speech act does (See “Searle’s
Classification of Illocutionary Acts.”)
An easy way to tell the difference: explicit speech acts can have the word “hereby” inserted
before the essential verb. So “I (hereby) call dibs” is an explicit speech act, whereas “I’ll call
you” isn’t (*“I’ll hereby call you” doesn’t work.)
Note that statements which, on their own, are not speech acts, can become (nonexplicit)
speech acts in the right context. For example, the word “Me!” is a speech act when it comes after
the question “Who will help me bake the bread?” (since it commits the speaker to help bake the
bread).
117
• Expressive. Speaker expresses an emotional state.
Examples: “I’m sorry I forgot to feed your cat.” “I’m so disappointed in you.” “I’m sorry
for your loss.” “Congratulations on your engagement!” “Welcome to the neighborhood!”
• Declaration. Speaker changes something’s status.
Examples: “I now pronounce you man and wife.” “I absolve you of your sins.” “We
surrender!” “You are sentenced to five years hard labor.”
Felicity Conditions
Speech acts require certain conditions to be met in order for the illocutionary force to take effect.
• Preparatory conditions: The speaker must have the necessary ability, authority, and
beliefs.
Failure example: At a baseball game, I scream, “You’re out!” But since I’m not the
umpire, nothing happens.
• Sincerity conditions: The speaker must mean it.
Failure example: My roommate asks me to do the dishes and I say “Yes,” but I have no
intention of actually doing it.
• Essential conditions: The hearer recognizes the intention of the speech act.
Failure example: Intending to propose marriage, I tell my girlfriend, “Let me share your
burden.” She thinks for a minute, and then hands me her books.
• Propositional content conditions: The propositions contained in the wording of the
speech act must be true.
Failure example: I say “I’m sorry I broke the lamp” when I have not, in fact, broken a
lamp.
Exercises
Consider each situation, and answer the questions.
118
a. What is the illocutionary force of Meredith’s (first) speech act?
b. Is Meredith’s speech act explicit or nonexplicit?
c. Are the felicity conditions met? If not, why not?
119
Going Beyond
Consider the speech act of calling shotgun (claiming for oneself the right to sit in the passenger
seat on car trip.) The website http://www.shotgunrules.com/ gives a number of rules limited the
power of the speech act, including, but not limited to:
• “You must say the word ‘Shotgun’ .... clearly and loud enough so that at least one other
to-be occupant of the vehicle can hear you.”
• “You must be outside to call Shotgun.”
• The deed (whatever you came in the car to do) must be done. “There is no crime greater
than calling Shotgun on Monday in reference to the ride to the concert on Friday. Some
people choose to play this way, and they are fools.” xvii
1. What kind of speech act is calling shotgun? That is to say, what illocutionary force
classification would you give it?
2. What is the distinction between saying “Shotgun!” and “I call shotgun!”?
3. Suppose my friends and I go to the mall and in the middle of the food court I try to call
shotgun for the way back. What felicity condition has been broken? What if I mutter
“shotgun” so quietly that nobody hears me?
4. In what circumstances would the utterance “Shotgun!” not be the kind of speech act
described here?
120
Brown & Levinson’s Politeness Theory
About Face
According to Penelope Brown & Stephen Levinson (1987) xviii , politeness is a linguistic and
behavior tool we use to help each other save face. Face refers to a person’s self-esteem or self-
respect. There are two types of face:
• Positive face is the desire to be appreciated by others. A person’s positive face depends
on their ability to show themselves and others that they are well-liked.
• Negative face is the desire to maintain autonomy. A person’s negative face depends on
their ability to show themselves and others that they do what they do because they choose
to; they’re independent, and not subordinate to anybody.
We use politeness to avoid or minimize face-threatening acts, or any act which infringes on the
listener’s ability to maintain face. For example, trying to get someone to do something for you
can be a face-threatening act (primarily, threatening their autonomy.)
Politeness Strategies
Four strategies, in ascending order of politeness:
Example Problems
1. Name the politeness strategy being used in each of the following utterances.
a. “You look ravishing today.”
Positive politeness. The speaker is complimenting the listener, building up their
positive face.
b. “I’m so busy, I don’t know how I’ll find time to get the dishes done.”
Indirect. Rather than telling or asking the listener to do the dishes, the speaker is
mentioning conversationally that it would be nice if they got done, hoping the
listener will take the hint and volunteer to do them.
121
c. “I apologize for taking up so much of your time.”
Negative politeness. The speaker is assuming that they have infringed on the
listener, showing respect for the listener’s time and independence.
d. “You’ll eat your peas and you’ll like it, mister.”
Bald on-record. This is a direct order, and no attempt to protect the listener’s face
is made. This is fairly obviously a request made by someone in a position of
power to a subordinate (e.g., parent to child).
Exercises
1. Name the politeness strategy being used in each of the following utterances.
a. “Well, I know you have a lot going on, so I’ll let you go.”
b. “You need to go pick up the dry-cleaning.”
c. “You’re such a sweetheart, I know I can count on you.”
d. “Mmm, those cookies smell good.”
2. Suppose you want someone to loan you five dollars. Give an example of four ways you
could request the money, one from each politeness strategy.
122
Answers to Exercises
Phonetics
IPA Transcription
Phonology
Natural Classes
Features
Minimal Pairs
Environments
Determining the Underlying Phoneme
Analyzing Data Sets
Phonological Processes
Morphology
Subcategorization Frames
Trees
Zero Derivation
Agglutinating Language Puzzle
Syntax
Constituency Tests
Semantics
Theta Roles
Tautologies, Contradictions, and Contingencies
Sense & Reference
Presupposition & Entailment
Pragmatics
Maxims, Maxim Violations & Implications
Speech Acts
Brown & Levinson’s Politeness Theory
123
IPA Transcription
I. Consonants
1. Write the IPA symbol for the first sound in these words:
sniffles s bronchitis b
cold k pneumonia n
flu f death d
2. Write the IPA symbol for the last sound in these words:
rash concussion n
lacerations z hemorrhage d
fracture r death
3. Write the IPA symbol for the highlighted sound in these words:
alcoholism h hemophilia f
diabetes b cancer s
hypothyroidism chronic death... syndrome k
II. Vowels
1. Write the IPA symbol for the first sound in these words (just the first sound of the first
word is fine if it’s a phrase):
obesity o or amnesia æ or
epilepsy aphasia
autism attention deficit disorder
2. Write the IPA symbol for the last sound in these words (just the last sound of the last
word is fine if it’s a phrase):
dystrophy i sciatica
polio o punched in the jaw
3. Write the IPA symbol for the highlighted sound in these words:
borderline aj schizoid
bipolar o paranoid oj
obsessive compulsive antisocial
124
fluffy bunnies flfi bniz sunshiney rainbows snajni renboz
pretty unicorns pri junkorn grim death and despair
rm d ænd dsper
Do no harm.
du no harm
A few days with a cooperative test subject could be worth months of theoretical research.
maj hajpolajsimi z nat mædnd. aj wz dstktli tld æt aj hæd bld r prablm
My hypoglycemia is not imagined. I was distinctly told that I had a blood sugar problem.
125
V. Going Beyond
1. If you know another language, what (if any) sounds in that language cannot be
represented using the symbols you now know?
2. Here’s a passage from another dialect of English. Try transcribing into standard
orthography. Can you guess what kind of dialect this is? ( is a new vowel sound.
To tell what it sounds like, try saying the word “nurse” without the “r”.)
fst rul v dntks: sprd dinz pa, j no! bt rojlz a bsst w,
“a ju rojl fæmli? a ju rojl mmb? wl n ju kn mæri mi kz j sem din pul,
nd a aj kjuz wl o dawn tojlt.” fæntæstk! ts waj e a no krezi rojlz,
e dst kajnd v, “hlo! hlo, wt d ju du? o, j plm! wt an z æt?” xix
First rule of genetics: spread the genes apart, you know! But the royals are obsessed with, “Are
you a royal family? Are you a royal member? Well then you can marry me cause you’re same
gene pool, and our IQs will go down the toilet.” Fantastic! That’s why there are no crazy royals,
they’re just kind of, “Hello! Hello, what do you do? Oh, you’re a plumber! What on Earth is
that?”
Natural Classes
I. List all the sounds in each class.
a. Voiceless – p, t, k, f, , s, , t, h
b. Fricative – f, v, , , s, z, h
c. Velar – k, ,
d. Alveolar stop -- t, d, n
e. Velar nasal --
f. Mid – e, o, , ,
g. Round – u, , o,
h. Low back non-round – a
i. High tense – i, u
II. Determine whether the following sets of sounds are in a natural class. If so, what is
the class?
a. {f, , s, , h} – yes, voiceless fricatives
b. {z, t, d} – no
c. {p, b, m, w} – yes, bilabial consonants
d. {b, d, , } – no
e. {u, , o} – no
f. {æ, a, } – yes, low vowels
g. {i, , } – no
h. {, , } – mid lax vowels
126
Features
Minimal Pairs
1. Are the following pairs of English words minimal pairs?
a. law, jaw l, d minimal pair (sound change between l & d)
b. crime, time krajm, taim not a minimal pair (two sound differences)
c. prison, prism prsn, prsm minimal pair (sound change between n & m)
2. Consider this data set from Thai, and determine whether the given pairs are minimal pairs or
not.
a. ba (“sheet”), pa (“to go”) minimal pair (sound change between b & p)
b. bryy (“very fast”), myy (“hand”) not a minimal pair (r is contrasted with ø)
c. pa (“to go”), pa (“danger”) minimal pair (change between p and p)
127
Environments
1. Consider the following data from English (use the IPA transcriptions, not the standard
orthography glosses.) According to this data, are [] and [] allophones of the same phoneme, or
different phonemes?
Environments:
o_# #_o
#_ æ_
æ_# #_e
o_#
#_
There is overlap on both sides (on the left, [o], [æ] and # occur on both lists; on the right, [] and
# occur on both lists). This is contrastive distribution. Therefore, [] and [] are different
phonemes.
2. Consider the following data from English. According to this data, are [n] and [] allophones of
the same phoneme, or different phonemes?
mntl “mental” æw “anguish”
nsttun “institution” træk “tranq”
lr “linger” jn “yawn”
hændl “handle”
Environments:
n
_t _
_s æ_
_# æ_k
æ_d
_#
128
There is overlap on the left, since [] and [æ] occur on both lists. However, there is no overlap on
the right. This is complementary distribution. According to this data, [n] and [] are allophones.
3. Consider the following data from Ganda. According to this data, are [l] and [r] allophones of
the same phoneme, or different phonemes?
Environments:
l
o_a r
#_w i_a
u_i i_o
#_y e_a
#_u e_y
u_a i_i
o_e e_i
a_i
o_u
#_a
There is overlap on the right, since [a], [o], [i], and [y] occur on both lists. However, there is no
overlap on the left. This is complementary distribution. According to this data, [l] and [r] are
allophones in Ganda.
Going Beyond: In English, are [n] and [] allophones of the same phoneme, or different
phonemes? Give data to support your answer. Does your answer support or conflict with your
answer to exercise #2? If there is a conflict, how might you explain it?
129
They are different phonemes. You can tell from minimal pairs like
where switching between /n/ and // results in different words, with different meanings.
There are a few explanations for the results in question 2. One simple one is that, by
manipulation of the data chosen, a phoneme pair can be made to look like an allophone pair
(although not vice versa. Can you explain why?) You just have to choose words where the
phonemes are shown in different contexts.
In actuality, this is a stable, rule-based difference in English. Although /n/ and // are different
phonemes in English, /n/ shows up as [] in certain situations. In the “Rules” section, you are
asked to write the rule which governs this.
Given the answer to this question in the previous section, we know that only the right side of the
environments is relevant for deciding which is the underlying phoneme. These environments are:
n
_t _
_s æ_
_# æ_k
æ_d
_#
The right-side environment of [] consists of [, k], which are both velar consonants. The right-
side environment of [n] can’t be grouped into one category, so [n] is the underlying phoneme.
130
2. Consider the following data from Ganda. According to this data, which sound of [l] and [r] is
the underlying phoneme?
Given the environments from the answer in the previous section, we know that the relevant side
is the left one (there is overlap on the right):
l r
o_a i_a
#_w i_o
u_i e_a
#_y e_y
#_u i_i
u_a e_i
o_e
a_i
o_u
#_a
The left-side environment of [r] consists of [i, e], which are both front vowels. The left-side
environment of [l] can’t be grouped into one category, so [l] is the underlying phoneme.
Going Beyond
1. Explain why we didn’t ask for the underlying phoneme of the first exercise from that section.
We didn’t ask for the underlying phoneme of the first exercise because in that exercise, there is
overlap on both sides of the sound, which means that the sounds both occur in all environments,
which makes them phonemes. There can be no underlying phoneme if there is no systematic
difference in the environment using which one can predict the appearance of one sound instead
of another.
131
2. Consider the Going Beyond question of the previous section (“Environments”): “In English,
are [n] and [] allophones of the same phoneme, or different phonemes? Give data to support
your answer. Does your answer support or conflict with your answer to exercise #2? If there is a
conflict, how might you explain it?” Does your answer to this question provide you with more
data which supports or contradicts the decision to label either [n] or [] as the underlying
phoneme?
Upon further thought, it is easy to see that actually, [n] are [] different phonemes in English (see
the above answer for proof). However, if one considers the circumstances in which [n] and []
are actually in allophonic distribution, it is evident that [n] is the underlying phoneme if one
considers how often the sound [n] is encountered versus the sound []. So, it is possible to argue
both ways: one, that the data is not complete, and that both [n] and [] are phonemes, or two, that
the data show one of the relationships that exist in English between [n] and [] and that this
relationship is correctly portrayed by the underlying phoneme [n] becoming the allophone [] in
certain environments.
Rules
1. Consider the following data from English. According to this data, what is the rule that predicts
the occurrence of the [n] vs. the [] sound?
Since we decided (see the answers to the last two sections) that [n] is the underlying phoneme,
we just need to describe the environment in which the allophone [] occurs. So:
In symbols: /n/ [] / _{, k}
In features: {+nasal, +alveolar} {+velar} / {+velar}_
2. Consider the following data from Ganda. According to this data, what is the rule that predicts
the occurrence of the [r] vs. the [l] sound?
132
omuliro ‘fire’ eraddu ‘lightning’
effirimbi ‘whistle’ wawaabira ‘accuse’
emmeeri ‘ship’ laira ‘command’
Since we decided (see the answers to the last two sections) that [l] is the underlying phoneme, we
just need to describe the environment in which the allophone [r] occurs. So:
In symbols: /l/ [r] / {i, e}_
In features: {+lateral} {-lateral} / {+front, +tense}_
Going Beyond
1. In example problem 2, when we developed a rule for plural pronunciation, we decided [z] was
the basic underlying phoneme. Given what the rule turned out to be, what’s an alternate
explanation for why [z] is in wider distribution than [s]? Could [z] and [s] actually be equally
ranked (neither one more “underlying” than the other, or you can’t tell)?
One could argue that [z] is in wider distribution than [s] because there are more voiced sounds in
English than there are voiceless sounds (consider that most consonants and all vowels are
+voice). However, because all the voiced sounds can be put in a class (namely, +voice) and all
the voiceless sounds can be put in another class (-voice), [z] is not actually in a wider
distribution than [s]. If viewed in this manner, [z] and [s] are equally ranked.
133
Step 3: Make the decision: phonemes or allophones?
Since there is no overlap on at least one side, this is complementary distribution. According to
this data, [n] and [] are allophones.
2. Consider the following data from English (use the IPA transcriptions, not the standard
orthography glosses.) According to this data, are [] and [] allophones of the same phoneme, or
different phonemes (if allophones, provide the rule in symbol and feature notations)?
o_# #_o
#_ æ_
æ_# #_e
o_#
#_
134
Step 4: Write the rule (symbol and feature notation)
This step cannot be completed because it only applies to allophones.
3. Consider the following data from Ganda. According to this data, are [r] and [l] phonemes or
allophones (if allophones, provide the rule in symbol and feature notations):
kola ‘do’ wulira ‘hear’
lwana ‘fight’ beera ‘help’
buulira ‘tell’ jjukira ‘remember’
lya ‘eat’ eryato ‘canoe’
luula ‘sit’ omuliro ‘fire’
omuole ‘bride’ effirimbi ‘whistle’
lumonde ‘sweet potato’ emmeeri ‘ship’
eddwaliro ‘hospital’ eraddu ‘lightning’
oluanda ‘Ganda language’ wawaabira ‘accuse’
olulimi ‘tongue’ laira ‘command’
135
4b. Write the rule:
In symbols: /l/ [r] / {i, e}_
In features: {+lateral} {-lateral} / {+front, +tense}_
4. Consider this data from Tongan, a Polynesian language. Are [s] and [t] different phonemes or
allophones of the same phoneme (if allophones, provide the rule in symbol and feature
notations)?
s t
#_i #_a
i_i o_u
o_i o_o
e_i #_o
a_i a_a
e_e
136
Going Beyond: Consider this data from Tojolabal. Are [t] and [t] separate phonemes or
allophones of the same phoneme? If allophones, what rule specifies their distribution?
Environments:
t t
i_a a_#
k_o u_#
o_o
#_i
This is clearly complementary distribution, but there’s no overlap on both sides! How do you
formulate a rule here? You have a couple of options.
(1) Include both sides in your rule.
The problem with this rule is that it is extremely specific. If we had more data, we would
probably see that only one of the sides was really important. Additionally, there’s the problem
that [a] and [u] don’t really go together very well, which we’ll see if we:
(2) Decide that the left side is more important here and write the rule as follows:
If you can find some similarity between [a] and [u] which is not shared by the sounds that
precede /t/ ([i], [k], [o]) and write the rule in features, that might lend some support.
If you check out the feature chart, you’ll see that there’s no similarity shared by [a] and [u] and
excluding [o] ([u] is the high version of [o], and [a] is low). This rule doesn’t look promising.
There’s no reason for [a] and [u] alone to be trigger environments when [o] is not. Let’s see if we
have better luck when we:
137
(3) Decide that the right side is more important here and write the rule as follows:
/t/ [t] / _#
“/t/ becomes [t] before a word boundary”
To me, this seems like the best rule. It’s simple and explainable. However, we’d need more data
to know for sure.
Phonological Processes
1. Consider the following data from a dialect of English sometimes spoken in Eastern
Massachusetts (“:” next to a vowel indicates a long vowel); what phonological process
could account for the transformation from underlying to surface form? Provide the rule:
The change from the underlying form to the surface form is in the addition, in all these words, of
the sound []. Since a sound is added, we know that this is epenthesis. Now, to come up with the
rule; let’s see what the immediate environment of the added sound is like:
:_
_#
_
_
We can’t classify the right side – all sorts of things appear there, including vowels, consonants,
and a word edge. However, the left side only has vowels; not only that, all the vowels are non-
high vowels. So, we can formulate a rule that states that [] is inserted after a non-high vowel:
Ø [] / [-high]V_
2. Consider the following data from English; what differences in pronunciation do you find
among the underlying word and the word with an attached suffix? Pay specific attention
to the last sound in the root word. What phonological process could account for the
transformation? Provide the rule:
When we look at the pronunciation of the two columns, we notice that in the first column, we do
not pronounce the last sound (d, , b), but in the second column we do. Why is this? The answer
138
is a little tricky because it relies on the information provided in the second column; namely, that
these last sounds do in fact exist underlyingly (otherwise, why would they appear in the second
column?). So, we have: (d, , b) are pronounced in the middle of the word, but not at the end.
We could formulate the rule that these sounds are deleted at the end of the word, and this would
be acceptable given the data set. However, if we think a little bit, we can come up with words
that end in (d, , b) and would therefore negate our rule: for example, tab, beg, shod. So, it must
be more precise than that. The only thing left to do is look at the other side of the environment:
the preceding sounds are all nasals. So, our more precise rule would state that (d, , b), or voiced
stops, are deleted at the end of a word if they follow a nasal:
/d, , b/ Ø / [+nasal]C_#
3. Consider the following data from English; keep in mind that all of these words have the
same underlying prefix: “con-”. What phonological process could account for the
transformation from underlying to surface form of the prefix “con-”? Provide the rule:
conjoin commerce
contraction compose
contemplate comply
conspire combine
configure
conclude
Since we know that “con-” is the underlying form, we must explain why in the second column, it
becomes “com-”. We can examine the environments immediately near the changed sound, “n/m”
to see if it provides a clue:
n m
_d _m
_t _p
_s _b
_f
_k
Since the environments on the left are the same, we focus on the environment on the right. The
environment of [n] consists of all kinds of consonants, whereas the environment of [m] is very
narrow: it only contains labial consonants. We can notice, then, that [n] is an alveolar sound, and
[m] is a labial sound. So, the alveolar [n] becomes the labial [m] when it precedes labial sounds.
This is assimilation because the sound becomes more like its environment.
/n/ [m] / _ [+labial]C
139
4. Consider the following data from English; keep in mind that all of these words have the
same underlying suffix: “-al”. What phonological process could account for the
transformation from underlying to surface form of the suffix “-al”? Provide the rule:
Noun Adjective Noun Adjective
person personal pole polar
region regional circle circular
autumn autumnal single singular
cause causal molecule molecular
Since we are given that the underlying suffix is “-al”, we must explain why in the second
column, the suffix changes to “-ar”. When we examine the environments, we have to look at the
last sound of the noun, since that is what the suffix attaches to:
-al -ar
n_ l_
m_
z_
Now, it is clear that “-ar” occurs after [l]. Why would “-al” change to “-ar” after [l]? Try
pronouncing “-al” after the nouns in the second column. Is it easy?
The difference between “-al” and “-ar” is in the final sound; one is +lateral, the other is –lateral.
The final sounds in the “-al” column are –lateral. The final sounds in the “-ar” column are
+lateral. So, the +lateral suffix changes to –lateral when the final sound in the word is +lateral.
This is dissimilation, because the sound change makes the sound more different from its
environment.
/al/ [ar] / [+lateral]C _
140
Subcategorization Frames
1. Give subcategorization frames for the following English morphemes, giving your evidence.
(Evidence will vary.)
Trees
Make morphology trees for the following words:
a. unmentionable
Adj
Adj
V
un + mention + able
b. revelations
N
V
reveal + (a)tion + s
c. undeniably
Adv
Adj
Adj
V
un + deny + able + ly
d. untieable
141
not able to be tied:
Adj
Adj
V
un + tie + able
able to be untied:
Adj
V
un + tie + able
e. deactivating
Adj
V
de + act + ive + ate + ing
142
Zero Derivation
1. Provide tree structures for the following words:
a. “actioning” as in, “I’ll talk to Todd in accounting and see about actioning that for
you.”
V
act + tion + Ø + ing
-tion: V N
Ø: N V
-ing: inflectional verb tense marker
N
table + Ø
Ø: N V
2. Provide tree structures for the first two words in the sentence “Verbing weirds language.”
N
verb + Ø1 + ing + Ø2
Ø 1: N V
-ing: inflectional verb tense marker
Ø 2: V N
143
V
Adj
weird + Ø + s
Ø: Adj V
-s: inflectional verb tense marker
Order of attachment:
subject + object + root + tense + aspect
144
Constituency Tests
Decide if the underlined portion of the following sentences is a constituent (show the
constituency tests used):
1. Meredith mocked John’s solutions in the science lab. YES
Substitution: Meredith mocked his solutions in the science lab.
Q/A: Whose solutions did Meredith mock in the science lab? John’s.
Cleft: (*) It was John’s that Meredith mocked solutions in the science lab.
Pseudocleft: (?) The solutions that Meredith mocked in the science lab were John’s.
2. Meredith mocked John’s solutions in the science lab. NO
Substitution: (*) ? his solutions in the science lab.
Q/A: (*) What happened to John’s solutions in the science lab? Meredith mocked.
Cleft: (*) It was Meredith mocked that John’s solutions in the science lab.
Pseudocleft: (*) The thing that happened to John’s solutions in the science lab was
Meredith mocked.
3. Meredith mocked John’s solutions in the science lab. YES
Substitution: Meredith mocked John’s solutions there.
Q/A: Where did Meredith mock John’s solutions? In the science lab.
Cleft: It was in the science lab that Meredith mocked John’s solutions.
Pseudocleft: The place where Meredith mocked John’s solutions was in the science lab.
4. Meredith mocked John’s solutions in the science lab. NO
Substitution: (*) Meredith mocked John’s them in the science lab.
Q/A: (*) What of John’s did Meredith mock in the science lab? Solutions.
Cleft: (*) It was solutions that Meredith mocked John’s in the science lab.
Pseudocleft: (*) The thing of John’s that Meredith mocked in the science lab was
solutions.
5. Meredith mocked John’s solutions in the science lab. YES
Substitution: Meredith mocked John’s solutions in the science lab. Jeannie did so as
well.
Q/A: (?) What did Meredith do? Mocked John’s solutions in the science lab.
Cleft: It was mock John’s solutions in the science lab that Meredith did.
Pseudocleft: What Meredith did was mock John’s solutions in the science lab.
6. Meredith mocked John’s solutions in the science lab. YES/NO (depending if “in the
science lab” refers to solutions or to mocking).
Substitution: (?) Meredith mocked them.
Q/A: (?) What did Meredith mock? John’s solutions in the science lab.
Cleft: (?) It was John’s solutions in the science lab that Meredith mocked.
Pseudocleft: (?) What Meredith mocked were John’s solutions in the science lab.
7. Meredith mocked John’s solutions in the science lab. YES/NO (depending if “in the
science lab” refers to solutions or to mocking).
Substitution: (?) Meredith mocked them in the science lab.
Q/A: (?) Whose did Meredith mock in the science lab? John’s solutions.
Cleft: (?) It was John’s solutions that Meredith mocked in the science lab.
Pseudocleft: (?)What Meredith mocked in the science lab were John’s solutions.
145
Lexical Categories
Exercises: Label the words in the following sentences with appropriate labels.
1. N V N
Meredith loves chocolate.
2. N V Adj N
Meredith loves chocolate truffles.
4. N V Adj N C N V P D N
Meredith loves chocolate truffles that John brought to the city.
6. N P N V D Adj N P D Adj N
Scientists from Canada detest the Antarctic winter with a fiery passion.
Phrase Structure/Trees
Using the structure rules above, draw the following trees:
1.
IP
I VP
[pres]
NP NP
N V N
Meredith loves chocolate.
2.
IP
VP
I NP
[pres]
NP AdjP
N V Adj N
Meredith loves chocolate truffles.
146
3.
IP
VP
NP
PP
NP
I
[pres]
AdjP
NP AdjP AdjP
4.
IP
VP
I
[pres] NP
NP NP NP
N V N Conj N
Meredith loves cookies and milk.
147
Tree for Meaning 1:
IP
NP VP
I
[past] PP
NP NP
N V D N P D N
John shot the marine with the gun.
IP
NP VP
I NP
[past] PP
NP
N V D N P D N
John shot the marine with the gun.
148
2. Meredith likes chocolate truffles and cake.
Meaning 1: Meredith likes chocolate truffles and chocolate cake.
Meaning 2: Meredith likes chocolate truffles and any kind of cake at all.
IP
NP VP
I
[pres]
NP
AdjP NP NP
N V Adj N Conj N
Meredith likes chocolate truffles and cake.
NP VP
I
[pres] NP
NP NP
AdjP
N V Adj N Conj N
Meredith likes chocolate truffles and cake.
149
3. John burned the picture of Elizabeth with her ex-boyfriend. (three possible meanings here,
one requires treating “with” as a conjunction)
Meaning 1: John and Elizabeth’s ex-boyfriend burned her picture together (“with her ex-
boyfriend” attaches to the “burned” VP).
Meaning 2: John burned the picture that showed Elizabeth and her ex-boyfriend (“with her ex-
boyfriend” attaches to the “Elizabeth” NP).
Meaning 3: John burned the picture of Elizabeth at the same time as he burned Elizabeth’s ex-
boyfriend (here, “with” function as “and”). This third meaning is actually a different kind of
ambiguity (it’s lexical ambiguity about the word “with”, not necessarily a structural ambiguity).
Nonetheless, it is included here, because it is a common reading of this sentence which does
actually have two addition syntactic ambiguities (Meaning 1 and 2).
IP
NP VP
I
[past] NP
PP PP
NP NP
N V D N P N P D N
John burned the picture of Elizabeth with her ex-boyfriend
150
Tree for Meaning 2:
IP
NP VP
I
[past] NP
PP
NP
PP
NP
N V D N P N P D N
John burned the picture of Elizabeth with her ex-boyfriend
IP
NP VP
I
[past] NP
NP
PP
NP NP
N V D N P N Conj D N
John burned the picture of Elizabeth with her ex-boyfriend
151
Complex Sentences
Construct the trees for the following complex sentences:
1. IP
NP VP
I
[pres] CP
IP
NP VP
I
[past]
NP
PP PP
NP NP
N V C N V D N P N P D N
John insists that Meredith brought the cake without citrus to the party.
2.
IP
NP I VP
[past]
CP
IP
NP VP
I
[past] PP
NP NP
D N C N V P D N V Quant N
The announcement that Meredith went to the party raised some eyebrows.
152
3. IP
NP VP
I
[past] NP CP
IP
NP VP
I
[past] PP
NP
N V D N C N V P D N
John played the game because Meredith insisted on his participation.
4. IP
NP VP
I
[pres] CP
IP
NP VP
I
[past]
NP
PP PP
NP NP
N V C N V D N P N P N
John thinks Ø Meredith sang the song about love for him
153
5a. Meaning: John heard yesterday that Meredith ate the cake.
IP
NP VP
I
[past] CP
IP
NP VP
I
[past] NP AdvP
N V C N V D N Adv
John heard that Meredith ate the cake yesterday.
IP
NP VP
I
[past] CP
IP
NP VP
I
[past] NP AdvP
N V C N V D N Adv
John heard that Meredith ate the cake yesterday.
154
Transformations
Draw the underlying and surface structure of the following sentences:
CP
Spec C
IP
NP I VP
NP
AP
N mod V A N
Meredith does love chocolate truffles
Surface structure:
CP
Spec C
IP
NP I VP
NP
AP
N mod V A N
Does Meredith tx love chocolate truffles
inversion
155
2. Who did John sell the device to?
Underlying structure: John did sell the device to who(m).
CP
Spec C
IP
NP I VP
PP
NP NP
N mod V D N P
John did sell the device to who
Surface structure:
CP
Spec C
IP
NP I VP
PP
NP NP
N mod V D N P
Whoy didx John tx sell the device to ty
inversion
wh- movement
156
3. Where did John sell the device?
Underlying structure: John did sell the device where.
CP
Spec C
IP
NP I VP
PP
NP
N mod V D N
John did sell the device where
Surface structure:
CP
Spec C
IP
NP I VP
PP
NP
N mod V D N
Wherey didx John tx sell the device ty
inversion
wh- movement
157
4. When will Meredith yell at the scientists?
Underlying structure: Meredith will yell at the scientists when
CP
Spec C
IP
NP I VP
PP AdvP
NP
N mod V P D N
Meredith will yell at the scientists when
Surface structure:
CP
Spec C
IP
NP I VP
PP AdvP
NP
N mod V P D N
Wheny willx Meredith tx yell at the scientists ty
inversion
wh- movement
158
Theta Roles
Label all the underlined phrases in the following sentences with the appropriate theta roles.
1. The explosion took out most of the western pier in the city.
The explosion is the cause (it’s not an agent or an experiencer, but it still performs the
action of “taking out”/destroying).
Most of the western pier is the patient (not theme, because presumably most of the
western pier is destroyed as a result of the explosion).
In the city is the location of the explosion.
2. Meredith activated the weapon from a distance using a special device.
Meredith is the agent (activation requires deliberate action on the part of the agent).
The weapon is the theme (the recipient of the activating action).
From a distance is the source because it indicates where the action came from.
Special device is the instrument with which the action was performed.
3. John carried the shiny toy from the closet to the office with the tongs.
John is an agent (carrying requires deliberate action).
The shiny toy is the theme (not patient, because presumably the act of carrying does not
change the properties of the toy).
From the closet is the source of the action.
To the office is the goal of the action.
With the tongs is the instrument of the action.
159
Sense & Reference
1. Identify the sense and reference of the following phrases.
c. Prime Minister of Canada: The sense is the actual office of the leader of the
Canadian government. As of this writing, the referent is Stephen Harper.
d. the last team to win the World Series: The sense is whatever baseball team
most recently won the World Series, which varies from year to year. As of this
writing, the referent is the St. Louis Cardinals.
e. my (the exercise-doer’s) favorite fruit: The sense is whatever fruit the exercise
doer prefers. The referent varies from person to person, time to time. For me, the
referent is a peach.
2. Consider the statements “Meredith’s boyfriend is not Meredith’s boyfriend” and “John is
not Meredith’s boyfriend.”
f. What is the referent of each of the underlined phrases? The referent for each
phrase is John.
g. What kind of statements are these (tautology, contradiction, or contingency)?
If they are different, why? The first phrase is a contradiction; the second is a
contingency. Although John is currently the referent for the phrase “Meredith’s
boyfriend”, the sense is simply whoever is currently fulfilling the role, which need
not be John (Meredith’s boyfriend might imaginably be somebody else). So, the
sentence “John is Meredith’s boyfriend” is contingency, because it need not be
true; stating it is informative. On the other hand, whoever is currently fulfilling
the role of Meredith’s boyfriend, that person is never anybody other than himself.
It is unnecessary to know the referent of “Meredith’s boyfriend” to determine that
“Meredith’s boyfriend is not Meredith’s boyfriend” is a contradiction.
1A. John did not realize that Meredith was working in Antarctica.
1B. Meredith was in Antarctica.
Presupposition.
160
5A. The rumor that John loves Meredith is true.
5B. It is rumored that John loves Meredith.
Presupposition.
Mutual Entailment: In the following sets of statements, does A entail B, does B entail A, does
both A entail B and B entail A (mutual entailment), or does neither entail the other?
161
3. JOHN: We just have to fly real close to the corona of the sun!
MEREDITH: You’re lucky you’re pretty.
What is Meredith implying?
John’s idea is stupid.
What maxim creates that implication, and why?
Relation. Meredith is going off topic, talking about John’s looks rather than his idea.
Speech Acts
1. JOHN: Are you busy?
MEREDITH: Could you not hover while I’m trying to work?
a. What is the illocutionary force of John’s speech act?
Question (subcategory of Directive). John is asking for information.
b. What is the illocutionary force of Meredith’s speech act?
Directive. Meredith is trying to get John to quit hovering.
162
3. ELIZABETH: You endangered the lives of everyone on this ship!
MEREDITH: Whoops.
ELIZABETH: Is that supposed to be an apology?
MEREDITH: That wasn’t clear?
a. What is the illocutionary force of Meredith’s (first) speech act?
Expressive. From context, it appears Meredith was attempting an apology.
b. Is Meredith’s speech act explicit or nonexplicit?
Nonexplicit. An explicit version would be saying, “I (hereby) apologize.”
c. Are the felicity conditions met? If not, why not?
No (not at first, anyway). The essential conditions are not met. Elizabeth isn’t sure that
it’s supposed to be an apology.
163
a. What is the illocutionary force of Meredith’s speech act?
This could be a read few different ways. To me, the most natural is that this is Meredith’s
way of naming the whale, in which case it’s a declarative. However, it could also be
considered a statement of fact (representative): perhaps Meredith has already officially
named the whale and is just informing John about it. It could even be read as a
commissive: Meredith is promising to name the whale “Sam” at some later date.
b. Is Meredith’s speech act explicit or nonexplicit?
If you read this as a declarative, then the phrase “going to call” is basically synonymous
with “name”/“baptize”/“declare” and the speech act is explicit. In the other cases, it looks
more like a nonexplicit speech act. The explicit version of the representative reading
would be something like “I declare that I am going to call him ‘Sam’.” The explicit
version of the comissive reading is “I swear to name him ‘Sam’.”
c. Are the felicity conditions met? If not, why not?
As far as we know, they are met, so long as Meredith actually does call the whale “Sam”
from then on. Whether Meredith possesses the authority to name a whale is debatable
(probably not from the whale’s point of view.)
Going Beyond
1. What kind of speech act is calling shotgun? That is to say, what illocutionary force
classification would you give it? I would be most likely to call it a declarative—staking
a claim on shotgun could be seen as changing your own status (non-shotgun-haver to
shotgun-haver) or the status of shotgun (not-yours to yours). I’m open to other
convincing explanations, however.
2. What is the distinction between saying “Shotgun!” and “I call shotgun!”?
“Shotgun!” is nonexplicit; “I call shotgun!” is explicit.
3. Suppose my friends and I go to the mall and in the middle of the food court I try to
call shotgun for the way back. What felicity condition has been broken? What if I
mutter “shotgun” so quietly that nobody hears me? In the first case, I am violating the
164
preparatory conditions, since, according to the shotgun rules, I don’t have the authority to
call shotgun indoors, with the deed (going to the mall) not yet done. In the second case, I
am violating both the preparatory conditions (by violating the rules) and the essential
conditions (by making it impossible for the hearer(s) to recognize my intention). Indeed,
the explicit Shotgun rule that somebody must hear you say “shotgun” has some overlap
with the idea of essential conditions.
4. In what circumstances would the utterance “Shotgun!” not be the kind of speech act
described here? If it were the answer to the question “What kind of gun is that in your
pocket?” or even “What’s that word that people use to call the front seat of the car?”—or
any circumstance in which uttering the word is not understood to be actually calling dibs
on anyone’s front seat.
165
166
Tables & Charts
Reprinted from their original contexts
Phonetics
Phonetic Alphabet Chart: Consonants of English
Phonetic Alphabet Chart: Vowels of English
IPA English Consonant Chart: Manner of Articulation, Place of Articulation, Voicing
IPA English Vowel Chart: Height, Frontness, Tense/Lax*, Roundness
List of Distinctive Features
Phonology
Rule-writing Conventions
Morphology
Basic Lexical Categories (also useful for syntax)
English Inflectional Morphemes (Complete Set)
Common Derivational Morphemes
Word-Coining Processes
Syntax
Lexical Categories Quick Reference
English Phrase Structure Rules
Pragmatics
Grice’s Cooperative Principle & Maxims of Conversation
Searle’s Classification of Illocutionary Acts
Felicity Conditions
Brown & Levinson’s Politeness Strategies
167
Phonetic Alphabet Chart: Consonants of English
168
Phonetic Alphabet Chart: Vowels of English
i superhero suprhiro
invisibility nvzbli
e cape kep
incredible nkrdbl
æ Batman bæmæn
Buffy bfi
ability bli
a Robin rabn
u Superman suprmæn
Doctor Octopus daktr aktps
o Rogue ro
law l
aj ( a ) kryptonite krptnajt
aw ( a ) powers pawrz
j ( ) Superboy suprbj
169
IPA English Consonant Chart: Manner of Articulation, Place of Articulation, Voicing*
Place of Articulation
Bilabial Labio- Inter- Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
dental dental
Stop p t k
b d
Manner of Articulation
Nasal m n
Fricative f s h
v z
Affricate t
d
Approximant w r j
Lateral l
Approximant
* Shaded sounds are voiceless
,
Low
æ a
* Shaded sounds are tense
**Bold sounds are round
170
List of Distinctive Features (Consonants)
Feature - Feature Included Sounds Definition
consonantal nonconsonantal Consonants (not vowels, also Consonants.
not /w/ or /j/).
sonorant obstruent Nasals, approximants. Equal air pressure inside &
outside the vocal tract.
syllabic nonsyllabic Vowels and sometimes Can be the nucleus of a syllable.
approximants
voice voiceless Vowels, voiced consonants Vocal chord vibration
continuant stop Fricatives, approximants. Continuous passage of air
(Not nasals!) through the mouth.
nasal oral {m, n, } Air passes through the nose.
sibilant nonsibilant {s, z, , , t, d} Jet of air through narrow
passage toward obstacle (teeth)
lateral rhotic {l} but not {r} Air flows along sides of the
tongue
labial non-labial {p, b, m, f, v, w} Use of lips
alveolar non-alveolar {t, d, n, s, z, l, r} Use of alveolar ridge
palatal non-palatal {, , t, d, j} Use of hard palate
velar non-velar {k, , } Use of velum
anterior posterior Labials, dentals, alveolars Produced forward of the
alveolar ridge.
coronal noncoronal Dentals, alveolars, palatals. Use of tip and/or blade of the
tongue.
171
Distinctive Feature Chart (Consonants)
Feature p b t d k m n f v s z h t d r l w j
consonantal + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + - -
sonorant - - - - - - - + + + - - - - - - - - - - - + + + +
syllabic - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - +/- +/- +/- +/-
voice - + - + - + - + + + - + - + - + - + - - + + + + +
continuant - - - - - - - - - - + + + + + + + + + - - + + + +
nasal - - - - - - - + + + - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -
sibilant - - - - - - - - - - - - - - + + + + - + + - - - -
lateral - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - + - -
labial + + - - - - - + - - + + - - - - - - - - - - - + -
alveolar - - + + - - - - + - - - - - + + - - - - - + + - -
palatal - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - + + - + + - - - +
velar - - - - + + - - - + - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -
anterior + + + + - - - + + - + + + + + + - - - - - + + + -
coronal - - + + - - - - + - - - + + + + + + - + + + + - -
172
Rule-writing Conventions
Symbols To Know
/X/ X is a phoneme (underlying sound)
[Y] Y is an allophone (actual pronunciation in this environment)
-> “becomes”; “shows up as”
/ “when”; “where”; “in the environment of”
_ placeholder for the allophone, showing its location in the environment
C consonants
V vowels
Ø null/nothing
# word boundary (can be used for the beginning or end of a word)
$ syllable boundary
Ø -> X / Y _ Z
“X is inserted between Y and Z”
173
Basic Lexical Categories (Noun, Verb, Adjective, Adverb)
Nouns Typically defined as “person, place, or thing.” Feelings and ideas are also nouns.
Examples: John, genius, Atlantis, space ship, computer, regret, sadness, loyalty, milk, noun
174
(dogs, churches) (running, jumping) (happier)
past tense –ed superlative –est
(walked, slept) (happiest)
past participle –en
(eaten, taken)
175
Word-Coining Processes
Process Examples
adding morphology A new word is created by adding direction < direct + tion
morphology to an existing word. embiggen < en- + big + -en
backformation A new word is created when people take pea < pease – -s
a word that appears to have been created by adding administrate < administration – -ation
morphology, and try to get at the root by removing the
morphology. Happens due to a misanalysis of the origin
of the original word.
compounding Two words are combined, retaining the weblog < web + log
entirety of both words.
blending Two words are combined, and some smog < smoke + fog
intermediate portions of both words are dropped. Bennifer < Ben + Jennifer
clipping A word is shortened. lab < laboratory
blog < weblog
initialism (or alphabetism) A sequence of initials used as ID < identification
a word (the individual letters are pronounced). SGA < Stargate: Atlantis
acronym A sequence of initials used as a word (sounded NASA < National Aeronautics and
out phonetically rather than saying individual letters). Space Administrations
laser < Light Amplification by
Stimulated Emission of Radiation
eponym A proper name is used to describe something sandwich < the 4th Earl of Sandwich
associated with that person.
generification A brand name is used to describe any Kleenex (meaning any tissue) <
similar product. Kleenex (meaning Kleenex brand
tissue)
lexical shift (or zero derivation; see “Zero Derivation”) to Xerox (V) < (a) Xerox (N)
Words that used to be one lexical cateogory are used in to blog/blogging (V) < (a) blog (N)
another, without adding morphology. The phrase “zero
derivation” refers to the idea that a null (unpronounced)
derivational morpheme is being added to the root to
change its lexical category.
176
Lexical Categories Quick Reference:
Category Examples
Determiner the, a, an, her, his, our, that (also possessives, like “John’s” in “John’s hat”)
Modal Verb could, should, might, must, will, would, can, may, shall
Auxiliary Verb any form of “be” or “have” when used with another verb
Preposition in, on, around, above, with, about, near, within, from, by
Qualifier rather, very, too, quite
Quantifier one, three, 300, some, several, many, much
Complementizer that, whether, because, if, which
Conjunction and, but, or
Abstract Rule
XP Spec X Comp
177
Grice’s Cooperative Principle
Make your conversational contribution such as is required, at the stage at which it occurs, by the
accepted purpose or direction of the talk exchange in which you are engaged.
Quantity
• Make your contribution as informative as required. (Don’t say too much or
too little.)
• Make the strongest statement you can.
Quality
• Do not say what you believe to be false.
• Do not say that for which you lack adequate evidence.
Relation
• Be relevant. (Stay on topic.)
Manner
• Avoid obscurity of expression.
• Avoid ambiguity.
• Be brief (avoid unnecessary prolixity).
• Be orderly.
178
Felicity Conditions
• Preparatory conditions: The speaker must have the necessary ability, authority, and
beliefs.
Failure example: At a baseball game, I scream, “You’re out!” But since I’m not the
umpire, nothing happens.
• Sincerity conditions: The speaker must mean it.
Failure example: My roommate asks me to do the dishes and I say “Yes,” but I have no
intention of actually doing it.
• Essential conditions: The hearer recognizes the intention of the speech act.
Failure example: Intending to propose marriage, I tell my girlfriend, “Let me share your
burden.” She thinks for a minute, and then hands me her books.
• Propositional content conditions: The propositions contained in the wording of the
speech act must be true.
Failure example: I say “I’m sorry I broke the lamp” when I have not, in fact, broken a
lamp.
179
References
i
From Eddie Izzard, “Dress to Kill.”
ii
Data set from Edward Finegan, Language: Its Structure and Use 3rd ed. Harcourt Brace &
Company 1999.
iii
Data set from Edward Finegan, Language: Its Structure and Use 3rd ed. Harcourt Brace &
Company 1999.
iv
Data set from Morris Halle & G.N. Clements, Problem Book in Phonology. MIT, 1983.
v
Data set from Michael Noonan, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Introduction to English
Linguistics Handout in Phonology.
http://www.uwm.edu/~noonan/Summer400Handouts/Phonology.Combined.pdf
vi
Data set from Edward Finegan, Language: Its Structure and Use 3rd ed. Harcourt Brace &
Company 1999.
vii
Data set from Edward Finegan, Language: Its Structure and Use 3rd ed. Harcourt Brace &
Company 1999.
viii
Data set from Michael Noonan, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Introduction to English
Linguistics Handout in Phonology.
http://www.uwm.edu/~noonan/Summer400Handouts/Phonology.Combined.pdf
ix
Data set from wa Mberia, K., “Nasal Consonant Processes in Kitharaka.” Nordic Journal of
African Studies, 11(2), 2002
x
Examples taken from Eugene Loos, “Glossary of Linguistic Terms.”
http://www.sil.org/linguistics/glossaryOfLinguisticTerms/Index.htm
xi
Examples taken from Wikipedia, “Turkish Language.”
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Turkish_language
xii
MFL = Matthew’s Fake Language. These data are not from an actual language, but were
invented by our friend for this study guide.
xiii
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and Conversation. In Martinich, A.P. (ed). Philosophy of
Language. (pp. 165-175) New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
xiv
Adapted from above.
xv
Levinson, S. (2000) Presumptive Meanings: The theory of generalized conversational
implicature. MIT.
xvi
Searle, J.R., "A Classification of Illocutionary Acts", Language in Society, Vol.5, (1976),
pp.1-24
xvii
Pearson, J., Reuter, T., & Henderson, W. (1996). The Official Shotgun Rules.
http://www.shotgunrules.com Accessed January 7, 2007.
xviii
Brown, P. & Levinson, S. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
xix
From Eddie Izzard, “Dress to Kill.”
xx
Data set from Michael Noonan, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Introduction to English
Linguistics Handout in Phonology.
http://www.uwm.edu/~noonan/Summer400Handouts/Phonology.Combined.pdf
180