Clifford 1978
Clifford 1978
3, 1978
INTRODUCTION
METHOD
Subjects
be obtained by writing to the author). Questions which were not originally design-
ed for this study were drawn from various sources, notably the surveys by Simon
and Gagnon (1967) and Hamilton (1929). While some questions were open-ended,
others had possible choices listed on cards presented to the subjects.
Reliability of some key items was checked by asking the questions again
later in the interview in a different context. For example, early in the schedule
the woman was asked how old she was the first time she stimulated herself, and
then near the end gave her age the first time she experienced 12 different sexual
activities including masturbation. When the two sets of questions were compared
using r, all the items were found to be reliable with ~"s of 0.79 or better except
two: the first experience of clitoral and of vaginal petting (7" 0.51 and 0.71, re-
spectively). These two experiences tended to be reversed in many cases.
In order to examine the labels of perceived sensations, a subjective response
checklist was constructed. Forty-four descriptions of subjective sensations during
sexual arousal and orgasm were gathered from three sources: the SAR Audit of
Adams (see Shope, 1967), Masters and Johnson's composite description (1966),
and essays about sexual feelings by women responding to an open-ended question-
naire distributed by the author during a pilot study. Subjects rated each item's
frequency and intensity during each of four sexual situations, as relevant for each
woman: masturbation, passive oral-genital stimulation, intercourse, and inter-
course with concurrent clitoral massage (hereafter referred to as "intercourse-
clitoral").
Procedure
RESULTS
Each of the five areas covered by the survey will be discussed in turn. How-
ever, first, a brief summary of the sexual experience of the sample will be pre-
sented in order to place the following results in their behavioral contexts.
In high school, most subjects (85% and 90%) had engaged in necking
("hugging and kissing") and light petting, excluding the genitalia, with their
clothes on. A smaller number, about 65%, had experienced heavy petting, in-
volving genital stimulation, in various forms. Approximately a third of the sample
had experienced sexual intercourse and oral-genital relations, i.e., cunnilingus, in
high school, and three women had experimented with inanimate objects of sexual
stimulation, such as pencils, vegetables, and electric vibrators. A modal number
of subjects had experienced necking and light petting with more than three male
partners, whereas for more intimate encounters one partner was the norm.
In college, 85% of these women had engaged in heavy petting, 70% had ex-
perienced intercourse and cunnilingus, and eight had tried inanimate objects. The
numbers of partners had increased, except in the cases of intercourse and oral
sex. Most subjects were in love with their partners or felt some emotion for them.
Twelve women had experienced sexual arousal during physical contact with
females: eight before high school, two in high school, and two in college. All
these women reported one to ten such experiences and, except in one case, con-
tacts with one or two partners. None described herself as a homosexual, viewing
her experiences with women as incidental to her heterosexual development.
Seventy-four women had masturbated, defined as arousing oneself by touch-
ing breasts or sex organs. Of these, 49 had reached orgasm through self-stimula-
tion, most commonly between the ages of 12 and 15. Sixty women continued to
masturbate during college with an average frequency of once every 2 weeks.
Of the total sample, 67 women said they had reached orgasm at least once,
23 said they had not, and 10 were unsure. There was little variation in the propor-
tion of subjects who had ever experienced orgasm with various types of stimula-
tion. Thus, as may be seen in Table I, between 56% and 76% of those who had
repeated experiences with clitoral and vaginal petting, intercourse, oral sex, and
masturbation reached orgasm at least once. The rate of orgasm was determined
only for two situations: in intercourse, the modal rate of orgasm was never, al-
though among women who were orgasmic in intercourse, the modal rate was 26-
50%. In masturbation the modal rate of orgasm was 100%.
A second question concerned specific sensations and their relative frequen-
cies. Responses in each of four situations, masturbation, cunnflingus, intercourse,
and intercourse-clitoral, were rated on a scale of frequency from "never" to "oc-
casionally," "half the time," "usually," and "always." The relative frequency of
these responses varied considerably depending on the situation; some cross-situa-
tional generalizations may be made. Sensations generally reported as high in fre-
quency were appearance of moisture in the genital area, a sense of anticipation,
clitoral sensitivity, and a desire to cuddle. Responses of generally low relative
frequency were sadness, nervousness, anal pulsations, and an urge to urinate. The
differences between situations may be summarized as foltows. Masturbation was
distinct in causing a high frequency of pelvic and leg contractions and clitoral
pulsations. In addition, it was unique in its relatively lower frequency of quicken-
ed breathing and desire for body contact. In common with the one other experi-
ence involving specific stimulation of the clitoris, intercourse-clitoral, a focus of
sensation in the clitoris was frequently described. Desire for penetration was fre-
quent in the two situations involving intercourse.
An attempt was made to identify the distinguishing features of an orgasm,
as subjectively experienced, compared to the sensations of arousal prior to that
point. The list of responses was rated on a scale of intensity ranging from "not
aroused" to "slightly aroused," "very aroused," and "orgasm." Table II lists the
responses labeled "orgasm" by at least 50% of the subjects who provided ratings
(as opposed to those who had had no more than one or two experiences with a
given type of stimulation and those who marked their answer as "unsure").
In this sample, 30 women achieved more than one orgasm in rapid succes-
sion during masturbation, with an average rate of under 10% of the tim% "Rapid
succession" was defined variously as an interval of only a few seconds up to
several minutes. Only four subjects claimed to require more than one orgasm
for satisfaction; the others regarded the additional orgasms as additionally plea-
surable but not necessary. One woman, although, masturbating to "several hun-
dred orgasms" on each occasion, denied any enjoyment of the experience, rather
regarding it as a compulsive habit which left her with sore muscles the next day.
When asked if they believed there to be a distinction between a clitoral and
a vaginal orgasm, 50 women said "yes," 26 said "no," and 24 were unsure. Those
who affirmed the statement gave a great range of specific distinctions. Sixteen
had read feminist literature proclaiming that the vaginal orgasm was a myth.
Other distinctions were drawn by a few individuals (one to three) each: difference
in the type of stimulation, either petting or intercourse; more shallow sensations
during clitoral orgasm; difference in the locus of stimulation ;less satisfaction from
clitoral orgasm; less likelihood of achieving vaginal orgasm than clitoral; greater
intensity of the ctitoral orgasm; and greater intensity of the vaginal orgasm. The
lack of consensus on this issue is notable.
190 Clifford
t'-,1
~2
©
~D
o~ ~ o ~
' ~ ~
~ ' ~ , ~ ~
Subjective Sexual Experience in College Women 191
How central is orgasm to achieving pleasure and satisfac~tion from sexual ac-
tivity? Subjects with coital experience were asked about their reactions when or-
gasm did not occur from intercourse. The most common reply was "worried
about my partner's reaction" (34 subjects). Other responses included feeling phys-
ically uncomfortable (28 subjects), feeling good (24 subjects), and feeling bad
about oneself (19).
A different perspective on the same question is obtained by asking whether
sexual behavior is motivated only by the desire for orgasm, or physical release of
tension or drive; or whether other motives even at a conscious level exist. Sub-
jects were given a list of possible reasons for masturbation and asked which ap-
plied to them. Pleasurable sensations (64 subjects) and physical release (52 sub-
jects) were most frequently endorsed. A moderate number of women also report-
ed masturbating to learn about their bodies (32 subjects), to get to sleep (25),
and to relieve boredom or loneliness (19 and 23 subjects, respectively). Individ-
uals mentioned proving their sexual responsivity to themselves, relieving men-
strual cramps, feeling independent of males for stimulation, inducing menstrua-
tion, and angering their partners. Desire for intercourse was similarly found to
involve several different goals. Subjects were allowed to endorse more than one
answer, so that, of 67 women, 60 wanted to have intercourse for their physical
pleasure, 59 for their partner's pleasure, and 55 for their partner's affection to-
ward them.
Different types of sexual stimulation vary in orgasm potential and enjoy-
ability. Sexual intercourse was expected to rank highest in order of preference,
which proved to be the case. Although subjects reported equally high rates of or-
gasm from clitoral stimulation and often described the latter experience as more
intense, almost twice as many of them ranked intercourse as their preferred sex-
ual activity (42 women compared to 23).
As a more formal test of the relationship between orgasm ability and pref-
erences for specific activities, the rank order of preference and the number of or-
gasms for each of five activities (clitoral and vaginal petting, intercourse, cun-
nflingus, and use of inanimate objects) were cross-tabulated. None of these re-
lationships was statistically significant, consistent with, although not proof of,
the notion that sexual preferences are not directly contingent on orgasm ex.
perience.
A related series of tests examined the association between orgasm ability
and frequency of feeling satisfied by intercourse, oral sex, and masturbation. In
the case of masturbation there was no significant association, but for intercourse
and oral sex these variables were highly positively related (p < 0.001 ; see Tables
III through V). Thus it seems that the components of satisfaction when alone are
not the same as those operating in the partner situation.
In testing the effects of types of stimulation on subjective responses, three
areas of the body were considered: the breasts, clitoris, and vagina. Each will be
discussed in turn.
192 Clifford
Frequency of satisfaction
in masturbation (%)
Half the time UsuaUy or
Orgasm rate (%) or less always Row totals
Up to 50 4 7 11
51-90 9 13 22
Over 90 16 51 67
Column totals 29 71 100
Frequency of satisfaction
in intercourse (%)
Half the time Usually or
Orgasm rate (%) or less always Row totals
Up to 50 23 35 58
51-90 3 28 31
Over 90 2 9 11
Column totals 28 72 100
Frequency of satisfaction
in oral sex (%)
One-tailed
significance
Response levela
Breasts
Frequency rating
Nipple erection (masturbationb) 0.194 0.05
Intensity ratings
Tingling breasts (masturbation) 0.273 0.05
Breast enlargement (masturbation) 0.330 0.05
Clitoris
Frequency ratings
Clitoral sensitivity
(oral sex; high schoolO 0.200 0.05
Clitoral Pulsations
(oral sex; college) 0.252 0.01
Clitoral focus
(intercourse; high School) 0.185 0.05
Clitoral focus
(intercourse-clitoral; high school) 0.222 0.05
Intensity ratings
Clitoral sensitivity
(masturbation; college) 0.300 0.01
Clitoral pulsation
(masturbation; high school) 0.300 0.01
Clitoral pulsation
(masturbation; college) 0.371 0.01
Clitoral pulsation
(oral sex; high school) 0.302 0.01
Clitoral pulsation
(oral sex; college) 0.295 0.05
Clitoral focus
(intercourse; high school) 0.263 0.05
Clitoral sensitivity
(intercourse-clitoral; high school) 0.391 0.01
Clitoral focus
(intercourse-clitoral, high school) 0.231 0.05
Clitoral sensitivity
(oral sex; high school) 0.192 0.05
Vagina
Frequency ratings
Vaginal tension
(masturbation; college) 0.197 0.05
Vaginal pulsation
(masturbation; college) 0.214 0.05
Desire for penetration
(oral sex; college) 0.238 0.05
Vaginal pulsations
(oral sex; college) 0.208 0.05
Vaginal tension
(intercourse; high school) 0.176 0.05
Vaginal pulsations
(intercourse; high school) 0.182 0.05
194 Clifford
Breast responses were defined in three ways: tingling sensation in the breasts,
appearance of breast enlargement, and nipple erection as subjectively perceived.
The frequency and intensity ratings of each of these sensations were cross-tabulat-
ed with the number of experiences the woman had had with light petting without
clothes during high school and in college. One relationship with a frequency rating
and two with intensity ratings were statistically significant in the expected posi-
tive direction (see Table VI).
Clitoral responses considered included sensitivity of the clitoris, sudden
focus of sensation in the clitoris, and clitoral pulsations. Four frequency ratings
were significantly positively related, as well as nine intensity ratings, all listed in
Table VI.
Finally, the vaginal responses of vaginal tension, desire for deep penetra-
tion of the vagina, and vaginal pulsations were considered. In this case there were
seven significant relationships with frequency ratings and six with intensity ratings
(see Table VI).
DISCUSSION
Before discussing the specific findings, a note concerning the level of sexual
experience of the sample is required. Experience, in terms of coital incidence, was
somewhat higher than natidnal norms: 71% compared to about 50% nationwide
(Gagnon, 1969). This figure was replicated at the Stony Brook campus in a survey
Subjective Sexual Experience in College Women 195
by Heiman 1 year later (Heiman, 1975). Whether this rate is indicative of increas-
ing rates of intercourse at the national level or of uniquely high rates among re-
spondents at this one university cannot be determined with certainty. In any case,
such rates seem to indicate not a massive "sexual revolution," but rather response
to social pressure to lose one's virginity and changing norms for sexual behavior
within love relationships (e.g., see Reiss, 1967).
The five areas investigated in the survey will be discussed in turn, beginning
with the first, description of sensations during sexual arousal. Although some
subjective responses were consistent across the four situations, some situation-
specific patterns were evident. These may be roughly seen as consistent with
Singer and Singer's (1972) formulation. Masturbation represents a situation in-
volving clitoral and vulvar stimulation for the majority of the subjects, and pro-
duces feelings of pulsation and clitoral focus. All the partner activities included
more vaginal invoIvement and resulted in more feelings of diffuse sensuality and
quickened breathing. In the case of intercourse-clitoral, both elements seemed
to be present, perhaps corresponding with Singer and Singer's combination type.
These findings are also consistent with the reports of Hite's subjects, although
Hite questioned whether the emotional feelings associated with changes in breath-
ing constituted a true orgasm.
With regard to the 10 women who were unsure whether or not they had
ever experienced orgasm, some readers will suggest that they were simply unwiiling
to admit their lack of orgasm ability. However, an equally plausible interpreta-
tion is that orgasm is not always easily distinguishable. Especially when it is low
in intensity, there may be few distinct subjective cues to which the label "or-
gasm" may be attached. Further, if there are few overt signs which an observer
can readily discriminate, he or she will have more difficulty labeling the ex-
perience than would otherwise be the case. Thus, to the extent that the label is
socially imparted, the woman has less access to this information.
A list of sensations reported to be of orgasmic intensity by at least half
the subjects was compiled, as a crude attempt to derive a social consensus on the
nature of orgasm. Masters and Johnson (1966) listed responses which were in-
cluded among those rated in this survey: vaginal pulsations, coming to a peak,
coming to a series of peaks, and sudden suspension or stoppage of sensation.
The last three, but not the first, were agreed to represent orgasmic sensations by
a majority of the present sample. In addition, several nonpulsating feelings, in-
cluding floating or being on a plateau, w e r e among the orgasmic sensations for
a majority of these women. This finding suggests that experiences other than
the ones described by Masters and Johnson may be considered to be orgasmic
by a significant number of women in the general population.
Multiple orgasm was not found to be more enjoyable or necessary for satis-
faction than single orgasm for these women in general, as was also true for Hite's
subjects. Nor was multiple orgasm ever experienced by most subjects. Although
25 women reported that they experienced two kinds of orgasm, one clitoral and
one vaginal, they defined them in extremely varied and sometimes contradictory
196 Clifford
ways. The two patterns described earlier which are distinguished in masturbation
compared to partner situations might be interpreted as a clitoral and a vaginal
pattern, in terms of their corresponding to predominant stimulation of one or-
gan vs. the other. However, there is no evidence that one is consistently preferred,
and in fact the sensations seem combinable rather than mutually exclusive. Again,
Hite's f'mdings corroborate this study's. In order to test the possibility that chtorial
methods of masturbation would be associated with reliance on clitoral stimula-
tion for orgasm during intercourse, these two variables were cross-tabulated. No
trend toward a relationship was found.
In examining the fourth question, the necessity of orgasm for satisfaction,
the answer seemed to depend on the situation: when alone, no relationship was
evident, but with a partner during intercourse or oral sex the relationship was
highly significant. The source of dissatisfaction, as these results suggest, was most
frequently a fear of displeasing one's partner. This finding highlights what seems
to be a current social expectation that women m u s t be brought to orgasm. As
one woman lamented, "These days you're not making love any more, you're
making orgasms." Other reasons for having sex, independent of reaching orgasm,
were given both for masturbation and for intercourse.
The last hypothesis, that sensitivity of specific body areas is related positive-
ly to experience with stimulation of those areas, was supported in the cases of
the clitoris and vagina, and possibly of the breasts. It seems likely that greater
awareness of feeling develops over successive experiences with stimulation of an
area, although women who enjoy greater subjective feeling in a particular organ
undoubtedly also seek out more frequent stimulation of those areas.
Two methodological notes should be made in the process of evaluating
the results of this study. First, self-reports of subjective sensations are difficult
to validate; reliability data are needed to strengthen the findings. Second, it is
possible that an independent researcher may gather new subjective response items
not included in the list rated for this study; it was feasible only to represent sen-
sations described ill the literature to date.
In the course of cross-validating these results, it is hoped that future studies
will turn to new populations as well as replicating the present sample. In partic-
ular, studies of women in cultures where orgasm is defined differently from ours
or is not believed to exist at all would be useful. Longitudinal designs would be
ideal for following up the suggestion that arousal is identified in part through a
social learning process. Finally, surveys of male subjective sexual responses may
uncover systematic variation in patterns currently unrecognized in the literature.
REFERENCES
Adams, C. R. (1953). A Preliminary Report on the Sexuat Responsiveness of Certain College
Wives, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pa.
Subjective Sexual Experience in College Women !97
Bardwick, J. (1971). The Psychology of Women: A Study o f Bio-cultural Conflicts, Harper and
Row, New York.
Bergler, E., and Kroger, W. S. (1954). Kinsey'sMyth o f Female Sexuality, Grune and Strat-
ton, New York.
Bunzl, M., and Mullen, S. (1974). A self-report investigation of two types of myotordc re-
sponse during sexual orgasm. J. Sex Res. 10: 10-20.
Chesser, E. (1957). The Sexual, Marital and Family Relationships o f the English Woman,
Roy Publishers, New York.
Clifford, R. E, (1975). Female masturbation in sexual development and clinical applica-
tion. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, SUNY, Stony Brook, N.Y.
Dickinson, R. L., and Beam, L. (1931). A Thousand Marriages, Williams and Wilkins,
Baltimore.
Gagnon, J. (1969). Religion, dating and the staging of socio-sexual experimentation in a
college sample. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago.
Gagnon, J., and Simon, W. (1973). Sexual Conduct: The Social Sources o f Human Sex-
uality, Aldine, Chicago.
Gebhard, P., Raboch, J., and Giese, H. (1970). Sexuality of Women, Stein and Day, New
York.
Glenn, J., and Kaplan, E. H. (1968). Types of orgasm in women: A critical review and
definition. Z Am. Psyehoanal. Assoc. 16: 549-564.
Hamilton, G. (1929). A Research in Marriage, Medical Research Press, New York.
Hardenbergh, E. W. (1953). The psychology of feminine sex experience. Int. or. Sexol.
2: No. 4.
Heiman, J. (1975). Responses to erotica: An exploration of physiological and psychological
correlates of human sexual response. Ph.D. thesis, SUNY, Stony Brook, N.Y.
Hite, S. (1976). The Hire Report, Macmillan, New York.
Landis, C., and Bolles, M. M. (1942). Personality and Sexuality of Physically Handicapped
Women, Paul Hoeber, New York.
Masters, W. H., and Johnson, W. (1966). Human Sexual Response, Little, Brown, Boston.
Reiss, I. (1967). The Social Context o f Premarital Sexual Permissiveness, Holt, Rinehart and
Winston, New York.
Reuben, D. (1969). Everything You Always Wanted to Know About Sex But Were Afraid
to Ask, McKay, New York.
Schaefer, L. (1964)o Sexual experiences and reactions of 30 women. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Columbia University.
Sherfey, M. J. (1966). The Nature and Evolution of Female Sexuality, Random Bouse,
New York.
Simon, W., and Gagnon, J. (1967). College Youth Study, Chicago National Opinion Research
Center, University of Chicago.
Shope, D. F. (1967). A comparison of selected college females on sexual responsivenss and
non-responsiveness. Diss. Abst. 27: No. 11.
Singer, J., and Singer, I. (1972). Types of female orgasm. J. Sex Res. 8(4): 255.
Terman, L. M. (1938). Psychological Factors in Marital Happiness, McGraw-Hill,New York.
Wallin, D. (1964). Self-perceived rate of physical maturation and personality in late adoles-
cence. ChildDev. 35: 1197-1210.