Full Download Bipolar Disorders Basic Mechanisms and Therapeutic Implications 2nd Edition Medical Psychiatry Jair C. Soares PDF
Full Download Bipolar Disorders Basic Mechanisms and Therapeutic Implications 2nd Edition Medical Psychiatry Jair C. Soares PDF
com
https://ebookfinal.com/download/bipolar-disorders-basic-
mechanisms-and-therapeutic-implications-2nd-edition-medical-
psychiatry-jair-c-soares/
OR CLICK BUTTON
DOWNLOAD EBOOK
https://ebookfinal.com/download/research-advances-in-genetics-and-
genomics-implications-for-psychiatry-1st-edition-nancy-c-andreasen/
ebookfinal.com
https://ebookfinal.com/download/metal-based-neurodegeneration-from-
molecular-mechanisms-to-therapeutic-strategies-2nd-edition-robert-
crichton/
ebookfinal.com
https://ebookfinal.com/download/itch-basic-mechanisms-and-therapy-
basic-and-clinical-dermatology-1st-edition-gil-yosipovitch/
ebookfinal.com
Deadly Karate Blows The Medical Implications Adams
https://ebookfinal.com/download/deadly-karate-blows-the-medical-
implications-adams/
ebookfinal.com
https://ebookfinal.com/download/handbook-of-diagnosis-and-treatment-
of-bipolar-disorders-1st-edition-terence-a-ketter/
ebookfinal.com
https://ebookfinal.com/download/chromosomal-instability-and-aging-
basic-science-and-clinical-implications-1st-edition-fuki-hisama/
ebookfinal.com
Edited by
Jair C. Soares
University of North Carolina School of Medicine
Chapel Hill, North Carolina, USA
Allan H. Young
University of British Columbia
Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
Informa Healthcare USA, Inc.
52 Vanderbilt Avenue
New York, NY 10017
This book contains information obtained from authentic and highly regarded sources. Reprinted
material is quoted with permission, and sources are indicated. A wide variety of references
are listed. Reasonable efforts have been made to publish reliable data and information, but the
author and the publisher cannot assume responsibility for the validity of all materials or for the
consequence of their use.
No part of this book may be reprinted, reproduced, transmitted, or utilized in any form by any elec-
tronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying,
microfilming, and recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system, without written
permission from the publishers.
For permission to photocopy or use material electronically from this work, please access www.
copyright.com (http://www.copyright.com/) or contact the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc.
(CCC) 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, 978-750-8400. CCC is a not-for-profit organization
that provides licenses and registration for a variety of users. For organizations that have been
granted a photocopy license by the CCC, a separate system of payment has been arranged.
Trademark Notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or registered trademarks, and
are used only for identification and explanation without intent to infringe.
Since the first edition of our book was published, increasing progress has been
seen in the understanding of the basic mechanisms involved in the pathophysiol-
ogy of bipolar mood disorders. When our first edition was prepared, there was
comparatively little research being conducted on the mechanisms involved in
this disorder. Recent years have seen an increasing interest in this field and an
increasing amount of activity. Important research initiatives have begun to eluci-
date the pathophysiology of this disorder. These research initiatives are beginning
to lead to breakthroughs in the understanding of the causation of bipolar mood dis-
orders and the development of novel treatments. Some of these new advances have
recently translated into newer treatments available for these disorders.
Of particular importance is the development of newer tools from neuro-
psychopharmacology, which have provided new ways to study various brain
systems, including post-receptor and transcriptional mechanisms. Developments
in neuroimaging have made possible the in vivo study of brain anatomy, neuro-
transmission, and metabolic processes. Important tools from genetics are becoming
available and are being applied to further the understanding of mechanisms
involved in bipolar disorders. Cognitive neuropsychology has also provided
improved tools for the more refined study of brain functions in these disorders.
These novel research avenues have provided new dimensions in exploring the bio-
logical mechanisms involved. New therapeutic developments have already become
available in the past few years. These advances are expected to gradually continue
to translate into new approaches for the treatment of bipolar disorder over the next
few to several years.
The updated findings from this research have not been comprehensively sum-
marized in a book focused specifically on the biological underpinnings of bipolar
mood disorders. There are some excellent books available on the subject of
bipolar disorders, but their focus is primarily on diagnostic issues, course of
illness, and treatment. To fill this gap, we are proud to present the second edition
of our book, Bipolar Disorders: Basic Mechanisms and Therapeutic Implications. This
volume presents outstanding manuscripts by the leaders in the particular areas
of biological research pertinent to bipolar disorders. Among other very important
topics, we have included chapters on genetics, neuroimaging, neuropsychology,
investigations of post-receptor and transcriptional abnormalities, potential inter-
actions between biology and psychosocial factors, childhood onset and late-life
bipolar disorder, and several other important topics. A chapter on the implications
of these research areas for ongoing therapeutic developments in this field is also
included. The potential therapeutic implications of new research, as in the first
edition, are emphasized throughout the book.
We are very happy to have had the collaboration of some of the leading scien-
tists in their respective fields of research, and believe this volume will be a valuable
resource for researchers in this field and in related areas. It is presented as a
iii
iv Preface
complete and accessible reference to the most updated information on the biologi-
cal basis and therapeutics of bipolar mood disorders. It should be useful as sup-
plemental reading for graduate and postgraduate courses on the neurobiology of
mental illness. Mental health practitioners will find it extremely useful as an
updated source with the most recent research progress in this field. We hope you
will share our excitement with these new developments.
Jair C. Soares
Allan H. Young
B Contents
Preface . . . . iii
Contributors . . . . vii
Index . . . . 409
B Contributors
Willem A. Nolen
Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center Groningen,
Groningen, The Netherlands
INTRODUCTION
The classification of mood disorders has been a subject of scientific debate for more
than 2500 years (1,2), and a precise delineation of these illnesses and its various
clinical manifestations has yet to emerge. Many aspects of this discussion have
recently been reviewed by Akiskal (2) and supplemented by commentaries from
authoritative researchers. In this chapter we will give an overview of the current
classification, the boundaries of bipolar disorder with other major psychiatric
illnesses, the validity and reliability of diagnosis, and the implications for neurop-
sychiatric research. As all classifications that are based on clinical description rather
than on etiology and pathophysiology are deemed to be temporary, we will begin
with a brief historical overview and end with some areas that need further
clarification.
of the natural course of bipolar disorder, since most patients in later longitudinal
studies have received both acute and prophylactic treatment, which may have
modified the course of illness for the better or the worse (4).
In 1957, Leonhard (5) proposed the distinction between unipolar depression
and bipolar illness, which was supported by Angst (6) and by Perris in 1966 (7),
and by Winokur, Clayton, and Reich in 1969 (8). Subsequently, the unipolar –
bipolar distinction was adopted by the American classification diagnostic and
statistical manual of mental disorders-III (DSM-III) in 1980 (9), and the revised
edition (DSM-III-R) in 1987 (10). At the same time the WHO classification Inter-
national Classification of Diseases-9 (ICD) (1978) (11) still described all types of
depressive and other forms of affective disorders under the category of manic-
depressive psychosis. However, in the current ICD-10 (1992) (12), bipolar affective
disorder is classified next to recurrent depressive disorder.
TABLE 2 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV Specifiers for Mood Disorders
Specifiers for most recent episodea Code Longitudinal course specifiersa
Severity
Mild xxx.x1
Moderate xxx.x2
Severe xxx.x3
Severe with psychotic features xxx.x4
Mood-congruent psychotic features
Mood-incongruent psychotic features
Course
In partial remission xxx.x5 With/without interepisode
recovery
In full remission xxx.x6 Seasonal patternb
Chronicb Rapid cyclingc
Postpartum onset
Associated features
Catatonic features
Melancholic featuresb
Atypical featuresb
a
DSM-IV-TR is unchanged from DSM-IV with regard to mood disorders.
b
Specifier only for depressive episodes.
c
Specifier only for bipolar I and II disorders.
change from normal functioning, which is observable by others, lasting at least four
days. This separates hypomania from normal elevations of mood or the very mild
mood elevations that occur in cyclothymic disorder. On the other hand, the
symptoms are not severe enough to cause “marked impairment in social or
occupational functioning” as in mania. In the ICD-10 clinical descriptions and diag-
nostic guidelines (12) hypomania does not lead to “severe or complete disruption of
work or result in social rejection,” although “considerable interference with work or
social activity is consistent with a diagnosis of hypomania.” Thus, ICD-10 hypoma-
nia includes conditions that would justify a diagnosis of (mild) mania according to
DSM-IV criteria. Interestingly, the ICD-10 diagnostic criteria for research (18) are
more in accordance with DSM-IV, stating that hypomania leads to “some inter-
ference with personal functioning in daily living.” The problems that arise when
defining the boundary between hypomania and (mild) mania are discussed by
Goodwin, who points out that this boundary depends entirely upon the meaning
of ill-defined qualifying words like “some,” “considerable,” “marked,” “severe,”
or “complete” functional impairment (19). He also notes a tendency to avoid the
somewhat pejorative diagnosis of “mania” in favor of “hypomania” in clinical
settings.
The validity of a minimum duration of four days for a hypomanic episode
has been tested by the Zurich group that found that patients with brief hypomania
of one to three days duration did not significantly differ from those whose
hypomanic episodes lasted at least four days (20).
In patients presenting with a current depressive episode, a history of prior
hypomanic episodes is easily missed, especially since depression is the prevailing
condition in bipolar II disorder (21). Thus, many of these patients will be misdiag-
nosed as suffering from (recurrent) unipolar depression. Revealing past hypomanic
episodes may benefit from systematic inquiry in all aspects of the syndrome,
especially the behavioral symptoms rather than elevation of mood. Patients and
relatives will remember the short nights and the energetic overactivity more
sharply than a period of cheerfulness or irritability.
In a recent survey among U.S. citizens using the Mood Disorders Question-
naire (22), which systematically checks (hypo)manic symptoms, 3.7% screened
positive for bipolar I or II disorder. Of these, only 19.8% had previously received
a diagnosis of bipolar disorder, 31.2% had received a diagnosis of unipolar
depression, and 49.0% had received neither of these diagnoses (23).
Hypomanic or manic episodes may appear relatively late in the course of
bipolar disorder, inevitably leading to an initial diagnosis of unipolar depression.
The rate of spontaneous conversion from unipolar to bipolar mood disorder has
been estimated at median 9.7%, with a reported maximum of up to 37.5% (24).
Over the course of 11 years, hypomanic or manic episodes occurred in 12.5% of
559 prospectively followed unipolar patients (25). This phenomenon may in
part explain the long delay between the occurrence of first mood symptoms and
the diagnosis of bipolar disorder, which was on average 10 years in a survey
among DMDA members (26) and also among clinical populations (27).
DEPRESSION
Depression is the main burden of bipolar illness. Longitudinal follow-up data from
the Collaborative Depression Study showed that patients with bipolar I disorder,
despite adequate treatment, on average reported depressive symptoms in 31.9%,
6 Kupka and Nolen
MIXED STATES
In mixed states the complexity of bipolar disorder reaches its maximum. Pure
depression and pure mania are the prototypical endpoints on a continuum of beha-
vioral and emotional disturbances. Mixed states have originally been described by
Kraepelin and his contemporary Weygandt as various admixtures of three dimen-
sions: mood, thinking, and psychomotor activity (38). They distinguished six
subtypes: depression with flight of ideas, excited depression, depressive-anxious
mania, mania with thought poverty, inhibited mania, and manic stupor.
In DSM-IV, a patient with a mixed episode meets both criteria for a manic
episode and a major depressive episode during at least one week, and may experi-
ence rapidly alternating moods of sadness, irritability, and dysphoria. It is thus
Classification of Bipolar Disorders 7
essentially a type of manic episode that also contains full syndromal depression
(“mixed mania”), which may be uncommon in clinical settings (39). This narrow
definition excludes many patients with combinations of syndromal and subsyndro-
mal symptoms of either polarity, for example, those with isolated depressive symp-
toms during a manic episode (“mixed mania”) or with some manic symptoms
during a major depressive episode (“mixed depression”). The prevalence of such
more broadly defined mixed states in clinical practice is probably much higher
than DSM-IV mixed episodes. It is estimated that mixed states occur in about
30% to 40% of acutely manic patients (32,40). Dysphoric mania, defined as a full
manic syndrome with the simultaneous presence of some depressive symptoms,
was present in 37% of manic patients in a clinical setting (41). In a recent study
among 908 treated bipolar outpatients mixed hypomania, that is, the co-occurrence
of depressive symptoms in patients with a hypomanic episode, was found in 57%
of visits for hypomanic episodes (42). Mixed hypomania was equally prevalent in
patients with bipolar I and II disorder, but more prevalent in women.
The concept and the terminology of mixed states are prone to confusion.
ICD-10 takes a somewhat broader view than DSM-IV on mixed states, defined
as either a mixture or a rapid alternation (usually within a few hours) of manic,
hypomanic, and depressive symptoms; the minimum duration of mixed episodes
is two weeks. The term “dysphoric mania,” originally meant to indicate a mixed
state, may be wrongly used to indicate “classic” mania with predominantly irritable
rather than euphoric mood. Moreover, the distinction between these two conditions
may be difficult and it is unclear whether this is clinically relevant since the
evidence that mixed states should be regarded as separate clinical entities is
controversial (40).
It is of clinical importance that mixed mania is less responsive to treatment, in
particular with lithium, than “classic” euphoric mania (43).
there is a significant risk of another switch. Finally, it is also of interest that with-
drawal of antidepressants can induce mania (44). It is likely that for this condition
the same diagnostic considerations are relevant.
would have been better (53). Recent studies by Angst et al. in Zurich (20) showed
that hypomanic episodes of one to three days were of comparable clinical
significance. Including brief hypomanic episodes into the definition of bipolar II
disorder changes the rate of all major and minor forms of unipolar versus bipolar
disorder (see later) (20).
Another area of potential diagnostic confusion lies in the distinction between
agitated unipolar depression and bipolar mixed states, especially the “mixed
depression” described earlier. There is some research evidence that agitated
unipolar depression is part of the bipolar spectrum (54).
Internal Boundaries
Separating bipolar II from bipolar I disorder depends on the definition of hypomania
and its upper limits towards mania (see earlier). In the longitudinal Collaborative
Depression Study, patients with bipolar I disorder had more severe episodes
whereas those with bipolar II disorder had a substantially more chronic course
with significantly more major and minor depressive episodes and shorter inter-
episodic well intervals (55). The authors conclude that these differences justify
classification as two separate subtypes, although the overall clinical similarities of
these subtypes suggest that they exist in a disease spectrum (55).
Rapid cycling, defined as the occurrence of at least four distinct mood
episodes in one year, has been introduced as a course specifier of bipolar I and II
disorder in DSM-IV (53,56). Rapid cycling is not mentioned at all in ICD-10.
Taking a conventional categorical approach, a meta-analysis of 20 studies compar-
ing rapid cyclers and nonrapid cyclers revealed some significant differences apart
from episode frequency, in particular a slight overrepresentation of women and of
bipolar II subtype among rapid cyclers (57). Associated with rapid cycling there
was also a trend for depressive episode at onset of illness, a history of serious
suicide attempts, a family history of affective disorder, and nonresponse to
lithium prophylaxis. However, recent large studies (29,58,59) have shed doubt
over the higher prevalence of rapid cycling among bipolar II patients. Moreover,
if one takes a dimensional approach, differences occur gradually with increasing
episode frequency and never reveal a cut-off point at four episodes per year or at
any other episode frequency, suggesting that rapid cycling is not a distinct
subtype but merely an extreme on a continuum of cycle frequency (29).
Finally, the boundary between mania and depression is blurred in patients
with mixed states. There is also a potential overlap between mixed states with
rapid mood shifts on the one hand, and ultra-rapid or ultradian cycling (45,47)
on the other. The latter conditions are not defined in DSM-IV but can be classified
as mixed episodes or alternatively in the residual category Bipolar Disorder Not
Otherwise Specified. A summary of key criteria defining the major boundaries of
bipolar disorder is given in Table 4.
is further exploded when thresholds are set even lower, such as in the Zurich
studies (20). Relaxing duration and severity criteria for hypomania resulted in an
almost equal lifetime prevalence of 24.6% for the depressive spectrum (including
major depression, dysthymia, minor depression, and brief recurrent depression)
and 23.7% for “soft” bipolar spectrum (including bipolar I and broadly defined
bipolar II, minor bipolar disorder, cyclothymia, and pure hypomania) (20). These
authors state that 11% constitutes the spectrum of bipolar disorders proper, and
another 13% “probably represent the softest expression of bipolarity intermediate
between bipolar disorder and normality.” If almost a quarter of the population is
included, it is questionable whether such broad definitions are still meaningful
indicators of psychopathology, given the lifetime prevalence of core bipolar I dis-
order of 0.5% in the same cohort, which is consistent with other epidemiological
studies (19).
data sources, every subject is scored in the range 1–100 on each of the four
dimensions. The instrument was developed within the context of family studies,
and incorporates the boundaries between unipolar, bipolar, schizoaffective,
and schizophrenic disorders (Table 4). It retains a measure of severity and also
includes mild or subclinical cases, and avoids hierarchical loss of information
inherent to classification systems. BADDS appears to be especially suitable to
assess bipolar spectrum disorders next to or beyond the strict DSM-IV or ICD-10
categories.
There are various symptom rating scales for measurement of severity of
mania and depression, such as the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) (74), the
Bech Rafaelsen Mania Scale (BR-MAS) (75), the Hamilton Rating Scale for
Depression (HAMD) (76), the Montgomery Åsberg Depression Rating Scale
(MADRS) (77), and the Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (IDS) (78). The
latter includes many “atypical” symptoms, which frequently occur in bipolar
depression, and is thus particularly suitable for this condition.
A problem typical for the assessment of change in bipolar disorder is that
improvement of a depressive episode may go hand in hand with emerging
mania, and vice versa, and that rapid changes of polarity may add to the overall
illness burden beyond depressive and manic symptoms per se. To address this
problem, the Clinical Global Impressions scale, Bipolar Version (CGI-BP), was
designed (79). This is a modification of the original CGI scale, providing global
severity ratings of both depression and mania, as well as a global severity rating
of overall bipolar disorder. A similar set of CGI scales was designed for the assess-
ment of change in clinical trials (79).
These scales all make a cross-sectional assessment of the manic or depressive
episode with a time span ranging from two days to two weeks. Given the waxing
and waning course of bipolar disorder, repeated cross-sectional assessments will
not reveal the frequently occurring mood episodes in between. For continuous
longitudinal assessments, either retrospective or prospective, a graphic method
such as the NIMH-Life Chart Methodology (NIMH-LCM) is more suitable
(Fig. 1) (80,81). This instrument has been validated in recent years (82,83), and is
available in a clinician-rated and a self-rated version. A similar instrument
for longitudinal assessment is the prospectively self-rated computerized ChronoR-
ecord (84).
FIGURE 1 One-year prospective daily life chart showing prototypical bipolar I disorder with a
continuous “rapid cycling” course in a man aged 67 with an illness history of over 50 years. Mania
above and depression below baseline. Source: From Ref. 29.
Classification of Bipolar Disorders 13
TOWARDS DSM-V
Despite the limitations and pitfalls of categorical diagnostic classifications (85),
DSM-IV and ICD-10 have greatly improved the reliability of the different mood
disorder diagnoses. For the sake of continuity in epidemiologic, neurobiologic,
and genetic research, we should be reluctant to change diagnostic criteria for
14 Kupka and Nolen
CONCLUSION
Polarity and cyclicity have been described as core dimensions of manic-depressive
illness (Fig. 1) (32), which, despite its heterogeneity, can still be regarded as one of
the most consistently described disorders in psychiatry (2,12). From these dimen-
sions, a bipolar spectrum can be constructed, and within this bipolar spectrum,
various subtypes have been defined. In this chapter we have highlighted the
main areas of uncertainty and controversy about the internal and external bound-
aries of this spectrum, which rely exclusively on clinical description and still
lack external validators. It may well be that the most specific validators for
bipolar disorder will be revealed by studying patients with the core bipolar I syn-
drome (66). Eventually, neurobiological and genetic studies must not only provide
such validators for accurate and valid diagnoses, but above all direct towards a
more targeted treatment for the individual patient with a variant of bipolar
spectrum disorder.
REFERENCES
1. Jackson SW. Melancholia and Depression. From Hippocratic Times to Modern Times.
New Haven: Yale University Press, 1986.
2. Akiskal H. Classification, diagnosis, and boundaries of bipolar disorders: a review.
In: Maj M, Akiskal HS, Lopez-Ibor JJ, Sartorius N, eds. Bipolar Disorder. Evidence and
Experience in Psychiatry. Vol. 6. Chichester: Wiley, 2002.
3. Kraepelin E. Psychiatrie. Ein Lehrbuch für Studirende und Aerzte. II. Band. Sechste
Auflage. Leipzig: Verlag Barth, 1899.
Classification of Bipolar Disorders 15
4. Angst J, Sellaro R. Historical perspectives and natural history of bipolar disorder. Biol
Psychiatry 2000; 48:445–457.
5. Leonhard K. Aufteilung der endogenen Psychosen. Berlin: Academie Verlag, 1957.
6. Angst J. Zur Ätiologie und Nosologie endogener depressiver Psychosen. Berlin:
Springer Verlag, 1966.
7. Perris C. A study of bipolar (manic-depressive) and unipolar recurrent depressive
psychoses. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1966; 42(suppl 194):68– 82.
8. Winokur G, Clayton PJ, Reich T. Manic-Depressive Illness. St. Louis: Mosby, 1969.
9. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders. 3rd ed. (DSM-III). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press, 1980.
10. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders. 3rd ed.—Revised (DSM-III-R). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press,
1987.
11. World Health Organization. The ICD-9 Classification of Mental and Behavioural
Disorders. Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva: WHO, 1978.
12. World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural
Disorders. Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva: WHO, 1992.
13. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders. 4th ed. (DSM-IV). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press, 1994.
14. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Dis-
orders. 4th ed.—Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington DC: American Psychiatric
Press, 2000.
15. Angst J, Gerber-Werder R, Zuberbuhler HU, Gamma A. Is bipolar I disorder hetero-
geneous? Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2004; 254:82 –91.
16. Dunner DL, Tay LK. Diagnostic reliability of the history of hypomania in bipolar II
patients and patients with major depression. Compr Psychiatry 1993; 34:303– 307.
17. Berk M, Dodd S. Bipolar II disorder: a review. Bipolar Disord 2005; 7:11 – 21.
18. World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural
Disorders. Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva: WHO, 1993.
19. Goodwin G. Hypomania: what’s in a name? Br J Psychiatry 2002; 181:94– 95.
20. Angst J, Gamma A, Benazzi F, Ajdacic V, Eich D, Rossler W. Toward a re-definition of
subthreshold bipolarity: epidemiology and proposed criteria for bipolar-II, minor
bipolar disorders and hypomania. J Affect Disord 2003; 73:133– 146.
21. Judd LJ, Akiskal HS, Schettler PJ, et al. A prospective investigation of the natural history
of the long-term weekly symptomatic status of bipolar II disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry
2003; 60:261–269.
22. Hirschfeld RM, Williams JB, Spitzer RL, et al. Development and validation of a screening
instrument for bipolar spectrum disorder: the Mood Disorder Questionnaire. Am J
Psychiatry 2000; 157:1873–1875.
23. Hirschfeld RM, Calabrese JR, Weissman MM, et al. Screening for bipolar disorder in the
community. J Clin Psychiatry 2003; 64: 53–59.
24. Chun BJ, Dunner DL. A review of antidepressant-induced hypomania in major
depression: suggestions for DSM-V. Bipolar Disord 2004; 6:32– 42.
25. Akiskal HS, Maser JD, Zeller PJ, et al. Switching from “unipolar” to bipolar II. An 11-year
prospective study of clinical and temperamental predictors in 559 patients. Arch Gen
Psychiatry 1995; 52:114–123.
26. Lish JD, Dime-Meenan S, Whybrow PC, Price RA, Hirschfeld RM. The National
Depressive and Manic-depressive Association (DMDA) survey of bipolar members.
J Affect Disord 1994; 31:281–294.
27. Suppes T, Leverich GS, Keck PE, et al. The Stanley Foundation Bipolar Treatment
Outcome Network. II. Demographics and illness characteristics of the first 261 patients.
J Affect Disord 2001; 67:45–59.
28. Judd LJ, Akiskal HS, Schettler PJ, et al. The long-term natural history of the weekly
symptomatic status of bipolar I disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002; 59: 530 –537.
29. Kupka RW, Luckenbaugh DA, Post RM, et al. A comparative study of rapid and non-
rapid cycling bipolar disorder using daily prospective mood ratings in 539 out-patients.
Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:1273–1280.
16 Kupka and Nolen
30. Joffe RT, MacQueen GM, Marriott M, Trevor Young L. A prospective, longitudinal study
of percentage of time spent ill in patients with bipolar I or bipolar II disorders. Bipolar
Disorders 2004; 6:62–66.
31. Beigel A, Murphy DL. Unipolar and bipolar affective illness. Differences in clinical
characteristics accompanying depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1971; 24:215– 220.
32. Goodwin FK, Jamison KR. Manic-Depressive Illness. New York: Oxford University
Press, 1990.
33. Winokur G, Coryell W, Endicott J, Akiskal H. Further distinctions between manic-
depressive illness (bipolar disorder) and primary depressive disorder (unipolar
depression). Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1176 –1181.
34. Winokur G, Coryell W, Keller M, Endicott J, Akiskal H. A prospective follow-up of
patients with bipolar and primary unipolar affective disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry
1993; 50:457–465.
35. Mitchell PB, Wilhelm K, Parker G, Austin MP, Rutgers P, Malhi GS. The clinical features
of bipolar depression: a comparison with matched major depressive disorder patients.
J Clin Psychiatry 2001; 62:212–216.
36. Ghaemi SN, Hsu DJ, Ko JY, Baldassano CF, Kontos NJ, Goodwin FK. Bipolar spectrum
disorder: a pilot study. Psychopathology 2004; 37:222– 226.
37. Bowden CL. A different depression: clinical distinctions between bipolar and unipolar
depression. J Affect Disord 2005; 84:117–125.
38. Marnereos A. Origin and development of concepts of bipolar mixed states. J Affect
Disord 2001; 67:229–240.
39. Swann AC. Depression, mania, and feeling bad: the role of dysphoria in mixed states.
Bipolar Disord 2000; 2:325–327.
40. McElroy SL, Keck PE Jr, Pope HG Jr, Hudson JI, Faedda GL, Swann AC. Clinical and
research implications of the diagnosis of dysphoric or mixed mania or hypomania.
Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149:1633–1644.
41. Akiskal HS, Hantouche EG, Bourgeois ML, et al. Gender, temperament, and the clinical
picture in dysphoric mixed mania: findings from a French national study (EPIMAN).
J Affect Disord 1998; 50:175–186.
42. Suppes T, Mintz J, McElroy SL, et al. Mixed hypomania in 908 patients with bipolar dis-
order evaluated prospectively in the Stanley Foundation Bipolar Treatment Network: a
sex-specific phenomenon. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62:1089– 1096.
43. Swann AC, Bowden CL, Morris D, et al. Depression during mania. Treatment response
to lithium or divalproex. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997; 54:37– 42.
44. Andrade C. Antidepressant-withdrawal mania: a critical review and synthesis of the
literature. J Clin Psychiatry 2004; 65:987–993.
45. Kramlinger KG, Post RM. Ultra-rapid and ultradian cycling in bipolar affective illness.
Br J Psychiatry 1996; 168:314–323.
46. Paris J. Borderline or bipolar? Distinguishing borderline personality disorder from
bipolar spectrum disorders. Harv Rev Psychiatry 2004;12:140– 145.
47. Mackinnon DF, Pies R. Affective instability as rapid cycling: theoretical and clinical
implications for borderline personality and bipolar spectrum disorders. Bipolar
Disord 2006; 8:1–14.
48. Akiskal HS, Yerevanian BI, Davis GC, King D, Lemmi H. The nosologic status of borderline
personality: clinical and polysomnographic study. Am J Psychiatry 1985;142:192–198.
49. Marneros A, Rottig S, Wenzel A, Bloink R, Brieger O. Schizoaffective mixed states.
In: Marneros A, Goodwin F, eds. Bipolar Disorders. Mixed States, Rapid Cycling and
Atypical Forms. Cambridge: University Press, 2005:187– 206.
50. Maj M, Pirozzi R, Formicola AM, Bartoli L, Bucci P. Reliability and validity of the DSM-
IV diagnostic category of schizoaffective disorder: preliminary data. J Affect Disord
2000; 57:95–98.
51. Craddock N, Owen MJ. The beginning of the end for the Kraepelinian dichotomy. Br J
Psychiatry 2005; 186:364–366.
52. Beckmann H, Pfuhlmann B. Re-examining the bipolar-schizophrenia dichotomy. In:
Kasper S, Hirschfeld RMA, eds. Handbook of bipolar disorder. Diagnosis and
therapeutic approaches. New York: Taylor and Francis, 2005:37– 48.
Classification of Bipolar Disorders 17
53. Dunner DL. Bipolar Disorders in DSM-IV: Impact of rapid cycling as a course modifier.
Neuropsychopharmacology 1998; 19:189–193.
54. Benazzi F, Koukopoulos A, Akiskal HS. Toward a validation of a new definition of
agitated depression as a bipolar mixed state (mixed depression). Eur Psychiatry 2004;
19:85–90.
55. Judd LL, Akiskal HS, Schettler PJ, et al. The comparative clinical phenotype and long
term longitudinal episode course of bipolar I and II: a clinical spectrum or distinct
disorders? J Affect Disord 2003; 73:19–32.
56. Bauer MS, Calabrese J, Dunner DL, et al. Multisite data reanalysis of the validity of rapid
cycling as a course modifier for bipolar disorder in DSM-IV. Am J Psychiatry 1994;
151:506–515.
57. Kupka RW, Luckenbaugh DA, Post RM, Leverich GS, Nolen WA. Rapid and non-rapid
cycling bipolar disorders: a meta-analysis of clinical studies. J Clin Psychiatry 2003;
64:1483–1494.
58. Coryell W, Solomon D, Turvey C, et al. The long-term course of rapid-cycling bipolar
disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003; 60:914 –920.
59. Schneck CD, Miklowitz DJ, Calabrese JR, et al. Phenomenology of rapid cycling bipolar
disorder: data from the first 500 participants in the Systematic Treatment Enhancement
Program. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:1902–1908.
60. Akiskal HS. The bipolar spectrum: new concepts in classification and diagnosis. In:
Grinspoon L, ed. Psychiatry Update 1983. Washington: American Psychiatric Press,
1983.
61. Akiskal HS. The bipolar spectrum: history, description, boundaries, and validity.
In: Kasper S, Hirschfeld RMA, eds. Handbook of Bipolar Disorder. Diagnosis and
Therapeutic Approaches. New York: Taylor and Francis, 2005:49– 68.
62. Katzow JJ, Hsu DJ, Nassir Ghaemi S. The bipolar spectrum: a clinical perspective.
Bipolar Disord 2003; 5:436–442.
63. Cassano GB, Rucci P, Frank E, et al. The mood spectrum in unipolar and bipolar
disorder: arguments for a unitary approach. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:1264– 1269.
64. Angst J, Cassano G. The mood spectrum: improving the diagnosis of bipolar disorder.
Bipolar Disord 2005; 7(suppl 4):4 –12.
65. Dunner DL. Clinical consequences of under-recognized bipolar spectrum disorder.
Bipolar Disorders 2003; 5:456–463.
66. Baldessarini RJ. A plea for the integrity of the bipolar disorder concept. Bipolar disorders
2000; 2:3 –7.
67. Judd LL, Akiskal HS. The prevalence and disability of bipolar spectrum disorders in the
US population: re-analysis of the ECA database taking into account subthreshold cases.
J Affect Disord 2003; 73:123–131.
68. First MB, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV
Axis I Disorders: SCID-I/P (Version 2.0). New York: Biometrics Research Department,
1996.
69. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric
Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychia-
tric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry 1998; 59(suppl 20):22– 33.
70. Wing JK, Babor T, Brugha T, et al. SCAN. Schedules for Clinical Assessment in
Neuropsychiatry. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1990; 47:589– 593.
71. Robins LN, Wing J, Wittchen HU, et al. The Composite International Diagnostic
Interview. An epidemiologic Instrument suitable for use in conjunction with
different diagnostic systems and in different cultures. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:
1069 –1077.
72. Regeer EJ, ten Have M, Rosso ML, Hakkaart-van Roijen L, Vollebergh W, Nolen WA.
Prevalence of bipolar disorder in the general population: a Reappraisal Study of the
Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2004;
110:374–382.
73. Craddock N, Jones I, Kirov G, Jones L. The Bipolar Affective Disorder Dimension Scale
(BADDS)—a dimensional scale for rating lifetime psychopathology in bipolar spectrum
disorders. BMC Psychiatry 2004; 4:19.
18 Kupka and Nolen
74. Young RC, Biggs JT, Ziegler VE, Meyer DA. A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity
and sensitivity. Br J Psychiatry 1978; 133:429–435.
75. Bech P, Rafaelsen OJ, Kramp P, Bolwig TG. The mania rating scale: scale construction
and inter-observer agreement. Neuropharmacology 1978; 17:430– 431.
76. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960;
23:56–62.
77. Montgomery SA, Asberg M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change.
Br J Psychiatry 1979; 134:382–389.
78. Rush AJ, Gullion CM, Basco MR, Jarrett RB, Trivedi MH. The Inventory of Depressive
Symptomatology (IDS): psychometric properties. Psychol Med 1996; 26:477– 486.
79. Spearing MK, Post RM, Leverich GS, Brandt D, Nolen W. Modification of the Clinical
Global Impressions (CGI) Scale for use in bipolar illness (BP): the CGI-BP. Psychiatry
Res 1997; 73:159–171.
80. Post RM, Roy-Byrne PP, Uhde TW. Graphic representation of the life course of illness in
patients with affective disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 1988; 145:844– 848.
81. Leverich GS, Post RM. Life Charting of Affective Illness. CNS Spectrums 1998; 3:21– 37.
82. Denicoff KD, Leverich GS, Nolen WA, et al. Validation of the prospective NIMH-Life-
Chart Method (NIMH-LCM-p) for longitudinal assessment of bipolar illness. Psychol
Med 2000; 30:1391–1397.
83. Meaden PM, Daniels RE, Zajecka J. Construct validity of life chart functioning scales for
use in naturalistic studies of bipolar disorder. J Psychiatr Res 2000; 34:187– 192.
84. Bauer M, Grof P, Gyulai L, Rasgon N, Glenn T, Whybrow PC. Using technology to
improve longitudinal studies: self-reporting with ChronoRecord in bipolar disorder.
Bipolar Disord 2004; 6:67–74.
85. Kendell R. The choice of diagnostic criteria for biological research. Arch Gen Psychiatry
1982; 32:1334–1339.
B 2 Prospects for the Development of Animal
Models for the Study of Bipolar Disorder
Haim Einat
College of Pharmacy, University of Minnesota, Duluth, Minnesota, U.S.A.
INTRODUCTION
Attitudes towards the study of lithium on behavior in animals and humans have
often been influenced by preconceived notions of the nature of psychiatry and
psychopharmacologic treatment. Many psychiatrists would like to see psychophar-
macological agents as “magic bullets” in a sense similar to antibiotics. Antibiotics
are not expected to have effects on organisms that are not infected with bacteria;
effects that do occur are seen as side effects unrelated to the mode of action. Thus
neuroleptic dopamine-blocking drugs are believed by many psychiatrists to have
no effects on persons who are not psychotic; antidepressants are seen as distin-
guished from stimulants of abuse as not having mood-elevating effects in the
absence of depression. The study of lithium’s effects has often been carried out in
a similar tradition. Moreover, lithium is seen as a difficult drug to give to normal
volunteers for the period of three weeks or a month that would approximate the
amount of time necessary for significant effect on a manic episode.
PSYCHOSTIMULANT-INDUCED MODELS
Animal studies have been a mainstay of the study of lithium effects on behavior
other than clinical studies of psychophathology. These animal studies were
reviewed in 1991 (1) and again in 2003 (2). The overriding majority of studies in
this field has used the concept of pharmacologically induced mania and depression
and has attempted to show lithium prevention. The usual agent for pharmacologi-
cal induction of mania has been amphetamine, although more specific and direct
dopamine agonists such as quinpirole have also been used (3). Using reserpine
or tetrabenazine to induce depression has also been studied (4,5). A background
concept has been the fact that low dose amphetamine has effects primarily on
open field activity whereas higher dose amphetamine causes stereotypy (6,7).
Dopamine blockers are well known to block both the low dose hyperactivity and
the high dose stereotypy of amphetamine, and this nicely fits their usefulness in
both mania and schizophrenia (8). Further support for the amphetamine-induced
hyperactivity model comes from the association of psychostimulants with the
onset of mania in susceptible individuals (9,10) and from some clinical studies
that support an effect of lithium in preventing the behavioral effects of stimulants
in people (11,12). To delineate the differences between the dissimilar responses to
19
20 Einat et al.
similar to the home cage had been demonstrated to hinder the development of
ambulatory activity (31).
It is possible to give amphetamine or methylphenidate to humans on chronic
lithium and this was done in many studies (11,14,21,32,33). While some have
claimed marked effects of lithium to attenuate the amphetamine-induced response,
others have found no effect at all (32). The numbers on subjects in these studies
are smaller than the large numbers of patients required to show a lithium effect
in a clinical situation and human heterogeneity may be the answer to the con-
tradictory results. We await a clear paradigm that will give robust findings in the
amphetamine hyperactivity model of mania in humans as in rodents. Perhaps
the issue is blood lithium levels since these have varied greatly between studies
and usually levels of 0.7 mM have been considered sufficiently similar to human
treatment to be an acceptable model. It is unclear why behavioral effects are so
difficult to demonstrate, whereas biochemical effects of lithium in normal subjects
are marked and highly replicable (34).
Interestingly, the notion of endophenotypes that had recently been strongly
emphasized in the research of bipolar disorder had renewed the interest of
scientists in the strength of amphetamine-induced behaviors and underlying
brain changes as important modeling tools.
Endophenotypes are quantifiable components in the genes-to-behaviors
pathways, distinct from psychiatric symptoms that make genetic and biological
studies of etiologies for disease categories more manageable (35). In the context
of modeling, endophenotypes approach can be helpful as it reduces the complexity
of symptoms and multifaceted behaviors, resulting in units of analysis that are
simpler to model in animals (36).
One of the tentative endophenotypes that had been repeatedly suggested
for bipolar disorder is dysregulation of dopaminergic function and hypersensi-
tivity to psychostimulants. This possible endophenotype was suggested based on
significant data in both human and animal studies. Impaired brain reward path-
ways, enhanced rewarding effects of psychostimulants in patients with affective
illness, possible relationship between dopamine release in the ventral striatum,
euphoric responses, and some evidence for genetic variance that may explain the
individual differences in brain response to psychostimulants all suggest that beha-
vioral changes observed after exposure to amphetamine may be useful as marker
for bipolar disorder (37). However, this new line of study, exploring amphetamine
responses not as a model of mania but as a model of an endophenotype of
bipolar disorder, must include additional experimentation that will look beyond
hyperactivity into different facets of amphetamine-induced behavior, the possible
relationship between such behaviors, the effects of mood stabilizers, and the
genetic predisposition related to individual variability in responses to amphet-
amine as well as to lithium effects on amphetamine-induced behavior. Some
suggestions along these lines are detailed later in this chapter.
LITHIUM AS AN ANTIDEPRESSANT
Beyond the amphetamine-related models, a very exciting advance in this field has
occurred recently, in a paper by O’Brien et al. (38) who used a specific regimen of
lithium administration to mice: mice received 0.2% lithium chloride in food for a
period of five days followed by 0.4% for 10 additional days and reported robust
effects in the Porsolt forced swim test. Previous studies of lithium in Porsolt
22 Einat et al.
forced swim test were equivocal, although Bourin’s group (39,40) showed that
lithium could reliably potentiate the effects of other antidepressants. The study of
Bourin et al. (39,40) used an acute lithium dose but it fit the preconceived notion
that in the clinic lithium is an augmenter of antidepressant response and not a
powerful antidepressant itself. However, the paradigm of O’Brien et al. (38)
suggests a powerful effect of lithium in the Porsolt forced swim test. This is unlikely
to be an artifact, since activity in the Porsolt forced swim test requires an increase in
struggling behavior. Previous concerns about lithium artifacts have usually pointed
out that lithium patients experience some sense of malaise and muscle weakness
and nausea. These would be unlikely to cause the reported effects in the Porsolt
forced swim test.
We (41) have been able to replicate the O’Brien et al. (38) finding and have
shown that it is dependent on blood levels. Blood levels greater than 1 mM are
necessary for the robust effect that O’Brien et al. (38) finds, whereas blood levels
of 0.7 mM show no effect in the Porsolt forced swim test at all. Many studies of
chronic lithium in the past were quite satisfied with levels of 0.7 mM on the
average and even studies with higher blood levels had a significant portion of
the animals with blood levels below 0.7 mM. This robust effect of lithium on the
Porsolt forced swim test provides an opening for behavioral pharmacological
analysis in the future. For instance, questions can be asked such as whether pre-
treatment with PCPA, a serotonin synthesis inhibitor, will prevent the effect of
lithium in the Porsolt forced swim test or whether presynaptic 5HT1a/1b or postsyn-
aptic 5HT2 or b-adrenergic receptors agonists/antagonists will modulate this effect.
˙
A recent hypothesis of antidepressant action is induction of neurogenesis in the
hippocampus. It could be an interesting question, whether TrkB (BDNF) receptor
agonists/antagonists will affect the lithium’s antidepressant effect in the Porsolt
forced swim test. Interestingly, inhibition of the Erk-MAP kinase pathway was
demonstrated to decrease immobility time in the forced swim test (42). However,
the same treatment also increased activity in an open field and this effect was
ameliorated by chronic lithium treatment. Hence, suggesting that the effects of
Erk inhibition is less likely to be antidepressant-like and more likely to be pro-
manic (42), a notion that is further supported by other studies on the behavioral
effects of manipulating the Erk pathway (43,44). A key question would be
whether other mood stabilizers such as valproate have a similar effect in the
Porsolt swim test. Another key question will be whether the weight loss due to
reduced appetite in chronically lithium-treated rats might cause increased activity
in the Porsolt swim test. This needs to be done by “yoking” mice to others who are
eating lithium and let them eat only the exact same amount a day as the lithium-
treated animals eat. It is also possible to add a nontoxic bitter taste to the control
food to reduce the food intake and to see if this affects Porsolt results. Our
finding that lithium effects in the Porsolt swim test require a blood level greater
than 1 mM is actually congruent with clinical reports that the antidepressant
effects of lithium require higher blood levels than the prophylactic effect.
scientists from making serious attempts to model it. Just a few attempts were done
over the years to model the entire scope of bipolar disorder with manipulations
such as sleep deprivation (45) or intermittent cocaine administration (46), but for
a variety of reasons, these tentative models did not become a central tool to
explore the biology of bipolar disorder or to screen new drugs for it (2,47).
Bioassays
Attempts had also been made over the years to develop models that are more
a bioassay than a comprehensive behavioral model. An example of such incomplete
model is the study of Bersudsky et al. (48) showing lithium inhibition of
forskolin-induced hypoactivity. This study is based on the fact that lithium bio-
chemically inhibits forskolin induced rises in cyclic AMP. The behavioral finding
is therefore a bioassay of the chemical finding. However, to become a model it
would need to be corroborated by a finding that forskolin induces hypoactivity
or a depressive-like syndrome in humans. Another example is lithium augmenta-
tion of pilocarpine-induced seizures. This phenomenon had been repeatedly
demonstrated and can be used to explore lithium-mimetic drugs. Furthermore,
the increase in seizure susceptibility after lithium treatment was demonstrated to
be dependant on inositol depletion as it is blocked by inositol administration (49)
and augmented by inositol reuptake inhibition (50). Since the inositol depletion
theory (51) is one of the leading hypotheses regarding the therapeutic effects of
lithium, the use of pilocarpine-induced seizures as a rudimentary screening
model can be justified. However, the behavioral phenomenon is unrelated to the
features of the disease and therefore the utility of this paradigm as a real model
is questionable.
Whereas the models mentioned above did contribute to the research efforts on
bipolar disorder and its treatment, there is clearly a lack of better and more appro-
priate animal models for the disease. This deficiency is repeatedly emphasized as
one of the major problems hindering bipolar disorder research (52). Some new
approaches recently suggested in the literature are summarized below.
aggression (55). Since intrusive and aggressive behaviors are one of the facets of
mania, this study tested the validity of a commonly used test for aggression, the
resident intruder paradigm in mice, as a tentative model for this facet of mania.
The results of the study demonstrate that chronic administration of lithium or
valproate, at therapeutically relevant doses, ameliorates the aggressive behavior
in the resident intruder paradigm without affecting other aspects of social behavior.
Accordingly, this study suggests that the paradigm has predictive validity and can
be used as part of a battery of models for the study of new mood stabilizers (54).
Additional new models emphasized aggression in a competition for food task
(56) and irritability measured as resistance to capture (57,58).
Interestingly, even within this relatively simple approach, a number of candi-
date manipulations were identified that had been previously demonstrated
to result in a number of behavioral changes that are similar to facets of mania.
Therefore, if all these specific models will be validated, the resulting battery will
represent a group of bipolar-like behaviors (54). For example, psychostimulant
administration does not result only in hyperactivity (as discussed above) but was
also reported to induce reduced sleep, distractibility, risk taking behavior, and
increased responses to reward—all facets of mania (54).
Individual Variability
Further exploration of the relationship between specific genes, molecules, path-
ways, and behavioral models, may be enhanced by looking at individual variability
of responses. The issue of individual differences in behavioral modeling has been
grossly neglected for many practical reasons, but this neglect may represent one
of our major failures. It is apparent that the etiology of bipolar disorder (as
of other psychiatric and nonpsychiatric diseases) is based on an interaction
between the underlying genetics and the environmental effects on the biology
where susceptible individuals that are exposed to environmental precipitating
factors will express the disease phenotype (60 –62). However, in most of our
attempts to model bipolar disorder we expose a group of “normal” animals to a
specific manipulation (whether it is a lesion, a drug, or an environmental stimulus),
and we expect them to become “sick” and allow us to explore possible new thera-
pies or the underlying biology of the “sickness.” Alternatively, with the develop-
ments in transgenic technology, we manipulate a mouse gene that is implicated
in bipolar disorder or its treatment and expect the entire population of mutant
mice to behave differently than the wild type controls. These approaches to
modeling can be helpful when there is an expectancy that a single gene mutation
may be responsible for a major part of a disease or its treatment, but this is probably
not the case for bipolar disorder, and accordingly, this approach may be limited to
demonstrating involvement of specific genes in the disease.
Any scientist who has been studying behavior knows the wide range of indi-
vidual variability within groups. Usually we try to overcome this variability by
increasing group size, but further attention to individual responses may in fact
be conducive to our research. If indeed subgroups of animals within a group
exposed to a specific manipulation can be identified as responders versus non-
responders (higher vs. lower behavioral change), it will enable us to (i ) use the
responders as a better model for the disease and (ii) explore the biological differ-
ences between the subgroups. Some work using such methods has been done in
the context of other psychiatric disorders with interesting results demonstrating a
relationship between the extent of a behavioral response and biological changes
(63– 66). For example, Cohen and her colleagues (67 –69) exposed outbred rats to
a traumatic experience (inescapable cat odor) and tested them for anxiety-like
measures immediately and 10 days after the exposure. Whereas all rats showed
anxiety-like responses immediately after exposure, only about 30% of animals
remained anxious at the later testing. Interestingly, the animals that had a long-
term effect on behavior also had long lasting changes in physiological measures
(heart rate variability) and biochemical measures (higher plasma corticosterone
and ACTH levels, increased sympathetic activity, diminished vagal tone, and
increased sympathovagal balance) suggesting that these animals may be an
26 Einat et al.
excellent model for post-traumatic stress disorder (66). It may now be interesting to
explore the underlying genetic differences that may account for the differential
responding in these subgroups of outbred rats. A number of attempts to look at
individual variability have also been done in the context of depression and
bipolar disorder (65,70,71), but for most of it, scientists who identified differential
responses tried to amplify them by breeding the different subgroups to create
different lines of responders versus nonresponders (72,73). This approach may
clarify some of the genetics by making an extreme “caricature” of the initial
strain. However, the process of inbreeding may also mask the variability of the
normal population since other biological changes that evolve during inbreeding
may overshadow the specific differences that were responsible for the initial differ-
ential responses.
Research that emphasizes diversity in responding presents two main pro-
blems. First, a technical issue: if we want to identify subgroups in a general popu-
lation, we must start with a much larger number of animals. In light of constraints
such as money, space, and constant ethical concerns about animal research, this
may not always be easy. The second problem is more conceptual. Looking at
individual variability demands that we first identify subgroups within a population
and then test them in the context of our study. For example, if one hypothesizes
that animals that show a higher response to psychostimulants may model the
susceptibility of manic patients to these drugs and wants to test the effects of a
new mood stabilizer in this model, the first stage would be to treat a large group
of animals with a psychostimulant, identify the high- versus low-responding
subgroups, then treat with the new mood stabilizer, and see if indeed it has an
effect in the susceptible group but not in the resilient animals (as we may expect
from a good mood stabilizer). However, in testing for the effects of the new drug,
the behavior is not only influenced by the new treatment or the initial differences
between the subgroups, but also by the experience the animals had during the
screening procedure. Yet, if we can identify screening procedures that are mini-
mally intrusive or invasive, further attention to individual variability may open
many new avenues for our research and may result in significantly better models.
Modeling Endophenotypes
An additional approach that in a way combines many of the tentative methods
described above is modeling endophenotypes of disease. As mentioned earlier,
endophenotypes are quantifiable components in the genes-to-behaviors pathways,
distinct from psychiatric symptoms. Endophenotypes are heritable; they are
associated with illness in the population; they are state-independent (manifest in
an individual whether or not illness is active) and may need to be elicited by a
challenge (36). In the context of animal models, it is important to emphasize that
endophenotypes are not synonymous with symptoms. As such, an animal model
of an endophenotype of bipolar disorder may not have face validity for any facet
of the disease but will have strong construct validity for the endophenotype [for
in-depth discussion of the validity of models in psychiatry see (2,47,74)]. Animal
models based on the endophenotypes approach may not be ideal for drug screening
purposes but may have great importance in the attempts to explore genes and
validate neurobiological mechanisms in model organisms (36).
Current theories regarding tentative endophenotypes for bipolar disorder
based on genetic and biological studies of patients and families include attention
Prospects for the Development of Animal Models 27
CONCLUSIONS
There are several directions in which this field can go heuristically:
1. Development of an entirely novel model. For instance, dogs are more difficult to
study than rats, involving more expense and greater ethical concerns. However,
male dogs exposed to the scent of vaginal secretions of a female dog in heat
become hyperactive, aggressive, hypersexual, and will not sleep or eat for
days while under the influence of this scent. Since hypersexuality and hyper-
activity are clearly parts of mania and since a new love affair is a frequent
stimulus for the onset of a manic episode, this model could have face validity.
The effects of lithium and other mood stabilizers on this model could be an
important direction. The biochemical effects of the pheromones of female
canines in heat on the brain of the male dog might also elicit important
information.
Recent papers (16,38) suggest that the classic field of study of lithium effects on
amphetamine hyperactivity or on the forced swim test might actually have been
a correct direction and that the contradictory results might have been due to
inadequate lithium dosing. A major effort is now underway to resolve
whether this is the case. If so, studies of other mood stabilizers and the bio-
chemical effects of higher dose lithium in these models could lead to rapid
new information.
2. Validation of additional facets of the disease may provide researchers with a
larger and broader arsenal of tools to explore the different components of
mania and depressive behavior, especially in the context of drug and mutant
animals screening (54).
3. Further attention to individual variability in behavioral response may be critical
for the development of more clinically relevant models and can forward the
understanding of genetic differences that may account for behavioral diversity.
Individual variability can also be of great importance in the exploration of
models for the endophenotypes of disease.
4. The notion of endophenotypes in bipolar disorder, suggesting an intermediate
level of exploration between symptoms and disease that may be genetically and
biologically relevant, poses a set of new challenges in modeling (36). Each of the
28 Einat et al.
REFERENCES
1. Kofman O, Belmaker RH. Animal models of mania and bipolar affective disorders,
In: Soubrie P, ed. Anxiety, Depression and Mania. New York: Karger, 1991:103– 121.
2. Einat H, Manji HK, Belmaker RH. New approaches to modeling bipolar disorder.
Psychopharmacol Bull 2003; 37(1):47–63.
3. Shaldubina A, Einat H, Szechtman H, et al. Preliminary evaluation of oral anticonvul-
sant treatment in the quinpirole model of bipolar disorder. J Neural Transm 2002;
109(3):433–440.
4. Lerer B, Ebstein RP, Felix A, et al. Lithium amelioration of reserpine-induced hypoactiv-
ity in rats. Int Pharmacopsychiatry 1980; 15(6):338– 343.
5. Einat H, Karbovski H, Korik J, et al. Inositol reduces depressive-like behaviors in
two different animal models of depression. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999; 144(2):
158 –162.
6. Belmaker RH, Lerer B, Klein E, et al. The use of behavioral methods in the search for
compounds with lithium-like activity. In: Levy A, Spiegelstein MY, eds. Behavioral
Models and the Analysis of Drug Action. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1982:343– 356.
7. Borison RL, Sabelli HC, Maple PJ, et al. Lithium prevention of amphetamine-induced
“manic” excitement and of reserpine-induced “depression” in mice: possible role of
2-phenylethylamine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1978; 59(3):259– 262.
8. Ebstein RP, Eliashar S, Belmaker RH, et al. Chronic lithium treatment and dopamine-
mediated behavior. Biol Psychiatry 1980; 15(3):459– 467.
9. Anand A, Verhoeff P, Seneca N, et al. Brain SPECT imaging of amphetamine-induced
dopamine release in euthymic bipolar disorder patients. Am J Psychiatry 2000;
157(7):1108 –1114.
10. Murphy DL, Brodie HK, Goodwin FK, et al. Regular induction of hypomania by L-dopa
in “bipolar” manic-depressive patients. Nature 1971; 229(5280):135 – 136.
11. Huey LY, Janowsky DS, Judd LL, et al. Effects of lithium carbonate on methylphenidate-
induced mood, behavior, and cognitive processes. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1981;
73(2):161–164.
12. Van Kammen DP, Murphy DL. Attenuation of the euphoriant and activating effects of d-
and l-amphetamine by lithium carbonate treatment. Psychopharmacologia 1975;
44(3):215–224.
13. Tecce JJ, Cole JO. Amphetamine effects in man: paradoxical drowsiness and lowered
electrical brain activity (CNV). Science 1974; 185(149):451 –453.
14. Angrist B, Gershon S. Variable attenuation of amphetamine effects by lithium. Am J
Psychiatry 1979; 136(6):806–810.
15. Gould TJ, Keith RA, Bhat RV. Differential sensitivity to lithium’s reversal of amphet-
amine-induced open-field activity in two inbred strains of mice. Behav Brain Res
2001; 118(1):95–105.
Prospects for the Development of Animal Models 29
16. Beaulieu JM, Sotnikova TD, Yao WD, et al. Lithium antagonizes dopamine-dependent
behaviors mediated by an AKT/glycogen synthase kinase 3 signaling cascade. Proc
Natl Acad Sci USA 2004; 101(14):5099–5104. Epub 2004 Mar 24.
17. Nixon MK, Hascoet M, Bourin M, et al. Additive effects of lithium and antidepressants
in the forced swimming test: further evidence for involvement of the serotoninergic
system. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1994; 115(1 –2):59– 64.
18. Berggren U. Effects of chronic lithium treatment on brain monoamine metabolism and
amphetamine-induced locomotor stimulation in rats. J Neural Transm 1985; 64(3– 4):
239 –250.
19. Berggren U, Tallstedt L, Ahlenius S, et al. The effect of lithium on amphetamine-induced
locomotor stimulation. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1978; 59(1):41– 45.
20. Berggren U, Engel J, Liljequist S. The effect of lithium on the locomotor stimulation
induced by dependence-producing drugs. J Neural Transm 1981; 50(2 – 4):157– 164.
21. Flemenbaum A. Does lithium block the effects of amphetamine? A report of three cases.
Am J Psychiatry 1974; 131(7):820–821.
22. Hamburger-Bar R, Robert M, Newman M, et al. Interstrain correlation between beha-
vioural effects of lithium and effects on cortical cyclic AMP. Pharmacol Biochem
Behav 1986; 24(1):9–13.
23. Lerer B, Globus M, Brik E, et al. Effect of treatment and withdrawal from chronic lithium
in rats on stimulant-induced responses. Neuropsychobiology 1984; 11(1):28– 32.
24. Arriaga F, Dugovic C, Wauquier A. Effects of lithium on dopamine behavioural super-
sensitivity induced by rapid eye movement sleep deprivation. Neuropsychobiology
1988; 20(1):23–27.
25. Cappeliez P, Moore E. Effects of lithium on an amphetamine animal model of bipolar
disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1990; 14(3):347– 358.
26. Fessler RG, Sturgeon RD, London SF, et al. Effects of lithium on behaviour induced
by phencyclidine and amphetamine in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1982; 78(4):
373 –376.
27. Pittman KJ, Jakubovic A, Fibiger HC. The effects of chronic lithium on behavioral and
biochemical indices of dopamine receptor supersensitivity in the rat. Psychopharma-
cology (Berl) 1984; 82(4):371–377.
28. Gould TD, O’Donnell KC, Picchini AM, Manji HK. Strain differences in lithium attenu-
ation of D-amphetamine-induced hyperlocomotion: a mouse model for the genetics of
clinical response to lithium. Neuropschopharmacology 2006.
29. Einat H, Einat D, Allan M, et al. Associational and nonassociational mechanisms in
locomotor sensitization to the dopamine agonist quinpirole. Psychopharmacology
(Berl) 1996; 127(2):95–101.
30. Decker S, Grider G, Cobb M, et al. Open field is more sensitive than automated activity
monitor in documenting ouabain-induced hyperlocomotion in the development of an
animal model for bipolar illness. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2000;
24(3):455–462.
31. Einat H, Szechtman H. Environmental modulation of both locomotor response and
locomotor sensitization to the dopamine agonist quinpirole. Behav Pharmacol 1993;
4(4):399–403.
32. Silverstone PH, Pukhovsky A, Rotzinger S. Lithium does not attenuate the effects of
D-amphetamine in healthy volunteers. Psychiatry Res 1998; 79(3):219–226.
33. Wald D, Ebstein RP, Belmaker RH. Haloperidol and lithium blocking of the
mood response to intravenous methylphenidate. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1978;
57(1):83–87.
34. Ebstein R, Belmaker R, Grunhaus L, et al. Lithium inhibition of adrenaline-stimulated
adenylate cyclase in humans. Nature 1976; 259(5542):411 – 413.
35. Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and
strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160(4):636– 645.
36. Gould TD, Gottesman II. Psychiatric endophenotypes and the development of valid
animal models. Genes Brain and Behavior 2006; 5(2):113 – 119.
37. Hasler G, Drevets WC, Gould TD, Gottesman II, Manji HK. Toward constructing an
endophenotype strategy for bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry 2006; 60(2):93– 105.
30 Einat et al.
38. O’Brien WT, Harper AD, Jove F, et al. Glycogen synthase kinase-3beta haploinsufficiency
mimics the behavioral and molecular effects of lithium. J Neurosci 2004; 24(30):
6791 –6798.
39. Bourin M, Hascoet M, Colombel MC, et al. Differential effects of clonidine, lithium
and quinine in the forced swimming test in mice for antidepressants: possible roles of
serotoninergic systems. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 1996; 6(3):231– 236.
40. Redrobe JP, Bourin M. Evidence of the activity of lithium on 5-HT1B receptors in the
mouse forced swimming test: comparison with carbamazepine and sodium valproate.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999; 141(4):370–377.
41. Bersudsky Y, Shaldubina A, Belmaker RH. Lithium’s effect in forced-swim test is blood
level dependent but not dependent on weight loss. Behav pharmacol 2007; 18(1):77– 80.
42. Einat H, Yuan P, Gould TD, et al. The role of the extracellular signal-regulated kinase
signaling pathway in mood modulation. J Neurosci 2003; 23(19):7311 – 7316.
43. Einat H, Manji HK, Gould TD, et al. Possible involvement of the ERK signaling cascade
in bipolar disorder: behavioral leads from the study of mutant mice. Drug News
Perspect 2003; 16(7):453–463.
44. Einat H, Manji HK. Cellular plasticity cascades: gene to behavior pathways in animal
models of bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry 2006; 59(12):1160 – 1171.
45. Gessa GL, Pani L, Fadda P, et al. Sleep deprivation in the rat: an animal model of mania.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 1995; 5(suppl):89 –93.
46. Antelman SM, Caggiula AR, Kucinski BJ, et al. The effects of lithium on a potential
cycling model of bipolar disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1998;
22(3):495–510.
47. Einat H, Kofman O, Belmaker RH. Animal models of bipolar disorder: from a single
episode to progressive cycling models. In: Myslobodsky M, Weiner I, eds. Contemporary
Issues in Modeling Psychopharmacology. Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers,
2000:165–180.
48. Bersudsky Y, Patishi Y, Bitsch Jensen J, et al. The effect of acute and chronic lithium on
forskolin-induced reduction of rat activity. J Neural Transm 1997; 104(8– 9):943– 952.
49. Kofman O, Sherman WR, Katz V, et al. Restoration of brain myo-inositol levels in rats
increases latency to lithium-pilocarpine seizures. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1993;
110(1 –2):229–234.
50. Einat H, Kofman O, Itkin O, et al. Augmentation of lithium’s behavioral effect by inositol
uptake inhibitors. J Neural Transm 1998; 105(1):31– 38.
51. Berridge MJ, Irvine RF. Inositol phosphates and cell signalling. Nature 1989; 341(6239):
197 –205.
52. Nestler EJ, Gould E, Manji H, et al. Preclinical models: status of basic research in
depression. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 52(6):503–528.
53. Willner P. The validity of animal models of depression. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1984;
83(1):1–16.
54. Einat H. Modelling facets of mania — new directions related to the notion of
endophenotypes. J Psychopharmacol 2006; 9:9.
55. Einat. Establishment of a battery of simple models for facets of bipolar disorder: a
practical approach to achieve increased validity, better screening and possible insights
into endophenotypes of disease. Behavior Genetics 2007; 37(1):244–255.
56. Malatynska E and Knapp RJ. Dominant-submissive behavior as models of mania and
depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2005; 29(4 –5):715– 737.
57. Kalynchuk LE, Pinel JP, Treit D, et al. Changes in emotional behavior produced by
long-term amygdala kindling in rats. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 41(4):438– 451.
58. Kalynchuk LE, Pinel JP, Treit D, et al. Persistence of the interictal emotionality produced
by long-term amygdala kindling in rats. Neuroscience 1998; 85(4): 1311 – 1319.
59. Machado-Vieira R, Kapczinski F, Soares JC. Perspectives for the development of animal
models of bipolar disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2004;
28(2):209–224.
60. Shih RA, Belmonte PL, Zandi PP. A review of the evidence from family, twin and
adoption studies for a genetic contribution to adult psychiatric disorders. Int Rev
Psychiatry 2004; 16(4):260–283.
Prospects for the Development of Animal Models 31
Amit Anand
Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis,
Indiana, U.S.A.
INTRODUCTION
The catecholamine (CA) hypothesis of bipolar disorder (BD)—a deficiency of CA in
depression and excess in mania—was proposed nearly three decades ago. CA
abnormalities remain the most replicated finding in the pathophysiology of BD.
However, the role of CA abnormalities in the pathophysiology of BD still
remains unclear. For example, it is unclear whether changes in CAs seen in
manic and depressed states are secondary to the mood state or primary, and it
remains to be clarified whether abnormalities in the CA system are presynaptic
or postsynaptic. Rapid advances in the field of neuroscience in the last three
decades have increased our knowledge of the role of CAs in the working of the
nervous system and provided new tools to explore CA abnormalities. Clinical
research in CA abnormalities in BD has evolved from measurement of changes in
CAs in bodily fluids and peripheral tissue to neuroendocrine challenge studies to
molecular analysis of postmortem tissue and direct visualization of CA system
with brain imaging methods such as single photon emission computed tomography
(SPECT) and positron emission tomography (PET).
Preclinical and clinical literature on the role of CAs in depression and
psychiatric illnesses and mode of action of psychotropic drugs is fairly extensive.
In this review, the main focus is on studies that have specifically investigated the
role of CAs in BD. There are only a few preclinical studies regarding pathophysiol-
ogy of BD because of a lack of suitable animal models for bipolar illness. However,
there is an extensive preclinical literature regarding pathophysiology of depression
using animal models of depression. In this review, findings from depression
research are reviewed where they are relevant to understanding of pathophysiol-
ogy of BD.
This chapter first reviews the role of CAs in physiology of mood and mood
regulation. Next, studies that have investigated CA abnormalities in BD, using
different methodological paradigms, are reviewed. The interaction of CAs with
other neuromodulators is discussed, and a model for the role of CAs in mood
regulation is presented. Finally, methodological difficulties in conducting research
in the pathophysiology of BD and future directions of research in this area are
discussed.
33
34 Garakani et al.