Learner's Attitute in The Italan Class
Learner's Attitute in The Italan Class
H. Jay Siskin
Editor
© 2008 Thomson Heinle, a part of The ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. No part of this work
Thomson Corporation. Thomson, the Star logo, covered by the copyright hereon may be
and Heinle are trademarks used herein under reproduced or used in any form or by any
license. means—graphic, electronic, or mechanical,
including photocopying, recording, taping,
web distribution or information storage and
Printed in the United States of America retrieval systems—without the written permis-
1 2 3 — 09 08 07 sion of the publisher.
155
Chapter 9
Before and After 101: Change in Learners’
Attitudes in the Italian as Foreign Language
Classroom
FL Learner Attitudes
Much attention has been paid to student attitudes toward FL learning.
Researchers have looked at student attitudes toward, for example, predominant FL
pedagogies (Bateman, 2002; Frantzen & Magnan, 2005; Mandell, 2002; Schulz,
1996), specific FLs (particularly in terms of their usefulness) (Antes, 1999; Dörnyei
& Csizér, 2002; Magnan, Frantzen, & Worth, 2004; Magnan, Murphy, & Garrett,
2004; Rifkin, 2000), FL learning and questions of gender (Chavez, 2001; Horwitz,
1988), and FL requirements (Alalou, 2001; Antes, 1999; Ely, 1986; Magnan,
Murphy, & Garrett; 2004; Morello, 1988; Oukada, 2001; Roberts, 1992). The major-
ity of FL learner attitude research, however, has been based on attitude surveys or
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 156
156 WORTH
Participants
The onset survey indicated that students in Italian 101 (N=147) were predomi-
nantly females (70%), 18–19 years old (56%), with the vast majority (97.9%)
reporting English as their first language (L1). The majority were undergraduates
(37% first-year students, 27% second-year students) and approximately half of the
total population (45%) were studying Italian to satisfy the FL requirement of two,
three, or four semesters depending on degree. Given the institution’s admission
policies, all students had completed at least two years of high school FL. The FL
they studied most frequently in high school was Spanish (63%), followed by French
(16%). Although the greatest number (46%) chose “personal interest” as their prin-
cipal reason for learning Italian, followed by study abroad and travel (25%), 4%
chose Italian because they heard it was “easier than other languages,” 5% chose
Italian because they were advised to take it, and 6% chose Italian because it fit into
their schedules. About 8% of respondents majored in or planned to major in a FL,
nearly all of them (93%) in a Romance language other than Italian.
Based on those demographic characteristics alone, there were clearly some
predisposing factors that may have impacted levels of motivation—or perhaps
more appropriately in a critical framework, investment (Norton Pierce, 1995;
Pavlenko, 2002)—very differently. Although the classrooms may have appeared
rather homogeneous (young English-L1 undergraduates who knew some
Spanish), the population was split almost equally in terms of FL requirement: 45%
were requirement learners; 55% were not. Moreover, the diversity of reasons for
choosing Italian specifically, as well as the very small percentage that planned to
major in any FL, underscores the fact that the program served students with a
wide variety of goals and future plans.
Limitations
The results of this particular study should not be overgeneralized. They are based
on the attitudes of learners at one level of study in a single language (Italian) at
one institution during one semester. However, the findings and methods may be
applicable to similar situations and indicate the need for additional studies about
student attitudes and change. The results and implications of the present study are
intended to shed light on issues of concern to LPDs and others who are responsi-
ble for curricular development, TA preparation, and other matters that can be
informed profitably by a sensitivity to learner attitudes.
158 WORTH
Table 1
Subscale means on onset AI, ranked in descending order (most to least positive
attitudes)
Subscale N Mean SD
Interest in foreign languages 144 1.85 .90
Integrative orientation 147 1.57 1.17
Attitudes toward learning Italian 141 1.57 .94
Desire to learn Italian 142 1.45 1.05
Motivational intensity 145 1.34 .79
Attitudes toward foreign language 112 .73 1.18
requirement
Instrumental orientation 143 .43 1.22
Italian class anxiety 143 -.29 1.20
Note: Mean scores can range from +3 to -3; 0 represents a neutral attitude. N = number
of respondents who answered all items in the relevant subscale.
The rankings show that respondents report the strongest level of agreement
with statements included in the subscale “Interest in foreign languages.” If we
interpret this finding as evidence that these respondents came to the Italian
course with positive attitudes toward foreign languages, the data supports previ-
ous findings that, overall, university students have positive attitudes about the
value of foreign languages (Antes, 1999; Frantzen & Magnan, 2005). The rela-
tively low level of agreement with the items in the “Instrumental orientation”
subscale also supports previous findings about language utility, that is, that stu-
dents do not view Italian as particularly helpful for their future career plans but,
instead, choose to study it for noninstrumental reasons such as personal interest,
curiosity, or travel (Magnan, Frantzen, & Worth, 2004; Magnan, Murphy, &
Garrett, 2004; Rifkin, 2000). Indeed, responses to items in the “Integrative orien-
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 159
tation” subscale are also quite positive, presumably indicating positive attitudes
about intercultural interaction and supporting the notion that Italian is viewed
by students as interesting or helpful for traveling but not necessarily useful for
career purposes.
2
Analysis of variances (ANOVAs) and t-tests were calculated to look for
significant differences in mean onset AI score across certain independent
variables. These analyses showed significant differences (p < .05) in terms of gen-
der (q73), requirement status (q78), year in school (q75), and reason for choosing
Italian (q91).
As may be expected, students who were studying Italian to fulfill the FL
requirement had a lower mean AI score (M = .52; SD = .77) than did students who
were not fulfilling a requirement (M = 1.38; SD = .60), (t(96) = 6.18, p = .000).
Females also had a significantly higher AI score (M = 1.12; SD = .80) than males
(M = .73; SD = .73), (t (95) = 2.33, p = .02). No significant differences were found
in mean AI score between age groups (F(4,144) = .637, p = .637), but an ANOVA
showed an overall significant difference by year in school (F(6 ,91) = 3.03, p = .01).
Analysis showed that mean AI scores decreased as students advanced through col-
lege, with one interesting anomaly. Fourth-year undergraduates had a much
higher mean AI score than second- and third-year students, nearly equal to the
mean for first-year students (Appendix B). One possibility was that the presence
of fourth-year students fulfilling their requirement at the last minute (predicted
to have relatively low scores) combined with fourth-year students who were tak-
ing the course purely out of interest (such as French and Spanish majors, n=14,
predicted to have relatively high scores) may have masked a relationship between
year in school and attitude. Consequently, a post hoc analysis of onset AI score by
year in school (q75) among requirement learners only, shows that AI scores were
increasingly lower as learners progressed through college, as shown in Table 2.
Table 2
Onset AI mean scores by year in school, requirement learners only
95% confidence
interval for mean
160 WORTH
Table 3
Onset mean AI scores by reason for choosing Italian, ranked descending
95% confidence
interval for mean
deteriorating attitudes, the more meaningful question might be, in what ways do
attitudes change over the course of the semester? To consider change in attitudes
toward the general variables represented by the eight subscales, a matched pair
t-test was performed to analyze significant differences between onset and exit sub-
scale scores (see Appendix C) among those respondents who completed both
administrations of the survey (n=114). Significant differences were found for six of
the subscales; and in five of the cases, the mean subscale scores were lower (less
positive) on the exit survey.
Significant onset-exit change (p < .05) was found for all subscales except
“Attitudes Toward FL Requirement” (p = .055) and “Interest in foreign languages”
(p=.62). Of those subscales showing significant change over time, change was in
the negative direction except for the subscale for “Italian class anxiety.” With cau-
tion, we might interpret the lack of significant change in the subscales “Attitudes
toward foreign language requirement” and “Interest in foreign languages” to indi-
cate relatively stable attitudes toward the greater context of FL learning, the
rationale, and the value in general. Attitudes that became significantly less favor-
able were toward more specific constructs such as attitudes toward classroom and
learning issues, Italian-specific attitudes, and questions of the perceived benefit of
studying Italian (the orientation subscales). The significant change in the variable
“Motivational intensity” supports the criticism that motivation, typically viewed as
a static and unchanging state or trait, may be better understood in learning situa-
tions in terms of investment, which permits a more dynamic view (Norton Pierce,
1995; Pavlenko, 2002).
“Italian class anxiety” was the only variable to show positive change, indicating
that, overall, reported anxiety diminished. This supports previous findings (Frantzen
& Magnan, 2005; Magnan, Frantzen, & Worth, 2004) that as students get to know
each other, they become more comfortable in the language classroom and report
feeling less anxious. Indeed, if we examine change on the individual item level, the
three items in that particular subscale that changed significantly (items 22, 26, and
27) indicated that students felt more confident, more sure of themselves, and less
embarrassed at the end of the semester than they did at the beginning.
To look at change in reported attitudes at an even more detailed level than
that represented by the subscale variables, paired-sample t-tests were performed to
analyze significant differences between onset and exit responses on individual
items. Only responses of students who completed both administrations of the sur-
vey were included. Significant change (p < .05) was shown in 38 of the 68 items, 33
in the negative direction and 5 in the positive direction. Table 4 lists the 10 items
showing the greatest change in mean score (all negative). For t-test results of
these items, see Appendix D.
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 162
162 WORTH
Table 4
Select paired samples statistics: items showing greatest negative onset-exit
change
Mean N SD Std. Error
Mean
Onset Onset Onset Onset
Item Number and Item Exit Exit Exit Exit
60. I keep up to date with Italian by 1.91 112 1.20 .11
working on it almost every day. .45 112 1.83 .17
4. I plan to learn as much Italian 1.47 113 1.49 .14
as possible. .43 113 1.98 .19
18. I find I’m losing any desire I ever 2.15 114 1.06 .10
had to know Italian. (-) 1.24 114 1.87 .17
66. I tend to approach my Italian 1.03 114 1.56 .15
homework in a random and .14 114 1.89 .18
unplanned manner. (-)
7. I would rather spend my time on 1.11 114 1.72 .16
courses other than Italian. (-) .23 114 2.02 .19
63. When I am studying Italian, I 1.04 112 1.42 .13
ignore distractions and stick to .24 112 1.65 .16
the job at hand.
14. I would like to learn as much 1.62 114 1.44 .13
Italian as possible. .84 114 1.98 .19
20. I haven’t any great wish to learn 1.63 114 1.42 .13
more than the basics of Italian. (-) .89 114 1.87 .17
13. I want to learn Italian so well that it 1.26 113 1.72 .16
becomes second nature to me. .58 113 1.98 .19
3. I love learning Italian. 1.66 113 1.26 .12
1.04 113 1.72 .16
Note: (-) indicates a reverse-coded item: Respondents disagreed with items showing
positive means; smaller exit means indicate that respondents disagreed less with the
statement at the end of the course than they did at the onset.
Individual items that showed the most change in the negative direction mir-
rored the results of change by subscale. In general, students reported having
become less interested in learning Italian and less enthusiastic about undertaking
the homework and studying connected to the Italian course. This implies that the
experiences related to the course itself, such as classroom activities, course require-
ments, and outcomes, are connected to the negative changes in attitudes. While
perhaps not surprising, this finding supports anecdotal hunches that students’
enthusiasm for language learning decreases as the semester wears on and issues of
workload may afford part of the explanation as to why waning enthusiasm occurs.
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 163
Open-Ended Questions
The open-ended questions lend more insight into issues of how and why stu-
dents’ interest in learning Italian decreased and, consequently, should be of
particular interest to LPDs and others charged with determining curriculum,
attending to questions of articulation, and preparing and supervising instruc-
tors. Because the survey was administered online, individual respondent codes
were collected as part of the password-protected log-on format. These codes
enabled the matching of each respondent’s onset and exit responses and
allowed for a consideration of changes in attitudes on the individual level, both
in terms of responses to AI items and as described in students’ own words.
Besides giving a measure of voice to each respondent, the open-ended ques-
tions were designed to give students the opportunity to express pressing or
meaningful issues related to their language learning expectations and experi-
ences and to allow for the expression of ideas and beliefs that were unantici-
pated in the design of the survey. The two sets of open-ended questions are
detailed in Table 5.
Table 5
Attitude survey open-ended questions
Item 71, which is intended to elicit opinions about the validity of the institu-
tion’s FL requirement, provides the onset-exit pair most clearly able to evidence
change in a particular attitude over time. Responses to this item were evaluated
and categorized as “completely positive” (+), “qualified positive” (+ if), “completely
negative” (-), or “qualified negative” (- if). The “qualified” categories were assigned
when the statement mentioned both positive and negative opinions of the FL
requirement and considered which opinion(s) seemed predominant. Of the 114
matched onset-exit responses, 100 gave valid responses to this item on both
administrations. Table 6 shows the responses to the FL requirement item by cate-
gory on course onset and exit administrations.
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 164
164 WORTH
Table 6
Responses to item 71, FL requirement, onset and exit administrations
Onset Exit
+ 64 54
+ if 14 23
Subtotal positive 78 77
- 20 23
- if 2 0
Subtotal negative 22 23
The number of total positive and negative responses between onset and exit
administrations remained essentially the same, which mirrors the lack of change in
the AI subscale pertaining to the FL requirement. That could be viewed as a problem-
atic result if we are of the belief that language study fosters an appreciation for FLs,
which, in theory, would help students come to appreciate the value of the require-
ment. We cannot necessarily determine whether the logic of such a cause-and-effect
is flawed or whether this course simply failed to produce that outcome. However, by
examining the onset and exit responses to this item in more detail, we can gain a
somewhat clearer picture of how attitudes indeed changed. Table 7 presents change
in attitude over time by response category and breaks those categories out according
to whether respondents were studying Italian to satisfy the FL requirement.
Table 7
Attitudes toward FL requirement, onset and exit, by requirement status
Attitude
toward FL
requirement Requirement learner
Onset Exit N Yes No
+ Attitude, No Change (n=60) + + 50 10 40
+ if + if 10 7 3
- Attitude, No Change (n=12) - - 12 12 0
+ Change (n=9) - + 3 2 1
+ if + 1 0 1
- + if 5 4 1
- Change (n=19) + - 6 6 0
+ + if 8 3 5
+ if - 3 3 0
- if - 2 2 0
Total 100 49 51
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 165
A more troubling outcome, however, was expressed by about one fifth of the stu-
dents; their attitudes toward the FL requirement became less positive—or even
worse, changed from positive to negative—after only one semester of language study.
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 166
166 WORTH
The six respondents with wholly positive statements about the FL requirement on the
onset administration and wholly negative statements on the exit survey represent the
most extreme cases of negative attitude change. Not surprisingly, all of these students
were studying Italian to satisfy the requirement. Their statements, which in some
cases are very strongly worded, merit consideration:
• Respondent 15: “a must. [This university] prides itself on having well
rounded students. Learning a second language makes students more
well rounded” (onset) versus “in theory a good idea” (exit).
• Respondent 35: “reasonable. A few people would rather die than take a
language, but [the university] offers a B.S. for them. For a B.A., I think it
is necessary to be cultured in a foreign language” (onset) versus “Not up
to date. The difference between BS and BA relies solely on language.
This should not be the case. I am glad I took one semester, but am now
forced to take another. When it is forced on you it is no longer a fun
experience. For my com arts degree it is important to have a BA, yet I
will never use Italian in my life, other than possible travels. Therefore I
think it should only be a one semester requirement” (exit).
• Respondent 91: “excellent—I wish there was also an option for three
languages, two semesters each. I love the option of learning two lan-
guages though, the more the better” (onset) versus “obsolete. I’ve heard
numerous good things about the Spanish program here, but I’ve also
found that the rest of the teachings of foreign languages aren’t, in terms
of enjoyability, up to that par” (exit).
• Respondent 40: “part of the college experience” (onset) versus “mis-
guided” (exit).
• Respondent 3: “fine by me” (onset) versus “stupid” (exit).
• Respondent 84: “a good idea” (onset) versus “dumb” (exit).
Terms such as misguided, obsolete, stupid, and dumb may seem extreme; but
they should not be discounted. Coupled with the results of the overall decrease in
AI scores over time, it appears that many students in this program felt dissatisfied
with their FL learning experiences—in some cases to the point that in less than
three months’ time, they reversed their thinking about the validity of the FL
requirement. To LPDs and others charged with attending to issues of vertical
articulation, this finding does not bode well for the attitudes that some learners
bring to subsequent courses. It also signals that for some students, the expecta-
tions for and actual experiences in Italian 101 diverged considerably.
Insight into why some learners’ attitudes become negative may be gleaned by
considering themes identified in the responses to this question. References to the
“usefulness” of a FL (e.g., Respondent 35) were common, and many students pre-
dicted that Italian would not be useful for their careers. Supporting the previous find-
ings of Frantzen and Magnan (2005) and Magnan, Murphy, and Garrett (2004), the
present study indicates that students believed that Italian was useful mostly for travel
purposes. However, such a utilitarian view of FL conflicts with this institution’s
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 167
rationale for the FL requirement, which prioritizes the broadening of student minds,
the appreciation of other cultures, and metalinguistic awareness.
Study of foreign language contributes in an important way to a broad
education in a world where the overwhelming majority of people do
not speak or read English and where much of the knowledge may
never appear in English.
Knowledge of a foreign language is important for an appreciation of
the culture of the people using that language, and helps the student to
understand the complexities of the native language. (Board of Regents,
University X, 2005)
168 WORTH
Curricular Reform
Being responsive to student needs hearkens back to the concerns of the students
in this study about questions of “usefulness.” Increasingly, the calls for consider-
ing the outcomes of requirement learners in a FL program demand that we pro-
vide “curricular closure” (Oukada, 2001, p. 110) to those who do not continue on
to upper-level courses. Providing closure implies that students need to come away
from the language sequence with more than the groundwork for further FL study.
If we agree that providing something “useful” is an important consideration, we
must begin by defining what “useful” might be and then determine what can be
achieved in the space of two, three, or four semesters. Schulz (2006) has proposed
that “lasting benefits of a short-term FL requirement experience, in addition to
some survival skills,” include
insight into (a) language as rule-governed behavior, (b) the systematic-
ity of language, (c) language as a living “organism” that changes over
time, (d) sociolinguistic variation of language use, (e) rules of pragmatic
use, (f) how language reflects culture, (g) how culture affects language
and language use, and (h) the language learning process and strategies
useful for language learning. (p. 254)
170 WORTH
TA Preparation
The finding that student attitudes toward FL learning may change, often nega-
tively, in a short amount of time has important implications for TA preparation
and supervision. In short, TAs need to be made aware that student experiences in
first-semester courses impact attitudes that students will bring with them to sec-
ond semester and beyond, both positively and negatively. It should go without say-
ing that TAs need to be aware of the goals of the individual courses they teach. But
just as importantly, TAs should understand the overall language program curricu-
lum and the philosophy of its articulation so that they may be better equipped to
bring about its success.
LPDs also should make TAs aware of the various goals and needs of the stu-
dents they will be teaching because too often, uninformed TAs make assumptions
about students based on their own language learning experiences, attitudes, and
values. TAs often are not as sensitive to questions of learner differences as are
more experienced LPDs. Given that, by definition, TAs who are graduate students
in FL programs tend to be motivated and successful language learners as well as
above-average students, they often unwittingly project their attitudes and beliefs
onto their students, resulting in frustration and disillusionment on both sides. TAs
in a program serving requirement learners must acknowledge the fact that not all
students are potential FL majors, not all intend to study abroad or even travel to
other countries. Yet the attitudes and beliefs of all learners in a classroom should
be viewed as valid, a critical perspective that becomes much easier to realize when
elementary and intermediate FL course goals are viewed as something broader
and more universally applicable than preparation for upper-level FL courses.
Regardless of the exact policy for English versus target language use adopted
by a course or program, TAs need to be prepared to execute the policy sensitively
and critically. When implementing a target language-only policy unquestion-
ingly, with no regard for students’ communicative, educational, or affective out-
comes, TAs may effectively force “communication to take a backseat to the
strictures of language policy” (Chavez, 2002, p. 194). To help TAs become more
adept at implementing course policies for classroom language use, LPDs may, for
example, lead the TAs in training exercises that sensitize them to the potentially
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 171
REFERENCES
ACTFL (1999). Standards for foreign language learning: Preparing for the 21st century.
Yonkers, NY:ACTFL.
Alalou,A. (2001). Reevaluating curricular objectives using students’ perceived needs:The
case of three language programs. Foreign Language Annals, 34, 453–469.
Antes,T.A. (1999). But will I ever use this foreign language?: Student perceptions of the
applicability of foreign language skills. Foreign Language Annals, 32, 219–233.
Bateman, B. (2002). Promoting openness toward culture learning: Ethnographic interviews
for students of Spanish. Modern Language Journal, 86, 318–331.
Bernhardt, E. B., & Berman, R.A. (1999). From German 1 to German Studies 001:A chronicle
of curricular reform. Die Unterrichtspraxis, 32, 22–31.
Brecht. D. R., & Ingold, C.W. (2000, Fall). Literacy, numeracy, and linguacy: Language and
culture and general education. Liberal Education, 86(4), 30–39.
Brecht, D. R., & Walton, A. R. (1995). The future shape of language learning in the new
world of global communication: Consequences for higher education and beyond. In
R. Donato & R. M.Terry (Eds.), Foreign language learning: The journey of a lifetime
(pp. 110–152). Lincolnwood, IL: National Textbook.
Byrnes, H. (2006). Perspectives. Modern Language Journal, 90, 244–246.
Calvin, L. M., & Rider, N.A. (2004). Not your parents’ language class: Curriculum revision
to support university language requirements. Foreign Language Annals, 37, 11–25.
Chavez, M. (2001). Gender in the language classroom. Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Chavez, M. (2002.) The diglossic foreign-language classroom: Learners’ views on L1 and
L2 functions. In S. Magnan (Series Ed.) & C. Blyth (Ed.), The sociolinguistics of
foreign-language classrooms (pp. 163–208). Boston: Heinle & Heinle.
Clément, R., Dörnyei, Z., & Noels, K. (1994). Motivation, self-confidence, and group
cohesion in the foreign language classroom. Language Learning, 44, 417–448.
Dawes, R.V. (1987). Scale construction. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 34, 481–489.
Dörnyei, Z. (1990). Conceptualizing motivation in foreign-language learning. Language
Learning, 40, 45–78.
Dörnyei, Z. (2001). Teaching and researching motivation. Harlow, UK: Longman.
Dörnyei, Z., & Csizér, K. (2002). Some dynamics of language attitudes and motivation:
Results of a longitudinal nationwide survey. Applied Linguistics, 23, 421–462.
Ely, C. M. (1986). Language learning motivation:A descriptive and causal analysis. Modern
Language Journal, 70, 28–35.
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 172
172 WORTH
Frantzen, D., & Magnan, S. (2005).Anxiety and the true beginner-false beginner dynamic
in beginning French and Spanish classes. Foreign Language Annals, 38, 171–190.
Gardner, R. C. (1985). Social psychology and second language learning: The role of
attitudes and motivation. London:Arnold.
Gardner, R. C., Tremblay, P., & Masgoret, A. (1997). Towards a full model of second lan-
guage learning:An empirical investigation. Modern Language Journal, 81, 344–362.
Horwitz, E. (1988). The beliefs about language learning of beginning university foreign
language students. Modern Language Journal, 72, 283–294.
Hotho, S. (2000).“Same”or “different”? A comparative examination of classroom factors in
second language settings. Foreign Language Annals, 33, 320–329.
Ingram, M. (2005). Recasting the foreign language requirement through study abroad: A
cultural immersion program in Avignon. Foreign Language Annals, 38, 211–222.
Kramsch, C. (2006). From communicative competence to symbolic competence. Modern
Language Journal, 90, 249–252.
Larson, P. (2006).The return of the text:A welcome challenge for less commonly taught
languages. Modern Language Journal, 90, 255–258.
Magnan, S. S. (in press). Reconsidering communicative language teaching for national
goals. Modern Language Journal.
Magnan, S., Frantzen, D., & Worth, R. (2004). Factoring in previous study of other foreign
languages when designing beginning courses. In S. Magnan (Series Ed.) & C. Barrette
& K. Paesani (Vol. Eds.), Issues in language program direction: Volume 2004.
Language program articulation: Developing a theoretical foundation
(pp. 149–171). Boston: Heinle & Heinle.
Magnan, S., Murphy, D., & Garrett, P. (2004). [Student motivation for undertaking language
study]. Unpublished raw data.
Mandell, P. (2002) On the background and motivation of students in a beginning Spanish
program. Foreign Language Annals, 35, 530–542.
Maxwell, D., & Garrett, N. (2002). Meeting national needs. The challenge to language
learning in higher education. Change, 34(3), 23–28.
Morello, J. (1988). Attitudes of students of French toward required language study.
Foreign Language Annals, 21, 435–442.
Norton Pierce, B. (1995). Social identity, investment, and language learning. TESOL
Quarterly, 29, 1–31.
Oukada, L. (2001). Toward responsive beginning language curricula. Foreign Language
Annals, 34, 107–117.
Pavlenko,A. (2002). Poststructuralist approaches to the study of social factors in second
language learning and use. In V. Cook (Ed.), Portraits of the L2 user (pp. 275–302).
Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Rifkin, B. (2000). Revisiting beliefs about foreign language learning. Foreign Language
Annals, 33, 394–420.
Rifkin, B. (2006). A ceiling effect for communicative language teaching? Modern
Language Journal, 90, 262–264.
Roberts, L. P. (1992). Attitudes of entering university freshmen toward foreign language
study:A descriptive analysis. Modern Language Journal, 76, 275–283.
Schulz, R. (1996). Focus on form in the foreign language classroom: Students’ and teachers’
views on error correction and the role of grammar. Foreign Language Annals, 29,
343–364.
Schulz, R.A. (2006). Reevaluating communicative competence as a major goal in postsec-
ondary language requirement courses. Modern Language Journal, 90, 252–255.
Sisson, D. A., & Stocker, H. R. (1989). Analyzing and interpreting Likert-type survey data.
The Delta Pi Epsilon Journal, 31(2), 81–85.
Steinhart, M. M. (2006). Breaching the artificial barrier between communicative compe-
tence and content. Modern Language Journal, 90, 258–262.
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 173
Swaffar, J. (2006). Terminology and its discontents: Some caveats about communicative
competence. Modern Language Journal, 90, 246–249.
Worth, R. (2006). Learner resistance in the university foreign language classroom.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Wisconsin.
Notes
1. The AMTB, which was created for English-speaking learners of French in Canada,
has subsequently been adapted and implemented in a variety of contexts (Gardner,
Tremblay, & Masgoret, 1997; Clément, Dörnyei, & Noels, 1994).Although care must
be taken when adapting such a scale to different populations and contexts, Clément
et al. (1994) demonstrated that this scale can be reliable, yet flexible enough across
populations to lend valuable results. Following Clément et al. (1994), Gardner et al.
(1997) and others, the wording of inventory items was modified to reflect the con-
text in question, usually just changing the word French to Italian.The ratio of posi-
tively and negatively worded statements (5 of each in every 10-item subscale) was
preserved, but the subscale that seeks to measure Attitudes Toward Target Language
Speakers was omitted because, as Dörnyei (1990) indicates, classroom FL learners
are notably different from L2 learners in that they typically have little or no interac-
tion with speakers of the target language.This lack of contact would indeed be the
case in the present context, a city that has relatively few L1 speakers of Italian, most
of whom are affiliated with the university and consequently are highly proficient
users of English. In addition, the subscale designed to measure Target Language (TL)
Use Anxiety was omitted because it was comprised of items such as “It would
bother me if I had to speak [TL] on the telephone” and “I would feel calm and sure
of myself if I had to order a meal in [TL].” Consequently, the TL Use Anxiety subscale
would be irrelevant at the time of the onset administration for beginners in a course
that had met for only four or five hours. Consequently, TL Use Anxiety was not
included in the exit survey because there would be no baseline for measuring
change and it would only serve to make the already lengthy survey even longer.
Because the potential influence of the FL requirement on attitude was of interest, a
new subscale of 10 items intended to measure attitudes toward that requirement
was created and refined with a series of pilot tests.
2. Parametric analyses (ANOVAs and T-tests) were used to determine statistically sig-
nificant relationships. Although these tests assume normal distribution and Likert-
scale responses are at the ordinal, not interval, level, it is generally accepted (Sisson
& Stocker, 1989) that because the ordinal level of responses is linear, normal distri-
bution is closely approximated when analyzing the averages of responses provided
the sample of respondents is large enough (N of at least 100, Dawes, 1987). Normal
distribution for Likert scales is thought to be adequately approximated at the
6-point level. Seven-point Likert scales were used in this study.
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 174
174 WORTH
Thank you!
qsec. Please indicate your section number for Italian 101:i
1. Sec 1 (8:50 – Instructor A)
2. Sec 2 (9:55 – Instructor B)
3. Sec 4 (11:00 – Instructor C)
4. Sec 6 (12:05 – Instructor D)
5. Sec 7 (12:05 – Instructor E)
6. Sec 8 (3:30 MW – Instructor F)
7. Sec 9 (1:20 – Instructor G)
8. Sec 11 (2:25 – Instructor H)
9. Sec 12 (3:30 MW – Instructor J)
10. Sec 13 (7:00 TR – Instructor K)
qreq. Are you enrolled in Italian 101 to meet a foreign language requirement for
your degree?
y. yes
n. no
qps.ii DId you complete the pre-course survey for this research project at the
beginning of this semester (Spring 2005)?
y. yes
n. no
I. The following section seeks to understand your attitudes and opinions about
Italian and foreign languages in general. Please indicate your level of agreement
with the following statements according to the scale indicated below:
Strongly Agree (3)
Moderately Agree (2)
Slightly Agree (1)
Neither Agree nor Disagree (0)
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 175
176 WORTH
q25 12 + Students who claim they get nervous in Italian class are just making
excuses.
q26 40 – I never feel quite sure of myself when I am speaking in our Italian class.
q27 7 – It embarrasses me to volunteer answers in our Italian class.
q28 60 – It worries me that other students in my class seem to speak Italian bet-
ter than I do.
q29 35 – I get nervous and confused when I am speaking in my Italian class.
q30 68 – I am sometimes afraid the other students will laugh at me when I speak
Italian.
Attitudes Towards Foreign Language Requirement
q31 26 + A foreign language requirement is a legitimate part of a college degree.
q32 19 + Taking foreign language courses is good for my GPA.
q33 2 + All degree programs at this university should have foreign language
requirements.
q34 1 + I would study a foreign language even if it weren’t required for me.
q35 31 + The foreign language requirement is particularly relevant in this day
and age.
q36 33 - There should be ways to be exempt from the L&S foreign language
requirement.
q37 47 – Taking foreign language courses brings my GPA down.
q38 34 – The foreign language degree requirement should be eliminated.
q39 4 – Studying a foreign language is a waste of my tuition.
q40 20 – The university foreign language requirement is obsolete in today’s society.
Interest in Foreign Languages
q41 65 + I would really like to learn many foreign languages.
q42 50 + I wish I could speak another language perfectly.
q43 8 + I often wish I could read newspapers and magazines in another language.
q44 57 + If I planned to stay in another country, I would make a great effort to learn
the language even though I could get along in English.
q45 25 + I enjoy meeting and listening to people who speak other languages.
q46 5 – Studying a foreign language is an unpleasant experience.
q47 17 – I really have no interest in foreign languages.
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 177
q48 44 – Seeing that the United States is relatively far from countries speaking
other languages, it is not important for Americans to learn foreign
languages.
q49 16 – Most foreign languages sound harsh and crude.
q50 6 – I would rather see a foreign film dubbed in English than see the film in
its original language with English sub-titles.
Instrumental Orientation
q51 63 + Studying Italian is important because it will make me appear more
cultured.
q52 3 + Studying Italian is important because it will give me an edge in competing
with others.
q53 11 + Studying Italian can be important to me because I think it will someday
be useful in getting a good job.
q54 39 + Studying Italian is important for me because it will increase my ability
to influence others.
Integrative Orientation
q55 15 + Studying Italian can be important for me because it will allow me to
meet and converse with more and varied people.
q56 9 + Studying Italian is important because it will allow me to participate
more freely in activities with other cultural groups
q57 55 + Studying Italian is important because it will allow me to be more at ease
around people who speak Italian.
q58 30 + Studying Italian is important because it will enable me to better under-
stand Italian life and culture.
Motivational Intensity
q59 41 + I make a point of trying to understand all the Italian I see and hear.
q60 14 + I keep up to date with Italian by working on it almost every day.
q61 36 + When I have a problem understanding something we are learning in my
Italian class, I always ask the instructor for help.
q62 61 + I really work hard to learn Italian.
q63 10 + When I am studying Italian, I ignore distractions and stick to the job
at hand.
q64 23 – I don’t pay too much attention to the feedback I receive in my Italian class.
q65 66 – I don’t bother checking my corrected assignments in my Italian class.
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 178
178 WORTH
II.ii The following section seeks to understand your attitudes and opinions about
Italian and foreign languages in general. Please answer the following questions
by entering your answers in the accompanying boxes.
69. The most important thing I got out of this course was...
70. I think studying foreign languages is...
71. I think the university foreign language requirement is…
72. Compared to other languages I’ve studied, Italian is…
77. Does your college or degree program have a foreign language requirement?
0. I don’t know
1. No, there is no language requirement that applies to me.
2. Yes, for me, it is two semesters
3. Yes, for me, it is three semesters
4. Yes, for me, it is four semesters
5. Not applicable
78. Are you taking this Italian course to fulfill a degree requirement?
0. no
1. yes
79. If this course is fulfilling a degree requirement, how many total semesters of this
language do you need to take?
0. not applicable
1. 1 semester
2. 2 semesters
3. 3 semesters
4. 4 semesters
80. If this course is fulfilling a degree requirement, do you plan to take more than the
required number of semesters?
0. no
1. yes
2. don’t know
3. not applicable
81. If this Italian course is NOT fulfilling a degree requirement, do you plan to take
more Italian after this course?
0. no
1. yes
2. don’t know
3. not applicable
82. What are your plans for next semester with regard to foreign language study?
0. I plan to continue with Italian.
1. I plan to switch to a different foreign language.
2. I do not plan to take any foreign language.
83. What is your primary (“native”) language(s)?
(Check all that apply)
0. English
1. Spanish.
2. French.
3. Korean.
4. Other.
84. Do members of your family, household or community speak Italian?
0. no
1. yes, but I don’t understand them.
2. yes, and I understand them but I respond to them in English.
3. yes, and I understand them and I respond to them in Italian.
85. How much Italian have you studied BEFORE COLLEGE?
0. none
1. less than one year elementary, middle or high school
2. 1 year elementary, middle or high school
3. 2 years elementary, middle and/or high school
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 180
180 WORTH
When you have completed the survey, please click the ‘Submit’ button.
(Online survey also collected NetID to distinguish among participants and provide
for onset and exit matching)
Notes
i Instructor names have been eliminated to maintain confidentiality.
ii Items on exit version only.
iii Items on onset version only.
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 182
182 WORTH
Appendix B
Onset AI Mean Scores by Year in School
95% Confidence Interval
for Mean
N Mean SD Std. Lower Upper
Error Bound Bound
Non-degree/ HS 2 2.05 .13 .09 .84 3.27
student
1st year 36 1.20 .67 .11 .97 1.42
undergrad
2nd year 27 .88 .75 .14 .58 1.17
undergrad
3rd year 17 .43 .85 .21 –.01 .87
undergrad
4th year 9 1.19 .95 .32 .46 1.93
undergrad
5th year or later 6 .82 .88 .36 –.09 1.75
undergrad
Graduate student 1 1.73 . . . .
Total 98 .98 .81 .08 .81 1.14
Note: N’s indicate numbers of respondents with complete responses on AI inventory.
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 183
Appendix C
Subscale Means for Onset and Exit Attitude Surveys
Mean N SD Std. Error
Mean
Onset Onset Onset Onset
Subscale Number and Name Exit Exit Exit Exit
1 Attitudes Toward Learning Italian 1.51 104 .98 .10
1.07 104 1.31 .13
2 Desire to Learn Italian 1.44 109 1.09 .10
1.03 109 1.38 .13
3 Italian Class Anxiety –.29 106 1.26 .12
–.07 106 1.39 .13
4 Attitudes Toward FL Requirement .71 79 1.22 .14
.50 79 1.42 .16
5 Interest in FLs 1.84 107 .89 .09
1.81 107 1.01 .10
6 Instrumental Orientation .50 110 1.19 .11
.27 110 1.32 .13
7 Integrative Orientation 1.65 112 1.13 .11
1.22 112 1.34 .13
8 Motivational Intensity 1.37 107 .79 .08
.85 107 1.10 .11
Note: Only respondents who completed both course onset and exit surveys were
included in onset-exit analysis. Incomplete responses were omitted.
95% Confidence
Interval of the Sig.
Subscale Number Std. Error Difference (2-
& Name Mean SD Mean Lower Upper t df tailed)
1 Attitudes Toward .44 1.01 .10 .24 .64 4.45 103 .000
Learning Italian
2 Desire to Learn .41 .96 .09 .22 .59 4.43 108 .000
Italian
3 Italian Class –.22 1.03 .10 –.42 –.02 –2.23 105 .028
Anxiety
4 Attitudes Toward .21 .97 .11 –.004 .43 1.95 78 .055
FL Requirement
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 184
184 WORTH
Paired Sample T-Test for Onset and Exit Subscale Means (continued)
Paired Differences (Onset-Exit)
95% Confidence
Interval of the Sig.
Subscale Number Std. Error Difference (2-
& Name Mean SD Mean Lower Upper t df tailed)
5 Interest in FLs .03 .66 .06 –.09 .16 .50 106 .620
6 Instrumental .23 .95 .09 .05 .41 2.52 109 .013
Orientation
7 Integrative .43 .99 .09 .24 .61 4.56 111 .000
Orientation
8 Motivational .52 .90 .09 .34 .69 5.93 106 .000
Intensity
30114_09_CTP 10/5/07 10:23 AM Page 185
Appendix D
Matched Pair T-Tests for 10 Items Showing Most Negative Change in
Population Mean Scores Between Onset and Exit Administrations.
Paired Differences
95%
Confidence
Mean Std. Interval of the Sig.
(Onset- Error Difference (2-
Item Exit) SD Mean Lower Upper t df tailed)
60. I keep up to date
with Italian by
working on it almost
every day. 1.46 1.65 0.16 1.16 1.77 9.40 111 .000
4. I plan to learn as
much Italian as
possible. 1.03 1.74 0.16 0.71 1.36 6.32 112 .000
18. I find I’m losing
any desire I ever had
to know Italian. (–) .91 1.71 0.16 0.59 1.23 5.69 113 .000
66. I tend to approach
my Italian homework
in a random and
unplanned manner. (–) .89 2.00 0.19 0.51 1.26 4.72 113 .000
7. I would rather
spend my time on
courses other than
Italian. (–) .88 1.83 0.17 0.54 1.22 5.12 113 .000
63.When I am
studying Italian, I
ignore distractions
and stick to the job
at hand. .80 1.55 0.15 0.51 1.09 5.50 111 .000
14. I would like to
learn as much Italian
as possible. .78 1.65 0.15 0.47 1.09 5.04 113 .000
20. I haven’t any great
wish to learn more
than the basics of
Italian. (–) .74 1.68 0.16 0.42 1.05 4.67 113 .000
13. I want to learn
Italian so well that it
will become second
nature to me. .68 1.80 0.17 0.35 1.02 4.03 112 .000
3. I love learning
Italian. .63 1.38 0.13 0.37 0.89 4.89 112 .000