Meaghan McCready - Homosexual Flirting Study
Meaghan McCready - Homosexual Flirting Study
Meaghan McCready
August 2016
GETTING THE BALLS ROLLING: FLIRTATIOUS BEHAVIORS
IN HOMOSEXUAL MEN
by
Meaghan McCready
A thesis
submitted in partial
fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of
Master of Arts in Psychology
in the College of Science and Mathematics
California State University, Fresno
August 2016
APPROVED
For the Department of Psychology:
We, the undersigned, certify that the thesis of the following student
meets the required standards of scholarship, format, and style of the
university and the student's graduate degree program for the
awarding of the master's degree.
Meaghan McCready
Thesis Author
Page
Page
which has been previously shown to be the least desirable type of pick-up line.
Therefore, for women, a verbal interaction does not seem to influence attraction as
much as physical appearances do. Several other studies conclude that nonverbal
flirtation behaviors are more effective for expressing interest than verbal flirtation
cues (Givens, 2005; Grammer, 1990). Even when conversing, nonverbal behaviors
guide perceptions of interest and receptive cues (Givens, 2005).
doing or saying. Strategic body positioning isolates the individual of interest from
having an open posture that may welcome sexual competitors. Actions of appeal
include maintaining eye contact and open palmed hand gestures while interacting
with an individual. These four nonverbal flirting categories continue to encompass
the most common flirting behaviors used to communicate attraction.
In 1971, Eibl-Eibesfeldt discretely observed nonverbal flirting behaviors of
men and women in several different cultures. Throughout these different cultures,
the same cues were used as displays of interest. Maintained eye contact, smiling,
raising an eyebrow, light touching, decreasing body proximity, body mirroring,
moistening lips, and excessive hand gestures were the observed flirting techniques
used by individuals to signal their interest during an interaction. These eight
behaviors fit within Scheflen’s (1965) four categories of nonverbal courtship
behaviors.
These common behaviors are shown to signal interest to strangers in any
situation (Grammer, 1990; Maxwell, Cook, & Burr, 1985; Perper, 1985). Perper
examined interactions of individuals in dance clubs. Perper found that when
strangers meet, they display their sexual or romantic interest with the same
behaviors: eye contact, smiling, touching the other person, self-grooming, and
posture mirroring. Grammer also found that unacquainted adults used the same
behaviors when observed in his laboratory. Even in a controlled, laboratory
setting, flirting behaviors remain consistent. Unacquainted teenagers also use the
same flirting behaviors as adults. Maxwell et al. looked at interactions between
high school teenagers. The teenagers attracted to each other showed a maintained
mutual gaze, smiling and alert facial expressions, and synchronized gestures and
movements. Grammer aimed to study laughter as a flirting signal among
unacquainted individuals, but found that amount of laughter did not correlate with
7
et al., 1993). Grammer (1990) found that men use an open body posture and lean
in toward a woman they are interested in, while Simpson et al. and Grammer et al.
found that women use an open body posture while interacting with a man they are
interested in. Several studies found that men may initiate touch in a flirtatious
interaction (Clark, Shaver, & Abrahams, 1999; McCormick & Jones, 1989; Willis
& Briggs, 1992), but women use touch more often (McCormick & Jones, 1989).
Ficheten, Taglakis, Judd, Wright, and Amsel (1992) did not find any significant
differences between gender flirting behaviors, and concluded that men and women
equally smile, touch, and lean in to people they are interested in when flirting. The
inconsistency in previous studies indicates that perhaps there are no differences in
flirting behaviors between men and women. An alternative explanation is that
differences in personality or in situations may cause a behavior frequency to
fluctuate.
The same core flirting behaviors are found in different cultures, different
ages, and at varying stages of relationships (Eibl-Eibsfeldt, 1975; Lockard &
Adams, 1980; Maxwell et al., 1985). It could also be assumed that differences in
sexual orientation may not affect flirting behaviors. However, a lack of research
exists as to whether sexual orientation influences flirting behaviors. There appear
to be no differences in flirting behavior between sexes, but men and women differ
in detection and interpretation of flirting behavior. It is unclear whether these
differences in detection and interpretation occur in same-sex courtship
interactions. An argument could be made that because there are no sex differences
in a same-sex couple, flirtation behaviors may differ from common heterosexual
flirtation behaviors.
9
Gender Differences
Although men and women display the same nonverbal flirting behaviors,
women tend to guide the courtship at several different stages. Women nonverbally
initiate the first interaction in a courtship through eye contact (Cary, 1976). In an
interaction between two strangers, a man will not approach a woman unless she
has glanced at him two or more times, or made eye contact while smiling (Cary,
1976). This seemingly innocuous behavior sends signals of interest to a man, and
essentially provides permission for him to approach. de Weerth and Kalma (1995)
found that both men and women are aware of this process. Both sexes know that
women initiate a flirting interaction, and that this initiation is done through eye
contact.
Women also regulate the speed with which a relationship progresses
(Birdwhistell, 1970; Eibl-Eibsfeldt, 1971; Morris, 1971). There are typical steps
with which a relationship progresses, from initial contact to intimacy
(Birdwhistell, 1970; Morris, 1971). The progression and regression of these steps
is determined by women (Birdwhistell, 1970; Morris, 1971). Birdwhistell offered
the example that when holding hands, a man may present the initial physical
move, but it is not until the woman reciprocates pressure onto his hand that he may
take the next step, and intertwine his fingers with hers.
Female signaling is so influential that Moore and Butler (1989) accurately
predicted outcomes of flirtatious interactions by examining the frequency of a
woman’s nonverbal signals, primarily smiling and coy glances. Surprisingly, the
physical attractiveness of a woman was less important than the number of signals
she displayed. Men approached average women who displayed a higher frequency
of nonverbal flirting signals more often than very attractive women who displayed
10
a low frequency of signals. Nonverbal flirting signals from women appear to have
an advantage over physical attractiveness in terms of influencing a man’s interest.
Considering the prevalence of nonverbal behavior in flirting, the detection
and accurate interpretation of nonverbal information is incredibly important.
Nonverbal sensitivity is defined as the ability to perceive and accurately interpret
the nonverbal cues of another person (Burgoon & Bacue, 2003). Without
nonverbal sensitivity, developing and maintaining relationships would be an
extremely difficult task (Burgoon & Bacue, 2003). The nonverbal signals of
flirting would be irrelevant without an accurate ability to detect and infer
appropriate behavior responses from them.
Several studies illustrate a significant difference between men and women
in their nonverbal sensitivity levels. Women, in general, are better at accurately
understanding nonverbal behaviors (Hall, Murphy, & Mast, 2006; Riggio &
Feldman, 2005; Watkins & Hall, 2014). The Social Skills Inventory (SSI), a self-
report measure that assesses encoding, decoding, and regulation skills of social
communication, consistently shows a sex difference in scores, with women
scoring higher than men on emotional and social sensitivity (Riggio & Feldman,
2005, Watkins & Hall, 2014). Women consistently score higher than men do on
the nonverbal sensitivity scale of the Profile of Nonverbal Sensitivity instrument
(Hall et al., 2006).
Women also excel at assessing flirting perceptions (Farris et al., 2008).
Good flirting perception is correctly recognizing when another individual is
displaying behaviors indicative of romantic or sexual interest (Watkins & Hall,
2014). There is a discrepancy between men and women in the level of sexual
interest signaled through flirting behaviors (Abbey, 1982). Men often overestimate
the sexual intent of a woman’s behaviors (Abbey & Melby, 1986; Henningsen,
11
2004; Henningsen, Kartch, Orr, & Brown, 2009; Moore, 2002; Ostler, 2003).
When men see any flirting behaviors from women, they interpret these behaviors
as more sexual, serious, and intense than when women interpret the same
behaviors (Moore, 2002). Haselton (2003) referred to this as the sexual
overperception bias. Men also interpret flirting behaviors as more intense and
sexual than the women displaying them intend them to be (Moore, 2010). Men
perceive women as trying to be more seductive than women intend to be (Haselton
& Buss, 2000; Levesque, Nave, & Lowe, 2006). Rejection signals are also judged
to be weaker when viewed by a man, than when viewed by a woman (Moore,
2002). This tendency to overestimate a woman’s flirting intentions may have been
something that developed evolutionarily. If men perceive more women as
interested, they have more opportunity to produce more offspring (Haselton &
Buss, 2000; Moore, 2002).
These gender differences in flirting perceptions have been found in a
number of studies. One reason for varying flirting perceptions can be attributed to
the motivations behind flirting that varies between the sexes. People do not always
flirt for the purpose of obtaining a sexual or romantic relationship (Mongeau et al.,
2004). In a review of previous flirting studies, Henningsen (2004) found six
motivations for flirting. These six motivations include facilitating sexual contact,
having fun, exploring potential for a relationship, maintaining an existing
relationship, raising self-esteem, and persuasion. Five similar reasons surfaced
when Mongeau et al. had college students identify their personal goals for their
most recent first date. Sex, fun, identifying romantic potential in flirting partners,
promoting friendship, and reducing uncertainty in determining attraction were
common motivations.
12
Differences have been found in the motivations underlying why men and
women flirt (Henningsen, 2004; Henningsen et al., 2008; Henningsen,
Henningsen, McWorthy, McWorthy, & McWorthy 2011). Men attribute sexual
motivations to flirting interactions more often than any other motivation
(Henningsen et al., 2008). Additionally, men attribute sexual motivations to
flirting interactions more often than women do (Henningsen, 2004; Henningsen et
al. 2008). Women attribute fun and relational motivations to flirting interactions
more than men (Henningsen, 2004). Women appear to have more diverse
motivations underlying their flirting interactions than men. These varying
motivations could contribute to the disparity and miscommunication experienced
between men and women in identifying flirting intentions.
Ostler (2003) also demonstrated a discrepancy between the sexes in
interpreting flirtatious and dating behaviors. Male and female participants were
given 16 different female dating behaviors and judged how much these behaviors
reflected sexual consent. Males rated more behaviors as reflective of sexual
consent than did females. Moreover, males judged more behaviors as “definitive”
of sexual consent, while females judged the same behaviors as “probative,” or
displaying intrigue, but absolutely not “definitive.”
Henningsen et al. (2009) had participants judge an interaction in a written
scenario wherein a woman either verbally or nonverbally initiates flirting
behaviors with a man. It was found that when the woman used verbal flirting cues,
sexual interest was perceived to be equally high by males and females. It appears
that verbal flirting techniques are unambiguous, and easily interpretable by both
sexes. In the nonverbal flirting scenario, males perceived a greater sexual interest
from the woman in the scenario than females did. Because nonverbal behaviors
are ambiguous, they leave room for misinterpretation. This causes a potential
13
problem for interactions between men and women, as women almost exclusively
flirt behaviorally and men flirt verbally and behaviorally.
Evolutionary Psychology
Although the same basic flirting behaviors are used by men and women,
there is a discrepancy in how men and women detect and interpret these flirting
behaviors. The Parental Investment Theory, proposed by Trivers (1972), provides
an evolutionary explanation for these discrepancies. This theory proposes that
because parental investment differs between the sexes, the process by which males
and females use mating strategies to select mates differs as well. Producing and
rearing offspring is costlier for females than males (Trivers, 1972). The
evolutionary goal for females is to create a better quality of offspring because the
risks in parental investment are greater for females than males. Thus, females are
more selective when choosing a partner (Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Trivers, 1972).
Because of this selectivity, males have to compete intrasexually for females. The
evolutionary goal for males is to produce as many offspring as possible. Unlike
females, producing offspring is of relatively low cost to males, as they can
reproduce throughout their entire lives and are physically unhindered by
pregnancy. These reproductive sex differences cause mating strategy differences,
which may lead to the discrepancy in detecting and interpreting flirting behaviors
between men and women (Schmitt, 2005).
Flirting is beneficial to both men and women in finding a mate. For men,
flirting is a way of displaying desirable traits to women. Desired traits, such as
pro-social behavior and sense of humor are shown through flirting (Greengross &
Miller, 2011). Good flirting skills are indicators of a high quality mate. Flirting is
a good way for men to gauge a woman’s interest level as well (Clark et al., 1999;
14
Henningsen, 2004). Men can effectively assess which women are interested and
which are not by evaluating the woman’s nonverbal behaviors (Ahmad & Fisher,
2010). When a woman displays rejection behaviors, the man can move on to a
different woman to give himself more opportunities for finding a mate (Ahmad &
Fisher, 2010).
Flirting also benefits females. Because females suffer a major loss when an
offspring fails, their flirtation strategies allow them to assess the beneficial
qualities and possible risks of a potential mate (Moore, 2002). Flirting allows
females to gain an understanding of whether the male would be a good investment
of her time and resources for producing offspring. Flirting gives females a sense of
the male’s personality and desirable traits (Clark et al., 1999; Greengross &
Miller, 2011). Male flirting can help females decide if he is a good investment.
Flirting also establishes a connection between men and women (Clark et al.,
1999). This connection can further aid women in accurately determining whether a
potential mate is a good investment.
Same-sex Flirting
Previous studies about flirting and courtship behavior have been primarily
focused on heterosexual men and women. Little research has examined gay or
lesbian flirting and courtship behavior. Reproductive constraints and culturally
defined gender roles give rise to male and female differences in flirting
(VanderMolen, 2013). Different flirting techniques may be used in an interaction
comprised of two partners of the same sex, because there are no sex differences.
Two partners of the same sex may not show differences in detecting, interpreting,
or motivations behind flirting. The interaction may not involve as many
misunderstandings between two individuals who have similar motivations or
15
flirting styles, which are the primary influences of the problems in opposite sex
flirting interactions.
Men tend to overly assume that a woman’s flirting behavior indicates a
sexual motivation (Henningsen, 2004; Moore, 2002; Ostler, 2003), men tend to
have more sexual motivations underlying their own flirting (Henningsen et al.,
2008), and men also utilize verbal flirtation techniques more often than women
(Clark et al., 1999; de Weerth & Kalma, 1995). A flirtatious interaction between
two homosexual men may therefore be quite brief, verbally direct, mutually well
received, and motivated by sex. In one of the few studies that examined the mating
preferences of homosexual men, Gobrogge et al. (2007) found that homosexual
men sought sexual encounters from dating more than heterosexual men. This
aligns with the previous findings that homosexual men report more casual sex than
heterosexual men (Bailey, Gaulin, Agyei, & Gladue, 1994). In an interaction
between two men, who both assume and exhibit the same sexual motivations
behind flirting, it would follow that homosexual men would have a high frequency
of sexual encounters. Heterosexual men are hindered by the availability and
acceptance of women.
Lesbian women may have an entirely different flirting experience than gay
men and heterosexual women. Women tend to assume less sexual intention from
flirting behaviors and flirt for many reasons other than sexual intent (Henningsen
et al., 2008). Women also tend to be more sensitive to nonverbal behaviors (Hall
et al., 2006; Riggio & Feldman, 2005). Women regulate the progression of
relationships, and initiate flirting with nonverbal signals (Cary, 1976; Morris,
1971). Flirting between lesbians may therefore be a more complicated interaction.
Lesbian courtship behavior primarily involves mutual nonverbal communication,
16
and shared dating goals between partners (Bernarte, Alday, Calajatan, Fraginal,
Lauchengco, 2015; Rose & Zand, 2002).
Rose and Zand (2002) surveyed lesbian women about their dating and
courtship initiation behaviors and relationship roles. It was found, unsurprisingly,
that lesbians use the same common verbal and nonverbal flirting techniques as
heterosexuals in order to initiate a relationship. However, there were several major
differences between lesbians and heterosexual individuals in their flirting
relationship roles. Heterosexual men and women often adhere to traditional gender
roles in dating (Hall, Carter, Cody, & Albribright, 2010). Few lesbians reported
adhering to traditional gender roles, primarily objecting to the notion that women
should limit sexual contact. The majority of lesbian women surveyed reported a
more serious relationship goal desired from dating. Flirting was primarily used by
lesbians to initiate a relationship, rather than any other motivation. After a
relationship had been established, lesbians tended to progress rapidly in their
relationships. This is probably due to the high communication and assertiveness
skills they displayed. This study supports the notion that flirting techniques and
behaviors are universal, but detection, interpretation, and motivation behind these
techniques vary between individuals.
Potapova (2012) conducted interviews with 15 lesbian women about how
they signal their erotic interest to other women. Nonverbal signals, not unlike in
the common flirting behaviors found in previous studies on heterosexual flirting,
were identified. Maintaining eye contact, light touching, and narrowing physical
proximity were the three most commonly reported behaviors used to signal
interest, and were identified as ways other women signal interest to them. An
atypical flirting technique utilized by lesbian women and not by heterosexual
women is the common use of verbal interaction. A majority of the lesbian women
17
flirting situation (Frisby, Dillow, Gaughan, & Nordlund, 2010). Homosexual men
and women also did not differ in their scores on the flirting styles. Homosexual
individuals may not identify with traditional gender roles, which explains their
overall low scores on the traditional flirting style. Homosexual individuals deviate
from traditional gender roles regarding dating. Homosexual individuals create new
dating scripts and stray from societal gender expectations (Riggle, Olson,
Whitman, Rotosky, & Strong, 2008).
Bernarte et al. (2015) found similar verbal and nonverbal flirting techniques
in interviews with homosexual men and women. Eye contact, smiling, touching,
alert posture, and reducing physical proximity were reported as the most common
flirting behaviors. Verbal flirting techniques included flattery, humor, and
disclosure. Homosexual men were found to excel in more verbal techniques. Like
heterosexual men, homosexual men preferred using verbal flirtation in expressing
their interest to other men, and also recognized verbal flirting techniques more
readily. Homosexual women excelled at nonverbal techniques. The majority of
these homosexual women reported a preference for using nonverbal techniques to
signal their interest in other women.
Nonverbal flirting techniques are universally used and identified as
conveying sexual or romantic interest in someone (Greer & Buss, 1994; Moore,
2010). Although the use of nonverbal flirting techniques is frequent, verbal flirting
techniques appear to be more common in same-sex interactions than in opposite
sex interactions. These verbal techniques may appear out of the necessity for
assessing the person of interest’s sexual orientation. Self-identified non-
heterosexual individuals account for roughly 4% of the population in America
(Gates, 2014). Because of the small likelihood of any given individual being
19
homosexual, nonverbal behaviors alone might not be adequate for detecting and
interpreting sexual or romantic intentions.
There is a lack of research in the area of the flirting behaviors, motivations,
and styles of homosexual men in particular. More research has focused on
homosexual women, but primarily all information known about flirting pertains to
heterosexuals, which represent only one specific sexual orientation. The present
study aims to fill this gap in knowledge about non-heterosexual flirting techniques
in an exploratory approach using two parts. The goal of Study 1 was to determine
whether non-heterosexual individuals use different flirting communication styles
and motivations behind flirting than heterosexual individuals. A separate goal for
Study 1 was to assess which approaches to flirting are most commonly used by
non-heterosexual individuals. The goal of Study 2 was to determine whether the
same nonverbal flirtation behaviors will be displayed at the same frequency by
homosexual men as by heterosexual men.
CHAPTER 3: METHODS AND MATERIALS
Study 1
Participants
Participants were recruited from a variety of online groups and forums and
are a non-probability sample of convenience. A total of 307 (167 males, 134
females, 6 others) heterosexual (N = 161) and non-heterosexual (N = 146)
individuals completed the survey. Non-heterosexual participants were recruited
from Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, Transgender, and sexual minority equality (LGBT+)
Facebook groups, local LGBT+ community websites, and Reddit forums for
LGBT+ members. Heterosexual participants were recruited from Facebook and
Reddit survey forums. Participants were primarily single (N = 153), White (N =
231) adults ranging from 18 to 60 years old (M = 26.6, SD = 8.16).
Materials
Participants were given a survey that assesses sexual orientation and flirting
style. Sexual orientation was assessed by a sliding scale similar to the Kinsey
Scale (Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin, 1948; see Appendix A). The Kinsey Scale is a
self-reported scale of sexual orientation. The scale rates exclusivity of sexual
orientation on a scale of “0” (exclusively heterosexual with no homosexual
tendencies or desires) to “6” (exclusively homosexual with no heterosexual
tendencies or desires). This adaptation of the Kinsey Scale is a sufficient measure
of sexual orientation because it is a self report measure, and sexual orientation can
only be identified by each individual for themselves.
Participants’ flirting style was assessed by The Flirting Styles Inventory-
Revised (FSI-R; Xing & Hall, 2015). The FSI-R (see Appendix B) assesses the
21
way a person communicates his or her romantic interest in others. These styles are
expressed through traditional, physical, playful, sincere, and polite approaches.
The traditional flirting style assesses whether an individual adheres to
traditional gender roles during courtship. A high score on the traditional style
indicates that an individual believes a man, not a woman, should initiate
interactions and display dominant and assertive behaviors when pursuing someone
of interest. The physical flirting style assesses the degree to which an individual
can comfortably and effectively communicate their sexual interest. A high score
on the physical flirting style indicates that an individual easily expresses their
courtship intentions through nonverbal behavior. The playful flirting style assesses
whether flirting is regarded as a fun activity, rather than a signal of interest. A high
score on the playful flirting style indicates that an individual flirts with people who
they are not interested in sexually or romantically. The sincere flirting style
assesses whether an individual flirts with the intention of creating an emotional
bond with a person they are genuinely interested in. A high score on the sincere
flirting style indicates that an individual flirts in ways that encourage self
disclosure and bonding. The polite flirting style assesses how strictly an individual
behaves according to societal rules in a flirtatious interaction. A high score on the
polite flirting style indicates an individual who has difficulty expressing sexual
desire and monitors their behavior to insure an adherence to proper manners.
Flirting style is determined by agreement ratings on a “1” (strongly
disagree) – “7” (strongly agree) Likert scale of 23 statements that correspond to
attitudes, beliefs, or behaviors defined by one of the five flirting styles. Each style
has been shown to be mutually exclusive of the other styles. The Flirting Styles
Inventory has good internal consistency ( = .68 - .87 for each item) and
predictive validity ( = .83; Hall et al., 2010).
22
Design and Procedure
Study 1 employed a non-experimental research design. Participants
completed an online survey consisting of an informed consent (see Appendix C),
demographic questions (see Appendix D), Kinsey Scale, and Flirting Styles
Inventory.
Study 2
Participants
Participants consisted of 30 homosexual and 30 heterosexual men from bars
and clubs in the city of Fresno. Men were assumed to be homosexual when
exhibiting flirtatious behaviors with a person of the same sex while inside
established gay bars and clubs. Although sexual orientation is fluid and may
change over an individual’s lifetime (Diamond, 2008), current sexual behavior
was the focus of the present study. Thus, current sexual orientation was
determined by the individual’s participation in same-sex interactions in a known
gay establishment. Men were assumed to be heterosexual when exhibiting
flirtatious behaviors with a person of the opposite sex. The sample was a
purposive, systematic observation of men who were exhibiting flirtatious
behaviors.
Materials
Participants were observed systematically in their nonverbal flirting
behaviors for 5 minutes. Judges used a phone timer app for consistency. The
specific flirting behaviors recorded were determined from a factor analysis on 163
commonly used flirting acts reported by heterosexual individuals (see Appendix
E). The factor analysis of the nonverbal flirting acts resulted in seven categories of
flirtatious behaviors: simple touch, eye contact, provocative display, attention
23
seeking behaviors, signaling interest, playful behaviors, and acting coy (see
Appendix F). Behaviors were recorded on a tally counter phone app (Tsopanakis,
2015). The flirtatious behaviors were added to the app as separate categories.
Study 1
The goal of study 1 was to determine whether heterosexual and non-
heterosexual individuals differ in their flirting and communication styles. This
goal was evaluated in two ways. First, mean scores of each flirting style were
compared within heterosexual, bisexual, and homosexual groups to determine
which styles are utilized the most. Secondly, mean scores of each flirting style
were compared between sexual orientation and gender. Individuals who reported
their sexual orientation as a 0, or 1 on the Kinsey scale were categorized as
heterosexual. Individuals who reported their sexual orientation as a 2, 3, or 4 on
the Kinsey scale were categorized as bisexual. Individuals who reported their
sexual orientation as a 5 or 6 on the Kinsey scale were categorized as homosexual.
Only the results from participants who completed the entire survey were retained.
The mean scores of each flirting style significantly differed for bisexual
individuals, F(2.75, 192.64) = 138.84, p < .001, p2 = .67. A Bonferroni post hoc
test determined that scores on the sincere flirting style (M = 6.27, SD = .63) were
significantly higher than all other flirting styles, and scores on the traditional
flirting style (M = 2.42, SD = .99) were significantly lower than all other flirting
styles (p < .001 for each comparison).
The mean scores of each flirting style significantly differed for homosexual
individuals, F(3.09, 287.07) = 234.73, p < .001, p2 = .72. A Bonferroni post hoc
test determined that scores on the sincere flirting style (M = 6.27, SD = .56) were
significantly higher than all other flirting styles, and scores on the traditional
flirting style (M = 2.43, SD = .84) were significantly lower than all other flirting
styles (p < .001 for each comparison). Figure 1 shows means and standard
deviations for heterosexual, bisexual, and homosexual individuals on each of the
five flirting styles.
Playful. Gender did not show a significant effect on playful style scores,
F(1, 295) = .14, p = .71, p2 < .001, meaning that men (M = 3.56, SD = .85) and
women (M = 3.60, SD = .90) did not differ in their scores on the playful flirting
style. The main effect for sexual orientation indicated a significant difference
between homosexual, heterosexual, and bisexual scores on the playful style F(2,
295) = 3.28, p = .039, p2 = .022. A Bonferroni post hoc test indicated that
29
Sincere. There was a significant main effect for gender on sincere style
scores, F(1, 295) = 5.02, p = .026, p2 = .017, meaning that men and women
differed in their scores on the sincere flirting style. A Bonferroni post hoc test
indicated that women (M = 6.32, SD = .57) scored significantly higher than men
(M = 6.19, SD = .60). Sexual orientation indicated no significant difference
between homosexual, heterosexual, and bisexual scores on the sincere style F(2,
295) = .49, p = .62, p2 = .003. There was no significant gender by sexual
orientation interaction effect on the sincere flirting style, F(2, 295) = .39, p = .68,
p2 = .003.
homosexual, heterosexual, and bisexual scores on the polite style F(2, 295) = .59,
p = .003, p2 = .039. A Bonferroni post hoc test indicated that heterosexual
individuals (M = 4.31, SD = .83) scored significantly higher on the polite style
than homosexual (M = 3.84, SD = .92, p = .04) and bisexual individuals (M = 3.82,
SD = .95, p = .01). There was a significant gender by sexual orientation interaction
effect on the polite flirting style, F(2, 295) = 4.13, p = .017, p2 = .03. This
interaction effect indicates that the effect of sexual orientation on flirting style
depends on the gender of the participant. A profile plot of the interaction effect
shows that heterosexual, bisexual, and homosexual men have less variance in their
polite style scores than heterosexual, bisexual, and homosexual women. The
interaction effect also shows that in bisexual individuals, men appear to score
higher than women on the polite style. Figure 3 shows means and standard
deviations of heterosexual, bisexual, and homosexual men and women’s scores on
the polite flirting style.
Table 1
Behavior M SD M SD t df p d
Eye contact 10.37 4.98 4.30 3.10 -5.67 48.53 <.001* 1.46
Provocative display 3.40 5.75 1.40 2.18 -1.78 37.13 .083 .46
Attention seeking 3.97 4.03 2.60 2.37 -1.60 46.94 .116 .41
Playful behavior 4.93 6.64 1.63 1.67 -2.64 32.66 .013* .68
Acting coy .03 .18 .63 1.63 2.01 29.73 .054 .51
Study 1
appears that heterosexual individuals would rather have an interaction occur using
any other style, than follow societal gender rules for their courtship behaviors.
The approach many scientists take in this kind of research aims to discover
the differences between heterosexual and non-heterosexual individuals. In the
present study, the most striking results are the similarities in trends between
heterosexual and non-heterosexual individuals.
heterosexual group whose scores were not significantly lower than heterosexual
scores on the two styles that indicate strong adherence to gender roles and societal
norms. Carballo‐Diéguez et al. (2004) concluded that assertive and receptive roles
in a sexual relationship between men are typically assigned by masculinity and
femininity characteristics of partners. Bisexual and homosexual men reported that
they take receptive sexual roles when their partner is more masculine, tall,
aggressive, or handsome. More assertive roles are taken when the individual’s
partner is more effeminate, shorter, less aggressive, or less handsome. Non-
heterosexual male courtship behavior is still influenced by the masculinity and
femininity of the partners, even though the partners are of the same sex. Bisexual
men utilize the traditional and polite flirting styles as much as heterosexual men
do. This is dissimilar to the scores of bisexual women. Bisexual women achieved
the lowest scores on these styles compared to heterosexual and homosexual
women. The interaction effect for the traditional and polite styles also indicated
less variance in men’s scores than women’s scores. The smaller variance of men’s
scores implies that women differ widely based on sexual orientation, but there is
less discrepancy between sexual orientations in men. This reluctance of men to
surrender societal norms and gender roles in their relationships may stem from
more intense social distain and general unacceptance of male homosexuality when
compared to female homosexuality (Herek, 2002a; LaMar & Kite, 1998).
An interesting sexual orientation difference was found in the playful flirting
style. Individuals who score highly on the playful flirting style may flirt with
people with whom they are not necessarily sexually or romantically interested
(Hall et al., 2010). Individuals employing the playful flirting style flirt for the fun
of flirting. The present study found that bisexual individuals scored significantly
higher than heterosexual individuals on this style. Xing and Hall (2015) similarly
38
bond with their potential partner. Hall et al. (2010) found the physical and sincere
flirting styles positively correlated with physical chemistry and emotional
connection in a relationship. Individuals who scored highly on the physical and
sincere flirting styles also commonly experience a rapid escalation in their
relationships (Hall et al., 2010). Although Rose and Zand (2002) found that
lesbian women progress quickly in their relationships, are more interested in long
term goals, and their primary goal for fliting is because they want a relationship,
no sexual orientation differences were found on the sincere flirting style. Xing and
Hall (2015) similarly did not indicate a difference between bisexual and
heterosexual individuals on this style.
The underlying goals of the physical and sincere flirting styles include
initiating a physical relationship and a desire for intimacy, respectively. The lack
of sexual orientation differences in these styles implies that these goals are based
more on individual preferences, such as a desire for a short or a long term
relationship, rather than being influenced by sexual orientation. Gender had a
significant effect on the physical and sincere flirting styles. Consistent with the
results of Hall et al. (2010) and McBain et al. (2013), women scored significantly
higher than men on both the physical and sincere styles. Women typically seek
long term relationships more often than men (Buss, 2015). This gender difference
in a desire for obtaining a long-term relationship leads to more women using a
physical and sincere style to communicate their attraction.
Although the gender inversion hypothesis has been highly criticized for its
incorrect assumption that gender identity and sexual orientation are
interchangeable (Fuss, 2013), it continues to be supported in multiple, modern
studies (Lippa 2005, 2008) as well as being a general belief of individuals in
society (Herek, 2002a; Herek, Gillis, & Cogan, 2015; Mosier, 2014).
The gender inversion hypothesis was not supported in the present study.
Were this the case, the sexual orientation differences in flirting styles would have
been similar to the gender differences in flirting styles. Non-heterosexual men
would utilize the physical and sincere styles significantly more than heterosexual
men, mirroring the scores of heterosexual women on those styles. Non-
heterosexual women would have scored lower on the physical style, mirroring the
scores of heterosexual men. This flirting style mirroring did not happen in the
present study. Sexual orientation and gender appear to influence different aspects
of flirting motivations and communication.
Study 2
Previous literature has emphasized the universal nature of specific flirting
behaviors. In Scheflen’s (1965) research on nonverbal courtship behaviors, four
categories of flirting were identified. These categories consisted of courtship
readiness, preening behaviors, positional cues, and actions of appeal. Eibl-
Eibesfeldt (1971) identified eight categories of flirting behaviors. These categories
consisted of eye contact, smiling, raising an eyebrow, light touching, decreasing
body proximity, body mirroring, moistening lips, and excessive hand gestures.
Since these two seminal studies, the same kinds of behaviors have been identified
as the most used flirting behaviors throughout decades of research (Grammer,
1990; Moore, 2010; Perper, 1985). Each of these previously identified behaviors
41
were accounted for in the seven categories created for the present study. These
seven categories consisted of simple touch, eye contact, provocative display,
attention seeking, signaling, playful behavior, and acting coy. These behaviors
were all seen in the present study’s observations, however, the behaviors varied
greatly in their frequency of use between homosexual and heterosexual men.
Eye contact appeared to be the most utilized nonverbal behavior for both
heterosexual and homosexual men. This is perhaps because when compared to the
other behaviors, eye contact is the least risky, and still communicates effectively
while using little effort. Neither heterosexual nor homosexual men were prone to
acting coy. The behavior of acting coy has connotations of being a feminine action
(Hrdy, 1986). Societal gender roles dictate that women must be conservative in
their mate selection, or deal with consequences of being viewed as promiscuous
(Bordini & Sperb, 2013). Acting coy became a woman’s solution to expressing
interest while maintaining her reputation. It continues to be seen that men remain
uncomfortable breaking a masculine barrier, despite being attracted to other men.
Significant differences were found between heterosexual and homosexual
men’s average frequency of simple touch, eye contact, signaling, and playful
behavior. Heterosexual men consistently displayed these behaviors at higher
frequencies within 5-minute interactions. It is entirely possible that although they
are in a safe place that welcomes sexual diversity, homosexual individuals are
accustomed to hiding their sexual orientation (Giddings & Smith, 2001; Harrison,
2001; Herek, 1993). Openly gay men are less likely to be offered jobs (Tilcsik,
2011) more likely to earn a lower pay rate (Berg, & Lien, 2002), and more likely
to experience violence and abuse (Comstock, 1992; Sue, 2010) because of their
sexual orientation. Perhaps homosexual men are not as comfortable displaying
openly flirtatious behaviors at the same liberal rates as heterosexual men.
42
Heterosexual men have not had to worry about the violence and discrimination
accompanied by their sexual expressions in the way homosexual men have.
General Discussion
A major issue that occurred in the present study was the persistent violation
of homogeneity of variances between sexual orientation groups. Heterogeneity of
variances occurred in every analysis conducted in this study. The original analysis
planned for examining this data included the comparison of two sexual orientation
groups (heterosexual and non-heterosexual) instead of three (heterosexual,
bisexual, and homosexual). However, when bisexual and homosexual individuals
were placed together in one ‘non-heterosexual’ group, the homogeneity of
variances test for every flirting style in the factorial ANOVA was violated. When
the group was separated into bisexual and homosexual groups, the variances were
equal in all flirting styles except for the polite style. Separating the sexual
orientations into different groups made the groups more legitimately comparable.
The main implication of this solution is simply that bisexual and homosexual
individuals are different, and problems arise when they are lumped together. This
can be seen in the discrepancies of scores for bisexual and homosexual individuals
for the traditional, playful, and polite styles. These sexual orientation boundaries
should be considered in future research before analyzing bisexual and homosexual
individuals as one group. Sexual orientation occurs on a spectrum, and is not a
dichotomy.
Another weakness of the present study is the lack of verbal communication
assessment. Previous research has concluded that verbal communication among
heterosexual flirtatious interactions is secondary to behavioral communications
(Givens, 2005; Grammer, 1990). However, Potapova (2012) suggested that verbal
43
Abbey, A., & Melby, C. (1986). The effects of nonverbal cues on gender
differences in perceptions of sexual intent. Sex Roles, 15(5-6), 283-298.
Ahmad, M., & Fisher, M. (2010). Men’s perspectives on women’s nonverbal cues
of sexual interest. EvoS Journal: The Journal of the Evolutionary Studies
Consortium, 2(2), 72-80.
Bailey, J. M., Gaulin, S., Agyei, Y., & Gladue, B. A. (1994). Effects of gender and
sexual orientation on evolutionarily relevant aspects of human mating
psychology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(6), 1081-
1091.
Berg, N., & Lien, D. (2002). Measuring the effect of sexual orientation on income:
Evidence of discrimination? Contemporary Economic Policy, 20(4), 394-
414.
Bordini, G. S., & Sperb, T. M. (2013). Sexual double standard: A review of the
literature between 2001 and 2010. Sexuality & Culture, 17(4), 686-704.
Buss, D. (2015). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind. New
York, NY: Routledge.
46
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary
perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100(2), 204-232.
Carballo‐Diéguez, A., Dolezal, C., Nieves, L., Díaz, F., Decena, C., & Balan, I.
(2004). Looking for a tall, dark, macho man…sexual‐role behaviour
variations in Latino gay and bisexual men. Culture, Health & Sexuality, 6(2),
159-171.
Comstock, G. D. (1992). Violence against lesbians and gay men. New York, NY:
Columbia University Press.
Farris, C., Treat, T. A., Viken, R. J., & McFall, R. M. (2008). Sexual coercion and
the misperception of sexual intent. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(1), 48-66.
Ficheten, Taglakis, Judd, Wright, and Amsel (1992). Verbal and nonverbal
communication cues in daily conversations and dating. The Journal of Social
Psychology, 132(6), 751-769.
Freud, S. (1905). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. New York, NY: Global
Grey.
Frisby, B. N., Dillow, M. R., Gaughan, S., & Nordlund, J. (2010). Flirtatious
communication: An experimental examination of perceptions of social-
sexual communication motivated by evolutionary forces. Sex Roles, 64, 682-
694.
Fuss, D. (2013). Inside/out: Lesbian theories, gay theories. New York, NY:
Routledge.
47
Gates, G. J. (2014). LGBT Demographics: Comparisons among population-based
surveys. Retrieved from http://escholarship.org/uc/item/0kr784fx
Geher, G., & Kaufman, S. B. (2013). Mating intelligence unleashed: The role of
the mind in sex, dating, and love. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Givens, D. (2006). Love signals: A practical field guide to the body language of
courtship. New York, NY: St. Martin’s Press.
Gobrogge, K. L., Perkins, P. S., Baker, J. H., Balcer, K. D., Breedlove, S. M., &
Klump, K. L. (2007). Homosexual mating preferences from an evolutionary
perspective: Sexual selection theory revisited. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
36(5), 717-723.
Grammer, K., Kruck, K. B., & Magnusson, M. S. (1998). The courtship dance:
Patterns of nonverbal synchronization in opposite-sex encounters. Journal of
Nonverbal Behavior, 22(1), 3-29.
Greengross, G., & Miller, G. (2011). Humor ability reveals intelligence, predicts
mating success, and is higher in males. Intelligence, 39(4), 188-192.
Greer, A. E., & Buss, D. M. (1994). Tactics for promoting sexual encounters.
Journal of Sex Research, 31(3), 185-201.
Hall, J. A., Murphy, N. A., & Mast, M. S. (2006). Recall of nonverbal cues:
Exploring a new definition of interpersonal sensitivity. Journal of Nonverbal
Behavior, 30(4), 141-155.
Hall, J. A., Carter, S., Cody, M. J., & Albright, J. M. (2010). Individual
differences in the communication of romantic interest: Development of the
flirting styles inventory. Communication Quarterly, 58(4), 365-393.
Harrison, J. (2001). It’s none of my business’: Gay and lesbian invisibility in aged
care. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal, 48(3), 142-145.
Henningsen, D. D., Braz, M., & Davies, E. (2008). Why do we flirt? Flirting
motivations and sex differences in working and social contexts. Journal of
Business Communication, 45(4), 483-502.
Henningsen, D. D., Kartch, F., Orr, N., & Brown, A. (2009). The perceptions of
verbal and nonverbal flirting cues in cross-sex interactions. Human
Communication, 12(4), 371-381.
Herek, G. M. (2002a). Gender gaps in public opinion about lesbians and gay men.
Public Opinion Quarterly, 66(1), 40-66.
Herek, G. M., Gillis, J. R., & Cogan, J. C. (2015). Internalized stigma among
sexual minority adults: Insights from a social psychological perspective.
Stigma and Health, 1, 18-34.
Howell, D. C. (2002). Statistical methods for psychology (5th ed). Pacific Grove,
CA: Duxbury/Thomson Learning
Hrdy, S. B. (1986). Empathy, polyandry, and the myth of the coy female. In
Bleier, R. (Ed.), Feminist approaches to science (pp. 119-146). New York,
NY: Pergamon.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behavior in the
human male. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.
49
Kleinke, C. L., Meeker, F. B., & Staneski, R. A. (1986). Preference for opening
lines: Comparing ratings by men and women. Sex Roles, 15(11-12), 585-600.
LaMar, L., & Kite, M. (1998). Sex differences in attitudes toward gay men and
lesbians: A multidimensional perspective. Journal of Sex Research, 35(2),
189-196.
Levine, T. R., King III, G., & Popoola, J. K. (1994). Ethnic and gender differences
in opening lines. Communication Research Reports, 11(2), 143-151.
Lippa, R. A., (2005) Sexual orientation and personality. Annual Review of Sex
Research 16, 119-153.
Maxwell, G. M., Cook, M. W., & Burr, R. (1985). The encoding and decoding of
liking from behavioral cues in both auditory and visual channels. Journal of
Nonverbal Behavior, 9(4), 239-263.
McBain, K. A., Hewitt, L., Maher, T., Sercombe, M., Sypher, S., & Tirendi, G.
(2013). Is this seat taken? The importance of context during the initiation of
romantic communication. International Journal of Humanities and Social
Science, 3(21), 79–89.
Mongeau, P. A., Serewicz, M. C. M., & Therrien, L. F. (2004). Goals for cross‐sex
first dates: identification, measurement, and the influence of contextual
factors. Communication Monographs, 71(2), 121-147.
Ochs, R., & Deihl, M. (1992). Moving beyond binary thinking. In W.J.
Blumenfeld (Ed.), Homophobia: How we all pay the price (pp. 67-75).
Boston, MA: Beacon Press.
Ostler, T. A. (2003). Verbal and nonverbal dating behavior and sexual consent:
Implication for miscommunication between men and women. Dissertation
Abstracts International, 64(07), 3584B.
Perper, T. (1985). Sex signals: The biology of love. Philadelphia, PA: ISI Press.
Renninger, L. A., Wade, T. J., & Grammer, K. (2004). Getting that female glance:
Patterns and consequences of male nonverbal behavior in courtship
contexts. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(6), 416-431.
Riggle, E. D., Olson, A., Whitman, J. S., Rotosky, S. S., & Strong, S. (2008,
February 8). The positive aspects of being a lesbian or gay man. Professional
Psychology: Research and Practice, 39(2), 210-217.
51
Rose, S. M., & Zand, D. (2002). Lesbian dating and courtship from young
adulthood to midlife. Journal of Lesbian Studies, 6(1), 85-109.
Simpson, J. A., Gangestad, S. W., & Biek, M. (1993). Personality and nonverbal
social behavior: An ethological perspective of relationship initiation. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 29(5), 434-461.
Watkins, J. L., & Hall, J. A. (2014). The association between nonverbal sensitivity
and flirting detection accuracy. Communication Research Reports, 31(4),
348-356.
Please rate your current feelings of your own sexual orientation on the following
scale
Exclusively 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 Exclusively
Heterosexual Homosexual
(with no homosexual tendencies) (with no heterosexual tendencies)
APPENDIX B: FLIRTING STYLES INVENTORY - REVISED
57
Traditional
13. Despite how our society is changing, it is still up to a man to take control in
initiating relationships
23. It doesn’t matter who makes the first move, as long as it happens *
20. I wish that we could go back to a time where formal dating was the norm
Physical
Playful
14. The primary reason I flirt is because it makes me feel good about myself
Sincere
11. I really look for an emotional connection with someone I’m interested in
Polite
10. People should be cautious when letting someone know they are interested
15. It is important not to say something overly sexual when showing interest
21. There are rules about how men and women should conduct themselves
If you decide to participate, you will be asked to fill out a survey that assesses
demographic information, personality traits, and flirting style. The survey should take no
longer than 20 minutes to complete. There are no major risks involved for the participant
in completing this survey. Any information that is obtained in connection with this study
and that can be identified with you will remain confidential and will be disclosed only
with your permission or as required by law.
Your decision whether or not to participate will not prejudice your future relations with
California State University, Fresno. If you decide to participate, you are free to withdraw
your consent and to discontinue participation at any time without penalty.
If you have any questions, please ask. If you have any additional questions later,
Meaghan McCready ([email protected]) will be happy to answer them.
Questions regarding the rights of research subjects may be directed to Constance Jones,
Chair, CSUF Committee on the Protection of Human Subjects, (559) 278-4468.
___ I consent
APPENDIX D: DEMOGRAPHIC QUESTIONNAIRE
62
Man
Woman
Other _____
2. Age: ______
3. Ethnicity:
White
Hispanic/Latino
Black/African American
Asian/Pacific Islander
Other
4. Marital Status:
In a committed relationship
Widowed
Divorced
Separated
APPENDIX E: FLIRTING ACTS
64
1. Simple Touch
2. Eye Contact
3. Provocative Displays
4. Attention Seeking
5. Signaling
6. Playful Behavior
He laughed and touched his hair while talking
He batted his eyes
He giggled
He tilted his head, smiled, and looked at him out of the corner of his eye
He blushed
7. Acting Coy
He made eye contact with him, then looked away
He looked at him, smiled, and looked away.
Fresno State
Non-Exclusive Distribution License
(to archive your thesis/dissertation electronically via the library’s eCollections database)
By submitting this license, you (the author or copyright holder) grant to Fresno State Digital
Scholar the non-exclusive right to reproduce, translate (as defined in the next paragraph), and/or
distribute your submission (including the abstract) worldwide in print and electronic format and
in any medium, including but not limited to audio or video.
You agree that Fresno State may, without changing the content, translate the submission to any
medium or format for the purpose of preservation.
You also agree that the submission is your original work, and that you have the right to grant the
rights contained in this license. You also represent that your submission does not, to the best of
your knowledge, infringe upon anyone’s copyright.
If the submission reproduces material for which you do not hold copyright and that would not be
considered fair use outside the copyright law, you represent that you have obtained the
unrestricted permission of the copyright owner to grant Fresno State the rights required by this
license, and that such third-party material is clearly identified and acknowledged within the text
or content of the submission.
If the submission is based upon work that has been sponsored or supported by an agency or
organization other than Fresno State, you represent that you have fulfilled any right of review or
other obligations required by such contract or agreement.
Fresno State will clearly identify your name as the author or owner of the submission and will not
make any alteration, other than as allowed by this license, to your submission. By typing your
name and date in the fields below, you indicate your agreement to the terms of this
distribution license.
Meaghan McCready
May 5, 2016
Date