Lesson 3.1 Handouts
Lesson 3.1 Handouts
Introduction
When talking about Sensation, we then think about our Sense organs- our eyes, ears, tongue, nose
and skin- allow us to appreciate our external world and make our living experience more wonderful. Our
sensory organ’s primary function is to act as biological transducers.
Transducers are devices that convert one kind of energy into another, for example a microphone. A
microphone is a tool that changes sound into electricity. When you talk or make noise near the microphone,
it picks up the sound. Inside the microphone, there’s a small part that moves with the sound waves. This
movement turns the sound into an electric signal. The electric signal can then be sent to a speaker or saved in
a recording. Essentially, a microphone helps turn what you say into something that can be heard through
speakers or saved as a recording.
In a similar context, the brain only works with one type of energy, which is electrical energy in the
form of action potential. However, information that's out in the world comes to use as many different forms
of energy and each one needs to be converted into electrical energy so that the brain can understand it.
Considering our sense organs as biological transducers, it can be thought of as "translators" converting the
different forms of energy in the world into electrical energy so that the brain can understand the information
from the outside world.
Sensation is the process of converting energy from the environment into a pattern of response by the
nervous system. For example, our eyes convert light energy, the ears translate mechanical energy from
sounds, and the nose and tongue translate chemical energy from odors and foods. Information arriving at the
brain from the sense organs creates sensory impressions. When our sensory organs are damaged and unable
to transduce energy, the brain cannot interpret the information from that sense, leading to difficulties with
seeing, hearing, smell, and so on. But with modern technology, scientist are able to design artificial devices
to help send electrical signal directly to the brain without passings to the damaged sense organs.
Let us have a short recall of action potential and how we measure sensory impressions before we
continue on Sensation. One of the main experts who tries to understand sensory impressions are called
Psychophysicists. Psychophysics studies how our minds interpret physical properties of stimuli, like sound
waves, light, or chemicals in food. It explores how the brain translates these physical properties into
experiences, such as loudness, brightness, or sweetness. Moreover, psychophysicists have shown that a
certain minimum amount of physical energy, known as the absolute threshold, is needed for a sensory
impression to occur 50% of the time. Additionally, the difference threshold measures the minimum
difference in energy needed between two stimuli for the difference to be noticed half the time.
You might wonder how our brain is capable of selecting the sensations that are constantly stimulated
in the different organs. Sensory transduction usually involves some selection. Psychophysical research has
found that vision also narrows what we can possibly observe. Like the other senses, vision acts as a data
selection system, meaning, it selects information in order to code and send to the brain only the most
important sensory information for further processing.
SENSORY SELECTION: THE FOUR WAYS TO REDUCE SENSORY OVERLOAD
1. Lack of Specific Transducers
Considerable selection occurs because human sensory receptors do not transduce all energies
that they encounter. For example, the eye transduces light waves, the ear transduces sound waves,
and so on. But many other types of stimuli cannot be sensed directly because we lack sensory
receptors to transduce their energy. For example, humans cannot sense the bioelectric fields of living
creatures, but sharks have special organs that can.
3. Sensory Adaptation
Sensory Adaptation is a process were sensory receptors response less overtime to
unchanging stimuli. Example is our sense of smell which can adapt quickly. When our olfactory
receptors are exposed to a constant odor, they send fewer and fewer nerve impulses to the brain until
the odor is no longer noticed. The reason for this concept is that there is usually little reason to keep
reminding the brain that a sensory input is unchanged, sensory receptors generally respond best to
changes in stimulation.
4. Feature Detection
As the senses collect information, feature detectors in the brain also reduce the flow of
sensory input by dividing the world into important perceptual features, which is the basic attributes
or stimulus patters, such as lines, shape, edges, or colors. A feature detector is a cell, or collection of
cells in the cerebral cortex that responds to a specific attribute of an object. Consequently, the brain
need only further process the perceptual feature rather than the underlying sensory pattern.
Visual pop-out is one example of feature detectors that our visual system have. Visual
pop-out occurs because our visual system is highly sensitive to these perceptual features. Although
our sensitivity to perceptual features is an innate characteristic of the nervous system, it also is
influenced by the experiences early in life. In summary, our sensory organs have remarkable capacity
to take in the rich and varied information that exists in the world around us.
VISION
When we open our eyes, we become aware of the visual richness of the world around us. However,
our eyes transduce only the tiniest fraction of the entire range of electromagnetic energies- which is the
visible spectrum. We cannot "see" the other electromagnetic spectrum such as microwaves, cosmic rays,
X-rays, or radio waves.
So how does sensory transduction in vision occur? Understanding how vision works is one of the
complex sense organs to understand, but it is wonderfully made. To better understand how it works, we must
first understand the characteristic of how the light is being processed by the eye, and then the process of how
it is being transduced or translated from a light energy into electrical energy. Lastly, we will also consider the
theories of color vision.
Characteristic of Light
The Visible spectrum- the sliver of electromagnetic energies to which the eyes respond- is made up of a
narrow range of wavelengths of electromagnetic radiation. Visible lights starts at "short" wavelengths of 400
nanometers, which we sense as purple or violet. Successively longer light waves produce blue, green,
yellow, orange, and red, which has a wavelength of 700 nanometers.
There are three (3) characteristics of light that is important for us to remember:
1. Hue (Colors). This refers to the various colors of light: red, orange, yellow, green, blue, indigo,
and violet. Note: Various hues, or color sensations, correspond to the wavelength of the light that
reaches our eyes. Hue are the colors of light a determined by its corresponding wavelength.
Additionally, white light, in contrast is a mixture of many wavelengths.
2. Saturation. Hues from a narrow band of wavelengths are very saturated, or pure. Example, an
intense, fire-engine red is more saturated than a muddy, brick red that might include some degree
of orange or brown.
3. Brightness. This corresponds roughly to the amplitude (or height) of light waves. Waves of
greater amplitude are taller, carry more energy, and cause the colors that we see to appear
brighter or more intense. For example, the same brick would look brighter in intense,
high-energy illumination and duller in dim light.
Although the visual system is much more complex than any digital camera, both cameras and eyes
have a lens to focus light rays on a light-sensitive surface at the back of an enclosed space, where the image
is created. The structure of the eye serves two purposes, first, it begins by focusing the light waves coming in
from the world, and second, it carries out the important work of transducing them so that the brain can make
sense of the incoming information and create an image. Let us understand further the purpose of the eye one
by one.
In cameras, focusing is done relatively simply- by changing the distance between the lens and
the image sensor. In the eye, most focusing is done by the cornea, a curved, transparent, protective layer
at the front of the eye that bends light inward. The lens, which is the clear structure behind the pupil that
bends light toward the retina, makes additional, smaller adjustments.
Our eye's focal point changes when the ciliary muscles attached to the lens alter its shape. This
process is called accommodation, and it's what allows you to focus on objects regardless of whether
they are several feet away or right in front of your nose.
There are two factors that can compromise our ability to focus, first is the shape of our eye,
and second is the flexibility of the lens.
If the eye is too short, nearby objects will be blurred, but distant objects will be sharp. This
is also called as Hyperopia (farsightedness), which is having difficulty focusing on nearby objects.
If the eyeball is too long, images fall short of the retina, this is called Myopia (nearsightedness) and
the person won't be able to focus on distant objects.
When the cornea or lens is misshapen, part of the person’s vision will be focused, and part
will be fuzzy. In this case, the eye has more than one focal point, and this is called Astigmatism,
whish is a defect in the cornea, lens, or eye that cause some areas of vision to be out of focus. Just
remember that all three visual defects can be corrected by placing glasses (or contact lenses) in front
of the eye to change the path of light.
As people age, the lens becomes less flexible and accommodating becomes more difficult
for the eye, this is called Presbyopia which comes from the Latin for “Old vision “ or farsightedness
caused by aging. As people age, people need bifocal lenses, which correct near vision and distance
vision.
Back to the concept similar to a camera, it is the job of the cornea and lens to focus light rays
on a light-sensitive surface at the back of an enclosed space. In a camera, the light-sensitive surface
is a layer of pixels in the digital image sensor. In the eye, it is a layer of light-sensitive cells called
photoreceptors that are located in the retina. The retina is the surface at the back f the eye onto
which the lens focuses light rays. It has an area about the size and thickness of a postage stamp.
The eye has two types of photoreceptors, namely the rods and cones, which are responsible
for transduction. Again, transduction is the process by which energy out there in the world- in this
case, light energy- is converted into electrical energy (action potentials) that can be understood by
the brain.
⮚ Cones
There are 5 million cones in each eye, which work best in bright light. They also produce
color sensations and fine details of what we can see. Cones, by definition, are the photoreceptors that
are sensitive to color, and it mainly lies at the center of the eye. In fact, the fovea, a tiny spot in the
center of the retina, contains only cones- about 50,000 of them. Like high-resolution digital sensors
made of many small pixels, the tightly packed cones in the fovea produce the highest level of acuity
(that is the sharpest images).
What is it like to be color blind? What causes color blindness? A person with color
blindness cannot perceive colors. It is as if the world were a black-and-white movie. The color-blind
person either lacks cones or has cones that do not function normally. Such total color blindness is
rare. In color weakness, or partial color blindness, a person can't see certain colors.
How can color-blind individuals drive? Don't they have trouble with traffic lights?
Red-Green color blind individuals have normal vision for yellow and blue, so the main problem is
telling red lights from green. In practice, that’s' not difficult. The red light is always on top, and the
green light is brighter than the red. Also, red traffic signals have yellow light mixed in with the red,
and a green light is really blue-green.
⮚ Rods
Areas outside the fovea also get light, creating a large region of peripheral (side) vision.
Peripheral vision are visions we see at the edge of the visual field. This is the area where the rods
take over from the cones. The rods are most numerous about 20 degrees from the center of the retina,
so much of our peripheral vision is rod vision.
Inside the rods and cones is where transduction occurs in the eye, but they also have a role to
play in visual acuity, or the sharpness of visual perception. Once those photoreceptors have
converted light energy into electrical energy, those action potentials travel along a series of
interneurons to the optic nerve, and then on to the brain.
The rods are also quite sensitive to movement in peripheral vision. People who suffer from
tunnel vision (a loss of peripheral vision) feel as if they are wearing blinders. There are about 120
million rods in the eye, however, rods can’t detect colors. Pure rod vision is black and white, with
this, rods are much more sensitive to light than cones. This allows the rods to help us see in very dim
light.
Dark adaptation is the dramatic increase in the eye's sensitivity to light that occurs after a
person enters the dark or under low-light conditions. Consider yourself walking into a movie theater.
If you enter from a brightly lit lobby, you practically need to be led to your seat. Almost
immediately, the pupil (the black opening inside the iris that allows light to enter the eye) the
opening surrounded by the colored iris ( the colored structure on the surface of the eye surrounding
the pupil) , begins to open to allow lighter to enter the eye. After a short time, you can see the entire
room in detail. The retina, however, also becomes more sensitive, taking about 30 to 35 minutes of
complete darkness to reach maximum visual sensitivity. At that point, your eye will be 100,000 times
more sensitive to light.
What causes dark adaptation? Like cones, which contain a pigment called iodopsin, rods
contain a light-sensitive visual pigment, rhodopsin, which allows them to see in black and white.
Moreover, he rods are insensitive to extremely red light. That's why submarines, airplane cockpits,
and ready rooms for fighter pilots are illuminated with red light. In each case, people can move
quickly from a light place into a dark one without having to adapt. Because the red light doesn't
stimulate the rods, it is as if they had already spent time in the dark.
Each retina has a blind spot because there are no photoreceptors at the location where the optic nerve
exits the eye to convey visual information to the brain, and blood vessels enter. Blind spot is the area in the
retina where the optic nerves exits that contains no photoreceptor cells. Moreover, Optic nerve is a structure
in the eye that conveys visual information away from the retina to the brain. The blind spot shows that vision
depends greatly on the brain. If you close an eye, some of the incoming light will fall on the blind spot of
your open eye.
If there is a hole in the Retina, then why isn't there a gap in our vision? The answer is that the
visual cortex of the brain actively fills in the gap with patterns from surrounding areas. The brain can also
"erase" distracting information. If you roll your eyes all the way to the right and then close your right eye.
You should clearly see your nose in your left eye's field of vision. Now open your right eye again, and your
nose nearly disappears because your brain disregards its presence.
Theories of Color Vision
The trichromatic theory holds that there are three types of cones, each most sensitive to either
red, green, or blue. Other colors result from combinations of these three. However, there are 2 basic
problems with the trichromatic theory that have been identified, first is that four colors of light- red,
green, blue, and yellow- seem to be primary. Second, this theory doesn’t account for the fact that it's
impossible to have a reddish green color, or a yellowish blue. These problems led to the development of
a second view of color vision.
Opponent-process theory of color vision, states that vision analyzes colors into "either-or"
messages. That is the virtual system can produce messages for either red or green, yellow or blue, or
black or white. Coding one color in a pair (red, for instance) seems to block the opposite message (green)
from coming through. As a result, a reddish green is impossible, but a yellowish red (orange) can occur.
According to opponent-process theory, fatigue caused by repeatedly having the cones respond to
one color produces an afterimage of the opposite color as the system recovers. Afterimages are visual
sensations that persist after a stimulus is removed-like seeing a spot after a flashbulb goes off.
Which color theory is correct? Both theories are correct! The three-color theory applies to the retina, in
which three different types of cones have been found. Each contains a different type of iodopsin, a
light-sensitive pigment that breaks down when struck by light. This triggers action potentials and send neural
messages to the brain. Each type of iodopsin is most sensitive to light in roughly the red, green, or blue
region. Other colors result from combination of these three. Thus, the three types of cones fire nerve
impulses at different rates to produce various color sensation. In contrast, the opponent-process theory better
explains what happens beyond the retina- in the optic pathways and the brain-after information leaves the
cones.
HEARING
Characteristics of Sound: What the Ear Hears
The sound travels as a series of invisible waves of compression (peaks) and rarefaction (valleys) in
the air. Any vibrating object - example a tuning fork, the string of a musical instrument, or the vocal
cords-will produce sound waves (rhythmic movement of air molecules).
The sound wave has two most important characteristics : its frequency and its amplitude. The
frequency of sound waves (the number of waves per second) corresponds to the perceived pitch (higher or
lower tone) of a sound. The amplitude (or physical "height" of a sound wave) tells how much energy it
contains. Psychologically, amplitude corresponds to sensed loudness (the volume of a sound, related to the
amplitude of a sound wave) or sound intensity.
Transduction, again, is the process by which a sensory organ- the ear in this case- converts some
form of energy into electrical energy (in the form of action potentials) so that the brain can interpret it. The
ear is responsible for transducing mechanical energy in the form of sound waves. Hearing involves a chain of
events that begins with the pinna, the visible, external part of the ear. The pinna also acts like a funnel to
focus sounds. After they are guided into the ear canal, sound waves collide with the eardrum (tympanic
membrane), setting it vibrating in response, thus transmitting them inward. By definition, the Eardrum, is a
membrane that vibrates in response to sound waves and transmits them inward. This in turn, causes three
small middle ear ossicles (or bones): the malleus (or hammer), incus (or anvil), and stapes (stirrup) to
vibrate. The ossicles link the eardrum with the cochlea (a snail-shaped organ in the inner ear that contains
sensory receptors for hearing.).
The stapes is attached to a membrane on the cochlea called the oval window. As the oval window
moves back and forth, it makes waves in a fluid inside the cochlea. Inside the cochlea, the fluid waves trigger
vibrations in the basilar membrane (the structure in the cochlea containing hair cells that convert sound
waves into action potentials.), which is the "floor" of the organ of Corti. In turn, tiny hair cells (are
receptor cells within the cochlea that transduce vibrations into nerve impulses) embedded in the basilar
membrane are pushed up against the tectorial membrane. As a consequence a set of stereocilia (or bristles)
atop each hair cell brush against the tectorial membrane whenever waves ripple through the fluid
surrounding the organ of Corti. As the stereo-cilia are bent, action potentials are triggered, which then flow
to the brain.
Hearing Loss
1. Conductive hearing loss occurs when the transfer of vibrations from the outer ear to the inner ear
weakens or poor transfer of sounds from the eardrum to the inner ear. Example, the eardrums or
ossicles may be damaged or immobilized by disease or injury. In many cases, conductive hearing
loss can be overcome with a hearing aid, which amplifies sounds, making them louder and clearer.
2. Sensorineural hearing loss is quite different, and results from damage to the inner ear hair cells or
the auditory nerve. Many jobs, hobbies, and pastimes can cause noise-induced hearing loss
(damage caused by exposing the hair cells to excessively loud sounds) which is a common subtype
of sensorineural hearing loss that occurs when very loud sounds damage fragile hair cells.
Note: If you work in a noisy environment or enjoy loud music, motorcycling, snowmobiling,
hunting, or similar pursuits, you may be risking noise-induced hearing loss. Be forewarned: Dead
hair cells are never replaced. When you abuse them, you lose them. By the time you are 65, more
than 40 percent of them will be gone, mainly those that transduce high pitches. How loud must a
sound be to be hazardous?Daily exposure to 85 decibels or more may cause permanent hearing loss.
Decibels are a measure of sound intensity. Every 20 decibels increases the sound pressure by
a factor of 10. In other words, a rock concert at 120 decibels is 1000 times stronger than a voice at 60
decibels. Short periods at 120 decibels can cause temporary hearing loss, and even one brief
exposure to 150 decibels (a jet airplane nearby) may cause permanent hearing loss.
Hearing aids are no help in cases of sensorineural hearing loss because auditory messages
are being blocked from reaching the brain. In many cases, however, the hair cells are damaged but
the auditory nerve is intact. This finding has spurred the development of cochlear implants that
bypass hair cells and stimulate the auditory nerves directly. Wires from a microphone carry electrical
signals to an external coil. A matching coil under the skin picks up the signals and carries them to
one or more areas of the cochlea.
Theories of Hearing Pitch
Remembering the two important characteristics of a sound are its loudness (which comes from the
amplitude of the sound wave) and its pitch (which is determined by the frequency). Two theories have been
put forward to explain how we distinguish different pitches.
The frequency theory of hearing (proposition that pitch is decoded from the rate at which hair cells
of the basilar membrane are firing) states that as pitch rises, nerve impulses of a corresponding frequency are
fed into the auditory nerve- that is, a 1,200-hertz tone produces 1,200 nerve impulses (action potentials) per
second. The term hertz refers to the number of vibrations per second. This explains how sounds up to about
4,000 hertz reach the brain, but the theory cannot account for how we hear pitches (that is, tones) that are
higher than that.
To explain how the brain can make out tones higher than 4,000 hertz, we turn to the place theory of
hearing, which states that higher and lower tones excite specific places in the cochlea. The place theory of
hearing (proposes that higher and lower tones excite specific areas of the cochlea). High tones register most
strongly at the base of the cochlea (near the oval window). Lower tones on the other hand, mostly move hair
cells near the narrow outer tip of the cochlea. Pitch is then signaled by the area of the cochlea that is most
strongly activated. Place theory is most useful in explaining why hunters sometimes lose their hearing in a
narrow pitch range. "Hunter's notch" as this condition is called, occurs when hair cells are damaged in the
specific area of the cochlea affected by the pitch of gunfire.
Our sense of smell is called Olfaction. Smell receptors respond to chemical molecules that are
airborne. Anything that you can smell- from flowers to fudge to formaldehyde- produces chemical molecules
that are picked up by your nose.
Again, transduction refers to the process by which one form of energy that's picked up by the senses
(in this case, chemical energy) is converted into electrical energy (in the form of action potentials) that can be
interpreted in the brain.
As air enters the nose, it flows over roughly 5 million nerve fibers embedded in the lining of the
upper nasal pages. Receptor proteins on the surface of the fibers are sensitive to various airborne chemical
molecules. When a fiber is stimulated, it creates an action potential that then travels to the brain.
The specific way that different odors are detected is still an unfolding mystery. One hint about the
process comes from a type of anosmia, a sort of "smell blindness" to a single odor. Risks for anosmia include
infections, allergies and blows to the head (which may tear the olfactory nerves). Moreover, repeated
exposure to chemicals such as ammonia, paints, solvents, and hairdressing "potions” also can cause anosmia.
Discovering that there exists a loss of sensitivity to specific types of odors in anosmia suggests the presence
of receptors in the nose for specific odors.
The lock-and-key theory of olfaction suggests that these receptors may bind with airborne chemical
molecules that have a matching "Shape" to create odors. By definition, it is a theory holding that odors are
related to the shapes of chemical molecules. Like a piece fit into a puzzle, airborne chemical molecules
produce odors when part of the molecule matches a hole on the receptor of the same shape (hence the name,
"lock" and key" theory).
Furthermore, chemical molecules trigger activity in different combinations of odor receptors. Thus,
humans can detect at least 10,000 different odors. Just as you can make hundreds of thousands of words in
English from the 26 letters of the Roman alphabet, there are many combinations of the 400 types of receptors
that can be activated, resulting in many different odors. Additionally, researchers have noted that scents are
also identified, in part, by the location of the receptors in the nose that a particular odor activates. And
finally, the number of activated receptors tells the brain the strength of an odor. The brain uses these
distinctive patterns of messages it gets from the olfactory receptors to recognize particular scents.
GUSTATION (TASTE)
Taste is also a chemical sense, in which chemical molecules are found in our food. Our sense of taste
is also called gustation. There are five basic taste sensations: sweet, salty sour, bitter, and umami. We are
generally most sensitive to bitter and sour going back to many generations, this may have helped prevent
poisonings when most humans foraged for food because bitter and sour foods are more likely to be inedible.
Umami which is a Japanese word describes a pleasant savory or "brothy" taste associated with certain amino
acids in chicken soup, some meat extracts, kelp, tuna, human milk, cheese, and soybeans. The receptors for
umami are sensitive to glutamate, a substance found in monosodium glutamate (MSG).
Taste buds, which are the clusters of taste-receptor cells, are located mainly on the top side of the
tongue, especially around the edges. As food is chewed, it dissolves and the chemical molecules enter the
taste buds, where they set off action potentials that travel to the brain.
Like smell, sweet, bitter, and umami tastes appear to be based on a lock-and-key match between the
molecules and intricately shaped receptors. Saltiness and sourness, however, are triggered by a direct flow of
charged atoms into the tips of taste cells.
If there are only five tastes, how can there be so many different flavors? Flavors seem more varied
because we include sensations of texture, temperature, pain (think "hot" chili peppers), and smell when we
taste things. Smell, in particular, is important in determining flavor. If you plug your nose and eat small bits
of apple, potato, and onion, they will "taste" almost exactly alike. So do gourmet jellybeans! That's also why
food loses its "taste" when you have a cold. It is probably fair to say that subjective flavor is at least half
based on smell.
Does the number of receptors in an area of skin relate to its sensitivity? Yes. Your skin can be
"mapped" by applying heat, cold, touch, pressure, or pain to points all over your body. Such testing would
show that the number of skin receptors varies, and that sensitivity generally matches the number of receptors
in a given area. Important areas such as the lips, tongue, face, hands, and genitals have a higher density of
receptors. Of course, what you ultimately feel will depend on brain activity. Yes. About 230 pain points per
square centimeter (about a half-inch) are found behind the knee, 180 per centimeter on the buttocks, 60 on
the pad of the thumb, and 40 on the tip of the nose.
Regarding pain, we have a body’s warning system, which is pain based on large nerve fibers, this
warns that bodily damage may be occurring. Pain carried by large nerve fibers is sharp, bright, and fast and
seems to come from specific body areas. Give yourself a small nab with a pin and you will feel this type of
pain. As you do this, notice that warning pain quickly disappears. Much as we dislike warning pain, it is
usually a signal that the body has been, or is about to be, damaged. Without warning pain, we would be
unable to detect or prevent injury.
A second type of somatic (bodily) pain is carried by small nerve fibers. This type of pain is slower,
nagging, aching, widespread, and very unpleasant. It gets worse if the pain stimulus is repeated. This is the
body's reminding system, which reminds the brain that the body has been injured. For instance, lower-back
pain often has this quality. Sadly, the reminding system can cause agony long after an injury has healed, or in
terminal illnesses, when the reminder is useless.
How is the gate closed? It may depend on what types of nerve fibers are carrying information about
the pain. Messages carried by large, fast nerve fibers seem to close the spinal pain gate directly. Doing so can
prevent slower, "reminding system" pain from reaching the brain. But messages from small, slow fibers
seem to take a different route. After going through the pain gate, they continue to a "central biasing system"
in the brain which sends a message back down the spinal cord, closing the pain gates.
Melzack believes that gate control theory may explain the painkilling effects of acupuncture, which is
the Chinese medical art of relieving pain and illness by inserting thin needles into the body. As the
acupuncturist’s needles are twirled, heated, or electrified, they activate small pain fibers. These relay through
the biasing system to close the gates to intense or chronic pain.
Three fluid-filled tubes- called the semicircular canals- are the sensory organs for balance. When
head movements occur, the fluid inside the tubes will also swirl in accordance with the head movement. As
the fluid moves, it bends a small "flap", or "float," called the crista, that detects movement in the
semicircular canals. The bending of each crista again stimulates hair cells and signals head rotation.