Murphy Joseph Demetriou Karimi-Mofrad (2017) - UPSR - AI - and - Authenticity
Murphy Joseph Demetriou Karimi-Mofrad (2017) - UPSR - AI - and - Authenticity
psychological well-being
David Murphy1, Stephen Joseph1, Evangelia Demetriou and Pegah Karimi Mofrad
1
School of Education, University of Nottingham, England: UK
Corresponding author for this paper is; David Murphy, School of Education, University of
Email: [email protected]
[Murphy, D. Joseph, S. Demetriou, E., & Karimi Mofrad, P. (2017). Unconditional positive
self-regard, intrinsic aspirations and authenticity: pathways to psychological wellbeing.
Journal of Humanistic Psychology, ]
Abstract
important determinant of well-being. However, until recently it has received little empirical
attention. The current study aims to examine the association between unconditional positive
self-regard and several key constructs consistent with the ideas of well-being within
scale. The statistically significant best fit for the data was a related two-factor model. Study 2
used the two-factors of the UPSR scale to explore the association with intrinsic aspirations.
The study showed positive self-regard was statistically significantly positively correlated with
the intrinsic aspirations total scale and with each of the separate scores for IA-importance and
with IA-importance but was not statistically significantly correlated to either the IA-total or
IA-chance scores. Study 3 considers the association between UPSR, intrinsic aspirations and
with the authenticity scale score. Only IA-chance scores showed a statistically significant and
aspirations and authenticity were not statistically significant. Results call for further empirical
psychological wellbeing
Introduction
In recent years there has been much interest in well-being and its determinants (e.g.,
psychological theorising about the nature of well-being dates back to the humanistic
psychologists, such as Carl Rogers. Rogers (1961) defined well-being as the fully functioning
person as someone who was open to experience, able to live in the moment, trusting of their
own judgements, free in making choices, and not governed by the values of others.
Studies in the psychology of well-being are concerned with describing, defining and
delineating the path to psychological well-being. The current study aims to examine several
key constructs that are consistent with the ideas of well-being from within contemporary
Rogers (1959) hypothesized that as infants develop, the inherent determination for
survival leads to a learned need for positive regard. However, without exception caregivers’
positive regard is only available inconsistently and is thus variable; at times it is consistent
with the infant’s valuing and at other times it is inconsistent with the infant’s valuing. Hence,
the infant might find that a behavior perceived as satisfying (such as hitting a sibling) leads to
disapproval from a caregiver. The infant comes to learn that disapproval is not pleasurable
and that even though behaviors that invoke some temporal sense of satisfaction, the urge to
do it again will be either denied or distorted so as not to attract further withdrawal of positive
regard from a care giver. The infant introjects the values from the caregiver and eventually
comes to experience these values as their own. As a result the infant comes to learn that some
self-experiences are not acceptable to significant social others. All other experiences of this
kind, or related self-experiences, subsequently become denied or distorted. When the infant
behaves in accord with introjected values, as if they were their own, they are considered to be
acting in a way that is consistent with a condition of worth (Standal, 1954). The infant is now
from personal valuations and feelings are increased and a state of incongruence between
organismic experience and self-concept arises (Rogers, 1959). Incongruence between self-
concept and experience is considered to be the single source of maladjustment that accounts
for the degree of subjective sense of well-being, or distress that a person experiences.
As the person develops a need for positive regard and internalises values from
significant social others, they also, in denying or distorting their experience to fit the self-
concept, become their own internal social other (Standal, 1954). The development of
conditions of worth enables the satisfaction of the need for positive regard. This process can
be termed positive self-regard and is used to describe the satisfaction of self-experiences that
empirical studies in the field of contingent self-worth (Crocker, Luhtanen, Cooper, &
Bouvrette, 2003; Croker & Wolfe, 2001). Emergent in the literature on contingent self-worth
is the notion of implicit self-worth (Bos, Huijding, Muris, Vogel, Beishcevel, 2010). The
implicit self-worth is not related to psychological disturbance whereas global and contingent
psychopathology.
positive self-regard they do not discriminate between any self-experiences as being more
worthy or valid than any other self-experience. The extent to which a person does not
discriminate between any self-experiences as more or less worthy refers directly to the extent
accepting a person is whatever their desires, intentions, motives and behaviors, the less
to humanistic theories and therapies, little research has yet investigated its relationship to
well-being. Two recent studies however have shown evidence that unconditional positive
regard is related to higher levels of well-being. In the first, it was found that over time
actual increase in wellbeing (Flannagan, Paterson, Hume, & Joseph, 2015). In the second, the
association between unconditional positive self-regard and posttraumatic growth was found
Building on these two studies, further work is needed to understand the relation of
psychological well-being. As already mentioned, Rogers used the term fully functioning to
determining and not reliant on the views or values of others to guide behavior (Ryan & Deci,
2000).
positive self-regard. In the first study we examine the construct of unconditional positive
self-regard and the association between unconditionality and positive regard. Having
established the two separate dimensions of unconditionality and positive regard in the second
and third studies we examine their associations with authenticity and intrinsic values.
Study 1
Introduction
unconditional positive self-regard, Patterson and Joseph (2006) developed a 12-item scale.
After carrying out principal components analysis on data collected, Patterson and Joseph
showed identified two factors. One factor indicated the level of positive self-regard and the
second factor indicated unconditionality of regard. The study carried out by Patterson and
Joseph (2006) was exploratory and as yet no confirmatory factor analytic study has been
carried out to test the factor structure of the 12-item unconditional positive self- regard scale.
The aim of this study was to conduct confirmatory factor analysis of the unconditional
Method
Design
In this study we carried out a confirmatory factor analysis using LISREL v 9.10
(student edition) to test the factor structure of the unconditional positive self-regard scale.
Where exploratory factor analysis was used by Patterson and Joseph (2006) to determine the
likely factors structure of items relating the concept of unconditional positive self-regard their
study was not able to test the probability of whether the data fit the two-factor structure they
proposed. Accordingly, a confirmatory factor analysis is required to test the probability that
the two-factor structure can be confirmed by the data. In confirmatory factor analysis the data
must fit and not significantly differ from the proposed or hypothesized model. Based on the
theory of unconditional positive self-regard a related two-factor model, where the two related
factors are self-regard and unconditionality, was proposed. In order to test the model
provided the best fit for the data of two further models were also proposed and tested. These
were a one-factor model with UPSR as a single factor; and the unrelated two-factor model
Participants
Data were collected from 239 participants with a mean age of 30.9 years. Participants
included 109 men (45.6%) and 130 women (54.4%). Of these 137 were Caucasian (53.7%),
56 (27.2%) were Asian, 19 (7.9%) were mixed ethnic origin and 17 (7.1%) identified as
Black African/African Caribbean. The participants completed the UPSR Scale as part of
postgraduate student research dissertation studies and were collated here into one sample for
Results
The statistical fit for the two-factor related, two-factor unrelated and one-factor
models were tested using the maximum likelihood estimation method of confirmatory factor
analysis on LISREL v9.10 (student version). The results for two goodness-of-fit indices are
Model X2 df p RMSEA
Both of the measures suggest that the one-factor model was not a satisfactory fit, as
chi-square is significant and the root mean square error of approximation was larger than
0.100. Chi-square for the unrelated two-factor model and related two-factor model suggested
that neither model was a satisfactory fit. However, chi-square is not necessarily the best
significant the greater the number of participants entered into the model (Cramer, 2003). The
root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) is not so susceptible to this bias. A value
of RMSEA below 0.100 suggests a good fit for the data (Loehlin, 2004). Both of the two-
factor models provided a good fit for the data. However, the chi-square difference test shows
the related two-factor model was a statistically significant better fit than the unrelated two-
factor model (X2 = 45.21, df 1 p < 0.001). This suggests that the related two-factor model
was a statistically significant good fit for the data. Figure 1 shows the path diagram for the
0.82
Self-Reg1
0.72 Self-Reg2
0.42
0.53
0.44 Self-Reg3
0.75
Self-Reg4
0.49 0.71 1.00
Self-Reg
0.56
0.68 Self-Reg5
0.72
0.48 Self-Reg6
0.53
0.54 Uncond 1
0.68
1.00 Uncond 2
-0.03
0.19 Uncond
Uncond 3 1.00
0.97
0.52
0.78
0.80 Uncond 5
0.39 Uncond 6
Introduction
Now that the measure of unconditional positive self-regard has been confirmed
further studies of the association between the two dimensions of unconditionality and positive
none of their self-experience as being more worthy of positive regard than any other. The
person is accepting of their self-experiences then there is little need for the use of defence
systems that become active when experience presents a threat to the satisfaction of the need
for positive regard. In Rogers’s (1959) theory of personality he suggested that two forms of
psychological defence are used. These were distortion and denial. Distortion is used as a
defence to transform experience that is not permissible into awareness because it does not fit
with the existing self-concept. Experience is transformed into something that can be
symbolized in a way that fits with the existing self-concept. Denial is used for the same
reason except the experience is not symbolized in awareness. The experience is totally denied
access to awareness.
Within Rogers’s (1959) theory, defensive behavior results from conditions of worth.
Conditions of worth develop through the introjection of values of significant others. When
people have high conditions of worth they tend to act in line with the values and goals of
others, to feel acceptable to them, and neglect their intrinsic valuing system. However, people
will follow their intrinsic valuing system when they are able to accurately symbolise
them rather than for attaining a reward or to avoid a negative consequence (Ryan & Deci,
2000); thus they develop and expand their capabilities (Sansone & Harackiewicz, 2000).
According to Deci and Ryan (1985), intrinsic motivation energizes significant growth-
oriented behaviours, such as seeking out new challenges, pursuing one’s interests and
fundamental contributor to self-determination (Deci & Ryan, 1985). People that experience
higher well-being act more in accord with their intrinsic aspirations and are likely to engage
in fulfilling life goals that feel most personally rewarding or satisfying and less likely to seek
fulfilment of goals that others consider more valuable (Kasser & Ryan, 1993). These goals
are referred to in the literature as intrinsic aspirations and have been shown repeatedly to be
of the organismic valuing. That is, the level of behaviour that is being guided by intrinsic
aspirations can be thought to represent the extent a person acts in accord with their own
valuing compared to that of other external demands. Research demonstrates that people who
value more extrinsic goals experience less well-being than those who value more intrinsic
goals (Ryan et al., 1999). Evidence in support of the intrinsic aspiration construct can be
found when looking at behaviour within academic achievement (Gottfried et al., 2009),
organizational psychology (Tremblay et al., 2009), eating behaviours (Jutta et al., 2009),
reading literacy (Becker et al., 2010) and schizophrenia (Barch et al., 2008). Each shows that
people that act in line with their intrinsic valuing will exhibit less defensive behavior because
they are more self-accepting and have fewer or less intense conditions of worth that require
extrinsic values and goals. Kasser and Ryan (1996) found that intrinsic goals were correlated
with better psychological well-being and more self-acceptance. Extrinsic aspirations on the
other hand were found not to be associated with well-being (Ryan et al., 1999). Intrinsic
aspirations within the Aspiration Index are related to the underlying factors of self-
acceptance, affiliation and community feeling. However, there is some uncertainty with
regards to the concept of aspirations and how this is related to the concept of unconditional
positive self-regard. For example, the notion of holding aspirations for one’s own self might
the idea of acceptance of self as one currently is. Aspirations might conceivably be related to
self-concept and experience. The discrepancy in this instance would be between the self-
Method
Participants
Data were collected from a subset of the sample used in Study 1. Questionnaires were
were 110 (55.3%) women and 89 (44.7%) were men with a mean age of 31.4 years (S.D.
11.3). The sample consisted of 121 (60.8%) identified as Caucasian, 51 (25.6%) were Asian,
Measures
adapted for use in this study. Intrinsic aspirations are spread over three domains; personal growth,
relatedness and community service. 12-items were used in the analysis with 4-items from each
domain. There are several approaches to scoring the intrinsic aspirations scale (Ryan et al., 1999); for
the current study the total score for intrinsic aspiration was selected by adding together the importance
of aspirations and the future expectation of attaining these aspirations. The scale is scored on a 5 point
Likert scale. Scores range from 1-5 for each item and therefore provide a range of 24-120 for the total
12-item measure. The sample for Study 2 provided an intrinsic aspirations mean score of 56.02
(S.D. 5.90) and item mean score 4.31. Cronbach’s alpha reliability was .85 for the intrinsic
aspirations items.
A detailed explanation of the USPR scale is provided in Study 1. Items from the
unconditional positive self-regard scale had a mean score of 45.64 (S.D. 5.0) and an item
mean score 3.80. Cronbach’s alpha reliability was .72 for the UPSR scale.
Results
each of the self-regard and unconditionality of regard subscales for UPSR scale with intrinsic
aspirations total score and the separate scores for importance and chance of achieving
intrinsic aspirations. Table 2 shows that positive self-regard was statistically significantly
positively correlated with the intrinsic aspirations total scale and with each of the separate
significantly negatively correlated with IA-importance but was not statistically significantly
Regression analysis
regard to predict intrinsic aspirations. Table 3 shows the results from the regression analysis.
First, all variables were standardized using the z transformation facility in SPSS. A linear
regression was conducted using the standardized intrinsic aspiration score entered as the
scores were entered into the regression as the predictor variables. Finally, an interaction term
was created for the standardized self-regard and unconditionality of regard scores and this
Discussion
In this study we partially supported the hypothesis that unconditional positive self-
measure that attempts to capture the extent to which a person perceives their experience
That is people that score highly on this scale would also be expected to score highly on
measures that assess the extent a person is following their intrinsic aspirations. In this study
the finding was supported. Importantly, positive self-regard was related to intrinsic
aspirations-total score and both subscales for importance and chance of attaining intrinsic
score or the intrinsic aspirations-chance subscale but was statistically negatively correlated
with the intrinsic aspirations-importance subscale. That means people with high
There might be a number of explanations for this finding. One possible interpretation
is relying on aspirations measured using the intrinsic aspirations index. In this scale
aspirations are worded as future oriented and thereby require the participant to project
themselves into a different view of themselves than what they currently experience, whereas,
currently are. Consequently, intrinsic aspirations are possibly more indicative of a concept of
the self that is desired but does not represent the self as perceived currently; therefore
to-be-self. Both the concepts of a self-ideal, and ought-to-be-self, are more susceptible to
discrepant to the current self-concept the person is in a state of incongruence and will
experience low unconditionality of self-regard but might feel it is important to achieve their
Study 3
Introduction
There appear to be two methods for assessing a state of well-being within Rogers’s
(1959) theory of personality. First is that the person is free from conditions of worth and thus
experiences a high degree of unconditional positive self-regard. This idea was first proposed
by Standal (1954). The theory of a need for positive regard and a need for positive self-regard
maturity required for maintaining a state of well-being. It also relates directly to the therapist
who provides a relationship climate in which the need for positive regard can be satisfied
(1959) theory is through the self-consistency approach. The second approach presents a
model of incongruence that proposes psychological maladjustment arises due to the threat
posed by self-experiences that are perceived as being inconsistent with the self-structure. A
distortion. When a person experiences being in a state of congruence, there is no need for the
defence system to become active. The person’s experiences can be symobilzed in awareness
without there being a threat to the self-concept. Experiences and the self-concept are
‘congruent’ with one another. When someone is in a state of congruence, their outward
behaviour is thought to be consistent with their inner experiencing. Rogers (1959) suggested
that the person is outwardly being their inner feelings. In a state of congruence, experience,
thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are, metaphorically speaking, symmetrically aligned with
each another. The person is in a state of balance, equilibrium – they are being authentic.
2006). Various approaches to counselling and clinical psychology use authenticity as a way
1965), positive psychology (Joseph & Linley, 2005) and social psychological (Sheldon,
includes self-alienation, authentic living and accepting external influence. Although their
reconceptualizes the notion of congruence. The construct of authenticity provides not only a
more contemporary approach to well-being but also presents it as a relational construct that
aspirations. This finding supports the theoretical proposition that people who do not have to
rely on their worth through the values of others are more likely to act in accord with their
own values and follow their intrinsic aspirations. However, unconditionality of self-regard
was negatively correlated with the importance of intrinsic aspirations. For this reason it is
important to understand the association of these variables for guiding future research and
clinical intervention. Likewise, there is a need to understand more the relationships, at the
construct level, the associations between unconditional positive self-regard and authenticity –
as the two primary approaches to defining psychological well-being within Rogers’s (1959)
Method
Participants
students (N= 100). Just over half of the students were (n = 53) were undertaking counselling
training courses at either postgraduate or undergraduate level. The respondents (n = 47) were
studying for programmes that did not contain any credits related to counselling. Fifty five
were female (55%) and 45 (45%) were male; the mean age of respondents was 31.4 years
(S.D. 8.1 years). The sample consisted of 22 (22%) identified as Caucasian, 51 (51%) were
Caribbean.
Measures
Joseph, 2006). Detailed explanation of this scale is presented above in study 1. For the
present sample the scale showed reliability with Cronbach’s α = .24 which is surprisingly
low. The Aspirations Index (Kasser & Ryan, 1996) measures the extent people are following
their intrinsic aspirations. The study used the measure in the same way described in Study 2.
In the current study the scale showed good reliability with Cronbach’s α = .85.
The third measure used in this study was the Authenticity Scale (Wood et al.). The AS
an inconsistent identity and the extent to which a person's self is incongruent with actual
experiences and deeply held beliefs (e.g., ‘I feel as if I don’t know myself very well’); (2)
accepting external influence, instead of self-directing (e.g., ‘I always feel I need to do what
others expect me to do’); and (3) authentic living, or behaving in ways consistent with beliefs
authentic living represents authenticity. Each item is rated in a 7-point scale (where 1 =
“Does not describe me at all” and 7 = “Describes me very well”) such that scores on each
subscale have a possible range of 4 to 28. With items summed to indicate greater
authenticity, higher scores indicating greater resistance to external influence, greater self-
alienation and greater authentic behaviour. The current study showed reliability with
Cronbach’s α = .58.
Results
Correlation analyses
Correlational analyses were carried out to test the association both of the self-regard
Intrinsic aspiration was scored as a total scale score, and separate correlations were calculated
for the importance and chance of achieving aspirations indices. The results are reported in
Table 3. Self-regard was statistically significantly positively correlated with the total score
for intrinsic aspirations and the IA-chances subscale but was not statistically significantly
correlated with IA-importance subscale or the total score for authenticity. The
unconditionality subscale was statistically significantly negatively correlated with both the
total intrinsic aspiration scale and the IA-importance score but was not significantly
significantly positively correlated with the authenticity scale score. Finally, only the IA-
chance score showed a significant and positive correlation with authenticity the remaining
correlations between intrinsic aspirations and authenticity were not statistically significant.
Regression analyses
Linear regression in SPSS using the Enter method was used to explore the interaction
aspirations. There were no significant interaction effects when using the total scores of
authenticity and intrinsic aspirations. However, based on the correlations in Table 3 above
and in Study 2, we explored the interaction effect of positive self-regard and unconditinality
coefficients with intrinsic aspiration - importance subscale as the dependent variable. The
indicative of low conditions of worth and high positive self-regard, has been shown to have
an acceptable factor structure for use as a two factor measure. Having confirmed the factor
structure we now recommend that future research studies use the UPSR scale as a
this measure can be considered to have developed a mature internal valuing system that is
resilient to the influence of introjected values from others. The measure would be suitable for
As the organismic valuing system matures it is more likely that a person will act in
accord with and follow their intrinsic aspirations. This theoretical proposition has been
partially supported by the current study. However, there is also some disagreement wherein
people that seem to have higher levels of conditions of worth, characterised by low scores on
the unconditionality of self-regard scale, also appear to rate intrinsic aspirations as being of
high importance. This finding is counter intuitive. Above in Study 2 we raised the possible
problem with the future orientation of the aspirations index. Completing the scale could
require participants to imagine a self that does not yet exist. This then might unintentionally
require participants to base their ratings of aspirations in terms of a self that they would like
theoretically, towards being self-accepting of the concept of self currently perceived. Thus
explanation for this finding is that people have developed conditions of worth that demand
self-acceptance. Hence they score high on positive self-regard but low on the
unconditionality of self-regard. That is to say, their concept of self is that they ‘must’,
‘should’, or ‘ought’ to be self-accepting. This too might explain the unexpected finding
reported here.
of values and is associated with positive self-regard. Likewise we explored the links between
proposed as a measure of personality trait (Wood et al, 2008) and provides an alternative way
to conceptualize the psychologically mature person. This finding was supported in Study 3
with authenticity scale scores. These two approaches to measuring well-being are possibly
assessing different components of the subjective experience of well-being and each has a
unique contribution to make for developing understanding about various pathways to further
self-regard on the other hand offers a pathway to well-being by accounting for the
being, whilst also providing a consistent explanation for the caregiver’s, parent’s, therapist’s,
or any other significant social other’s role in the development of maladjustment, readjustment
and maintenance of well-being. The theory for unconditional positive self-regard is also
grounded in a theory of how therapy helps people and therefore provides consistency across
There are some other possible explanations for the findings in the current study. Ryan
et al. (1999) studied intrinsic aspirations of an American and Russian sample of college
students. They found that Americans more than Russians associated the intrinsic over
extrinsic aspirations with better well-being. The cultural differences in this sample might be
able explain how those values and goals identified as intrinsic for some might be extrinsic for
others or that intrinsic aspirations cannot always be associated with better well-being. An
alternative approach for identifying intrinsic aspirations would be to have each individual
participant identify their own unique intrinsic aspirations rather than select them from a
predefined list, as was done in the Ryan et al. (1999) study. However, this is unlikely to solve
the issue entirely because intrinsic aspirations are currently measured by being future oriented
and actually requiring participants to engage in imagining themselves as they would like to
be.
The findings suggest that for some people intrinsic aspirations might be considered a
higher conditions of worth also have more future oriented aspirations. If this is the case there
aspiration might be indicative of the client’s struggle. Hence, rather than accepting all
intrinsic aspirations as inherently suggestive of well-being for the client, it might be helpful
to listen for and be attuned to the possibility that some future oriented intrinsic aspirations
highlight a level of conditionality in self-regard, that is, a lack of self-acceptance of the self
that one currently experiences. What might appear as figure in intrinsic aspiration might be
set in the ground of conditional self-regard. Therapists will be most helpful to clients
therefore when they respond with unconditional positive regard to each of these different
factors that explain different pathways to well-being. The direction of a causal relation
between authenticity and UPSR was not considered in the current study. However, because of
the connection of UPSR with conditions of worth and the likelihood that conditions of worth
shape the way humans process their experiences (Hook & Murphy, In Press), it is reasonable
to assume that UPSR has a causal effect on authenticity. For practice this implies that it
would be most beneficial for clients when therapists maintain a goal for the experiencing of
unconditional positive regard for the client in a bid to enhance client self-acceptance leading
to greater authentic experiencing. One important factor for the experiencing of unconditional
positive regard is the therapist’s own unconditional positive self-regard. For this we suggest
that all therapists pay constant attention to their own personal development and engage in
activities then enable their positive self-regard to flourish and maximise their openness and
Conclusion
In this paper we have confirmed the two factor structure of the unconditional positive
regard scale. This scale is now recommended for use in process outcome studies of
psychotherapy. The scale is also a useful for research in the field of social psychology.
Further work is required to understand the relationship between the concept of unconditional
positive self-regard and intrinsic aspirations and the humanistic concept of authenticity.
Developing our understanding of which aspects of personality function that are explained by
processes.
nature of human wellbeing in the field of humanistic psychology. For example, being future
oriented and accepting of one’s current self-concept is an area for future research to explore.
The drive for change, for aspiring to growth, might need to be reconsidered in the light that
References
Barch, D. M., Yodkovik, N., Sypher-Locke, H., & Hanewinkel, M. (2008) Intrinsic
Becker, M., McElvany, N., & Kortenbruck, M. (2010) Intrinsic and extrinsic reading
Bos, A. E. R., Huijding, J., Muris, P., Vogel, L. R. R., & Biesheuvel, J. (2010). Global,
Cramer, D. (2003). Advanced quantitative data analysis. Maidenhead, UK: Open University
Press.
Crocker, J., Luhtanen, R. K., Cooper, M. L., & Bouvrette, A. (2003). Contingencies of self-
worth in college students: Theory and measurement. Journal of Personality and Social
Crocker, J., & Wolfe, C. T. (2001). Contingencies of self-worth. Psychological Review, 108,
593–623.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985) Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human
Gottfried, A. E., Marcoulides, G. A., Gottfried, A. W., & Oliver, P. H. (2009) A latent curve
model of parental motivational practices and developmental decline in math and science
Hook, L., & Murphy, D. (In Press). Related but not replaceable: A response to Warner’s
Psychotherapies.
Huta, V. (2015). The complementary roles of eudaimonia and hedonia and how they can be
flourishing in work, health, education and everyday life (2nd ed.) (pp. 159-182). Hoboken, NJ:
Wiley.
Joseph, S. (2016). Authentic. How to be yourself and why it matters. Piatkus Little-Brown:
London.
Joseph, S., & Linley, P. A (2005) Positive Adjustment to Threatening Events: An Organismic
Valuing Theory of Growth Through Adversity. Review of General Psychology, 9(3), pp. 262-
280.
Sardinha, L. B., & Teixeira, P. J. (2009) Motivational “spill-over” during weight control:
Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: Correlates of financial
success as a central life aspiration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 410-
422.
Loehlin, J. C. (2004). Latent variable models: An introduction to factor, path and structural
positive self-regard. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 79,
557-590.
science. Vol. 3: Formulations of the person and social context (pp.184-256). New York, NY:
McGraw-Hill.
Ryan, R. M., Chirkov, V. I., Little, T. D., Sheldon, K. M., Timoshina, E., & Deci, E. L.
(1999). The American Dream in Russia: Extrinsic aspirations and well-being in two
Ryan, R. M., Little, T. D., Sheldon, K. M., Timoshima, E., & Deci, E. L. (1999) The
Sansone, C. & Harackiewicz, J.M. (Eds.). (2000). Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation: The
Search for Optimal Motivation and Performance. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Standal, S. (1954). The need for positive regard: A contribution to client-centered theory.
Tremblay, M. A., Blanchard, C. M., Taylor, S., Pelletier, L. G., & Villeneuve, M. (2009)
Work Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation Scale: Its value for organizational psychology
(Eds.), Character strengths and virtues (pp. 249–272). New York: Oxford University Press.
Winnicott, D. W. (1965). The maturational processes and the facilitating environment. New
Wood, A. M., Linley, P. A., Maltby, J., Baliousis, M., & Joseph, S. (2008). The Authentic