A Central Role For The Lateral Prefrontal Cortex in Goal-Directed and Atimulus-Drive Attention
A Central Role For The Lateral Prefrontal Cortex in Goal-Directed and Atimulus-Drive Attention
Attention is the process that selects which sensory information is preferentially processed and ultimately reaches our awareness.
Attention, however, is not a unitary process; it can be captured by unexpected or salient events (stimulus driven) or it can be
deployed under voluntary control (goal directed), and these two forms of attention are implemented by largely distinct ventral
and dorsal parieto-frontal networks. For coherent behavior and awareness to emerge, stimulus-driven and goal-directed behavior
© 2010 Nature America, Inc. All rights reserved.
must ultimately interact. We found that the ventral, but not dorsal, network can account for stimulus-driven attentional limits to
conscious perception, and that stimulus-driven and goal-directed attention converge in the lateral prefrontal component of that
network. Although these results do not rule out dorsal network involvement in awareness when goal-directed task demands are
present, they point to a general role for the lateral prefrontal cortex in the control of attention and awareness.
Reading this manuscript requires attention to be voluntarily deployed, cognitive processes is necessary to capture attention exogenously but
in a ‘top-down’ fashion, to this task. As a consequence of selectively all are known to engage the dorsal attentional network1,3–6.
attending to the page, one might become oblivious to surrounding The same concern also applies to our current understanding of how
sounds and sights. However, if a fire alarm suddenly blares, this salient attention controls awareness. Both the ventral and dorsal attentional
stimulus will probably capture attention in a ‘bottom-up’ manner networks have been associated with conscious perception5,17–23, lend-
and interrupt the ongoing task so that an appropriate course of action ing support to theories of awareness in which widespread changes in
can be initiated. This simple example illustrates a fundamental aspect of brain activity accompany conscious perception17,24. However, there
attention: what ultimately reaches our awareness and guides our behavior has been no specific attempt to assess the relative contributions of the
depends on the interaction between goal-directed and stimulus- dorsal and ventral networks to the neural basis of attentional limits to
driven attention1,2. conscious perception by using tasks that dissociate between stimulus-
Although much is known about the neural mechanisms that support driven and goal-directed attentional processes. Hence, the extent to
goal-directed1,3–7 and stimulus-driven attention1,5,8–12, how these two which each of these attention networks is necessary for awareness is
forms of attention are ultimately coordinated is not yet understood. currently unclear22,23,25.
The finding that these attentional forms are supported by largely dis- We have recently developed an experimental procedure that reveals
tinct neural networks, with a dorsal network that includes the frontal a profound but fleeting deficit in visual awareness resulting from the
eye field (FEF) and superior parietal cortex1,4–7,13 supporting goal- foveal presentation of an unexpected, task-irrelevant stimulus that
directed attention and a ventral one that consists of the lateral and involves neither an overt response nor a shift in spatial attention.
inferior frontal/prefrontal cortex and the temporo-parietal junction The deficit, termed surprise-induced blindness (SiB), is triggered by
(TPJ) underlying stimulus-driven attention1,3,8,9,12,13, has further com- an event that is absent from the observer’s goal-directed attentional
plicated the issue. As a result, several hypotheses have been proposed set and is not under the observer’s initial control26. As such, the
to explain how these two forms of attention might be coordinated: procedure represents a powerful way to assess whether stimulus-
through an interaction between the ventral and dorsal networks1, driven attentional limits to conscious perception can arise within the
across the dorsal14,15 or ventral5 attention network, or in the anterior ventral attention network in the absence of dorsal network involve-
component of the ventral network16. However, many of these proposals ment. Moreover, because the unexpected event ultimately affects the
are based on studies that used tasks that conflate stimulus-driven and goal-directed task of detecting a target, SiB experiments are also well
goal-directed attention, thereby making it difficult to determine the suited to reveal the neural mechanisms by which stimulus-driven
relative contributions of bottom-up and top-down neural processes attention affects goal-directed behavior.
to task performance. For example, the brain mechanisms of stimulus-
driven attention cannot easily be dissociated from those of goal-directed RESULTS
behavior if the stimulus-driven attention task involves spatial shifts of We scanned 30 participants while they were searching for a target
attention, goal-oriented processes, or motor responses, as none of these in a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) stream of distractor
1Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt Vision Research Center, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee, USA. 2Department of Psychology, University of Chicago,
Chicago, Illinois, USA. Correspondence should be addressed to C.L.A. ([email protected]) or R.M. ([email protected]).
Received 16 September 2009; accepted 29 January 2010; published online 7 March 2010; corrected online 14 March 2010 (details online); doi:10.1038/nn.2509
e
m
Ti
B 20
portion of trials (surprise trials), causing SiB (Fig. 1b). Specifically, Surprise stimulus presentation number
two surprise trials was far better than in the respective surprise trials to improve statistical power; see Online Methods).
(sign tests, P’s < 0.0001; Fig. 1b), indicating that SiB does not result A statistical parametric map (SPM) revealed several brain areas that
from an initial difficulty with the target detection task. Rather, the were recruited more during surprise than during search trials (Table 1,
finding that unexpected, task-irrelevant stimuli triggered a profound Fig. 2 and Supplementary Fig. 1). Several of these areas showed invari-
but short-lived impairment in target detection that was essentially ant BOLD responses across the six surprise stimuli presentations, most
dissipated by the third surprise stimulus presentation is consistent notably the fusiform gyrus in visual cortex, suggesting that SiB might be
with a stimulus-driven, attention-based origin for this deficit26. a primarily central phenomenon that occurs at later stages than visual
information processing. Correspondingly, the only two regions that
Neural correlates of SiB demonstrated a BOLD response that quickly habituated after the first
To identify the neural substrates that underlie stimulus-driven two surprise stimulus presentations were in association cortex (Fig. 2):
attentional limits to conscious perception, we first isolated the brain the inferior frontal junction (IFJ), located in the posterior aspect of the
regions that were sensitive to the surprise stimuli, irrespective of presen inferior frontal sulcus (parts of Brodmann areas 9, 44, 6), and the temporo-
tation number (see Online Methods). We then examined the BOLD parietal junction (TPJ), at the intersection of the superior temporal
(blood-oxygen-level dependent) signal from these brain regions, gyrus, supramarginal gyrus and superior temporal sulcus (parts of
Brodmann areas 40, 22, 39). For these two
brain regions, in both hemispheres, the peak
Table 1 Anatomical location and statistical assessment of activation for the ROIs isolated
response to SS1+2 was higher than the response
from surprise trials in Experiment 1 (surprise trial–search trial contrast)
to the two other SS pairs (two-tailed paired
Talairach coordinates SS1+2 vs. SS3+4 vs. SS1+2 vs.
Region Hemi (x, y, z) SS3+4 (t) SS5+6 (t) SS5+6 (t)
t tests, t29’s > 2.05, P’s < 0.049), whereas the
peak responses to SS3+4 and SS5+6 were indis-
TPJ Right 46, −56, 27 2.11* 1.07 2.76*
TPJ Left −49, −56, 23 2.35* 1.30 2.86* tinguishable (t29’s < 1.30, P’s > 0.20; Fig. 2).
IFJ Right 37, 5, 29 2.72* −0.37 2.05* Thus, the IFJ and TPJ exhibited an activity
IFJ Left −40, 8, 25 2.46* 0.05 2.18* pattern that mirrored the magnitude of SiB.
FG Right 30, −44, −11 0.42 −0.92 −0.53 This activity modulation was caused by the
FG Left −32, −51, −10 1.05 −0.70 0.05
IFG Right 40, 19, 13 0.75 0.30 1.09
surprise stimuli, not the perceived absence
IFG Left −48, 19, 7 −0.57 0.77 0.19 of a target, which co-varies with SiB, because
OFC Right 34, 27, −10 −0.22 −0.19 −0.53 the peak responses in target-absent trials and
OFC Left −37, 26, −8 0.27 0.15 0.40 search trials (see Online Methods) were indis-
Pulvinar Bilateral −7/9, −27, 1 1.46 0.71 1.83
tinguishable (two-tailed paired t tests, t29’s
PG Right 32, −3, −13 −0.09 −0.70 −0.80
STG Right 33, 12, −27 0.95 0.07 1.26 < 1.17, P’s > 0.25). No other brain regions
MTS Left −51, 2, −12 −0.17 −0.28 −0.36 showed an SiB-like pattern of activation, as an
SFG Right 13, 24, 49 0.49 0.74 0.90 additional SPM that directly contrasted SS1+2
Amygdala/SLEA Right 16, −9, −8 1.58 −0.17 1.26 with SS3+4 and SS5+6 demonstrated.
The three rightmost columns list the t-values resulting from paired t-tests of the given surprise stimulus pairs. Together, these results indicate that the
Amygdala/SLEA, amygdala and sublenticular extended amygdale; FG, fusiform gyrus (Brodmann area 37);
IFG, inferior frontal gyrus (Brodmann areas 44, 45); IFJ, inferior frontal junction (Brodmann areas 9, 44, 6); presentation of unexpected, task-irrelevant
MTS, middle temporal sulcus (Brodmann area 21); OFC, orbitofrontal cortex (Brodmann area 47); PG, parahippocam- stimuli activates a large network of cortical
pal gyrus; SFG, superior frontal gyrus (Brodmann area 8); STG, superior temporal gyrus (Brodmann area 38)45; TPJ,
temporo-parietal junction (Brodmann areas 39, 40, 22). With the exception of the IFJ and TPJ, none of these brain
and subcortical regions, but only a subset of
regions showed activation differences between any of the three surprise stimulus pairs (all P’s > 0.1), although the this network in the frontal/prefrontal and
amygdala/SLEA and pulvinar showed non-significant trends for greater SS1+2 activation relative to the two other temporo-parietal cortex shows a rapid BOLD
SS pairs. Note that the TPJ foci are anatomically distinct from, and superior to, regions of the superior temporal
46
sulcus involved in processing facial expressions and eye gaze . response adaptation that is commensurate
*P < 0.05. with the behavioral performance. This subset
0.6 Left IFJ t 0.6 Right IFJ Figure 2 SiB experiment (Experiment 1) SPM.
Percent signal change
ventral attention network1,13. by the surprise stimuli, but unlike the swift activation pattern in the
ventral network following presentations of the first two surprise
Late dorsal network activation stimuli, the dorsal network appears to respond too late (by about 3 s)
In contrast to the ventral network, the SPMs (even with a liberal for it to cause SiB. However, this conclusion depends on the dorsal
threshold of P < 0.001, uncorrected) provided no evidence for sur- activation delay reflecting a genuine late neural response rather than
prise stimulus-related activation in the core brain regions associated inherent differences in the hemodynamic properties of the ventral
with the goal-directed attention network1,13, namely the frontal eye and dorsal parieto-frontal networks. We performed a follow-up
field (FEF) and the intraparietal sulcus (IPS). To analyze the dorsal ‘spatial SiB’ experiment (Experiment 2) to distinguish between
network’s association with SiB with greater sensitivity, for each par- these possibilities. Given the role of the dorsal network in the con-
ticipant, we functionally defined regions of interest (ROIs) for the trol of visuo-spatial attention1,4,6, we predicted that the sporadic
putative FEF and IPS based on their activation in search trials, as presentations of highly salient, categorically distinct task-irrelevant
these regions were strongly activated by the task of searching for and stimuli in the periphery instead of in the center of the RSVP stream
responding to targets (see Online Methods, Fig. 3 and Table 2). The (Fig. 4a) would not only persist in capturing attention26 but also
anatomical locations of the resulting ROIs corresponded well to the lead to shifts of visual-spatial attention or eye movements, thereby
conventional positions of the FEF and IPS in goal-directed attention promptly recruiting the FEF and IPS in addition to the ventral network.
tasks1,3–6,12,13,16. When probed during the surprise trials, activity in Consistent with our hypothesis, we found a persistent SiB effect
these dorsal regions was greater during SS1+2 than in search trials and robust activations in both the dorsal and ventral networks
(t29’s > 2.50, P’s < 0.018 except for a marginal effect in the right FEF (Fig. 4b, and Supplementary Tables 1 and 2). Notably, there was no
at t29 = 1.79, P = 0.084) but not during subsequent pairs (Table 2). longer a delay in activation between the dorsal and ventral networks
Thus, the more sensitive ROI analysis revealed that the FEF and IPS (P’s > 0.36), in marked contrast to Experiment 1 (timing delay differ-
are also activated by the first two surprise stimulus presentations. ence across experiments: one-tailed P = 0.038; see Online Methods).
Notably, however, the time courses of activation specific to the first These results indicate that the delay in activation of the dorsal brain
pair of surprise stimuli, revealed by subtracting the underlying search- regions during the surprise trials of Experiment 1 had a neural,
related activity from the first two surprise stimulus trials (see Online not hemodynamic, origin. Additional behavioral evidence indicates
Methods), showed that the IPS and FEF responded significantly later that this delayed activation might reflect the modulation of top-down
than did the IFJ and TPJ (all pair-wise comparisons P < 0.048; see attentional settings in anticipation of post-surprise stimulus trials
FEF
trials. (b) Surprise stimulus-specific waveform 0.4
0.1
in dorsal (FEF, IPS) and ventral (IFJ, TPJ)
regions of interest (ROIs) defined in individual R 0.2 0
participants (see Online Methods). Each time
course was constructed by subtracting the –0.1
0
search trial time course from the time course
IPS –0.2
for the first two surprise stimulus trials. The t 8.00
surprise stimulus appears at approximately time –0.2 –0.3
2.82 –4 0 4 8 12 16 20 0 4 8 12 16 20 24
zero. (c) Search trial activation time course qFDR < 0.05 Time from surprise stimulus (s) Time from trial onset (s)
over the same period of time as in b for the
same ROIs. Arrows mark each trial’s onset. Note that the activation pattern is cyclical, mirroring the trial structure (one trial every 8 s). The observed
hemodynamic responses in IPS, FEF and IFJ match the predicted responses for the hypothesized search-related activity (Supplementary Fig. 3).
during the cue-related period (t5’s > 3.53, P’s < 0.017; see Online
Table 2 Average anatomical location and statistical assessment
of activation for the individually defined ROIs from search trials in Methods), as expected of brain regions involved in goal-directed
Experiment 1 (open contrast SPM) attention1,3,6 (Fig. 5b). Furthermore, the IFJ was also activated by the
Talairach coordinates SS1+2 vs. SS3+4 vs. SS5+6 vs.
cue (t5 = 2.74, P = 0.041) and at the same time as the FEF and IPS. These
Region Hemi (x, y, z) ± s.d. search (t) search (t) search (t) results were obtained regardless of whether the ROIs were defined
IPS Right 26 ± 4, −65 ± 6, 36 ± 5 2.81* 0.46 0.28 in the cueing task or in the search trials of the RSVP task, attesting
IPS Left −22 ± 4, −66 ± 6, 39 ± 6 3.45* 0.44 1.38 to the fact that the brain regions that showed goal-directed activity in
FEF Right 34 ± 4, −7 ± 3, 51 ± 5 1.79 0.51 0.63 the search trials are also involved in visuo-spatial shifts of attention.
FEF Left −32 ± 5, −8 ± 3, 51 ± 5 2.50* 1.95 0.96 We therefore conclude that IFJ supports goal-directed behavior, as it
TPJ Right 47 ± 5, −55 ± 5, 28 ± 5 4.52* 3.21* 1.65 is activated along with core members of the dorsal network during
TPJ Left −49 ± 2, −58 ± 5, 24 ± 4 5.33* 2.37* 1.73
the cue period of a classic goal-directed attention task. These con-
IFJ Right 40 ± 4, 6 ± 3, 27 ± 3 4.99* 1.67 1.06
IFJ Left −42 ± 3, 8 ± 3, 25 ± 2 5.92* 2.51* 1.12
clusions are consistent with previous reports suggesting that similar
The three rightmost columns list the t-values resulting from paired t-tests of the given brain regions are activated in other cued attention shift tasks37,38.
surprise stimulus pairs versus target-only activity. See Table 1 for abbreviations. These Together, the results of our experiments indicate that the IFJ parti
ROI coordinates closely matched those isolated from the surprise trials (Table 1).
*P < 0.05.
cipates in both stimulus-driven and goal-directed attention. While
the pattern of IFJ activity is consistent with brain regions involved
in stimulus-driven attention during the presentation of a surprise
(Supplementary SiB RT Experiment and Supplementary Fig. 2). stimulus, its activity profile is instead more consistent with those of
Regardless of the function of this delayed dorsal response to sur- goal-directed brain regions during the search task. It follows from
prise stimulus presentations, it does not appear to be involved in these results that the IFJ should be more functionally integrated with
© 2010 Nature America, Inc. All rights reserved.
SiB, as the FEF and IPS are activated after the events that trigger the core members of the dorsal network during goal-directed attention
perceptual deficit. but with core members of the ventral network during stimulus-
driven attention. These predictions were borne out by a functional
Interaction of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention connectivity analysis (see Online Methods), which showed that IFJ
If the core components of the dorsal attentional network (the FEF activity was correlated positively with activity in the FEF and IPS (IFJ-
and IPS) are not responsible for SiB, then how do the surprise stimuli FEF, t29 = 9.13, P < 0.0001; IFJ-IPS, t29 = 12.19, P < 0.0001; FEF-IPS,
ultimately impair the goal-directed task of searching for and respond- t29 = 6.87, P < 0.0001), but negatively with TPJ activity (t29 = −4.27,
ing to a target? That is, how does stimulus-driven attention disrupt P = 0.00019), during the search trials. Conversely, following the
goal-directed behavior? The answer is provided by an examination presentation of a surprise stimulus, the IFJ-FEF and IFJ-IPS correla-
of the temporal dynamics of activation in the two attentional net- tions decreased (paired t test of the changes: t29 = −2.12, P = 0.043;
works during the search trials of Experiment 1. In these trials, the t29 = −2.36, P = 0.025), whereas those between IFJ and TPJ increased
FEF and IPS showed the pattern expected of brain regions associ- (t29 = 2.07, P = 0.047) and those between FEF and IPS did not
ated with goal-directed behavior, namely an activation profile that change (t29 = −1.07, P = 0.29). These connectivity results provide
tightly correlated with performing the primary search task (Fig. 3c). additional evidence that the IFJ underlies stimulus-driven and goal-
By contrast, TPJ activity was out of phase with activity in the dorsal directed attention, with its response profile and network allegiance
brain regions, showing deactivation when the others were activated depending on task demands. In that context, SiB would result from
(onset shifts of ~4 s, P’s < 0.0001; Fig. 3c), consistent with the finding the ‘bottom-up’ engagement of IFJ by the presentation of a surprise
that attention-demanding cognitive tasks are often accompanied by stimulus, thereby transiently disrupting or altering this brain
suppression of TPJ activity13,21,33–35. Notably, the time course of IFJ region’s control of the target detection task. With behavioral and
activity no longer closely followed that of its ventral network cohort, neuronal habituation to the repeated surprise stimulus presenta-
the TPJ (onset shifts of ~4 s, P’s < 0.0001), but instead closely tracked tions, the IFJ might be able to maintain its goal-oriented activity
the activation time course of the dorsal brain regions (Fig. 3c). This even in the face of task-irrelevant stimulus presentations.
IFJ activity in the search trials does not simply reflect target detec-
tion, for the same activity pattern was found in target-absent trials.
These findings suggest that the IFJ might be not only a core member
a G
b 0.6
TPJ IPS
Target IFJ FEF
of the ventral attention network that supports stimulus-driven atten-
Percent signal change
X
0.4
tion, but also functionally integrated with the dorsal network during
Y
goal-directed behavior.
Surprise K 0.2
To test the hypothesis that the IFJ ROIs identified in Experiment 2 stimulus
are important in goal-directed behavior, we carried out an additional C
e
m
0
Ti
target presented at a location that was cued by the color of a central Figure 4 Spatial SiB experiment (Experiment 2). (a) Trial design.
fixation point, with the cue validly predicting the location of the The procedure was identical to that in Experiment 1 save that in a
target on 80% of the trials. The task was successful in engaging small proportion of trials, a colorful surprise stimulus was shown before
the target away from fixation (Fig. 1a). (b) Surprise stimulus-specific
goal-directed attention, evidenced by the fact that participants were
waveforms in dorsal and ventral attention network ROIs defined in
faster at detecting the target at validly cued than at invalidly cued individual participants. Time courses were constructed in the same
positions (reaction time ± s.d., 317 ± 36 ms versus 396 ± 64 ms, fashion as those in Experiment 1 (see Fig. 3b). Note that all four regions
t5 = 4.53, P = 0.0062). The dorsal brain regions were activated show an immediate response to the surprise stimulus presentations.
0.4
Tim
(3,967–9,967 ms) functionally interacts with both ventral and dorsal brain structures 16.
0.2
The IFJ has also been implicated in task-switching and cognitive
Target (33 ms)
control42,43 more generally. This brain region is therefore ideally
Variable delay 0
suited to act as the neural site of coordination for stimulus-driven
(2,000–8,000 ms)
and goal-directed attention. Moreover, the IFJ’s involvement in
–0.2
both the non-spatial and spatial SiB tasks (Experiments 1 and 2)
Cue (2,000 ms) 0 4 8 12 16 indicates that this brain region’s function generalizes across both
Time from cue onset (s)
spatial and non-spatial forms of attention. While it remains to be seen
Figure 5 Endogenous cueing task experiment (Experiment 3). (a) Trial whether all these attentional processes are mediated by the same sub-
design. A color cue predicted the location of an upcoming target, to
populations of IFJ neurons, a central role for this brain region in the
which the participant then responded in a speeded manner. (b) Cue-
related activity in dorsal and ventral attention network ROIs isolated from co-ordination of stimulus-driven and goal-directed attention across
Experiment 2 (see Online Methods). The arrow marks cue onset. ITI, both spatial and non-spatial domains resonates well with the proposal
inter-stimulus interval. that the IFJ is a critical neural substrate that underlies our limited
attentional capacities44.
DISCUSSION
Our study reveals that both functional divergence and convergence Methods
of the dorsal and ventral attentional networks underlie attention Methods and any associated references are available in the online
© 2010 Nature America, Inc. All rights reserved.
and awareness. Divergence of function between these two networks version of the paper at http://www.nature.com/natureneuroscience/.
is clearly shown by the SiB procedure, as it shows that stimulus-driven
Note: Supplementary information is available on the Nature Neuroscience website.
attentional limits to conscious perception can arise from the ven-
tral attentional network in the absence of dorsal network or visual Acknowledgments
cortex modulation. In contrast, previous neurobiological investiga- We thank B. Rogers, J. Swisher and E. Conser. This work was supported by National
tions17–22,39,40 have frequently implicated regions of both the ven- Science Foundation grant 0094992 and National Institute of Mental Health grant
R01 MH70776 (R.M.).
tral and dorsal networks in awareness. Such large-scale activation
patterns are consistent with ‘global workspace’ models that posit AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
that awareness emerges from the reverberating activity of a widely C.L.A. designed and performed experiments, analyzed data, and wrote the
distributed cortical network17,22,24. The difference in activation pat- manuscript. J.J.T. and A.P.S. designed and performed experiments. R.M. designed
terns between these previous studies and the present one is prob- experiments and wrote the manuscript.
ably the result of differences in task design. Whereas previous tasks COMPETING FINANCIAL INTERESTS
have included spatial shifts of attention3,5,14,15,18,39 or covert or overt The authors declare no competing financial interests.
responses3,5,11,12,18–21,39,40 to the critical attention-capturing stimu-
lus, our task was specifically designed to exclude these components Published online at http://www.nature.com/natureneuroscience/.
Reprints and permissions information is available online at http://www.nature.com/
as they are unnecessary for exogenous attentional capture but can reprintsandpermissions/.
activate the dorsal attention network1,3–6. As such, our task shows
that the dorsal network’s contribution to conscious perception might
1. Corbetta, M. & Shulman, G.L. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention
be negligible under these controlled circumstances. This conclusion in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215 (2002).
is consistent with the suggestion that awareness is not necessarily an 2. Egeth, H.E. & Yantis, S. Visual attention: control, representation, and time course.
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 48, 269–297 (1997).
emergent property of the dorsal network25 and poses a challenge to 3. Corbetta, M., Kincade, J.M., Ollinger, J.M., McAvoy, M.P. & Shulman, G.L. Voluntary
global network theories of awareness. orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex.
These conclusions, however, do not imply that the dorsal network Nat. Neurosci. 3, 292–297 (2000).
4. Kastner, S., Pinsk, M.A., De Weerd, P., Desimone, R. & Ungerleider, L.G. Increased
never has a role in attentional limits to explicit perception. Dorsal activity in human visual cortex during directed attention in the absence of visual
structures might contribute to, and be essential for, conscious percep- stimulation. Neuron 22, 751–761 (1999).
tion during tasks that involve top-down or goal-oriented processing, 5. Serences, J.T. et al. Coordination of voluntary and stimulus-driven attentional control
in human cortex. Psychol. Sci. 16, 114–122 (2005).
such as change detection or binocular rivalry17,18,22,40. Indeed, just as 6. Yantis, S. et al. Transient neural activity in human parietal cortex during spatial
deficits of awareness in different visual domains often have dissociable attention shifts. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 995–1002 (2002).
7. Chiu, Y.C. & Yantis, S. A domain-independent source of cognitive control for task
neural origins (for example, prosopagnosia versus achromatopsia)22, sets: Shifting spatial attention and switching categorization rules. J. Neurosci. 29,
awareness might also be fractionated at central, attentional stages of 3930–3938 (2009).
information processing. Additional research, aided by better deline- 8. Downar, J., Crawley, A.P., Mikulis, D.J. & Davis, K.D. A multimodal cortical network
for the detection of changes in the sensory environment. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 277–283
ation of the topographically distinct sub-regions of the IPS41, will be (2000).
necessary to assess the specific contributions of the dorsal network to 9. Downar, J., Crawley, A.P., Mikulis, D.J. & Davis, K.D. A cortical network sensitive
attentional limits to conscious perception and awareness in general. to stimulus salience in a neutral behavioral context across multiple sensory
modalities. J. Neurophysiol. 87, 615–620 (2002).
As well as revealing a functional dissociation between the ventral 10. Horovitz, S.G., Skudlarski, P. & Gore, J.C. Correlations and dissociations between
and dorsal attentional networks in awareness, the very nature of BOLD signal and P300 amplitude in an auditory oddball task: a parametric approach
to combining fMRI and ERP. Magn. Reson. Imaging 20, 319–325 (2002).
SiB, a profound deficit in the detection of a goal-relevant target as a 11. Linden, D.E. et al. The functional neuroanatomy of target detection: an fMRI study
result of the presentation of an unexpected and task-irrelevant stim- of visual and auditory oddball tasks. Cereb. Cortex 9, 815–823 (1999).
ulus, underscores that stimulus-driven and goal-directed attention 12. Marois, R., Leung, H.C. & Gore, J.C. A stimulus-driven approach to object identity
and location processing in the human brain. Neuron 25, 717–728 (2000).
must ultimately interact1,2,5,13–16. Our results indicate that the ventral 13. Corbetta, M., Patel, G. & Shulman, G.L. The reorienting system of the human brain:
attention network’s lateral prefrontal component, the inferior frontal from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58, 306–324 (2008).
14. Buschman, T.J. & Miller, E.K. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in 31. Heilman, K.M. & Watson, R.T. Mechanisms underlying the unilateral neglect
the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315, 1860–1862 (2007). syndrome. Adv. Neurol. 18, 93–106 (1977).
15. Gottlieb, J. From thought to action: the parietal cortex as a bridge between 32. Karnath, H.O., Milner, A.D. & Vallar, G. The Cognitive and Neural Bases of Spatial
perception, action and cognition. Neuron 53, 9–16 (2007). Neglect (Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2002).
16. He, B.J. et al. Breakdown of functional connectivity in frontoparietal networks 33. Todd, J.J., Fougnie, D. & Marois, R. Visual short-term memory load suppresses
underlies behavioral deficits in spatial neglect. Neuron 53, 905–918 (2007). temporo-parietal junction activity and induces inattentional blindness. Psychol. Sci.
17. Dehaene, S., Changeux, J.P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J. & Sergent, C. Conscious, 16, 965–972 (2005).
preconscious and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 34. Shulman, G.L., Astafiev, S.V., McAvoy, M.P., d’Avossa, G. & Corbetta, M. Right TPJ
10, 204–211 (2006). deactivation during visual search: functional significance and support for a filter
18. Beck, D.M., Rees, G., Frith, C.D. & Lavie, N. Neural correlates of change detection hypothesis. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2625–2633 (2007).
and change blindness. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 645–650 (2001). 35. Fox, M.D. et al. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated
19. Rees, G., Russell, C., Frith, C.D. & Driver, J. Inattentional blindness versus inattentional functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102, 9673–9678 (2005).
amnesia for fixated, but ignored, words. Science 286, 2504–2507 (1999). 36. Posner, M.I., Snyder, C.R.R. & Davidson, B.J. Attention and the detection of signals.
20. Marois, R., Chun, M.M. & Gore, J.C. Neural correlates of the attentional blink. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 109, 160–174 (1980).
Neuron 28, 299–308 (2000). 37. Corbetta, M. et al. A common network of functional areas for attention and eye
21. Marois, R., Yi, D.J. & Chun, M.M. The neural fate of consciously perceived and movements. Neuron 21, 761–773 (1998).
missed events in the attentional blink. Neuron 41, 465–472 (2004). 38. Kastner, S. et al. Topographic maps in human frontal cortex revealed in memory-guided
22. Rees, G., Kreiman, G. & Koch, C. Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. saccade and spatial working-memory tasks. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 3494–3507
Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 261–270 (2002). (2007).
23. Husain, M. & Nachev, P. Space and the parietal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 39. Huettel, S.A., Güzeldere, G. & McCarthy, G. Dissociating the neural mechanisms
30–36 (2007). of visual attention in change detection using functional MRI. J. Cogn. Neurosci.
24. Baars, B.J. In the theatre of consciousness: global workspace theory, a rigorous 13, 1006–1018 (2001).
scientific theory of consciousness. J. Conscious. Stud. 4, 292–309 (1997). 40. Lumer, E.D., Friston, K.J. & Rees, G. Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the
25. Goodale, M.A. & Milner, A.D. Separate visual pathways for perception and action. human brain. Science 280, 1930–1934 (1998).
Trends Neurosci. 15, 20–25 (1992). 41. Konen, C.S. & Kastner, S. Representation of eye movements and stimulus motion in
26. Asplund, C.L., Todd, J.J., Snyder, A.P., Gilbert, C.M. & Marois, R. Surprise-induced topographically organized areas of human PPC. J. Neurosci. 28, 8361–8375
blindness: a stimulus-driven attentional limit to conscious perception. J. Exp. (2008).
Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. (in the press). 42. Brass, M., Derrfuss, J., Forstmann, B. & von Cramon, D.Y. The role of the inferior
© 2010 Nature America, Inc. All rights reserved.
27. Yamaguchi, S., Hale, L.A., D’Esposito, M. & Knight, R.T. Rapid prefrontal- frontal junction in cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 314–316 (2005).
hippocampal habituation to novel events. J. Neurosci. 24, 5356–5363 (2004). 43. Koechlin, E., Ody, C. & Kouneiher, F. The architecture of cognitive control in the
28. Knight, R.T. Decreased response to novel stimuli after prefrontal lesions in man. human prefrontal cortex. Science 302, 1181–1185 (2003).
Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 59, 9–20 (1984). 44. Marois, R. & Ivanoff, J. Capacity limits of information processing in the brain.
29. Opitz, B., Mecklinger, A., Friederici, A.D. & von Cramon, D.Y. The functional Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 296–305 (2005).
neuroanatomy of novelty processing: integrating ERP and fMRI results. Cereb. Cortex 45. Talairach, J. & Tournoux, P. Co-planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain (Thieme
9, 379–391 (1999). Medical Publishers, New York, 1988).
30. Courchesne, E., Hillyard, S.A. & Galambos, R. Stimulus novelty, task relevance and 46. Hoffman, E.A. & Haxby, J.V. Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in
the visual evoked potential in man. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 39, the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 80–84
131–143 (1975). (2000).
no surprise stimulus). Participants practiced search trials exclusively before the 10%). Finally, these onset values (10,000 per ROI) were compared across ROIs.
fMRI session. Feedback was given only during practice, and participants were For example, right IFJ onsets occurred before right IPS onsets for 9,977 of the
required to reach target accuracy above 80% before scanning. At no time were samples. From this count, we computed a P-value, which in the example would
participants informed about the surprise stimuli. be 0.0066, two-tailed. Interactions were computed by first subtracting the onset
To assess the effect of repeated surprise stimulus presentations, we used values for surprise-related activity from those for search-related activity and then
Cochran Q tests for categorical data of dependent samples47. We then applied comparing these differences across regions.
sign tests to determine the significance of the relevant pair-wise comparisons. As no hemispheric differences were found in any of the above analyses, and
These and all subsequently described tests were two-tailed with α at 0.05 unless to increase statistical power, we collapsed the data across hemispheres for all
otherwise noted. subsequent analyses.
Anatomical 3D high-resolution images were acquired using conventional The correlation analyses were performed on the data derived from the GLM
parameters on a 3T GE MRI system. Nineteen 7-mm-thick axial slices (0 mm skip; analyses after further processing steps had been applied. Global signal fluctuations
3.75 × 3.75 mm in-plane) were taken parallel to the anterior commissure–posterior in this dataset were removed by regressing out the time courses from a ventricu-
commissure (AC-PC) line. T2*-weighted image parameters: 25 ms echo time, 70° lar region of interest, a white matter region of interest, and the average signal
flip angle, 240 mm FOV, 64 × 64 matrix, 2,000 ms repetition time. The functional across the entire brain. Second, the data were filtered using a zero-phase forward
scan included 166 brain volumes, with the first 6 volumes discarded for signal and reverse band-pass filter (0.01 < f < 0.2 Hz). Next, we segmented the data
stabilization. Trials were presented using Psychophysics ToolBox48,49 for Matlab on from each individually defined ROI by trial, performing a percent signal change
an Apple G4 Macintosh. Stimuli were back-projected from an LCD projector onto transform on each trial as described above. Trials were then concatenated by
a screen viewed through a prism mirror by the supine participant. condition, yielding 28 points associated with surprise trials (SS1+2) and 28 points
Data analysis was performed using BrainVoyager 4.9.1, BrainVoyager QX 1.7.9 with search trials (two randomly selected trials from about 10 that were at least
(Brain Innovation), and custom Matlab software. Data preprocessing included three trials away from any surprise trials). Time courses for each ROI (collapsed
image realignment, 3D motion correction, linear trend removal and correction across hemispheres) pair of interest in each participant were then correlated by
for slice acquisition timing. Statistical parametric maps (SPMs) of BOLD activa- condition and the resulting values converted using Fisher’s z transformation. The
tion were created using a multiple regression analysis, with regressors defined correlations between regions were then tested for significance across participants
for the six surprise stimuli, search trials, and no-target trials; boxcar functions with one-sample t-tests, and the change in correlations between search and sur-
for each trial type were convolved with a canonical double-γ hemodynamic prise trials compared across participants with paired t-tests.
function (SPM2, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) to generate each regressor.
The resulting maps from all participants were spatially smoothed with a 6-mm Spatial SiB experiment (Experiment 2). Six right-handed individuals (three
Gaussian kernel (full width at half maximum), standardized to Talairach space45, females) participated. The timing of each trial was as in Experiment 1 except
and superimposed to create composite maps. The model fit was assessed using that 29 stimuli were shown on each trial and the ISI was 17 ms. There were
t statistics, with significance determined by the false discovery rate (FDR) threshold 40 trials during each of six fMRI runs. The target (present on 80% of trials) appeared
at q < 0.05 (random-effects analysis). between frames 23 and 27. In four of the trials per run (three target-present and
For the group region of interest (ROI) analysis, the center of mass and sur- one target-absent), a surprise stimulus appeared 350 ms before the target at one of
rounding activated voxels for each activated focus were selected, up to 1 cm3. four spatial locations centered 4.2° from fixation. The surprise stimuli consisted
The time-course for each surprise trial was extracted from each ROI for each of distinct, large (6.5° × 6.5°), colorful items, each shown only once. Surprise
participant and then converted to percent signal change (baseline from the time trials occurred between trials 2 and 38, and were separated by a minimum of
point of surprise stimulus (SS) onset and two preceding points). The average three search trials. Participants practiced the search task before the fMRI session.
time courses for pairs of SS (SS1+SS2, SS3+SS4, SS5+SS6) were computed for Participants were not informed about the surprise stimuli.
each participant. For statistical tests of amplitude, we identified the time point Anatomical 3D high-resolution images were acquired using conventional
with the largest percent signal change between 6 and 8 s after surprise stimulus parameters on a 3T Philips MRI system. Thirty-three 3.5-mm thick axial slices
presentation for each SS pair for each participant, and then used paired t-tests (0.5 mm skip; 1.875 × 1.875 mm in-plane) were taken parallel to the AC–PC
for the appropriate comparisons. line. T2*-weighted image parameters: 35 ms echo time, 79° flip angle, 240 mm
For the individually-defined ROI analyses, we identified ROIs whose activity FOV, 128 × 128 matrix, 2,000 ms repetition time. There were 161 brain volumes
correlated with the performance of the primary target-detection task (SPM of per functional scan. Trials were presented using Psychophysics ToolBox48,49 for
open contrast of the predictor for search trials). Positive β-weights for the predictor Matlab on an Apple MacBook Pro, and stimuli back-projected to the participant
were associated with the FEF, IPS and IFJ, whereas negative ones were associated as in Experiment 1.