Effects of Barefoot and Footwear Conditions On Learning of A Dynamic Balance Task - A Randomized Controlled Study
Effects of Barefoot and Footwear Conditions On Learning of A Dynamic Balance Task - A Randomized Controlled Study
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00421-018-3997-6
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Abstract
Purpose Although barefoot balancing has shown to be more challenging compared to shod balancing, it is still unclear
whether this may also influence the balance learning effects. The purpose of this study was to explore the impact of barefoot
and shod exercising on learning of a dynamic balance task.
Methods Sixty healthy and physically active adults (mean age 25.3 ± 3.4 years) were randomly allocated into one of three
groups (barefoot, shod and controls). The barefoot and shod intervention groups exercised once weekly over 7 weeks on a
stability platform with an unstable surface. Each training session included 15 trials over 30 s. Before and after the interven-
tion period, all participants completed two balance tests (stability platform and Balance Error Scoring System = BESS) under
barefoot and shod conditions. Group effects in stability gains (pre to post-test differences) were analysed using ANOVA.
Development of balance learning curves during the intervention period was analysed using a mixed effects model.
Results Balance times improved in both intervention groups (p < 0.001, 95% CI barefoot 5.82–9.22 s, shod 7.51–10.92 s)
compared to controls. The barefoot intervention group showed a significantly less sloped balance learning curve compared to
the shod intervention group (p = 0.033). No changes over time or differences between groups were found for the BESS test.
Conclusions Improvements in the dynamic balance task did not differ between individuals exercising barefoot or with
footwear although the progression was slower in the barefoot group. The lack of changes in the BESS supports the task-
specificity of balance learning effects.
Abbreviations
ANOVA Analysis of variance
BESS Balance error scoring system
CNS Central nervous system
Communicated by Håkan Westerblad. ßF,C Intercept for the shod group
ß∆B,C Differential intercept for the barefoot group
* Astrid Zech
astrid.zech@uni‑jena.de compared to the shod group
ßF,L Slope coefficient for the linear learning effect
1
Department of Human Movement Science and Exercise in the shod group
Physiology, Institute of Sport Science, University of Jena, ß∆B,L Differential slope coefficient for the linear
Seidelstraße 20, 07749 Jena, Germany
learning effect in the barefoot group compared
2
Institute of Human Movement Science, Universität Hamburg, to the shod group
Hamburg, Germany
ßF,Q Slope coefficient for the quadratic learning
3
Institute of Psychology, Biological Psychology effect in the shod group
and Neuropsychology, Universität Hamburg, Hamburg,
Germany ß∆B,Q Differential slope coefficient for the quadratic
4 learning effect in the barefoot group compared
Institute of Statistics, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany
to the shod group
5
Department of Sports and Rehabilitation Medicine, BG
Trauma Hospital of Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
6
Department of Sports and Exercise Medicine, Universität
Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
13
Vol.:(0123456789)
European Journal of Applied Physiology
13
European Journal of Applied Physiology
Fig. 1 Stability platform (stabilometer) used for balance exercising and testing
13
European Journal of Applied Physiology
the stabilometer platform during the 30 s trial. A longer time complete the study (two in the shod group, one in the bare-
indicated a better balance performance. For pre- vs. post-test foot group and four in the control group) indicating a drop
analysis the mean score of the three trials was calculated for out rate of 11.7%. Mean adherence to balance exercising was
barefoot and shod conditions in the three groups. To analyse 97.9% ± 5.1% (barefoot group: 97.1% ± 5.9%, shod group:
the balance learning curve in both intervention groups, the 98.6% ± 4.3%).
mean time over the 15 trials was calculated for each of the
seven exercise sessions. Pre to post‑test effects
Secondary outcome was the cumulative number of errors
of the BESS test at pre and post-test. Table 1 shows the summarized statistics of stabilometer
measurements at pre- and post-tests, for each group and each
Statistics test condition. Regarding pre to post-test effects, one-way
ANOVA for differenced (post-minus pre-test) data showed
The group × time interaction effect in the paired (post- and significant between group effects (p < 0.001) for both testing
pre-test) sample was analysed by testing for a group effect conditions (Table 2). Post hoc t tests (Bonferroni adjusted)
in the differenced (post-minus pre- test) data using ANOVA. showed significant differences between the barefoot group
Balance learning curves were analysed using a mixed effects and control group (p < 0.001), and the shod group and con-
model with a random intercept component and orthogo- trol group (p < 0.001) for both balance test conditions. No
nal polynomial contrasts. For computing the denominator differences were found between both intervention groups
degrees of freedom the Kenward–Roger approximation was (shod balancing: p = 0.35; barefoot balancing: p = 1.000)
applied. P values were adjusted for multiple testing (Ben- although there was a tendency towards higher improvements
jamini and Hochberg 1995). Temporal improvements were in the shod test condition for the shod intervention group
modelled using orthogonal linear and quadratic polynomial (Fig. 2).
contrasts, where the linear components model the slope of
the regression curves (mean learning curves) and the quad- Balance learning curves
ratic components their curvatures. The degree of the poly-
nomials was determined using a forward model selection The estimation results for balance learning differences
process. To account for the individual effects in the repeated between both intervention groups are shown in Table 3.
measurements design, we used a mixed effects regression Mean curves significantly differ in their slope coefficients
model with Gaussian mean zero random intercept per par- of the linear learning effects (p = 0.033). As seen in Fig. 3,
ticipant and interaction effects between intervention groups the barefoot group initially showed better balance skills on
and each component of the regression curves. The following the stabilometer than the shod group while the shod group
model parameters were used: ßF,C = intercept for the shod catches up over the first three training sessions. There were
group; ß∆B,C = differential intercept for the barefoot group no significant differences between the intervention groups
compared to the shod group; ßF,L = slope coefficient for the in each exercise session.
linear learning effect in the shod group; ß∆B,L = differential
slope coefficient for the linear learning effect in the barefoot Transfer effects to other balance skills
group compared to the shod group; ßF,Q = slope coefficient
for the quadratic learning effect in the shod group; ß∆B,Q = No group effects were found for pre (barefoot group:
differential slope coefficient for the quadratic learning effect 16.6 ± 5.9; shod group: 13.9 ± 4.2; controls: 15.6 ± 5.1) vs.
in the barefoot group compared to the shod group. P-val- post-tests (barefoot group: 14.5 ± 5.4; shod group: 12.4 ± 3.3;
ues were adjusted to control the false discovery rate of this controls: 14.1 ± 5.1) differences in the BESS score (Fig. 4).
multiple testing problem (Benjamini and Hochberg 1995).
Statistical analysis was performed using R (version 3.2.3).
13
European Journal of Applied Physiology
Shod testing
Barefoot group 7.52 3.53 5.815–9.217 < 0.001 22.163 0.470
Shod group 9.21 3.43 7.509–10.916
Controls 2.12 2.50 0.789–3.453
Barefoot testing
Barefoot group 8.14 3.46 6.472–9.803 < 0.001 14.638 0.369
Shod group 8.54 3.91 6.600–10.488
Controls 2.77 2.80 1.281–4.268
Fig. 2 Comparisons between groups for the pre- to post-test differences (time in s) and 95% family-wise confidence intervals regarding balance
time on the stabilometer under shod and barefoot testing
Table 3 Estimation results for the fixed effects model components of Discussion
motor learning curve comparison between both intervention groups
Parameter Estimate Std. Error Adj. p value The major finding is that footwear differently influences the
learning of a dynamic balance task over time, but has no
ßF,C 17.801 0.928 < 0.001
impact on the amount of improvements after completion of
ß∆B,C − 0.044 1.359 0.974
the intervention period. The improvements in standing pos-
ßF,L 4.784 0.408 < 0.001
tural control are in accordance with other studies evaluating
ß∆B,L − 1.381 0.597 0.033
the effects of balance exercising in healthy, physically active
ßF,Q − 1.214 0.408 0.007
and young adults (Taubert et al. 2010; Zech et al. 2010;
ß∆B,Q 0.635 0.597 0.347
Lesinki et al. 2015; Hrysomallis 2011). We observed signifi-
F = shod group; B = barefoot group; C = intercept; L = slope coeffi- cant improvements in both intervention groups compared to
cient for the linear learning effect; Q = slope coefficient for the quad- non-trained controls. This is in agreement with Taubert et al.
ratic learning effect; Δ = differential effects compared to the shod
(2010), who used the same dynamic balance task on the sta-
group
bilometer for six training sessions in healthy young adults.
However, the improvements in our study were exclusively
One-way ANOVA showed no significant effects between found in the same balance task that was used for exercis-
groups for the pre- to post-test differences (p = 0.902). ing. No changes were observed in the BESS test. The task-
specificity of balance tasks have previously been reported
(Kümmel et al. 2016; Giboin et al. 2015; Zech et al. 2014).
13
European Journal of Applied Physiology
Fig. 3 Mean learning curves (with 95% confidence intervals) for balance time on the stabilometer in both intervention groups over the seven
training sessions
Considering that standing postural control seems particu- improvements in balance performance should consider the
larly influenced by joint somatosensory information from the use of electromyography, magnetic resonance imaging or
lower legs (Fitzpatrick and McCloskey 1994) we expected other neurophysiology approaches (e.g., peripheral nerve
that balance learning would be influenced by barefoot or stimulation, transcranial magnetic stimulation) to get a bet-
shod conditions. Indeed, we found significantly different ter insight into the adaptation processes.
learning curves between the barefoot vs. shod groups. The Although this indicates that footwear is more benefi-
shod group improved balance control faster, whereas the cial than barefoot situations for learning of a dynamic bal-
barefoot group showed flatter learning curves. A possible ance task several limitations should be taken into account
explanation may be that barefoot activities are more unfa- for interpretation. First, all participants in our study were
miliar or / and provide a greater sense of instability due habitual shod living individuals. The habituation to footwear
to lower stiffness around the ankle or foot or alterations in or barefoot activities influences foot anthropometrics and
muscle activation (Bishop et al. 2006; Shultz et al. 2012; running gait control (Hollander et al. 2017a, 2018). Habitu-
Federolf et al. 2012). However, due to the lack of additional ally barefoot living individuals may, therefore, respond dif-
assessments the attribution of the observed footwear vs. ferently to balance interventions with or without footwear.
barefoot impact to one or multiple underlying physiologi- Second, the effects were shown only for the stabilometer
cal mechanisms remains speculative. In this context, future balance task. Due to the task-specificity of balance abilities
studies investigating the influence of barefoot exercising on (Giboin et al. 2015; Kümmel et al. 2016), other balance task
13
European Journal of Applied Physiology
could be differently influenced by the presence or absence Bell DR, Guskiewicz KM, Clark MA, Padua DA (2011) System-
of footwear. It is also possible that different exercise pro- atic review of the balance error scoring system. Sports Health
3(3):287–295
tocols with longer intervention periods and a higher num- Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y (1995) Controlling the false discovery
ber or frequency of sessions could change the amount of rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R
improvements in the balance task. Third, the participants Stat Soc Ser B Stat Methodol 57(1):289–300
were recruited in a healthy sports student population limit- Bishop M, Fiolkowski P, Conrad B, Brunt D, Horodyski M (2006)
Athletic footwear, leg stiffness, and running kinematics. J Athl
ing the generalizability of findings to other populations. The Train 41(4):387–392
greatest development in balance abilities can be observed Broscheid K, Zech A (2016) Influence of barefoot, minimalist, and
during childhood and adolescent years (Woll et al. 2011). standard footwear conditions on gait and balance in healthy
Furthermore, older individuals particularly benefit from bal- older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 64(2):435–437
Corbin DM, Hart JM, McKeon PO, Ingersoll CD, Hertel J (2007)
ance interventions due to positive effects on fall risk man- The effect of textured insoles on postural control in double and
agement, motor function, cognitive function and quality of single limb stance. J Sport Rehabil 16(4):363–372
life. Barefoot activities in these populations may be more Docherty CL, McLeod TCV, Shultz SJ (2006) Postural control defi-
demanding for motor coordination (Broscheid and Zech cits in participants with functional ankle instability as meas-
ured by the balance error scoring system. Clin J Sport Med
2016) and could differently influence improvements com- 16(3):203–208
pared to the healthy adult population. Nevertheless, our find- Federolf PA, Roos L, Nigg B (2012) The effect of footwear on postural
ings favour the use of footwear for quick improvements in control in bipedal quiet stance. Footwear Sci 4(2):115–122
balance control in healthy, habitually shod living individuals Fitzpatrick R, McCloskey DI (1994) Proprioceptive, visual and ves-
tibular thresholds for the perception of sway during standing in
compared to barefoot situations. However, both conditions humans. J Physiol 478(1):173–186
lead to similar improvements after at least seven exercise Giboin LS, Gruber M, Kramer A (2015) Task-specifity of balance train-
sessions using a single balance task. ing. Hum Mov Sci 44:22–31
Hall JP, Barton C, Jones PR, Morrissey D (2013) The biomechani-
cal differences between barefoot and shod distance running: a
systematic review and preliminary meta-analysis. Sports Med
Conclusions 43(12):1335–1353
Hollander K, Riebe D, Campe S, Braumann KM, Zech A (2014) Effects
of footwear on treadmill running biomechanics in preadolescent
In conclusion, balance improvements after a 7-week exercise children. Gait Posture 40(3):381–385
period did not differ between individuals exercising bare- Hollander K, Argubi-Wollesen A, Reer R, Zech A (2015) Comparison
foot or shod. However, the progression of improvements of minimalist footwear strategies for simulating barefoot running:
a randomized crossover study. PLoS One 10(5):e0125880
was slower in the barefoot group. The specific mechanism Hollander K, de Villiers JE, Sehner S, Wegscheider K, Braumann KM,
contributing to this effect and dimensions of adaptations dur- Venter R, Zech A (2017a) Growing-up (habitually) barefoot influ-
ing learning should be further investigated using different ences the development of foot and arch morphology in children
populations with or without physical impairments as well and adolescents. Sci Rep 7(1):8079
Hollander K, Heidt C, van der Zwaard BC, Braumann KM, Zech A
as various balance tasks for exercising. (2017b) Long-term effects of habitual barefoot running and walk-
ing: a systematic review. Med Sci Sports Exerc 49(4):752–762
Acknowledgements We would like to thank Dr. Simon Steib for help- Hollander K, de Villiers JE, Venter R, Sehner S, Wegscheider K, Brau-
ing with the Stabilometer pictures. The study was funded by the Min- mann KM, Zech A (2018) Foot strike patterns differ between chil-
istry for Science and Research in Hamburg, Germany (Grant number dren and adolescents growing up barefoot vs. shod. Int J Sports
LFF-FV13). Med 39(2):97–103
Horak FB, Nashner LM, Diener HC (1990) Postural strategies asso-
Author contributions AZ, KaHo and KM conceived and designed ciated with somatosensory and vestibular loss. Exp Brain Res
research. SM and KaHo conducted experiments. DL performed the 82(1):167–177
statistical analysis. SM, KaHo and KiHo analyzed data. AZ wrote the Horak FB, Henry SM, Shumway-Cook A (1997) Postural perturba-
manuscript. All authors read and approved the manuscript. tions: new insights for treatment of balance disorders. Phys Ther
77(5):517–533
Hrysomallis C (2011) Balance ability and athletic performance. Sports
Compliance with ethical standards Med 41(3):221–232
Kümmel A, Kramer A, Giboin LS, Gruber M (2016) Specificity of
Conflict of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of balance training in healthy individuals: a systematic review and
interest related to the publication of this article. meta-analysis. Sports Med 46:1261–1271
Kurup HV, Clark CI, Dega RK (2012) Footwear and orthopaedics. Foot
Ankle Surg 18(2):79–83
Lesinki M, Hortobágyi T, Muehlbauer T, Gollhofer A, Granacher U
References (2015) Dose–response relationships of balance training in healthy
young adults: a systematic review an meta-analysis. Sports Med
Altman AR, Davis IS (2016) Prospective comparison of running 45(5):557–576
injuries between shod and barefoot runners. Br J Sports Med Lieberman DE, Venkadesan M, Werbel WA, Daoud AI, D’Andrea S,
50(8):476–480. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2014-094482 Davis IS, Mang’eni RO, Pitsiladis Y (2010) Foot strike patterns
13
European Journal of Applied Physiology
and collision forces in habitually barefoot versus shod runners. Taubert M, Draganski B, Anwander A, Müller K, Horstmann A,
Nature 463(7280):531–535 Villringer A, Ragert P (2010) Dynamic properties of human brain
Maurer C, Mergner T, Bolha B, Hlavacka F (2001) Human balance structure: learning-related changes in cortical areas and associated
control during cutaneous stimulation of the plantar soles. Neurosci fiber connections. J Neurosci 30(35):11670–11677
Lett 302(1):45–48 Woll A, Kurth BM, Opper E, Worth A, Bos K (2011) The ‘Motorik-
Mildren RL, Bent LR (2016) Vibrotactile stimulation of fast-adapting Modul’ (MoMo): physical fitness and physical activity in German
cutaneous afferents from the foot modulates proprioception at the children and adolescents. Eur J Pediatr 170(9):1129–1142
ankle joint. J Appl Physiol (1985) 120(8):855–864 Zech A, Hübscher M, Vogt L, Banzer W, Hänsel F, Pfeifer K (2010)
Nigg BM, Baltich J, Hoerzer S, Enders H (2015) Running shoes and Balance training for neuromuscular control and performance
running injuries: mythbusting and a proposal for two new para- enhancement: a systematic review. J Athl Train 45(4):392–403
digms: ‘preferred movement path’ and ‘comfort filter’. Br J Sports Zech A, Klahn P, Hoeft J, Eulenburg C, Steib S (2014) Time course
Med 49(20):1290–1294 and dimensions of postural control changes following neuromus-
Rao UB, Joseph B (1992) The influence of footwear on the preva- cular training in youth field hockey athletes. Eur J Appl Physiol
lence of flat foot. A survey of 2300 children. J Bone Joint Surg 114:395–403
Br 74(4):525–527 Zech A, Argubi-Wollesen A, Rahlf AL (2015) Minimalist, standard
Rogge AK, Röder B, Zech A, Nagel V, Hollander K, Braumann KM, and no footwear on static and dynamic postural stability following
Hötting K (2017) Balance training improves memory and spatial jump landing. Eur J Sport Sci 15(4):279–285
cognition in healthy adults. Sci Rep 7:5661 Zech A, Venter R, de Villiers JE, Sehner S, Wegscheider K, Hollander
Shultz SJ, Schmitz RJ, Tritsch AJ, Montgomery MM (2012) Methodo- K (2018) Motor skills of children and adolescents are influenced
logical considerations of task and shoe wear on joint energetics by growing up barefoot or shod. Front Pediatr 6:115
during landing. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 22(1):124–130
13