Neurobiology of Dementia
Neurobiology of Dementia
REVIEW OF
Neurobiology
Volume 84
SERIES EDITORS
RONALD J. BRADLEY
Department of Psychiatry, College of Medicine
The University of Tennessee Health Science Center
Memphis, Tennessee, USA
R. ADRON HARRIS
Waggoner Center for Alcohol and Drug Addiction Research
The University of Texas at Austin
Austin, Texas, USA
PETER JENNER
Division of Pharmacology and Therapeutics
GKT School of Biomedical Sciences
King’s College, London, UK
EDITORIAL BOARD
The appearance of the code at the bottom of the first page of a chapter in this book
indicates the Publisher’s consent that copies of the chapter may be made for
personal or internal use of specific clients. This consent is given on the condition,
however, that the copier pay the stated per copy fee through the Copyright Clearance
Center, Inc. (www.copyright.com), for copying beyond that permitted by
Sections 107 or 108 of the U.S. Copyright Law. This consent does not extend to
other kinds of copying, such as copying for general distribution, for advertising
or promotional purposes, for creating new collective works, or for resale.
Copy fees for pre-2007 chapters are as shown on the title pages. If no fee code
appears on the title page, the copy fee is the same as for current chapters.
0074-7742/2007 $35.00
Permissions may be sought directly from Elsevier’s Science & Technology Rights
Department in Oxford, UK: phone: (þ44) 1865 843830, fax: (þ44) 1865 853333,
E-mail: [email protected]. You may also complete your request on-line
via the Elsevier homepage (http://elsevier.com), by selecting ‘‘Support & Contact’’
then ‘‘Copyright and Permission’’ and then ‘‘Obtaining Permissions.’’
ISBN-13: 978-0-12-374833-1
PRINTED AND BOUND IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA
08 09 10 11 12 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
CONTRIBUTORS
Numbers in parentheses indicate the pages on which the authors’ contributions begin.
Katherine J. Bangen (81), Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine,
University of California, San Diego 92093, USA
Yvonne Bannon (1), Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Medicine,
University of South Florida, 3515 East Fletcher Avenue, MDC-14, Tampa,
Florida 33613, USA
Lars Bertram (167), Neuropsychiatric Genetics Group, Department of
Vertebrate Genomics, Max-Planck Institute for Molecular Genetics, Berlin
14195, Germany; and Genetics and Aging Research Unit, Department of
Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, Massachusetts,
02129, USA
Mona K. Beyer (49), The Norwegian Centre for Movement Disorders; and
Department of Radiology, Stavanger University Hospital, Stavanger, Norway
Mark W. Bondi (81), Veterans AVairs San Diego Healthcare System, San
Diego 92161, USA; and Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine,
University of California, San Diego 92093, USA
Aimee Borazanci (245), Department of Neurology, Louisiana State University
School of Medicine-Shreveport, Shreveport, Louisiana 71103, USA
Andrea C. Bozoki (185), Department of Neurology, Michigan State University,
East Lansing, Michigan, 48824, USA
Maria T. Caserta (1), Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Medicine,
University of South Florida, 3515 East Fletcher Avenue, MDC-14, Tampa,
Florida 33613, USA
Jody Corey-Bloom (81), Department of Neurosciences, School of Medicine,
University of California, San Diego 92093, USA; and Veterans AVairs San
Diego Healthcare System, San Diego 92161, USA
Turi O. Dalaker (49), The Norwegian Centre for Movement Disorders,
Stavanger University Hospital, Stavanger, Norway; and Department of
Neurology, BuValo Neuroimaging Analysis Center, State University of
New York at BuValo, BuValo, New York 14203, USA
Lisa Delano-Wood (81), Veterans AVairs San Diego Healthcare System,
San Diego 92161, USA; and Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine,
University of California, San Diego 92093, USA
xi
xii CONTRIBUTORS
xv
xvi PREFACE
Next, Lippa and Hanson give an update on Lewy body dementia. This extensive
review is followed by Robottom and Weiner’s chapter on the dementia in Parkin-
son’s disease, presenting readers with the most recent findings on this subject.
The final section of this issue of the International Review of Neurobiology concen-
trates on important subjects which we as neurologists encounter on a daily
basis. First, Minagar and colleagues discuss various causes of dementia in younger
individuals and present the readers with a systematic approach for the diagnosis
of this uncommon presentation. Then, Glen Finney and Anahid Kabasakalian
discuss the pathogenesis and clinical features, as well as the diagnostic procedures
for investigating normal pressure hydrocephalus and other reversible dementias.
In summary, various contributors to this issue of the International Review of
Neurobiology have attempted to improve the readers’ knowledge concerning a
broad spectrum of dementing disorders. Each contribution provides the inquisi-
tive reader with the latest developments in our understanding of that particular
topic. The editor and the very knowledgeable contributors to this issue hope
that this volume of the International Review of Neurobiology stimulates the readers’
scientific curiosity and promotes basic and clinical research in the dementia.
Finally, I would like to acknowledge the excellent contributions of the various
authors to this issue of the International Review of Neurobiology. Without their effort
and dedication, compiling this volume would have never become a reality.
I would also like to thank Ms. Narmada Thangavelu and Mr. Charles Prem
Kumar Neelakandan from Elsevier for their time and effort expended to process
and complete on this of the journal.
ALIREZA MINAGAR
NORMAL BRAIN AGING: CLINICAL, IMMUNOLOGICAL,
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL, AND NEUROIMAGING FEATURES
I. Introduction
II. Epidemiology of Aging: Risks and Implications
III. Normal Aging and Functional Performance
A. Quality of Life and Protective Factors
B. Centenarians
IV. T- and B-Lymphocyte Loss of Function in the Aging Immune System
V. Neuropsychology of Aging
A. Changes in Neuropsychological Function in Normal Aging
B. Staving off Cognitive Decline
VI. Imaging Studies in Aging
References
quite preserved even to old age. Factors identified for healthy cognitive (brain)
aging are multifactorial, and likely incorporate biological systems as well as
cognitive reserve.
I. Introduction
The percentage of the population over age 65 is growing rapidly in the United
States. In 2000, it was 12.4% and in 2050 it is projected to be 20.6% (Health,
United States, 2005). Many people in this older age group are healthy but it is well
known that functional disability increases with age. There are many theories
of aging including ‘‘programmed aging’’ and the ‘‘wear and tear’’ theories.
The former regards aging as being regulated by an internal clock and is seen as
an extension of development, which is highly regulated and timed process. How-
ever, the interplay of disease, environment and genetics is evident not only in
epidemiological studies of aging and longevity, but also in functional assessments of
the elderly, and in such concepts as cognitive reserve and neural compensation.
Structural and functional imaging studies of intact older individuals have
delineated specific areas where there is loss of brain volume and also a reduction
in the lateralization of brain activity during cognitive performance in elderly
individuals. However, not all of these changes are detrimental to maintaining
performance levels and it is not yet clear which changes are precursors to further
cognitive decline and which may be true compensatory mechanisms.
Through the year 2000, approximately 35 million of people living in the USA
were over 65 years. It is estimated that by 2050, this number will more than
double to 82 million with 20% of the US population age 65 years or older (see
Table I; Ferrucci et al., 2008). With the life expectancy in old age increasing, and
death rates reported to decrease, these numbers may be an underestimate of what
is to come (Minino et al., 2007). While older adults are generally healthy and
independent and are active in various roles contributing significantly to their
families and society, the increase in concurrent medical conditions causing excess
morbidity and mortality as well as disability and decline in functional per-
formance negatively impact a significant number of the elderly (Ferrucci et al.,
2008). Health status in the elderly is a function of the negative consequences
and impairment in functional performance caused by medical co-morbidities.
CHARACTERISTICS OF NORMAL BRAIN AGING 3
TABLE I
ACTUAL AND PROJECTED GROWTH OF THE OLDER US POPULATION, 1900–2050 (MILLIONS)
Year Total population (all ages) Number Percent of total Number Percent of >65 years
Note : This table from Ferrucci et al. (2008) is used with permission.
Any and all diseases occurring in the elderly cause excess disability and decre-
ments in function in a variety of domains but mostly in independent function. For
example, chronic health conditions such as hypertension, hyperglycemia, muscu-
loskeletal disorders, coronary artery disease, cancer, and mixed states of anxiety
and depression aVect a significant number of the elderly (see Table II). By race
and ethnicity these numbers vary among minority men and women when com-
pared with that in White men and women. The proportion of these with cognitive
impairment and dementing illness is also rising. Similarly, preexisting anxiety and
depression may increase the risk of developing cognitive impairment (Backman,
2008; Loebach et al., 2002).
In large epidemiologic studies such as the Canadian Study of Health and
Aging, the authors evaluated the occurrence of an adverse event (symptom, sign,
or disease), or the accumulation of a number of such co-morbid events, and
modeled the events as a logistic function of chronologic age in a population
(Graham et al., 1999). In those who were cognitively normal, a linear relation
between the log of the odds of events and chronologic age was present for the
majority of symptoms and signs. Thus, the dynamics of aging are a complex
process of accumulation of deficits (morbidity), whereby decline from some
previously healthy level of synergistically associated symptoms and signs results
in distinct patterns of disease.
But these studies are only part of the larger picture. It is a familiar epidemio-
logic concept that individuals vary not only in whether they develop a disease but
also in the ages at which disease occurs and the rates at which pre-morbid changes
progress to any specific disease. Available reports of genetic epidemiologic studies
on diseases of aging are adding a new dimension—trait genomics—to assess the
presence or absence of a disease (or occurrence of an adverse event) or evaluating
the level of a given risk factor at one point in time (NIA Aging and Genetic
Epidemiology Working Group, 2000). Moreover, two very specific types of age-
specific traits are survival traits (the age at which a specified outcome has occurred
or has not occurred) and rate-of-change traits (the rate at which individuals’
4 CASERTA et al.
TABLE II
a
AGE-ADJUSTED AND AGE-SPECIFIC MORTALITY IN THE UNITED STATES, 1950 AND 2004 AND
PERCENT CHANGE
Note : This table from Ferrucci et al. (2008) is used with permission.
a
Data for ‘‘all ages’’ are age-adjusted using the US 2000 standard population.
The biophysiological changes that occur as part of normal aging can impact
and limit functional performance in the elderly. The extent of impact to functional
performance in the elderly is dependant on numerous factors, including cognitive
status, physical disability, and medical illness (Njegovan et al., 2001), as well as,
emotional factors, quality of life, and numerous protective factors (Vaillant, 2002).
The concept of expressing functional performance in terms of a person’s ability
to carry out routine activities of daily living (ADLs) was first introduced by Katz
et al. (1963) and expanded by Lawton and Body (1969) to include more complex
functions called instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs). ADLs include
basic self-care activities such as bathing, dressing, eating, transferring, ambulation,
and toileting: IADLs include shopping, managing finances, cooking, cleaning, and
telephone use. The US Centers for Disease Control database tracks changes in
functional performance in the elderly.
The association between cognitive impairment and functional performance
has been studied extensively. A number of studies have demonstrated a relation-
ship between cognitive status and functional performance independent of social,
demographic, or medical factors (Hertzog and Wallace, 1997; Moritz et al., 1995;
Royall et al., 2005). Predicting and measuring change in ADLs and IADLs with
regards to advancing age has been the focus of many studies. Katz (1965)
hypothesized that older persons with progressive cognitive decline lost ability to
perform ADLs in the opposite order to which the skill(s) were acquired in
childhood. Additionally, functional performance of IADLs was more likely to
deteriorate prior to ADLs during the course of cognitive decline associated with
pathological process. Although many studies have looked at changes in functional
performance, and more specifically cognitive decline in the elderly with a disease
process, few studies have looked at the correlation between normal physiological
cerebral aging and cognitive decline (see below).
6 CASERTA et al.
The term ‘‘successful aging’’ first appeared in medical journals in 1961 (Motta
et al., 2005). Today the term is used to describe the absence of significant disease
and disabilities, maintenance of high levels of physical and cognitive function, and
the preservation of social and productive activities (Motta et al., 2005). The
Harvard Study of Adult Development is considered the landmark study of adult
development and has provided key predictors to ‘‘successful aging.’’ This research
followed 824 individuals from three separate prospective, longitudinal studies,
selected as teenagers from various facets of mental and physical health, through-
out their lives. The three groups consisted of 268 Harvard sophomores, a sample
of 456 Inner City youths, and 90 women from Stanford—all selected as teenagers
and followed throughout their lives. The research focused on (1) adaptation to
stress, mental health, and defense mechanisms; (2) the eVects of habits—especially
alcoholism and aVective disorders upon physical health and mortality; (3) the
eVect of childhood risk factors upon adult adaptation; (4) the unfolding of adult
development; and (5) the natural history of alcohol and substance abuse (Valliant,
2002). Although the findings are numerous, key findings suggest the following
factors predictive of healthy or ‘‘successful aging’’: being a non-smokers; adaptive
coping styles, mature defense styles, and optimism; absences of alcohol abuse;
maintaining a healthy weight; stable marriage or relationships; some physical
activity; social engagement; and education. Education, including attainment and
childhood intellectual ability, is also thought to provide a cognitive reserve in later
life (Whalley et al., 2004), protecting from cognitive decline associated with normal
aging, as well as, cognitive decline associated to disease process.
B. CENTENARIANS
The number of people living beyond the age of 100 is growing throughout the
industrialized world (Motta et al., 2005). Several studies of centenarians have been
conducted to determine their level of independence, clinical condition, and
CHARACTERISTICS OF NORMAL BRAIN AGING 7
cerebral deterioration and whether centenarians are the prototype for ‘‘successful
aging.’’ The Italian Multicenter Study on Centenarians (IMUSCE) is one such
study (Motta et al., 2008). The IMUSCE was an epidemiological study which
identified 1173 centenarians ranging in age from 100 to 109 years. A sub-sample
of 346 centenarians was studied to determine the functional performance and
cognitive status. The sub-sample was further divided into groups based on physical
health and MMSE scores. True independence in all functional performance
(IADLs) was seen in only six centenarians (1.7%): while 21 centenarians (6.1%)
presented with slight dependence in functional performance. Additionally, 68
centenarians (19.67%) had only a moderate dependency in IADLs and 251
(72.6%) were severely dependent. Of the six independent functioning centenarians
and the 21 centenarians with only slight dependence in IADLs, all have had
significant changes in their level of social and productive activities, thus they cannot
be considered prototypes of successful aging based on the current definition.
functional capability (EVros et al., 2005; Fann et al., 2005; Pawelec, 2005;
Tarazona et al., 2000). Further, it seems that clonal expansion of CD28!CD8þ
T-cells seems directly responsible for increased infection rates and the common
failed response to vaccines in the elderly (Almanzar et al., 2005). Decreased
adaptive immunity also involves changes in the B-cell repertoire not unlike
those observed in the T-cell pool (Ghia et al., 2000; Szbo et al., 1999; Weksler,
2000). The quality of the humoral immune response is decreased with age owing
to low serum immunoglobulin concentrations and low numbers of antigen-
specific, immunoglobulin-secreting plasma cells. Further, antibody specificity,
isotype and aYnity changes are typical features of old age. For example, immu-
noglobulins produced in aged mice have lower aYnity and are less protective
versus those of young animals (Yang et al., 1996). This may be secondary to aging’s
adverse eVects on the germinal centre reaction in secondary lymphoid tissues
(Zheng et al., 1997) and leads to a diminished quantity of germinal centers in
response to tetanus toxoid stimulation (Kraft et al., 1987). The quantity of B
cells also decreases in the elderly (Franchesci et al., 1995). At the cellular level,
alteration in immunoglobulin generation (through class switching) in B cells is
observed in aged individuals (Frasca et al., 2005), which may contribute to the
decline of the adaptive immune response in the elderly.
V. Neuropsychology of Aging
It has long been recognized that some cognitive processes decline with age
while others appear less aVected, giving rise to the traditional dichotomy between
the so-called ‘‘crystallized’’ abilities (e.g., accumulated knowledge and expertise)
versus those termed ‘‘fluid’’ abilities (e.g., fluid reasoning, working memory,
visuoperceptual abilities, processing speed, etc.) (e.g., Craik and Salthouse,
2000; WoodruV-Pak, 1997 for review). Within this framework, crystallized abil-
ities have been considered generally stable with increasing age, while fluid abilities
decline with increasing age, some thought to begin declining as early as in one’s
20s (Babcock, 1930; Craik and Salthouse, 2000; Lezak, 1995; Wechsler, 1958;
WoodruV-Pak, 1997 for review). Until recently, however, much of the previous
knowledge regarding age-related cognitive decline have been criticized for lack of
longitudinal data, with most derived from cross-sectional studies or other studies
having small samples and/or short follow-up time frames (e.g., Baltes and Mayer,
1999; Hertzog et al., 2003; Schaie, 1996).
Changes in normal aging are now being understood from longitudinal data
from several sources, including the Berlin Aging Study (Baltes and Mayer, 1999),
Mayo’s Older Americans Normative Studies (MOANS; Ivnik et al., 1992), Seattle
Longitudinal Study (Schaie, 1996), and Victoria Longitudinal Study (Dixon and
de Frias, 2004; Hertzog et al., 2003). Combined, these studies provide longitudinal
data of individuals beginning in participants’ 20s up to 100 years old and beyond
(e.g., Singer et al., 2003). While data are incomplete, one consistent finding has
been a decline in select cognitive domains beginning as early as the middle to late
20s. Alternatively, other cognitive functions remain quite preserved even to very
old age (Singer et al., 2003). Schaie (1994) found the average performances of
arithmetic/numerical ability and perceptual speed beginning to decline at around
age 25 years old. Alternatively, a general decline in performance on tasks of
inductive reasoning, verbal ability, and episodic memory did not become apparent
until the fifth or sixth decade of life. While episodic memory is known to decrease
with age, some research suggesting as early as the third or fourth decade of life,
other research suggests the decline may be much less precipitous and is gradual, at
least until the seventh decade of life (Backman et al., 2000). Similarly, Singer et al.
(2003) found performance on tasks of processing speed, episodic memory, and
verbal fluency declined from 70 to 100 years old. Alternatively, word knowledge
has consistently demonstrated resilience to aging, remaining quite stable from
one’s 70s through 100þ years old (e.g., Lezak, 1995; Singer et al., 2003). Executive
functions (rapid problem solving, verbal fluency, inhibition, and flexibility) show
early and considerable age-related declines (Mittenberg et al., 1989; Parkin, 1996).
Indeed, Mittenberg et al. (1989) found performance on neuropsychological tests of
executive skills, thought to measure frontal lobe functioning, exhibited more age-
related decline than other tests in a comprehensive battery. Further complicating
CHARACTERISTICS OF NORMAL BRAIN AGING 11
The changes that occur with aging in the brain are complex and are asso-
ciated with large interindividual variability. There is, however, a pattern of
selective loss and preservation delineated in numerous studies. The structural
12 CASERTA et al.
from older individuals performing the same task at the same level (Stern et al.,
2005). These authors suggest that cognitive reserve consists of two separate
components: neural reserve and neural compensation. Neural reserve may result
from innate diVerences and/or be modulated through life events such as educa-
tional experience. The substrate for this may be neural networks that are highly
eYcient or have greater capacity in the face of higher demand. Neural compen-
sation on the other hand is defined as a change in neural network use such that
diVerent brain networks are used in a specific task, due to the physiological eVects
of aging or brain pathology. The study of the neural substrates of cognitive reserve
is in its infancy, but imaging techniques assessing both functional and structural
changes in the brains of older adults are progressing rapidly.
References
Allman, D., and Miller, J. P. (2005). B cell development and receptor diversity during aging. Curr. Opin.
Immunol. 17, 463–467.
Almanzar, G., Schwaiger, S., Jenewein, B., Keller, M., Herndler-Brandstetter, D., Wurzner, R.,
Schonitzer, D., Grubeck-Lobenstein, B., and Almanzar, G. (2005). Long-term cytomegalovirus
infection leads to significant changes in the composition of the CD8þ T-cell repertoire, which
may be the basis for an imbalance in the cytokine production profile in elderly persons. J. Virol.
79, 3675–3683.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). ‘‘Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,’’
4th Ed. text revision ed. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.
Anderson, N. D., Iidaka, T., McIntosh, A. R., Kapur, S., Cabeza, R., and Craik, F. I. M. (2000). The
eVects of divided attention on encoding- and retrieval related brain activity. A PET study of
younger and older adults. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 775–792.
Aspinall, R. (2000). Longevity and the immune response. Biogerontology 1, 273–278.
Aspinall, R., and Andrew, D. (2000). Immunosenescence: Potential causes and strategies for reversal.
Biochem. Soc. Trans. 28, 250–254.
Azuma, M., Phillips, J. H., and Lanier, L. L. (1993). CD28- T lymphocytes antigenic and functional
properties. J. Immunol. 150, 1147–1159.
Babcock, H. (1930). An experiment in the measurement of mental deterioration. Arch. Psych. 18,
5–105.
Backamn, L., Small, B. J., Wahlin, A., and Larsson, M. (2000). Cognitive functioning in very old age.
In ‘‘The handbook of aging and cognition’’ (F. I. M. Craik and T. A. Salthouse, Eds.), 2nd,
pp. 499–558. Laurence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.
Backman, L. (2008). Memory and cognition in preclinical dementia: What we know and what we do
not know. Can. J. Psych. 53(6), 354–360.
Baltes, P. B., and Mayer, K. U. (1999). ‘‘The Berlin. Aging Study: Aging from 70 to 100.’’ Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge.
Bartzokis, G., Beckson, M., Lu, P. H., Neuchertlein, K. H., Edwards, N., and Mintz, J. (2001). Age
related changes in temporal lobe volumes in men: A magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch. Gen.
Psychiatry 58, 461–465.
CHARACTERISTICS OF NORMAL BRAIN AGING 15
Beason-Held, L. L., Kraut, M. A., and Resnick, S. M. (2008). Longitudinal changes in aging brain
function. Neurobiol. Aging 29, 483–496.
Blatter, D. D., Bigler, E. D., Gale, S. D., et al. (1995). Quantitative volumetric analysis of brain MR:
Normative database spanning 5 decades of life. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 16, 241–251.
Brickman, A. M., Buchsbaum, M. S., Shihabuddin, L., et al. (2005). Age associated change in orbital,
cingulate, and dorsolateral frontal lobe gray and white matter volume [abstract]. J. Int. Neuropsy-
chol. Soc. 11, 176.
Boone, K. B., Miller, B. L., Lesser, I. M., Mehringer, C. M., Hill-Gutierrez, E., Goldberg, M. A., and
Berman, N. G. (1992). Neuropsychological correlates of white-matter lesions in healthy elderly
subjects. A threshold eVect. Arch. Neurol. 49, 549–554.
Cabeza, R., Grady, C. L., Nyberg, L., McIntosh, A. R., Tulving, E., Kapus, S., Jennings, J. M.,
Houle, S., and Craik, F. I. M. (1997). Age-related diVerences in neural activity during memory
encoding and retrieval. A positron emission tomography study. J. Neurosci. 17, 391–400.
Cabeza, R., Anderson, N. D., Locantore, J. K., and McIntosh, A. R. (2002). Aging gracefully:
Compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. NeuroImage 17, 1394–1402.
Cossarizza, A., Ortolani, C., Paganelli, R., Barbieri, D., Monti, D., Sansoni, P., Fagiolo, U.,
Castellani, G., Bersani, F., Londei, M., Franceschi, C., Cossarizza, A., et al. (1996), CD45 isoforms
expression on CD4þ and CD8þ T cells throughout life, from newborns to centenarians:
Implications for T cell memory. Mech. Ageing Dev. 86, 173–195.
Craik, F. I. M., and Salthouse, T. A. (2000). ‘‘The Handbook of Aging and Cognition,’’ 2nd Ed.
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. Publishers, Mahwah, NJ.
Crook, T., Bartus, R. T., Ferris, S. H., Whitehouse, P., Cohen, G. D., and Gershon, S. (1986). Age-
associated memory impairment: Proposed diagnostic criteria and measures of clinical change.
Report of a National Institute of Mental Health. Work. Group. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2, 261–276.
DeCarli, C., Murphy, D. G., Gillette, J. A., et al. (1994). Lack of age-related diVerences in temporal
lobe volume of very healthy adults. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 15, 689–696.
DeCarli, C., Murphy, D. G. M., Tran, M., Grady, C. L., Haxby, J. V., Gillette, J. A., Salerno, J. A.,
Gonzales-Aviles, A., Horwitz, B., Rapoport, S. I., and Schapiro, M. B. (1995). The eVect of white
matter hyperintensity volume on brain structure, cognitive performance and cerebral metabolism
in 51 healthy adults. Neurology 45, 2077–2084.
Dixon, R. A., and de Frias, C. M. (2004). The Victoria Longitudinal Study: From characterizing
cognitive aging to illustrating changes in memory compensation. Aging Neuropsych. Cogn. 11,
346–376.
Dixon, R. A., de Frias, C. M., and Bäckman, L. (2001). Characteristics of self-reported memory
compensation in late life. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 23, 650–661.
Dixon, R. A., Wahlin, A., Maitland, S. B., Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., and Backman, L. (2004).
Episodic memory change in late adulthood: Generalizability across samples and performance.
Mem. Cogn. 32(5), 768–778.
EVros, R. B., Dagarag, M., Spaulding, C., and Man, J. (2005). The role of CD8þ T-cell replicative
senescence in human aging. Immunol. Rev. 205, 147–157.
Ferrucci, L., Giallauria, F., and Guralnik, J. M. (2008). Epidemiology of aging. Radiol. Clin. N. Am. 46,
643–652.
Franceschi, C., Capri, M., Monti, D., Giunta, S., Olivieri, F., Sevini, F., Panourgia, M. P., Invidia, L.,
Celani, L., Scurti, M., Cevenini, E., Castellani, G. C., et al. (2007). Inflammaging and anti-
inflammaging: A systemic perspective on aging and longevity emerged from studies in humans.
Mech. Ageing Dev. 128, 92–105.
Franceschi, C., and Cossarizza, A. (1995). Introduction: The reshaping of the immune system with
age. Int. Rev. Immunol. 12, 1–4.
16 CASERTA et al.
Frasca, D., Riley, R. L., and Blomberg, B. B. (2005). Humoral immune response and B-cell functions
including immunoglobulin class switch are downregulated in aged mice and humans. Semin.
Immunol. 17, 378–384.
Ghia, P., Melchers, F., and Rolink, A. G. (2000). Age-dependent changes in B lymphocyte develop-
ment in man and mouse. Exp. Gerontol. 35, 159–165.
Gold, D. P., Andreas, D., Etezadi, J., Arbuckle, T., Schwartman, A., and Chailelson, J. (1995).
Structural equation model of intellectual change and continuity and predictors of intelligence in
older men. Psychol. Aging 10, 294–303.
Goronzy, J. J., and Weyand, C. M. (2005). T cell development and receptor diversity during aging.
Curr. Opin. Immunol. 17, 468–475.
Graham, J. E., Rockwood, K., Beattie, B. L., Eastwood, R., Gauthier, S., Tuokko, H., et al. (1997).
Prevalence and severity of cognitive impairment with and without dementia in an elderly
population. Lancet 349(9068), 1793–1796.
Graham, J. E., Mitnitski, A. B., Mogilner, A. J., and Rockwood, K. (1999). Dynamics of cognitive
aging: Distinguishing functional age and disease from chronologic age in a population.
J. Epidemiol. 150, 1045–1054.
Gutierrez, R., Atkinson, J. H., and Grant, I. (1993). Mild neurocognitive disorder: Needed
addition to the nosology of cognitive impairment (organic mental) disorders. J. Neuropsychiatry
Clin. Neurosci. 5, 161–177.
Guttmann, C. R., Jolesz, F. A., Kikinis, R., et al. (1998). White matter changes with normal aging.
Neurology 50, 972–978.
Haynes, L., Eaton, S. M., Burns, E. M., Randall, T. D., and Swain, S. L. (2005). Newly generated CD4
T cells in aged animals do not exhibit age-related defects in response to antigen. J. Exp. Med. 201,
845–851.
Hertzog, D. J., and Wallace, R. B. (1997). Measures of cognitive functioning in the AHEAD study.
J. Gerontol. Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 52B, 37–48.
Hertzog, C., Dixon, R. A., Hultsch, D. F., and MacDonald, S. W. S. (2003). Latent change models of
adult cognition: Are changes in processing speed and working memory associated with changes in
episodic memory? Psychol. Aging 18, 755–770.
Hughes, C. P., Berg, L., et al. (1982). A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Brit. J. Psychiatry
140, 566–572.
Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Small, B. J., and Dixon, R. A. (1999). Use it or lose it? Engaged life style as
a buffer of cognitive decline in aging. Psychol. Aging 19 14(2), 245–263.
Ivnik, R. J., Malec, J. F., Smith, G. E., Tangalos, E. G., Petersen, R. C., Kokmen, E., et al. (1992).
Mayo’s older Americans normative studies: WAIS-R norms for ages 56 to 97. Clin. Neuropsychol. 6,
1–30.
Ivnik, R. J., Smith, G. E., Lucas, J. A., Petersen, R. C., Boeve, B. F., Kokmen, E., et al. (1999). Testing
normal older people three or four times at 1- to 2-year intervals: Defining normal variance.
Neuropsychology 13(1), 121–127.
Jernigan, T. L., Archibald, S. L., Fenema-Notestine, C., Gamst, A. C., Stout, J. C., Bonner, J., and
Hesselink, J. R. (2001). Effect of age on tissues and regions of cerebrum and cerebellum. Neurobiol.
Aging 22, 581–594.
Kang, I., Hong, M. S., Nolasco, H., Park, S. H., Dan, J. M., Choi, J. Y., and Craft Kang. (2004).
I: Age-associated change in the frequency of memory CD4þ T cells impairs long term CD4þ
T cell responses to influenza vaccine. J. Immunol. 173, 673–681.
Katz, S., Ford, A. B., Moskowitz, R. W., Jackson, B. A., and JaVe, M. W. (1963). The index of ADL:
A standardized measure of biological and psychosocial function. JAMA 185, 914–919.
Keller, B. K., Morton, J. L., Thomas, V. S., and Potter, J. F. (1999). The eVect of visual and hearing
impairments on functional status. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 47, 1319–1325.
CHARACTERISTICS OF NORMAL BRAIN AGING 17
Kovaiou, R. D., Weiskirchner, I., Keller, M., Pfister, G., Cioca, D. P., and Grubeck-Loebensten, B.
(2005). Age-related differences in phenotype and function of CD4þ T cells are due to a pheno-
typic shift from naive to memory effector CD4þ T cells. Int. Immunol. 17, 1359–1366.
Kraft, R., Bachmann, M., Bachmann, K., Buerki, H., Hess, M. W., Cottier, H., and Stoner, R. D.
(1987). Satisfactory primary tetanus antitoxin responses but markedly reduced germinal centre
formation in first draining lymph nodes of ageing mice. Clin. Exp. Immunol. 447–453.
Kral, V. A. (1962). Senescent forgetfulness: Benign and malignant. Can. Med. Assoc. J. 86, 257–260.
Lawton, M. P., and Body, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: Self-maintaining and instrumental
activities of daily living. Gerontologist 9, 179–186.
Levy, R. (1994). Aging-associated cognitive decline. Int. Psychogeriatr. 6, 63–68.
Lezak, M. (1995). Neuropsychological Assessment Third Edition. Oxford University Press, New York.
Linton, P. J., Li, S. P., Zhang, Y., Bautista, B., Huynh, Q., Trinh, T., and Linton, P. J. (2005). Intrinsic
versus environmental influences on T-cell responses in aging. Immunol. Rev. 205, 207–219.
Loebach Wetherell, J., Reynolds, C. A., Gatz, M., and Pederson, N. L. (2002). Anxiety, cognitive
performance, and cognitive decline in normal aging. J. Gerontol. Psychol. Sci. 57B(3), 246–255.
MacDonald, S. W. S., Hultsch, D. F., and Dixon, R. A. (2008). Predicting impending death: Inconsis-
tency in speed is a selective and early marker. Psychol. Aging 23, 595–607.
Milgram, N. W., Siwak-Tapp, C. T., Araujo, J., and Head, E. (2006). Neuroprotective eVects of
cognitive enrichment. Ageing Res. Rev. 5, 354–369.
Minino, A. M., Heron, M., Smith, B. L., et al. (2007). Deaths: Final data for 2004. Nat. Vital Stat. Rep.
55(9), 1–120.
Mittenberg, W., Seidenberg, M., O’Leary, D. S., and DiGiulio, D. V. (1989). Changes in cerebral
functioning associated with normal aging. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 11, 918–932.
Mitchell, D. B. (1993). Implicit and explicit memory for pictures: Multiple views across the lifespan.
In ‘‘Implicit Memory: New directions in Cognition, development, and neuropsychology’’ (P. Graf
and M. E. J. Mason, Eds.), pp. 171–190. Laurence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.
Morris, J. C., and Cummings, J. (2005). Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) represents early-stage
Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 7(3), 235–239; discussion 255-262.
Morris, J. C., Storandt, M., Miller, J. P., McKeel, D. W., Price, J. L., Rubin, E. H., et al. (2001). Mild
cognitive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 58(3), 397–405.
Mortiz, D. J., Kasl, S. V., and Berkman, L. F. (1995). Cognitive functioning and the incidence of
limitations in activities of daily living in an elderly community sample. Am. J. Epidemiol. 141, 41–49.
Motta, M., Bennati, E., Ferlito, L., Malaguarnera, M., and Motta, L. (2005). Successful aging in
centenarians: Myths and reality. Arch. Gerontol. Geriat. 40, 241–251.
Motta, M., Ferlito, L., Magnolfi, S. U., Petruzzi, E., Pinzani, P., Malentacchi, F., Petruzzi, I.,
Bennati, E., and Malaguarnera, M. (2008). Cognitive and functional status in the extreme
longevity. Arch. Gerontol. Geriat. 46, 245–252.
Myerson, J., Hale, S., Wagstaff, D., Poon, L. W., and Smith, G. A. (1990). The information-loss model:
A mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychol. Rev. 97, pp. 475–487.
National Institute on Aging and Genetic Epidemiology Working Group. (2000). http://www.nih.gov/
nia/conferences/GeneticReport111199.htm.
Njegovan, V., Man-Son-Hing, M., Mitchell, S. L., and Molnar, F. J. (2001). The hierarchy of
functional loss associated with cognitive decline in older persons. J. Gerontol. 56A(10),
M638–M643.
Ouyang, Q., Wagner, W. M., Voehringer, D., Wikby, A., Klatt, T., Walter, S., Muller, C. A.,
Pircher, H., and Pawelec, G. (2003a). Age-associated accumulation of CMV-specific CD8þ T
cells expressing the inhibitory killer cell lectin-like receptor G1 (KLRG1). Exp. Gerontol. 38,
911–920.
18 CASERTA et al.
Ouyang, Q., Wanger, W. M., Wikby, A., Walter, S., Aubert, G., Dodi, A. I., Travers, P., Pawelec, G.,
and Ouyan, Q. (2003b). Large numbers of dysfunctional CD8þ T lymphocytes bearing receptors
for a single dominant CMV epitope in the very old. J. Clin. Immunol. 23, 247–257.
Pawelec, G. (2005). When T cells get old. Sci. Aging Knowl. Environ. 2005, 39.
Parkin, A. J. (1996). Explorations in cognitive neuropsychology. Blackwell Publishers, Oxford.
Petersen, R. C. (2003). ‘‘Mild Cognitive Impairment’’. Oxford Press, New York.
Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., and Kokmen, E. (1999).
Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Arch. Neurol. 56(3), 303–308.
Pfefferbaum, A., Mathalon, D. H., Sullivan, E. V., Rawles, J. M., Zipursky, R. B., and Lim, K. O.
(1994). A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from
infancy to late adulthood. Arch. Neurol. 51, 874–887.
Pierpaoli, C., and Basser, P. J. (1996). Towards a quantitative assessment of diVusion anisotropy. Magn.
Reson. Med. 36, 893–906.
Raz, N. (1996). Neuroanatomy of aging brain: Evidence from structural MRI. In ‘‘Neuroimaging II:
Clinical Applications’’ (E. D. Bigler, Ed.), pp. 153–182. Academic Press, New York.
Raz, N. (2000). Aging of the brain and it’s impact on cognitive performance: Integration of structural
and functional findings. In ‘‘The Handbook of Aging and Cognition’’ (F. I. M. Craik and
T. A. Salthouse, Eds.), 2nd, pp. 1–90. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. Publishers,
Mahwah, NJ.
Raz, N., Gunning, F. M., Head, D., et al. (1997). Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex observed
in vivo: DiVerential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. Cereb. Cortex 7, 268–282.
Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., Head, D., and Williamson, A. (2005).
Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: General trends, individual diVerences, and
modifiers. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1676–1689.
Resnick, S. M., Pham, D. L., Kraut, M. A., et al. (2003). Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging
studies of older adults: A shrinking brain. J. Neurosci. 23, 3295–3301.
Royall, D. R., Palmer, R., Chiodo, L. K., and Polk, M. J. (2005). Normal rates of cognitive change in
successful aging: The freedom house study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 11, 899–909.
Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol. Rev.
103, 403–428.
Scarmeas, N., Levy, G., Tang, M. X., Manly, J., and Stern, Y. (2001). Influence of leisure activity on
the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 12, 2236–2242.
Schaie, K. W. (1994). The course of adult intellectual development. Am. Psychol. 49, 304–313.
Schaie, K. W. (1996). Intellectual development in adulthood: The Seattle longitudinal study.
Cambridge University Press, New York.
Shamy, J. L., Buonocore, M. H., Makaron, L. M., Amaral, D. G., Barnes, C. A., and Rapp, P. R.
(2006). Hippocampal volume is preserved and fails to predict recognition memory in aged rhesus
monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Neurobiol. Aging 27, 1405–1415.
Singer, T., Verhaeghen, P., Ghisletta, P., Lindenberger, U., and Baltes, P. B. (2003). The fate of
cognition in very old age: Six-year longitudinal findings in the Berlin Aging Study [BASE]. Psychol.
Aging 18, 318–331.
Stern, Y. (2006). Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 20, 112–117.
Stern, Y., Harbeck, C., Moeller, J., Scarmeas, N., Anderson, K. E., Hiltron, H. J., Flynn, J.,
Sackheim, H., and van Heertum, R. (2005). Brain networks associated with cognitive reserve in
healthy young and old adults. Cerebr. Cortex 15, 394–402.
Sullivan, E. V., and PfeVerbaum, A. (2007). Neuroradiological characterization of normal adult aging.
Brit. J. Radiol. S99–S108.
Sullivan, E. V., Marsh, L., Mathalon, D. H., Lim, K. O., and PfeVerbaum, A. (1995). Age-related
decline in MRI volumes of temporal lobe gray matter but not hippocampus. Neurobiol. Aging 16,
591–606.
CHARACTERISTICS OF NORMAL BRAIN AGING 19
Sullivan, E. V., PfeVerbaum, A., Swan, G. E., and Carmelli, D. (2001). Heritability pf hippocampal
size in elderly twin men, equivalent influence from genes and environment. Hippocampus 11,
754–762.
Sullivan, E. V., Rosenbloom, M., Serventi, K. L., and PfeVerbaum, A. (2004). EVects of age and sex on
volumes of the thalamus, pons, and cortex. Neurobiol. Aging 25, 185–192.
Sullivan, E. V., Marsh, A. L., and PfeVerbaum, A. L. (2005). Preservation of hippocampal volume
through adulthood in healthy men and women. Neurobiol. Aging 26, 1093–1098.
Swain, S., Clise-Dwyer, K., and Haynes, L. (2005). Homeostasis and the age-associated defect of CD4
T cells. Semin. Immunol. 17, 370–377.
Szabo, P., Shen, S., and Weksler, M. E. (1999). Age-associated defects in B lymphocyte development.
Exp. Gerontol. 34, 431–434.
Tarazona, R., DelaRosa, O., Alonso, C., Ostos, B., Espejo, J., Pena, J., and Solana, R. (2000).
Increased expression of NK cell markers on T lymphocytes in aging and chronic activation of
the immune system reflects the accumulation of effector/senescent T cells. Mech. Ageing Dev. 121,
77–88.
Thompson, P. M., Hayashi, K. M., de Zubicaray, G., Scarmeas, N., Levy, G., Tang, M. X., Manly, J.,
and Stern, Y. (2003). Dynamics of gray matter loss in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. 23,
994–1005.
Tisserand, D. J., Pruessner, J. C., Sanz Arigita, E. J., van Boxtel, M. P., Evans, A. C., Jolles, J., and
Uylings, H. B. (2002). Regional frontal cortical volumes decrease diVerentially in aging: An MRI
study to compare volumetric approaches and voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage 17, 657–669.
Trends in Health and Aging Database, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention; National Center for Health Statistics.
Vaillant, G. E. (2002). ‘‘Aging Well: Surprising Guideposts to a Happier Life from the Landmark
Harvard Study of Adult Development.’’ Little, Brown, and Company, Boston.
Wechsler, D. (1958). ‘‘The Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence’’ 4th Ed., Williams &
Wilkins, Baltimore, MD.
Weksler, M. E. (2000). Changes in the B-cell repertoire with age. Vaccine 18, 1624–1628.
Weng, N. P. (2006). Aging of the immune system: How much can the adaptive immune system adapt?
Immunity 24, 495–499.
Whalley, L. J., Deary, I. J., Appleton, C. L., and Starr, J. M. (2004). Cognitive reserve and the
neurobiology of cognitive aging. Ageing Res. Rev. 3, 369–382.
WoodruV-Pak, D. S. (1997). ‘‘The Neuropsychology of Aging’’. Blackwell Publishers Inc., Maiden,
MA.
World Health Organization (1978). Mental disorders: Glossary and guide to their classification in
accordance with the ninth revision of the International Classification of Diseases. Geneva,
Switzerland.
Yang, X., Stedra, J., and Cerny, J. (1996). Relative contribution of T and B cells to hypermutation and
selection of the antibody repertoire in germinal centers of aged mice. J. Exp. Med. 183, 959–970.
Zheng, B., Han, S., Takahashi, Y., and Kelsoe, G. (1997). Immunosenescence and germinal center
reaction. Immunol. Rev. 160, 63–77.
SUBCORTICAL ISCHEMIC CEREBROVASCULAR DEMENTIA
I. Terminology
II. Various Classifications of ‘‘Vascular’’ Dementia
III. Cognition and Aging
IV. Subcortical Strokes and Cognitive Impairment
V. Pathophysiology of Cognitive Impairment in Subcortical Vascular Lesions
VI. Treatment and Prevention
References
It has become increasingly apparent, especially with the advent of MRI brain
scanning, that a large number of patients develop signal intensity changes in the
subcortical white matter and periventricular region as they age. This appears to
be accelerated by risk factors for small vessel cerebrovascular disease such as
hypertension, smoking, diabetes mellitus and hyperlipidemia. The major question
becomes when such changes become clinically significant. It is obvious that
subcortical lacunar-type infarction can be identified by the clinical presentation.
For example, typical examples of so-called ‘‘lacunar syndrome’’ include pure
motor hemiparesis, pure sensory stroke, sensorimotor stroke, clumsy hand-
dysarthria, and hemiataxia-hemiparesis. The issue becomes a measure of impact
on functional ability. This is influenced by several factors. Baseline IQ and
educational level, as well as expectations of age, certainly play a role. A person
who develops cognitive impairment and long tract signs in their 50s or 60s is
certainly going to be recognized as more impaired than an 80 year old individual
who is retired and primarily is engaged in recreational activity. It would be
expected that a person born with limited intellectual capacity and/or limited
educational opportunity would be less likely to be identified as impaired than
a person who has achieved substantial economic achievement through their
innate talents.
The concept of tissue loss or lesion load becomes important when determining
how pronounced the ischemic cerebrovascular changes translate into functional
impairment. Correlative pathology may include cortical atrophy and ventricular
I. Terminology
These are some of the commonly used criteria to define the dementia asso-
ciated with vascular lesions in the brain (Wetterling, et al., 1996):
1. DSM-IV Criteria for Vascular dementia
2. NINDS-AIREN criteria for the diagnosis of probable vascular dementia
(Roman et al., 1993)
3. ICD-10 research criteria (DCR-10) for dementia
4. Hachinski ischemic score
SUBCORTICAL ISCHEMIC DEMENTIA 23
Changes appear in the normal brain with aging which contribute to mental
decline. It has been estimated that after the age of 65, 6.4% of patients show
evidence of some cognitive impairment and this figure increases steadily with age
(Ankri and Poupard, 2003). With aging, vascular lesions are seen commonly in the
brain and opinions vary as to whether these lesions are clinically significant or not
with regards to cognitive changes (Fig. 1). It has also been the subject of discussion
about the location and volume of these lesions and whether that may have an
impact on cognition.
Also with aging, the risk of vascular disease increases and the prevalence has been
shown to be 0.3% during 65–69 years of age and as much as 5.2% at age 90 years
(Chabriat and Bousser, 2006). The Rotterdam study showed that the prevalence
of ‘‘vascular’’ dementia is higher in women, despite the higher incidence in men,
because of increased longevity in women by 10 years (Lobo et al., 2000).
Asymptomatic lacunar-type infarcts have been found to be present in 11–28%
of normal elderly persons in multiple studies (Longstreth et al., 1998; Price et al.,
1997; Vermeer et al., 2003). Atrial fibrillation (Ott et al., 1997) hormonal treatment
(estrogen þ progesterone) of women after age 65 (Shumaker et al., 2003), diabetes
mellitus (Leibson et al., 1997) and smoking in persons above age 60 years ( Juan
et al., 2004) have all been shown to have higher risk for ‘‘vascular’’ dementia.
FIG. 1. Subcortical white matter hyperintensities, seen on FLAIR (transaxial) MRI brain scan,
which demonstrates mild (A) and somewhat more advanced (B) periventricular findings. On noncon-
trast CT brain scan (C), there is prominent hypodensity in the periventricular region. These findings
have been referred to as Binswanger’s disease in the past, but more recently the term leukoaraiosis has
been preferred because of the nonspecific clinical correlates of such findings.
24 MENON AND KELLEY
Atrial fibrillation has also been shown to be an independent risk factor for
cognitive impairment even in the absence of a stroke (Kilander et al., 1998;
O’Connell et al., 1998). With the presence of hypertension, the greatest risk factor
for stroke, the increased risk of cognitive impairment has been shown with
diVerent studies and patient cohorts (EVA Group, 1999; Launer et al., 2000).
Stroke has been proved as a significant risk factor for cognitive impairment
and dementia with as many as 25% of patients at 3 months and up to 50% within
a year developing some degree of clinically significant cognitive impairment
(Desmond et al., 2002; Mackowiak-Cordoliani et al., 2005; Tang et al., 2004;
Tatemichi TK et al., 1994). A reported range of 25–80% of elderly subjects with
dementia has Alzheimer’s disease coexisting with the vascular lesions. This makes
the assessment of pure cerebrovascular lesions diYcult ( Jellinger, 2008).
It has long been believed that the small vessel or lacunar strokes in the white
matter of the brain contribute minimally to the changes in cognition. However, a
review of the NINDS-AIREN criteria (Roman et al., 1993) demonstrates that it
includes multiple basal ganglia and white matter lacunes and extensive periven-
tricular white matter lesions on neuroimaging as causes of probable ‘‘vascular’’
dementia. Ischemic lesions in the brain can occur in various locations. However,
with the current level and techniques of neuroimaging, it is not possible to
visualize all the microinfarcts.
Areas of white matter ischemia, such as microinfarcts, often termed lacunes
have been shown to be independent predictors of development of dementia
(DeGroot et al., 2000; Kovari et al., 2004; Pasquier et al., 2000; White et al.,
2002) particularly impairments with executive functions. Lacunes, when present
in certain specific locations like the thalamus and basal ganglia, can cause
cognitive disturbances (Fig. 2). Conversely, their presence in the deep white
matter of the frontal, temporal, or parietal lobes does not seem to do so. Silbert
et al. (2008) reported that an increase in total and periventricular white matter
signal intensity changes, over time, correlates with progressive gait impairment
while the progression of the total volume hyperintensity changes in the subcortical
region is associated with memory decline in elderly subjects who are relatively
intact from a cognitive standpoint.
The hippocampus is extremely sensitive to hypoperfusion which results in
sclerosis. Both sclerosis and lacunes in the hippocampus can lead to decline in
cognition (Bastos-Leite et al., 2007; Fein et al., 2000; Kril et al., 2002) but this decline
appears to be less than that seen in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (Du et al.,
2002). Age appears to be the best and strongest predictor of the presence of white
SUBCORTICAL ISCHEMIC DEMENTIA 25
FIG. 2. Coronal T2-weighted MRI brain scan which reveals bilateral lacunar-type infarcts (arrows)
in the thalamic region.
matter lesions in elderly subjects and whereas these white matter lesions maybe
present in the elderly persons with normal cognitive function (Breteler et al., 1994;
Carey et al., 2008; Liao et al., 1997; Lindgren et al., 1994; Longstreth et al., 1996;
Soderlund et al., 2003; Ylikoski et al., 1993), although elderly patients with demen-
tia have been noted to have an increased load of lesions. There exists conflicting
data as to whether these white matter lesions indeed contribute to cognitive decline.
Some studies (Bracco et al., 1993; Gold et al., 2007; Schmidt et al., 2002) were not
able to find any definite evidence that these white matter lesions are responsible
for the cognitive impairment in these subjects whereas some others including
the subcortical ischemic vascular dementia (SIVD) program project (Chui, 2007;
Mosley et al., 2005; Prins et al., 2005) have found that an increasing load of white
matter lesions, over time, is associated with progressive cognitive decline (Fig. 3).
Price et al. (2005) report that despite the presence of white matter abnormal-
ities, executive dysfunction is not seen until at least 25% of the white matter in the
hemisphere is involved. Furthermore, when 50% of the hemisphere is involved,
executive dysfunction exceeds the memory impairment. This forms the basis of
research criteria proposed for subcortical vascular dementia (Erkinjuntti et al.,
2000). Understandably, there is no correlation between the MMSE score and
white matter lesion load as the MMSE assesses the memory and cognitive
functions and not the executive functions.
Carey et al. (2008) found that the presence of lacunes even when not producing
any clinical deficits could cause decline in cognition and supports the view upheld
by many others that in SIVD there is a disruption in the connecting pathways of
the subcortical areas and the frontal cortex.
26 MENON AND KELLEY
FIG. 3. Pronounced subcortical hyperintensities in the subcortical white matter, specifically the
centrum semiovale, in a patient with progressive gait and cognitive impairment. This is a FLAIR
(transaxial) MRI brain scan with two adjacent sections.
et al., 1994), again supporting the theory that remote eVects in the cortices, resulting
from possible disruption of the connecting tracts and circuits, may be responsible
for the cognitive changes.
References
AHA/ASA Stroke (2006). Council. Primary prevention of ischemic stroke: A guideline. Stroke 37,
1583–1593.
Ankri, J., and Poupard, M. (2003). Prevalence and incidence of dementia in the very old (review). Rev.
Epidemiol. Sante Publique 51, 349–360.
Baron, J. C., D’Antona, R., Pantano, P., Serdaru, M., Samson, Y., and Bousser, M. G. (1986). EVects
of thalamic stroke on energy metabolism of the cerebral cortex. A PET study in man. Brain 109,
1243–1259.
Bastos-Leite, A. J., van der Flier, W. M., van Staaten, E. C., Staekenborg, S. S., Scheltens, P., and
Barkhof, F. (2007). The contribution of medial temporal atrophy and vascular pathology to
cognitive impairment in vascular dementia. Stroke 38, 3182–3187.
Black, S., Roman, G. C., Geldmacher, D. S., Salloway, S., Hecker, J., Burns, A., Perdomo, C.,
Kumar, D., and Pratt, R. (2003). EYcacy and tolerability of donezepil in vascular dementia:
Positive results of a 24-week multi-center, international, randomized, placebo-controlled clinical
trial. Stroke 34, 2323–2330.
Bocti, C., Swartz, R. H., Gao, F-Q, Sahlas, D. J., Behl, P., and Black, S. E. (2005). A new visual rating
scale to assess strategic white matter hyperintensities within cholingergic pathways in dementia.
Stroke 36, 2126–2131.
Bracco, L., Campani, D., Baratti, E., Lippi, A., Inzitari, D., Pracucci, G., and Amaducci, L. (1993).
Relation between MRI features and dementia in cerebrovascular disease patients with leukoar-
iosis: A longitudinal study. J. Neurol. Sci. 120, 131–136.
Breteler, M. M., van Armerongen, N. M., van Swieten, J. C., Claus, J. J., Grobbee, D. E., van Gijn, J.,
Hofman, A., and van Harskamp, F. (1994). Cognitive correlates of ventricular enlargement and
cerebral white matter lesions on MRI. The Rotterdam Study. Stroke 25, 1109–1115.
Brickman, A., Honig, L. S., Scarmeas, N., Tatarina, O., Sanders, L., Albert, M. S., Brandt, J.,
Blacker, D., and Stern, Y. (2008). Measuring cerebral atrophy and white matter hyperintensity
burden to predict the rate of cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Arch. Neurol. 65, 1202–1208.
Cappizzano, A. A., SchuV, N., Amend, D. L., Tanabe, J. L., Norman, D., Maudsley, A. A., Jagust, W.,
Chui, H. C., Fein, G., Segal, M. R., and Weiner, M. W. (2000). Subcortical ischemic vascular
dementia: Assessment with quantitative MRI and H-MRS. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 21, 621–630.
Carey, C. L., Kramer, J. H., Josephson, A., Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., Schuff, N., Weiner, M. W., and
Chui, H. C. (2008). Subcortical lacunes are associated with executive dysfunction in cognitively
normal elderly. Stroke 39, 397–402.
Carrera, E., and Bogousslavsky, J. (2004). Anteromedian, central and posterolateral infarcts of the
thalamus: Three variant types. Stroke 35, 2826–2831.
Chabriat, H., and Bousser, M. G. (2006). Vascular dementia: Potential of antiplatelet agents in
prevention. Eur. Neurol. 55, 61–69.
Chobanian, A. V., Bakris, G. L., and Black, H. R., Cushman, W. C., Green, L. A., Izzo, J. L., Jr.,
Jones, D. W., Materson, B. J., Oparil, S., Wright, J. T., Jr., Roccella, E. J., National Heart, Lung and
Blood Institute Joint National committee on Prevention, Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of
High Blood Pressure; National High Blood Pressure Eduction Program Coordinating Committee.
(2003). The Seventh Report of the Joint National Committee on prevention, detection, evaluation
and treatment of high blood pressure: The JNC 7 Report. JAMA 289, 2560–2572.
Chui, H. C. (2007). Subcortical ischemic vascular dementia. Neurol. Clin. 25, 717–740.
DeGroot, J. C., DeLeeuw, F. E., Oudkerk, M., van Gijn, J., Hofman, A., Jolles, J., and Breteler, M. M.
(2002). Periventricular cerebral white matter lesions predict rate of cognitive decline. Ann. Neurol.
52, 335–341.
Desmond, D. W., Moroney, J. T., Sano, M., and Stern, Y. (2002). Incidence of dementia after ischemic
stroke: Results of a longitudinal study. Stroke 33, 2254–2260.
30 MENON AND KELLEY
Du, A. T., SchuV, N., Laakso, M. P., Zhu, X. P., Jagust, W. J., Yaffe, K., Kramer, J. H., Miller, B. L.,
Reed, B. R., Norman, D., Chui, H. C., and Weiner, M. W. (2002). EVects of subcortical ischemic
vascular dementia and AD on entorhinal cortex and hippocampus. Neurology 58, 1635–1641.
Erkinjuntti, T., Inzitari, D., Pantoni, L., Wallin, A., Scheltens, P., Rockwood, K., Roman, G. C.,
Chui, H., and Desmond, D. W. (2000). Research criteria for subcortical vascular dementia in
clinical trials. J. Neural. Transm. (Suppl) 59, 23–30.
Erkinjuntti, T., Kurz, A., Gauthier, S., Bullock, R., Lilienfeld, S., and Damaraju, C. V. (2002). EYcacy
of galantamine in probable vascular dementia and AD combined with cerebrovascular disease:
A randomized trial. Lancet 359, 1282–1290.
EVA Group (1999). Cognitive decline in individuals with high blood pressure: A longitudinal study in
the elderly. Neurology 53, 1948–1952.
Fein, G., Di Sclafoni, V., Tanabe, J., Cardenas, V., Weiner, M. W., Jagust, W. J., Reed, B. R.,
Norman, D., Schuff, N., Greenfield, T., and Chui, H. (2000). Hippocampal and cortical atrophy
predictsdementia in subcortical ischemic vascular disease. Neurology 55, 1626–1635.
Forette, F., Seux, M. L., Staessen, J. A., Thijs, L., Birkenhager, W. H., Bulpitt, C. J., Girerd, X.,
Jaaskivi, M., Vanhanen, H., Kiviner, P., Yodfat, Y., Vanska, O., et al. (1998). Prevention of
dementia in randomized double-blind, placebo-controlled systolic hypertension in Europe (Syst-
Eur) trial. Lancet 352, 1347–1351.
Gold, G., Kovari, E., Hof, P. R., Michel, J. P., Bouras, C., and Giannakopoulos, P. (2007). Sorting out
the clinical consequences of ischemic lesions in brain aging: A clinicopathological approach.
J. Neurol. Sci. 257, 17–22.
Hojer-Pederson, E., and Friz-Peterson, O. (1989). Changes in cerebral blood flow in the cerebral
cortex after subcortical ischemic infarction. Stroke 20, 211–216.
Jellinger, K. A. (2008). Morphological diagnosis of ‘‘vascular dementia’’—A critical update. J. Neurolog.
Sci. 270, 1–12.
Juan, D., Zhou, D. H., Li, J., Wang, J. Y., Gao, C., and Chen, M. (2004). A 2 year follow-up study of
cigarette smoking and the risk of dementia. Eur. J. Neurol. 11, 277–282.
Kattapong, V. J., Brooks, W. M., Wesley, M. H., Kodituwakku, P. W., and Rosenberg, G. A. (1996).
Proton MRS of vascular and Alzheimer-type dementia. Arch. Neurol. 53, 167–174.
Kilander, L., Andren, B., Nyman, H., Lind, L., Bober, M., and Lithell, H. (1998). Atrial fibrillation is
an independent determinant of low cognitive function: Case-control study. Stroke 29, 1816–1820.
Kovari, E., Gold, G., Herrmann, F. R., Canuto, A., Hof, P. R., Michel, J. P., Bouras, C., and
Giannakopoulos, P. (2004). Cortical microinfarcts and demyelination significantly aVect cognition
in brain aging. Stroke 35, 410–414.
Kril, J. J., Patel, S., Harding, A. J., and Halliday, G. M. (2002). Patients with vascular dementia due to
microvascular pathology have significant hippocampal neuronal loss. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry
72, 747–751.
Launer, I. J., Ross, G. W., Petrovich, H., Masaki, K., Foley, D., White, R., and Havlik, R. J. (2000).
Midlife blood pressure and dementia. The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Neurobiol. Aging 21, 49–55.
Leibson, C. L., Rocca, W. A., Hanson, V. A., Cha, R., Kokmen, E., O’Brien, P. C., and Palumbo, P. J.
(1997). The risk of dementia among persons with diabetes mellitus: A population-based cohort
study. NY Acad. Sci. 826, 422–427.
Leuchter, A. F., Dunkin, J. J., Lufkin, R. B., Anzai, Y., Cook, I. A., and Newton, T. F. (1994). EVects
of white matter disease on functional connections in the aging brain. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry
57, 1347–1354.
Liao, D., Cooper, L., Cai, J., Toole, J., Bryan, N., Burke, G., Shahar, E., Nieto, J., Mosley, T., and
Heiss, G. (1997). The prevalence and severity of white matter lesions; their relationship to age,
ethnicity, gender, and cardiovascular risk factors. The ARIC Study. Neuroepidemiology 16, 149–162.
Lindgren, A., Roijer, A., Rudling, O., Norrving, B., Larsson, E. M., Eskilsson, J., Wallin, L.,
Olsson, B., and Johansson, B. B. (1994). Cerebral lesions on MRI, heart disease and vascular
risk factors on subjects without stroke. A population-based study. Stroke 25, 929–934.
SUBCORTICAL ISCHEMIC DEMENTIA 31
Lobo, A., Launer, L. J., Fritglioni, L., Anderson, K., Di Carlo, A., Breteler, M. M., Copeland, J. R.,
Dartigues, J. F., Jagger, C., Martinez-Lage, J., Soininen, H., and Hofman, A. (2000). for the
Neurologic Diseases in the Elderly Research Group. Prevalence of dementia and major subtypes
in Europe: A collaborative study of population-based cohorts. Neurology 54, S4–S9.
Longstreth, W. T., Jr, Manolio, T. A., Arnold, A., Burke, G. L., Bryan, N., Jungreis, C. A.,
Enright, P. L., O’Leary, D., and Fried, L. (1996). Clinical correlates of white matter findings on
cranial MRI of 3301 elderly people. The Cardiovascular Health Study. Stroke 27, 1274–1282.
Longstreth, W. T., Jr, Bernick, C., Manolio, T. A., Bryan, N., Jungreis, C. A., and Price, T. R. (1998).
Lacunar infarcts defined by MRI of 3660 elderly people. The Cardiovascular Health Study. Arch.
Neurol. 55, 1217–1225.
Mackowiak-Cordoliani, M. A., Bombois, S., Memin, A., Henon, H., and Pasquier, F. (2005). Post-
stroke dementia in the elderly. Drugs Aging 22, 483–493.
McKay, S., MeyerhoV, D. J., Constans, J-M, Norman, D., Fein, G., and Weiner, M. W. (1996).
Regional gray and white matter metabolite diVerences in subjects with AD, with subcortical
ischemic vascular dementia, and elderly controls with H MRS imaging. Arch. Neurol. 53, 167–174.
Mosley, T. H., Knopman, D. S., Catellier, D. J., Bryan, N., Hutchinson, R. G., Grothues, C. A.,
Folsom, A. R., Cooper, L. S., Burke, G. L., Liao, D., and Szklo, M. (2005). Cerebral MRI findings
and cognitive functioning. The ARIC Study. Neurology 64, 2056–2062.
Nagata, K., Maruya, H., Yuya, H., Terashi, H., Mito, Y., Kato, H., Sato, M., Satoh, Y., Watahiki, Y.,
Hirata, Y., Yokoyama, E., and Hatazawa, J. (2000). Can PET data diVerentiate Alzheimer’s
disease from vascular dementia. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 903, 252–261.
O’Connell, J. E., Gray, C. S., French, J. M., and Robertson, I. H. (1998). Atrial fibrillation and
cognitive function: Case-control study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 65, 386–389.
Ott, A., Breteler, M. M., de Bruyne, M. C., van Harskamp, F., Grobbee, De, and Hofman, A. (1997).
Atrial fibrillation and dementia in a population-based study. The Rotterdam Study. Stroke 28,
316–321.
Pantoni, L., Bianchi, C., Beneke, M., Inzitari, D., Wallin, A., and Erkinjuntti, T. (2000). The
Scandinavian Multi-Infarct Dementia Trial: A double blind, placebo-controlled trial of nimodi-
pine in multi-infarct dementia. J. Neurol. Sci. 175, 116–123.
Pantoni, L., del Ser, T., Soglian, A. G., Amigoni, S., Spadari, G., Binelli, D., and Inzitari, D. (2005).
EYcacy and safety of nimodipine in subcortical vascular dementia: A randomized placebo-
controlled study. Stroke 36, 619–624.
Pasquier, F., Henon, H., and Leys, D. (2000). Relevance of white matter changes to pre- and post-
stroke dementia. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 903, 466–469.
Price, T. R., Manolio, T. A., Kronmal, R. A., et al. for the Cardiovascular Health Study Collaborative
Research Group. (1997). Silent brain infarction on MRI and neurological abnormalities in
community-dwelling older patients. The Cardiovascular Health Study. Arch. Neurol. 28,
1158–1164.
Price, C. C., JeVerson, A. L., Merino, M. D., Heilman, K. M., and Libon, D. J. (2005). Subcortical
vascular dementia; integrating neuropsychological and neuroradiological data. Neurology 65,
376–382.
Prins, N. D., van Djik, E. J., den Heijer, T., Vermeer, S. E., Jolles, J., Koudstaal, P. J., Hofman, A., and
Breteler, M. M. (2005). Cerebral small vessel disease and decline in information processing speed,
executive function and memory. Brain 128, 2034–2041.
PROGRESS Collaborative Group (2001). Randomized controlled trial of perindopril-based blood
pressure lowering regimen among 6105 individuals with previous stroke or TIA. Lancet 358,
1033–1041.
Roman, G. C. (2004). Brain hypoperfusion: A critical factor in vascular dementia. Neurol. Res. 26,
454–458.
32 MENON AND KELLEY
Roman, G. C., Tatemichi, T. K., Erkinjuntti, T., Cummings, J. L., Masdeu, J. C., Garcia, J. H.,
Amaducci, L., Orgogozo, J. M., Brun, A., and Hofman, A. (1993). Vascular dementia: Diagnostic
criteria for research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology 43,
250–260.
Rovaris, M., Filippi, M., Minicucci, L., Iannucci, G., Santuccio, G., Possa, F., and Comi, G. (2000).
Cortical/subcortical disease burden and cognitive impairment in patients with multiple sclerosis
AJNR. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 21, 402–408.
Rovaris, M., Iannucci, G., Falautano, M., Possa, F., Martinelli, V., Comi, G., and Filippi, M. (2002).
Cognitive dysfunction in patients with mildly disabling relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis: An
exploratory study with diVusion tensor MR imaging. J. Neurol. Sci. 195, 103–109.
Sahlas, D. J., Swartz, R. H., and Black, S. E. (2002). Clasmatodendrosis correlating with periventri-
cular hyperintensity in mixed dementia. Ann. Neurol. 52, 378–381.
Schmidt, R., Schmidt, H., Kapeller, P., Lechner, A., and Fazekas, F. (2002). Evolution of white matter
lesions. Cerebrovasc. Dis. 13, 16–20.
Schmierer, K., Scaravilli, F., Altmann, D. R., Barker, G. J., and Miller, D. H. (2004). Magneitization
transfer ratio and myelin in postmortem MS brain. Ann. Neurol. 56, 407–415.
SchuV, N., Capizzano, A. A., Du, A. T., Amend, D. L., O’Neill, J., Norman, D., Jagust, W. J.,
Chui, H. C., Kramer, J. H., Reed, B. R., Miller, B. L., Yaffe, K., and Weiner, M. W. (2003).
DiVerent patterns of N-acetylaspartate loss in subcortical ischemic vascular dementia and AD.
Neurology 61, 358–364.
Shumaker, S. A., Legault, C., Rapp, S. R., Thal, L., Wallace, R. B., Ockene, J. K., Hendrix, S. L.,
Jones, B. N., 3rd., Assaf, A. R., Jackson, R. D., Kotchen, J. M., Wassertheil-Smaller, J., and
Wactawski–Wende, J., WHIMS Investigators. (2003). Estrogen þ Progestin and the incidence of
dementia and mild cognitive impairment in post-menopausal women: A randomized, controlled
trial. JAMA 289, 2651–2662.
Silbert, L. C., Nelson, C., Howieson, D. B., Moore, M. M., and Kaye, J. A. (2008). Impact of white
matter hyperintensity volume progression on rate of cognitive and motor decline. Neurology 71,
108–113.
Soderlund, H., Nyberg, L., Adolfsson, R., Nilsson, L. G., and Launer, L. J. (2003). High prevalence of
white matter hyperintensities in normal aging. Relation to BP and cognition. Cortex 39,
1093–1105.
Swartz, R. H., and Black, S. E. (2006). Anterior medial thalamic lesions in dementia: Frequent and
volume dependently associated with sudden cognitive decline. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 77,
1307–1312.
Tang, W. K., Chan, S. S., Chiu, H. F., Ungvari, G. S., Wong, K. S., Kwok, T. C., Mok, V.,
Wong, K. T., Richards, P. S., and Ahuja, A. T. (2004). Frequency and determinants of post-
stroke dementia in Chinese. Stroke 35, 930–935.
Tatemichi, T. K., Paik, M., Bagiella, E., Desmond, B. W., Stern, Y., Sano, M., Hauser, A., and
Mayeux, R. (1994). Risk of dementia after stroke in a hospitalized cohort: Results of a longitudinal
study. Neurology 44, 1885–1891.
Tullberg, M., Fletcher, E., DeCarli, C., Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., Harvey, D. J., Weiner, M. W.,
Chui, H. C., and Jagust, W. M. (2004). White matter lesions impair frontal lobe function
regardless of their location. Neurology 63, 246–253.
Tzourio, C, Anderson, C., Chapman, N., Woodward, M., Neal, B., MacMahon, S., Chalmers, J., and
PROGRESS Collaborative Group. (2003). Effects of blood pressure lowering with perindopril
and indapamide therapy on dementia and cognitive decline in patients with cerebrovascular
disease. Arch. Int. Med. 163, 1069–1075.
Vermeer, S. E., Den Heijer, T., Koudstaal, P. J., Oudkerk, M., and Breteler, M. M. (2003). Incidence
and risk factors of silent brain infarcts in population-based Rotterdam scan study. Stroke 34,
392–396.
SUBCORTICAL ISCHEMIC DEMENTIA 33
Wetterling, T., Kanitz, R. D., and Borgis, K. J. (1996). Comparison of diVerent diagnostic criteria for
vascular dementia (ADDTC, DSM-IV, ICD-10, NINDS-AIREN). Stroke 27, 30–36.
White, L., Petrovich, H., Hardman, J., Launer, L. J., Curb, J. D., and Schmidt, R. (2002). Cerebro-
vascular pathology and dementia in autopsied Honolulu Aging Study participants. Ann. NY Acad.
Sci. 997, 9–23.
Wilkinson, D., Doody, R., Taubman, K., Mintzer, J., Kertesz, A., Pratt, R. D., and Donepezil 308
Study Group. (2003). Donepezil in vascular dementia: A randomized, placebo-controlled study.
Neurology 61, 479–486.
Ylikoski, R., Ylikoski, A., Erkinjuntti, T., Suldava, R., Raininko, R., and Tilvis, R. (1993). White
matter changes in healthy elderly persons correlate with attention and speed of mental processing.
Arch. Neurol. 50, 818–824.
Young, V. G., Halliday, G. M., and Kril, J. J. (2008). Neuropathologic correlates of white matter
hyperintensities. Neurology 71, 804–808.
CEREBROVASCULAR AND CARDIOVASCULAR PATHOLOGY
IN ALZHEIMER’S DISEASE
Jack C. de la Torre
Sun Health Research Institute, Center for Alzheimer’s Research, Sun City,
Arizona 85351, USA
I. Introduction
II. Risk Factors to AD
A. Cardiovascular Disease
B. Preclinical Detection of AD
C. Treatment of AD
III. Conclusions
References
I. Introduction
Cerebral energy supply can be interrupted after cardiac arrest or stroke, two
conditions that result in global or local brain cell death. However, brain energy
supply can dwindle slowly over long periods of time following non-fatal carotid
artery stenosis or cardiac disease, two major disorders that result in brain hypo-
perfusion. Brain hypoperfusion (not an ischemic process) does not necessarily kill
or damage nerve cells right away by significantly arresting energy nutrients
delivered via the circulation but may chronically ‘‘gnaw’’ at nerve cells over a
long period of time by destabilizing their normal function (Fig. 1).
Neuronal states
Metabolically
Functional neuron D dysfunctional neuron
A
Nissl bodies
Nisssl
C
Nuclear shift Nissl loss
eosinophilia
Nuclear pyknosis
B
Dead neuron
FIG. 1. Functional and pathological states deranging brain neurons resulting in degenerative
processes. Healthy, functional neurons (A) can either (B) die (solid arrow), (C) undergo metabolic-
structural damage (dashed arrow), (D) undergo metabolic only damage (dotted arrow). In the (C) state,
intracellular damage can be assessed histologically by noting nuclear or cytoplasmic changes in
organelles (chromatolysis, eosinophilia, nuclear shift, pyknosis, etc.). In the (D) state, bio-molecular
markers reflecting neuronal distrenss are needed to evaluate extent of metabolic dysfunction. Neuronal
metabolic dysfunction following chronic brain hypoperfusion is likely the first step in the pathologic
pathway to cytostructural damage, atrophy, and death and how such dysfunction may explain
cognitive disability, as in pre-clinical Alzheimer’s disease.
Since then, compelling evidence from a variety of clinical and basic studies has
convince us that AD should be treated as a vasocognopathy (de la Torre, 2004)
(a vascular-related, cognitive disorder) and we shall explore some of this evidence
in our review here to determine whether proof of concept is met.
TABLE I
SUMMARY OF FINDINGS FROM EPIDEMIOLOGICAL STUDIES (SEE TEXT) OF VASCULAR-RELATED
RISK FACTORS TO ALZHEIMER’S DISEASE
Highly prevalent conditions such as stroke, hypertension, atherosclerosis and heart disease are
common precursors to AD.
to maintain neuronal signaling during active ion channel flux. Ion flux is the
basis for propagation of action potentials and neurotransmission. Impaired brain
energy production also upregulates the A! rate-limiting enzyme BACE1, thus
promoting A! overproduction (Velliquette et al., 2005) (Fig. 1B). Consequently,
when mitochondrial function in neurons is disrupted by failing glucose/O2 supply
for energy production, rapid damage to brain cells occurs. Brain hypoperfusion is
the pathogenic trigger that pushes the neuronal energy crisis and the cascade of
molecular and cytopathologic changes that characterize AD (Fig. 2).
CEREBROVASCULAR AND CARDIOVASCULAR PATHOLOGY 39
Nucleus APP
UPR
tau
ATP Microtubules
TransGolgi
Nissl (rER) Reduced ATP synthesis
Oxidative stress = hi free radicals
+ +
Na K ATPase pump disturbed
Signal transduction impairment
Protein synthesis Neurotransmitter dysfunction
Protein assembly Increased BACE-1 increased Ab
Protein folding [UPR] Microtubule disassembly = death
Protein cleavage
FIG. 2. (A) A fundamental principle in cell biology is seen by the use of chemical energy in the form
of ATP derived from glucose/oxygen delivery to assemble, disassemble and alter protein structure.
Since proteins work to keep a neuron healthy, defects in their synthesis, assembly, folding, or cleavage
can impair the normal intracellular and extracellular secretory transport pathway and damage brain
cells. Normal neuronal energy metabolism in the brain includes optimal ATP production by mito-
chondrial oxidative phosphorylation, RNA transcription and protein synthesis, cell signaling, neuro-
transmission, axonal transport of molecules between cytoplasm and nerve ending via microtubules.
Normal brain has little or no A! accumulation. Following initial cerebral hypoperfusion, reduced ATP
synthesis can result in non-specific protein and oxidative stress changes that may lead to MCI.
Theoretically, overexpression of A! peptides and specific neurodegenerative pathology may be lacking.
40 JACK C. DE LA TORRE
Risk factors for AD are either the result of genetic susceptibility (e.g., carrying
an ApoE e4 allele) or environmental exposure of a person’s health to an event that
can introduce, accelerate, or further compromise cognitive dysfunction. At the
present time, only environmental risk factor exposure is modifiable or preventable
but in the future, genetic engineering of potential risk susceptibilities to AD could
become a realistic therapeutic target for AD.
Vascular risk factors to AD as shown in Table I have been extensively
reviewed (Aguero-Torres et al., 2006; Breteler, 2000; de la Torre and
Mussivand, 1993; Sjogren et al., 2006; Sparks et al., 2005) and only some recently
reported risk factors will be briefly discussed here.
One example of a potentially modifiable vascular risk to AD of increasing
research interest is hypercholesterolemia and dietary fat intake. A steady amount
of research has confirmed a link between high levels of cholesterol and the
development of AD (Sjogren et al., 2006).
Hypercholesterolemia acts as a precursor of atherosclerosis, cardiovascular
disease, and diabetes. For obvious reasons, high serum cholesterol levels have
generated a rapid-growing market for lipid-lowering drugs prescribed to patients
at risk of cardiovascular or cerebrovascular conditions. Statins that lower choles-
terol levels have been suggested as useful in both prevention and treatment of AD
but this conclusion needs further verification (Sparks et al., 2005). The mechanism
by which statins may provide a benefit against cerebrovascular disease including
AD remains speculative and is likely multifactorial. In 1998, it was suggested, on
the basis of murine studies, that the health benefits of statins did not merely aim at
lowering lipids (Laufs et al., 1998). Statins were shown to protect against injury by
a mechanism involving the selective upregulation of endothelial nitric oxide
synthase (Laufs et al., 1998), the enzyme that generates nitric oxide in blood
vessels and is involved with regulating vascular function. Further research along
(B) Persistent reduction of glucose and oxygen delivery to ischemic-sensitive neurons following chronic
brain hypoperfusion leads to critical ATP depletion and promotes oxidative stress, reactive oxygen
species and abnormalities in protein synthesis activating their disassembly, misfolding (generating
unfolded protein response, UPR), and abnormal cleavage. Altered protein synthesis can subsequently
form molecules that can ravage nucleic acids, protein kinases, phosphatases, lipids, and harm cell
structure. ATP cutback also hastens ionic pump dysfunction, signal transduction breakdown, neurotrans-
mitter failure, faulty cleavage of amyloid precursor protein (APP) leading to BACE-1 upregulation and
A! overproduction and microtubule damage from tau hyperphosphorylation. Retrograde axonal
transport (curved arrow) of trophic and growth factors (e.g., NGF, BDNF) essential for neuronal survival
is diminished due to energy-starved motor protein deficiency. This selective neuronal energy crisis is
caused by blood flow supply not meeting cell energy demand in highly active brain regions whose vascular
reserve capability has reached a critical threshold. About 75% of ATP energy is used on signaling
(action potentials, postsynaptic potentials, etc.). Reduced ATP synthesis is the precursor of the molecular
cascade that leads to Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and to the region-specific neurono-glial death pathway.
Key: # ¼ reduction or loss; 6 ¼ impaired reaction.
CEREBROVASCULAR AND CARDIOVASCULAR PATHOLOGY 41
these lines has demonstrated that statins improve endothelial function, increase
nitric oxide bioavailability, provide antioxidant properties, inhibit inflammatory
responses, exert immunomodulatory actions, regulate progenitor cells, and stabi-
lize atherosclerotic plaques (Endres et al., 1998) and several studies have shown a
benefit for statins in the treatment of AD (Wolozin et al., 2000).
Another important modifiable risk factor to AD is metabolic syndrome (MeS).
MeS is a cluster of factors that can lead to the development of cardiovascular
disease. MeS is characterized by abnormal insulin, glucose, and lipoprotein
metabolism as well as by hypertension and obesity (Grundy et al., 2005). These
factors are frequently found in people with either excess abdominal body fat or a
decreased ability of the body to use insulin, which is known as insulin resistance
(Grundy et al., 2005).
MeS has been found to be associated with AD even when diabetic patients are
excluded from analysis (Vanhanen et al., 2006), suggesting that obesity associated
with cardiovascular dysfunction or hypercholesterolemia is suYcient to initiate
cognitive impairment that later can convert to AD. The number of people
with MeS increases with age and is estimated to aVect 40% of people beyond
the age of 60. People with uncontrolled diabetes mellitus type 2 and those with
heart disease or prone to stroke are most likely to develop MeS (Martins et al.,
2006).
Since obesity and lack of exercise are two conditions that can lead to MeS,
correcting these by lifestyle changes that include a healthy diet and physical
activity could help prevent or reverse MeS. The cardiovascular risk factors
associated with MeS contribute to the development of atherosclerosis that can
lead to a heart attack or a stroke. People with the MeS are also more likely to
develop type 2 diabetes mellitus (Martins et al., 2006). Insulin resistance is central
to type 2 diabetes and is also implicated in the pathogenesis of AD (Vanhanen
et al., 2006). This has prompted ongoing clinical trials in AD patients to test the
eYcacy of improving insulin-like signaling with dietary omega-3 fatty acids or
insulin-sensitizing drugs as well as by exercise regimens.
It is well established that hypertension can increase the risk of stroke and heart
problems and decrease life expectancy (de la Torre, 2006). Many studies including
the Framingham and the Honolulu-Asia Aging studies have implicated impaired
cognitive function to hypertension in geriatric patients (Elias et al., 1993). It has
also been known for some time that hypertension in the elderly is a potential risk
factor to AD (de la Torre, 2009). What is not clear is how hypertension increases
the incidence of AD, particularly in those not-treated with antihypertensives.
People with hypertension are six times more likely to have a stroke that can
lead to AD (Ivan et al., 2004). It is calculated that reducing the number of cases
aVected by stroke which are amenable to prevention or treatment, would have a
major impact in lowering the incidence and societal costs of Alzheimer’s disease
which is now estimated to aVect 25 million people worldwide.
42 JACK C. DE LA TORRE
A. CARDIOVASCULAR DISEASE
B. PRECLINICAL DETECTION OF AD
RVOT Aorta
Septum
AV Aortic valve
LVOT
LV
LA
MV
Inferior wall
RVOT
Septum
FIG. 3. Echocardiography image (top) is obtained by placing the ultrasound probe on the surface of
the chest over the heart. Image shows all four chambers of the heart, useful for detecting chamber
enlargement. Valvular disease, hypertension, arrhythmias, left ventricular function, ejection fraction,
and cardiac output can be determined and measured. These cardiac measurements oVer a noninva-
sive, cost-eVective and reliable approach that can identify often correctable or treatable early lesions of
the heart which may contribute to chronic brain hypoperfusion and possibly AD in the elderly patient
with mild memory complaints. Key: RVOT ¼ right ventricular outlet tract; LVOT ¼ left ventricular
outlet tract; LV ¼ left ventricle; MV ¼ mitral valve; LA ¼ left atrium.
C. TREATMENT OF AD
There is presently no eVective treatment for AD. Five drugs are available in
the US market for prescriptive use in AD: Cognex (tacrine), Aricept (donepezil),
Excelon (rivastigmine tartrate), Reminyl (galantamine hydrobromide), and
CEREBROVASCULAR AND CARDIOVASCULAR PATHOLOGY 45
Namenda (memantine). The first four above act to slow the synaptic breakdown
of acetylcholine, one of the neurotransmitters important in memory and learning.
Reminyl also targets both AD and ‘‘mixed’’ dementia, that is, AD complicated by
cerebrovascular pathology.
A fifth drug, memantine, is a relatively newer medication for the treatment of
Alzheimer’s disease that works ostensibly by antagonizing the N-methyl-D-
aspartate receptors. Nonprescriptive treatments for AD in present use are nonste-
roidal anti-inflammatory agents (NSAIDs), ginkgo biloba, estrogen, vitamin E,
and prescriptive statins. All these treatments, whether prescriptive or over-the-
counter, at best, provide only very modest control of symptoms generally at the
early stages of AD and oVer little to no cost-benefit ratio at the later stages of the
disease (Loveman et al., 2006). Their modest benefit to AD patients must be
weighed against their frequent side-eVects, particularly the anti-cholinergic agents
which include nausea, vomiting, dizziness, headaches, and depression. Moreover,
since most clinical trials on the eVectiveness of these medicines were sponsored by
industry or industry-paid researchers, a prominent shadow for positive bias has to
be considered (Sismondo, 2008).
It should be noted that the common link binding practically all the medicines
and therapies available or tested experimentally for AD so far is their ability to
mildly increase cerebral perfusion (de la Torre, 2004b). However, since cerebral
perfusion is only slightly improved, the beneficial eVect of medicines such as those
listed above is transient and limited. Interestingly, most of the products that
improve AD symptoms also improve the symptoms of VaD. This activity by
medicaments with diVering pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics, lend
additional support to the concept of brain hypoperfusion in AD and VaD and
reinforce potential therapeutic targets aimed at patients who initially present with
mild cognitive dysfunction and who display declining perfusion to the brain.
New technologies, whose primary goal is to significantly increase cerebral perfu-
sion in such patients, could delay, reverse, or prevent the neuropathological
process that can lead to dementia.
III. Conclusions
conditions responsible for reducing carotid artery and cardiac blood flow to the
brain may be crucial clues in better understanding the physiopathology of AD.
This shift in thinking acknowledges the notion that current complacency on the
state of the art has created the gridlock which ignores that something is wrong in
our ability to clinically manage AD after a century of research. In this brief review,
the many vascular-related risk factors to AD including some that are preventable
or amenable to treatment are discussed. Considerable human data now point in
a new direction for guiding future research into AD. A more data driven new
research direction should open a window of opportunity that can lead to new
technologies, create new subfields by revolutionizing the field of dementia and
provide decisive management of this devastating disorder.
Acknowledgment
This research was supported by an Investigator-Initiated Research Grant from the Alzheimer’s
Association.
References
Aguero-Torres, H., Kivipelto, M., and von Strauss, E. (2006). Rethinking the dementia diagnoses in a
population-based study: What is Alzheimer’s disease and what is vascular dementia? A study from
the Kungsholmen project. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 22(3), 244–249.
Ahmed, S. N., Syed, F. M., and Porembka, D. T. (2007). Echocardiographic evaluation of hemody-
namic parameters. Crit. Care Med. 35(8 Suppl.), S323–S329.
Alves, T. C., and Busatto, G. F. (2006). Regional cerebral blood flow reductions, heart failure and
Alzheimer’s disease. Neurol. Res. 28, 579–587.
Alves, T. C., and Busatto, G. F. (2006). Regional cerebral blood flow reductions, heart failure and
Alzheimer’s disease. Neurol. Res. 28(6), 579–587.
Breteler, M. M. (2000). Vascular risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease: An epidemiologic perspective.
Neurobiol. Aging 21(2), 153–160.
de la Torre, J. C., and Mussivand, T. C. (1993). Can disturbed brain microcirculation cause
Alzheimer’s disease? Neurol. Res. 15, 146–153.
de la Torre, J. C. (2000). Critically attained threshold of cerebral hypoperfusion: The CATCH
hypothesis of Alzheimer’s pathogenesis. Neurobiol. Aging 903, 424–436.
de la Torre, J. C. (2004a). Alzheimer’s disease is a vasocognopathy: A new term to describe its nature.
Neurological. Res. 26(5), 517–524.
de la Torre, J. C. (2004b). Is Alzheimer’s a neurodegenerative or a vascular disorder? Data, dogma and
dialectics. Lancet Neurol. 3, 184–190.
CEREBROVASCULAR AND CARDIOVASCULAR PATHOLOGY 47
de la Torre, J. C. (2006). How do heart disease and stroke become risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease?
Neurol. Res. 28, 637–644.
de Leeuw, F. E., Barkhof, F., and Scheltens, P. (2004). White matter lesions and hippocampal atrophy
in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 62, 310–312.
de la Torre, J. C. (2008). Alzheimer disease prevalence be lowered with non-invasive testing.
J. Alzheimer Dis. 14(3), 353–359.
de la Torre, J. C. (2009). Carotid artery ultrasound and echocardiography (CAUSE) testing to lower
the prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Stroke Cerebrovasc. Dis., in press.
de Leon, M. J., Convit, A., and Wolf, O. T. (2001). Prediction of cognitive decline in normal elderly
subjects with 2-[18F]fluoro-2-deoxy-d-glucose/positron-emission tomography (FDG/PET). Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98, 10966–10971.
Elias, M. F., Wolf, P. A., D’Agostino, R. B., Cobb, J., and White, L. R. (1993). Blood pressure level is
inversely related to cognitive functioning: The Framingham Study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 138(6),
353–364.
Endres, M., Laufs, U., and Huang, Z. (1998). Stroke protection by 3-hydroxy-3-methylglutaryl
(HMG)-CoA reductase inhibitors mediated by endothelial nitric oxide synthase. PNAS 95,
8880–8885.
Grundy, S. M., Cleeman, J. I., Daniels, S. R., and Donato, K. A. (2005). Diagnosis and management of
the metabolic syndrome: An American Heart Association/National Heart, Lung, and Blood
Institute Scientific Statement. Circulation 112, 2735–2752.
Ivan, C. S., Seshadri, S., and Beiser, A. (2004). Dementia after stroke: The Framingham Study. Stroke
35, 1264–1268.
Johnson, K. A., Jones, K., Holman, B. L., Becker, J., Spiers, P. A., Satlin, A., and Albert, M. S. (1998).
Prediction of Alzheimer’s disease using SPECT. Neurology 50, 1563–1571.
Laufs, U., La Fata, V., Plutzky, J., and Liao, J. K. (1998). Upregulation of endothelial nitric oxide
synthase by HMG CoA reductase inhibitors. Circulation 97(12), 1129–1135.
Leenders, K. L., Perani, D., Lammertsma, A. A., Heather, J. D., Buckingham, P., Healy, M. J.,
Gibbs, J. M., Wise, R. J., Hatazawa, J., and Herold, S. (1990). Cerebral blood flow, blood volume
and oxygen utilization. Normal values and eVect of age. Brain 113, 27–47.
Loveman, E., Green, C., Kirby, J., Takeda, A., Picot, J., Payne, E., and Clegg, A. (2006). The clinical
and cost-eVectiveness of donepezil, rivastigmine, galantamine and memantine for Alzheimer’s
disease. Health Technol. Assess 10(1):iii-iv, ix-xi, 1–160.
Martins, I. J., Hone, E., Foster, J. K., Sunram-Lea, S. I., Gnjec, A., Fuller, S. J., Nolan, D.,
Gandy, S. E., and Martins, R. N. (2006). Apolipoprotein E, cholesterol metabolism, diabetes,
and the convergence of risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease and cardiovascular disease. Mol.
Psychiatry 11(8), 721–736.
Miklossy, J. (2003). Cerebral hypoperfusion induces cortical watershed microinfarcts which may
further aggravate cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurol. Res. 25(6), 605–610.
Ruitenberg, A., den Heiker, T., and Bakker, S. L. (2005). Cerebral hypoperfusion and clinical onset of
dementia: The Rotterdam study. Ann. Neurol. 57, 789–794.
Sjogren, M., Mielke, M., Gustafson, D., Zandi, P., and Skoog, I. (2006). Cholesterol and Alzheimer’s
disease—is there a relation?. Mech. Ageing Dev. 127(2), 138–147.
Silverman, D. H., and Phelps, M. E. (2001). Application of positron emission tomography for
evaluation of metabolism and blood flow in human brain: Normal development, aging, dementia,
and stroke. Mol. Genet. Metab. 74(1-2), 128–138.
Sismondo, S. (2008). How pharmaceutical industry funding aVects trial outcomes: Causal structures
and responses. Soc. Sci. Med. 66(9), 1909–1914.
Sparks, D. L., Sabbagh, M. N., and Connor, D. J. (2005). Atorvastatin for the treatment of mild to
moderate Alzheimer disease: Preliminary results. Arch. Neurol. 62, 753–757.
48 JACK C. DE LA TORRE
Vanhanen, M., Koivisto, K., Moilanen, L., Helkala, E. L., Hanninen, T., Soininen, H., Kervinen, K.,
Kesaniemi, Y. A., Laakso, M., and Kuusisto, J. (2006). Association of metabolic syndrome with
Alzheimer disease: A population-based study. Neurology 67(5), 843–847.
Velliquette, R. A., O’Connor, T., and Vassar, R. (2005). Energy inhibition elevates beta-secretase levels
and activity and is potentially amyloidogenic in APP transgenic mice: Possible early events in
Alzheimer’s disease pathogenesis. J. Neurosci. 25, 10874–10883.
Wolozin, B., Kellman, W., Ruosseau, P., Celesia, G., and Siegel, G. (2000). Decreased prevalence of
Alzheimer disease associated with 3-hydroxy-3-methyglutaryl coenzyme A reductase inhibitors.
Arch. Neurol. 57, 1439–1443.
NEUROIMAGING OF COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENTS
IN VASCULAR DISEASE
I. Introduction
A. Structural Brain Imaging
B. Functional Imaging
C. Metabolic Imaging
II. Vascular Cognitive Impairment
A. Terminology
B. Classification
C. Histopathology of VCI
D. Diagnostic Neuroimaging Criteria in VCI
E. Therapeutic Strategies
III. Updates on Neuroimaging Techniques in VCI
A. Conventional Structural MRI
B. Nonconventional Techniques
C. Metabolic Imaging
D. Functional Imaging
E. Correlations Between Imaging Findings and Pathology
IV. Conclusions
References
Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) has become the most important neuroim-
aging methodology for assessing and monitoring the pathological changes involved
in the onset and progression of vascular neurological diseases. Conventional MRI
sequences are usually not suYcient to provide full details about the diVerent degrees
of brain damage underlying vascular disorders. However, newer and more
advanced nonconventional MRI techniques have the capacity to detect invisible
brain damage that would otherwise not be detected. This review outlines some of
the most important neuroimaging techniques commonly used in the diagnosis,
assessment, and monitoring of cognitive impairment in vascular diseases. More
detailed applications of these techniques, specifically for VCI, are discussed.
I. Introduction
A B C
FIG. 1. Magnetization transfer imaging: sample scan with (A) and without (B) magnetic transfer
pulse. A relative magnetization transfer ratio map (C) is generated.
is lower in highly structured tissues such as WM and gray matter (GM) than in pure
water. ADC values depend on the orientation of the tissue relative to the measure-
ment. Conventionally, the average ADC is calculated from three (DWI) or more
than three (DTI) linearly independent directions that provide information on the
overall diVusivity in the tissue. Pathological processes that modify tissue organiza-
tion can cause abnormal water motion, thereby altering ADC values. Ischemic
events can alter diVusion parameters of brain tissue in vivo, resulting in a decrease of
water diVusivity measurable with diVerent DWI and DTI indices. These measures
include mean diVusivity (MD), fractional anisotropy (FA), entropy and subsequently
tractography outcomes (Fig. 2). It is important to emphasize that, in most instances,
very acute stages of ischemic events can be detected through this technique within
the first hour of onset. On the other hand, such changes are often not recognized on
other MRI sequences. The increased sensitivity of DWI in detecting acute ischemia
is thought to be the result of the water shift restricting motion of water protons
intracellularly (cytotoxic edema), whereas conventional T2-WI shows signal
alteration mostly as a result of vasogenic edema. However, the exact origin of the
decreased ADC measurements observed in early ischemia has not been fully
established. The diminished ADC value also could be a result of decreased temper-
ature in the malperfused tissues or a combination of all these factors (Busto et al.,
1987; Crain et al., 1991).
Regardless of the cause, decreased apparent ADC is a very sensitive indicator
of an early ischemic brain event at a stage when ischemic tissue remains poten-
tially salvageable (Wolpert et al., 1993).
Atrophy measures. The measurement of brain atrophy is of growing clinical
relevance as a biomarker of the disease process, especially when correlated
with other anatomical and functional information from conventional and non-
conventional MRI. Various tools are available for semiautomatic evaluation of
brain volume, longitudinal studies and intra-/intergroup cross-sectional analysis,
MRI AND COGNITION IN VASCULAR DISEASES 53
A B
C D
FIG. 2. DiVusion tensor imaging: Mean diVusivity (A), fractional anisotropy (B), and color-
fractional anisotropy (C) maps are presented. The maps are defined as functions of the eigenvalues
(i.e., the invariants of the diVusion tensor at each voxel); in the RGB (red-green-blue) FA map, the
corresponding eigenvectors are also displayed through color coding. A streamline tractographic
reconstruction (D) is displayed; fibers are reconstructed according to a linear algorithm following the
direction of maximum FA until a threshold value is exceeded.
A B
C D
FIG. 3. Brain segmentation with statistical parametric mapping (SPM) software: (A) 3D-T1 image,
(B) cerebrospinal fluid, (C) gray matter, and (D) white matter segmentation maps are displayed.
B. FUNCTIONAL IMAGING
1. Perfusion-Weighted Imaging
Perfusion-weighted imaging (PWI) techniques are sensitive to microscopic
levels of blood flow, defined as the amount of blood flowing into a voxel in a
certain temporal span and measured in ml blood/min/gr tissue. This index may
be altered in disease conditions and accurate monitoring is clinically relevant,
improving our understanding of the pathophysiology of the disease.
With respect to other techniques that noninvasively investigate cerebral blood
flow, such as positron emission tomography (PET), single photon emission
computed tomography (SPECT), and xenon-enhanced computed tomography
(Xe-CT), PWI shows some advantages by not using ionizing radiations, allowing a
better spatial resolution and a better signal-to-noise-ratio (SNR). In addition, PWI
is typically acquired over a short time, needing only a few extra minutes after
a conventional MRI exam.
Contrast-enhanced relative cerebral blood volume (rCBV) is the most
used perfusion imaging technique for parameter mapping. In this and other techni-
ques, both the ready availability and the T2* susceptibility eVects of gadolinium,
MRI AND COGNITION IN VASCULAR DISEASES 55
rather than the T1 shortening eVects, make it a suitable agent for use in PWI. As long
as the bolus transit through the tissue takes only a few seconds, high temporal
resolution imaging is required to obtain sequential images during the wash-in and
wash-out of the contrast material and for processing and calculation of hemody-
namic maps (including mean transit time (MTT), time to peak (TTP), time of arrival
(T0), negative integral (N1). An important neuroradiological indication for MRI is
the evaluation of incipient or acute stroke via PWI and DWI. DWI can demonstrate
the central eVect of a stroke on the brain, whereas PWI visualizes the larger second
ring delineating blood flow and blood volume. Echo planar and, potentially, echo
volume techniques (such as PRESTO) together with appropriate computing power
oVer real time images of dynamic variations in water characteristics reflecting
perfusion, diVusion, oxygenation, and flow; qualitative and in some instances
quantitative maps of regional organ perfusion can thus be obtained (Fig. 4).
A B
C D
FIG. 4. Perfusion-weighted imaging (PWI): Time to peak (A), mean transit time (B), relative
cerebral blood flow (C), and relative cerebral blood volume (D) maps are displayed. These and other
possible maps that characterize entity and timing of tissue perfusivity are powerful and sensitive tools
for summarizing the dynamic changes conveyed by PWI scans.
56 DI PERRI et al.
C. METABOLIC IMAGING
A B
C D
FIG. 5. Multi-voxel sMR: a TE ¼ 25 ms (A) and TE ¼ 144 ms (B) scan are reported with the fitted
spectrum and estimated peak ratio reported for each voxel. A mixed Gaussian-Lorenztian peak shape
was chosen for the fit as the most general. Single-voxel sMR: a TE ¼ 25 ms (C) and a TE ¼ 136 ms
(D) scan are reported with the corresponding fit overlaid. The higher biochemical specificity of short TE
scans is evident at the expense of the less accurate fit due to an irregular baseline and to peak overlap.
A. TERMINOLOGY
After AD, CVD is recognized as the second most common cause of acquired
cognitive impairment and dementia, alone or in conjunction with other neurode-
generative disorders (O’Brien, 2006).
Progress in the concept of vascular cognitive impairment (VCI) has been
seriously limited by diYculties in finding agreement with respect to terminology.
O’Brien et al. (2003) have proposed using VCI as an umbrella term, including all
aspects and degrees of cognitive impairment resulting from vascular brain dam-
age. Conversely, some authors favor regarding vascular dementia (VaD) and VCI
as two diVerent entities (Roman et al., 2004). Although several definitions exist,
both DSM-IV and ICD-10 require the presence of memory impairment as an
absolute requirement, in addition to one or more other cognitive domains
being aVected. This has been largely criticized, since the presence of memory
MRI AND COGNITION IN VASCULAR DISEASES 59
B. CLASSIFICATION
FIG. 6. Acute stage of left medium cerebral artery ischemic stroke: as shown on the images, the
ischemic acute stage is visible on DWI-weighted images earlier than on conventional MRI: (A) ADC
(B) eADC, and (C) conventional T2-weighted MRI scan.
alcoholics, who have evidence of brain atrophy. Cerebral atrophy increases the
length the bridging veins have to traverse between the two meningeal layers (dura
and arachnoidea), hence increasing the likelihood of shearing forces causing
a tear. It is also more common in patients on anticoagulants, who can have a
subdural hematoma with a minor injury. Chronic subdural bleeds can develop
over a period of days to weeks, often after minor head trauma, though such a
cause is not identifiable in 50% of patients. They may not be discovered until they
present clinically months or years after a head injury and they are rather common
in the elderly (Kushner, 1998) (Fig. 7).
Strategic-infarct dementia. It is characterized by focal, often small, ischemic
lesions involving specific sites critical for specific higher cortical functions.
Cortical sites more often aVected are the hippocampal formation, angular gyrus
and cingulate gyrus and thalamic infarcts (bilateral or monolateral).
Dementia caused by specific arteriopathies. Arteriopathies such as cerebral vasculitis,
that is, acute or chronic inflammatory changes of small, medium, and large
arteries or veins, can be cause of multiple ischemic lesions and lead to VCI.
They are often accompanied by systemic signs of fever, malaise, and weight loss.
Some rare arteriopathies such as inflammatory arteriopathy (e.g., polyarteritis
nodosa, temporal arteritis) and noninflammatory arteriopathy (e.g., moyamoya
disease, fibromuscular dysplasia) can cause multiple infarcts and lead to vascular
dementia as well. In cerebral amyloid angiopathy-associated vasculopathy, aneu-
rysm formation and stenosis in the leptomeningeal and cortical vessels cause
damage to the subcortical WM. In hereditary cystatin-C amyloid angiopathy,
patients have recurrent cerebral hemorrhages before the age of 40 that can lead
to dementia.
FIG. 7. Coronal and axial images of subdural hemorrhage on conventional MRI T2-weighted
sequence (A and B).
62 DI PERRI et al.
FIG. 8. Axial FLAIR sequence displays sample images of CADASIL MRI pattern of conventional
MRI in a 32 y.o. patient.
64 DI PERRI et al.
C. HISTOPATHOLOGY OF VCI
Vascular disease produces either focal or diVuse eVects on the brain and may
cause cognitive decline. Focal CVD occurs mainly secondarily to thrombotic or
embolic vascular occlusions. Common areas of the brain associated with cognitive
decline are the WM of the cerebral hemispheres and the deep GM nuclei, especially
the striatum and the thalamus. Hypertension is the major cause of diVuse disease
and, in many patients, both focal and diVuse disease forms are observed together.
The five most common pathological findings of VaD are white matter hyperinten-
sities (WMH), macroscopic, lacunar and/or microscopic infarcts and microbleeds.
2. Macroscopic Infarcts
Infarctions involving areas of major cerebral arteries may result in large
territorial lesions with a variable amount of tissue loss. The infarcts develop in
typical stages of gliosis with increased water content. The intensity of gliotic
scarring is used in pathological studies to judge the degree and age of
the infarction ( Jellinger, 2007). Hypertension, diabetes, hypercolesterolemia,
smoking, and heart disease are the most common risk factors for infarcts.
Multiple cortical infarcts are believed to be the neuropathological cause of
multi-infarct dementia (Hachinski et al., 1974). In multi-infarct dementia, the
combined eVects of diVerent infarcts produce cognitive impairment by aVecting
neural networks. The cognitive impact of large complete infarcts depends on
localization and the extent of the ischemic lesion.
3. Lacunar Infarcts
Lacunes (small infarcts in WM and deep GM structures with diameters
ranging from 3 to 15 mm) ( Jellinger, 2007), represent diVerent stages of ischemic
injury exhibiting loss of axons and myelin together with reactive changes (Ishii
et al., 1986). In pathological studies, they have been associated with cognitive
MRI AND COGNITION IN VASCULAR DISEASES 65
4. Microscopic Infarcts
Recently, there has been increasing focus on the contribution of cortical micro-
infarcts (not visualized by current available neuroimaging techniques) to cognitive
decline both in normal aging (Kovari et al., 2004), VaD and mixed dementia (Gold
et al., 2007a). Cortical microinfarcts might be as strong a predictor of cognitive
decline as is the presence of neurofibrillary tangles in AD (Gold et al., 2007b).
5. Microbleeds
Microbleeds (MBs), characterized histopathologically by the presence of
hemosiderin around small vessels (Fazekas et al., 1999), display themselves in
several ways, mainly as hypertensive arteriopathy or amyloid angiopathy
(Fazekas et al., 1999). MBs are often detected in demented individuals, such as
patients with AD (Hanyu et al., 2003), CADASIL (Dichgans et al., 2002), subcortical
vascular dementia (Won Seo et al., 2007) or memory loss (Cordonnier et al., 2006).
Cordonnier et al. (2006) studied the prevalence of MBs in a large cohort of patients
(772 patients) attending a memory clinic. They found that 65% of patients with
vascular dementia exhibited MBs, vs 18% of AD patients, 20% of mild cognitive
impairment patients, and 10% of patients with subjective complaints. The
presence of MBs was associated with age, WMH, lacunar infarcts, and infarcts.
This finding of a relatively high proportion of MBs in AD and mild cognitive
impairment also provides further evidence for the involvement of vascular factors
in neurodegenerative diseases such as AD.
Of the available diagnostic criteria for VaD, only two of them, the NINDS-
AIREN (Roman et al., 1993) and the State of California AD Diagnostic and
Treatment Centers (ADDTC) criteria (Chui et al., 1992), require radiological
findings of CVD in the form of infarcts or WMH.
E. THERAPEUTIC STRATEGIES
FIG. 9. T2w (A) and GRE axial images (B) in a case of amyloid angiopathy.
MRI AND COGNITION IN VASCULAR DISEASES 67
Conventional MRI techniques play an important role both for diagnosis and
prognosis in VCI patients. Our knowledge of both incidental and disease-related
findings has greatly increased during the last decades and allowed a better
understanding of the pathological basis of VCI. However, VCI shows no patho-
gnomonic imaging features. Infarct location often does not correlate with the
cognitive profile, and neuroimaging can establish the chronology of lesions only
with certain limits and cannot oVer information about the relative contribution of
neurodegenerative versus ischemic processes to the clinical presentation.
FIG. 10. Normal findings in healthy individuals on conventional MRI: (A) axial FLAIR image,
(B) axial T2-weighted image.
68 DI PERRI et al.
common in the elderly (de Leeuw et al., 2001; Liao et al., 1997; Wen and
Sachdev, 2004). In a population-based study, de Leeuw et al. (2001) showed
that, of 1077 subjects between 60 and 90 years of age, only 5% had no WMH
detectable on MRI. Other studies have shown up to 90% prevalence of WMH
(Sachdev et al., 2005). Most studies show that women have more WMH changes
than men (de Leeuw et al., 2001; Sachdev et al., 2007; van den Heuvel et al., 2004),
while some others show contradictory findings (Wen and Sachdev, 2004).
Accumulation of WMH around the ventricles, also called leukoaraiosis,
appears as a bright signal on T2-WI, indicating pathology involving increased
water content and gliotic scarring in the WM of the brain. The FLAIR sequence
is commonly used for detection of these WMH. Typical WMH in subcortical
ischemic disease include extensive periventricular and deep WM signal abnorm-
alities, usually located in the genu and anterior limb of the internal capsule,
anterior corona radiata and anterior centrum semiovale (O’Brien et al., 2003).
In nondisabled elderly living independently, age-dependent WMH have been
recently reported as relating to both global cognitive dysfunction and various specific
cognitive performances such as executive function, attention, and speed processing
(Verdelho et al., 2007). Despite evidence that age-related WMH influence cognitive
status, some studies report that there is no such relationship, suggesting that MRI
might be oversensitive when it comes to WMH, and that higher water content in the
brain does not necessarily result in loss of function (Schmidtke and Hull, 2005). The
association between WMH (especially low grade) and cognition in nondemented
individuals is at the present time complex and not fully understood.
In subjects aVected by VaD, the NINDS-AIREN criteria (Roman et al., 1993)
require that WMH involve at least 25% of total brain WM. In order to be
significant, WMH must be diVuse and characterized by irregular periventricular
lesions extending into deep WM, sparing areas thought to be protected from
perfusion insuYciency (e.g., subcortical U-fibers and external capsule-claustrum-
extreme capsule). Studies on the relationship between WMH in VaD and cogni-
tive impairment have led to contradictory results (Cohen et al., 2002; Fernando
and Ince, 2004).This can be explained by the fact that the relationship between
WMH and dementia might be driven by a threshold eVect (Libon et al., 2008;
Wright et al., 2008), which implies that a certain amount of WMH is needed to
have clinical consequence. Once this threshold is reached, other factors might
contribute more to cognitive impairment than WMH volume (Sweet et al., 2003).
In VaMCI patients, recent studies have also found an association between WMH
volume and deficits on cognitive tests (Debette et al., 2007; Sachdev et al., 2006;
Yoshita et al., 2006), especially for executive dysfunction (Bombois et al., 2007).
Furthermore, periventricular WMH have been found to predict conversion to
VaD and mixed dementia, but not to increase the risk of developing other
dementias like AD, DLB, and fronto-temporal dementia (Bombois et al., 2008).
MRI AND COGNITION IN VASCULAR DISEASES 69
3. Microbleeds
In patients with lobar intracranial hemorrhage, cognitive decline has been
shown to be more severe in subjects with larger numbers of MBs at baseline
(Werring et al., 2004). In a recent study, MBs appear to be primarily associated
with global cognitive impairment even in subjects with no history of neurological
disorder (Cordonnier et al., 2006; Werring et al., 2004).
4. Hereditary VCI
In hereditary VCI such as CADASIL, conventional MRI reveals extensive
cerebral WM lesions, subcortical infarcts and MBs. CADASIL is characterized by
WMH on T2-WI in the subcortical WM and basal ganglia. Two major types of
abnormalities were first observed by Skehan et al. (1995) and later confirmed by
other studies (Lesnik Oberstein et al., 2003). The most striking findings were large
confluent patches of high-signal change on T2- and proton density-weighted
images present throughout the white matter, especially in the anterior part
of the temporal lobes and the periventricular portion of the occipital lobes.
Additionally, small linear and punctuate lacunes were detected, present not only
70 DI PERRI et al.
in the periventricular white matter but also in the brain stem, basal ganglia,
thalamus, external capsule, and corpus callosum (Skehan et al., 1995).
One of the main disabling symptoms of this disease is the cognitive
impairment, providing CADASIL as a pure VCI entity. A recent study by
Viswanathan et al. (2007) showed that, among the lesions observed on conven-
tional MRI in CADASIL, the overall lacunar lesion burden seems to have the
most important impact on cognitive function and disability. These findings
suggest that preventive strategies to decrease the risk of lacunar lesions as
observed on MRI may reduce disease-related impairment in CADASIL. These
results suggest that lacunar lesions may also play a key role in disability and
cognitive impairment in more common forms of small-vessel disease.
5. Brain Atrophy
Analyses estimating brain volumes have been widely used with respect to
ischemic pathology. In normal aging, a large epidemiological study found that
high WMH volume correlated with GM atrophy (Wen et al., 2006). Medial
temporal lobe atrophy (MTA) was recently demonstrated to predict cognitive
status after stroke alone (Firbank et al., 2007) or in conjunction with brain infarct,
WMH volume, and the subject’s educational level (Pohjasvaara et al., 2000).
In CADASIL, atrophy is currently investigated as an additional important
characteristic of the disease (O’Sullivan et al., 2007; Peters et al., 2006). Cerebral
volumetric assessment might thus be an additional aspect in understanding the
pathogenesis behind neuropsychiatric and neurobehavioral changes in ischemic
brain diseases. A limitation to keep in mind is that brain atrophy might develop
rather late in the course of some diseases and for the time being serves mostly to
characterize the natural development of the pathology.
B. NONCONVENTIONAL TECHNIQUES
WMH volume in patients with ischemic lesions. The correlation was strongest for
diVusivity of normal appearing WM (NAWM), and remained significant after
controlling for conventional MRI parameters, including brain parenchymal
volume and T1 and WMH volumes. Regarding conventional measurements,
brain volume predicted cognitive impairment best. Of special interest is the
finding that DWI/DTI was able to detect specific pathology in NAWM, demon-
strating the technique as a highly sensitive and early predictor of brain damage.
This is in line with a pathological study combining postmortem MRI and
histology (Brown et al., 2007), and shows the great potential of newer imaging
techniques. DWI might thus serve as a promising imaging technique for further
knowledge about vascular changes in the brain not visible on traditional MRI
scans and for exploring the mechanisms of cognitive dysfunction in these patients.
C. METABOLIC IMAGING
frontal NAA/Cr ratio might serve as a possible biomarker for identifying patients
at risk for cognitive decline after stroke/TIA.
D. FUNCTIONAL IMAGING
1. Perfusion-Weighted Imaging
Zimny et al. (2007) studied 64 patients with dementia with diVerent degrees of
cognitive impairment to assess the relationship between cognitive impairment
according to the MMSE and values of CBF, CBV, and MTT obtained in
perfusion CT (pCT). The results of this study showed that CBF and CBV
calculated with pCT correlate with cognitive impairment in patients with demen-
tia and thus may play a role in monitoring disease progression or therapeutic
response (Zimny et al., 2007). This may be a good setting for developing new
studies using either pCT or perfusion MRI, the latter losing in spatial resolution
but gaining in tissue specificity.
IV. Conclusions
Acknowledgments
Dr Di Perri was supported by the Dr. Larry D. Jacobs Fellowship of the BuValo Neuroimaging
Analysis Center. Dr Dalaker was supported by the Dr. Larry D. Jacobs Fellowship of the BuValo
Neuroimaging Analysis Center and a grant from the Research Council of Norway (grant# 186966).
Dr Beyer is supported by funds from the Norwegian Society of Radiology. The authors thank Eve
Salczynski for technical assistance in the preparation of this manuscript.
References
Babikian, V., and Ropper, A. H. (1987). Binswanger’s disease: A review. Stroke 18(1), 2–12.
Barba, R., Martinez-Espinosa, S., Rodriguez-Garcia, E., Pondal, M., Vivancos, J., and Del Ser, T.
(2000). Poststroke dementia: Clinical features and risk factors. Stroke 31(7), 1494–1501.
Bombois, S., Debette, S., Delbeuck, X., Bruandet, A., Lepoittevin, S., Delmaire, C., Leys, D., and
Pasquier, F. (2007). Prevalence of subcortical vascular lesions and association with executive
function in mild cognitive impairment subtypes. Stroke 38(9), 2595–2597.
Bombois, S., Debette, S., Bruandet, A., Delbeuck, X., Delmaire, C., Leys, D., and Pasquier, F. (2008).
Vascular subcortical hyperintensities predict conversion to vascular and mixed dementia in MCI
patients. Stroke 39, 2046–2051.
Brown, W. R., Moody, D. M., Thore, C. R., Challa, V. R., and Anstrom, J. A. (2007). Vascular
dementia in leukoaraiosis may be a consequence of capillary loss not only in the lesions, but in
normal-appearing white matter and cortex as well. J. Neurol. Sci. 257(1-2), 62–66.
MRI AND COGNITION IN VASCULAR DISEASES 75
Busto, R., Dietrich, W. D., Globus, M. Y., Valdes, I., Scheinberg, P., and Ginsberg, M. D. (1987).
Small diVerences in intraischemic brain temperature critically determine the extent of ischemic
neuronal injury. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 7(6), 729–738.
Carey, C. L., Kramer, J. H., Josephson, S. A., Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., SchuV, N., Weiner, M. W.,
and Chui, H. C. (2008). Subcortical lacunes are associated with executive dysfunction in
cognitively normal elderly. Stroke 39(2), 397–402.
Chui, H. C., VictoroV, J. I., Margolin, D., Jagust, W., Shankle, R., and Katzman, R. (1992). Criteria
for the diagnosis of ischemic vascular dementia proposed by the State of California Alzheimer’s
Disease Diagnostic and Treatment Centers. Neurology 42(3 Pt 1), 473–480.
Clarke, S., Assal, G., Bogousslavsky, J., Regli, F., Townsend, D. W., Leenders, K. L., and Blecic, S.
(1994). Pure amnesia after unilateral left polar thalamic infarct: Topographic and sequential
neuropsychological and metabolic (PET) correlations. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 57(1), 27–34.
Cohen, R. A., Paul, R. H., Ott, B. R., Moser, D. J., Zawacki, T. M., Stone, W., and Gordon, N. (2002).
The relationship of subcortical MRI hyperintensities and brain volume to cognitive function in
vascular dementia. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 8(6), 743–752.
Cordonnier, C., van der Flier, W. M., Sluimer, J. D., Leys, D., Barkhof, F., and Scheltens, P. (2006).
Prevalence and severity of microbleeds in a memory clinic setting. Neurology 66(9), 1356–1360.
Crain, M. R., Yuh, W. T., Greene, G. M., Loes, D. J., Ryals, T. J., Sato, Y., and Hart, M. N. (1991).
Cerebral ischemia: Evaluation with contrast-enhanced MR imaging. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol.
12(4), 631–639.
Cummings, J. L. (1994). Vascular subcortical dementias: Clinical aspects. Dementia 5(3-4), 177–180.
de la Torre, J. C. (2004). Alzheimer’s disease is a vasocognopathy: A new term to describe its nature.
Neurol. Res. 26(5), 517–524.
de Leeuw, F. E., de Groot, J. C., Achten, E., Oudkerk, M., Ramos, L. M., Heijboer, R., Hofman, A.,
Jolles, J., van Gijn, J., and Breteler, M. M. (2001). Prevalence of cerebral white matter lesions in
elderly people: A population based magnetic resonance imaging study. The Rotterdam Scan
Study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 70(1), 9–14.
Debette, S., Bombois, S., Bruandet, A., Delbeuck, X., Lepoittevin, S., Delmaire, C., Leys, D., and
Pasquier, F. (2007). Subcortical hyperintensities are associated with cognitive decline in patients
with mild cognitive impairment. Stroke 38(11), 2924–2930.
Decarli, C. (2004). Vascular factors in dementia: An overview. J. Neurol. Sci. 226(1-2), 19–23.
Dichgans, M., Holtmannspotter, M., Herzog, J., Peters, N., Bergmann, M., and Yousry, T. A. (2002).
Cerebral microbleeds in CADASIL: A gradient-echo magnetic resonance imaging and autopsy
study. Stroke 33(1), 67–71.
Enzinger, C., Fazekas, F., Matthews, P. M., Ropele, S., Schmidt, H., Smith, S., and Schmidt, R.
(2005). Risk factors for progression of brain atrophy in aging: Six-year follow-up of normal
subjects. Neurology 64(10), 1704–1711.
Enzinger, C., Smith, S., Fazekas, F., Drevin, G., Ropele, S., Nichols, T., Behrens, T., Schmidt, R., and
Matthews, P. M. (2006). Lesion probability maps of white matter hyperintensities in elderly
individuals: Results of the Austrian stroke prevention study. J. Neurol. 253(8), 1064–1070.
Erkinjuntti, T. (2007). Vascular cognitive deterioration and stroke. Cerebrovasc. Dis. 24(Suppl. 1),
189–194.
Erkinjuntti, T., Kurz, A., Gauthier, S., Bullock, R., Lilienfeld, S., and Damaraju, C. V. (2002). EYcacy
of galantamine in probable vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease combined with cerebro-
vascular disease: A randomised trial. Lancet 359(9314), 1283–1290.
Fazekas, F. (1989). Magnetic resonance signal abnormalities in asymptomatic individuals: Their
incidence and functional correlates. Eur. Neurol. 29(3), 164–168.
Fazekas, F., Kleinert, R., OVenbacher, H., Schmidt, R., Kleinert, G., Payer, F., Radner, H., and
Lechner, H. (1993). Pathologic correlates of incidental MRI white matter signal hyperintensities.
Neurology 43(9), 1683–1689.
76 DI PERRI et al.
Fazekas, F., Kleinert, R., Roob, G., Kleinert, G., Kapeller, P., Schmidt, R., and Hartung, H. P. (1999).
Histopathologic analysis of foci of signal loss on gradient-echo T2*-weighted MR images in
patients with spontaneous intracerebral hemorrhage: Evidence of microangiopathy-related
microbleeds. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 20(4), 637–642.
Federico, A., Bianchi, S., and Dotti, M. T. (2005). The spectrum of mutations for CADASIL diagnosis.
Neurol. Sci. 26(2), 117–124.
Fein, G., Di Sclafani, V., Tanabe, J., Cardenas, V., Weiner, M. W., Jagust, W. J., Reed, B. R.,
Norman, D., SchuV, N., Kusdra, L., Greenfield, T., and Chui, H. (2000). Hippocampal and cortical
atrophy predict dementia in subcortical ischemic vascular disease. Neurology 55(11), 1626–1635.
Fernando, M. S., and Ince, P. G. (2004). Vascular pathologies and cognition in a population-based
cohort of elderly people. J. Neurol. Sci. 226(1-2), 13–17.
Fernando, M. S., Simpson, J. E., Matthews, F., Brayne, C., Lewis, C. E., Barber, R., Kalaria, R. N.,
Forster, G., Esteves, F., Wharton, S. B., Shaw, P. J., O’Brien, J. T., et al. (2006). White matter
lesions in an unselected cohort of the elderly: Molecular pathology suggests origin from chronic
hypoperfusion injury. Stroke 37(6), 1391–1398.
Firbank, M. J., Burton, E. J., Barber, R., Stephens, S., Kenny, R. A., Ballard, C., Kalaria, R. N., and
O’Brien, J. T. (2007). Medial temporal atrophy rather than white matter hyperintensities predict
cognitive decline in stroke survivors. Neurobiol. Aging 28(11), 1664–1669.
Gold, G., Kovari, E., Herrmann, F. R., Canuto, A., Hof, P. R., Michel, J. P., Bouras, C., and
Giannakopoulos, P. (2005). Cognitive consequences of thalamic, basal ganglia, and deep white
matter lacunes in brain aging and dementia. Stroke 36(6), 1184–1188.
Gold, G., Giannakopoulos, P., Herrmann, F. R., Bouras, C., and Kovari, E. (2007a). Identification of
Alzheimer and vascular lesion thresholds for mixed dementia. Brain 130(Pt 11), 2830–2836.
Gold, G., Kovari, E., Hof, P. R., Bouras, C., and Giannakopoulos, P. (2007b). Sorting out the clinical
consequences of ischemic lesions in brain aging: A clinicopathological approach. J. Neurol. Sci.
257(1-2), 17–22.
Hachinski, V. C., Lassen, N. A., and Marshall, J. (1974). Multi-infarct dementia. A cause of mental
deterioration in the elderly. Lancet 2(7874), 207–210.
Hanyu, H., Tanaka, Y., Shimizu, S., Takasaki, M., and Abe, K. (2003). Cerebral microbleeds in
Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol. 250(12), 1496–1497.
Horsfield, M. A., Barker, G. J., Barkhof, F., Miller, D. H., Thompson, A. J., and Filippi, M. (2003).
Guidelines for using quantitative magnetization transfer magnetic resonance imaging for moni-
toring treatment of multiple sclerosis. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 17(4), 389–397.
Ishii, N., Nishihara, Y., and Imamura, T. (1986). Why do frontal lobe symptoms predominate in
vascular dementia with lacunes? Neurology 36(3), 340–345.
Jagust, W. J., Zheng, L., Harvey, D. J., Mack, W. J., Vinters, H. V., Weiner, M. W., Ellis, W. G.,
Zarow, C., Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., Kramer, J. H., SchuV, N., et al. (2008). Neuropathological
basis of magnetic resonance images in aging and dementia. Ann. Neurol. 63(1), 72–80.
Jellinger, K. A. (2007). The enigma of vascular cognitive disorder and vascular dementia. Acta
Neuropathol. 113(4), 349–388.
Kapeller, P., Schmidt, R., and Fazekas, F. (2004). Qualitative MRI: Evidence of usual aging in the
brain. Top. Magn. Reson. Imaging 15(6), 343–347.
Kerrouche, N., Herholz, K., Mielke, R., HolthoV, V., and Baron, J. C. (2006). 18FDG PET in vascular
dementia: DiVerentiation from Alzheimer’s disease using voxel-based multivariate analysis.
J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 26(9), 1213–1221.
Korf, E. S., White, L. R., Scheltens, P., and Launer, L. J. (2006). Brain aging in very old men with
type 2 diabetes: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Diabetes Care 29(10), 2268–2274.
Kovari, E., Gold, G., Herrmann, F. R., Canuto, A., Hof, P. R., Michel, J. P., Bouras, C., and
Giannakopoulos, P. (2004). Cortical microinfarcts and demyelination significantly aVect cognition
in brain aging. Stroke 35(2), 410–414.
MRI AND COGNITION IN VASCULAR DISEASES 77
Kushner, D. (1998). Mild traumatic brain injury: Toward understanding manifestations and
treatment. Arch Intern. Med. 158(15), 1617–1624.
Lesnik Oberstein, S. A., van Duinen, S. G., van den Boom, R., Maat-Schieman, M. L.,
van Buchem, M. A., van Houwelingen, H. C., Hegeman-Kleinn, I. M., Ferrari, M. D.,
Breuning, M. H., and Haan, J. (2003). Evaluation of diagnostic NOTCH3 immunostaining in
CADASIL. Acta Neuropathol. 106(2), 107–111.
Leys, D., Henon, H., Mackowiak-Cordoliani, M. A., and Pasquier, F. (2005). Poststroke dementia.
Lancet Neurol. 4(11), 752–759.
Liao, D., Cooper, L., Cai, J., Toole, J., Bryan, N., Burke, G., Shahar, E., Nieto, J., Mosley, T., and
Heiss, G. (1997). The prevalence and severity of white matter lesions, their relationship with age,
ethnicity, gender, and cardiovascular disease risk factors: The ARIC Study. Neuroepidemiology 16(3),
149–162.
Libon, D. J., Price, C. C., Giovannetti, T., Swenson, R., Bettcher, B. M., Heilman, K. M., and
Pennisi, A. (2008). Linking MRI hyperintensities with patterns of neuropsychological impairment:
Evidence for a threshold eVect. Stroke 39(3), 806–813.
Liem, M. K., van der Grond, J., Haan, J., van den Boom, R., Ferrari, M. D., Knaap, Y. M.,
Breuning, M. H., van Buchem, M. A., Middelkoop, H. A., and Lesnik Oberstein, S. A. (2007).
Lacunar infarcts are the main correlate with cognitive dysfunction in CADASIL. Stroke 38(3),
923–928.
Linden, T., Skoog, I., Fagerberg, B., Steen, B., and Blomstrand, C. (2004). Cognitive impairment and
dementia 20 months after stroke. Neuroepidemiology 23(1-2), 45–52.
Lojkowska, W., Ryglewicz, D., Jedrzejczak, T., Sienkiewicz-Jarosz, H., Minc, S., Jakubowska, T., and
Kozlowicz-Gudzinska, I. (2002). SPECT as a diagnostic test in the investigation of dementia.
J. Neurol. Sci. 203-204, 215–219.
Lovblad, K. O., Baird, A. E., Schlaug, G., Benfield, A., Siewert, B., Voetsch, B., Connor, A.,
Burzynski, C., Edelman, R. R., and Warach, S. (1997). Ischemic lesion volumes in acute stroke
by diVusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging correlate with clinical outcome. Ann. Neurol.
42(2), 164–170.
Maxeiner, H., and WolV, M. (2007). Pure subdural hematomas: A postmortem analysis of their form
and bleeding points. Neurosurgery 61(1 Suppl.), 267-272; discussion 272–273.
Meyer, J. S., Rogers, R. L., McClintic, K., Mortel, K. F., and Lotfi, J. (1989). Randomized clinical trial
of daily aspirin therapy in multi-infarct dementia. A pilot study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 37(6), 549–555.
Mielke, R., Pietrzyk, U., Jacobs, A., Fink, G. R., Ichimiya, A., Kessler, J., Herholz, K., and
Heiss, W. D. (1994). HMPAO SPET and FDG PET in Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia:
Comparison of perfusion and metabolic pattern. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. 21(10), 1052–1060.
Murray, A. D., StaV, R. T., Shenkin, S. D., Deary, I. J., Starr, J. M., and Whalley, L. J. (2005). Brain
white matter hyperintensities: Relative importance of vascular risk factors in nondemented elderly
people. Radiology 237(1), 251–257.
Nagata, K., Maruya, H., Yuya, H., Terashi, H., Mito, Y., Kato, H., Sato, M., Satoh, Y., Watahiki, Y.,
Hirata, Y., Yokoyama, E., and Hatazawa, J. (2000). Can PET data diVerentiate Alzheimer’s
disease from vascular dementia? Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 903, 252–261.
Nagata, K., Saito, H., Ueno, T., Sato, M., Nakase, T., Maeda, T., Satoh, Y., Komatsu, H., Suzuki, M.,
and Kondoh, Y. (2007). Clinical diagnosis of vascular dementia. J. Neurol. Sci. 257(1-2), 44–48.
Narayana, P. A. (2005). Magnetic resonance spectroscopy in the monitoring of multiple sclerosis.
J. Neuroimaging 15(4 Suppl.), 46S–57S.
Narayanan, S., Francis, S. J., Sled, J. G., Santos, A. C., Antel, S., Levesque, I., Brass, S., Lapierre, Y.,
Sappey-Marinier, D., Pike, G. B., and Arnold, D. L. (2006). Axonal injury in the cerebral normal-
appearing white matter of patients with multiple sclerosis is related to concurrent demyelination in
lesions but not to concurrent demyelination in normal-appearing white matter. Neuroimage 29(2),
637–642.
78 DI PERRI et al.
Nicoll, J. A., Yamada, M., Frackowiak, J., Mazur-Kolecka, B., and Weller, R. O. (2004). Cerebral
amyloid angiopathy plays a direct role in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease. Pro-CAA
position statement. Neurobiol. Aging 25(5), 589–597; discussion 603-4.
O’Brien, J. T. (2006). Vascular cognitive impairment. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 14(9), 724–733.
O’Brien, J. T., Erkinjuntti, T., Reisberg, B., Roman, G., Sawada, T., Pantoni, L., Bowler, J. V.,
Ballard, C., DeCarli, C., Gorelick, P. B., Rockwood, K., Burns, A., et al. (2003). Vascular cognitive
impairment. Lancet Neurol. 2(2), 89–98.
O’Sullivan, M., Morris, R. G., Huckstep, B., Jones, D. K., Williams, S. C., and Markus, H. S. (2004).
DiVusion tensor MRI correlates with executive dysfunction in patients with ischaemic leukoar-
aiosis. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 75(3), 441–447.
O’Sullivan, M., Ngo, E., Viswanathan, A., Jouvent, E., Gschwendtner, A., Saemann, P. G.,
Duering, M., Pachai, C., Bousser, M. G., Chabriat, H., and Dichgans, M. (2007). Hippocampal
volume is an independent predictor of cognitive performance in CADASIL. Neurobiol. Aging.
In press.
Peters, N., Holtmannspotter, M., Opherk, C., Gschwendtner, A., Herzog, J., Samann, P., and
Dichgans, M. (2006). Brain volume changes in CADASIL: A serial MRI study in pure subcortical
ischemic vascular disease. Neurology 66(10), 1517–1522.
Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., and Kokmen, E. (1999).
Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Arch. Neurol. 56(3), 303–308.
Pohjasvaara, T., Mantyla, R., Salonen, O., Aronen, H. J., Ylikoski, R., Hietanen, M., Kaste, M., and
Erkinjuntti, T. (2000). How complex interactions of ischemic brain infarcts, white matter lesions,
and atrophy relate to poststroke dementia. Arch. Neurol. 57(9), 1295–1300.
Pratt, R. D., and Perdomo, C. A. (2002). Donepezil-treated patients with probable vascular dementia
demonstrate cognitive benefits. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 977, 513–522.
Reed, B. R., Eberling, J. L., Mungas, D., Weiner, M., Kramer, J. H., and Jagust, W. J. (2004). EVects of
white matter lesions and lacunes on cortical function. Arch. Neurol. 61(10), 1545–1550.
Rockwood, K., Wentzel, C., Hachinski, V., Hogan, D. B., MacKnight, C., and McDowell, I. (2000).
Prevalence and outcomes of vascular cognitive impairment. Vascular Cognitive Impairment
Investigators of the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. Neurology 54(2), 447–451.
Roman, G. C., Tatemichi, T. K., Erkinjuntti, T., Cummings, J. L., Masdeu, J. C., Garcia, J. H.,
Amaducci, L., Orgogozo, J. -M., Brun, A., Hofman, A., Moody, D. M., O’Brien, M. D., et al.
(1993). Vascular dementia: Diagnostic criteria for research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN
International Workshop. Neurology 43(2), 250–260.
Roman, G. C., Sachdev, P., Royall, D. R., Bullock, R. A., Orgogozo, J. M., Lopez-Pousa, S.,
Arizaga, R., and Wallin, A. (2004). Vascular cognitive disorder: A new diagnostic category
updating vascular cognitive impairment and vascular dementia. J. Neurol. Sci. 226(1-2), 81–87.
Ross, A. J., Sachdev, P. S., Wen, W., Brodaty, H., Joscelyne, A., and Lorentz, L. M. (2006). Prediction of
cognitive decline after stroke using proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. J. Neurol. Sci. 251(1-2),
62–69.
Rothwell, P. M., Coull, A. J., Giles, M. F., Howard, S. C., Silver, L. E., Bull, L. M., Gutnikov, S. A.,
Edwards, P., Mant, D., Sackley, C. M., Farmer, A., Sandercock, P. A., et al. (2004). Change in
stroke incidence, mortality, case-fatality, severity, and risk factors in Oxfordshire, UK from 1981
to 2004 (Oxford Vascular Study). Lancet 363(9425), 1925–1933.
Rovaris, M., Gass, A., Bammer, R., Hickman, S. J., Ciccarelli, O., Miller, D. H., and Filippi, M.
(2005). DiVusion MRI in multiple sclerosis. Neurology 65(10), 1526–1532.
Sachdev, P. S., Wen, W., Christensen, H., and Jorm, A. F. (2005). White matter hyperintensities are
related to physical disability and poor motor function. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 76(3), 362–367.
Sachdev, P. S., Brodaty, H., Valenzuela, M. J., Lorentz, L., Looi, J. C., Berman, K., Ross, A., Wen, W.,
and Zagami, A. S. (2006). Clinical determinants of dementia and mild cognitive impairment
following ischaemic stroke: The Sydney Stroke Study. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord 21(5-6), 275–283.
MRI AND COGNITION IN VASCULAR DISEASES 79
Sachdev, P. S., Parslow, R., Wen, W., Anstey, K. J., and Easteal, S. (2007). Sex diVerences in the causes
and consequences of white matter hyperintensities. Neurobiol. Aging. In Press.
Saha, G. (2006). ‘‘Physics and Radiobiology of Nuclear Medicine,’’ 3rd edition. Springer, New York.
Schmidt, R., Petrovic, K., Ropele, S., Enzinger, C., and Fazekas, F. (2007). Progression of leukoar-
aiosis and cognition. Stroke 38(9), 2619–2625.
Schmidtke, K., and Hull, M. (2005). Cerebral small vessel disease: How does it progress? J. Neurol. Sci.
229-230, 13–20.
Silverman, D. H. (2004). Brain 18F-FDG PET in the diagnosis of neurodegenerative dementias:
Comparison with perfusion SPECT and with clinical evaluations lacking nuclear imaging. J. Nucl.
Med. 45(4), 594–607.
Skehan, S. J., Hutchinson, M., and MacErlaine, D. P. (1995). Cerebral autosomal dominant arterio-
pathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy: MR findings. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol.
16(10), 2115–2119.
Skoog, I., Andreasson, L. A., Landahl, S., and Lernfelt, B. (1998). A population-based study on blood
pressure and brain atrophy in 85-year-olds. Hypertension 32(3), 404–409.
Sobesky, J., Zaro Weber, O., Lehnhardt, F. G., Hesselmann, V., Neveling, M., Jacobs, A., and
Heiss, W. D. (2005). Does the mismatch match the penumbra? Magnetic resonance imaging
and positron emission tomography in early ischemic stroke. Stroke 36(5), 980–985.
Spilt, A., Goekoop, R., Westendorp, R. G., Blauw, G. J., de Craen, A. J., and van Buchem, M. A.
(2006). Not all age-related white matter hyperintensities are the same: A magnetization transfer
imaging study. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 27(9), 1964–1968.
Starkstein, S. E., Jorge, R., and Capizzano, A. A. (2005). Uncommon causes of cerebrovascular
dementia. Int. Psychogeriatr. 17(Suppl. 1), S51–64.
Sultzer, D. L., Mahler, M. E., Cummings, J. L., Van Gorp, W. G., Hinkin, C. H., and Brown, C.
(1995). Cortical abnormalities associated with subcortical lesions in vascular dementia. Clinical
and position emission tomographic findings. Arch. Neurol. 52(8), 773–780.
Sweet, L. H., Paul, R. H., Cohen, R. A., Moser, D., Ott, B. R., Gordon, N., Browndyke, J. N.,
Shah, P., and Garrett, K. D. (2003). Neuroimaging correlates of dementia rating scale perfor-
mance at baseline and 12-month follow-up among patients with vascular dementia. J. Geriatr.
Psychiatry Neurol. 16(4), 240–244.
Takahashi, W., Takagi, S., Ide, M., Shohtsu, A., and Shinohara, Y. (2000). Reduced cerebral glucose
metabolism in subjects with incidental hyperintensities on magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurol.
Sci. 176(1), 21–27.
Tatemichi, T. K., Foulkes, M. A., Mohr, J. P., Hewitt, J. R., Hier, D. B., Price, T. R., and Wolf, P. A.
(1990). Dementia in stroke survivors in the Stroke Data Bank cohort. Prevalence, incidence, risk
factors, and computed tomographic findings. Stroke 21(6), 858–866.
Tatemichi, T. K., Desmond, D. W., Mayeux, R., Paik, M., Stern, Y., Sano, M., Remien, R. H.,
Williams, J. B., Mohr, J. P., Hauser, W. A., et al. (1992). Dementia after stroke: Baseline frequency,
risks, and clinical features in a hospitalized cohort. Neurology 42(6), 1185–1193.
Tian, M., Zhang, H., Oriuchi, N., Higuchi, T., and Endo, K. (2004). Comparison of 11C-choline PET
and FDG PET for the diVerential diagnosis of malignant tumors. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging
31(8), 1064–1072.
van den Heuvel, D. M., Admiraal-Behloul, F., ten Dam, V. H., Olofsen, H., Bollen, E. L.,
Murray, H. M., Blauw, G. J., Westendorp, R. G., de Craen, A. J., and van Buchem, M. A.
(2004). DiVerent progression rates for deep white matter hyperintensities in elderly men and
women. Neurology 63(9), 1699–1701.
van der Flier, W. M., van Straaten, E. C., Barkhof, F., Verdelho, A., Madureira, S., Pantoni, L.,
Inzitari, D., Erkinjuntti, T., Crisby, M., Waldemar, G., Schmidt, R., Fazekas, F., et al. (2005).
Small vessel disease and general cognitive function in nondisabled elderly: The LADIS study.
Stroke 36(10), 2116–2120.
80 DI PERRI et al.
Vataja, R., Pohjasvaara, T., Mantyla, R., Ylikoski, R., Leppavuori, A., Leskela, M., Kalska, H.,
Hietanen, M., Aronen, H. J., Salonen, O., Kaste, M., and Erkinjuntti, T. (2003). MRI correlates
of executive dysfunction in patients with ischaemic stroke. Eur. J. Neurol. 10(6), 625–631.
Verdelho, A., Madureira, S., Ferro, J. M., Basile, A. M., Chabriat, H., Erkinjuntti, T., Fazekas, F.,
Hennerici, M., O’Brien, J., Pantoni, L., Salvadori, E., Scheltens, P., et al. (2007). DiVerential impact
of cerebral white matter changes, diabetes, hypertension and stroke on cognitive performance
among non-disabled elderly. The LADIS study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 78(12), 1325–1330.
Viswanathan, A., Gschwendtner, A., Guichard, J. P., BuVon, F., Cumurciuc, R., O’Sullivan, M.,
Holtmannspotter, M., Pachai, C., Bousser, M. G., Dichgans, M., and Chabriat, H. (2007).
Lacunar lesions are independently associated with disability and cognitive impairment in
CADASIL. Neurology 69(2), 172–179.
Vrenken, H., Barkhof, F., Uitdehaag, B. M., Castelijns, J. A., Polman, C. H., and Pouwels, P. J. (2005).
MR spectroscopic evidence for glial increase but not for neuro-axonal damage in MS normal-
appearing white matter. Magn. Reson. Med. 53(2), 256–266.
Wallin, A., Milos, V., Sjogren, M., Pantoni, L., and Erkinjuntti, T. (2003). Classification and subtypes
of vascular dementia. Int. Psychogeriatr. 15(Suppl. 1), 27–37.
Wen, W., and Sachdev, P. (2004). The topography of white matter hyperintensities on brain MRI in
healthy 60- to 64-year-old individuals. Neuroimage 22(1), 144–154.
Wen, W., Sachdev, P. S., Chen, X., and Anstey, K. (2006). Gray matter reduction is correlated with
white matter hyperintensity volume: A voxel-based morphometric study in a large epidemiologi-
cal sample. Neuroimage 29(4), 1031–1039.
Wentzel, C., Rockwood, K., MacKnight, C., Hachinski, V., Hogan, D. B., Feldman, H., Ostbye, T.,
Wolfson, C., Gauthier, S., Verreault, R., and McDowell, I. (2001). Progression of impairment in
patients with vascular cognitive impairment without dementia. Neurology 57(4), 714–716.
Werring, D. J., Frazer, D. W., Coward, L. J., LosseV, N. A., Watt, H., Cipolotti, L., Brown, M. M., and
Jager, H. R. (2004). Cognitive dysfunction in patients with cerebral microbleeds on T2*-weighted
gradient-echo MRI. Brain 127(Pt 10), 2265–2275.
Whitwell, J. L. (2008). Longitudinal imaging: Change and causality. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 21(4), 410–416.
Wolpert, S. M., Bruckmann, H., Greenlee, R., Wechsler, L., Pessin, M. S., and del Zoppo, G. J. (1993).
Neuroradiologic evaluation of patients with acute stroke treated with recombinant tissue plasmin-
ogen activator. The rt-PA Acute Stroke Study Group. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 14(1), 3–13.
Won Seo, S., Hwa Lee, B., Kim, E. J., Chin, J., Sun Cho, Y., Yoon, U., and Na, D. L. (2007). Clinical
significance of microbleeds in subcortical vascular dementia. Stroke 38(6), 1949–1951.
Wright, C. B., Festa, J. R., Paik, M. C., Schmiedigen, A., Brown, T. R., Yoshita, M., DeCarli, C.,
Sacco, R., and Stern, Y. (2008). White matter hyperintensities and subclinical infarction: Associa-
tions with psychomotor speed and cognitive flexibility. Stroke 39(3), 800–805.
Yang, D. W., Kim, B. S., Park, J. K., Kim, S. Y., Kim, E. N., and Sohn, H. S. (2002). Analysis of
cerebral blood flow of subcortical vascular dementia with single photon emission computed
tomography: Adaptation of statistical parametric mapping. J. Neurol. Sci. 203-204, 199–205.
Yoshita, M., Fletcher, E., Harvey, D., Ortega, M., Martinez, O., Mungas, D. M., Reed, B. R., and
DeCarli, C. S. (2006). Extent and distribution of white matter hyperintensities in normal aging,
MCI, and AD. Neurology 67(12), 2192–2198.
Zhu, L., Viitanen, M., Guo, Z., Winblad, B., and Fratiglioni, L. (1998). Blood pressure reduction,
cardiovascular diseases, and cognitive decline in the mini-mental state examination in a community
population of normal very old people: A three-year follow-up. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 51(5), 385–391.
Zimny, A., Leszek, J., Kiejna, A., and Sasiadek, M. (2007). Analysis of correlation between the degree
of cognitive impairment and the results of perfusion CT in patients with dementia. Med. Sci. Monit.
13(Suppl. 1), 23–30.
Zivadinov, R., and Bakshi, R. (2004). Role of MRI in multiple sclerosis I: Inflammation and lesions.
Front Biosci. 9, 665–683.
Zivadinov, R., and Cox, J. L. (2007). Neuroimaging in multiple sclerosis. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 79,
449–474.
CONTRIBUTIONS OF NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND
NEUROIMAGING TO UNDERSTANDING CLINICAL SUBTYPES OF
MILD COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT
I.Introduction
II.Neuropsychological Presentation
III.Stability of Diagnosis
IV. Conversion to Dementia
V. MCI and Health Variables
VI. Daily Functioning and MCI
VII. Neuroimaging
A. Structural MRI
B. Diffusion Tensor Imaging
C. Functional MRI
VIII. Treatment
IX. Conclusions
References
findings specific to clinical subtypes of MCI as well as the issue of daily function-
ing. Although investigations of non-amnestic subtypes using advanced neuroim-
aging techniques and clinical trials are quite limited, we briefly review these topics
in MCI because these data provide a framework for future investigations specifi-
cally examining additional clinical subtypes of MCI. Finally, the review comments
on select methodological issues involved in studying this heterogeneous popula-
tion, and future directions to continue to improve our understanding of MCI and
its clinical subtypes are oVered.
I. Introduction
Di Carlo et al., 2007; Jungwirth et al., 2005; Lopez et al., 2003), and single domain
non-amnestic MCI ranges from approximately 3 to 15% (Bickel et al., 2006; Busse
et al., 2003; Di Carlo et al., 2007). There is only limited information on prevalence
rates of multi-domain non-amnestic MCI, in which one group has identified less
than a 5% prevalence rate (Bickel et al., 2006). In addition to vastly diVerent
methodological approaches to defining MCI, the prevalence rates also diVer due
to varying sample origins, with hospital samples having generally higher preva-
lence rates across subtypes than community samples (Bickel et al., 2006; Busse
et al., 2003). Presence of the APOE e4 allele may also contribute to diVering
prevalence rates. Amnestic MCI presentations have a higher proportion of
individuals with the apolipoprotein e4 allele as compared to non-amnestic groups
(Gabryelewicz et al., 2007; Manly et al., 2005; Whitwell et al., 2007). Thus, if
genetic risk is not determined, samples are likely to have diVering proportions of
individuals with the e4 allele and, therefore, diVerent prevalence rates.
Certainly the prevalence rates are impacted by the definition of MCI applied
by each study. Basing amnestic subtype diagnoses on the presence of either verbal
and/or visual memory deficits results in a larger proportion of individuals identi-
fied as amnestic MCI than just relying on verbal memory alone (Alladi et al.,
2006). Varying the cutoV score for defining impairment also alters diagnostic
outcomes by up to 12%; use of a more stringent statistical cutoV for impairment
(1.5–2.0 SD below normative expectations) increases positive predictive power
compared to lower cut points (Busse et al., 2003); although, a more liberal cutoV
for impairment has been shown to have higher sensitivity and specificity for future
development of dementia (Busse et al., 2006). These sensitivity and specificity
determinations are problematical; however, as they are based on a quite limited
neuropsychological assessment.
Aside from the use in diagnosis and determination of objective cognitive
deficits in MCI, additional neuropsychological findings may help diVerentiate
MCI subtypes. Perhaps not surprisingly, Lopez and colleagues (2006) found that,
compared to aMCI and normal cognition, mMCI was characterized by poorer
language, psychomotor speed, fine motor control, and visuoconstructional func-
tioning. What is of note is that, although the mMCI group had memory deficits,
they were to a lesser degree than the deficits noted in the aMCI group (Lopez
et al., 2006). A substantial minority of the MCI cases did not have any memory
impairment (Lopez et al., 2006), further emphasizing that examining only amnes-
tic subtypes fails to capture the full spectrum of possible cognitive declines
associated with MCI.
Spatial navigation skills of those with multi-domain amnestic MCI tend to be
more similar to spatial navigation skills of AD patients than to non-amnestic MCI
subtype groups, with both the AD and multi-domain amnestic MCI groups
impaired on virtually all portions of a spatial navigation task (Hort et al., 2007).
However, the multi-domain amnestic MCI group was generally more impaired
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND IMAGING OF MCI SUBTYPES 85
than other groups across all neuropsychological tests, so it is not clear that these
spatial navigation diYculties occurred in isolation from the other impaired func-
tions. Visuospatial skills specific to facial emotional processing have also been
found to be intact in those with single domain amnestic MCI but impaired in
those with multi-domain amnestic MCI, particularly in facial aVect discrimina-
tion (Teng et al., 2007).
Multiple studies indicate that not all individuals diagnosed with MCI will
decline and progress to a dementia diagnosis. In fact, a proportion of individuals
appear to ‘‘improve’’ over time such that, at follow up, those initially identified as
MCI are later categorized as cognitively normal. Anywhere from 20 (Fischer et al.,
2007) to 40% (Bickel et al., 2006) of those with MCI appear to revert to the normal
range upon retesting. Single domain classifications appear particularly susceptible
to this instability, with single domain non-amnestic subtype often exhibiting the
least stability over time (Bickel et al., 2006; Busse et al., 2006; Fischer et al., 2007;
Jak et al., 2007). For example, one study reported that 50% of those with single
domain MCI were normal upon later retesting whereas only 12% of those with
multi-domain MCI ‘‘recovered’’ (Bickel et al., 2006). Additional sources of insta-
bility in the MCI diagnosis can manifest via individuals changing MCI subtypes
over time. Approximately 6% of those with MCI at baseline changed subtypes at
follow up (Fischer et al., 2007; Jak et al., 2007). In contrast, over a 3-year interval,
Zanetti and colleagues (2006) identified a more anticipated trajectory of their
MCI cohort; all MCI subtypes either converted to dementia (about a quarter) or
retained their MCI status (Zanetti et al., 2006).
76% were initially diagnosed with aMCI, 11% initially presented with single
domain non-amnestic MCI, and 13% were initially identified as mMCI (YaVe
et al., 2006). Conversely, of those who progressed to vascular dementia, 50% were
initially diagnosed with aMCI, 8% had single domain non-amnestic MCI, and
42% had mMCI (YaVe et al., 2006). Rozzini and colleagues (2007) reported that,
in a group of amnestic MCI individuals, poor global cognitive performance at
baseline and worsening executive functioning, but not worsening memory perfor-
mance, were associated with conversion to AD over a 1-year follow-up period.
Those with non-amnestic multiple domain subtype appear more likely to convert
to a non-AD dementia (Busse et al., 2006), with the single domain non-amnestic
MCI at particular risk to progress to a frontal dementia syndrome (YaVe et al.,
2006). There are reports, however, in which detailed information about MCI
subtypes does not add significant benefit in determining who may be at greatest
risk for conversion to dementia (Maioli et al., 2007; Ravaglia et al., 2006; Rountree
et al., 2007).
compared to the cognitively normal group. However, unlike the other two MCI
groups, individuals with non-amnestic MCI did not diVer from the cognitively
normal participants in terms of IADL performance. Notably, at least one study
(Boeve et al., 2003) did not find diVerences between amnestic MCI individuals and
cognitively normal older adults in terms of functional abilities. However, it should
be noted that the MCI participants who were included in this study were
characterized as MCI based, in part, on intact ADL.
Farias and colleagues (2006) administered self-report and informant-based
versions of the Daily Function Questionnaire (DFQ) and calculated a diVerence
score by subtracting patients’ DFQ from informants’ DFQ. DFQ diVerence
scores, which indicate a lack of awareness of deficits, were greater in demented
individuals relative to their cognitively normal counterparts and MCI subtype
groups (i.e., MCI with memory impairment and MCI without memory im-
pairment). However, the diVerence scores did not diVer between MCI groups
and cognitively normal older adults. Based on these findings, Farias and
colleagues (2006) argued that individuals with MCI do not underestimate func-
tional changes as is often the case for demented individuals.
What seems clear from the above discussion is that changes in ADL and IADL
are not uncommon across the spectrum of MCI; although, it remains to be
determined whether many of these changes would be conceptualized as frank
‘‘deficits’’ or ‘‘impairments.’’ Comparing the ADL and IADL changes of MCI to
overt dementia groups would be helpful in determining cutoV criteria for im-
pairment, or administering performance-based ADL and IADL measures with
normative reference standards would also help to delineate whether such changes
represent impairment.
VII. Neuroimaging
A. STRUCTURAL MRI
C. FUNCTIONAL MRI
VIII. Treatment
TABLE I
CLINICAL TRIALS IN AMCI
trials in MCI, most have used a ‘‘progression to AD’’ design with the focus on
slowing cognitive decline and delaying conversion to AD. As a whole, the trials
have been disappointing with one possible exception, the Alzheimer’s Disease
Cooperative Study (ADCS)-sponsored trial (Petersen et al., 2005) (see Table I).
The ADCS-sponsored study of Vitamin E and donepezil for MCI involved
769 subjects at 69 centers in the US and Canada over 3 years. There were three
treatment arms: Vitamin E 2000 IU/day, donepezil 10 mg/day, and placebo.
The primary trial endpoint was conversion to AD. Although conversion to AD
favored donepezil at 1 year, there were no diVerences among groups with regard
to conversion to AD at 3 years. However, possession of the APOE e4 allele was
noted to be associated with a threefold greater risk of conversion from aMCI to
dementia and, thus, clearly an important predictor of progression. When the
authors looked at the progression to AD for APOE e4 positive participants by
treatment group, they found that the eVect of donepezil was greater in e4 positive
individuals and persisted for 2 years. While neither of the two active arms reduced
the risk of progressing to AD over the entire 36 months, donepezil reduced the
risk of progression to AD for the first 12 months in all subjects and up to 24
months in those who were positive for the APOE e4 allele. No treatment eVect
was noted for Vitamin E.
Other treatment trials have been less promising for halting conversion from
MCI to dementia over time. A large trial of rivastigmine, an acetylcholinesterase
inhibitor, was a double blind, placebo-controlled trial of 1018 patients that had
many of the same features as the ADCS trial, but was conducted in 14 countries
using multiple languages and translations of the neuropsychological instruments
(Feldman et al., 2007). At baseline, arms were not well matched with regard to
frequency of APOE e4 genotype, which was 46% in the placebo arm but only
37% in the rivastigmine arm. The study also had a lower conversion rate than
expected and had to be extended to 4 years; over that time, 21.4% of placebo
treated, but only 17.3% of rivastigmine treated, subjects progressed to AD.
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND IMAGING OF MCI SUBTYPES 95
Although rivastigmine was favored, the results were not statistically significant,
and secondary assessments were also not significant. Investigation of the eYcacy
of another acetylcholinesterase inhibitor, galantamine, also failed to reveal a
significant eVect of galantamine on conversion to dementia in those with MCI
in either of two trials (Winblad et al., 2008).
Finally, another large randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind study
examined the ability of the COX 2 inhibitor, rofecoxib, to delay disease progres-
sion in 1457 aMCI subjects (Thal et al., 2005). Once again, there was a lower than
expected annual rate of conversion to AD. Conversion to AD actually favored
placebo in this trial but the authors dismissed the significance of this finding
because the secondary cognitive measures did not corroborate the primary
outcome.
In hindsight, several important factors likely influenced the results of these
studies, including, perhaps first and foremost, the variable rate of progression
from aMCI to AD. Sources of this variability likely include subject heterogeneity,
with regard to impairment level, culture, language, and APOE e4 carrier status, in
addition to even simple diVerences in implementation of enrollment criteria. MCI
patients may show increased awareness of, or lower tolerability for, adverse
events, resulting in higher discontinuation rates. Our current outcome measures
may be insensitive; for example, the conversion design dichotomizes a continuous
variable and most of the currently used eYcacy measures follow an AD trial
model of decline. Rather than decline, however, MCI patients may show im-
provement on cognitive measures, no matter which treatment group they are
assigned to, because of at least some preservation in their ability to learn. Future
MCI trials may benefit from less heterogeneous recruitment with stricter entry
criteria and enriched populations, more sensitive cognitive and global outcome
measures that reflect subtle impairments in complex activities, novel imaging
outcomes, and longer trials.
IX. Conclusions
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (K24 AG026431, R01
AG012674, and P50 AG05131), by Career Development Awards from the Department of Veterans
AVairs, and by Investigator-Initiated and New Investigator Research Grants from the Alzheimer’s
Association. The authors gratefully acknowledge the assistance of staV, patients, and volunteers of the
UCSD Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, and the UCSD Laboratory of Cognitive Imaging.
References
Alexopoulos, P., Grimmer, T., Perneczky, R., Domes, G., and Kurz, A. (2006). Progression to
dementia in clinical subtypes of mild cognitive impairment. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 22, 27–34.
Alladi, S., Arnold, R., Mitchell, J., Nestor, P. J., and Hodges, J. R. (2006). Mild cognitive impairment:
Applicability of research criteria in a memory clinic and characterization of cognitive profile.
Psychol. Med. 36, 1–9.
Bartzokis, G. (2004). Age-related myelin breakdown: A developmental model of cognitive decline and
Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 25, 5–18.
Becker, J. T., Davis, S. W., Hayashi, K. M., Meltzer, C. C., Toga, A. W., Lopez, O. L., and
Thompson, P. M. (2006). Three-dimensional patterns of hippocampal atrophy in mild cognitive
impairment. Arch. Neurol. 63, 97–101.
Bell-McGinty, S., Lopez, O. L., Meltzer, C. C., Scanlon, J. M., Whyte, E. M., DeKosky, S. T., et al.
(2005). Differential cortical atrophy in subgroups of mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol. 62,
1393–1397.
Bickel, H., Mösch, E., Seigerschmidt, E., Siemen, M., and Förstl, H. (2006). Prevalence and persis-
tence of mild cognitive impairment among elderly patients in general hospitals. Dement. Geriatr.
Cogn. Disord. 21, 242–250.
98 JAK et al.
Boeve, B., McCormick, J., Smith, G., Ferman, T., Rummans, T., Carpenter, T., Ivnik, R.,
Kokmen, E., Tangalos, E., Edland, S., Knopman, D., and Petersen, R. (2003). Mild cognitive
impairment in the oldest old. Neurology 60, 477–480.
Boyle, P. A., Wilson, R. S., Aggarwal, N. T., Arvanitakis, Z., Kelly, J., Bienias, J. L., and Bennett, D. A.
(2005). Parkinsonian signs in subjects with mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 65, 1901–1906.
Bozzali, M., Falini, A., Franceschi, M., Cercignani, M., ZuY, M., Scotti, G., Comi, G., and Filippi, M.
(2002). White matter damage in Alzheimer’s disease assessed in vivo using diVusion tensor
magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 72, 742–746.
Bruscoli, M., and Lovestone, S. (2004). Is MCI really just early dementia? A systematic review of
conversion studies. Int. Psychogeriatr. 16, 12–40.
Busse, A., Bischkopf, J., Riedel-Heller, S. G., and Angermeyer, M. C. (2003). Subclassifications for
mild cognitive impairment: Prevalence and predictive validity. Psychol. Med. 33, 1029–1038.
Busse, A., Hensel, A., Guhne, U., Angermeyer, M. C., and Riedel-Heller, S. G. (2006). Mild cognitive
impairment: Long-term course of four clinical subtypes. Neurology 67, 2176–2185.
Celone, K. A., Calhoun, V. D., Dickerson, B. C., Atri, A., Chua, E. F., Miller, S. L., DePeau, K.,
Rentz, D. M., Selkoe, D. J., Blacker, D., Albert, M. S., and Sperling, R. A. (2006). Alterations in
memory networks in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: An independent
component analysis. J. Neurosci. 26, 10222–10231.
Cho, H., Dong, W. Y., Shon, Y. M., Kim, B. S., Kim, Y. I., Choi, Y. B., Lee, K. S., Shim, Y. S.,
Yoon, B., Kim, W., and Ahn, K. J. (2008). Abnormal integrity of corticocortical tracts in mild
cognitive impairment: A diVusion tensor imaging study. J. Korean Med. Sci. 23, 477–483.
Das, S. K., Bose, P., Biswas, A., Dutt, A., Banerjee, T. K., Hazra, A., Raut, D. K., Chaudhuri, A., and
Roy, T. (2007). An epidemiologic study of mild cognitive impairment in Kolkata, India. Neurology
68, 2019–2026.
Debette, S., Bombois, S., Bruandet, A., Delbeuck, X., Lepoittevin, S., Delmaire, C., Leys, D., and
Pasquier, F. (2007). Subcortical hyperintensities are associated with cognitive decline in patients
with mild cognitive impairment. Stroke 38, 2924–2930.
Delano-Wood, L., Jak, A. J., Schweinsburg, B., Wierenga, C., Horne, N., Salmon, D. P., Thal, L. J.,
Frank, L. R., and Bondi, M. W. (2007). Posterior white matter changes in MCI: Associations with
cognition and stroke risk. J. Int. Neuropscyhol. Assoc. 13, 81.
Desgranges, B., Baron, J. C., de la Sayette, V., Petit-Taboue, M. C., Benali, K., Landeau, B.,
Lechevalier, B., and Eustache, F. (1998). The neural substrates of memory systems impairment
in Alzheimer’s disease. A PET study of resting brain glucose utilization. Brain 121(Pt 4), 611–631.
Di Carlo, A., Lamassa, M., Baldereschi, M., Inzitari, M., Scafato, E., Farchi, G., and Inzitari, D.
(2007). CIND and MCI in the Italian elderly: Frequency, vascular risk factors, progression to
dementia. Neurology 68, 1909–1916.
Dickerson, B. C., Salat, D. H., Bates, J. F., Atiya, M., Killiany, R. J., Greve, D. N., Dale, A. M.,
Stern, C. E., Blacker, D., Albert, M. S., and Sperling, R. A. (2004). Medial temporal lobe function
and structure in mild cognitive impairment. Ann. Neurol. 56, 27–35.
Dickerson, B. C., Salat, D. H., Greve, D. N., Chua, E. F., Rand-Giovannetti, E., Rentz, D. M.,
Bertram, L., Mullin, K., Tanzi, R. E., Blacker, D., Albert, M. S., and Sperling, R. A. (2005).
Increased hippocampal activation in mild cognitive impairment compared to normal aging and
AD. Neurology 65, 404–411.
Dohnel, K., Sommer, M., Ibach, B., Rothmayr, C., Meinhardt, J., and Hajak, G. (2008). Neural
correlates of emotional working memory in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsycho-
logia 46, 37–48.
Fagan, A. M., Csernansky, C. A., Morris, J. C., and Holtzman, D. M. (2005). The search for
antecedent biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 8, 347–358.
Farias, S. T., Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., Harvey, D., Cahn-Weiner, D., and DeCarli, C. (2006). MCI is
associated with deficits in everyday functioning. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 20, 217–223.
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND IMAGING OF MCI SUBTYPES 99
Feldman, H. H., Ferris, S., Winblad, B., Sfikas, N., Mancione, L., He, Y., Tekin, S., Burns, A.,
Cummings, J., del Ser, T., Inzitari, D., Orgogozo, J. M., et al. (2007). EVect of rivastigmine on
delay to diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease from mild cognitive impairment: the InDDEx study.
Lancet 6, 501–512.
Fellgiebel, A., Muller, M. J., Wille, P., Dellani, P. R., Scheurich, A., Schmidt, L. G., and Stoeter, P
(2005). Color-coded diVusion-tensor-imaging of posterior cingulate fiber tracts in mild cognitive
impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 26, 1193–1198.
Fischer, P., Jungwirth, S., Zehetmayer, S., Weissgram, S., Hoenigschnabl, S., Gelpi, E., Krampla, W.,
and Tragl, K. H. (2007). Conversion from subtypes of mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer
dementia. Neurology 68, 288–291.
Gabryelewicz, T., Styczynska, M., Luczywek, E., Barczak, A., PfeVer, A., Androsiuk, W.,
Chodakowska-Zebrowska, M., Wasiak, B., Peplonska, B., and Barcikowska, M. (2007). The rate
of conversion of mild cognitive impairment to dementia: Predictive role of depression. Int. J.
Geriatr. Psychiatry 22, 563–567.
Gauthier, S., Reisberg, B., Zaudig, M., Petersen, R. C., Ritchie, K., Broich, K., Belleville, S.,
Brodaty, H., Bennett, D., Chertkow, H., Cummings, J. L., de Leon, M., et al. (2006). Mild
cognitive impairment. The Lancet 367, 1262–1270.
Giovanetti, T., Bettcher, B. M., Brennan, L., Libon, D. J., Burke, M., Duey, K., Nieves, C., and
Wambach, D. (2008). Characterization of everyday functioning in mild cognitive impairment:
A direct assessment approach. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 25, 359–365.
GriYth, H. R., Belue, B. S., Sicola, A., Krzywanski, S., Zamrini, E., Harrell, L., and Marson, D. C.
(2003). Impaired financial abilities in mild cognitive impairment: A direct assessment approach.
Neurology 60, 449–457.
Hamalainen, A., Pihlajamaki, M., Tanila, H., Hanninen, T., Niskanen, E., Tervo, S.,
Karjalainen, P. A., Vanninen, R. L., and Soininen, H. (2007). Increased fMRI responses during
encoding in mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 28, 1889–1903.
Head, D., Buckner, R. L., Shimony, J. S., Williams, L. E., Akbudak, E., Conturo, E., McAvoy, M.,
Morris, J. C., and Snyder, A. Z. (2004). DiVerential vulnerability of anterior white matter in
nondemented aging with minimal acceleration in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: Evidence
form diVusion tensor imaging. Cerebral. Cortex 14, 410–423.
Hort, J., Laczo, J., Vyhnalek, M., Bojar, M., Bures, J., and Vlcek, K. (2007). Spatial navigation deficit
in amnesic mild cognitive impairment. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 4042–4047.
Jak, A. J., Corey-Bloom, J., and Bondi, M. W. (2007). Diagnostic characterization of MCI subtypes in a
naturalistic sample. J. Int. Neuropscyhol. Assoc. 13, 4.
Janvin, C. C., Larsen, J. P., Aarsland, D., and Hugdahl, K. (2006). Subtypes of mild cognitive
impairment in Parkinson’s disease: Progression to dementia. Mov. Disord. 21, 1343–1349.
Johnson, S. C., Baxter, L. C., Susskind-Wilder, L., Connor, D. J., Sabbagh, M. N., and Caselli, R. J.
(2004). Hippocampal adaptation to face repetition in healthy elderly and mild cognitive
impairment. Neuropsychologia 42, 980–989.
Johnson, S. C., Schmitz, T. W., Moritz, C. H., Meyerand, M. E., Rowley, H. A., Alexander, A. L.,
Hansen, K. W., Gleason, C. E., Carlsson, C. M., Ries, M. L., Asthana, S., Chen, K., et al. (2006).
Activation of brain regions vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease: The eVect of mild cognitive
impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 27, 1604–1612.
Jungwirth, S., Weissgram, S., Zehetmayer, S., Tragl, K. H., and Fischer, P. (2005). VITA: Subtypes of
mild cognitive impairment in a community-based cohort at the age of 75 years. Int. J. Geriatr.
Psychiatry 20, 452–458.
Kantarci, K., Petersen, R. C., Boeve, B. F., Knopman, D. S., Weigand, S. D., O’Brien, P. C., et al.
(2005). DWI predicts future progression to Alzheimer disease in amnestic mild cognitive
impairment. Neurology 64, 902–904.
100 JAK et al.
Kircher, T. T., Weis, S., Freymann, K., Erb, M., Jessen, F., Grodd, W., Heun, R., and Leube, D. T.
(2007). Hippocampal activation in patients with mild cognitive impairment is necessary for
successful memory encoding. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 78, 812–818.
Kryscio, R. J., Schmitt, F. A., Salazar, J. C., Mendiondo, M. S., and Markesbery, W. R. (2006). Risk
factors for transitions from normal to mild cognitive impairment and dementia. Neurology 66,
828–832.
Lopez, O. L., Jagust, W. J., DeKosky, S. T., Becker, J. T., Fitzpatrick, A., Dulberg, C., Breitner, J.,
Lyketsos, C., Jones, B., Kawas, C., Carlson, M., and Kuller, L. H. (2003). Prevalence and
classification of mild cognitive impairment in the cardiovascular health study cognition study:
Part 1. Arch. Neurol. 60, 1385–1389.
Lopez, O. L., Becker, J. T., Jagust, W. J., Fitzpatrick, A., Carlson, M. C., DeKosky, S. T., Breitner, J.,
Lyketsos, C. G., Jones, B., Kawas, C., and Kuller, L. H. (2006). Neuropsychological character-
istics of mild cognitive impairment subgroups. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 77, 159–165.
Machulda, M. M., Ward, H. A., Borowski, B., Gunter, J. L., Cha, R. H., O’Brien, P. C.,
Petersen, R. C., Boeve, B. F., Knopman, D., Tang-Wai, D. F., Ivnik, R. J., Smith, G. E., et al.
(2003). Comparison of memory f MRI response among normal, MCI, and Alzheimer’s patients.
Neurology 61, 500–506.
Maioli, F., Coveri, M., Pagni, P., Chiandetti, C., Marchetti, C., Ciarrocchi, R., Ruggero, C.,
Nativio, V., Onesti, A., D’Anastasio, C., and Pedone, V. (2007). Conversion of mild cognitive
impairment to dementia in elderly subjects: A preliminary study in memory and cognitive
disorder unit. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 44(Suppl. 1), 233–241.
Mandzia, J. L., McAndrews, M. P., Grady, C. L., Graham, S. J., and Black, S. E. (2009). Neural
correlates of incidental memory in mild cognitive impairment: An f MRI study. Neurobiol. Aging 30,
717–730.
Manly, J. J., Bell-McGinty, S., Tang, M., Schupf, N., Stern, Y., and Mayeux, R. (2005). Implementing
diagnostic criteria and estimating frequency of mild cognitive impairment in an urban community.
Arch. Neurol. 62, 1739–1746.
Mariani, E., Monastero, R., Ercolani, S., Mangialasche, F., Caputo, M., Feliziani, F. T., Vitale, D. F.,
Senin, U., and Mecocci, P. (2007). Vascular risk factors in mild cognitive impairment subytpes.
Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 24, 448–456.
Mariani, E., Monastero, R., Ercolani, S., Rinaldi, P., Mangialasche, F., Costanzi, E., Vitale, D. F.,
Senin, U., and Mecocci, P. (2008). Influence of comorbidity and cognitive status on instrumental
activities of daily living in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: Results from the ReGAI project.
Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 23, 523–530.
Masdeu, J. C., Zubieta, J. L., and Arbizu, J. (2005). Neuroimaging as a marker of the onset and
progression of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 236, 55–64.
Matsuda, H. (2001). Cerebral blood flow and metabolic abnormalities in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann.
Nucl. Med. 15, 85–92.
Medina, D., deToledo-Morrell, L., Urresta, F., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Moseley, M., Fleischman, D.,
Bennett, D. A., Leurgans, S., Turner, D. A., and Stebbins, G. T. (2006). White matter changes
in mild cognitive impairment and AD: A diVusion tensor imaging study. Neurobiol. Aging 27,
663–672.
Miller, S. L., Fenstermacher, E., Bates, J., Blacker, D., Sperling, R. A., and Dickerson, B. C. (2008).
Hippocampal activation in adults with mild cognitive impairment predicts subsequent cognitive
decline. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 79, 630–635.
Naggara, O., Oppenheim, C., Rieu, D., Raoux, N., Rodrigo, S., Barba, G. D., and Meder, J.-F. (2006).
DiVusion tensor imaging in early Alzheimer’s disease. Psychiatry Res. 146, 663–672.
Palmer, K., Berger, A. K., Monastero, B., Winblad, B., Backman, L., and Fratiglioni, L. (2007).
Predictors of progression from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer disease. Neurology 68,
1596–1602.
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND IMAGING OF MCI SUBTYPES 101
Palmer, K., Backman, L., Winblad, B., and Fratiglioni, L. (2008). Mild cognitive impairment in the
general population: Occurrence and progression to Alzheimer disease. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 16,
603–611.
Peres, K., Chrysostome, V., Fabrigoule, C., Orgogozo, J. M., Dartigues, J. F., and Barberger-
Gateau, P. (2006). Restriction in complex activities of daily living in MCI: Impact on outcome.
Neurology 67, 461–466.
Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J. Int. Med. 256, 183–194.
Petersen, R. C., and Morris, J. C. (2005). Mild cognitive impairment as a clinical entity and treatment
target. Arch. Neurol. 62, 1160–1163.
Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., and Kokmen, E. (1999).
Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Arch. Neurol. 56, 303–308.
Petersen, R. C., Doody, R. S., Kurz, A., Mohs, R. C., Morris, J. C., Rabins, P. V., Ritchie, K.,
Rossor, M. N., Thal, L. J., and Winblad, B. (2001). Current concepts in mild cognitive
impairment. Arch. Neurol. 58, 1985–1992.
Petersen, R. C., Thomas, R. G., Grundman, M., Bennett, D., Doody, R., Ferris, S., Galasko, D.,
Jin, S., Kaye, J., Levey, A., PfeiVer, E., Sano, M., et al. (2005). Vitamin E and donepezil for the
treatment of mild cognitive impairment. N. Engl. J. Med. 52, 2379–2388.
Petrella, J. R., Prince, S. E., Wang, L., Hellegers, C., and Doraiswamy, P. M. (2007a). Prognostic value
of posteromedial cortex deactivation in mild cognitive impairment. PLoS ONE 2, e1104.
Petrella, J. R., Wang, L., Krishnan, S., Slavin, M. J., Prince, S. E., Tran, T. T., and Doraiswamy, P. M.
(2007b). Cortical deactivation in mild cognitive impairment: High-field-strength functional MR
imaging. Radiology 245, 224–235.
PfeVerbaum, A., Sullivan, E. V., Hedehus, M., Lim, K. O., Adalsteinsson, E., and Moseley, M. (2000).
Age-related decline in brain white matter anisotropy measured with spatially corrected echo-
planar diVusion tensor imaging. Magn. Reson. Med. 44, 259–268.
Poettrich, K., Weiss, P. H., Werner, A., Lux, S., Donix, M., Gerber, J., von Kummer, R., Fink, G. R.,
and HolthoV, V. A. (2009). Altered neural network supporting declarative long-term memory in
mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 30, 284–298.
Portet, F., Ousset, P. J., Visser, P. J., Frisoni, G. B., Nobili, F., Scheltens, P., Vellas, B., and Touchon, J.
the MCI Working Group of the European Consortium on Alzheimer’s Disease (EADC) (2006).
Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in medical practice: A critical review of the concept and new
diagnostic procedure. Report of the MCI Working Group of the European Consortium on
Alzheimer’s Disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 77, 714–718.
Rasquin, S. M. C., Lodder, J., Visser, P. J., Lousberg, R., and Verhey, F. R. J. (2005). Predictive
accuracy of MCI subtypes for Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia in subjects with mild
cognitive impairment: A 2-year follow-up study. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 19, 113–119.
Ravaglia, G., Forti, P., Maioli, F., Martelli, M., Servadei, L., Brunetti, N., Pantieri, G., and Mariani, E.
(2006). Conversion of mild cognitive impairment to dementia: Predictive role of mild cognitive
impairment subtypes and vascular risk factors. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 21, 51–58.
Raz, N., and Rodrigue, K. M. (2006). DiVerential aging of the brain: Patterns, cognitive correlates and
modifiers. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 730–748.
Reiman, E. M., Caselli, R. J., Yun, L. S., Chen, K., Bandy, D., Minoshima, S., Thibodeau, S. N., and
Osborne, D. (1996). Preclinical evidence of Alzheimer’s disease in persons homozygous for the
epsilon 4 allele for apolipoprotein E. N. Engl. J. Med. 334, 752–758.
Reiman, E. M., Chen, K., Alexander, G. E., Caselli, R. J., Bandy, D., Osborne, D., Saunders, A. M.,
and Hardy, J. (2004). Functional brain abnormalities in young adults at genetic risk for late-onset
Alzheimer’s dementia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101, 284–289.
Reiman, E. M., Chen, K., Alexander, G. E., Caselli, R. J., Bandy, D., Osborne, D., Saunders, A. M.,
and Hardy, J. (2005). Correlations between apolipoprotein E epsilon4 gene dose and brain-
imaging measurements of regional hypometabolism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102, 8299–8302.
102 JAK et al.
Ries, M. L., Jabbar, B. M., Schmitz, T. W., Trivedi, M. A., Gleason, C. E., Carlsson, C. M.,
Rowley, H. A., Asthana, S., and Johnson, S. C. (2007). Anosognosia in mild cognitive impairment:
Relationship to activation of cortical midline structures involved in self-appraisal. J. Int. Neurop-
sychol. Soc. 13, 450–461.
Rose, S. E., Chen, F., Chalk, J. B., Zelaya, F. O., Strugnell, W. E., Benson, M., Semple, J., and
Doddrell, D. M. (2000). Loss of connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease: An evaluation of white matter
tract integrity with colour coded MR diVusion tensor imaging. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 69,
528–530.
Rose, S. E., McMahon, K. L., Janke, A. L., O’Dowd, B., de Zubicaray, G., Strudwick, M. W., and
Chalk, J. B. (2006). DiVusion indices on magnetic resonance imaging and neuropsychological
performance in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychaitry 77, 1122–1128.
Rountree, S. D., Waring, S. C., Chan, W. C., Lupo, P. J., Darby, E. J., and Doody, R. S. (2007).
Importance of subtle amnestic and nonamnestic deficits in mild cognitive impairment: Prognosis
and conversion to dementia. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 24, 476–482.
Rozzini, L., Chilovi, B. V., Conti, M., Delrio, I., Borroni, B., Trabucchi, M., and Padovani, A. (2008).
Neuropsychiatric symptoms in amnestic and nonamnestic mild cognitive impairment. Dement.
Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 25, 32–36.
Seo, S. W., Im, K., Lee, J., Kim, Y., Kim, S. T., Kim, S. Y., Yang, D. W., Kim, S. I., Cho, Y. S., and
Na, D. L. (2007). Cortical thickness in single- versus multiple-domain amnestic mild cognitive
impairment. NeuroImage 36, 289–297.
Smith, G. E., and Ivnik, R. J. (2003). Normative neuropsychology. In ‘‘Mild Cognitive Impairment:
Aging to Alzheimer’s Disease’’ (R. C. Petersen, Ed.), pp. 63–88. Oxford University Press,
New York.
Sperling, R. (2007). Functional MRI studies of associative encoding in normal aging, mild cognitive
impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1097, 146–155.
Sullivan, E. V., and PfeVerbaum, A. (2006). DiVusion tensor imaging and aging. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.
30, 749–761.
Tabert, M. H., Manly, J. J., Liu, X., Pelton, G. H., Rosenblum, S., Jacobs, M., Zamora, D.,
Goodkind, M., Bell, K., Stern, Y., and Devanand, D. P. (2006). Neuropsychological prediction
of conversion to alzheimer disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry
63, 916–924.
Takahashi, S., Yonezawa, H., Takahashi, J., Kudo, M., Inoue, T., and Tohgi, H. (2002). Selective
reduction of diVusion anisotropy in white matter of Alzheimer disease brains measured by 3.0
Tesla magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci. Lett. 332, 45–48.
Tam, C., Lam, L., Chiu, H. F. K., and Lui, V. W. C. (2007). Characteristic Profiles of Instrumental
Activities of Daily Living in Chinese Older Persons with Mild Cognitive Impairment. Am. J.
Alzheimer’s Dis. Other Dement. 22, 211–217.
Teng, E., Lu, P. H., and Cummings, J. L. (2007). Deficits in facial emotion processing in mild cognitive
impairment. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 23, 271–279.
Thal, L. J., Ferris, S. H., Kirby, L., Block, G. A., Lines, C. R., Yuen, E., Assaid, C., Nessly, M. L.,
Norman, B. A., Baranak, C. C., and Reines, S. A. (2005). A randomized, double-blind, study of
rofecoxib in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 1204–1215.
Ukmar, M., Makuc, E., Onor, M. L., Garbin, G., Trevisiol, M., and Cova, M. A. (2008). Evaluation of
white matter damage in patients with Alzheimer’s disease and in patients with mild cognitive
impairment by using diVusion tensor imaging. Radiol. Med. 113, 915–922.
Vannini, P., Almkvist, O., Dierks, T., Lehmann, C., and Wahlund, L. O. (2007). Reduced neuronal
eYcacy in progressive mild cognitive impairment: A prospective fMRI study on visuospatial
processing. Psychiatry Res. 156, 43–57.
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND IMAGING OF MCI SUBTYPES 103
Verghese, J., Robbins, M., Holtzer, R., Zimmerman, M. E., Wang, C., Xue, X., and Lipton, R. B.
(2008). Gait dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment syndromes. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 56,
1244–1251.
Wadley, V. G., Crowe, M., Marsiske, M., Cook, S. E., Unverzagt, F. W., Rosenberg, A. L., et al. (2007).
Changes in Everyday Function in Individuals with Psychometrically Defined Mild Cognitive
Impairment in the Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly Study. J. Am.
Geriatr. Soc. 55, 1192–1198.
Westervelt, H. J., Bruce, J. M., Coon, W. G., and Tremont, G. (2008). Odor identification in mild
cognitive impairment subtypes. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 30, 151–156.
Whitwell, J. L., Przybelski, S. A., Weigand, S. D., Knopman, D., Boeve, B. F., Petersen, R. C., and
Jack, C. R. (2007). 3D maps from multiple MRI illustrate changing atrophy patterns as subjects
progress from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 130, 1777–1786.
Winblad, B., Palmer, K., Kivipelto, M., Jelic, V., Fratiglioni, L., Wahlund, L.-O., Nordberg, A.,
Backman, L., Albert, M., Almkvist, O., Arai, H., Basun, H., et al. (2004). Mild cognitive
impairment—Beyond controversies, towards a consensus: Report of the International Working
Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Int. Med. 256, 240–246.
Winblad, B., Gauthier, S., Scinto, L., Feldman, H., Wilcock, G. K., Truyen, L., Mayorga, A. J.,
Wang, D., Brashear, H. R., and Nye, J. S. (2008). Safety and eYcacy of galantamine in subjects
with mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 70, 2024–2035.
Wolf, H., Jelic, V., Gertz, H. J., Nordberg, A., Julin, P., and Wahlund, L. O. (2003). A critical discussion
of the role of neuroimaging in mild cognitive impairment. Acta Neurol. Scand. Suppl. 179, 52–76.
YaVe, K., Petersen, R. C., Lindquist, K., Kramer, J., and Miller, B. (2006). Subtype of mild cognitive
impairment and progression to dementia and death. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 22, 312–319.
Yetkin, F. Z., Rosenberg, R. N., Weiner, M. F., Purdy, P. D., and Cullum, C. M. (2006). FMRI of
working memory in patients with mild cognitive impairment and probable Alzheimer’s disease.
Eur. Radiol. 16, 193–206.
Zanetti, M., Ballabio, C., Abbate, C., Cutaia, C., Vergani, C., and Bergamaschini, L. (2006). Mild
cognitive impairment subtypes and vascular dementia in community-dwelling elderly people:
A 3-year follow-up study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 54, 580–586.
Zhang, Y., SchuV, N., Jahng, G. H., Bayne, W., Mori, S., Schad, L., Mueller, S., Du, A. T.,
Kramer, J. H., YaVe, K., Chui, H. C., Jagust, W. J., et al. (2007). DiVusion tensor imaging of
cingulum fibers in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease. Neurology 68, 13–19.
PROTON MAGNETIC RESONANCE SPECTROSCOPY IN
DEMENTIAS AND MILD COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT
I. Introduction
II. Metabolites in 1H MRS
III. 1H MRS Findings in Dementias
A. 1H MRS Findings in Alzheimer’s Disease
B. 1H MRS Findings in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment
C. 1H MRS Findings in Frontotemporal Dementia
D. 1H MRS Findings in Vascular Dementia
E. 1H MRS Findings in Dementia with Lewy Bodies
F. 1H MRS Findings in Parkinson’s Disease Dementia
G. Utility of 1H MRS for Discriminating Among Dementias
H. Utility of 1H MRS for Monitoring Treatment Effects in Dementias
IV. Discussion
References
treatment eVects. Such developments will hopefully benefit the search for eVective
treatments of dementias in the twenty-first century.
I. Introduction
The aging demographic of persons in the United States and other industrialized
nations creates the prospect for a future major healthcare crisis from providing care
to patients with dementia worldwide. Clearly, there is a great need for better
treatments to emerge for these neurodegenerative illnesses within the next decade.
A prime issue in clinical investigation of treating neurodegenerative diseases is the
need for better markers of the disease process that can be used as measures of
treatment eYcacy (Mueller et al., 2006). The current review focuses on proton
magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H MRS) as a marker of metabolic brain
changes in dementias and the potential for use as a biomarker in clinical trials.
Findings are discussed from studies of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), amnestic mild
cognitive impairment (MCI), frontotemporal dementia (FTD), vascular dementia
(VAD), Lewy body dementia (LBD), and Parkinson’s disease dementia (PDD).
8
NAA
7
Cr
6
Cho
5
4
mI
sI
Glu 3
FIG. 1. Example spectra obtained from posterior cingulate gyrus at 3 T. mI, myo-Inositol;
sI, scyllo-Inositol; Cho, Choline; Cr, Creatine; Glu, glutamate; NAA, N-acetyleaspartate.
(Valenzuela and Sachdev, 2001), as white and gray matter have comparable NAA
levels and white matter NAA is reduced in white matter diseases (e.g., multiple
sclerosis). Normalization of NAA levels in neurological disorders has been noted
(e.g., following stroke); however, it remains unclear whether NAA ‘‘recovery’’
represents actual neuronal ‘‘recovery’’ or ‘‘regeneration’’ (Mueller et al., 2006).
Choline. The Choline (Cho) peak (3.2 ppm) represents a combination of several
choline-containing compounds, including free Cho, phosphorylcholine and gly-
cerophosphorylcholine, and to a small extent acetylcholine (Firbank et al., 2002;
Valenzuela and Sachdev, 2001). Free Cho acts as a precursor to acetylcholine,
while glycerophosphorylcholine is a product of breakdown of membrane phos-
phatidylcholine and acts as an osmoregulator (Firbank et al., 2002). Cho is slightly
more concentrated in white versus gray matter (Mueller et al., 2006) and is likely
present in glia. The Cho peak is often viewed as a marker of membrane turnover
or inflammation in 1H MRS studies (Mueller et al., 2006).
Creatine. The Creatine (Cr) peak (3.0 ppm) is composed of creatine and
phosphocreatine. Together, these metabolites buVer the energy use and energy
storage of cells (Valenzuela and Sachdev, 2001). Cr is thought to exist primarily in
glia and in neurons to a lesser extent (Firbank et al., 2002) and is found in greater
108 GRIFFITH et al.
matter, and the temporal gyrus mixed gray/white matter. Compared to healthy
controls, VAD patients exhibited global reductions in NAA/Cr ratios in all voxels,
along with elevations in mI/Cr ratios in parietal gray and white matter, frontal
white matter, and the temporal lobe, and elevations in Glx ratios in parietal gray
matter and the temporal lobe. In another study, Kantarci and colleagues
(Kantarci et al., 2004) reported that in the posterior cingulate VAD patients
had lower ratios of NAA/Cr, but normal ratios of mI/Cr and Cho/Cr, when
compared with healthy controls. These studies suggest that VAD involves multiple
neuropathological processes, even in regions distant from actual vascular pathol-
ogy (Kantarci et al., 2004), including neuronal dysfunction/loss, axonal damage,
myelin loss, and gliosis ( Jones and Waldman, 2004). Kantarci et al. (2004) have
speculated that reductions in NAA in distal brain areas may be secondary to
retrograde Wallerian degeneration caused from brain regions showing actual
vascular neuropathology.
studies on DLB, which have yielded inconsistent findings regard to neuronal loss
in DLB (Cordato et al., 2000; Gomez-Isla et al., 1999). Moreover, methodological
diVerences, including diVerences in symptom severity of patients, voxel place-
ment, and (Mueller et al., 2006) 1H MRS acquisition parameters (e.g., short versus
long echo time), may also account for some of the variable results across
these studies.
AD, VaD, MCI, and major depression reported that the NAA/mI ratio had an
81.5% sensitivity and 72.7% specificity for discriminating AD from the other
groups (Martinez-Bisbal et al., 2004).
We are aware of only two studies to date that have compared 1H MRS
across patients with Parkinson-spectrum dementias to patients with other
dementias. Kantarci and colleagues (Kantarci et al., 2004) reported that patients
with DLB had normal NAA/Cr ratios when compared to patients with AD and
patients with FTD; patients with DLB also had normal mI/Cr ratios compared
to patients with FTD. No diVerences were observed between DLB patients and
patients with VAD. GriYth and colleagues (GriYth et al., 2008c) very recently
reported that patients with PDD showed abnormally low Glu/Cr ratios when
compared with patients with AD, while patients with AD showed a trend toward
higher mI/Cr ratios when compared with patients with PDD. The Glu/Cr ratio
was able to discriminate between AD and PDD at rates greater than chance.
No studies to date that we are aware of have compared (Mueller et al., 2006)
1
H MRS between patients with DLB versus patients with PDD, although
both dementias show considerable clinical and neuropathological overlap
(Galvin et al., 2006).
IV. Discussion
The search for viable biomarkers of dementing illnesses is critical for develop-
ment of new treatments. Mueller and colleagues outlined their six criteria for a
viable biomarker of treatment eVects in patients with dementias: (1) links between
the markers and treatment outcomes, (2) test-retest reliability; (3) sensitivity to
desired stage of treatment; (4) link to the biomarker and amyloid burden, (5) non-
invasiveness and tolerability; and (6) availability and cost (Mueller et al., 2006).
In considering 1H MRS in light of these criteria, the current state of the literature
suggests that several of the criteria are being approached. 1H MRS appears to
have some links to potential measurable outcomes, in that studies have reported
associations with cognitive functioning and instrumental daily activities, as well as
showing sensitivity to treatment eVects. Test-retest reliability has been supported
in at least one study, although interscan variability may not be at the desired level
for reliable use in individual patients. Studies in 1H MRS of preclinical dementias
have shown that early metabolic changes are observable at the stage when
treatment is most desirable (Petersen, 2004). 1H MRS is arguably among one of
the most widespread imaging modalities, in that many of the MRI scanners
commercially available have the capability of collecting 1H MRS data, although
subsequent data processing and interpretation require expertise. In our experi-
ence, 1H MRS is well tolerated in patients with mild to moderate stages of
dementia as well as individuals with movement disorders.
The link of 1H MRS with amyloid burden, one of the Mueller et al. (2006)
criteria, is questionable. While amyloid deposition is clearly correlated with NAA
and mI levels in patients with AD, abnormalities in these metabolites are also seen
in non-amyloid neuropathologies such as FTD. 1H MRS can likely play a role as
a marker of neuronal integrity with sensitivity to a number of neuropathologies
(i.e., Lewy bodies, tauopathies, cerebrovascular disease) rather than having a
specificity to amyloid. Given that questions have arisen as to the key role that
amyolid deposition plays in the dementing process in AD (Arends et al., 2000;
Dickson, 1997; Terry et al., 1991), the importance of an amyloid biomarker over a
biomarker of neuronal health is debatable.
Future directions in 1H MRS of dementias should focus on developing stable
and reliable longitudinal techniques and applying these to investigate longitudinal
changes, especially in dementias other than AD as well as longitudinal comparative
studies of AD with other dementias. Measuring correspondence of change on
clinical and functional measures with changes in 1H MRS measures is also
needed. In addition, studies are needed to further investigate whether 1H MRS
measures are sensitive to medication eVects, such as cholinesterase inhibitor use,
in patients where these treatments are considered to be beneficial. The association
of 1H MRS measures with imaging of amyloid, such as comparison with the
Pittsburgh B Compound PET procedure (Klunk et al., 2004), would be beneficial.
Lastly, as technological advances occur it would be worthwhile to expand from
124 GRIFFITH et al.
Acknowledgments
Funding was provided by grants from the National Institute of Aging (Alzheimer’s Disease
Research Center—1P50 AG16582-01: Harrell, PI; 1R01 AG021927-01: Marson, PI) and Alzheimer’s
of Central Alabama.
References
Aarsland, D., Litvan, I., Salmon, D., Galasko, D., Wentzel-Larsen, T., and Larsen, J. P. (2003).
Performance on the dementia rating scale in Parkinson’s disease with dementia and dementia
with Lewy bodies: Comparison with progressive supranuclear palsy and Alzheimer’s disease.
J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 74(9), 1215–1220.
Ackl, N., Ising, M., Schreiber, Y. A., Atiya, M., Sonntag, A., and Auer, D. P. (2005). Hippocampal
metabolic abnormalities in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci. Lett.
384(1–2), 23–28.
Adalsteinsson, E., Sullivan, E. V., Kleinhans, N., Spielman, D. M., and Pfefferbaum, A. (2000).
Longitudinal decline of the neuronal marker N-acetyl aspartate in Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet
355(9216), 1696–1697.
Antuono, P. G., Jones, J. L., Wang, Y., and Li, S. J. (2001). Decreased glutamate þ glutamine in
Alzheimer’s disease detected in vivo with 1H-MRS at 0.5 T. Neurology 56(6), 737–742.
Arends, Y. M., Duyckaerts, C., Rozemuller, J. M., Eikelenboom, P., and Hauw, J. J. (2000). Microglia,
amyloid and dementia in alzheimer disease. A correlative study. Neurobiol. Aging 21(1), 39–47.
Bartha, R., Smith, M., Rupsingh, R., Rylett, J., Wells, J. L., and Borrie, M. J. (2008). High field
(1)H MRS of the hippocampus after donepezil treatment in Alzheimer disease. Prog. Neuropsycho-
pharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 32(3), 786–793.
Bartnik Olson, B. L., Holshouser, B. A., Britt, W., 3rd, Mueller, C., Baqai, W., Patra, S., Petersen, F.,
and Kirsch, W. M. (2008). Longitudinal Metabolic and Cognitive Changes in Mild Cognitive
Impairment Patients. Alzheimer. Dis. Assoc. Disord.
Bartres-Faz, D., Junque, C., Clemente, I. C., Lopez-Alomar, A., Bargallo, N., Mercader, J. M., and
Moral, P. (2002). Relationship among (1)H-magnetic resonance spectroscopy, brain volumetry
and genetic polymorphisms in humans with memory impairment. Neurosci. Lett. 327(3), 177–180.
Benecke, R. (2003). Diffuse Lewy body disease – a clinical syndrome or a disease entity? J. Neurol. 250
(Suppl 1), 39–42.
Block, W., Jessen, F., Traber, F., Flacke, S., Manka, C., Lamerichs, R., Keller, E., Heun, R., and Schild, H.
(2002). Regional N-acetylaspartate reduction in the hippocampus detected with fast proton magnetic
resonance spectroscopic imaging in patients with Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 59(5), 828–834.
Bowen, B. C., Block, R. E., Sanchez-Ramos, J., Pattany, P. M., Lampman, D. A., Murdoch, J. B., and
Quencer, R. M. (1995). Proton MR spectroscopy of the brain in 14 patients with Parkinson
disease. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 16(1), 61–68.
1
H MRS IN DEMENTIAS AND MILD COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT 125
Braak, H., and Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta
Neuropathol. (Berl) 82(4), 239–259.
Braak, H., Ghebremedhin, E., Rub, U., Bratzke, H., and Del Tredici, K. (2004). Stages in the
development of Parkinson’s disease-related pathology. Cell Tissue Res. 318(1), 121–134.
Brand, A., Richter-Landsberg, C., and Leibfritz, D. (1993). Multinuclear NMR studies on the energy
metabolism of glial and neuronal cells. Dev. Neurosci. 15(3–5), 289–298.
Brooks, D. J., and Piccini, P. (2006). Imaging in Parkinson’s disease: The role of monoamines in
behavior. Biol. Psychiatry 59(10), 908–918.
Catani, M., Cherubini, A., Howard, R., Tarducci, R., Pelliccioli, G. P., Piccirilli, M., Gobbi, G.,
Senin, U., and Mecocci, P. (2001). (1)H-MR spectroscopy differentiates mild cognitive
impairment from normal brain aging. Neuroreport 12(11), 2315–2317.
Catani, M., Mecocci, P., Tarducci, R., Howard, R., Pelliccioli, G. P., Mariani, E., Metastasio, A.,
Benedetti, C., Senin, U., and Cherubini, A. (2002). Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy
reveals similar white matter biochemical changes in patients with chronic hypertension and early
Alzheimer’s disease. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 50(10), 1707–1710.
Chantal, S., Braun, C. M., Bouchard, R. W., Labelle, M., and Boulanger, Y. (2004). Similar 1H
magnetic resonance spectroscopic metabolic pattern in the medial temporal lobes of patients with
mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease. Brain Res. 1003(1–2), 26–35.
Chantal, S., Labelle, M., Bouchard, R. W., Braun, C. M., and Boulanger, Y. (2002). Correlation of
regional proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic metabolic changes with cognitive deficits in
mild Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 59(6), 955–962.
Chen, J. G., Charles, H. C., Barboriak, D. P., and Doraiswamy, P. M. (2000). Magnetic resonance
spectroscopy in Alzheimer’s disease: Focus on N-acetylaspartate. Acta. Neurol. Scand. Suppl. 176,
20–26.
Cheng, L. L., Newell, K., Mallory, A. E., Hyman, B. T., and Gonzalez, R. G. (2002). Quantification of
neurons in Alzheimer and control brains with ex vivo high resolution magic angle spinning proton
magnetic resonance spectroscopy and stereology. Magn. Reson. Imaging 20(7), 527–533.
Christiansen, P., Schlosser, A., and Henriksen, O. (1995). Reduced N-acetylaspartate content in the
frontal part of the brain in patients with probable Alzheimer’s disease. Magn. Reson. Imaging 13(3),
457–462.
Cordato, N. J., Halliday, G. M., Harding, A. J., Hely, M. A., and Morris, J. G. (2000). Regional brain
atrophy in progressive supranuclear palsy and Lewy body disease. Ann. Neurol. 47(6), 718–728.
Coulthard, E., Firbank, M., English, P., Welch, J., Birchall, D., O’Brien, J., and Griffiths, T. D. (2006).
Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in frontotemporal dementia. J. Neurol. 253(7), 861–868.
Cummings, J. L., Vinters, H. V., Cole, G. M., and Khachaturian, Z. S. (1998). Alzheimer’s disease:
Etiologies, pathophysiology, cognitive reserve, and treatment opportunities. Neurology 51(1 Suppl 1),
S2–S17; discussion S65–17.
Dickson, D. (2000). Alzheimer-Parkinson disease overlap: Neuropathology. In ‘‘Neurodegenerative
dementias’’ (C Clark and J Trojanowski, Eds.), pp. 247–259. McGraw-Hill, New York City.
Dickson, D. W. (1997). The pathogenesis of senile plaques. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 56(4), 321–339.
Dixon, R. M., Bradley, K. M., Budge, M. M., Styles, P., and Smith, A. D. (2002). Longitudinal
quantitative proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the hippocampus in Alzheimer’s disease.
Brain 125(Pt 10), 2332–2341.
Doraiswamy, P. M., Charles, H. C., and Krishnan, K. R. (1998). Prediction of cognitive decline in
early Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet 352(9141), 1678.
Ernst, T., Chang, L., Melchor, R., and Mehringer, C. M. (1997). Frontotemporal dementia and early
Alzheimer disease: Differentiation with frontal lobe H-1 MR spectroscopy. Radiology 203(3),
829–836.
Fenili, D., Brown, M., Rappaport, R., and McLaurin, J. (2007). Properties of scyllo-inositol as a
therapeutic treatment of AD-like pathology. J. Mol. Med. 85(6), 603–611.
126 GRIFFITH et al.
Firbank, M. J., Harrison, R. M., and O’Brien, J. T. (2002). A comprehensive review of proton
magnetic resonance spectroscopy studies in dementia and Parkinson’s disease. Dement. Geriatr.
Cogn. Disord. 14(2), 64–76.
Franczak, M., Prost, R. W., Antuono, P. G., Mark, L. P., Jones, J. L., and Ulmer, J. L. (2007). Proton
magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the hippocampus in patients with mild cognitive impairment:
A pilot study. J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr. 31(5), 666–670.
Frederick, B., Satlin, A., Wald, L. L., Hennen, J., Bodick, N., and Renshaw, P. F. (2002). Brain proton
magnetic resonance spectroscopy in Alzheimer disease: Changes after treatment with xanomeline.
Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 10(1), 81–88.
Frederick, B. B., Satlin, A., Yurgelun-Todd, D. A., and Renshaw, P. F. (1997). In vivo proton magnetic
resonance spectroscopy of Alzheimer’s disease in the parietal and temporal lobes. Biol. Psychiatry.
42(2), 147–150.
Galvin, J. E., Pollack, J., and Morris, J. C. (2006). Clinical phenotype of Parkinson disease dementia.
Neurology 67(9), 1605–1611.
Ganguli, M., Dodge, H., Shen, C., and DeKosky, S. T. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment, amnestic
type: An epidemiological study. Neurology 63, 115–121.
Garcia Santos, J. M., Gavrila, D., Antunez, C., Tormo, M. J., Salmeron, D., Carles, R., Jimenez
Veiga, J., Parrilla, G., Torres del Rio, S., Fortuna, L., and Navarro, C. (2008). Magnetic
resonance spectroscopy performance for detection of dementia, Alzheimer’s disease and mild
cognitive impairment in a community-based survey. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 26(1), 15–25.
Garrard, P., Schott, J. M., MacManus, D. G., Hodges, J. R., Fox, N. C., and Waldman, A. D. (2006).
Posterior cingulate neurometabolite profiles and clinical phenotype in frontotemporal dementia.
Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 19(4), 185–189.
Godbolt, A. K., Waldman, A. D., MacManus, D. G., Schott, J. M., Frost, C., Cipolotti, L., Fox, N. C.,
and Rossor, M. N. (2006). MRS shows abnormalities before symptoms in familial Alzheimer
disease. Neurology 66(5), 718–722.
Gomez-Isla, T., Growdon, W. B., McNamara, M., Newell, K., Gomez-Tortosa, E., Hedley-
Whyte, E. T., and Hyman, B. T. (1999). Clinicopathologic correlates in temporal cortex in
dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology 53(9), 2003–2009.
Griffith, H. R., den Hollander, J. A., Okonkwo, O. C., O’Brien, T., Watts, R. L., and Marson, D. C.
(2008a). Brain metabolism differs in Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease dementia
Alzheimer’s & Dementia, In press.
Griffith, H. R., den Hollander, J. A., Okonkwo, O. C., O’Brien, T., Watts, R. L., and Marson, D. C.
(2008b). Brain N-acetylaspartate is Reduced in Parkinson Disease With Dementia. Alzheimer. Dis.
Assoc. Disord. 22(1), 54–60.
Griffith, H. R., den Hollander, J. A., Stewart, C. C., Evanochko, W. T., Buchthal, S. D., Harrell, L. E.,
Zamrini, E. Y., Brockington, J. C., and Marson, D. C. (2007). Elevated brain scyllo-inositol
concentrations in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. NMR Biomed 20, 709–716.
Griffith, H. R., Hollander, J. A., Okonkwo, O., Evanochko, W. T., Harrell, L. E., Zamrini, E. Y.,
Brockington, J. C., and Marson, D. C. (2007). Executive function is associated with brain proton
magnetic resonance spectroscopy in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol.
29(6), 599–609.
Griffith, H. R., Netson, K. L., Harrell, L. E., Zamrini, E. Y., Brockington, J. C., and Marson, D. C.
(2006). Amnestic mild cognitive impairment: Diagnostic outcomes and clinical prediction over a
two-year time period. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 12(2), 166–175.
Griffith, H. R., Okonkwo, O. C., den Hollander, J. A., Belue, K., Lanza, S., Harrell, L. E.,
Brockington, J. C., Clark, D. G., and Marson, D. C. (2007a). Brain proton MRS is correlated
with financial abilities in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Imaging & Behavior 1, 23–29.
Griffith, H. R., Okonkwo, O. C., den Hollander, J. A., Belue, K., Lanza, S., Harrell, L. E.,
Brockington, J. C., Clark, D. G., and Marson, D. C. (2007b). Brain proton MRS is correlated
with financial abilities in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Imaging & Behavior 1, 23–29.
1
H MRS IN DEMENTIAS AND MILD COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT 127
Griffith, H. R., Okonkwo, O. C., O’Brien, T., and Hollander, J. A. (2008). Reduced brain glutamate in
patients with Parkinson’s disease. NMR Biomed. 21(4), 381–387.
Griffith, H. R., Stewart, C. C., den Hollander, J. A., Evanochko, W. T., Buchthal, S. D., Harrell, L. E.,
Zamrini, E. Y., Brockington, J. C., and Marson, D. C. (2006). In-vivo 3T 1H Magnetic
Resonance Spectroscopy of the Brain Reveals Elevated Scyllo-inositol in Patients with Mild
Alzheimer’s Disease. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the International Society of Magnetic
Resonance in Medicine, Seattle, WA.
Hattori, N., Abe, K., Sakoda, S., and Sawada, T. (2002). Proton MR spectroscopic study at 3 Tesla on
glutamate/glutamine in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroreport 13(1), 183–186.
Herminghaus, S., Frolich, L., Gorriz, C., Pilatus, U., Dierks, T., Wittsack, H. J., Lanfermann, H.,
Maurer, K., and Zanella, F. E. (2003). Brain metabolism in Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia
assessed by in vivo proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Psychiatry. Res. 123(3), 183–190.
Heun, R., Schlegel, S., Graf-Morgenstern, M., Tintera, J., Gawehn, J., and Stoeter, P. (1997). Proton
magnetic resonance spectroscopy in dementia of Alzheimer type. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 12(3),
349–358.
Holmes, C., Cairns, N., Lantos, P., and Mann, A. (1999). Validity of current clinical criteria for
Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. Br. J. Psychiatry 174,
45–50.
Jessen, F., Block, W., Traber, F., Keller, E., Flacke, S., Lamerichs, R., Schild, H. H., and Heun, R.
(2001). Decrease of N-acetylaspartate in the MTL correlates with cognitive decline of AD patients.
Neurology 57(5), 930–932.
Jessen, F., Block, W., Traber, F., Keller, E., Flacke, S., Papassotiropoulos, A., Lamerichs, R., Heun, R.,
and Schild, H. H. (2000). Proton MR spectroscopy detects a relative decrease of N-acetylaspartate
in the medial temporal lobe of patients with AD. Neurology 55(5), 684–688.
Jessen, F., Traeber, F., Freymann, K., Maier, W., Schild, H. H., and Block, W. (2006). Treatment
monitoring and response prediction with proton MR spectroscopy in AD. Neurology 67(3),
528–530.
Jones, R. S., and Waldman, A. D. (2004). 1H-MRS evaluation of metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease
and vascular dementia. Neurol. Res. 26(5), 488–495.
Kaiser, L. G., Schuff, N., Cashdollar, N., and Weiner, M. W. (2005). Scyllo-inositol in normal aging
human brain: 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy study at 4 Tesla. NMR. Biomed. 18(1), 51–55.
Kantarci, K., Jack, C. R., Jr., Xu, Y. C., Campeau, N. G., O’Brien, P. C., Smith, G. E.,
Ivnik, R. J., Boeve, B. F., Kokmen, E., Tangalos, E. G., and Petersen, R. C. (2000). Regional
metabolic patterns in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: A 1H MRS study.
Neurology 55(2), 210–217.
Kantarci, K., Knopman, D. S., Dickson, D. W., Parisi, J. E., Whitwell, J. L., Weigand, S. D.,
Josephs, K. A., Boeve, B. F., Petersen, R. C., and Jack, C. R., Jr. (2008). Alzheimer disease:
Postmortem neuropathologic correlates of antemortem 1H MR spectroscopy metabolite mea-
surements. Radiology 248(1), 210–220.
Kantarci, K., Petersen, R. C., Boeve, B. F., Knopman, D. S., Tang-Wai, D. F., O’Brien, P. C.,
Weigand, S. D., Edland, S. D., Smith, G. E., Ivnik, R. J., Ferman, T. J., Tangalos, E. G., et al.
(2004). 1H MR spectroscopy in common dementias. Neurology 63(8), 1393–1398.
Kantarci, K., Reynolds, G., Petersen, R. C., Boeve, B. F., Knopman, D. S., Edland, S. D.,
Smith, G. E., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., and Jack, C. R., Jr. (2003). Proton MR spectroscopy
in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease: Comparison of 1.5 and 3 T. AJNR. Am. J.
Neuroradiol. 24(5), 843–849.
Kantarci, K., Smith, G. E., Ivnik, R. J., Petersen, R. C., Boeve, B. F., Knopman, D. S.,
Tangalos, E. G., and Jack, C. R., Jr. (2002). 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy, cognitive
function, and apolipoprotein E genotype in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment and
Alzheimer’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 8(7), 934–942.
128 GRIFFITH et al.
Kantarci, K., Weigand, S. D., Petersen, R. C., Boeve, B. F., Knopman, D. S., Gunter, J., Reyes, D.,
Shiung, M., O’Brien, P. C., Smith, G. E., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., et al. (2007). Longitudinal
1H MRS changes in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging. 28(9),
1330–1339.
Kantarci, K., Xu, Y., Shiung, M. M., O’Brien, P. C., Cha, R. H., Smith, G. E., Ivnik, R. J.,
Boeve, B. F., Edland, S. D., Kokmen, E., Tangalos, E. G., Petersen, R. C., et al. (2002).
Comparative diagnostic utility of different MR modalities in mild cognitive impairment and
Alzheimer’s disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 14(4), 198–207.
Kario, K., Matsuo, T., Hoshide, S., Umeda, Y., and Shimada, K. (1999). Effect of thrombin inhibition
in vascular dementia and silent cerebrovascular disease. An MR spectroscopy study. Stroke 30(5),
1033–1037.
Kattapong, V. J., Brooks, W. M., Wesley, M. H., Kodituwakku, P. W., and Rosenberg, G. A. (1996).
Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy of vascular- and Alzheimer-type dementia. Arch. Neurol.
53(7), 678–680.
Kickler, N., Krack, P., Fraix, V., Lebas, J. F., Lamalle, L., Durif, F., Krainik, A., Remy, C.,
Segebarth, C., and Pollak, P. (2007). Glutamate measurement in Parkinson’s disease using MRS
at 3 T field strength. NMR. Biomed.
Kizu, O., Yamada, K., Ito, H., and Nishimura, T. (2004). Posterior cingulate metabolic changes in
frontotemporal lobar degeneration detected by magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Neuroradiology
46(4), 277–281.
Klunk, W. E., Engler, H., Nordberg, A., Wang, Y., Blomqvist, G., Holt, D. P., Bergstrom, M.,
Savitcheva, I., Huang, G. F., Estrada, S., Ausen, B., Debnath, M. L., et al. (2004). Imaging
brain amyloid in Alzheimer’s disease with Pittsburgh Compound-B. Ann. Neurol. 55(3), 306–319.
Klunk, W. E., Panchalingam, K., McClure, R. J., Stanley, J. A., and Pettegrew, J. W. (1998). Metabolic
alterations in postmortem Alzheimer’s disease brain are exaggerated by Apo-E4. Neurobiol. Aging.
19(6), 511–515.
Kovari, E., Gold, G., Herrmann, F. R., Canuto, A., Hof, P. R., Bouras, C., and Giannakopoulos, P.
(2003). Lewy body densities in the entorhinal and anterior cingulate cortex predict cognitive
deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Acta. Neuropathol. (Berl) 106(1), 83–88.
Krishnan, K. R., Charles, H. C., Doraiswamy, P. M., Mintzer, J., Weisler, R., Yu, X., Perdomo, C.,
Ieni, J. R., and Rogers, S. (2003). Randomized, placebo-controlled trial of the effects of donepezil
on neuronal markers and hippocampal volumes in Alzheimer’s disease. Am. J. Psychiatry. 160(11),
2003–2011.
Kwock, L. (1998). Localized MR spectroscopy: Basic principles. Neuroimaging Clin. N. Am. 8(4),
713–731.
Lazeyras, F., Charles, H. C., Tupler, L. A., Erickson, R., Boyko, O. B., and Krishnan, K. R. (1998).
Metabolic brain mapping in Alzheimer’s disease using proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy.
Psychiatry. Res. 82(2), 95–106.
Litvan, I., Mohr, E., Williams, J., Gomez, C., and Chase, T. N. (1991). Differential memory and
executive functions in demented patients with Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol.
Neurosurg. Psychiatry 54(1), 25–29.
MacKay, S., Ezekiel, F., Di Sclafani, V., Meyerhoff, D. J., Gerson, J., Norman, D., Fein, G., and
Weiner, M. W. (1996). Alzheimer disease and subcortical ischemic vascular dementia: Evaluation
by combining MR imaging segmentation and H-1 MR spectroscopic imaging. Radiology 198(2),
537–545.
Magistretti, P. J., and Pellerin, L. (1999). Cellular mechanisms of brain energy metabolism and their
relevance to functional brain imaging. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 354(1387), 1155–1163.
Markesbery, W. R., Schmitt, F. A., Kryscio, R. J., Davis, D. G., Smith, C. D., and Wekstein, D. R.
(2006). Neuropathologic substrate of mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol. 63(1), 38–46.
1
H MRS IN DEMENTIAS AND MILD COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT 129
Martinez-Bisbal, M. C., Arana, E., Marti-Bonmati, L., Molla, E., and Celda, B. (2004). Cognitive
impairment: Classification by 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Eur J. Neurol. 11(3), 187–193.
McKeith, I. G., Dickson, D. W., Lowe, J., Emre, M., O’Brien, J. T., Feldman, H., Cummings, J.,
Duda, J. E., Lippa, C., Perry, E. K., Aarsland, D., Arai, H., et al. (2005). Diagnosis and
management of dementia with Lewy bodies: Third report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology
65(12), 1863–1872.
McLaurin, J., Golomb, R., Jurewicz, A., Antel, J. P., and Fraser, P. E. (2000). Inositol stereoisomers
stabilize an oligomeric aggregate of Alzheimer amyloid beta peptide and inhibit abeta -induced
toxicity. J. Biol. Chem. 275(24), 18495–18502.
McMurtray, A., Clark, D. G., Christine, D., and Mendez, M. F. (2006). Early-onset dementia:
frequency and causes compared to late-onset dementia. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 21(2), 59–64.
Metastasio, A., Rinaldi, P., Tarducci, R., Mariani, E., Feliziani, F. T., Cherubini, A., Pelliccioli, G. P.,
Gobbi, G., Senin, U., and Mecocci, P. (2006). Conversion of MCI to dementia: Role of proton
magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Neurobiol. Aging. 27(7), 926–932.
Meyerhoff, D. J., MacKay, S., Constans, J. M., Norman, D., Van Dyke, C., Fein, G., and
Weiner, M. W. (1994). Axonal injury and membrane alterations in Alzheimer’s disease suggested
by in vivo proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging. Ann. Neurol. 36(1), 40–47.
Mihara, M., Hattori, N., Abe, K., Sakoda, S., and Sawada, T. (2006). Magnetic resonance spectro-
scopic study of Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia/Pick complex. Neuroreport 17(4),
413–416.
Moats, R. A., Ernst, T., Shonk, T. K., and Ross, B. D. (1994). Abnormal cerebral metabolite
concentrations in patients with probable Alzheimer disease. Magn. Reson. Med. 32(1), 110–115.
Modrego, P. J., Fayed, N., and Pina, M. A. (2005). Conversion from mild cognitive impairment to
probable Alzheimer’s disease predicted by brain magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Am. J. Psychiatry.
162(4), 667–675.
Modrego, P. J., Pina, M. A., Fayed, N., and Diaz, M. (2006). Changes in metabolite ratios after
treatment with rivastigmine in Alzheimer’s disease: A nonrandomised controlled trial with
magnetic resonance spectroscopy. CNS. Drugs. 20(10), 867–877.
Molina, J. A., Garcia-Segura, J. M., Benito-Leon, J., Gomez-Escalonilla, C., del Ser, T., Martinez, V.,
and Viano, J. (2002). Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in dementia with Lewy bodies. Eur.
Neurol. 48(3), 158–163.
Mueller, S. G., Schuff, N., and Weiner, M. W. (2006). Evaluation of treatment effects in Alzheimer’s
and other neurodegenerative diseases by MRI and MRS. NMR. Biomed. 19(6), 655–668.
Osaki, Y., Morita, Y., Fukumoto, M., Akagi, N., Yoshida, S., and Doi, Y. (2005). Three-dimensional
stereotactic surface projection SPECT analysis in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia.
Mov. Disord. 20(8), 999–1005.
Parnetti, L., Lowenthal, D. T., Presciutti, O., Pelliccioli, G. P., Palumbo, R., Gobbi, G., Chiarini, P.,
Palumbo, B., Tarducci, R., and Senin, U. (1996). 1H-MRS, MRI-based hippocampal volumetry,
and 99mTc-HMPAO-SPECT in normal aging, age-associated memory impairment, and proba-
ble Alzheimer’s disease. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 44(2), 133–138.
Parnetti, L., Tarducci, R., Presciutti, O., Lowenthal, D. T., Pippi, M., Palumbo, B., Gobbi, G.,
Pelliccioli, G. P., and Senin, U. (1997). Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy can differentiate
Alzheimer’s disease from normal aging. Mech. Ageing. Dev. 97(1), 9–14.
Petersen, R. C. (2000). Mild cognitive impairment: Transition between aging and Alzheimer’s disease.
Neurologia 15(3), 93–101.
Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J. Intern. Med. 256(3),
183–194.
Petersen, R. C., Doody, R., Kurz, A., Mohs, R. C., Morris, J. C., Rabins, P. V., Ritchie, K.,
Rossor, M., Thal, L., and Winblad, B. (2001). Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment.
Arch. Neurol. 58(12), 1985–1992.
130 GRIFFITH et al.
Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., and Kokmen, E. (1999).
Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Arch. Neurol. 56(3), 303–308.
Pfefferbaum, A., Adalsteinsson, E., Spielman, D., Sullivan, E. V., and Lim, K. O. (1999). In vivo brain
concentrations of N-acetyl compounds, creatine, and choline in Alzheimer disease. Arch. Gen.
Psychiatry. 56(2), 185–192.
Pletnikova, O., West, N., Lee, M. K., Rudow, G. L., Skolasky, R. L., Dawson, T. M., Marsh, L., and
Troncoso, J. C. (2005). Abeta deposition is associated with enhanced cortical alpha-synuclein
lesions in Lewy body diseases. Neurobiol. Aging. 26(8), 1183–1192.
Rai, G. S., McConnell, J. R., Waldman, A., Grant, D., and Chaudry, M. (1999). Brain proton
spectroscopy in dementia: An aid to clinical diagnosis. Lancet. 353(9158), 1063–1064.
Rami, L., Caprile, C., Gómez-Ansón, B., Sánchez-Valle, R., Monte, G. C., Bosch, B., and
Molinuevo, J. L. (2008). Naming is associated with left temporal pole metabolite levels in
neurodegenerative diseases. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 25(3), 212–217.
Rami, L., Gomez-Anson, B., Bosch, B., Sanchez-Valle, R., Monte, G. C., Villar, A., and
Molinuevo, J. L. (2007). Cortical brain metabolism as measured by proton spectroscopy is related
to memory performance in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s
disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 24(4), 274–279.
Rami, L., Gomez-Anson, B., Sanchez-Valle, R., Bosch, B., Monte, G. C., Llado, A., and
Molinuevo, J. L. (2007). Longitudinal study of amnesic patients at high risk for Alzheimer’s
disease: Clinical, neuropsychological and magnetic resonance spectroscopy features. Dement.
Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 24(5), 402–410.
Riederer, P., and Hoyer, S. (2006). From benefit to damage. Glutamate and advanced glycation end
products in Alzheimer brain. J. Neural. Transm. 113(11), 1671–1677.
Rose, S. E., de Zubicaray, G. I., Wang, D., Galloway, G. J., Chalk, J. B., Eagle, S. C., Semple, J., and
Doddrell, D. M. (1999). A 1H MRS study of probable Alzheimer’s disease and normal aging:
Implications for longitudinal monitoring of dementia progression. Magn. Reson. Imaging. 17(2),
291–299.
Rosen, Y., and Lenkinski, R. E. (2007). Recent advances in magnetic resonance neurospectroscopy.
Neurotherapeutics 4(3), 330–345.
Ross, B., and Bluml, S. (2001). Magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the human brain. Anat. Rec. 265(2),
54–84.
Schubert, F., Gallinat, J., Seifert, F., and Rinneberg, H. (2004). Glutamate concentrations in human
brain using single voxel proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy at 3 Tesla. Neuroimage 21(4),
1762–1771.
Schuff, N., Amend, D., Ezekiel, F., Steinman, S. K., Tanabe, J., Norman, D., Jagust, W.,
Kramer, J. H., Mastrianni, J. A., Fein, G., and Weiner, M. W. (1997). Changes of hippocampal
N-acetyl aspartate and volume in Alzheimer’s disease. A proton MR spectroscopic imaging and
MRI study. Neurology 49(6), 1513–1521.
Shonk, T. K., Moats, R. A., Gifford, P., Michaelis, T., Mandigo, J. C., Izumi, J., and Ross, B. D. (1995).
Probable Alzheimer disease: Diagnosis with proton MR spectroscopy. Radiology 195(1), 65–72.
Storandt, M., Grant, E. A., Miller, J. P., and Morris, J. C. (2002). Rates of progression in mild cognitive
impairment and early Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 59(7), 1034–1041.
Strange, K., Emma, F., Paredes, A., and Morrison, R. (1994). Osmoregulatory changes in myo-inositol
content and Naþ/myo-inositol cotransport in rat cortical astrocytes. Glia 12(1), 35–43.
Summerfield, C., Gomez-Anson, B., Tolosa, E., Mercader, J. M., Marti, M. J., Pastor, P., and
Junque, C. (2002). Dementia in Parkinson disease: A proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy
study. Arch. Neurol. 59(9), 1415–1420.
Terry, R. D., Masliah, E., Salmon, D. P., Butters, N., DeTeresa, R., Hill, R., Hansen, L. A., and
Katzman, R. (1991). Physical basis of cognitive alterations in Alzheimer’s disease: Synapse loss is
the major correlate of cognitive impairment. Ann. Neurol. 30(4), 572–580.
1
H MRS IN DEMENTIAS AND MILD COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT 131
Tierney, M. C., Szalai, J. P., Snow, W. G., Fisher, R. H., Nores, A., Nadon, G., Dunn, E., and
St George-Hyslop, P. H. (1996). Prediction of probable Alzheimer’s disease in memory-impaired
patients: A prospective longitudinal study. Neurology 46(3), 661–665.
Tiraboschi, P., Hansen, L. A., Alford, M., Sabbagh, M. N., Schoos, B., Masliah, E., Thal, L. J., and
Corey-Bloom, J. (2000). Cholinergic dysfunction in diseases with Lewy bodies. Neurology 54(2),
407–411.
Valenzuela, M. J., and Sachdev, P. (2001). Magnetic resonance spectroscopy in AD. Neurology 56(5),
592–598.
Waldman, A. D., and Rai, G. S. (2003). The relationship between cognitive impairment and in vivo
metabolite ratios in patients with clinical Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia: A proton
magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Neuroradiology 45(8), 507–512.
Waldman, A. D., Rai, G. S., McConnell, J. R., Chaudry, M., and Grant, D. (2002). Clinical brain
proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy for management of Alzheimer’s and sub-cortical ische-
mic vascular dementia in older people. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 35(2), 137–142.
Watanabe, T., Akiguchi, I., Yagi, H., Onishi, K., Kawasaki, T., Shiino, A., and Inubushi, T. (2002).
Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy and white matter hyperintensities on magnetic reso-
nance imaging in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 977, 423–429.
Weiss, U., Bacher, R., Vonbank, H., Kemmler, G., Lingg, A., and Marksteiner, J. (2003). Cognitive
impairment: Assessment with brain magnetic resonance imaging and proton magnetic resonance
spectroscopy. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 64(3), 235–242.
Wiederkehr, S., Simard, M., Fortin, C., and van Reekum, R. (2008). Comparability of the clinical
diagnostic criteria for vascular dementia: A critical review. Part I. J Neuropsychiatry. Clin. Neurosci.
20(2), 150–161.
Xuan, X., Ding, M., and Gong, X. (2008). Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy detects a relative
decrease of N-acetylaspartate in the hippocampus of patients with dementia with Lewy bodies.
J. Neuroimaging 18(2), 137–141.
APPLICATION OF PET IMAGING TO DIAGNOSIS OF ALZHEIMER’S
DISEASE AND MILD COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT1
I. Background
A. Neurobiology of Alzheimer Disease
B. Epidemiology of Alzheimer Disease
C. [18F]2-Fluoro-2-Deoxy-D-Glucose
D. Pittsburgh Compound B-PET
II. Application of PET to Primary and Specialty Care Settings
A. Diagnostic Workup of Dementia in Primary Care
B. Diagnostic Workup of Dementia in Specialty Care Settings
III. Appropriate Use of PET
A. Using Likelihood Ratio Tables
B. Using a Likelihood Ratio ‘‘Mnemonic’’
C. Using Bayesian Calculations
D. Conclusions from Using the Likelihood Ratios for PET and AD
IV. Summary
References
Alzheimer disease (AD) is the most common type of dementia and will become
increasingly prevalent with the growing elderly population. Despite established
clinical diagnostic tools, the workup for dementia among primary caregivers can
be complicated and specialist referral may not be readily available. A host of AD
diagnostic tests has been proposed to aid in diagnosis, including functional
neuroimaging such as positron emission tomography (PET). We review the
basis for FDG-PET and PiB-PET, as well as available operating statistics. From
this we advise scenarios for use of PET in primary settings and referral centers,
approach to its interpretation, and outline a clinical prediction model based on
findings.
1
Disclosures: Neither Drs. Noble nor Scarmeas have any relevant conflicts of interest.
I. Background
45% for those older than 85 years (Alzheimer’s Association, 2008; Gurland et al.,
1999; Hebert et al., 2003; Mayeux, 2003b). In the US alone, an estimated 5.2
million have AD in 2008 and this may increase to 13.2 million by 2050
(Alzheimer’s Association, 2008). Sporadic AD represents 98% of all AD cases and
is likely due to a complex interaction of environmental, vascular, and genetic risk
factors (Mayeux, 2003a; Tang et al., 1998).
The epidemiologic data for patients at risk for late onset AD are equally
daunting. In the US alone, 25 million persons are over the age of 65 (18 million
aged 65–74 years, 12 million aged 75–84 years, and 4 million aged 85 years and
over) (He et al., 2005). Before developing AD, many patients may transition
through amnestic MCI (Petersen et al., 2001), or memory complaints with com-
mensurate objective neuropsychological test deficits, but without appreciable
impact in general cognition or daily independent function.
MCI portends to a significantly increased annual risk of conversion to AD
compared to others in their age group (Manly et al., 2008; Petersen et al., 2001).
In a multiethnic urban American population, amnestic MCI (including single or
multiple domain impairment) is prevalent in 3.8% of nondemented elderly aged
65–75 and in 6.3% of those over 75 years of age (Manly et al., 2005). Overall, most
studies suggest that MCI patients have an annual conversion rate (i.e., progres-
sion) to AD of approximately 10–15% per year (Morris et al., 2001; Petersen et al.,
1999). Recently, age-stratified data from our center have suggested that of normal
elderly individuals, the annual conversion rate from normal cognition to amnestic
MCI is 1.1% of those aged 65–69 years, 2.1% aged 70–74, 2.2% aged 75–79, and
3.4% aged 80 years and above. Moreover, among those with amnestic MCI, the
annual conversion rate to AD is 3.2% of those aged 65–69, 4.5% aged 70–74,
9.7% aged 75–79, and 11.1% aged 80 years and above (Manly et al., 2008).
C. [18F]2-FLUORO-2-DEOXY-D-GLUCOSE
(Lockhart et al., 2007). Although the data are limited, the CAA pattern of PiB-
PET signal may be distinguishable from AD; CAA may have a greater degree of
occipital amyloid identified on PiB-PET than AD patients ( Johnson et al., 2007).
Given the data from these studies outlined above, it is worth considering the
application and utility of PET in clinical practice. Prior to outlining the approach
to its use, the current typical sequence leading to the workup and diagnosis of
dementia bears review, to contextualize the potential role of PET within the
diagnostic workup.
Before delving into the statistics of available studies regarding PET, the use of
sensitivity, specificity, and related statistical tools also bears brief review. From the
Table I, Sensitivity ¼ A/(A þ C) is the probability of having a positive test result
among all those with disease. The complement of sensitivity is the false negative
proportion ¼ C/(A þ C). Specificity ¼ D/(B þ D) is the probability of having a
negative test result among those without disease. Similarly, the complement of
specificity is the false positive proportion ¼ B/(B þ D). Drawn from Table I are terms
that are often cited to guide clinicians’ use for practical matters, and perhaps
incorrectly so: the predictive value terms. The positive predictive value ¼ A/(A þ B) is
the probability of having the disease given a positive test result. Similarly, the
negative predictive value ¼ D/(C þ D) is the probability of not having the disease
given a negative test result. These predictive values are often applied incorrectly;
their usefulness is dependent upon the prevalence of disease within a given
population, which may be unknown to the clinician. Furthermore, if data are
derived from case-control studies, the predictive value tools cannot be used unless
an equal sampling fraction of controls is established, and this is often diYcult.
In place of these predictive value tools, some have suggested using likelihood
ratio statistics (Grimes and Schulz, 2005), but these have not become as widely
reported, despite their relative ease of using an appropriate table or computer
program. The benefit of the likelihood ratios is that they incorporate statistics
regarding all disease states and test results, without depending on prevalence.
Instead, using Bayesian statistics these ratios allow the user to apply a likelihood
ratio to a pretest probability (either according to known disease prevalence or
clinical probability based on additional data) to derive a posttest probability.
Thus, again from Table I, the Likelihood ratio for disease if test positive ¼ sensitivity/
(1-specificity) and the Likelihood ratio for disease if test negative ¼ (1-sensitivity)/
specificity. One can also consider the likelihood ratio positive to be the ratio of
true positives to false positives, and the likelihood ratio negative as the ratio of false
negatives to true negatives. The likelihood ratios in and of themselves have little
inherent meaning but become relevant when applying them to individual
patients.
TABLE I
THE 2 # 2 TABLE
Disease No disease
FIG. 1. Likelihood ratio tables of FDG-PET in the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in several age-stratified scenarios. (A) The likelihood ratio table. (B-D)
Likelihood ratio tables for age stratified elderly populations, with pretest probability using only age-based prevalence statistics (Alzheimer’s Association, 2008;
Gurland et al., 1999; Hebert et al., 2003; Mayeux, 2003b). (B) Age less than 75 years (3–5% AD prevalence). (C) Age 75–84 years (15–18% AD prevalence).
(D) Age 85 years and above (45% AD prevalence). Solid line: FDG-PET with sensitivity ¼ 86% and specificity ¼ 86% (Patwardhan et al., 2004); (LR þ ¼ 6.1
LR$ ¼ 0.16). Dotted line: FDG-PET with sensitivity ¼ 94% and specificity ¼ 73% (Silverman et al., 2001); LR þ ¼ 3.5 and LR$ ¼ 0.08. Figure (A) reprinted
with permission from Elsevier (Grimes and Schulz, 2005).
APPLICATION OF PET IMAGING IN AD AND MCI 143
Finally, in the Fig. 1B-D, we have applied the likelihood ratios derived from
two studies of FDG-PET statistics (Patwardhan et al., 2004; Silverman et al., 2001)
to visually demonstrate the utility of applying likelihood in several diVerent
scenarios. In this figure, we derived the pretest probability in each Fig. 1B-D
from age-stratified prevalence statistics of AD (Alzheimer’s Association, 2008;
Gurland et al., 1999; Hebert et al., 2003; Mayeux et al., 2003b) in an eVort to
demonstrate the utility of FDG-PET without additional screening or further
workup. Clearly, the degree of certainty in diagnosis does not appreciably improve
from this single test until applied to those older than 85 years of age.
Aside from using likelihood ratio tables or the mnemonic as suggested above,
one can determine the exact posttest probabilities through a series of calculations,
bearing in mind that pretest probabilities may be imprecise and based on clinical
judgment. These calculations are
1. Calculate the pretest odds ¼ pretest probability /(1-pretest probability)
2. Posttest odds ¼ Pretest odds # likelihood ratio
3. Posttest probability ¼ Posttest odds/(posttest odds þ1)
144 NOBLE AND SCARMEAS
Returning to the 50% pretest example using sensitivity and specificity for PET
derived from the meta-analysis (Patwardhan et al., 2004), for a positive test
Pretest odds ¼ 0.50/(1$0.50) ¼ 1 (‘‘1:1 odds’’)
Posttest odds ¼ 1 # 6.1 ¼ 6.1
Posttest probability ¼ 6.1/(6.1 þ 1) ¼ 86%
For a negative test
Posttest odds ¼ 1 # 0.16 ¼ 0.16
Posttest probability ¼ 0.16/(1 þ 0.16) ¼ 14%
From this example we can see that the likelihood ratio table, menomnic, and
calculations yield similar results for a test with 86% sensitivity and specificity,
regarding a positive test result (posttest probability using the likelihood ratio table,
90%; mnemonic, 80%; calculation, 86%) and a negative test result (posttest proba-
bility using the likelihood ratio table, 10%; mnemonic, 5%; calculation, 14%).
IV. Summary
References
de Leon, M. J., Convit, A., Wolf, O. T., Tarshish, C. Y., DeSanti, S., Rusinek, H., Tsui, W., Kandil, E.,
Scherer, A. J., Roche, A., Imossi, A., Thorn, E., et al. (2001). Prediction of cognitive decline in
normal elderly subjects with 2-[(18)F]fluoro-2-deoxy-D-glucose/poitron-emission tomography
(FDG/PET). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98(19), 10966–10971.
Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Jacova, C., Dekosky, S. T., Barberger-Gateau, P., Cummings, J.,
Delacourte, A., Galasko, D., Gauthier, S., Jicha, G., Meguro, K., O’Brien, J., et al. (2007).
Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Revising the NINCDS-ADRDA
criteria. Lancet Neurol. 6(8), 734–746.
Edison, P., Archer, H. A., Hinz, R., Hammers, A., Pavese, N., Tai, Y. F., Hotton, G., Cutler, D.,
Fox, N., Kennedy, A., Rossor, M., and Brooks, D. J. (2007). Amyloid, hypometabolism, and
cognition in Alzheimer disease: An [11C]PIB and [18F]FDG PET study. Neurology 68(7), 501–508.
Farkas, T., Ferris, S. H., Wolf, A. P., De Leon, M. J., Christman, D. R., Reisberg, B., Alavi, A.,
Fowler, J. S., George, A. E., and Reivich, M. (1982). 18F-2-deoxy-2-fluoro-D-glucose as a tracer in
the positron emission tomographic study of senile dementia. Am. J. Psychiatry 139(3), 352–353.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). ‘‘Mini-mental state’’. A practical method for
grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12(3), 189–198.
Forsberg, A., Engler, H., Almkvist, O., Blomquist, G., Hagman, G., Wall, A., Ringheim, A.,
Langstrom, B., and Nordberg, A. (2008). PET imaging of amyloid deposition in patients with
mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 29, 1456–1465.
Fortinsky, R. H., Leighton, A., and Wasson, J. H. (1995). Primary care physicians’ diagnostic,
management, and referral practices for older persons and families aVected by dementia.
Res. Aging 17, 124–148.
Foster, N. L., Heidebrink, J. L., Clark, C. M., Jagust, W. J., Arnold, S. E., Barbas, N. R.,
DeCarli, C. S., Turner, R. S., Koeppe, R. A., Higdon, R., and Minoshima, S. (2007). FDG-
PET improves accuracy in distinguishing frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain
130(Pt 10), 2616–2635.
Friedland, R. P., Brun, A., and Budinger, T. F. (1985). Pathological and positron emission tomographic
correlations in Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet 1(8422), 228.
Friedland, R. P., Budinger, T. F., Ganz, E., Yano, Y., Mathis, C. A., Koss, B., Ober, B. A.,
Huesman, R. H., and Derenzo, S. E. (1983). Regional cerebral metabolic alterations in dementia
of the Alzheimer type: Positron emission tomography with [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose. J. Comput.
Assist. Tomogr. 7(4), 590–598.
Geldmacher, D. S. (2002). Cost-eVective recognition and diagnosis of dementia. Semin. Neurol. 22(1),
63–70.
Grimes, D. A., and Schulz, K. F. (2005). Refining clinical diagnosis with likelihood ratios. Lancet
365(9469), 1500–1505.
Gurland, B. J., Wilder, D. E., Lantigua, R., Stern, Y., Chen, J., Killeffer, E. H., and Mayeux, R. (1999).
Rates of dementia in three ethnoracial groups. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 14(6), 481–493.
He, W., Sengupta, M., Velkoff, V. A., and DeBarros, K. (2005). 65þ in the United States: 2005 U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services, National Institutes of Health, National Institute on
Aging and the U.S. Department of Commerce, Economics and Statistics Administration, U.S.
Census Bureau.
Hebert, L. E., Scherr, P. A., Bienias, J. L., Bennett, D. A., and Evans, D. A. (2003). Alzheimer disease
in the US population: Prevalence estimates using the 2000 census. Arch. Neurol. 60(8), 1119–1122.
HoVman, J. M., Welsh-Bohmer, K. A., Hanson, M., Crain, B., Hulette, C., Earl, N., and
Coleman, R. E. (2000). FDG PET imaging in patients with pathologically verified dementia.
J. Nucl. Med. 41(11), 1920–1928.
Jack, C. R., Jr., Lowe, V. J., Senjem, M. L., Weigand, S. D., Kemp, B. J., Shiung, M. M.,
Knopman, D. S., Boeve, B. F., Klunk, W. E., Mathis, C. A., and Petersen, R. C. (2008).
APPLICATION OF PET IMAGING IN AD AND MCI 147
11C PiB and structural MRI provide complementary information in imaging of Alzheimer’s
disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Brain 131(Pt 3), 665–680.
Jagust, W., Reed, B., Mungas, D., Ellis, W., and Decarli, C. (2007). What does fluorodeoxyglucose
PET imaging add to a clinical diagnosis of dementia? Neurology 69(9), 871–877.
Johnson, K. A., Gregas, M., Becker, J. A., Kinnecom, C., Salat, D. H., Moran, E. K., Smith, E. E.,
Rosand, J., Rentz, D. M., Klunk, W. E., Mathis, C. A., Price, J. C., et al. (2007). Imaging of
amyloid burden and distribution in cerebral amyloid angiopathy. Ann. Neurol. 62(3), 229–234.
Kemppainen, N. M., Aalto, S., Wilson, I. A., Nagren, K., Helin, S., Bruck, A., Oikonen, V.,
Kailajarvi, M., Scheinin, M., Viitanen, M., Parkkola, R., and Rinne, J. O. (2007). PET amyloid
ligand [11C]PIB uptake is increased in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 68(19), 1603–1606.
Klunk, W. E., Engler, H., Nordberg, A., Wang, Y., Blomqvist, G., Holt, D. P., Bergstrom, M.,
Savitcheva, I., Huang, G. F., Estrada, S., Ausen, B., Debnath, M. L., et al. (2004). Imaging brain
amyloid in Alzheimer’s disease with Pittsburgh Compound-B. Ann. Neurol. 55(3), 306–319.
Knopman, D. (2001). Cerebrospinal fluid beta-amyloid and tau proteins for the diagnosis of Alzheimer
disease. Arch. Neurol. 58(3), 349–350.
Knopman, D. S., DeKosky, S. T., Cummings, J. L., Chui, H., Corey-Bloom, J., Relkin, N.,
Small, G. W., Miller, B., and Stevens, J. C. (2001). Practice parameter: Diagnosis of dementia
(an evidence-based review). Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American
Academy of Neurology. Neurology 56(9), 1143–1153.
Lockhart, A., Lamb, J. R., Osredkar, T., Sue, L. I., Joyce, J. N., Ye, L., Libri, V., Leppert, D., and
Beach, T. G. (2007). PIB is a non-specific imaging marker of amyloid-beta (Abeta) peptide-related
cerebral amyloidosis. Brain 130(Pt 10), 2607–2615.
Manly, J. J., Bell-McGinty, S., Tang, M. X., Schupf, N., Stern, Y., and Mayeux, R. (2005). Implement-
ing diagnostic criteria and estimating frequency of mild cognitive impairment in an urban
community. Arch. Neurol. 62(11), 1739–1746.
Manly, J. J., Tang, M. X., Schupf, N., Stern, Y., Vonsattel, J. P., and Mayeux, R. (2008). Frequency
and course of mild cognitive impairment in a multiethnic community. Ann. Neurol. 63, 494–506.
Matsuda, H. (2007). The role of neuroimaging in mild cognitive impairment. Neuropathology 27(6),
570–577.
Mayeux, R. (2003a). Apolipoprotein E, Alzheimer disease, and African Americans. Arch. Neurol. 60(2),
161–163.
Mayeux, R. (2003b). Epidemiology of neurodegeneration. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 26, 81–104.
McGeer, P. L., Kamo, H., Harrop, R., McGeer, E. G., Martin, W. R., Pate, B. D., and Li, D. K.
(1986a). Comparison of PET, MRI, and CT with pathology in a proven case of Alzheimer’s
disease. Neurology 36(12), 1569–1574.
McGeer, P. L., Kamo, H., Harrop, R., Li, D. K., Tuokko, H., McGeer, E. G., Adam, M. J.,
Ammann, W., Beattie, B. L., Calne, D. B., et al. (1986b). Positron emission tomography in patients
with clinically diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease. CMAJ 134(6), 597–607.
McGeer, E. G., McGeer, P. L., Harrop, R., Akiyama, H., and Kamo, H. (1990a). Correlations of
regional postmortem enzyme activities with premortem local glucose metabolic rates in Alzhei-
mer’s disease. J. Neurosci. Res. 27(4), 612–619.
McGeer, E. G., Peppard, R. P., McGeer, P. L., Tuokko, H., Crockett, D., Parks, R., Akiyama, H.,
Calne, D. B., Beattie, B. L., and Harrop, R. (1990b). 18Fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission
tomography studies in presumed Alzheimer cases, including 13 serial scans. Can. J. Neurol. Sci.
17(1), 1–11.
McMahon, P. M., Araki, S. S., Sandberg, E. A., Neumann, P. J., and Gazelle, G. S. (2003). Cost-
eVectiveness of PET in the diagnosis of Alzheimer disease. Radiology 228(2), 515–522.
Mielke, R., Schroder, R., Fink, G. R., Kessler, J., Herholz, K., and Heiss, W. D. (1996). Regional
cerebral glucose metabolism and postmortem pathology in Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neuropathol.
91(2), 174–179.
148 NOBLE AND SCARMEAS
Mintun, M. A., Larossa, G. N., Sheline, Y. I., Dence, C. S., Lee, S. Y., Mach, R. H., Klunk, W. E.,
Mathis, C. A., DeKosky, S. T., and Morris, J. C. (2006). [11C]PIB in a nondemented population:
Potential antecedent marker of Alzheimer disease. Neurology 67(3), 446–452.
Morris, J. C., Storandt, M., McKeel, D. W., Jr., Rubin, E. H., Price, J. L., Grant, E. A., and Berg, L.
(1996). Cerebral amyloid deposition and diVuse plaques in ‘‘normal’’ aging: Evidence for pre-
symptomatic and very mild Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 46(3), 707–719.
Morris, J. C., Storandt, M., Miller, J. P., McKeel, D. W., Price, J. L., Rubin, E. H., and Berg, L. (2001).
Mild cognitive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 58(3), 397–405.
Mortimer, J. A., Snowdon, D. A., and Markesbery, W. R. (2003). Head circumference, education and
risk of dementia: Findings from the Nun Study. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 25(5), 671–679.
Mosconi, L., Perani, D., Sorbi, S., Herholz, K., Nacmias, B., Holthoff, V., Salmon, E., Baron, J. C.,
De Cristofaro, M. T., Padovani, A., Borroni, B., Franceschi, M., et al. (2004). MCI conversion to
dementia and the APOE genotype: A prediction study with FDG-PET. Neurology 63(12),
2332–2340.
Nestor, P. J., Fryer, T. D., Smielewski, P., and Hodges, J. R. (2003). Limbic hypometabolism in
Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Ann. Neurol. 54(3), 343–351.
Neuropathology Group of the Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study
(MRC-CFAS) (2007). Pathological correlates of late-onset dementia in a multicentre,
community-based population in England and Wales. Lancet 357(9251), 169–175.
Ng, S., Villemagne, V. L., Berlangieri, S., Lee, S. T., Cherk, M., Gong, S. J., Ackermann, U.,
Saunder, T., Tochon-Danguy, H., Jones, G., Smith, C., O’Keefe, G., et al. (2007). Visual
assessment versus quantitative assessment of 11C-PIB PET and 18F-FDG PET for detection of
Alzheimer’s disease. J. Nucl. Med. 48(4), 547–552.
Pathological correlates of late-onset dementia in a multicentre, community-based population in
England and Wales (2001). Neuropathology Group of the Medical Research Council Cognitive
Function and Ageing Study (MRC CFAS). Lancet 357(9251), 169–75.
Patwardhan, M. B., McCrorg, D. C., Matchar, D. B., Samsa, G. P., and Rutschmann, O. T. (2004).
Alzheimer disease: Operating characteristics of PET—A meta-analysis. Radiology 231(1), 73–80.
Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., and Kokmen, E. (1999).
Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Arch. Neurol. 56(3), 303–308.
Petersen, R. C., Stevens, J. C., Ganguli, M., Tangalos, E. G., Cummings, J. L., and DeKosky, S. T.
(2001). Practice parameter: Early detection of dementia: Mild cognitive impairment (an evidence-
based review). Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of
Neurology. Neurology 56(9), 1133–1142.
Phelps, M. E., Mazziotta, J. C., and Huang, S. C. (1982). Study of cerebral function with positron
computed tomography. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 2(2), 113–162.
Pike, K. E., Savage, G., Villemagne, V. L., Ng, S., Moss, S. A., Maruff, P., Mathis, C. A., Klunk, W. E.,
Masters, C. L., and Rowe, C. C. (2007). Beta-amyloid imaging and memory in non-demented
individuals: Evidence for preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 130(Pt 11), 2837–2844.56.
Pimlott, N. J., Siegel, K., Persaud, M., Slaughter, S., Cohen, C., Hollingworth, G., Cummings, S.,
Drummond, N., Dalziel, W., Sylvius, J., Pringle, D., and Eliasziw, T. (2006). Management of
dementia by family physicians in academic settings. Can. Fam. Physician 52(9), 1108–1109.
Riedel-Heller, S. G., Schork, A., Matschinger, H., and Angermeyer, M. C. (1999). The role of referrals
in diagnosing dementia at the primary care level. Int. Psychogeriatr. 11(3), 251–262.
Silverman, D. H., Gambhir, S. S., Huang, H. W., Schwimmer, J., Kim, S., Small, G. W., Chodosh, J.,
Czernin, J., and Phelps, M. E. (2002). Evaluating early dementia with and without assessment of
regional cerebral metabolism by PET: A comparison of predicted costs and benefits. J. Nucl. Med.
43(2), 253–266.
Silverman, D. H., Small, G. W., Chang, C. Y., Lu, C. S., Kung De Aburto, M. A., Chen, W.,
Czernin, J., Rapoport, S. I., Pietrini, P., Alexander, G. E., Schapiro, M. B., Jagust, W. J., et al.
APPLICATION OF PET IMAGING IN AD AND MCI 149
(2001). Positron emission tomography in evaluation of dementia: Regional brain metabolism and
long-term outcome. JAMA 286(17), 2120–2127.
Tang, M. X., Stern, Y., Marder, K., Bell, K., Gurland, B., Lantigua, R., Andrews, H., Feng, L.,
Tycko, B., and Mayeux, R. (1998). The APOE-epsilon4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer disease
among African Americans, whites, and Hispanics. JAMA 279(10), 751–755.
UN (2003). ‘‘The United Nations Population Division. World Population Prospects: The 2002
Revision.’’ United Nations, New York.
US Preventive Services Task Force (2003). Screening for dementia: Recommendations and rationale.
Guide to Clinical Prevention Services. Ann. Intern. Med. 139, 925–926.
Wimo, A., Winblad, A., and Jönsson, A. (2007). An estimate of the total worldwide societal costs of
dementia in 2005. Alzheimers Dement. J. Alzheimers Assoc. 3(2), 81–91.
World Health Organization (2005). ‘‘Mental Health Atlas.’’ p. 471.
THE MOLECULAR AND CELLULAR PATHOGENESIS OF DEMENTIA
OF THE ALZHEIMER’S TYPE: AN OVERVIEW
I. Introduction
II. Cellular Pathogenesis and Investigational Strategies
III. Molecular and Genetic Studies Resulting in Therapeutic Strategies
References
I. Introduction
Over 100 years ago, Alzheimer described a patient with memory disturbance
and a brain pathological picture which included miliary bodies (plaques) and
dense bundles of fibrils (tangles). These observations remain the clinical
and pathological hallmarks of dementia of the Alzheimer’s type (DAT). DAT is
the most common form of dementia and accounts for 50-60% of dementia
presentations. Presently there are approximately 5.1 million patients in the
USA with DAT; and it is estimated that in the next 20 years, there will be an
additional 15 million cases. DAT incidence increases with age. Before age 60, the
incidence is about 5%; but after age 85, it increases to 50%. The annual cost
attributed to DAT in this country is $148 billion. The clinical symptoms of DAT
include alterations of abstract thinking, skilled movements, emotional expression,
executive function, and memory. The majority of DAT cases are sporadic, but
around 10% are inherited. Autosomal dominant cases are related to mutations in
the amyloid precursor protein (APP) (chromosome 21), presenilin 1 (PS1) (chro-
mosome 14), and presenilin 2 (PS2) (chromosome 1) genes. Similar pathology
with a dementing process often occurs in Down syndrome (trisomy 21).
The pathogenesis of DAT remains perplexing. Dysfunction in APP metabolism,
Ab42 (Ab) degradation and clearance, signal transduction, tau metabolism, protein
traYcking, acetylcholine and cholesterol metabolism, and homeostasis of heavy
metals may be involved (Bertram and Tanzi, 2008). DAT neurodegeneration may
be linked to dysfunction of the degradation process of Ab or other proteins, or Ab
generation with production of toxic by-products may be responsible, and aggregate
formation may be a benign phenomenon. On the other hand, Ab aggregates may be
primarily toxic interfering with the degradation of various proteins by interfering
with chaperone function. Protein folding, protein-protein interaction, and protein
membrane association are all thermodynamically driven processes utilizing enzymes
known as chaperones. The importance of chaperones for proteosomal degradation
has been corroborated in the endoplasmic reticulum (ER) where newly synthesized
proteins that are abnormal or misfolded are removed by a quality control mechanism
termed ER-associated degradation (Tai and Schuman, 2008; Zhang et al., 2004).
DAT histological brain changes are especially prevalent in the frontal and
temporal lobes and include neuronal loss, extracellular deposition of Ab as ‘‘senile
plaques,’’ and the intracellular accumulation of neurofibrillary tangles composed
of microtubule-associated protein tau arrayed as paired helical filaments. One
of the earliest neuropathological changes is the presence of a large number of
reactive astrocytes at the site of Ab deposition. Ab deposition also leads to
degeneration of the microvascular basement membrane and thus alteration in
blood-brain barrier permeability (Berzin et al., 2000) (Fig. 1).
As previously noted, it is unclear whether the process of formation and accu-
mulation, precursors occurring during that process, the actual accumulated Ab,
and/or abnormal degradation initiates a series of neurotoxic events including
THE MOLECULAR AND CELLULAR PATHOGENESIS OF DAT 153
FIG. 1. Neuropathology of Alzheimer’s disease. (A) Low power amyloid plaques, (B) high power
amyloid plaque, (C) neurofibrillary tangles, silver stained, and (D) electron micrograph of neurofibrillary
tangles composed of hyperphosphorylated tau (Courtesy of Sisodia and St. George-Hyslop, 2002; with
permission from Nature Publishing Group).
the translation of APP. Thus iron levels could modulate the generation of both
plaques and tangles. Besides the binding of metals to Ab, TF has been shown to
modulate several physiochemical properties of Ab including the rate of aggregation.
The ability of neurons to regulate the influx, eZux, and subcellular compart-
mentalization of calcium is compromised in DAT. This appears due to age-related
oxidative stress, and impaired metabolism/accumulation of Ab oligomers. Ab can
promote cellular calcium overload via oxidative stress and by forming pores in the
membrane, rendering neurons vulnerable to excitotoxicity and apoptosis
(Bezprozvanny et al., 2008). Mutations of presenilin genes that cause early onset
DAT also cause ER (endoplasmic reticulum) calcium overload by impairing the
normal ER calcium leak-channel function of the presenilins. A perturbation in
calcium homeostasis may be involved in early DAT by altering APP processing
and thus Ab production. Calcium activated neural proteinases (CANPs or cal-
pains) are key enzymes in the intracellular signaling cascade and thus may mediate
calcium dependent neural degeneration. Pharmacological modulation of the
CANP system could be a potential therapeutic strategy in Alzheimer’s disease
(Saito et al., 1993).
De Luigi et al. (2002) studied the presence of circulating cytokines and the
ability of blood cells to release them in response to inflammatory stimuli in
patients with diVerent forms of dementia. A significant increase in circulating
interleukin-1b was found in moderate DAT. Chronic inflammation as found in
head trauma is an important risk factor for DAT, and the inflammatory process
aVects glial-neuronal interactions. The activation of the glial inflammatory pro-
cess, either caused by genetic or environmental insults to neurons, produces glial
derived cytokines and other proteins that results in neurodegeneration. Interleu-
kin 1 is key in initiating and coordinating neuronal synthesis and in the processing
of APP, resulting in the continuous deposition of Ab. It also promotes astrocytic
synthesis and release of inflammatory and neuroactive molecules such as S100b,
which is a neurite growth promoting cytokine. This cytokine can stress neurons
fostering neuronal cell dysfunction, causing apoptosis by increasing intraneuronal
free calcium concentrations. This neuronal injury eVect activates microglial cells
which then overexpress inteleukin 1, thereby producing feedback amplification
and self-propagation of this cytokine cycle (GriYn et al., 1998).
(i.e., tumor necrosis factor converting enzyme). The b site APP cleaving enzyme-1
(BACE1), b secretase, is a type I integral membrane protein from the pepsin
family of aspartyl proteases. Gamma secretase is an intramembranous complex of
enzymes composed of presenilin 1 or 2, nicastrin, anterior pharynx defective-1,
and presenilin enhancer 2, with the presenilin being the active site (Wolfe et al.,
1999; Yu et al., 2000).
There are two pathways in the cleavage and processing of APP. One is non-
amyloidogenic. APP is cleaved by " secretase at amino acid position 83, the
carboxy terminus, producing a large amino terminal ectodomain (sAPP"). This is
secreted into the extracellular medium where it is in turn cleaved by ! secretase
producing a short peptide, p3. The cleavage by " secretase is in the middle of the
APP region, and therefore there is no Ab production. The amyloidogenic pathway
leads to Ab generation. Initially the b secretase cleaves at amino acid position 99,
releasing sAPP b into the extracellular space. Meanwhile the C99 fragment stays in
the membrane where it is cleaved by ! secretase between residues 38 and 43,
liberating a peptide which is 40 amino acids in length (Ab40). Only a small
proportion (10%) composes the 42 residue, the Ab peptide (Ab42). Ab is more
hydrophobic and thus prone to form fibrils, and is abundant in senile plaques
( Jarrett et al., 1993; Martin, 1999; Suh and Checler, 2002; Younkin, 1998). Muta-
tions in the APP gene linked to early-onset DAT produce a product that is preferen-
tially cleaved to release Ab 42 (Selkoe, 2001; Sisodia and St. George-Hyslop, 2002)
(see Fig. 2).
Non-amyloidogenic Amyloidogenic
sAPPa
sAPPb
Presenilin 1or 2
Nicastrin
ADAM9, PEN2
10 or 17 BACE1 APH-1 Ab
FIG. 2. Pathways in the cleavage and processing of APP (Courtesy of LaFerla et al., 2007; with
permission from Nature Publishing Group).
THE MOLECULAR AND CELLULAR PATHOGENESIS OF DAT 157
Copper and Zinc ions can induce Ab aggregation. The chelator, clioquinol
(PBT-1), reduces brain deposition of Ab in DAT transgenic mice. Clinical trials
were stopped when compound impurities produced toxicity. However, PBT-2
(a second generation metal protein attenuating compound which does not contain
iodine) is now undergoing clinical trial. In a DAT transgenic mouse model,
preliminary results suggested improvement in a subset of cognitive measures, as
well as reduction in cerebrospinal fluid and brain Ab. Apomorphine and its
derivatives promote the oligomerization of Ab but inhibit the fibrillization.
Structural activity demonstrated that 10, 11-dihydroxy substitutions in the D
ring of apomorphine are required for the inhibitory eVects. Methylation of
these hydroxyl groups reduces the inhibitory potency. The ability of these
molecules to inhibit Ab formation appears to be linked to their tendency to
undergo rapid auto-oxidation, suggesting that auto-oxidation products act
directly or indirectly on Ab and inhibit fibrillization. This paradigm oVers the
potential for designing more eYcient inhibitors of Ab amyloidogenesis in vivo
(Lashuel et al., 2002).
Nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs) reduce brain Ab accumula-
tion in DAT transgenic mice. This eVect may be due to inhibition of COX or via
direct eVect on ! secretase. However, clinical trials with typical NSAIDs and
COX-2 inhibitors (celocoxib and refecoxib) as well as other anti-inflammatories
including prednisone and hydroxychloroquine, in established DAT showed no
positive eVects on cognition levels. It is postulated that this failure was due to the
lack of ability to reverse already established pathology, and that the intervention
has to be in the early stages of mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Statins also
reduce brain Ab accumulation in DAT transgenic mice. However, prospective
studies showed no association between the use of statins and the risk of DAT
development, nor were there any changes in CSF Ab levels. Large, extended,
randomized clinical trials are needed to determine if any benefit can be obtained
with this drug class.
Nitric oxide may be involved in DAT neurodegeneration. By using redox
proteomic techniques, 10 proteins have been associated with an increase of nitro-
tyrosine immunoreactivity in the brains of DAT patients. Of these proteins,
"-enolase has been shown to be oxidized in the brains of DAT patients (Calabrese
et al., 2007; Castegna et al., 2002). It is one of the subunits of enolase which catalizes
the reversible conversion of 2-phosphoglycerate to phosphoenolpyruvate in glycoly-
sis. This, in addition to the increase of nitration of the triosephosphate isomerase that
interconverts dihydroxyacetone phosphate and 3-phosphoglyceraldehyde in glycol-
ysis, may explain the altered glucose tolerance and metabolism seen in DAT
patients. Neuropeptide h3 (a phosphatidylethanolamine-binding protein), hippo-
campal cholinergic neurostimulating peptide, and raf-kinase inhibitor protein have
various functions in the brain. In vitro, they upregulate levels of choline acetyltrans-
ferase in cholinergic neurons following N-methyl D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor
THE MOLECULAR AND CELLULAR PATHOGENESIS OF DAT 159
activation. The activity of this enzyme is reduced in DAT patients. Together with
cholinergic deficits, the nitration of neuropolypeptide h3 and the subsequent loss of
neurotrophic action on cholinergic neurons of the hippocampus and basal forebrain
may be part of the explanation of cognitive decline in DAT (Giacobini, 2003; Rossor
et al., 1982).
The polyphenolic molecule, curcumin, shows promise in reducing nitrosative
brain injury and delaying the onset of neurodegenerative disorders. It is a strong
antioxidant that inhibits lipid peroxidation intercepting and neutralizing ROS
and reactive nitrogen species (RNS) and increasing haeme-oxygenase I expression
in astrocytes and neurons. Dietary curcumin suppressed indicators of inflamma-
tion and oxidative damage in the brains of DAT transgenic mice (Butterfield et al.,
2002; Scapagnini et al., 2006). However, antioxidants including vitamin E did not
show any eVect on the rate of progression of MCI to DAT in large studies.
Drugs that reduce tau phosphorylation by inhibiting tau such as CDK5 (cyclin-
dependent kinase 5) and GSK-3b (glycogen synthase kinase 3b) are being tested.
Since tau phosphorylation results from the action of multiple kinases and
phosphatases, these drugs may not eVect normalization of tau phosphorylation.
Despite the identification of mutations in three genes associated with early-
onset DAT (i.e., the APP, PS1, and PS2 genes) and one genetic risk factor for
sporadic DAT (the gene for apolipoprotein E), our understanding of the patho-
logical genetic mechanism remains elusive. There are additionally over 500
diVerent gene candidates which may be involved in DAT pathogenesis, and at
least 20 loci with modest but significant eVects on the risk for this disease (Bertram
and Tanzi, 2008). The APP gene is located at chromosome 21q21.3. Twenty-nine
mutations in 78 aVected families have been identified. These mutations are close
to the b and ! secretase APP cleavage sites (Goate et al., 1991). The PS1 gene is
located at chromosome 14q24.3 and 166 mutations have been identified in 362
families. These mutations are associated with an increase in the Ab42/Ab40
ratio, and are located throughout the gene for the PS1 molecule where ! secretase
complexing occurs (Sherrington et al., 1995). PS2 localizes to chromosome 1q31-42.
Ten mutations have been identified in 18 aVected families. There is an increase in
the Ab42/Ab40 ratio, similar to the eVects of PS1 gene mutations. The mutations
are found throughout the gene, and thus ! secretase complexing is aVected
(Levy-Lahad et al., 1995; Rogaev et al., 1995).
Roberson et al. (2007) studied a hybrid mouse expressing mutant APP
and having the tau gene inactivated. The elimination of tau did not alter the
accumulation of senile plaques but did prevent the memory loss, behavioral
disturbances and sudden death associated with this DAT-like phenotype. The
complete elimination of tau was not necessary, a reduction of 50% producing
significant improvements. On the other hand, soluble Ab assemblies cause mem-
ory loss and bind to dendritic spines. It would appear that soluble Ab assemblies
and not the precipitated senile plaques are the cause of impaired cognition, but
160 LUQUE AND JAFFE
the exact mechanism remains ill-defined (Lacor et al., 2004). Roberson appropri-
ately sounded a note of caution in extrapolating mouse data into human patho-
physiology in which other compensatory mechanisms may be playing a role
(Avila et al., 2004).
Bertram and Tanzi (2008) discussed 10 diVerent selected genes potentially
implicated in DAT. Angiotensin converting enzyme (ACE) has been shown
in vitro to degrade naturally secreted Ab and this could explain the increased
risk of DAT in carriers of the ACE I allele; however, the relevance in vivo needs to
be demonstrated. CH25H (cholesterol 25-hydrolase) which is responsible for the
synthesis of 25-hydroxycholesterol, is a potent regulator of transcription of genes
involved in cholesterol and lipid metabolism. There seems to be an association
between variants of CH25H and DAT. One study (Papassotiropoulus et al., 2005)
suggested an increased DAT risk associated with a certain CH25H haplotype
(containing the rs13500 risk allele) leading to an increase in CSF lathosterol
(a metabolite precursor of cholesterol) as well as a higher brain Ab load with
lower levels of Ab in the CSF of non-demented elderly subjects. Both CH25H and
lipase A (LIPA) as well as APOE are involved in cholesterol metabolism, and Ab
production and clearance have been shown to be regulated by cholesterol. Drugs
that inhibit cholesterol synthesis lower Ab in cellular and animal models.
Nicotinic acetylcholine receptors are widely expressed in the brain, and the
b2 (CHRNB2) subunit is particularly abundant and forms a heteropentameric
receptor ("4b2) with the "4 subunit. The CHRNB2 gene which encodes the
nAChR b2 subunit maps to chromosome 1q21. Two independent genetic associ-
ation studies have investigated the potential involvement of this gene in DAT risk.
Wu et al. (2004) found that Ab directly inhibits the "4b2 receptor complex, and
that nicotine possibly by activating the NACHR can mitigate the toxic eVects
of Ab, as well as influencing tau phosphorylation in vitro and in vivo (Oddo
et al., 2005).
Grb-2-associated binder 2 (GAB2) is a highly conserved scaVolding/adaptor
protein involved in signaling pathways and in particular in the transduction of
cytokine and growth-receptor signaling (Liu and Rohrschneider, 2002; Sarmay
et al., 2006). GAB2 is expressed ubiquitously, although in greatest amounts in
white blood cells, the prefrontal cortex, and the hypothalamus. All 10 GAB2 gene
single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) showed significant association with DAT.
A protective role is predicated for all the minor alleles. All 10 of the SNPs display a
high LOD score (log10 ratio of probability data occurring if loci linked, to
probability data occurring with unlinked loci), and therefore probably point to a
single underlying signal eVect. Of the 10 associated markers, only one,
rsl1385600, is predicted to map within the coding region for GAB2 where it
does not appear to produce a change in amino acid sequence. It has been
suggested that changes in GAB2 expression could potentially aVect glycogen
synthase kinase 3 (GSK3)-dependent phosphorylation of tau, and thus formation
THE MOLECULAR AND CELLULAR PATHOGENESIS OF DAT 161
References
Andersen, O. M., Schmidt, V., Spoelgen, R., Gliemann, J., Behlke, J., Galatis, D., Mckinstry, J.,
Parker, M. W., Masters, C. L., Hyman, B. T., Cappai, R., and Willnow, T. E. (2006). Molecular
dissection of the interaction between amyloid precursor protein and its neuronal traYcking
receptor Sort LA/LR11. Biochemistry 45, 2618–2628.
Avila, J., Lucas, J. J., Perez, M., and Hernandez, F. (2004). Role of tau protein in both physiological
and pathological conditions. Physiol. Rev. 84, 361–384.
162 LUQUE AND JAFFE
Ballatore, C., Lee, V. M., and Trojanowski, J. Q. (2007). Tau mediated neurodegeneration in
Alzheimer’s disease and related disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 663–672.
Bertram, L., and Tanzi, R. E. (2008). Thirty years of Alzheimer’s disease genetics: The implications of
systematic meta-analyses. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 768–778.
Berzin, T. M., Zipser, B. D., Rafii, M. S., Kuo-Leblanc, V., Yancopoulos, G. D., Glass, D. J.,
Fallon, J. R., and Stopa, E. G. (2000). Agrin and microvascular damage in Alzheimer’s disease.
Neurobiol. Aging 21, 349–355.
Bezprozvanny, I., and Mattson, M. P. (2008). Neuronal calcium mishandling and the pathogenesis of
Alzheimer’s Disease. Trends Neurosci. 31, 454–463.
Brewer, G. J. (2007). Iron and copper toxicity in diseases of aging, particularly atherosclerosis and
Alzheimer’s disease. Exp. Biol. Med. (Maywood) 232, 323–335.
Butterfield, D. A., Castegna, A., Pocernich, C. B., Drake, J., Scapagnini, G., and Calabrese, V. (2002).
Nutritional approaches to combat oxidative stress in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Nutr. Biochem. 13, 444.
Calabrese, V., Mancuso, C., Calvani, M., Rizzarelli, E., Butterfield, D. A., and Stella, A. M. (2007).
Nitric oxide in the central nervous system: Neuroprotection versus neurotoxicity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
8, 766–775.
Castegna, A., Aksenov, M., Thongboonkerd, V. R., Klein, J. B., Pierce, W. M., Booze, R.,
Markesbery, W. R., and Butterfield, D. A. (2002). Proteomic identification of oxydatively modified
proteins in Alzheimer’s disease brain. Part II: Dihydropyrimidinase related protein 2,
alpha-enolase and heat shock cognate 71. J. Neurochem. 82, 1524–1532.
De Luigi, A., Pizzimenti, S., Quadri, P., Lucca, U., Tettamanti, M., Fragiacomo, C., and
De Simoni, M. G. (2002). Peripheral inflammatory response in Alzheimer’s disease and multi-
infarct dementia. Neurobiol. Dis. 11, 308–314.
De Vos, K. J., Grierson, A. J., Ackerley, S., and Miller, C. C. (2008). Role of axonal transport in
neurodegenerative diseases. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31, 151–173.
Giacobini, E. (2003). Cholinergic function and Alzheimer’s disease. Int. J. Geriat. Psychiatry 18, S1–S5.
Goate, A., Chartier-Harlin, M. C., Mullan, M., Brown, J., Crawford, F., Fidani, L., GluVra, L.,
Haynes, A., Irving, N., James, L., Mant, R., Newton, P., et al. (1991). Segregation of a missense
mutation in the amyloid precursor protein gene with familial Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 349,
704–706.
GriYn, W. S. T., Sheng, J. G., Royston, M. C., Gentleman, S. M., Mckenzie, J. E., Graham, D. I.,
Roberts, G. W., and Mrak, R. E. (1998). Glial-neuronal interactions in Alzheimer’s disease:
The potential role of a ‘cytokine cycle’ in disease progression. Brain Pathol. 8, 65–72.
Grupe, A., Abraham, R., Li, Y., Rowland, C., Hollingworth, P., Morgan, A., Jehu, L., Segurado, R.,
Stone, D., Schadt, E., Karnoub, M., Nowotny, P., et al. (2007). Evidence for novel susceptibility
genes for late-onset Alzheimer’s disease from a genome-wide association study of putative
functional variants. Hum. Mol. Genet. 16, 865–873.
Hamacher, M., Meyer, H. E., and Marcus, K. (2007). New access to Alzheimer’s and other neurode-
generative diseases. Expert Rev. Proteomics 4, 591–594.
Hiruma, H., Katakura, T., Takahashi, S., Ichikawa, T., and Kawakami, T. (2003). Glutamate and
amyloid beta-protein rapidly inhibit fast axonal transport in cultured rat hippocampal neurons by
diVerent mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 23, 8967–8977.
Jarrett, J. T., Berger, E. P., and Lansbury, P. T., Jr. (1993). The carboxy terminus of the b amyloid
protein is critical for seeding of amyloid formation: Implications for the pathogenesis of Alzhei-
mer’s disease. Biochemistry 32, 4693–4697.
Knott, A. B., Perkins, G., Schwarzenbacher, R., and Bossy-Wetzel, E. (2008). Mitochondrial fragmen-
tation in neurodegeneration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 505–518.
Lacor, P. N., Buniel, M. C., Chang, L., Fernandez, S. J., Gong, Y., Viola, K. L., Lambert, M. P.,
Velasco, P. T., Bigio, E. H., Finch, C. E., Kraft, G. A., and Klein, W. L. (2004). Synaptic targeting
by Alzheimer’s-related amyloid beta oligomers. J. Neurosci. 24, 10191–10200.
THE MOLECULAR AND CELLULAR PATHOGENESIS OF DAT 163
LaFerla, F. M., Green, K. N., and Oddo, S. (2007). Intracellular amyloid-beta in Alzheimer’s disease.
Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 499–509.
Lashuel, H. A., Hartley, D. M., Balakhaneh, D., Aggarwal, A., Teichberg, S., and Callaway, D. J.
(2002). New class of inhibitors of amyloid-beta fibril formation. Implications for the mechanism of
pathogenesis in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Biol. Chem. 277, 42881–42890.
Levy-Lahad, E., Wasco, W., Poorkaj, P., Romano, D. M., Oshima, J., Pettingell, W. H., Yu, C. E.,
Jondro, P. D., Schmidt, S. D., and Wang, K. (1995). Candidate gene for the chromosome 1
familial Alzheimer’s disease locus. Science 269, 973–977.
Liu, Y., and Rohrschneider, L. R. (2002). The gift of Gab. FEBS Lett. 515, 1–7.
Manczak, M., Anekonda, T. S., Henson, E., Park, B. S., Quinn, J., and Reddy, P. H. (2006). Mitochon-
dria are a direct site of A beta accumulation in Alzheimer’s disease neurons: Implications for free
radical generation and oxidative damage in disease progression. Hum. Mol. Genet. 15, 1437–1449.
Martin, J. B. (1999). Molecular basis of the neurodegenerative disorders. N. Engl. J. Med. 340,
1970–1980.
Matos, M., Augusto, E., Oliveira, C. R., and Agostinho, P. (2008). Amyloid-beta peptide decreases
glutamate uptake in cultured astrocytes: Involvement of oxidative stress and mitogen-activated
protein kinase cascades. Neuroscience 156, 898–910.
Meyer-Luehmann, M., Spires-Jones, T. L., Prada, C., Garcia-Allosa, M., de Calignon, A.,
Rozkalne, A., Koeningsknecht-Talboo, J., Holtzman, D. M., Bacskai, B. J., and Hyman, B. T.
(2008). Rapid appearance and local toxicity of amyloid-beta plaques in a mouse model of
Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 451, 720–724.
Nizzari, M., Venezia, V., Repetto, E., Caorsi, V., Magrassi, R., Gagliani, M. C., Carlo, P., Florio, T.,
Schettini, G., Tacchetti, C., Russo, T., Diaspro, A., et al. (2007). Amyloid precursor protein and
presenilin 1 interact with the adaptor GRB2 and modulates ERK 1,2 signaling. J. Biol. Chem. 282,
13833–13844.
Oddo, S., Caccamo, A., Green, K. N., Liang, K., Tran, L., Chen, Y., Leslie, F. M., and LaFerla, F. M.
(2005). Chronic nicotine administration exacerbates tau pathology in a transgenic model of
Alzheimer’s disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102, 3046–3051.
Papassotiropoulos, A., Lambert, J. C., Wavrant-De Vrieze, F., Wollmer, M. A., von der Kammer, H.,
StreVer, J. R., Maddalena, A., Huynh, K. D., Wolleb, S., Lutjohann, D., Schneider, B.,
Thal, D. R., et al. (2005). Cholesterol 25-hydroxylase on chromosome 10q is a susceptibility
gene for sporadic Alzhmeimer’s disease. Neurodegener. Dis. 2, 233–241.
Permanne, B., Adessi, C., Saborio, G. P., Fraga, S., Frossard, M. J., Van Dorpe, J., Dewachter, I.,
Banks, W. A., Van Leuven, F., and Soto, C. (2002). Reduction of amyloid load and cerebral
damage in transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease by treatment with a beta-sheet breaker
peptide. FASEB J. 16, 860–862.
Pigino, G., Morfini, G., Pelsman, A., Mattson, M. P., Brady, S. T., and Busciglio, J. (2003). Alzheimer’s
presenilin 1 mutations impair kinesin-base axonal transport. J. Neurosci. 23, 4499–4508.
Rajendra, L., Schneider, A., Schlechtingen, G., Weidlich, S., Ries, J., Braxmeier, T., Schwille, P.,
Schulz, J., Schroeder, C., Simons, M., Jennings, G., Knolker, H. J., et al. (2008). EYcient
inhibition of the Alzheimer’s disease beta-secretase by membrane targeting. Science 320, 520–523.
Roberson, E. D., Scearce-Levie, K., Palop, J. J., Yan, F., Cheng, I. H., Wu, T., Gerstein, H., Yu, G. Q.,
and Mucker, L. (2007). Reducing endogenous tau ameliorates amyloid b-induced deficits in an
Alzheimer’s disease mouse model. Science 316, 750–754.
Rogaev, E. I., Sherrington, R., Rogaeva, E. A., Levesque, G., Ikeda, M., Liang, Y., Chin, H., Lin, C.,
Holman, K., Tsuda, T., Mar, L., Sorbi, S., et al. (1995). Familial Alzheimer’s disease in kindreds
with missense mutations in a gene on chromosome 1 related to Alzheimer’s disease type 3 gene.
Nature 376, 775–778.
164 LUQUE AND JAFFE
Rossor, M. N., Svendsen, C., Hunt, S. P., Mountjov, C. Q., Roth, M., and Iversen, L. L. (1982). The
substantia innominata in Alzheimer’s disease: An histochemical and biochemical study of cholin-
ergic marker enzymes. Neurosci. Lett. 28, 217–222.
Sadowski, M., Pankiewicz, J., Scholtzova, H., Ripellino, J. A., Li, Y., Schmidt, S. D., Mathews, P. M.,
Fryer, J. D., Holtzman, D. M., Sigurdsson, E. M., and Wisniewski, T. (2004). A synthetic peptide
blocking the apolipoprotein E/beta-amyloid binding mitigates beta-amyloid toxicity and fibril
formation in vitro and reduces beta-amyloid plaques in transgenic mice. Am. J. Pathol. 165,
937–948.
Saito, K., Elce, J. S., Hamos, J. E., and Nixon, R. A. (1993). Widespread activation of calcium-
activated neutral proteinase (calpain) in the brain in Alzheimer’s disease: A potential molecular
basis for neuronal degeneration. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 90, 2628–2632.
Sarmay, G., Angyal, A., Kertesz, A., Maus, M., and Medgyesi, D. (2006). The multiple function of
Grb2 associated binder (Gab) adaptor/scaVolding protein in immune cell signaling. Immunol. Lett.
104, 76–82.
Scapagnini, G., Clombrita, C., Amadio, M., D’Agata, V., Arcelli, E., Sapienza, M., Quattrone, A., and
Calabrese, V. (2006). Curcumin activates defensive genes and protects neurons against oxidative
stress. Antioxid. Redox. Signal. 8, 395–403.
Schilling, S., Zeitschel, U., HoVmann, T., Heiser, U., Francke, M., Kehlen, A., Holzer, M., Hutter-
Paier, G., Prokesch, M., Windisch, M., Jagla, W., Schlenzig, D., et al. (2008). Glutaminyl cyclase
inhibition attenuates pyroglutamate Ab and Alzheimer’s disease-like pathology. Nat. Med. 14,
1106–1111.
Schwarze-Eicker, K., Keyvani, K., Gortz, N., Westaway, D., Sachser, N., and Paulus, W. (2005). Prion
protein (PrPc) promotes beta-amyloid plaque formation. Neurobiol. Aging 26, 1177–1182.
Selkoe, D. J. (2001). Alzheimer’s disease: Genes, proteins, and therapy. Physiol. Rev. 81, 741–766.
Sherrington, R., Rogaev, E. I., Liang, Y., Rogaeva, E. A., Levesque, G., Ikeda, M., Chi, H., Lin, C.,
Li, G., Holman, K., Tsuda, T., Mar, L., et al. (1995). Cloning of a gene bearing missence mutations
in early-onset familial Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 375, 754–760.
Sisodia, S. S., and St George-Hyslop, P. H. (2002). Gamma-secretase, notch, Abeta and Alzheimer’s
disease: Where do the presenilins fit in? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 281–290.
Suh, Y. H., and Checler, F. (2002). Amyloid precursor protein, presenilins, and alpha-synuclein:
Molecular pathogenesis and pharmacologal applications in Alzheimer’s Disease. Pharmacol. Rev.
54, 469–525.
Tai, H. C., and Schuman, E. M. (2008). Ubiquitin, the proteasome and protein degradation in
neuronal function and dysfunction. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 826–838.
Wolfe, M. S., Xia, W., Ostaszewski, B. L., Diehl, T. S., Kimberly, W. T., and Selkoe, D. J. (1999). Two
transmembrane aspartates in presenilin-1 required for presenilin endoproteolysis and gamma-
secretase activity. Nature 398, 513–517.
Wu, J., Kuo, Y. P., George, A. A., Xu, L., Hu, J., and Lukas, R. J. (2004). Beta- amyloid directly
inhibits human alpha 4 beta 2 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors heterologously expressed in
human SH-EP1 cells. J. Biol. Chem. 279, 37842–37851.
Yamamoto, A., Shin, R. W., Hasegawa, K., Naiki, H., Sato, H., Yoshimasu, F., and Kitamoto, T.
(2002). Iron (III) induces aggregation of hyperphosphorylated tau and its reduction to iron (II)
reverses the aggregation: Implications in the formation of neurofibrillary tangles of Alzheimer’s
disease. J. Neurochem. 82, 1137–1147.
Yamazaki, H., Bujo, H., Kusonoki, J., Seimiya, K., Kanaki, T., Morisaki, N., Scheneider, W. J., and
Saito, Y. (1996). Elements of neural adhesion molecules and a yeast vacuolar protein sorting
receptor are present in a novel mammalian low density lipoprotein receptor family member.
J. Biol. Chem. 271, 24761–24768.
Younkin, S. G. (1998). The role of A beta 42 in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Physiol. (Paris) 92, 289–292.
THE MOLECULAR AND CELLULAR PATHOGENESIS OF DAT 165
Yu, G., Niashimura, M., Arawaka, S., Levitan, D., Zhang, L., Tandon, A., Song, Y. Q., Rogaeva, E.,
Chen, F., Kawarai, T., Supala, A., Levesque, L., et al. (2000). Nicastrin modulates presenilin
mediated notch/glp-1 signal transduction and betaAPP processing. Nature 407, 48–54.
Zhang, Q., Powers, E. T., Nieva, J., HyV, M. E., Dendle, M. A., Bieschle, J., Glabe, C. G.,
Eschenmoser, A., Wentworth, P., Lerner, R. A., and Kelly, J. W. (2004). Metabolite-initiated
protein misfolding may trigger Alzheimer’s disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101, 4752–4757.
ALZHEIMER’S DISEASE GENETICS: CURRENT STATUS
AND FUTURE PERSPECTIVES
Lars Bertram
Neuropsychiatric Genetics Group, Department of Vertebrate Genomics, Max-Planck
Institute for Molecular Genetics, Berlin 14195, Germany
Genetics and Aging Research Unit, Department of Neurology,
Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown,
Massachusetts, 02129, USA
complicated by gene-gene interactions that are diYcult to predict and model, and
a sizeable but diYcult to detect and measure environmental component.
Notwithstanding these challenges, several chromosomal regions thought to
harbor novel LOAD genes have been identified via whole genome linkage
analyses, some, for example, on chromosomes 9, 10, and 12, overlapping across
diVerent samples (Bertram et al., 2000; Blacker et al., 2003; Ertekin-Taner et al.,
2000; Hamshere et al., 2007; Kehoe et al., 1999; Li et al., 2002; Myers et al., 2000;
Pericak-Vance et al., 2000). Furthermore, well over 1000 ‘‘candidate gene’’
studies—that is, studies that focus on certain genes based on some prior hypothesis
regarding their potential involvement in the disease process—have been published
over the past two decades claiming or refuting genetic association between putative
AD genes and aVection status and/or certain endophenotypes (Bertram et al., 2007).
Currently, nearly 150 AD genetic association papers are published each year,
at increasing pace. However, with the exception of one genetic variant, the
e4-allele of the apolipoprotein E gene (APOE; Saunders et al., 1993; Strittmatter
et al., 1993) none of these candidates has been proved to consistently influence
disease risk or onset age in more than a handful of samples (Bertram and
Tanzi, 2008). Instead, most studies of ‘‘novel AD genes’’ have been followed by a
large number of conflicting reports, challenging prior claims that they may play an
important role in contributing to disease risk. For geneticists as well as clinicians the
growing number of (mostly conflicting) genetic association findings in AD has
become increasingly diYcult to follow, evaluate, and interpret.
Grupe et al. (2007) Case-control USA and UK 17,343 380 (1428) 396 (2062) APOE*, ACAN, BCR, CTSS,
EBF3, GALP,
GWA_14q32.13,
GWA_7p15.2, LMNA,
LOC651924, MYH13,
PCK1, PGBD1, THEM5,
TNK1, TRAK2, UBD
170
Coon et al. (2007) and Case-control USA, Netherlands 502,627 446 (861) 290 (550) APOE*, GAB2
Reiman et al. (2007)
Li et al. (2008) Case-control Canada 469,438 753 (1071) 736 (985) APOE*, GOLM1,
GWA_15q21.2,
GWA_9p24.3
Bertram (2008) Family-based USA 484,522 941 (2708) 404 (1242) APOE*, ATXN1, CD33,
GWA_14q31,
Modified after content on the AlzGene Web site (http://www.alzgene.org; current on October 31st, 2008). Studies are listed in order of publication date.
‘‘Featured Genes’’ are those genes/loci that were declared as ‘‘associated’’ in the original publication, note that criteria for declaring association may vary across
studies.
*
Indicates that surrogate markers were used for APOE.
a
Numbers of ‘‘AD cases’’ and ‘‘controls’’ refer to sample sizes used in initial GWA screening, whereas ‘‘total’’ refers to initial sample plus any follow-up
samples (where applicable); please consult AlzGene Web site for more details on these studies.
CURRENT STATUS OF ALZHEIMER’S GENETICS 171
Again, with the exception of a single SNP in strong LD with APOE-e4, no other
genome-wide significant signals were observed. In a follow-up paper (Reiman
et al., 2007), the same group reported evidence of association with variants in the
gene encoding GRB2-associated binding protein 2 (GAB2) on chromosome
11q14, which only became evident after their samples were stratified for APOE-
e4, that is, when AD cases and controls were divided into carriers and non-carriers
of the e4-allele. A third group (Li et al., 2008) tested nearly "470,000 markers and
reported association with variants in golgi membrane protein 1 (GOLM1) on
chromosome 9q22 and two currently uncharacterized loci on chromosomes 9p
and 15q. Finally, our own group (Bertram et al., 2008a) recently reported the
results of the first GWAS using family-based samples ("1400 DNAs from over
400 families), as opposed the three previously summarized studies which were
solely based on case-control datasets. Applying the same 500 K marker array as in
Coon et al. (2007) and Li et al. (2008) we found—in addition to APOE-e4—
evidence for a number of potential AD loci (Table I). Although the findings
from these first GWAS hold the promise of pinpointing novel pathways and
mechanisms potentially important in AD pathogenesis, it will be important to
await consistent replication by independent investigators.
While these and several of the forthcoming GWAS have the potential to
significantly advance our understanding of the genetics and pathogenetic
mechanisms of AD and other neurodegenerative disorders, it needs to be empha-
sized that in many ways genome-wide screens are actually not so diVerent from
conventional candidate gene association analyses. Both search for significantly
diVerent allele or genotype distributions or transmissions in subjects aVected by
the disease/phenotype as compared to presumably healthy individuals. The two
approaches diVer mostly on a quantitative level: instead of testing a few tens of
markers (or less), GWAS simultaneously screen a few hundreds of thousands of
markers (or more). The major qualitative diVerence between a GWAS and candi-
date gene analysis is that the former investigates the whole genome in a more or
less unbiased fashion, whereas the latter only investigates a limited number of
specific loci proved or thought to be involved in disease predisposition or progres-
sion (e.g., in AD many of these loci are involved in the production, traYcking, or
removal of A!). On the downside and owing to their sheer number of polymorph-
isms tested, GWAS actually substantially compound the problem that has plagued
genetic studies of complex phenotypes in the past, that is, to determine which of
the many reported putative risk alleles are ‘‘real’’ as opposed to which merely
reflect statistical artifacts. The first essential step in diVerentiating these two
alternatives is to provide independent replication of the association (McCarthy
et al., 2008), just as for any result emerging from ‘‘old-fashioned’’ candidate gene
analyses.
172 LARS BERTRAM
173
hCG2039140 rs1903908 1.23 (1.06-1.44) 0.007 1368 1497 4 ALL
TFAM rs2306604 0.82 (0.72-0.94) 0.003 1059 792 5 ALL
IL1B rs1143634 1.18 (1.03-1.34) 0.02 1011 1244 5 ALL
TF rs1049296 1.18 (1.04-1.33) 0.01 1916 4058 9 ALL
TNK1 rs1554948 0.86 (0.80-0.93) 0.0002 2743 2984 6 ALL
LOC439999 rs498055 1.15 (1.03-1.29) 0.02 2787 2498 7 ALL
GAPDHS rs4806173 0.87 (0.75-1.00) 0.05 1687 1775 4 ALL
DAPK1 rs4878104 0.88 (0.82-0.95) 0.002 2753 3036 7 ALL
PRNP rs1799990 0.88 (0.81-0.96) 0.03 2280 2943 10 CAU
GWA_14q32.13 rs11622883 0.88 (0.80-0.97) 0.01 2685 2893 6 ALL
GALP rs3745833 1.13 (1.00-1.29) 0.05 2739 2975 6 ALL
(Continued)
TABLE II (Continued)
174
List of loci, in descending order of genetic eVect size, containing at least one polymorphism showing nominally significant (P-value %0.05) summary ORs in
AlzGene on October 31st, 2008. To be considered as ‘‘Top Result,’’ summary OR needs to be significant across samples from all ethnic backgrounds (‘‘ALL’’) or
in Caucasians only (‘‘CAU’’). Whenever nominally statistically-significant results are observed for both, that is, ALL and CAU, only the analysis that has the
largest genetic eVect size (OR deviating the most from 1) is reported here. Note that AlzGene is continuously updated, so results displayed online may diVer from
the results above; consult the AlzGene Web site for up-to-date numbers and additional meta-analyses in these and other loci (http://www.alzgene.org) (see also
Table I).
a
Summary ORs and 95% confidence intervals (CI) are based on random-eVects allelic contrasts comparing minor versus major alleles at each
polymorphism.,
b
Number of samples refers to the number of independent case-control samples used in the meta-analyses; multiple samples may be reported in the same
publication and are considered separately if they are independent, that is, non-overlapping. Samples overlapping across publications are only used once, usually
by including the datasets with the largest number of available genotypes,
c
Results are based on comparing "4- versus "3-alleles at this locus.
CURRENT STATUS OF ALZHEIMER’S GENETICS 175
The strengths of AlzGene are obvious: assuming that the literature searches,
inclusion criteria, data management, and data analysis procedures actually provide a
correct and exhaustive account of the available literature, AlzGene is the single most
comprehensive resource for the status of genetics research in AD available to date. In
our original data freeze (Bertram et al., 2007) we could show that literature searches
in AlzGene outperformed those of several other literature/genetics databases, and
that the results of our meta-analyses were in very good agreement with estimates
published previously in nearly two dozen individual papers. Similar observations
were made in related database projects from our group (Allen et al., 2008; Bagade
et al., 2006). Published meta-analyses, however, have one important disadvantage:
by nature of their design, they run the risk of becoming outdated quickly, possibly as
soon as new data from one or two additional studies are published. Provided that
suYcient funding remains available, AlzGene does not have this caveat. Also, any
meta-analysis in the database can be updated literally within days after the publica-
tion of novel data. Another strength of AlzGene is that it is not limited to meta-
analyses on certain genes or networks of genes (e.g., those that are in the same
pathway or gene family), but considers all published loci simultaneously, making the
comparison of results across studies, genes, pathways, chromosomal regions, and so
on extremely easy. Finally, as outlined above, all loci containing at least one
polymorphism nominally significant by meta-analysis are separately highlighted
on the database’s homepage in a section called ‘‘Top Results.’’ Thus, consulting
this section of the AlzGene Web site will provide the user with a complete—and
essentially real time—snapshot of the ‘‘most promising’’ AD candidate genes, based
on the systematic evaluation of literally hundreds of individual studies and thousands
of data points. As such, the ‘‘Top Results’’ list could help prioritize future genetic
association studies (e.g., for further independent replication, or fine mapping), and
guide functional genetics and molecular studies investigating the potential pathoge-
netic mechanisms underlying the genetic associations.
While AlzGene undoubtedly represents a leap forward in managing and
displaying the data gathered within the domain of AD genetics research, its overall
approach, too, comes with some strings attached. First and foremost, despite our
comprehensive and systematic searches of the scientific literature, we cannot
exclude the possibility that some AD association studies were overlooked or
entered erroneously. This can be partly alleviated with the help of database users
who are explicitly encouraged to alert the curatorial team of any errors or omis-
sions, which will be fixed as soon as possible. Other limitations include our
restriction to allele contrasts in the meta-analyses (which allows no inference
of the true underlying mode of inheritance and is usually less powerful than
genotype-based tests), the non-consideration of haplotype-based genotype data or
176 LARS BERTRAM
high-density case-control GWAS (Li et al., 2008; Reiman et al., 2007) showed
association in a total of four family-based datasets comprising nearly 4200 DNAs
from over 1300 independent AD families. This was an important question as many
of the investigated variants had been thoroughly tested across relatively large
numbers of independent case-control datasets, but only a handful were also
previously assessed in family-based samples, which may be genetically diVerent
from unrelated, population-based cases and controls. Family-based methods have
the additional advantage of being robust against bias due to undetected population
stratification and phenotype misspecifications (Laird and Lange, 2006), which may
have aVected some of the case-control meta-analysis results. After combining the
results across all four datasets, we observed nominally significant association with
variants in ACE, CHRNB2, GAB2, TF, and an as yet unidentified locus on chromo-
some 7p15.2 (Table III). Two of these loci, that is, ACE and TF, were also found to
be associated with A! levels in CSF in an independent collection of AD cases and
controls (Kauwe et al., 2009). The independent convergence of (i) significant meta-
analysis results across case-control samples, (ii) replication of these associations in
AD family samples, and (iii) in the case of ACE and TF, a significant genotype-
dependent correlation with one of the few established biomarkers in AD, strongly
implies a genuine disease-risk modifying role of these loci, arguably more so than
for any of the other hundreds of suggested AD candidate genes besides APOE.
Functionally, all of these potential new AD susceptibility loci are interesting
and, if confirmed, may not only lead to a better understanding of the pathogenetic
mechanisms driving neurodegeneration, but may also help to pinpoint novel
treatment options for AD. ACE encodes angiotensin converting enzyme-1
(ACE-1), a ubiquitously expressed zinc metalloprotease that is involved in blood
pressure regulation. Several epidemiological studies suggest that high mid-life
blood pressure may increase the risk for AD in later life (Takeda et al., 2008).
More recently, ACE-1 activity has been reported to be increased in AD brains
proportionately to parenchymal A! load (Miners et al., 2008). The interpretation
of ACE’s role in AD pathogenesis is complicated by the observation that it is able
to degrade naturally secreted A! in vitro (Hu et al., 2001), which would predict an
increased risk in individuals with reduced ACE-1 levels/activity, that is, opposite
to what would be expected from the epidemiological data and the genetic associ-
ation results. CHRNB2 encodes the neuronal beta-2 nicotinic cholinergic receptor
(!2nAChR). Nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (nACcRs) are widely expressed in
the CNS, where the !2nAChR subunit is particularly abundant, forming hetero-
pentameric receptors with !4nAChR subunits (#4!2; Kalamida et al., 2007).
Pathologically, the reduction of nAChRs and the loss of cholinergic neurons in
disease-relevant brain regions is one of the major neurochemical hallmarks of
AD (Oddo and LaFerla, 2006), and several studies have suggested that an
age-dependent decrease in protein and/or mRNA levels of the #4!2-subtype
(in particular !2nAChR) occurs in the cortex and hippocampus of healthy
TABLE III
ASSESSMENT OF CASE-CONTROL ASSOCIATIONS IN AD FAMILY DATASETS
Gene SNP Ethnicity Model OR (95% CI) P-value No. Fams OR $2 P-valuea
ACE rs4291 CAU T vs. A 0.87 (0.76-0.99) 0.03 425 0.94 14.6 0.07
CHRNB2 rs4845378 ALL T vs. G 0.67 (0.50-0.90) 0.007 170 0.79 17.4 0.03
GAB2 rs7101429 CAU G vs. A 0.74 (0.59-0.93) 0.008 432 0.76 24.4 0.002
178
GWA_7p15.2 rs1859849 ALL C vs. T 1.11 (0.97-1.29) 0.1 335 1.26 17.5 0.03
TF rs104296 ALL C2 vs. C1 1.18 (1.04-1.33) 0.01 295 1.17 21.1 0.007
Comparison of the association evidence of AlzGene ‘‘Top Results’’ (current on October 31, 2008) and family-based analyses (Schjeide et al., 2009a,b). Note
that the meta-analyses for rs1859849 in GWA_7p15.2 are currently only marginally significant (P-value 0.1), but were nominally significant at the time of
genotyping in the Schjeide et al. (2009a) project. For up-to-date meta-analysis results on these variants, please consult the AlzGene Web site (http://www.alzgene.
org). ALL ¼ combining samples of all ethnic groups; CAU ¼ combining samples of Caucasian ethnicity only; No. Fams ¼ No. informative families; ‘‘OR’’
¼ summary odds ratio; ‘‘95% CI’’ ¼ 95% confidence intervals.
a
Test statistics are based on combining one-tailed P-values for each variant across all four samples using Fisher’s combined probability test, which results in
an 8 d.f. test. Single-locus analyses with variants in ACE only show marginally significant eVects (P-value 0.07), while haplotype-based analyses yielded P-values
<0.05 (see Schjeide et al.(2009a) for more details).
CURRENT STATUS OF ALZHEIMER’S GENETICS 179
VI. Conclusions
APOE
Non-
genetic Inflammation
Neurodegeneration
FIG. 1. Potential pathogenetic roles of AD susceptibility genes. Simplified summary of the potential patho-
physiologic implications of the currently most promising AD susceptibility loci, including those of recent
GWAS. Note that none of the genetic associations (except APOE ) can be considered unequivocally
established; the same is true for any of the proposed potential functional implications. Highlighted
eVects are likely further influenced by currently unknown gene-gene interactions and the contribution
of nongenetic factors as well as inflammatory reactions (which themselves are likely genetically con-
trolled). For didactic purposes, not all presumed or established functional implications have been
indicated, for example, GAB2’s role on APP metabolism via binding of Grb2. GWA 14q indicates
GWAS locus identified by Bertram et al. (2008a). For a more detailed discussion of the potential role of
these and other AD candidate genes see Bertram and Tanzi (2008).
popular, but it remains to be seen whether GWAS will live up to these expectations.
In the interim, systematic bioinformatics approaches synthesizing the results from
both candidate gene and genome-wide analyses will help researchers to keep track
of the myriad of genetic association findings to come. One such approach, the
AlzGene database, is now available, and already highlights a number of promising
AD loci by means of systematic, unbiased meta-analysis.
As should be clear from the above discussion, AlzGene does not aim to deliver
the last piece in the puzzle that AD genetic epidemiology research is trying to
solve. Rather, it attempts to provide a tool that can help researchers of many
disciplines decide which piece of the puzzle to try next. In the best case scenario, it
will also help to sharpen the overall picture of the genetic forces driving AD
predisposition and pathogenesis. Eventually, only the concerted eVorts of genet-
ics, genomics, proteomics, and clinical disciplines will give rise to new diagnostic
and therapeutic targets that, hopefully in the not too distant future, will benefit the
millions of patients aZicted with this devastating disorder.
Acknowledgments
Funding for AlzGene was provided by the Cure Alzheimer’s Fund (to LB). We are grateful to the
Alzheimer Research Forum (http://www.alzforum.org) for hosting AlzGene on their Web site.
CURRENT STATUS OF ALZHEIMER’S GENETICS 181
References
Allen, N. C., Bagade, S., McQueen, M. B., Ioannidis, J. P., Kavvoura, F. K., Khoury, M. J.,
Tanzi, R. E., and Bertram, L. (2008). Systematic meta-analyses and field synopsis of genetic
association studies in schizophrenia: The SzGene database. Nat. Genet. 40, 827–834.
Bagade, S., Allen, N. C., Tanzi, R. E., and Bertram, L. (2006). PD Gene–A novel and publicly
available database of genetic associated studies in Parkinson’s disease. 36th Meeting of the Society
for Neuroscience (Atlanta, GA), p. 173.
Bergem, A. L., Engedal, K., and Kringlen, E. (1997). The role of heredity in late-onset Alzheimer
disease and vascular dementia. A twin study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 54, 264–270.
Bertram, L. (2008). Genetic research in schizophrenia: New tools and future perspectives. Schizophr
Bull. 34, 806–812.
Bertram, L., Blacker, D., Mullin, K., Keeney, D., Jones, J., Basu, S., Yhu, S., McInnis, M. G.,
Go, R. C., Vekrellis, K., Selkoe, D. J., Saunders, A. J., et al. (2000). Evidence for genetic linkage
of Alzheimer’s disease to chromosome 10q. Science 290, 2302–2303.
Bertram, L., Lange, C., Mullin, K., Parkinson, M., Hsiao, M., Hogan, M. F., Schjeide, B. M.,
Hooli, B., Divito, J., Ionita, I., Jiang, H., Laird, N., et al. (2008a). Genome-wide association
analysis reveals putative Alzheimer’s disease susceptibility loci in addition to APOE. Am. J. Hum.
Genet. 83, 623–632.
Bertram, L., McQueen, M. B., Mullin, K., Blacker, D., and Tanzi, R. E. (2007). Systematic meta-
analyses of Alzheimer disease genetic association studies: The AlzGene database. Nat. Genet. 39,
17–23.
Bertram, L., Schjeide, B. M., Hooli, B., Mullin, K., Hiltunen, M., Soininen, H., Ingelsson, M.,
Lannfelt, L., Blacker, D., and Tanzi, R. E. (2008b). No association between CALHM1 and
Alzheimer’s disease risk. Cell 135, 993–994.
Bertram, L., and Tanzi, R. E. (2005). The genetic epidemiology of neurodegenerative disease. J. Clin.
Invest. 115, 1449–1457.
Bertram, L., and Tanzi, R. E. (2008). Thirty years of Alzheimer’s disease genetics: The implications of
systematic meta-analyses. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 768–778.
Blacker, D., Bertram, L., Saunders, A. J., Moscarillo, T. J., Albert, M. S., Wiener, H., Perry, R. T.,
Collins, J. S., Harrell, L. E., Go, R. C., Mahoney, A., Beaty, T., et al. (2003). Results of a high-
resolution genome screen of 437 Alzheimer’s disease families. Hum. Mol. Genet. 12, 23–32.
Brewer, G. J. (2007). Iron and copper toxicity in diseases of aging, particularly atherosclerosis and
Alzheimer’s disease. Exp. Biol. Med. (Maywood) 232, 323–335.
Coon, K. D., Myers, A. J., Craig, D. W., Webster, J. A., Pearson, J. V., Lince, D. H., Zismann, V. L.,
Beach, T. G., Leung, D., Bryden, L., Halperin, R. F., Marlowe, L., et al. (2007). A high-density
whole-genome association study reveals that APOE is the major susceptibility gene for sporadic
late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. J. Clin. Psychiatry 68, 613–618.
Ertekin-Taner, N., Graff-Radford, N., Younkin, L. H., Eckman, C., Baker, M., Adamson, J.,
Ronald, J., Blangero, J., Hutton, M., and Younkin, S. G. (2000). Linkage of plasma Abeta42 to
a quantitative locus on chromosome 10 in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease pedigrees. Science 290,
2303–2304.
Gatz, M., Reynolds, C. A., Fratiglioni, L., Johansson, B., Mortimer, J. A., Berg, S., Fiske, A., and
Pedersen, N. L. (2006). Role of genes and environments for explaining Alzheimer disease. Arch.
Gen. Psychiatry 63, 168–174.
Goate, A., Chartier-Harlin, M. C., Mullan, M., Brown, J., Crawford, F., Fidani, L., Giuffra, L.,
Haynes, A., Irving, N., James, L., et al. (1991). Segregation of a missense mutation in the amyloid
precursor protein gene with familial Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 349, 704–706.
182 LARS BERTRAM
Grupe, A., Abraham, R., Li, Y., Rowland, C., Hollingworth, P., Morgan, A., Jehu, L., Segurado, R.,
Stone, D., Schadt, E., Karnoub, M., Nowotny, P., et al. (2007). Evidence for novel susceptibility
genes for late-onset Alzheimer’s disease from a genome-wide association study of putative
functional variants. Hum. Mol. Genet. 16, 865–873.
Hamshere, M. L., Holmans, P. A., Avramopoulos, D., Bassett, S. S., Blacker, D., Bertram, L.,
Wiener, H., Rochberg, N., Tanzi, R. E., Myers, A., Wavrant-De Vrieze, F., Go, R., et al.
(2007). Genome-wide linkage analysis of 723 affected relative pairs with late-onset Alzheimer’s
disease. Hum. Mol. Genet. 16, 2703–2712.
Hardy, J., and Selkoe, D. J. (2002). The amyloid hypothesis of Alzheimer’s disease: Progress and
problems on the road to therapeutics. Science 297, 353–356.
Hoggart, C. J., Clark, T. G., De Iorio, M., Whittaker, J. C., and Balding, D. J. (2008). Genome-wide
significance for dense SNP and resequencing data. Genet. Epidemiol. 32, 179–185.
Hu, J., Igarashi, A., Kamata, M., and Nakagawa, H. (2001). Angiotensin-converting enzyme degrades
Alzheimer amyloid beta-peptide (A beta); retards A beta aggregation, deposition, fibril formation;
and inhibits cytotoxicity. J. Biol. Chem. 276, 47863–47868.
Ioannidis, J. P. (2005). Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Med. 2, e124.
Ioannidis, J. P., Ntzani, E. E., Trikalinos, T. A., and Contopoulos-Ioannidis, D. G. (2001). Replication
validity of genetic association studies. Nat. Genet. 29, 306–309.
Kalamida, D., Poulas, K., Avramopoulou, V., Fostieri, E., Lagoumintzis, G., Lazaridis, K., Sideri, A.,
Zouridakis, M., and Tzartos, S. J. (2007). Muscle and neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptors.
Structure, function and pathogenicity. Febs J. 274, 3799–3845.
Kauwe, J. S., Wang, J., Mayo, K., Morris, J. C., Fagan, A. M., Holtzman, D. M., and Goate, A. M.
(2008). Alzheimer’s disease risk variants show association with cerebrospinal fluid amyloid beta.
Neurogenetics.
Kehoe, P., Wavrant-De Vrieze, F., Crook, R., Wu, W. S., Holmans, P., Fenton, I., Spurlock, G.,
Norton, N., Williams, H., Williams, N., Lovestone, S., Perez-Tur, J., et al. (1999). A full genome
scan for late onset Alzheimer’s disease. Hum. Mol. Genet. 8, 237–245.
Kennedy, J. L., Farrer, L. A., Andreasen, N. C., Mayeux, R., and St. George-Hyslop, P. (2003). The
genetics of adult-onset neuropsychiatric disease: Complexities and conundra? Science 302,
822–826.
Laird, N. M., and Lange, C. (2006). Family-based designs in the age of large-scale gene-association
studies. Nat. Rev. Genet. 7, 385–394.
Levy-Lahad, E., Wasco, W., Poorkaj, P., Romano, D. M., Oshima, J., Pettingell, W. H., Yu, C. E.,
Jondro, P. D., Schmidt, S. D., Wang, K., et al. (1995). Candidate gene for the chromosome 1
familial Alzheimer’s disease locus. Science 269, 973–977.
Li, H., Wetten, S., Li, L., St. Jean, P. L., Upmanyu, R., Surh, L., Hosford, D., Barnes, M. R.,
Briley, J. D., Borrie, M., Coletta, N., Delisle, R., et al. (2008). Candidate single-nucleotide
polymorphisms from a genomewide association study of Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol 65,
45–53.
Li, Y. J., Scott, W. K., Hedges, D. J., Zhang, F., Gaskell, P. C., Nance, M. A., Watts, R. L.,
Hubble, J. P., Koller, W. C., Pahwa, R., Stern, M. B., Hiner, B. C., et al. (2002). Age at onset in
two common neurodegenerative diseases is genetically controlled. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 70, 985–993.
Liu, Y., and Rohrschneider, L. R. (2002). The gift of Gab. FEBS Lett. 515, 1–7.
Loeffler, D. A., Connor, J. R., Juneau, P. L., Snyder, B. S., Kanaley, L., DeMaggio, A. J., Nguyen, H.,
Brickman, C. M., and LeWitt, P. A. (1995). Transferrin and iron in normal, Alzheimer’s disease,
and Parkinson’s disease brain regions. J. Neurochem. 65, 710–724.
Lohmueller, K. E., Pearce, C. L., Pike, M., Lander, E. S., and Hirschhorn, J. N. (2003). Meta-analysis
of genetic association studies supports a contribution of common variants to susceptibility to
common disease. Nat. Genet. 33, 177–182.
CURRENT STATUS OF ALZHEIMER’S GENETICS 183
McCarthy, M. I., Abecasis, G. R., Cardon, L. R., Goldstein, D. B., Little, J., Ioannidis, J. P., and
Hirschhorn, J. N. (2008). Genome-wide association studies for complex traits: Consensus, uncer-
tainty and challenges. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9, 356–369.
Meyer, J. M., and Breitner, J. C. (1998). Multiple threshold model for the onset of Alzheimer’s disease
in teh NAS-NRC twin panel. Am. J. Med. Genet. 81, 92–97.
Miners, J. S., Ashby, E., Van Helmond, Z., Chalmers, K. A., Palmer, L. E., Love, S., and Kehoe, P. G.
(2008). Angiotensin-converting enzyme (ACE) levels and activity in Alzheimer’s disease, and
relationship of perivascular ACE-1 to cerebral amyloid angiopathy. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol.
34, 181–193.
Myers, A., Holmans, P., Marshall, H., Kwon, J., Meyer, D., Ramic, D., Shears, S., Booth, J.,
DeVrieze, F. W., Crook, R., Hamshere, M., Abraham, R., et al. (2000). Susceptibility locus for
Alzheimer’s disease on chromosome 10. Science 290, 2304–2305.
Nizzari, M., Venezia, V., Repetto, E., Caorsi, V., Magrassi, R., Gagliani, M. C., Carlo, P., Florio, T.,
Schettini, G., Tacchetti, C., Russo, T., Diaspro, A., et al. (2007). Amyloid precursor protein and
Presenilin1 interact with the adaptor GRB2 and modulate ERK 1,2 signaling. J. Biol. Chem. 282,
13833–13844.
Oddo, S., and LaFerla, F. M. (2006). The role of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in Alzheimer’s
disease. J. Physiol. Paris 99, 172–179.
Pericak-Vance, M. A., Grubber, J., Bailey, L. R., Hedges, D., West, S., Santoro, L., Kemmerer, B.,
Hall, J. L., Saunders, A. M., Roses, A. D., Small, G. W., Scott, W. K., et al. (2000). Identification of
novel genes in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Exp. Gerontol 35, 1343–1352.
Reiman, E. M., Webster, J. A., Myers, A. J., Hardy, J., Dunckley, T., Zismann, V. L., Joshipura, K. D.,
Pearson, J. V., Hu-Lince, D., Huentelman, M. J., Craig, D. W., Coon, K. D., et al. (2007). GAB2
alleles modify Alzheimer’s risk in APOE epsilon4 carriers. Neuron 54, 713–720.
Reynolds, C. H., Garwood, C. J., Wray, S., Price, C., Kellie, S., Perera, T., Zvelebil, M., Yang, A.,
Sheppard, P. W., Varndell, I. M., Hanger, D. P., and Anderton, B. H. (2008). Phosphorylation
regulates tau interactions with SH3 domains of phosphatidylinositol-3-kinase, phospholipase
cgamma 1, GRB2 and SRC-family kinases. J. Biol. Chem. 283, 18177–18186.
Rogaev, E. I., Sherrington, R., Rogaeva, E. A., Levesque, G., Ikeda, M., Liang, Y., Chi, H., Lin, C.,
Holman, K., Tsuda, T., et al. (1995). Familial Alzheimer’s disease in kindreds with missense
mutations in a gene on chromosome 1 related to the Alzheimer’s disease type 3 gene. Nature
376, 775–778.
Sarmay, G., Angyal, A., Kertesz, A., Maus, M., and Medgyesi, D. (2006). The multiple function of
Grb2 associated binder (Gab) adaptor/scaffolding protein in immune cell signaling. Immunol. Lett.
104, 76–82.
Saunders, A. M., Strittmatter, W. J., Schmechel, D., George-Hyslop, P. H., Pericak-Vance, M. A.,
Joo, S. H., Rosi, B. L., Gusella, J. F., Crapper-MacLachlan, D. R., Alberts, M. J., et al. (1993).
Association of apolipoprotein E allele epsilon 4 with late-onset familial and sporadic Alzheimer’s
disease. Neurology 43, 1467–1472.
Schjeide, B. M., McQueen, M. B., Mullin, K., Divito, J., Hogan, M. F., Parkinson, M., Hooli, B.,
Lange, C., Blacker, D., Tanzi, R. E., and Bertram, L. (2008). Assessment of Alzheimer’s disease
case-control associations using family-based methods. Neurogenetics.
Schjeide, B.-M. M., Hooli, B., Parkinson, M., Hogan, M. F., Divito, J., Mullin, K., Blacker, D., Tanzi, R.,
and Bertram, L. (in press). GAB2 as an Alzheimer’s disease susceptibility gene: Follow-up of genome-
wide association results. Arch. Neurol.
Sherrington, R., Rogaev, E. I., Liang, Y., Rogaeva, E. A., Levesque, G., Ikeda, M., Chi, H., Lin, C.,
Li, G., Holman, K., et al. (1995). Cloning of a gene bearing missense mutations in early-onset
familial Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 375, 754–760.
Smith, M. A., Harris, P. L., Sayre, L. M., and Perry, G. (1997). Iron accumulation in Alzheimer disease
is a source of redox-generated free radicals. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 94, 9866–9868.
184 LARS BERTRAM
Strittmatter, W. J., Saunders, A. M., Schmechel, D., Pericak-Vance, M., Enghild, J., Salvesen, G. S.,
and Roses, A. D. (1993). Apolipoprotein E: High-avidity binding to beta-amyloid and increased
frequency of type 4 allele in late-onset familial Alzheimer disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 90,
1977–1981.
Takeda, S., Sato, N., Ogihara, T., and Morishita, R. (2008). The renin-angiotensin system, hyperten-
sion and cognitive dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: New therapeutic potential. Front. Biosci. 13,
2253–2265.
Tanzi, R. E. (1999). A genetic dichotomy model for the inheritance of Alzheimer’s disease and
common age-related disorders. J. Clin. Invest. 104, 1175–1179.
Tanzi, R. E., and Bertram, L. (2005). Twenty years of the Alzheimer’s disease amyloid hypothesis:
A genetic perspective. Cell 120, 545–555.
Tohgi, H., Utsugisawa, K., Yoshimura, M., Nagane, Y., and Mihara, M. (1998). Age-related changes
in nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subunits alpha4 and beta2 messenger RNA expression in
postmortem human frontal cortex and hippocampus. Neurosci. Lett. 245, 139–142.
Wacholder, S., Chanock, S., Garcia-Closas, M., El Ghormli, L., and Rothman, N. (2004). Assessing
the probability that a positive report is false: An approach for molecular epidemiology studies.
J. Natl. Cancer Inst. 96, 434–442.
Yamamoto, A., Shin, R. W., Hasegawa, K., Naiki, H., Sato, H., Yoshimasu, F., and Kitamoto, T.
(2002). Iron (III) induces aggregation of hyperphosphorylated tau and its reduction to iron (II)
reverses the aggregation: Implications in the formation of neurofibrillary tangles of Alzheimer’s
disease. J. Neurochem. 82, 1137–1147.
FRONTOTEMPORAL LOBAR DEGENERATION: INSIGHTS FROM
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND NEUROIMAGING
I. Introduction
A. Diagnosis of FTLD
B. Clinical Criteria
C. Histopathology
D. Genetics
II. Classification of FTLD Subtypes and Their Features
A. Frontotemporal Degeneration
B. Primary Progressive Aphasia or Progressive Nonfluent Aphasia
C. Semantic Dementia
III. Neuropsychological Assessment
A. Distinguishing FTLD from AD
B. Distinguishing FTLD Subtypes
IV. Neurobehavioral Assessment
A. FTD
B. PPA
C. SD
V. Neuroimaging of FTLD
A. Differentiation of FTLD from AD
B. Differentiation of Different Subtypes of FTLD
VI. Summary
References
are reviewed, highlighting those findings that are likely to be most valuable to
physicians. Finally, we analyze some of the major findings from the large body of
work on neuroimaging of FTLD, again focusing on those studies that potentially
help discriminate FTLD subtypes or assist with the discrimination of FTLD
from AD.
I. Introduction
A. DIAGNOSIS OF FTLD
Clinically and pathologically, FTLD is distinct from AD, although the distinc-
tion is sometimes clinically challenging (Klatka et al., 1996; Mendez et al., 1993).
A proportion of patients who meet the criteria for AD have FTLD confirmed at
autopsy, occasionally co-occurring with AD pathology. A recent pathologic study
examining the overlap of FTLD and AD clinical phenotypes described coexisting
histopathological AD in almost one fourth of FTLD cases (Liscic et al., 2007).
Autopsy-proved FTLD cases, alone or in combination with AD, can meet the
National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke, and
the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association (NINDS-ADRDA)
clinical criteria for AD during life (McKhann et al., 1984; Snowden et al., 2001),
suggesting a lack of specificity in the criteria for both diseases. Despite these
concerns (or perhaps because of them), several groups have put forward criteria
for the clinical diagnosis of FTLD. The sophistication of these classifications and
rating scales for FTLD has evolved since the 1980s.
B. CLINICAL CRITERIA
C. HISTOPATHOLOGY
FIG. 1. Classification of Pick’s disease based on histopathologic features. It is important to note that
all three major clinical phenotypes have been documented with each of the pathologic subtypes.
now form the largest single pathological variety of Pick’s complex (Hodges et al.,
2004; Kertesz et al., 2005; Munoz et al., 2003) (Fig. 1).
D. GENETICS
2004; Jackson et al., 1996). The phenotypic and pathologic variations of these
mutations closely match sporadic disease and provide strong evidence for the
cohesion of Pick’s complex.
Like histopathology, genetic testing is also not specific for the subtypes of
FTLD, with significant syndromic overlap within single mutation types. Tau
protein mutations were first reported for the phenotypes of the FTDP-17 com-
plex, which varied widely (some patients with parkinsonism, some with prominent
aphasia, some with obvious amyotrophy. Several tau mutations have been dis-
covered so far (Clark et al., 1998; Hutton et al., 1998; Spillantini et al., 1998).
DiVerent tau mutations diVerently alter the biochemical properties of tau iso-
forms, but so far these mutations do not predict the clinical presentation. In
addition, the tau protein is not abnormal in all forms of FTLD. The amount of
hyperphosphorylated tau in FTLD can be low and sometimes even absent.
A. FRONTOTEMPORAL DEGENERATION
FTD is the most common diagnostic subgroup of FTLD; over half of FTLD
patients fall in this category (Hodges et al., 2003). FTD also has the earliest age at
onset, often between 55 and 65, and almost never after age 75 ( Johnson et al.,
2005; Rosso et al., 2003). FTD is characterized by personality and behavioral
changes. The initial manifestations can be subtle and diYcult to diagnose begin-
ning with apathy and disinterest which is often misdiagnosed as depression
(Kertesz, 2003). These patients later develop lack of insight and disinhibition,
impulsive and inappropriate behavior, poor financial judgment, changes in libido
and inappropriate sexual behavior, blunting of appropriate behavioral response
and at the extreme, self-destructive behavior (McKhann et al., 2001). Their
dietary habits may change, resulting in overeating or eating of only certain
types of food. The overall appearance and personal hygiene of these patients is
frequently aVected as they show little personal concern for their actions. These
patients can have problems with their memory, and formal memory testing can be
abnormal, but they can track day-to-day events and do not have a typical
amnestic syndrome (McKhann et al., 2001). Frontal lobe functions are impaired
in these patients but some can continue to perform well on formal measures of
frontal tasks early in the disease course. Orientation and episodic memory is also
relatively preserved; problems of forgetfulness are mainly due to inattention and
poor organization of incoming information (Kertesz, 2003). Language is not
aVected early in the course of this disease; however, most patients eventually
develop decreased verbal output and often progress to mutism in later stages
(Kertesz, 2003).
FTLD: INSIGHTS FROM NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND NEUROIMAGING 191
The term PPA was first used by Mesulam (1982) to describe a group of six
patients who developed a slowly progressive aphasic disorder that ‘‘began in the
presenium’’ (before age 65). These patients present with language diYculty
characterized by a combination of word finding diYculties, abnormal speech
patterns, decreased comprehension, and impaired spelling. In the early part of the
disease these patients are able to perform their daily activities and have normal
memory and visuospatial function. Mesulam’s original description stressed the
lack of a generalized dementia, and his criteria eventually included a stipulation
that the patient’s language diYculties should remain the only feature for at
least 2 years before more generalized deficits develop (Weintraub et al., 1990).
Language deficits ultimately progress to mutism, however, these patients instead
of developing a severe dementia in the advanced stages of their disease may have
relatively preserved memory and can function well in the community, further
helping to distinguish them from patients with AD (Kertesz, 2003). While the
typical PPA patient does not have florid psychiatric features the way many FTD
patients do, some can develop the behavioral problems seen in patients with other
forms of FTLD in the later part of the disease and the distinction may become
diYcult in advanced cases, when both mutism and inappropriate behavior are seen.
C. SEMANTIC DEMENTIA
Snowden et al. described a distinct, fluent form of PPA that is diVerent from
the more common nonfluent type and called this semantic dementia (Snowden
et al., 1989). In this condition patients slowly and progressively lose the meaning of
words; however, fluency remains intact and they are able to carry on a (somewhat
empty) conversation. They have diYculty naming and comprehending even
single nouns (Hodges et al., 1992; Kertesz, 2003). These patients diVer from the
fluent aphasia of AD as they have relatively preserved episodic and autobiograph-
ical memory with a rather selective loss of semantic memory. These patients retain
information that has immediate relevance to their environment or to their person;
however, they lose the meanings of other common things. Some of these patients
have visual agnosia (Kertesz, 2003). Patients with SD ultimately become non-
fluent and even mute. They also develop behavioral problems in the later part of
their disease.
We note that some authors categorize PPA and SD together as the language
and aphasic variant of FTLD. In this nosologic schema, FTD represents the
frontal variant of FTLD and PPA/SD together represent the temporal/parietal
variant (Kertesz, 2003).
192 BOZOKI AND FAROOQ
2. FTD
Attempts to distinguish FTD from AD based only on neuropsychological
testing have a long and controversial history. Numerous papers on this topic,
spanning several decades, have failed to agree on whether it is possible to do this
reliably. One group (Gregory et al., 1997) failed to find diVerences between patient
groups in memory, attention, language, and executive abilities, while another
(Pasquier et al., 1995) did not find any diVerence in letter and category verbal
fluency (a frequently used bedside test for evaluating frontal lobe function).
FTLD: INSIGHTS FROM NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND NEUROIMAGING 193
0.8
0 Processing speed/mental
flexibility
FTD SD PPA AD
−0.2
−0.4
−0.6
−0.8
FIG. 2. Factor analysis of a multitest neuropsychological battery demonstrated five major cognitive
factors. Between- and within-group analyses of the factors demonstrate that each patient group is
associated with a specific profile of neuropsychological impairment, in which each group is significantly
worse than the others on at least one factor (data from Libon et al., 2007).
Conversely, several more recent studies have found the opposite; that a double
dissociation between AD and FTD exists and can be identified with careful
neuropsychological assessment. Perri and colleagues (2005) performed a stepwise
discriminant analysis on a number of neuropsychometric variables and deter-
mined that subjects with AD were significantly worse on the Rey figure copy
(a measure of visuospatial praxis) and significantly better on letter fluency than the
FTD group, such that a combination of these two tests plus a measure of apathy
(see Section IV) was suYcient to distinguish the two diseases with approximately
83% sensitivity and 82% specificity.
Also in 2005, a group from the UK (Thompson et al., 2005), observing that
patients with FTD tend to make diVerent types of mistakes from those with AD,
developed a way to assess qualitative performance and error types on their
neuropsychometric battery. They found that ‘‘FTD patients displayed features
associated with frontal lobe dysfunction, such as concrete thought, perseveration,
confabulation, and poor organization, which disrupted performance across the
range of neuropsychological tests.’’ They advise that keeping track of the types and
not just the number of errors made in performing neuropsychological tests can
greatly enhance discrimination of these patient populations.
194 BOZOKI AND FAROOQ
Nonetheless, while this chapter finds that FTD and AD can be discriminated
neuropsychologically, they agree with the findings of the first two studies cited,
that actual scores did not diVer significantly between groups, especially on the
classic tests of executive functioning. They attribute this to the idea that AD
patients are failing at executive tasks ‘‘for non-frontal reasons.’’ That is, executive
tasks are complex and actually make demands on multiple cognitive domains
(particularly memory and visuospatial abilities) that are weak in AD.
This is similar to the findings from the most recent group to weigh in on the
matter. This year, a relatively large study (40 FTD, 77 AD, and 91 normal
controls) (Giovagnoli et al., 2008) was unable to find a significant diVerence
between the patient groups with regard to executive functions. In this study, the
AD patients were significantly more impaired than the FTD group in episodic
memory, selective attention, visual perception, visuomotor coordination, and
constructive praxis, whereas no diVerences were found in executive, intellective,
and linguistic abilities between the two patient groups. Logistic regression ana-
lyses revealed that episodic memory significantly predicted the diagnosis of AD
while no executive deficit was able to predict the diagnosis of FTD. The authors
hypothesize that this may be due to a more widespread distribution of executive
functions in the brain than is currently acknowledged, with other brain regions
providing an extended network of functions that are interrelated with prefrontal
cortex.
Another reason for the failure of some groups to find diVerences between
FTD and AD may be that not all studies distinguished between FTD and SD in
their samples; that is, they included both types of patients in their testing. Because
FTD and SD overlap with AD deficits in very diVerent ways, the net eVect may
have been to ‘‘cancel out’’ the diVerences between groups. This is the hypothesis
of a study which in fact did find a double dissociation between AD, FTD and SD
groups in their study (Perry and Hodges, 2000).
3. PPA
In contrast to the relatively diYcult AD-FTD distinction neuropsychologi-
cally, distinguishing between AD and PPA is more straightforward. PPA patients
maintain relatively normal episodic memory function—the hallmark decline of
early AD, while developing progressively worsening language impairments. The
original report on this clinical syndrome was published in 1982 by Mesulam, who
reported on six patients with progressively worsening language impairment in the
absence of dementia (Mesulam, 1982). Some years later, his lab published a
follow-up study (Weintraub et al., 1990) further characterizing three patients
with sequential neuropsychological assessments. They demonstrated progressive-
ly worsening decline on measures such as the Token Test, sentence repetition and
the Boston Naming test. Importantly, cognition was stable over time on measures
such as spatial and temporal orientation, design recall, line orientation, and face
FTLD: INSIGHTS FROM NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND NEUROIMAGING 195
4. SD
In 1975, Elizabeth Warrington described three patients with a combination of
visual associative agnosia (diYculty recognizing objects based on their visual
features), anomia, and impaired comprehension of word meaning (Warrington,
1975). The diagnostic term ‘‘Semantic Dementia’’ was coined by Snowden and
colleagues, in their description of a diVerent three patients in whom they found
not only a progressive fluent aphasia (in contradistinction to the slowed, agram-
matic and eVortful speech characteristic of PPA), but also deficits in word com-
prehension and in knowledge of objects and people (Snowden et al., 1989). That is,
spontaneous speech is characterized by anomia in the context of relatively normal
phonology and grammar. As the disease progresses, patients will more and more
replace specific terms with more nondescript words such as ‘‘thing’’ or ‘‘that.’’
Neuropsychologically, the hallmark of SD is an associative agnosia and/or
prosopagnosia (inability to recognize faces), coupled with a ‘‘loss of memory for
words’’ which is quite diVerent from the impairment of episodic memory seen in
early AD. In addition, orientation to time and place, simple calculation and
drawing skills are all preserved in SD. In the study by Perry et al. cited above,
subjects with SD showed severe deficits in semantic memory (Pyramid Palm trees
and the Graded Naming test) with preservation of attention and executive
function, while the AD subjects had very little trouble on those same semantic
memory tasks but were grossly impaired in episodic memory, as measured by the
logical memory portion of the Wechsler Memory Scale-R and the Rey figure
recall.
A final point of distinction between SD and AD regards the temporal gradient
of their amnesia. In general, SD patients do not have a significant impairment of
episodic or recent memory, but several authors have found that autobiographical
memory for remote events can be significantly impaired in SD (Hou et al., 2005;
Nestor et al., 2002). This is distinct from the relatively intact remote memories of
most early AD patients, and speaks to the presumed extrahippocampal localiza-
tion of the damage in SD. Overall, the consensus opinion seems to be that, like
PPA, the neuropsychological profile of SD is suYciently diVerent from that of AD
to make discrimination relatively straightforward on this basis alone.
196 BOZOKI AND FAROOQ
1. FTD vs PPA
In contrast to the relatively small distinction between executive processing
deficits in AD vs FTD, those between FTD and PPA are considerable, as patients
with PPA have essentially normal executive processing ability. PPA also seems to
have more intact episodic memory function than FTD (Libon et al., 2007), which
is somewhat surprising because early FTD is not generally acknowledged to have
significant declines in episodic memory function. Quite recently, Marra et al.
published a study comparing both neuropsychological and neurobehavioral char-
acteristics of 22 FTD and 10 PPA patients (Marra et al., 2007). The latter results
are given in Section IV. Their findings on the neuropsychological profiles corrob-
orate the Libon et al. group’s documentation of better (normal-range) scores on
immediate and delayed recall measures and executive processing. In turn, their
group of FTD patients did significantly better on letter fluency. Surprisingly, the
two groups were about nearly equal on a test of category fluency (both groups
performed poorly).
2. FTD vs SD
In addition to being dramatically worse on tasks that hinge on semantic
knowledge, SD patients are also clearly worse than FTD patients on naming
and other language-based tasks. An early study comparing FTD and SD (Hodges
et al., 1999) found that patients with SD were significantly worse on a category-
fluency task, while being quite closely matched on the letter-fluency version of
that task. The study by Libon et al. (2007) replicated this finding. In a related
study, subjects with SD showed severe deficits in semantic memory with preser-
vation of attention and executive function. Subjects with FTD showed the reverse
pattern. The double dissociation in performance on semantic memory and
attention/executive function clearly separated the temporal and frontal variants
of FTD (Perry and Hodges, 2000).
3. SD vs PPA
The overarching diVerence between these two dementias neuorpsycho-
logically is the diVerence between a pure speech/language impairment (PPA)
and a relatively pan-modal agnosia (SD) which includes (but is not limited to)
FTLD: INSIGHTS FROM NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND NEUROIMAGING 197
A. FTD
By far the most challenging of the FTLD subtypes to care for is the individual
with FTD, also known as behavioral-variant FTD. This is due not to the cognitive
impairment that develops, but to the challenging set of aberrant behaviors.
Neuropsychiatric symptoms are usually the earliest manifestation of this subtype,
and such patients are often referred to psychiatrists with working diagnoses of
depression, bipolar disorder and even late-onset schizophrenia. Because the
cognitive impairments early on are rather subtle, careful neuropsychological
testing is necessary (as described in detail earlier ) to determine that such indivi-
duals actually have a dementia rather than a purely psychiatric condition. Up
until about 10 years ago, when these patients finally did get to the neuropsychol-
ogist, there was a paucity of instruments to delineate and quantify the often florid
behavioral problems. Two eVorts to better characterize these behavioral changes
resulted in a measurement instrument called the frontal behavioral inventory
(FBI), a 24-item questionnaire aimed at assessing the most commonly seen
psychopathologies present in FTD (Kertesz et al., 1997), and a shortened form
of the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (Cummings et al., 1994), called the NPI-Q
(Kaufer et al., 2000), which contains 12 items that must be assessed as to presence/
absence and if present, the severity (mild, moderate, or severe).
The authors compared three groups of patients, those with FTD, those with
AD and those with ‘‘depressive dementia’’ (aka pseudo- or psychodementia).
They found that the FTD group scored much higher on the FBI then either
198 BOZOKI AND FAROOQ
Ea p b .
∧
+
Irr n ~
Ap h..
ee ..
An n
s
Ag ns
ep ion
eh
y
y
Sl beh
lu ion
y
io
ilit
et
/O
p
th
tio
tio
ss
t
xi
eu
Ab itab
ita
a
s
D
na
bi
re
u
or
n/
el
g
hi
ci
ot
io
tin
D
in
m
at
D
is
al
El
D
H
FIG. 3. Frequency of behaviors and mean frequency by severity product score for all individual
Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) behaviors across groups. Significant diVerences (p < 0.05) between
groups are indicated by the special characters (data from Rosen et al., 2006).
B. PPA
the end of the third year the PPA group met criteria for FTD. The more recently
published study by Banks and Weintraub uses the NPI-Q. This study, which
compared individuals with FTD and those with PPA and divided the subjects into
short-duration (<5 years) and long-duration (!5 years) symptoms, found that the
number of symptoms in long-duration PPA patients did not significantly diVer
from patients with FTD regardless of their disease duration. However, when
comparing the short- and long-duration PPA patients, a trend toward decreased
depression and increased disinhibition and nocturnal behavior was noted.
C. SD
V. Neuroimaging of FTLD
(GraV-Radford and WoodruV, 2007). The utility of brain imaging lay in determining
whether additional neuropathology was seen (e.g., ischemia, subdural hematoma).
The advent of sensitive, automated methods for quantifying regional atrophy has
provided a new and better way to evaluate this distinguishing feature of FTLD. In
addition, functional studies which are used to measure neurochemical breakdown
products, regional cerebral blood flow or cerebral metabolism can help to diagnose
FTLD at the earlier stages, and can distinguish FTLD from AD. Functional imaging
with SPECT or PET can achieve a >90% sensitivity of detecting FTLD (Mendez
et al., 2007). It is also becoming possible to distinguish the diVerent types of FTLD
with these more advanced imaging techniques. Nonetheless, there is still a need for
larger randomized studies to determine the sensitivity and specificity of functional
imaging modalities in the diagnosis of FTLD, its phenotypes and their diVerential
from other types of dementia especially AD.
2. MR Spectroscopy
Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) allows in vivo noninvasive
estimation of brain metabolites. MRS analysis reveals peaks which correspond to
diVerent compounds and metabolites in brain tissue. Two of these compounds are
FTLD: INSIGHTS FROM NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND NEUROIMAGING 201
FIG. 4. Distribution of cerebral atrophy in a patient with AD (A) and frontotemporal dementia
(FTD) (B). Both patients had similar interscan intervals (11 months) and annualized rates of global
cerebral atrophy (approximately 4%). Areas of volume loss are highlighted in red. Note the generalized
distribution of atrophy in AD and the preferential anterior volume loss in FTD (Reprinted from
Neurology 57, Chan, D., Fox, N. C., Jenkins, R., Scahill, R. I., Crum, W. R., and Rossor, M. N.
(2001a) with permission from Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins).
relevant in the work up of patients with dementia, N-acetyl aspartate (NAA) which
is predominantly intracellular, and myoinositol (MI) which is predominantly
found in glial cells. NAA is a marker of neuronal activity and MI is a marker of
gliosis. MRS can help to diVerentiate FTLD from AD as these disorders are
accompanied by relatively distinct neurochemical abnormalities. Ernst et al. found
that patients with FTLD had a statistically significant increase in myoinositol (MI)
and decrease in N-acetyl (NA) and glutamate in the frontal region, which was
distinct from AD. The NA concentration in a frontal region of interest allowed the
best discrimination of FTLD from AD and controls. In the temporoparietal
regions, changes in MI concentration occurred prior to observable changes in
NA concentration, indicating that glial proliferation may be more readily
detected than neuronal loss in the early stages of FTLD (Ernst et al., 1997).
course of disease, making PET a relatively sensitive imaging tool for discerning
both the presence and the type of neurodegeneration. In fact, the Centers of
Medicaid and Medicare recently approved reimbursement of clinical 18F-FDG-
PET for the indication of distinguishing AD from FTLD. FTLD patients have
prominent hypometabolism of the frontal and anterior temporal cortices as
compared with AD patients who show decreased metabolism in the posterior
cingulate and parietotemporal cortices (Diehl-Schmid et al., 2007; Ishii et al., 1998;
Jeong et al., 2005a; Mosconi et al., 2008). PET statistical parametric mapping
studies in patients with FTLD have shown hypometabolism in other areas of
brain such as the anterior cingulate, uncus, insula, and subcortical regions
including basal ganglia ( Jeong et al., 2005a). Moreover, hemispheric metabolic
asymmetry is common in patients with FTLD (Garraux et al., 1999; Jeong et al.,
2005a) (Fig. 5).
The novel PET tracer 11C-PIB has a high aYnity for fibrillar A-beta protein
which is a pathological hallmark of AD and is not a part of the FTLD spectrum
(Klunk et al., 2003). Because of this, 11C-PIB can help to discriminate AD from
FTLD. Engler et al. conducted a study of 10 patients with FTLD using PIB. Eight
of these FTLD patients showed significantly lower PIB retention as compared to
MCI
AD
FTD
DLB
FIG. 5. Representative cortical 18F-FDG PET patterns in NL, AD, DLB, and FTD. 3D-SSP maps
and corresponding Z scores showing CMRglc reductions in clinical groups as compared with the NL
database are displayed on a color-coded scale ranging from 0 (black) to 10 (red). From left to right: 3D-
SSP maps are shown on the right and left lateral, superior and inferior, anterior and posterior, and right
and left middle views of a standardized brain image (Reprinted by permission of the Society of Nuclear
Medicine from Mosconi, L., Tsui, W. H., Herholz, K., Pupi, A., Drzezga, A., Lucignani, G., Reiman, E. M.,
HolthoV, V., Kalbe, E., Sorbi, S., Diehl-Schmid, J., Perneczky, R., et al. (2008)).
FTLD: INSIGHTS FROM NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND NEUROIMAGING 203
AD in diVerent brain regions. In fact PIB uptake in these FTLD patients did not
diVer significantly from healthy controls in any region (Engler et al., 2008).
Another novel PET tracer, which is relatively a small molecule, is called
FDDNP. It is an in vivo chemical marker for cerebral amyloid and tau proteins.
Initial FDDNP studies of FTD show binding in the frontal and temporal regions
but not in the parietal regions suggesting that FDDNP labels regional tau tangles
and thus can diVerentiate FTD from AD according to the binding patterns
(Silverman et al., 2002). However, these are preliminary studies and further
pathological correlation and larger scale studies are warranted to confirm the
utility of this promising modality in clinical practice.
atrophy of the anterior temporal lobes. As the disease progresses this degenerative
process evolves caudally to the posterior temporal lobes or rostrally to the
posterior, inferior frontal lobes, or both. Utilizing volumetric MRI methods,
Chao et al. demonstrated that patients with SD had both white matter and gray
matter atrophy in the temporal lobes, and that therefore adding temporal white
matter volume to temporal gray matter volume significantly improved the
discrimination between SD and FTD (Chao et al., 2007).
Gorno-Tempini et al. in their voxel-based morphometry MRI study showed
that all of their patients with PPA had atrophy of the left perisylvian region,
anterior temporal lobes bilaterally and the basal ganglia bilaterally (Gorno-
Tempini et al., 2004). Their study also showed distinctive patterns for PPA,
SD, and logopenic progressive aphasia. PPA was associated with left inferior
frontal and insular atrophy, SD with anterior temporal damage and logopenic
progressive aphasia with atrophy of left posterior temporal cortex and inferior
parietal lobule (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004). Mummery et al. conducted a voxel-
based morphometry study of SD patients and identified well-circumscribed
regions of atrophy in individual patients including the bilateral temporal poles
(left more than right), the left inferior temporal gyrus, the left middle temporal
gyrus, the left amygdaloid complex, and the ventromedial frontal cortex
(Mummery et al., 2000). The degree of semantic deterioration correlated with
the extent of left anterior temporal damage and not with that of adjacent
ventromedial frontal cortex.
An excellent recent summary of the ‘‘triple dissociation’’ between the three
subtypes of FTLD can be seen in the recent study by Schroeter, et al. (2007). This
group undertook a large quantitative meta-analysis of 267 FTLD patients (and
351 control subjects) across 19 MRI and PET studies. Their results indicated
specific neural networks for each of the three clinically defined subtypes that did
not overlap. The study further related each subtype’s clinical features to its neural
substrate, demonstrating a close structure-function correlation for the diseases
(Schroeter et al., 2007) (Fig. 6).
y = 12 x = −5 z = −7
0.016
0.010
FTLD
y = 37 x = −1 z = −7
0.014
FTD 0.008
y = 11 x = −29 z = −9
0.015
SD 0.008
y = 11 x = −49 z = −5
0.008
PNFA 0.006
FIG. 6. Results of the quantitative meta-analyses (using activation likelihood estimates) for the three
subtypes of FTLD individually, and for FTLD all subtypes pooled. Left side is left (Reprinted from
NeuroImage 36(3), Schroeter, M. L., Raczka, K., Neumann, J., and Yves von Cramon, D. (2007) with
permission from Elsevier).
FTLD (Borroni et al., 2007). Another study using tensor-based morphometry has
reported white matter atrophy in temporal lobes of patients with SD (Studholme
et al., 2004).
206 BOZOKI AND FAROOQ
3. MR Spectroscopy
NAA and creatine (Cr) on MRS have been reported to diVerentiate not only
AD from FTLD, but the diVerent types of FTLD as well. Coulthard et al.
performed a study on FTLD patients showing that MRS can reveal regionally
selective abnormalities in patients with FTLD. MRS was performed on the
temporal, parietal, and anterior cingulate cortices of five patients with established
SD, and two patients with FTD. All the patients with FTLD had reduced NAA/
Cr in frontal and temporal lobes and not in parietal lobes; however, the patients
with FTD had increased MI/Cr in their cingulate cortices diVerentiating then
from SD (Coulthard et al., 2006).
VI. Summary
The accurate diagnosis of FTLD in all of its guises is very important for the
development of new therapeutic options and disease modifying therapies. Clinical
diagnosis is based on the recognition of all the core and necessary neurologic,
neuropsychological, and neuropsychiatric features of FTLD. However, patients
often lack all the core features at the initial assessment, and subtle personality or
behavioral changes can be caused by a range of other disorders, making diagnosis
diYcult (Mendez and Perryman, 2002; Mendez et al., 2007). Furthermore, there is
no single hallmark genetic mutation or susceptibility locus that can be tested to
provide the diagnosis in most cases. Therefore, careful neuropsychological and
neurobehavioral assessment should be performed in all suspected cases to increase
the reliability of discrimination between AD and FTLD, esp. FTD. Several
authors have come to the same conclusion (Marra et al., 2007; Perri et al., 2005).
Furthermore, neuroimaging plays a significant role in diagnosis, not only in
excluding alternative pathologies, but as a positive, ‘‘rule-in’’ test in which the
characteristic features of frontal and anterior-mid temporal atrophy and hypo-
metaboloism should be evaluated. New and improved methods for evaluating
these features are on the horizon, and adoption of a quantitative volumetric
approach to MR as well as the addition of PET or SPECT to the clinical
evaluation algorithm will add greatly to our diagnostic acumen.
References
Adlam, A. L., Patterson, K., Rogers, T. T., Nestor, P. J., Salmond, C. H., Acosta-Cabronero, J., and
Hodges, J. R. (2006). Semantic dementia and fluent primary progressive aphasia: Two sides of the
same coin? Brain 129, 3066–3080.
Banks, S. J., and Weintraub, S. (2008). Neuropsychiatric symptoms in behavioral variant frontotem-
poral dementia and primary progressive aphasia. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 21, 133–141.
208 BOZOKI AND FAROOQ
Barnes, J., Whitwell, J. L., Frost, C., Josephs, K. A., Rossor, M., and Fox, N. C. (2006). Measurements
of the amygdala and hippocampus in pathologically confirmed Alzheimer disease and fronto-
temporal lobar degeneration. Arch. Neurol. 63, 1434–1439.
Bocti, C., Rockel, C., Roy, P., Gao, F., and Black, S. E. (2006). Topographical patterns of lobar atrophy
in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 21, 364–372.
Bonte, F. J., Harris, T. S., Roney, C. A., and Hynan, L. S. (2004). DiVerential diagnosis between
Alzheimer’s and frontotemporal disease by the posterior cingulate sign. J. Nucl. Med. 45, 771–774.
Borroni, B., Brambati, S. M., Agosti, C., Gipponi, S., Bellelli, G., Gasparotti, R., Garibotto, V., Di
Luca, M., Scifo, P., Perani, D., and Padovani, A. (2007). Evidence of white matter changes on
diVusion tensor imaging in frontotemporal dementia. Arch. Neurol. 64, 246–251.
Chan, D., Fox, N. C., Jenkins, R., Scahill, R. I., Crum, W. R., and Rossor, M. N. (2001a). Rates of
global and regional cerebral atrophy in AD and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 57,
1756–1763.
Chan, D., Fox, N. C., Scahill, R. I., Crum, W. R., Whitwell, J. L., Leschziner, G., Rossor, A. M.,
Stevens, J. M., Cipolotti, L., and Rossor, M. N. (2001b). Patterns of temporal lobe atrophy in
semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 49, 433–442.
Chao, L. L., SchuV, N., Clevenger, E. M., Mueller, S. G., Rosen, H. J., Gorno-Tempini, M. L.,
Kramer, J. H., Miller, B. L., and Weiner, M. W. (2007). Patterns of white matter atrophy in
frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Arch. Neurol. 64, 1619–1624.
Charpentier, P., Lavenu, I., Defebvre, L., Duhamel, A., LecouVe, P., Pasquier, F., and Steinling, M.
(2000). Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia are diVerentiated by discriminant
analysis applied to (99m)Tc HmPAO SPECT data. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 69, 661–663.
Chawluk, J. B., Mesulam, M. M., Hurtig, H., Kushner, M., Weintraub, S., Saykin, A., Rubin, N.,
Alavi, A., and Reivich, M. (1986). Slowly progressive aphasia without generalized dementia:
Studies with positron emission tomography. Ann. Neurol. 19, 68–74.
Chow, T. W., Miller, B. L., Hayashi, V. N., and Geschwind, D. H. (1999). Inheritance of frontotem-
poral dementia. Arch. Neurol. 56, 817–822.
Clark, L. N., Poorkaj, P., Wszolek, Z., Geschwind, D. H., Nasreddine, Z. S., Miller, B., Li, D.,
Payami, H., Awert, F., Markopoulou, K., Andreadis, A., D’Souza, I., et al. (1998). Pathogenic
implications of mutations in the tau gene in pallido-ponto-nigral degeneration and related
neurodegenerative disorders linked to chromosome 17. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95,
13103–13107.
Constantinidis, J., Richard, J., and Tissot, R. (1974). Pick’s disease. Histological and clinical correla-
tions. Eur. Neurol. 11, 208–217.
Coulthard, E., Firbank, M., English, P., Welch, J., Birchall, D., O’Brien, J., and GriYths, T. D. (2006).
Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in frontotemporal dementia. J. Neurol. 253, 861–868.
Cummings, J. L., Mega, M., Gray, K., Rosenberg-Thompson, S., Carusi, D. A., and Gornbein, J.
(1994). The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: Comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in
dementia. Neurology 44, 2308–2314.
Diehl, J., Grimmer, T., Drzezga, A., Riemenschneider, M., Forstl, H., and Kurz, A. (2004). Cerebral
metabolic patterns at early stages of frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia. A PET
study. Neurobiol. Aging 25, 1051–1056.
Diehl-Schmid, J., Grimmer, T., Drzezga, A., Bornschein, S., Riemenschneider, M., Forstl, H.,
Schwaiger, M., and Kurz, A. (2007). Decline of cerebral glucose metabolism in frontotemporal
dementia: A longitudinal 18F-FDG-PET-study. Neurobiol. Aging 28, 42–50.
Engler, H., Santillo, A. F., Wang, S. X., Lindau, M., Savitcheva, I., Nordberg, A., Lannfelt, L.,
Langstrom, B., and Kilander, L. (2008). In vivo amyloid imaging with PET in frontotemporal
dementia. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging 35, 100–106.
FTLD: INSIGHTS FROM NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND NEUROIMAGING 209
Ernst, T., Chang, L., Melchor, R., and Mehringer, C. M. (1997). Frontotemporal dementia and early
Alzheimer disease: DiVerentiation with frontal lobe H-1 MR spectroscopy. Radiology 203,
829–836.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). ‘‘Mini-mental state’’. A practical method for
grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198.
Foster, N. L., Wilhelmsen, K., Sima, A. A., Jones, M. Z., D’Amato, C. J., and Gilman, S. (1997).
Frontotemporal dementia and parkinsonism linked to chromosome 17: A consensus conference.
Conference Participants. Ann. Neurol. 41, 706–715.
Foster, N. L., Heidebrink, J. L., Clark, C. M., Jagust, W. J., Arnold, S. E., Barbas, N. R.,
DeCarli, C. S., Turner, R. S., Koeppe, R. A., Higdon, R., and Minoshima, S. (2007). FDG-
PET improves accuracy in distinguishing frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain
130, 2616–2635.
Fukui, T., and Kertesz, A. (2000). Volumetric study of lobar atrophy in Pick complex and Alzheimer’s
disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 174, 111–121.
Galton, C. J., Gomez-Anson, B., Antoun, N., Scheltens, P., Patterson, K., Graves, M., Sahakian, B. J.,
and Hodges, J. R. (2001). Temporal lobe rating scale: Application to Alzheimer’s disease and
frontotemporal dementia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 70, 165–173.
Garraux, G., Salmon, E., Degueldre, C., Lemaire, C., and Franck, G. (1999). Medial temporal lobe
metabolic impairment in dementia associated with motor neuron disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 168,
145–150.
Giovagnoli, A. R., Erbetta, A., Reati, F., and Bugiani, O. (2008). DiVerential neuropsychological
patterns of frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in a study of
diagnostic concordance. Neuropsychologia 46, 1495–1504.
Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Dronkers, N. F., Rankin, K. P., Ogar, J. M., Phengrasamy, L., Rosen, H. J.,
Johnson, J. K., Weiner, M. W., and Miller, B. L. (2004). Cognition and anatomy in three variants
of primary progressive aphasia. Ann. Neurol. 55, 335–346.
GraV-Radford, N. R., and WoodruV, B. K. (2007). Frontotemporal dementia. Semin. Neurol. 27, 48–57.
Gregory, C. A., Orrell, M., Sahakian, B., and Hodges, J. R. (1997). Can frontotemporal dementia and
Alzheimer’s disease be diVerentiated using a brief battery of tests? Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 12,
375–383.
Grossman, M., Mickanin, J., Onishi, K., Hughes, E., D’Esposito, M., Ding, X., Alavi, A., and
Reivich, M. (1996). Progressive nonfluent aphasia: Language, cognitive, and PET measures
contrasted with probable Alzheimer’s disease. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 135–154.
Gustafson, L. (1987). Frontal lobe degeneration of non-Alzheimer type. II. Clinical picture and
diVerential diagnosis. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 6, 209–223.
Gustafson, L., and Nilsson, L. (1982). DiVerential diagnosis of presenile dementia on clinical grounds.
Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 65, 194–209.
Hodges, J. R., and Patterson, K. (2007). Semantic dementia: A unique clinicopathological syndrome.
Lancet Neurol. 6, 1004–1014.
Hodges, J. R., Patterson, K., Oxbury, S., and Funnell, E. (1992). Semantic dementia. Progressive
fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. Brain 115(Pt 6), 1783–1806.
Hodges, J. R., Patterson, K., Ward, R., Garrard, P., Bak, T., Perry, R., and Gregory, C. (1999). The
diVerentiation of semantic dementia and frontal lobe dementia (temporal and frontal variants of
frontotemporal dementia) from early Alzheimer’s disease: A comparative neuropsychological
study. Neuropsychology 13, 31–40.
Hodges, J. R., Davies, R., Xuereb, J., Kril, J., and Halliday, G. (2003). Survival in frontotemporal
dementia. Neurology 61, 349–354.
Hodges, J. R., Davies, R. R., Xuereb, J. H., Casey, B., Broe, M., Bak, T. H., Kril, J. J., and
Halliday, G. M. (2004). Clinicopathological correlates in frontotemporal dementia. Ann. Neurol.
56, 399–406.
210 BOZOKI AND FAROOQ
Hou, C. E., Miller, B. L., and Kramer, J. H. (2005). Patterns of autobiographical memory loss in
dementia. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 20, 809–815.
Hutton, M., Lendon, C. L., Rizzu, P., Baker, M., Froelich, S., Houlden, H., Pickering-Brown, S.,
Chakraverty, S., Isaacs, A., Grover, A., Hackett, J., et al. (1998). Association of missense and 50 -
splice-site mutations in tau with the inherited dementia FTDP-17. Nature 393, 702–705.
Ibach, B., Poljansky, S., Marienhagen, J., Sommer, M., Manner, P., and Hajak, G. (2004). Contrasting
metabolic impairment in frontotemporal degeneration and early onset Alzheimer’s disease.
Neuroimage 23, 739–743.
Ishii, K., Sakamoto, S., Sasaki, M., Kitagaki, H., Yamaji, S., Hashimoto, M., Imamura, T.,
Shimomura, T., Hirono, N., and Mori, E. (1998). Cerebral glucose metabolism in patients with
frontotemporal dementia. J. Nucl. Med. 39, 1875–1878.
Jack, C. R., Jr., Petersen, R. C., Xu, Y. C., Waring, S. C., O’Brien, P. C., Tangalos, E. G., Smith, G. E.,
Ivnik, R. J., and Kokmen, E. (1997). Medial temporal atrophy on MRI in normal aging and very
mild Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 49, 786–794.
Jackson, M., Lennox, G., and Lowe, J. (1996). Motor neurone disease-inclusion dementia. Neurodegen-
eration 5, 339–350.
Jeong, Y., Cho, S. S., Park, J. M., Kang, S. J., Lee, J. S., Kang, E., Na, D. L., and Kim, S. E. (2005a).
18F-FDG PET findings in frontotemporal dementia: An SPM analysis of 29 patients. J. Nucl. Med.
46, 233–239.
Jeong, Y., Park, K. C., Cho, S. S., Kim, E. J., Kang, S. J., Kim, S. E., Kang, E., and Na, D. L. (2005b).
Pattern of glucose hypometabolism in frontotemporal dementia with motor neuron disease.
Neurology 64, 734–736.
Johnson, N., Barion, A., Rademaker, A., Rehkemper, G., and Weintraub, S. (2004). The activities of
daily living questionnaire: A validation study in patients with dementia. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord.
18, 223–230.
Johnson, J. K., Diehl, J., Mendez, M. F., Neuhaus, J., Shapira, J. S., Forman, M., Chute, D. J.,
Roberson, E. D., Pace-Savitsky, C., Neumann, M., Chow, T. W., Rosen, H. J., et al. (2005).
Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: Demographic characteristics of 353 patients. Arch. Neurol. 62,
925–930.
Kaufer, D. I., Cummings, J. L., Ketchel, P., Smith, V., MacMillan, A., Shelley, T., Lopez, O. L., and
DeKosky, S. T. (2000). Validation of the NPI-Q, a brief clinical form of the Neuropsychiatric
Inventory. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 12, 233–239.
Kempler, D., Metter, E. J., Riege, W. H., Jackson, C. A., Benson, D. F., and Hanson, W. R. (1990).
Slowly progressive aphasia: Three cases with language, memory, CT and PET data. J. Neurol.
Neurosurg. Psychiatry 53, 987–993.
Kertesz, A. (2003). Pick Complex: An integrative approach to frontotemporal dementia: Primary
progressive aphasia, corticobasal degeneration, and progressive supranuclear palsy. Neurologist 9,
311–317.
Kertesz, A. (2006). Progress in clinical neurosciences: Frontotemporal dementia-pick’s disease. Can. J.
Neurol. Sci. 33, 141–148.
Kertesz, A., Davidson, W., and Fox, H. (1997). Frontal behavioral inventory: Diagnostic criteria for
frontal lobe dementia. Can. J. Neurol. Sci. 24, 29–36.
Kertesz, A., McMonagle, P., Blair, M., Davidson, W., and Munoz, D. G. (2005). The evolution and
pathology of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 128, 1996–2005.
Klatka, L. A., SchiVer, R. B., Powers, J. M., and Kazee, A. M. (1996). Incorrect diagnosis of
Alzheimer’s disease. A clinicopathologic study. Arch. Neurol. 53, 35–42.
Klunk, W. E., Wang, Y., Huang, G. F., Debnath, M. L., Holt, D. P., Shao, L., Hamilton, R. L.,
Ikonomovic, M. D., DeKosky, S. T., and Mathis, C. A. (2003). The binding of 2-(40 -methylami-
nophenyl)benzothiazole to postmortem brain homogenates is dominated by the amyloid compo-
nent. J. Neurosci. 23, 2086–2092.
FTLD: INSIGHTS FROM NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND NEUROIMAGING 211
Knopman, D. S., Mastri, A. R., Frey, W. H., 2nd, Sung, J. H., and Rustan, T. (1990). Dementia lacking
distinctive histologic features: A common non-Alzheimer degenerative dementia. Neurology 40,
251–256.
Libon, D. J., Xie, S. X., Moore, P., Farmer, J., Antani, S., McCawley, G., Cross, K., and
Grossman, M. (2007). Patterns of neuropsychological impairment in frontotemporal dementia.
Neurology 68, 369–375.
Liscic, R. M., Storandt, M., Cairns, N. J., and Morris, J. C. (2007). Clinical and psychometric
distinction of frontotemporal and Alzheimer dementias. Arch. Neurol. 64, 535–540.
Marczinski, C. A., Davidson, W., and Kertesz, A. (2004). A longitudinal study of behavior in
frontotemporal dementia and primary progressive aphasia. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 17, 185–190.
Marra, C., Quaranta, D., Zinno, M., Misciagna, S., Bizzarro, A., Masullo, C., Daniele, A., and
Gainotti, G. (2007). Clusters of cognitive and behavioral disorders clearly distinguish primary
progressive aphasia from frontal lobe dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn.
Disord. 24, 317–326.
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., and Stadlan, E. M. (1984).
Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under
the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease.
Neurology 34, 939–944.
McKhann, G. M., Albert, M. S., Grossman, M., Miller, B., Dickson, D., and Trojanowski, J. Q.
(2001). Clinical and pathological diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia: Report of the Work
Group on Frontotemporal Dementia and Pick’s Disease. Arch. Neurol. 58, 1803–1809.
Mendez, M. F., and Perryman, K. M. (2002). Neuropsychiatric features of frontotemporal dementia:
Evaluation of consensus criteria and review. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 14, 424–429.
Mendez, M. F., Selwood, A., Mastri, A. R., and Frey, W. H., 2 nd (1993). Pick’s disease versus
Alzheimer’s disease: A comparison of clinical characteristics. Neurology 43, 289–292.
Mendez, M. F., Shapira, J. S., McMurtray, A., Licht, E., and Miller, B. L. (2007). Accuracy of the
clinical evaluation for frontotemporal dementia. Arch. Neurol. 64, 830–835.
Mesulam, M. M. (1982). Slowly progressive aphasia without generalized dementia. Ann. Neurol. 11,
592–598.
Mosconi, L., Tsui, W. H., Herholz, K., Pupi, A., Drzezga, A., Lucignani, G., Reiman, E. M.,
HolthoV, V., Kalbe, E., Sorbi, S., Diehl-Schmid, J., Perneczky, R., et al. (2008). Multicenter
standardized 18F-FDG PET diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease, and
other dementias. J. Nucl. Med. 49, 390–398.
Mummery, C. J., Patterson, K., Price, C. J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., and Hodges, J. R. (2000).
A voxel-based morphometry study of semantic dementia: Relationship between temporal lobe
atrophy and semantic memory. Ann. Neurol. 47, 36–45.
Munoz, D. G., Dickson, D. W., Bergeron, C., Mackenzie, I. R., Delacourte, A., and Zhukareva, V.
(2003). The neuropathology and biochemistry of frontotemporal dementia. Ann. Neurol. 54(Suppl 5),
S24–S28.
Neary, D., Snowden, J. S., Northen, B., and Goulding, P. (1988). Dementia of frontal lobe type.
J. Neurol. Neurosurg Psychiatry 51, 353–361.
Neary, D., Snowden, J. S., Mann, D. M., Northen, B., Goulding, P. J., and Macdermott, N. (1990).
Frontal lobe dementia and motor neuron disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 53, 23–32.
Neary, D., Snowden, J. S., Gustafson, L., Passant, U., Stuss, D., Black, S., Freedman, M., Kertesz, A.,
Robert, P. H., Albert, M., Boone, K., Miller, B. L., et al. (1998). Frontotemporal lobar degenera-
tion: A consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 51, 1546–1554.
Nestor, P. J., Graham, K. S., Bozeat, S., Simons, J. S., and Hodges, J. R. (2002). Memory consolidation
and the hippocampus: Further evidence from studies of autobiographical memory in semantic
dementia and frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia 40, 633–654.
212 BOZOKI AND FAROOQ
Nestor, P. J., Graham, N. L., Fryer, T. D., Williams, G. B., Patterson, K., and Hodges, J. R. (2003).
Progressive non-fluent aphasia is associated with hypometabolism centred on the left anterior
insula. Brain 126, 2406–2418.
Newberg, A. B., Mozley, P. D., Sadek, A. H., Grossman, M., and Alavi, A. (2000). Regional cerebral
distribution of [Tc-99m] hexylmethylpropylene amineoxine in patients with progressive aphasia.
J. Neuroimaging 10, 162–168.
Osher, J. E., Wicklund, A. H., Rademaker, A., Johnson, N., and Weintraub, S. (2007). The mini-
mental state examination in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and primary progressive
aphasia. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Demen. 22, 468–473.
Pasquier, F., Lebert, F., Grymonprez, L., and Petit, H. (1995). Verbal fluency in dementia of frontal
lobe type and dementia of Alzheimer type. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 58, 81–84.
Perri, R., Koch, G., Carlesimo, G. A., Serra, L., Fadda, L., Pasqualetti, P., Pettenati, C., and
Caltagirone, C. (2005). Alzheimer’s disease and frontal variant of frontotemporal dementia—A
very brief battery for cognitive and behavioural distinction. J. Neurol. 252, 1238–1244.
Perry, R. J., and Hodges, J. R. (2000). DiVerentiating frontal and temporal variant frontotemporal
dementia from Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 54, 2277–2284.
Ratnavalli, E., Brayne, C., Dawson, K., and Hodges, J. R. (2002). The prevalence of frontotemporal
dementia. Neurology 58, 1615–1621.
Rosen, H. J., Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Goldman, W. P., Perry, R. J., SchuV, N., Weiner, M., Feiwell, R.,
Kramer, J. H., and Miller, B. L. (2002). Patterns of brain atrophy in frontotemporal dementia and
semantic dementia. Neurology 58, 198–208.
Rosen, H. J., Allison, S. C., Ogar, J. M., Amici, S., Rose, K., Dronkers, N., Miller, B. L., and Gorno-
Tempini, M. L. (2006). Behavioral features in semantic dementia vs other forms of progressive
aphasias. Neurology 67, 1752–1756.
Rosso, S. M., Donker Kaat, L., Baks, T., Joosse, M., de Koning, I., Pijnenburg, Y., de Jong, D.,
Dooijes, D., Kamphorst, W., Ravid, R., Niermeijer, M. F., Verheij, F., et al. (2003). Frontotem-
poral dementia in The Netherlands: Patient characteristics and prevalence estimates from a
population-based study. Brain 126, 2016–2022.
Schroeter, M. L., Raczka, K., Neumann, J., and Yves von Cramon, D. (2007). Towards a nosology for
frontotemporal lobar degenerations-a meta-analysis involving 267 subjects. Neuroimage 36,
497–510.
Silverman, D. H., Gambhir, S. S., Huang, H. W., Schwimmer, J., Kim, S., Small, G. W., Chodosh, J.,
Czernin, J., and Phelps, M. E. (2002). Evaluating early dementia with and without assessment of
regional cerebral metabolism by PET: A comparison of predicted costs and benefits. J. Nucl. Med.
43, 253–266.
Sjogren, M., Gustafson, L., Wikkelso, C., and Wallin, A. (2000). Frontotemporal dementia can be
distinguished from Alzheimer’s disease and subcortical white matter dementia by an anterior-to-
posterior rCBF-SPET ratio. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 11, 275–285.
Snowden, J. S., Gouding, P. J., and Neary, D. (1989). Semantic dementia: A form of circumscribed
cerebral atrophy. Behav. Neurol. 2, 167–182.
Snowden, J. S., Bathgate, D., Varma, A., Blackshaw, A., Gibbons, Z. C., and Neary, D. (2001). Distinct
behavioural profiles in frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg.
Psychiatry 70, 323–332.
Spillantini, M. G., Crowther, R. A., Kamphorst, W., Heutink, P., and van Swieten, J. C. (1998). Tau
pathology in two Dutch families with mutations in the microtubule-binding region of tau. Am. J.
Pathol. 153, 1359–1363.
Stevens, M., van Duijn, C. M., Kamphorst, W., de KnijV, P., Heutink, P., van Gool, W. A.,
Scheltens, P., Ravid, R., Oostra, B. A., Niermeijer, M. F., and van Swieten, J. C. (1998). Familial
aggregation in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 50, 1541–1545.
FTLD: INSIGHTS FROM NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND NEUROIMAGING 213
Studholme, C., Cardenas, V., Blumenfeld, R., SchuV, N., Rosen, H. J., Miller, B., and Weiner, M.
(2004). Deformation tensor morphometry of semantic dementia with quantitative validation.
Neuroimage 21, 1387–1398.
Talbot, P. R., Goulding, P. J., Lloyd, J. J., Snowden, J. S., Neary, D., and Testa, H. J. (1995a). Inter-
relation between ‘‘classic’’ motor neuron disease and frontotemporal dementia: Neuropsychologi-
cal and single photon emission computed tomography study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 58,
541–547.
Talbot, P. R., Snowden, J. S., Lloyd, J. J., Neary, D., and Testa, H. J. (1995b). The contribution of
single photon emission tomography to the clinical diVerentiation of degenerative cortical brain
disorders. J. Neurol. 242, 579–586.
The Lund and Manchester Groups. (1994). Clinical and neuropathological criteria for frontotemporal
dementia.. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 57, 416–418.
Thompson, J. C., Stopford, C. L., Snowden, J. S., and Neary, D. (2005). Qualitative neuropsychologi-
cal performance characteristics in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol.
Neurosurg. Psychiatry 76, 920–927.
Tyrrell, P. J., Warrington, E. K., Frackowiak, R. S., and Rossor, M. N. (1990). Heterogeneity in
progressive aphasia due to focal cortical atrophy. A clinical and PET study. Brain 113(Pt 5),
1321–1336.
Warrington, E. K. (1975). The selective impairment of semantic memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 27,
635–657.
Weintraub, S., Rubin, N. P., and Mesulam, M. M. (1990). Primary progressive aphasia. Longitudinal
course, neuropsychological profile, and language features. Arch. Neurol. 47, 1329–1335.
Wilhelmsen, K. C., Lynch, T., Pavlou, E., Higgins, M., and Nygaard, T. G. (1994). Localization of
disinhibition-dementia-parkinsonism-amyotrophy complex to 17q21-22. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 55,
1159–1165.
LEWY BODY DEMENTIA
I. Introduction
II. DLB Clinical Features
III. PD-D
IV. DLB and PD-D
V. Pathology
VI. Genetics
VII. Biomarkers
VIII. Management
References
Additionally, many DLB cases are also associated with beta-amyloid and tau-
containing neurofibrillary tangles, features that are associated with AD. Frequently,
AD patients are also found to have LB. The reason for this overlap is unknown.
However, the greater the Alzheimer’s pathology in DLB patients, the more the
clinical features of DLB overlaps with AD.
In this chapter, we will review DLB including clinical, pathological, and
radiological features as well as biomarkers and treatments.
I. Introduction
Alzheimer’s Most common >65 years old General cortical atrophy, especially in Memory impairment
disease medial temporal lobe
Genetic susceptibility factors Amyloid plaques in cortex DiYculty in learning new information
(mostly for late-onset)
Autosomal dominant inheritance Neuritic plaques Little fluctuation or hallucinations
(more often present in early onset) Neurofibrillary tangles containing tau Apraxia
and ubiquitin
Amyloid angiopathy Rigidity may be a late feature
Vascular dementia >40 years old Multiple infarcts—often in subcortical Step-wise deterioration
areas
Risk factors: May improve
217
Hypertension Fibrous and hyaline degeneration of Pyramidal signs
small arteries
Smoking
Vascular disease
Other vascular risk factors White matter infarction Pseudobulbar palsy
Dementia with Usually sporadic General cortical atrophy; may be normal Parkinsonism
Lewy body Often elderly, especially when Depigmentation of substantia nigra Fluctuating mental state with slow processing,
(DLB) cognitive presentations attentional, and visuo-spatial problems
LB in limbic and cortical neurons; often Visual hallucinations
brainstem LB
Amyloid deposits are common Neuroleptic sensitivity
REM behavioral disorder
Parkinson’s 30% of PD patients Neuronal cell death in substantia nigra Parkinsonism before onset of dementia
disease Usually sporadic LB in nigral neurons; often in limbic and Fluctuating confusion with similar features
dementia cortical regions (attentional problems, slow processing) to DLB
Visual hallucinations
218 HANSON AND LIPPA
In 2005, the DLB consortium issued its third report on the Current Interna-
tional Consensus Diagnostic Criteria for DLB (McKeith et al., 2005). The central
features necessary for a diagnosis of DLB include the presence of a dementia,
fluctuating cognition, hallucinations, and parkinsonian symptoms. Numerous
supportive features are commonly found in DLB patients, but are not necessary
for the diagnosis.
As in AD, DLB patients have a progressive cognitive decline suYcient enough
to interfere with normal social or occupational functioning (DSM IV-TR, 2004).
However, in contrast to AD, the memory impairment of DLB may not be
prominent in the early stages (McKeith et al., 2005). The cognitive problems
fluctuate with pronounced variation in attention and alertness. This feature is
hard to monitor in clinical practice due to its diYculty to observe on a consistent
basis. When they do appear, the deficits on tests of attention, executive function,
and visuo-spatial ability in DLB are generally prominent (McKeith et al., 2005).
Subjects with DLB have better delayed memory and spared recall, but worse
executive function and visuo-spatial abilities than patients with early AD. Such
diVerences in visuo-spatial abilities can be demonstrated by the intersecting
pentagons used as part of the mini mental-status exam. An example of this is
shown in Fig. 1.
In DLB, the cognitive deficits represent both cortical and subcortical impair-
ments, with greatest deficits being verbal fluency, visual perception, and perfor-
mance tasks while there is preservation of confrontation naming, recognition, and
short-term recall (Connor et al., 1998; Mormont et al., 2003; Walker et al., 1997).
A B C D
FIG. 1. Figure drawings of normal, Alzheimer’s dementia, and Dementia with Lewy body (DLB)
patients. Patient drawings demonstrate the visuo-spatial dysfunction in DLB patients compared with
normal aging patients and AD patients. Image A is a model of connecting pentagons, normally used to
test visuo-spatial function. The patient is asked to copy this image during the test. Image B shows a
normal patient’s response. Note that this image has five sides and that the adjacent sides intersect
in both pentagons. Image C is drawn by an AD patient. This subject drew a hexagon rather than
a pentagon. This is a cognitive error. Image D is from a patient meeting the criteria for probable
DLB. There is marked loss of visuo-spatial relationships, and it is hard to tell what they are trying
to copy.
LEWY BODY DEMENTIA 219
Both DLB and Parkinson’s disease dementia (PD-D) patients have more pro-
nounced subcortical cognitive impairment profiles with less marked memory
impairment than AD patients. However, those patients with a pattern of promi-
nent cortical involvement have more severe memory impairment like that of AD
( Janvin et al., 2006).
Recurrent visual hallucinations are prominent and are typically well formed
and detailed in DLB patients. Hallucinations of animals and people are common.
They are usually frightening and it is diYcult to convince the patient that they are
not real. This may pose safety problems, as the patient will feel that they will be
attacked or their home invaded. In contrast, visual hallucinations are uncommon
in AD, particularly early in the disease course. Therefore, report of such vivid
hallucinations in the context of dementia should alert the physician to the
likelihood of DLB. LB concentrations in the posterior temporal lobes and amyg-
dale, areas critical for emotion and visual processing, are associated with the
presence of these hallucinations (Harding et al., 2002). As such, it has been
speculated that visual hallucinations are positive phenomenon, caused by an
irritating eVect of the LB in relevant areas.
The onset of parkinsonian symptoms occurs spontaneously in DLB. At the
time of diagnosis, approximately 50% of DLB patients have extrapyramidal
motor symptoms, with 75% developing them during some stage of the disease
course (McKeith et al., 1992b). Patients have postural instability, gait disorders
that may be characterized by hunched posture and festination, and facial
immobility (Burn et al., 2003). Tremor may be present, but is not as prominent
as in PD. The Unified PD Rating Scale can be used to monitor parkinsonian
progression, but only for those symptoms that can be assessed in the face of
dementia (Fahn et al., 1987). Objective findings include tremor at rest, intention
tremor, bradykinesia, rigidity, and facial expression. When compared to AD
patients of a similar cognitive level, DLB patients have greater functional
impairment due to the presence of extrapyramidal symptoms (McKeith et al.,
2005).
Many DLB patients have parasomnias. The most common is REM behavior-
al disorder (RBD) which often begins concurrently or after the onset of parkin-
sonism or dementia (Boeve et al., 2007). It is marked by lack of muscle atonia in
the presence of vivid dreams. In RBD that lack of muscle paralysis allows the
patient to engage fully in the physical activities of the dream, resulting in vocaliza-
tions and sometimes wildly violent behavior. Patients are often unaware of the
disorder, but bed partners may report it as a presenting symptom when asked.
RBD is associated with daytime somnolence. It can be diagnosed by polysomno-
graphy which shows elevation in EMG tone during REM sleep and unusual
movements during sleep (Fantini et al., 2005).
DLB patients are notorious for their neuroleptic sensitivity, and reactions
occur in 30–50% of DLB patients (Aarsland et al., 2005b). Such neuroleptic
220 HANSON AND LIPPA
III. PD-D
V. Pathology
Found in 50% of dementia patients (Hamilton, 2000; Lippa et al., 1998), the
main identifying pathologic feature of DLB is the LB. LB are spherical intracy-
toplasmic protein deposits around the nucleus and throughout the dendrite of
subcortical and cortical neurons. They consist of filamentous protein granules
composed of AS and ubiquitin, and are surrounded by a halo of neurofilaments.
Widespread LB diVerentiate the LB dementias from other dementia subtypes.
The number of LB present does not correlate strongly with either the duration or
severity of the dementia (Harding et al., 2001). However, the number of cortical
LB is variably correlated with the severity of DLB (Samuel, 1996). When located
in the temporal lobe, LB are associated with the visual hallucinations of DLB
(Harding et al., 2002).
222 HANSON AND LIPPA
FIG. 2. Lewy bodies (LB) and Lewy neurites. This is a moderately high-power magnification of
brain tissue from the amygdale of a patient with DLB. Tissue is stained with antibodies against alpha-
synuclein (AS). It demonstrates numerous AS containing Lewy bodies and Lewy neurites.
A B C
FIG. 3. High-power magnification images of LB. Image A is stained using a routine H & E stain. It
shows the classical nigral LB with an eosinophilic core and clearer peripheral region. Note the
neuromelanin pigment. Image B is a nigral LB from the same case, but stained with antibodies to
AS. It demonstrates that AS is concentrated at the periphery of the LB (the clearer region on H & E
stains). Image C shows a cortical LB stained with AS. These typically occur in smaller neurons, and
they stain more uniformly with AS; cortical LB lack a halo.
VI. Genetics
cause aggregation of a variety of CNS proteins, and no single cause has been
linked to dementias. Additionally, mutations causing dementia and Parkinsonism
may lead to mixed or variable protein aggregates (Rajput et al., 2006).
Genetic abnormalities in AS or other proteins that cause LB diseases are rare,
but may lead to the cognitive or motor features of the disease. Dementia and
parkinsonism are not always related to pathology; other genetic localizations may
also lead to symptoms of parkinsonism and dementia. For example, some patients
with tau mutations may have features of PD-D and DLB but lack LB, while others
may have clinical and neuropsychological features that diVer.
Factors determining the distribution of pathology in relation to the symptoms
are incompletely understood. Schiesling et al. (2008) aptly stated that, ‘‘The
identification of the first gene in familial Parkinson’s disease (PD) only 10 years
ago was a major step in the understanding of the molecular mechanisms in
neurodegeneration. AS aggregation was not only recognized as a key event in
neurodegeneration in patients carrying mutations in this gene, but it turned out to
be the most consistent marker to define LB pathology also in nonheritable
idiopathic PD.’’ Numerous other genes have been found associated with PD,
and many individuals with ‘‘idiopathic’’ forms may prove to carry susceptibility
factors.
VII. Biomarkers
VIII. Management
References
Aarsland, D., Litvan, I., Salmon, D., Galasko, D., Wentzel-Larsen, T., and Larsen, J. P. (2003).
Performance on the dementia rating scale in Parkinson’s disease with dementia and dementia
with Lewy bodies: Comparison with progressive supranuclear palsy and Alzheimer’s disease.
J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 74(9), 1215–1220.
Aarsland, D., Perry, R., Larsen, J. P., McKeith, I. G., O’Brien, J. T., Perry, E. K., Burn, D., and
Ballard, C. G. (2005b). Neuroleptic sensitivity in Parkinson’s disease and parkinsonian dementias.
J. Clin. Psychiatr. 66(5), 633–637.
Aarsland, D., Kurz, M., Beyer, M., Bronnick, K., Pippenstock, N., and Ballard, C. (2008). Early
discriminatory diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies. The emerging role of CSF and imaging
biomarkers. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 25(3), 195–205.
American Psychiatric Association (2004). ‘‘Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
(DSM-IV TR).’’ Vol 157, 4th ed., 3rd revision, American Psychiatric Association, Arlington, VA.
Baba, M., Nakajo, S., Tu, P. H., Tomita, T., Nakaya, K., Lee, V. M., Trojanowski, J. Q., and
Iwatsubo, T. (1998). Aggregation of alpha-synuclein in Lewy bodies of sporadic Parkinson’s
disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Am. J. Pathol. 152(4), 879–884.
Ballard, C. G., Chalmers, K. A., Todd, C., McKeith, I. G., OBrien, J. T., Wilcock, G., Love, S., and
Perry, E. K. (2007). Cholinesterase inhibitors reduce cortical Abeta in dementia with Lewy bodies.
Neurology 68(20), 1726–1729.
Barber, R., Panikkar, A., and McKeith, I. G. (2001). Dementia with Lewy bodies: Diagnosis and
management. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatr. 16, S12–S18.
Boeve, B. F., Silber, M. H., and Ferman, T. J. (2004). REM sleep behavior disorder in Parkinson’s
disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. J. Geriatr. Psychiatr. Neurol. 17(3), 146–157.
LEWY BODY DEMENTIA 227
Boeve, B. F., Silber, M. H., Saper, C. B., Ferman, T. J., Dickson, D. W., Parisi, J. E.,
Benarroch, E. E., Ahlskog, J. E., Smith, G. E., Caselli, R. C., Tippman-Peikert, M., et al.
(2007). Pathophysiology of REM sleep behavior disorder and relevance to neurodegenerative
disease. Brain 130(Pt. 11), 2770–2788.
Burn, D. J., and McKeith, I. G. (2003). Current treatment of dementia with Lewy bodies and
dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 18(Suppl. 6), S72–S79.
Burn, D. J., Rowan, E. N., Minett, T., Sanders, J., Myint, P., Richardson, J., Thomas, A.,
Newby, J., Reid, J., OBrien, J. T., and McKeith, I. G. (2003). Extrapyramidal features
in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies: A
cross-sectional comparative study. Mov. Disord. 18(8), 884–889.
Connor, D. J., Salmon, D. P., Sandy, T. J., Galasko, D., Hansen, L. A., and Thal, L. J. (1998).
Cognitive profiles of autopsy-confirmed Lewy body variant vs pure Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol.
55(7), 994–1000.
Duda, J. E., Giasson, B. I., Mabon, M. E., Lee, V. M., and Trojanowski, J. Q. (2002). Novel
antibodies to synuclein show abundant striatal pathology in Lewy body diseases. Ann. Neurol. 52(2),
205–210.
Emre, M., Aarsland, D., Brown, R., Burn, D. J., Duyckaerts, C., Mizuno, Y., Broe, G. A.,
Cummings, J., Dickson, D. W., Gauthier, S., Goldman, J., Goetz, C., et al. (2007). Clinical
diagnostic criteria for dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 22(12),
1689–1707.
Fahn, S., and Elton, R. L., and UPDRS program members (1987). Unified Parkinson’s disease rating
scale. In ‘‘Recent Developments in Parkinsons Disease’’ (S. Fahn, C. D. Marsden, M. Goldstein, and
D. B. Calne, Eds.), Vol. 2, pp. 153–163. Macmillan Healthcare Information, Florham Park, NJ.
Fantini, M. L., Ferini-Strambi, L., and Montplaisir, J. (2005). Idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder:
Toward a better nosologic definition. Neurology 64, 780–786.
Hamilton, R. L. (2000). Lewy bodies in Alzheimer’s disease: A neuropathological review of 145 cases
using alpha-synuclein immunohistochemistry. Brain Pathol. 10(3), 378–384.
Harding, A. J., and Halliday, G. M. (2001). Cortical Lewy body pathology in the diagnosis of dementia.
Acta Neuropathol. 102, 355–363.
Harding, A. J., Broe, G. A., and Halliday, G. M. (2002). Visual hallucinations in Lewy body disease
relate to Lewy bodies in the temporal lobe. Brain 125(Pt. 2), 391–403.
Janvin, C. C., Larsen, J. P., Salmon, D. P., Galasko, D., Hugdahl, K., and Aarsland, D. (2006).
Cognitive profiles of individual patients with Parkinson’s disease and dementia: Comparison with
dementia with lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease. Mov. Disord. 21(3), 337–342.
Kramer, M. L., and Schulz-SchaeVer, W. J. (2007). Presynaptic alpha-synuclein aggregates, not Lewy
bodies, cause neurodegeneration in dementia with Lewy bodies. J. Neurosci. 27(6), 1405–1410.
Lippa, C. F., Fujiwara, H., Mann, D. M. A., Giasson, B., Baba, M., Schmidt, M. L., Nee, L. E.,
O’Connell, B., Pollen, D. A., St. George-Hyslop, P., Ghetti, B., Nochlin, D., et al. (1998). Lewy
bodies contain altered alpha-synuclein in brains of many familial Alzheimer’s disease patients with
mutations in presenilin and amyloid precursor protein genes. Am. J. Pathol. 153, 1365–1370.
Lippa, C. F., Smith, T. W., and Perry, E. (1999). Dementia with Lewy bodies: Choline acetyltransferase
parallels nucleus basalis pathology. J. Neural Transm. 106, 525–535.
Lippa, C. F., Schmidt, M. L., Trojanowski, J. Q., and Lee, V. M. Y. (2001). Alpha-synuclein and
familial Alzheimer’s disease: Epitope mapping parallels DLB and Parkinson’s disease. Arch. Neurol.
58, 1817–1820.
Lippa, C. F., Boeve, B. F., Parisi, J. E., and Keegan, B. M. (2007a). A 75-year-old man with cognitive
impairment and gait changes. Neurology 69(11), 1183–1189.
Lippa, C. F., Duda, J. E., Grossman, M., Hurtig, H. I., Aarsland, D., Boeve, B. F., Brooks, D. J.,
Dickson, D. W., Dubois, B., Emre, M., Fahn, S., Farmer, J. M., et al. DLB/PD-D
Working Group (2007b). DLB and PD-D boundary issues: Diagnosis, treatment, molecular
pathology, and biomarkers. Neurology 68(11), 812–819.
228 HANSON AND LIPPA
McKeith, I. G., and Mosimann, U. P. (2004). Dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson’s disease.
Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 10(Suppl. 1), S15–S18.
McKeith, I., Fairbaim, A., Perry, R., et al. (1992a). Neuroleptic sensitivity in patients with senile
dementia of Lewy body type. BMJ 305, 673–678.
McKeith, I. G., Galasko, D., Kosaka, K., Perry, E. K., Dickson, D. W., Hansen, L. A.,
Salmon, D. P., Lowe, J., Mirra, S. S., Byrne, E. J., Lennox, G., Quinn, N. P., et al. (1996).
Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies:
Report of the consortium on DLB international workshop. Neurology 47(5):1113–1124.
McKeith, I. G., Perry, R. H., Fairbairn, A. F., Jabeen, S., and Perry, E. K. (1992b). Operational
criteria for senile dementia of Lewy body type (SDLT). Psychol. Med. 22, 911–922.
McKeith, I., Mintzer, J., Aarsland, D., et al. (2004). Dementia with Lewy bodies. Lancet Neurol. 3,
19–28.
McKeith, I. G., Dickson, D. W., Lowe, J., et al. (2005). Consortium on DLB. Diagnosis and manage-
ment of dementia with Lewy bodies: Third report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology 65(12),
1863–1872.
McKeith, I. G., Rowan, E., Askew, K., et al. (2006). More severe functional impairment in dementia
with Lewy bodies than Alzheimer disease is related to extrapyramidal motor dysfunction. Am. J.
Geriatr. Psychiatr. 14(7), 582–588.
Mormont, E., Laurier-Grymonprez, L., Baisset-Mouly, C., and Pasquier, F. (2003). The profile of
memory disturbance in early Lewy body dementia diVers from that in Alzheimer’s disease. Rev.
Neurol. 159(8–9), 762–766.
Mosimann, U. P., Rowan, E. N., Partington, C. E., Collerton, D., Littlewood, E., O’Brien, J. T.,
Burn, D. J., and McKeith, I. G. (2006). Characteristics of visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s
disease dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 14(2),153–160.
Mukaetova-Ladinska, E. B., and McKeith, I. G. (2006). Pathophysiology of synuclein aggregation in
Lewy body disease. Mech. Ageing. Dev. 127(2),188–202.
O’Brien, J. T., Firbank, M. J., Mosimann, U. P., et al. (2005). Change in perfusion, hallucinations
and fluctuations in consciousness in dementia with Lewy bodies. Psychiatr. Res. 139(2), 79–88.
Perry, E. K., Haroutunian, V., Davis, K. L., et al. (1994). Neocortical cholinergic activities diVerentiate
Lewy body dementia from classical Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroreport 5, 747–749.
Rajput, A., Dickson, D. W., Robinson, C. A., et al. (2006). Parkinsonism, Lrrk2 G2019S, and tau
neuropathology. Neurology 67(8), 1506–1508.
Rockenstein, E., Hansen, L., Mallory, M., et al. (2001). Altered expression of the synuclein family
mRNA in Lewy body and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Res. 914, 48–56.
Rogan, S., and Lippa, C. F. (2002). Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias: A review. Am. J. Alzheimers
Dis. Other Demen. 17(1), 11–17.
Samuel, W., Galaski, D., Masliah, E., and Hanson, L. A. (1996). Neocortical Lewy body counts
correlate with dementia in the Lewy body variant of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol.
55, 44–52.
Samuel, W., Caligiuri, M., Galasko, D., et al. (2000). Better cognitive and psychopathologic response to
donepezil in patients prospectively diagnosed as dementia with Lewy bodies: A preliminary study.
Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatr. 15, 794–802.
Schiesling, C., Kieper, N., Siedel, K., and Kruger, R. (2008). Review: Familial Parkinson’s disease—
Genetics, clinical phenotype and neuropathology in relation to the common sporadic form of the
disease. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 34(3), 255–271.
Walker, Z., Allen, R. L., Shergill, S., and Katona, C. L. (1997). Neuropsychological performance in
Lewy body dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Br. J. Psychiatr. 170, 156–158.
DEMENTIA IN PARKINSON’S DISEASE
I. Introduction
II. Epidemiology
III. Early Cognitive Changes in Parkinson’s Disease
IV. Clinical Features of Parkinson’s Disease Dementia
V. Pathology
VI. Neuroimaging
VII. Diagnosis
VIII. Treatment
References
I. Introduction
In the 1817 monograph, ‘‘An Essay on the Shaking Palsy,’’ James Parkinson
wrote that ‘‘the senses and intellect remain uninjured’’ (Parkinson, 1817). PD is
still diagnosed based on the motor features that Parkinson described (resting
tremor, bradykinesia, rigidity), but it has become clear that cognitive changes
and dementia in PD are common. The cognitive eVects of PD are varied, ranging
from subtle executive dysfunction in the early stages of illness to dementia in
advanced PD. For the purpose of this chapter, cognitive changes in early PD will
be defined as changes seen in patients with recent onset of disease (<5 years
disease duration) or with mild motor symptoms (Hoehn and Yahr stages I or II).
This chapter will review epidemiology of PDD, clinical features of early and late
cognitive changes, pathology, neuroimaging, diagnosis, and treatment of PDD.
II. Epidemiology
The association between family history of dementia and the development of PDD
has led to a number of gene alleles being evaluated as risk factors for PDD.
However, no conclusive evidence has been found (Rippon and Marder, 2005).
Deficits in executive function are among the first cognitive changes demon-
strable in early PD and may be present in a majority of patients with PD (Cooper
et al., 1991; Levin et al., 1989). Intact executive functioning is necessary for
cognitive tasks that require planning, anticipation, goal selection, and using
feedback to guide behavior (Levin and Katzen, 2005). Patients may perceive
these diYculties as bradyphrenia, or slowness of thought (Fahn and Jankovic,
2007). Deficits may be subtle and diYcult to detect on standard memory testing
such as the Mini-Mental State Examination. More in-depth neuropsychological
tests such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task show a tendency toward persever-
ative errors (Canavan, 1989; Levin et al., 1989). Abnormalities of set shifting and
maintaining set can be demonstrated in early PD using Raven’s Progressive
Matrices (Farina et al., 2000). The modified Tower of London task may also
show diYculty with set shifting and maintaining the correct response set, even
in early PD patients (Owen et al., 1992). In Owen’s study of non-medicated
patients with PD, there were no diVerences in accuracy and initial thinking
time, reinforcing that executive function deficit may be subtle. These early, subtle
changes in executive function may not have clinical consequence. There are
many early patients with high level professional and management positions.
Memory impairment occurs early in PD and may be related primarily to
executive dysfunction leading to disruption of the memorization process (Levin
and Katzen, 2005). Memory impairment in early PD is independent of dementia.
Patients with PD perform comparably to controls on tasks of recall of unrelated
words and spatial position (Taylor et al., 1990). However, there may be deficits in
the recall of semantically related words (California Verbal Learning Test), defi-
cient source memory, and increased sensitivity to interference (Taylor et al., 1990).
PD patients are less likely to use strategies such as clustering that facilitate recall.
Supporting the findings from Taylor’s study, increased sensitivity to interference
was also found in newly diagnosed PD patients by using the Brown-Peterson
Distractor Task (Cooper et al., 1993). Impaired inhibition is theorized to be the
cause of PD patients’ inability to maintain relevant information during distraction
tasks (Kensiger et al., 2003). These studies support the contention that memory
impairment in early PD is related to executive dysfunction, causing a disruption in
the necessary processes of sequencing and encoding new memories.
232 ROBOTTOM AND WEINER
Current evidence does not suggest that these findings are analogous to mild
cognitive impairment and AD. Further research is needed to determine the
significance of executive dysfunction in early PD.
V. Pathology
(Braak and Braak, 1991; Paulus and Jellinger, 1991). In particular, there is a
higher burden of cortical AD pathology found in PDD patients (Paulus and
Jellinger, 1991). It has been hypothesized that Lewy body and AD pathology
are triggered by a common cause, which would explain the correlation between
the two (Apaydin et al., 2002). Unlike the early involvement of sensory association
areas seen in AD (Vermersch et al., 1992), AD pathology found in PDD patients is
found predominantly in frontal, temporal, and entorhinal cortices (Vermersch
et al., 1993). It seems likely that a combination of disrupted neurotransmitter
systems, AD pathology, and Lewy body pathology all contribute to the clinical
manifestations of PDD.
VI. Neuroimaging
VII. Diagnosis
Prior to 1997, PDD was diagnosed according to DSM IV criteria, which were
relatively nonspecific (Rippon and Marder, 2005). In 2007, the Movement Dis-
orders Society created a task force which developed clinical diagnostic criteria for
PDD. PDD is a slowly progressive dementia that develops with the context of
established PD. There must be deficits in at least two of four core cognitive
domains (attention, memory, executive, and visuospatial). The deficits must be
severe enough to interfere with normal functioning. Behavioral features such as
hallucinations, delusions, apathy, and changes in mood are frequently present but
not necessary for diagnosis of PDD. The label of ‘‘possible PDD’’ is given to
patients who do not have behavioral features, who have an atypical cognitive
profile, or who have another abnormality which may explain the cognitive
dysfunction (such as significant vascular disease, major depression, or a concur-
rent toxic/metabolic encephalopathy). A diagnosis of ‘‘probable PDD’’ requires a
diagnosis of PD, a slowly progressive dementia defined by deficits in at least two of
four core cognitive domains (attention, memory, executive, and visuospatial), and
the presence of a behavioral feature (Emre et al., 2007).
VIII. Treatment
There are no pharmacologic agents that have been developed specifically for
the treatment of PDD. The agents that are currently used were first developed for
AD and were subsequently studied in PDD. Randomized, double-blind, placebo
controlled trials (RCTs) are limited in number and are confined to acetylcholin-
esterase inhibitors. Three small, RCTs of donepezil for treatment of PDD showed
improvement in Mini-Mental State Examination of two points (Aarsland et al.,
2002; Ravina et al., 2005) and improvement in the memory subscale of the Mattis
Dementia Rating Scale (Leroi et al., 2004). Overall the drug was well tolerated
with few reports of increased parkinsonism. The overall benefit of donepezil is
modest, and the American Academy of Neurology Practice Parameters state that
donepezil ‘‘should be considered for the treatment of dementia in PD’’ (Miyasaki
et al., 2006). One large, RCT of rivastigmine was performed, and the results
showed moderate improvements in dementia that were accompanied by worsen-
ing tremor in 10% of patients (Emre, 2004). Like donepezil, the AAN recom-
mends that rivastigmine ‘‘should be considered for the treatment of dementia in
PD’’ (Miyasaki et al., 2006). Small, open label studies of tacrine and galantamine
also report modest benefits on cognition (Aarsland, 2002; Hutchinson and
Fazzini, 1996), but no RCTs of these agents have been performed. Memantine,
238 ROBOTTOM AND WEINER
an NMDA antagonist used in the treatment of AD, has not been studied for the
treatment of PDD.
Treatment of the neuropsychiatric or behavioral features of PDD may take a
variety of approaches. The first strategy employed in the treatment of psychosis is
to reduce the dose of those drugs which are least potent with respect to motor
impairment (Fig. 1). Medications that are known to cause psychosis (e.g., dopa-
mine agonists, monoamine oxidase B (MAO-B) inhibitors, catechol-O-methyl
transferase (COMT) inhibitors, amantadine) should be discontinued. If this is
unsuccessful in resolving psychosis, then the daily levodopa dose should be reduced
until unacceptable motor complications arise. If psychosis persists after reducing
dopaminergic medications, then the addition of an antipsychotic medication may
be necessary. The AAN Practice Parameters state that clozapine is ‘‘probably an
eVective treatment and improves psychosis’’ and quetiapine ‘‘possibly improves
psychosis’’ in PD (Miyasaki et al., 2006). Clozapine is the only antipsychotic that
has been proved eVective for psychosis in PD as both RCTs of quetiapine have
failed to show benefit (Ondo et al., 2005; Rabey et al., 2007). However, quetiapine is
often prescribed first because of the risk of agranulocytosis seen with clozapine.
Other antipsychotics are not recommended because of their eVects on motor
function in PD. Open label trials of rivastigmine and donepezil have suggested
that acetylcholinesterase inhibitors may be beneficial for psychosis in PDD
Step 1:
Reduce dopaminergic medications
• Discontinue dopamine agonists, MAO-B inhibitors,
COMT inhibitors, amantadine
• Reduce levodopa dosage
Step 2:
Psychosis persists
• Start quetiapine at a low dose (25 mg)
• Titrate up as tolerated
Step 3:
Psychosis persists
• Transition to clozapine
• Monitor complete blood counts for agranulocytosis
(Bergman and Vladimir, 2002; Reading et al., 2001). One RCT of rivastigmine
suggested that PDD patients with psychosis (visual hallucinations) may have
greater cognitive benefit from rivastigmine than those without psychosis. The
study was not powered to examine behavioral diVerences (Burn et al., 2005).
Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors may be a reasonable treatment option for psychosis
in PD, though they do not have the proved eYcacy of antipsychotic medications.
References
Aarsland, D. (2002). Galantamine for Parkinson’s disease with dementia. Eur. Neuropsychopharm.
12(Suppl. 3), 378–379.
Aarsland, D., Larsen, J. P., Tandberg, E., and Laake, K. (2000). Predictors of nursing home placement
in Parkinson’s disease: A population-based, prospective study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 48, 938–942.
Aarsland, D., Anderson, K., Larsen, J. P., Lolk, A., Nielsen, H., and Kragh-Sorensen, P. (2001a). Risk
of dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 56, 730–736.
Aarsland, D., Ballard, C., Larsen, J. P., and McKeith, I. (2001b). A comparative study of psychiatric
symptoms in dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia.
Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 16, 528–536.
Aarsland, D., Laake, K., Larsen, J. P., and Janvin, C. (2002). Donepezil for cognitive impairment in
Parkinson’s disease: A randomised controlled study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 72, 708–712.
Aarsland, D., Litvan, I., Salmon, D., Galasko, D., Wentzel-Larsen, T., and Larsen, J. P. (2003).
Performance on the dementia rating scale in Parkinson’s disease with dementia and dementia
with Lewy bodies: Comparison with progressive supranuclear palsy and Alzheimer’s disease.
J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 74, 1215–1220.
Aarsland, D., Zaccai, J., and Brayne, C. (2005). A systemic review of prevalence studies of dementia in
Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 20, 1255–1263.
Aarsland, D., Bronnick, K., Ehrt, U., De Deyn, P. P., Tekin, S., Emre, M., and Cummings, J. L. (2007).
Neuropsychiatric symptoms in patients with PD and dementia: Frequency, profile, and associated
caregiver stress. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 78, 36–42.
Antonini, A., De Notaris, R., Benti, R., De Gaspari, D., and Pezzoli, G. (2001). Perfusion ECD/
SPECT in the characterization of cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Neurol. Sci. 22(1), 45–46.
Apaydin, H., Ahlskog, J. E., Parisi, J. E., Boeve, B. F., and Dickson, D. W. (2002). Parkinson disease
neuropathology: Later-developing dementia and loss of the levodopa response. Arch. Neurol. 59(1),
102–112.
Ballard, C. G., Aarsland, D., McKeith, I., O’Brien, J., Gray, A., Cormack, F., Burn, D., Cassidy, T.,
Starfeldt, R., Larsen, J. P., Brown, R., and Tovee, M. (2002). Fluctuations in attention: PD
dementia vs. DLB with parkinsonism. Neurology 59(11), 1714–1720.
Bergman, J., and Vladimir, L. (2002). Successful use of donepezil for the treatment of psychotic
symptoms in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Clin. Neuropharmacol. 25(2), 107–110.
Biggins, C. A., Boyd, J. L., Harrop, F. M., Madeley, P., Mindham, R. H., Randall, J. I., and
Spokes, E. G. (1992). A controlled, longitudinal study of dementia in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol.
Neurosurg. Psychiatry 55, 566–571.
Bohnen, N. I., Kaufer, D. I., and Ivanco, L. S. (2003). Cortical cholinergic function is more severely
aVected in Parkinsonian dementia than in Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 60, 1745–1748.
240 ROBOTTOM AND WEINER
Boller, F., Passafiume, D., Keefe, N. C., Rogers, K., Morrow, L., and Kim, Y. (1984). Visuospatial
impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Role of perceptual and motor factors. Arch. Neurol. 41(5),
485–490.
Braak, H., and Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological staging of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta
Neuropathol. 82(4), 239–259.
Braak, H., Del Tredici, K., Bohl, J., Bratzke, H., and Braak, E. (2000). Pathological changes in the
parahippocampal region in select non-Alzheimer dementias. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 911, 221–239.
Braak, H., Del Tredici, K., Rub, U., de Vos, R. A., Jansen Steur, E. N., and Braak, E. (2003). Staging
of brain pathology related to sporadic Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging. 24(2), 197–211.
Burn, D., Emre, M., McKeith, I. G., De Deyn, P. P., Aarsland, D., Hsu, C., and Lane, R. (2005).
EVects of rivastigmine in patients with and without visual hallucinations in dementia associated
with Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 21(11), 1899–1907.
Burton, E. J., McKeith, I. G., Burn, D. J., and O’Brien, J. T. (2005). Brain atrophy rates in Parkinson’s
disease with and without dementia using serial magnetic resonance imaging. Mov. Disord. 20(12),
1571–1576.
Cahn-Weiner, D. A., Grace, J., Ott, B. R., Fernandez, H. H., and Friedman, J. H. (2002). Cognitive
and behavioral features discriminate between Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychiatry
Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol. 15, 79–87.
Canavan, A. G. M. (1989). The performance on learning tasks of patients in the early stages of
Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 27, 141–156.
Chui, H. C., Mortimer, J. A., Slager, U., Zarow, C., Bondareff, W., and Webster, D. D. (1986).
Pathologic correlates of dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Arch. Neurol. 43(10), 991–995.
Colloby, S. J., Williams, E. D., Burn, D. J., Lloyd, J. J., McKeith, I. G., and O’Brien, J. T. (2005).
Progression of dopaminergic degeneration in dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson’s disease
with and without dementia assessed using 123I-FP-CIT SPECT. Eur. J. Nucl. Mol. Imaging 32(10),
1176–1185.
Cooper, J. A., Sagar, H. J., Jordan, N., Harvey, N. S., and Sullivan, E. V. (1991). Cognitive
impairment in early, untreated Parkinson’s disease and its relationship to motor disability. Brain
114, 2095–2122.
Cooper, J. A., Sagar, H. J., and Sullivan, E. V. (1993). Short-term memory and temporal ordering in early
Parkinson’s disease: EVects of disease chronicity and medication. Neuropsychologia 31, 933–949.
Cooper, J. A., Sagar, H. J., Tidswell, P., and Jordan, N. (1994). Slowed central processing in simple and
go/no-go reaction time tasks in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 117(3), 517–529.
Cummings, J. L., Darkins, A., Mendez, M., Hill, M. A., and Benson, D. F. (1988). Alzheimer’s disease
and Parkinson’s disease: Comparison of speech and language alterations. Neurology 38(5), 680–684.
de Lau, L. M., Schipper, C. M., Hofman, A., Koudstall, P. J., and Breteler, M. M. (2005). Prognosis of
Parkinson disease: Risk of dementia and mortality: The Rotterdam study. Arch. Neurol. 62,
1265–1269.
Dujardin, K., Blairy, S., Defebvre, L., Duhem, S., Noel, Y., Hess, U., and Destee, A. (2004). Deficits in
decoding emotional facial expressions in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 42(2), 239–250.
Ebmeier, K. P., Calder, S. A., Crawford, J. R., Stewart, L., Besson, J. A., and Mutch, W. J. (1990).
Clinical features predicting dementia in idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: A follow-up study.
Neurology 40(8), 1222–1224.
Emre, M. (2003). Dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2(4), 229–237.
Emre, M. (2004). Dementia in Parkinson’s disease: Cause and treatment. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 17(4),
399–404.
Emre, M., Aarsland, D., Albanese, A., Byrne, E. J., Deuschl, G., De Deyn, P. P., Durif, F.,
Kulisevsky, J., van Laar, T., Lees, A., Poewe, W., Robillard, A., et al. (2004). Rivastigmine for
dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. NEJM 351, 2509–2518.
DEMENTIA IN PARKINSON’S DISEASE 241
Emre, M., Aarsland, D., Brown, R., Burn, D. J., Duychaerts, C., Mizuno, Y., Broe, G. A.,
Cummings, J., Dickson, D. W., Gauthier, S., Goldman, J., Goetz, C., et al. (2007). Clinical
diagnostic criteria for dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 22(12),
1689–1707.
Fahn, S., and Jankovic, J. (2007). ‘‘Principles and Practices of Movement Disorders.’’ Churchill
Livingstone, Philadelphia.
Farina, E., Gattellaro, G., and Pomati, S. (2000). Researching a diVerential impairment of frontal
functions and explicit memory in early Parkinson’s disease. Eur. J. Neurol. 7, 259–267.
Fenelon, G., Mahieux, F., Huon, R., and Ziegler, M. (2000). Hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease:
Prevalence, phenomenology, and risk factors. Brain 123, 733–745.
Firbank, M. J., Colloby, S. J., Burn, D. J., McKeith, I. G., and O’Brien, J. T. (2003). Regional cerebral
blood flow in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia. Neuroimage 20(2), 1309–1319.
Galvin, J. E., Lee, V. M. Y., and Trojanowski, J. Q. (2001). Synucleinopathies. Clinical and pathologi-
cal implications. Arch. Neurol. 58, 186–190.
Girotti, F., Soliveri, P., Carella, F., Piccolo, I., Caffara, P., Musicco, M., and Caraceni, T. (1988).
Dementia and cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosrug. Psychiatry 51(12),
1498–1502.
Goto, I., Taniwaki, T., Hosokawa, S., Otsuka, M., Ichiya, Y., and Ichimiya, A. (1993). Positron
emission topographic (PET) studies in dementia. J. Neurol. Sci. 114(1), 1–6.
Grossman, M., Carvell, S., Stern, M. B., Gollomp, S., and Hurtig, H. I. (1992). Sentence comprehen-
sion in Parkinson’s disease: The role of attention and memory. Brain Lang. 42(4), 347–384.
Hanyu, H., Asano, T., Sakurai, H., Tanaka, Y., Takasaki, M., and Abe, K. (2002). MR analysis of the
substantia innominata in normal aging, Alzheimer disease, and other types of dementia. AJNR
Am. J. Neuroradiol. 23, 27–32.
Hobson, P., and Meara, J. (2004). Risk and incidence of dementia in a cohort of older subjects with
Parkinson’s disease in the United Kingdom. Mov. Disord. 19(9), 1043–1049.
Huber, S. J., Shuttleworth, E. C., Christy, J. A., Chakeres, D. W., Curtin, A., and Paulson, G. W.
(1989a). Magnetic resonance imaging in dementia of Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg.
Psychiatry 52(11), 1221–1227.
Huber, S. J., Shuttleworth, E. C., and Freidenberg, D. L. (1989b). Neuropsychological diVerences
between the dementias of Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases. Arch. Neurol. 46(12), 1287–1291.
Hughes, T. A., Ross, H. F., Musa, S., Bhattacherjee, S., Nathan, R. N., Mindham, R. H., and
Spokes, E. G. (2000). A 10-year study of the incidence of and factors predicting dementia in
Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 54, 1596–1603.
Hutchinson, M., and Fazzini, E. (1996). Cholinesterase inhibition in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol.
Neurosurg. Psychiatry 61(3), 324–325.
Illes, J. (1989). Neurolinguistic features of spontaneous language production dissociate three forms of
neurodegenerative disease. Brain Lang. 37, 628–642.
Jacobs, D. M., Marder, K., Cote, L. J., Sano, M., Stern, Y., and Mayeux, R. (1995). Neuropsychologi-
cal characteristics of preclinical dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 45, 1691–1696.
Jellinger, K. A. (1997). Morphological substrates of dementia in parkinsonism. A critical update.
J. Neurol. Transm. Suppl. 51, 57–82.
Jordan, N., Sagar, H. J., and Cooper, J. A. (1992). Cognitive components of reaction time in
Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 55, 658–664.
Junque, C., Ramirez-Ruiz, B., Tolosa, E., Summerfield, C., Marti, M. J., Pastor, P., Gomez-Anson, B.,
and Mercador, J. M. (2005). Amygdalar and hippocampal MRI volumetric reductions in Parkin-
son’s disease with dementia. Mov. Disord. 20(5), 540–544.
Kensiger, E. A., Shearer, D. K., Locascio, J. J., Growdon, J. H., and Corkin, S. (2003). Working
memory in mild Alzheimer’s disease and early Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology 17(2), 230–239.
Korczyn, A. D. (2001). Dementia in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol 248(Suppl. 3), 1–4.
242 ROBOTTOM AND WEINER
Laakso, M. P., Partanen, K., Riekkinen, P., Lehtovirta, M., Helkala, E. L., Hallikainen, M.,
Hanninen, T., Vainio, P., and Soininen, H. (1996). Hippocampal volumes in Alzheimer’s disease,
Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia, and in vascular dementia: An MRI study. Neurology
46(3), 678–681.
Lees, A. J., and Smith, E. (1983). Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson’s disease. Brain 106,
257–270.
Leroi, I., Brandt, J., Reich, S. G., Lyketsos, C. G., Grill, S., Thompson, R., and Marsh, L. (2004).
Randomized placebo-controlled trial of donepezil in cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease.
Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 19(1), 1–8.
Levin, B. E. (1990). Spatial cognition in Parkinson’s disease. Alzheimers Dis. Assoc. Disord. 4, 161–170.
Levin, B. E., and Katzen, H. L. (2005). Early cognitive changes and nondementing behavioral
abnormalities in Parkinson’s disease. In ‘‘Advances in Neurology: Behavioral Neurology of
Movement Disorders’’ (K. E. Anderson, W. J. Weiner, and A. E. Lang, Eds.), Vol. 96,
pp. 84–94. Lippincott, Williams, and Wilkins, Baltimore.
Levin, B. E., Llabre, M. M., and Weiner, W. J. (1989). Cognitive impairment associated with early
Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 39, 557–561.
Levy, G., Tang, M. X., Cote, L. J., Louis, E. D., Alfaro, B., Mejia, H., Stern, Y., and Marder, K. (2000).
Motor impairment in PD: Relationship to incident dementia and age. Neurology 55(4), 539–544.
Levy, G., Jacobs, D. M., Tang, M. X., Cote, L. J., Louis, E. D., Alfaro, B., Mejia, H., Stern, Y., and
Marder, K. (2002a). Memory and executive function impairment predict dementia in Parkinson’s
disease. Mov. Disord. 17(6), 1221–1226.
Levy, G., Tang, M. X., Cote, L. J., Louis, E. D., Alfaro, B., Mejia, H., Stern, Y., and Marder, K.
(2002b). Do risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease predict dementia in Parkinson’s disease? An
exploratory study. Mov. Disord. 17(2), 250–257.
Levy, G., Schupf, N., Tang, M. X., Cote, L. J., Louis, E. D., Mejia, H., Stern, Y., and Marder, K.
(2002c). Combined eVect of age and severity on the risk of dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Ann.
Neurol. 51(6), 722–729.
Lewis, F. M., Lapointe, L. L., Murdoch, B. E., and Chenery, H. J. (1998). Language impairment in
Parkinson’s disease. Aphasiology 12(3), 193–206.
Litvan, I., Mohr, E., Williams, J., Gomez, C., and Chase, T. N. (1991). DiVerential memory and
executive functions in demented patients with Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol.
Neurosurg. Psychiatry 54(1), 25–29.
Marder, K., Flood, P., Cote, L., and Mayeux, R. (1990). A pilot study of risk factors for dementia in
Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 5(2), 156–161.
Marder, K., Tang, M. X., Alfaro, B., Mejia, H., Cote, L., Louis, E., Stern, Y., and Mayeux, R. (1996).
The frequency and associated risk factors for dementia in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Arch.
Neurol. 52, 695–701.
Marder, K., Tang, M. X., Cote, L., Stern, Y., and Mayeux, R. (1999). Risk of Alzheimer’s disease in
relatives of Parkinson’s disease patients with and without dementia. Neurology 52(4), 719–724.
Menza, M. A., Robertson, H. D., and Bonapace, A. S. (1993). Parkinson’s disease and anxiety:
Comorbidity with depression. Biol. Psychiatry 34, 465–470.
Miyasaki, J. M., Shannon, K., Voon, V., Ravina, B., Kleiner-Fisman, G., Anderson, K.,
Shulman, L. M., Gronseth, G., and Weiner, W. J. (2006). Practice parameter: Evaluation and
treatment of depression, psychosis, and dementia in Parkinson disease (an evidence-based review).
Neurology 66, 996–1002.
Morens, D. M., Grandinetti, A., Reed, D., White, L. R., and Ross, G. W. (1995). Cigarette smoking
and protection from Parkinson’s disease: False association or etiologic clue. Neurology 45(6),
1041–1051.
DEMENTIA IN PARKINSON’S DISEASE 243
Mosimann, U. P., Mather, G., Wesnes, K. A., O’Brien, J. T., Burn, D. J., and McKeith, I. G. (2004).
Visual perception in Parkinson disease dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology 63,
2091–2096.
Mosimann, U. P., Rowan, E. N., Partington, C. E., Collerton, D., Littlewood, E., O’Brien, J. T.,
Burn, D. J., and McKeith, I. G. (2006). Characteristics of visual hallucinations in Parkinson
disease dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 14, 153–160.
Noe, E., Marder, K., Bell, K. L., Jacobs, D. M., Manly, J. J., and Stern, Y. (2004). Comparison
of dementia with Lewy bodies to Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease with dementia.
Mov. Disord. 19, 60–67.
Ondo, W. G., Tintner, R., Voung, K. D., Lai, D., and Ringholz, G. (2005). Double-blind, placebo-
controlled, unforced titration parallel trial of quetiapine for dopaminergic-induced hallucinations
in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 20(8), 958–963.
Owen, A. M., James, M., and Leigh, P. N. (1992). Fronto-striatal cognitive deficits at diVerent stages of
Parkinson’s disease. Brain 115, 1727–1751.
Owen, A. M., Roberts, A. C., Hodges, J. R., Summers, B. A., Polkey, C. E., and Robbins, T. W. (1993).
Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage
or Parkinson’s disease. Brain 116, 1159–1175.
Paolo, A. M., Axelrod, B. M., Troster, A. I., Blackwell, K. T., and Koller, W. C. (1996). Utility of a
Wisconsin card sorting test short form in persons with Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. J. Clin.
Exp. Neuropsychol. 18, 892–897.
Parkinson, J. (1817). ‘‘An Essay on the Shaking Palsy.’’ Sherwood, Neely and Jones, London.
Paulus, W., and Jellinger, K. (1991). The neuropathologic basis of diVerent clinical subgroups of
Parkinson’s disease. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 50(6), 743–755.
Peppard, R. F., Martin, W. R., Carr, G. D., Grochewski, E., Schulzer, M., Guttman, M.,
McGeer, P. L., Phillips, A. G., Tsui, J. K., and Calne, D. B. (1992). Cerebral glucose metabolism
in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia. Arch. Neurol. 49(12), 1262–1268.
Pillon, B., Dubois, B., Cusimano, G., Bonnet, A. M., Lhermitte, F., and Agid, Y. (1989). Does cognitive
impairment in Parkinson’s disease result from nondopaminergic lesions. J. Neurol. Neurosurg.
Psychiatry 52(2), 201–206.
Pillon, B., Deweer, B., Agid, Y., and Dubois, B. (1993). Explicit memory in Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s,
and Parkinson’s diseases. Arch. Neurol. 50(4), 374–379.
Pillon, B., Dubois, B., and Agid, Y. (1996). Testing cognition may contribute to the diagnosis of
movement disorders. Neurology 46(2), 329–334.
Pollanen, M. S., Dickson, D. W., and Bergeron, C. (1993). Pathology and biology of the Lewy body.
J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 52, 183–191.
Postle, B. R., Jonides, J., and Smith, E. E. (1997). Spatial, but no object, delayed response is impaired in
early Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology 11(2), 171–179.
Rabey, J. M., Prokhorov, T., Miniovitz, A., Dobronevsky, E., and Klein, C. (2007). EVect of quetiapine
in psychotic Parkinson’s disease patients: A double-blind labeled study of 3 months duration. Mov.
Disord. 22(3), 313–318.
Ravina, B., Putt, M., Siderowf, A., Farrar, J. T., Gillespie, M., Crawley, A., Fernandez, H. H.,
Trieschmann, M. M., Reichwein, S., and Simuni, T. (2005). Donepezil for dementia in Parkin-
son’s disease: A randomised, double blind, placebo controlled, crossover study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg.
Psychiatry 76, 934–939.
Reading, P. J., Luce, A. K., and McKeith, I. G. (2001). Rivastigmine in the treatment of parkinsonian
psychosis and cognitive impairment: Preliminary findings from an open trial. Mov. Disord. 16(6),
1171–1174.
Rippon, G. A., and Marder, K. S. (2005). Dementia in Parkinson’s disease. In ‘‘Advances in Neurology:
Behavioral Neurology of Movement Disorders’’ (K. E. Anderson, W. J. Weiner, and A. E. Lang,
Eds.), Vol. 96, pp. 95–113. Lippincott, Williams, and Wilkins, Baltimore.
244 ROBOTTOM AND WEINER
Seppi, K., and Schocke, M. (2005). An update on conventional and advanced magnetic resonance
imaging techniques in the diVerential diagnosis of neurodegenerative parkinsonism. Curr. Opin.
Neurol. 18(4), 370–375.
Sharpe, M. H. (1990). Distractibility in early Parkinson’s disease. Cortex 26(2), 239–246.
Starkstein, S. E., Sabe, L., Petracca, G., Chemerinski, E., Kuzis, G., Merello, M., and Leiguarda, R.
(1996). Neuropsychological and psychiatric diVerences between Alzheimer’s disease and Parkin-
son’s disease with dementia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 61, 381–387.
Stern, Y., Richards, M., Sano, M., and Mayeux, R. (1993). Comparison of cognitive changes in
patients with Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. Arch. Neurol. 50(10), 1040–1045.
Summerfield, C., Gomez-Anson, B., Tolosa, E., Mercador, J. M., Marti, M. J., Pastor, P., and
Junque, C. (2002). Dementia in Parkinson disease: A proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy
study. Arch. Neurol. 59(9), 1415–1420.
Tachibana, H., Kawabata, K., Tomino, Y., Sugita, M., and Fukuchi, M. (1993). Brain perfusion
imaging in Parkinson’s disease and Alzheimer’s disease demonstrated by three-dimensional
surface display with 123I-iodoamphetamine. Dementia 4(6), 334–341.
Taylor, A. E., Saint-Cyr, J. A., and Lang, A. E. (1990). Memory and learning in early Parkinson’s
disease. Brain Cogn. 2, 211–232.
Vander Borght, T., Minoshima, S., Giordani, B., Foster, N. L., Frey, K. A., Berent, S., Albin, R. L.,
Koeppe, R. A., and Kuhl, D. E. (1997). Cerebral metabolic diVerences in Parkinson’s and
Alzheimer’s diseases matched for dementia severity. J. Nucl. Med. 38(5), 797–802.
Vermersch, P., Delacourte, A., Javoy-Agid, F., Hauw, J. J., and Agid, Y. (1992). Mapping of neurofi-
brillary degeneration in Alzheimer’s disease: Evaluation of heterogeneity using the quantification
of abnormal Tau proteins. Acta Neuropathol. 85(1), 48–54.
Vermersch, P., Frigard, B., and Delacourte, A. (1993). Dementia in Parkinson’s disease: Biochemical
evidence for cortical involvement using the immunodetection of abnormal Tau proteins. Ann.
Neurol. 33(5), 445–450.
Whitehouse, P. J., Hedreen, J. C., White, C. L., and Price, D. L. (1983). Basal forebrain neurons in the
dementia of Parkinson disease. Ann. Neurol. 13(3), 243–248.
Zgaljardic, D. J., Foldi, N. S., and Borod, J. C. (2004). Cognitive and behavioral dysfunction
in Parkinson’s disease: Neurochemical and clinicopathological contributions. J. Neural. Transm.
111, 1287–1301.
Zimmerman, P., Sprengelmeyer, R., Fimm, B., and Wallesch, C. W. (1992). Cognitive slowing in
decision tasks in early and advanced Parkinson’s disease. Brain Cogn. 18, 60–69.
Zweig, R. M., Cardillo, J. E., Cohen, M., Giere, S., and Hedreen, J.C (1993). The locus ceruleus and
dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 43(5), 986–991.
EARLY ONSET DEMENTIA
I. Introduction
II. Diagnostic Approach
III. DiVerential Diagnosis
A. Vascular Diseases
B. Infectious Diseases
C. Toxic-Metabolic Disorders
D. Immune-Mediated Disorders
E. Neoplastic/Metastatic Disorders
F. Neurodegenerative Disorders
G. Miscellaneous Causes of EOD
References
the reader, the various etiologies are grouped under the general categories of
vascular, infectious, toxic-metabolic, immune-mediated, neoplastic/metastatic,
and neurodegenerative.
I. Introduction
The EOD diagnostic process consists of the following steps: obtaining the
patient’s history including interviewing a caregiver or a family member, and
performance of a physical/neurological examination, formal cognitive assess-
ment, laboratory testing, and neuroimaging. Important historical information
includes the patient’s age at onset of cognitive impairment; other associated
neurological or systemic symptoms; the presence of co-morbidities (i.e., hyperten-
sion, diabetes mellitus, hyperlipidemia); dementia risk factors such as repetitive
head trauma, alcohol abuse, or a family history of dementia; and the course of
illness pattern (i.e., gradual in Alzheimer’s disease versus stepwise in vascular
EARLY ONSET DEMENTIA 247
TABLE I
NEUROLOGICAL SIGNS OR SYNDROMES AND CORRESPONDING POTENTIAL DIAGNOSES
TABLE II
NON-NEUROLOGICAL SIGNS OR SYNDROMES AND CORRESPONDING POTENTIAL DIAGNOSES
A. VASCULAR DISEASES
TABLE III
DIFFERENTIAL DIAGNOSIS OF EARLY ONSET DEMENTIA
patient with multiple vascular risk factors such as hypertension and diabetes
mellitus. It may also be observed in association with cocaine abuse. Subcortical
microvascular leukoencephalopathy (also known as Binswanger’s syndrome) was
initially described by Binswanger (1894), and is clinically characterized by an
insidiously progressive dementia in association with persistent hypertension.
Brain imaging usually reveals multifocal and confluent lesions aVecting the
periventricular regions and/or deep white matter ( Jellinger, 2007).
The most common hereditary cerebral angiopathy is cerebral autosomal
dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy
252 FADIL et al.
B. INFECTIOUS DISEASES
1. HIV-Associated Dementia
HIV-associated dementia (HAD) is the most devastating neurological complica-
tion of HIV infection consisting of a constellation of progressively disabling symp-
toms involving cognitive faculties, motor function, and behavior (Minagar et al.,
2008). AIDS patients with less serious degrees of cognitive and motor impairments
not meeting the diagnostic criteria for dementia are diagnosed as HIV-associated
cognitive/motor disorder (MCMD). The classical neuropsychological deficits in
patients with HAD indicate dysfunction of frontal and subcortical circuits. HAD,
as a predominantly subcortical dementia, presents with memory loss (mainly for
retrieval of recorded information), psychomotor slowing, depression, and withdrawal
from ADLs. Price and Brew (1988) developed the Memorial Sloan Kettering scale
for HAD which classifies these patients as normal, mild, moderate, severe, and end
stage (Table IV). Neuropathological findings in HAD consist of frontotemporal
atrophy, and the presence of multiple small nodules (microglial nodules) containing
macrophages, lymphocytes, and microglia scattered throughout the cerebral white
matter, and the subcortical gray matter of the thalamus, basal ganglia, and brain-
stem. Brain imaging, particularly MRI with gadolinium, is necessary to exclude
other causes of dementia which may imitate the HAD clinical picture. These other
disorders in AIDS patients include toxoplasmosis, progressive multifocal leukoence-
phalopathy, cryptococcal infection, cytomegalovirus encephalitis, and CNS lympho-
ma. Brain MRI in patients with HAD reveals diVuse, non-enhancing, ill-defined
areas of hyperintense signal in deep white matter along with cerebral atrophy,
ventricular enlargement, and caudate region atrophy. Proton magnetic resonance
spectroscopy (MRS) in HAD patients demonstrates increased myoinositol (indicat-
ing gliosis) and choline (indicating demeyelination) levels in gray and white matter
along with decrease in levels of N-acetyl aspartate in gray matter (indicating
neuronal/axonal loss). HAART treatment has been reported to have decreased
the present incidence of HIV dementia (Sacktor et al., 2003). However, HAD
EARLY ONSET DEMENTIA 253
TABLE IV
CLINICAL STAGING OF THE AIDS DEMENTIA COMPLEX
Stage 0 (normal)
Normal mental and motor function
Stage 0.5 (equivocal and subclinical)
Absent, minimal, or equivocal symptoms without impairment of work or capacity to perform ADLs.
Mild signs (snout response, slowed ocular, or extremity movements) may be present. Gait and
strength are normal
Stage 1 (mild)
Able to perform all but the more demanding aspects of work or ADLs, but with unequivocal evidence
(signs or symptoms that may include performance on neuropsychological testing) of functional
intellectual or motor impairment. Can walk without assistance
Stage 2 (moderate)
Able to perform basic activities of self-care, but cannot work or maintain the more demanding aspects
of daily life. Ambulatory, but may require a single prop
Stage 3 (severe)
Major intellectual incapacity (cannot follow news or personal events, cannot sustain complex
conversation, considerable slowing of all outputs) and motor disability (cannot walk unassisted,
requires walker, or personal support, usually with slowing and clumsiness of arms as well
Stage 4 (end stage)
Nearly vegetative. Intellectual and social comprehension and output are at a rudimentary level. Nearly
or absolutely mute. Paraparetic or paraplegic with urinary and fecal incontinence
Developed by Price and Brew at Memorial Sloan Kettering (Price and Brow 1988)
prevalence has actually risen due to the increasing number of infected subjects as a
result of increased life expectancy.
2. Syphilitic Dementia
Syphilis is caused by Treponema pallidum, and worldwide is still a common
disease. Certain factors such as poverty, illiteracy, and prostitution contribute to
the prevalence of this infection with its tertiary stage dementia. Neurological
manifestations of syphilis include meningoencephalitis and chronic vasculitis
which can both cause stroke, tabes dorsalis, myelopathy, and dementia. Syphilitic
dementia (which, since the advent of penicillin, is now a rare complication)
occurs 20 years following the primary infection and presents with memory loss,
apathy, seizures, dysarthria, and cortical deficits such as aphasia and apraxia.
Examination of the serum and CSF demonstrates positive serology, and the CSF
examination shows pleocytosis, elevated protein, and an increased IgG index.
followed by myoclonus, ataxia, motor deficits, and spasticity. In its final stage,
autonomic abnormalities and coma develop. The EEG is pathognomonic with
periodic high-amplitude polyspike and sharp- and slow-wave complexes lasting
0.5–2 s. Neuropathological examination demonstrates patchy areas of demyelin-
ation and gliosis as well as the presence of Cowdry types A and B eosinophilic
inclusions. There is no specific treatment for SSPE and preventive vaccination
against measles virus remains the only way to avoid this complication.
4. Prion Diseases
The transmissible spongiform encephalopathies are an uncommon group of
neurodegenerative diseases causing rapid onset dementia. This group of disorders
is linked to abnormal metabolism of the prion protein. Prion diseases aVect both
human and animal hosts and remain invariably fatal. The transmissible agent is
an abnormal isoform of the prion protein (PrP) which is designated as the protease
resistant scrapie form (PrPsc). The gene for the human PrP is located on the short
arm of chromosome 20. Human prion diseases include Kuru, Creutzfeldt-Jakob
disease (CJD), variant Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease (vCJD), Gerstmann-Strussler-
Scheinker disease, and fatal familial insomnia. CJD which occurs worldwide
can be acquired (iatrogenic), familial, or sporadic (sCJD). Acquired cases linked
to iatrogenic transmission have been reported following corneal and dural graft
transplantation. Variant CJD has been linked to bovine spongiform encephalop-
athy. sCJD which accounts for up to 85% of all cases of prion diseases is primarily
a disease of older adults but can also aVect young people. Patients with sCJD
present with the triad of rapidly progressive dementia, myoclonus, and ataxia.
Cognitive decline in these patients is manifested by poor concentration, diYculty
with mental calculations, impaired abstract thought, aphasia, and apraxia, and
can be associated with abnormal involuntary movements such as choreoathetosis
(Eggenberger, 2007). Diagnosis of sCJD depends on the presence of rapidly
progressive dementia associated with at least two of the following four findings:
Myoclonus, visual manifestations or cerebellar signs, pyramidal or extrapyrami-
dal signs, and akinetic mutism. The presence of bilaterally synchronous, periodic
sharp waves or spikes against a slow background on the EEG, and elevated CSF
levels of the 14-3-3 protein support the clinical diagnosis. Brain MRI of patients
with sCJD may reveal the presence of bilateral hyperintense signals in the basal
ganglia, corpus striatum, and thalamus, which are best observed on diVusion
weighted images. In contrast to sCJD, patients with vCJD present primarily
with psychiatric symptoms, and later in the course of disease develop
dementia, ataxia, and involuntary movements. The T2-weighted MRI reveals
abnormal hyperintense signals in the posterior thalamus (pulvinar sign) (Zeidler
et al., 2000).
EARLY ONSET DEMENTIA 255
5. Lyme Disease
Lyme disease is a systemic infectious disease caused by the spirochete, Borrelia
burgdorferi, which is transmitted to the human host by the bite of the Ixodes tick.
It predominantly aVects the nervous system, skin, heart, and joints. The infection
develops within a period of 3–32 days after tick bite with a characteristic acute
rash, erythema chronicum migrans. About 15% of patients with Lyme disease
develop neurological manifestations, which consist of lymphocytic meningitis,
cranial neuritis, and radiculitis. The cognitive dysfunction in patients with Lyme
disease is characterized by impaired executive function and attention. However, a
clear dementia due to Lyme disease is infrequent. Depression, emotional lability,
irritability and psychosis may also be present. CSF examination shows a pleocy-
tosis with elevated protein levels and the presence of oligoclonal bands and
Borrelia DNA; at times, T2-weighted and FLAIR MRI show increased signal in
the white matter.
6. Whipple’s Disease
Whipple’s disease (WD) is a rare systemic infection caused by the bacterium
Tropheryma whipplei. WD, which aVects particularly middle-aged males, primarily
involves the digestive system and most often manifests with weight loss, diarrhea
associated with malabsorption, abdominal pain, lymphadenopathy, cardiomyop-
athy, hyperpigmentation, and hypotension. WD is responsible for primary neu-
rological disorders in rare cases. The most common neurological manifestations
include slowly progressive dementia (56%), supranuclear ophthalmoplegia, ocu-
lomasticatory myoryhthmia, oculofacioskeletal myorythmia, and myoclonus. The
presence of periodic acid-SchiV (PAS) positive macrophages in biopsy specimens
( jejunal) and the demonstration of ‘‘Whipple’s bacilli’’ by electron microscopy are
diagnostic of active WD. CSF polymerase chain reaction (PCR) analysis for
detection of T. whipplei DNA is a very sensitive diagnostic technique, and the
CSF protein may occasionally be elevated. Brain CT and MRI are often normal
but may show cortical/subcortical atrophy, hydrocephalus, and mass lesions.
All WD patients must be treated and monitored as if they have CNS involvement
if CSF PCR results are positive, even if they are neurologically asymptomatic.
7. Neurocysticercosis
Cysticercosis is a zoonotic infectious disease caused by the parasitic tape worm
Taenia solium which infests both pigs and humans. Cycticercosis is the most
frequent parasitic infection of the human nervous system (neurocycticercosis).
Neurocysticercosis (NCC) may be clinically asymptomatic or present with epilep-
sy, altered mental status, focal neurologic deficits, headache, hydrocephalus, and
dementia (López et al., 2008). Although the cognitive deficits in NCC have been
recognized for a long time, they have been systematically evaluated only recently.
256 FADIL et al.
C. TOXIC-METABOLIC DISORDERS
D. IMMUNE-MEDIATED DISORDERS
1. Multiple Sclerosis
This group of dementing disorders includes multiple sclerosis (MS), CNS
vasculitis, and Hashimoto’s encephalopathy. MS is an immune-mediated neuro-
degenerative disorder of the human CNS which mainly aVects young adults
(Frohman et al., 2006). The immunopathogenesis of MS is associated with activa-
tion of both the cellular and humoral arms of the immune system against specific
CNS antigens (such as the myelin basic protein family), disruption of the blood-
brain barrier, transendothelial migration of activated leukocytes into the CNS,
and loss of the myelin/oligodendrocyte complex as well as neuronal/axonal
degeneration. Up to 65% of patients with MS present with neuropsychiatric
features and suVer from cognitive decline. In many patients with MS, cognitive
decline and memory loss may be the predominant manifestations causing signifi-
cant disability. Usually dementia occurs late in the course of MS; however, certain
EARLY ONSET DEMENTIA 257
3. Hashimoto’s Encephalopathy
Hashimoto’s encephalopathy (HE) refers to a presumably immune-mediated
encephalopathy which is associated with elevated serum titers of antiperoxidase
and antithyroglobulin antibodies. HE has been reported around the globe
aVecting all age groups, with females being more aVected. This uncommon
disorder manifests with fluctuating consciousness followed by cognitive decline
and dementia. HE may have a relapsing course, and almost all cases respond
favorably to high doses of corticosteroids (Mocellin et al., 2007).
E. NEOPLASTIC/METASTATIC DISORDERS
Both primary and metastatic brain neoplasms may cause dementia, with
location and the rate of tumor growth being the major severity determinants.
Another etiology for development of cognitive impairment and dementia in the
context of systemic malignancy involves a paraneoplastic mechanism. Paraneo-
plastic limbic encephalitis is the most common paraneoplastic syndrome and
occurs in association with lung and breast cancer. Limbic encephalitis is
258 FADIL et al.
F. NEURODEGENERATIVE DISORDERS
also known as frontal variant FTD (fv-FTD), semantic dementia (SD) (or temporal
variant), and progressive non-fluent aphasia (PNFA). In addition to these three
clinical forms, FTLD has been linked to CBD, progressive supranuclear palsy
(PSP), motor neuron disease (amyotrophic lateral sclerosis), and apraxia of
speech. FTLD typically aVects individuals between 45 and 65 years of age; in
up to 40% of cases, a positive family history may be identified. fv-FTD which is
more common in males comprises up to 56% of FTLD cases and has the earliest
age of onset (mean ¼ 58 years). Patients with fv-FTD demonstrate behavioral
abnormalities consisting of distractibility, decline in self-care, perseverant behavior,
hyperorality, apathy, disinhibition, and language disturbance. Patients with SD
develop a progressive loss of word knowledge and meaning. While speech fluency
is retained, patients develop problems with word finding and have diYculty with
naming and word recognition. These patients make speech errors which are
typically characterized by circumlocution and overuse of generic words. In
advanced cases, patients with SD may develop prosopagnosia. In patients with
PNFA, the disease process commences insidiously with progressive deficits in
speech or language. These patients have decreased word output and commonly
exhibit diYculties with understanding grammar. Patients develop anomia but
have relatively preserved memory and nonverbal cognition. Some of these
patients may demonstrate upper-limb or orofacial apraxia. Word comprehension
usually remains unchanged. Neuroimaging demonstrates left perisylvian atrophy.
3. Corticobasal Degeneration
CBD (also known as cortical-basal ganglionic degeneration) is a neurodegen-
erative disorder classified as a tauopathy, which presents with asymmetrical motor
dysfunction having both parkinsonian and dystonic components, as well as
postural tremor/myoclonus. Cortical features include aphasia, ideational and
ideomotor apraxia, astereognosis and alien limb syndrome. Patients with CBD
also display intellectual decline as well as frontal lobe release signs. Mean age of
onset is 60 years, but onset may occur as early as 45 years. Neuropathological
examination of the brain reveals intracellular filament accumulation consisting of
microtubule-associated tau protein. PET and MRI studies have demonstrated
symmetrical cortical and basal ganglion hypometabolism (McMonagle et al.,
2006). As a tauopathy, CBD has been linked to FTD and Pick’s disease; however,
its tau is hyperphosphorylated and makes unusual twisted filaments which share
certain similarities with the paired helical filaments observed in Alzheimer’s
disease. The etiology and pathogenesis of CBD remain unknown and currently
there is no specific treatment for this condition.
recessive genes have been linked to PD. IPD remains a clinical diagnosis, and its
major clinical features include bradykinesia, resting tremor, limb rigidity, loss of
corrective postural reflexes, and a gait disorder. Up to one-third of patients with IPD
develop dementia which is characterized by significant impairment of executive
functions, attention, concentration, working memory, and visuospatial function.
Personality changes and behavioral disorders are common.
References
Binswanger, O. (1894). Die Abregrenzung der allgemeinen progressiven paralyse. Berl Klin. Wochenscher
31, 1102–1105, 1137–1139.
Bowler, J. V. (2005). Vascular cognitive impairment. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 76(Suppl. 5), 35–44.
Cooper, S., and Greene, J. D. (2005). The clinical assessment of the patient with early dementia.
J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 76(Suppl. 5), v15–v24.
Darvesh, S., Leach, L., Black, S. E., Kaplan, E., and Freedman, M. (2005). The behavioural neurology
assessment. Can. J. Neurol. Sci. 32, 167–177.
262 FADIL et al.
Glen R. Finney
Memory and Cognitive Disorders Program, University of Florida
Department of Neurology, Gainesville, Florida 32610-0236, USA
I. History
A. History of Present Illness
B. Past Medical History
C. Medications
II. Physical Examination
III. Laboratory Serologies
IV. Neuroimaging
V. Modern Diagnostic Criteria
A. Probable Idiopathic Normal Pressure Hydrocephalus
B. Possible Idiopathic Normal Pressure Hydrocephalus
C. Unlikely Idiopathic Normal Pressure Hydrocephalus
VI. Treatment
VII. Predictors of Response to Shunting in Normal Pressure Hydrocephalus
VIII. Recent Work
References
1
Also known as: Hakim-Adams Syndrome, Hakim’s Disease, Hakim’s syndrome, extraventricular
obstructive hydrocephalus.
I. History
C. MEDICATIONS
and can raise the concern of the possibility of intracranial venous sinus thrombosis.
Beyond the optic nerve, any other cranial nerve abnormality brings the diagnosis of
an idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus into question as this can be an
additional sign of significant raised intracranial pressures as in the false localizing
signs of sixth nerve palsies, or sign of structural abnormalities in the posterior fossa
such as in basilar meningitis, which might be an underlying etiology for a secondary
normal pressure hydrocephalus. Ophthalmoplegia can indicate any number of
other etiologies that may share some features with normal pressure hydrocephalus,
including Wernicke-KorsakoV Syndrome, central nervous system Whipple’s
disease, and especially Progressive Supranuclear Palsy (particularly if there is
restriction of down-gaze) which presents early with gait instability and may have
cognitive impairment quite similar to that seen in normal pressure hydrocephalus.
Motor examination should demonstrate normal motor strength with no focal
deficits. Focal weakness might point one away from normal pressure hydroceph-
alus and toward a more focal or multifocal etiologies like vascular damage. Tone
may be normal or increased in normal pressure hydrocephalus, though asymmetric
increased tone should bring up the possibility of Parkinson’s disease, especially if
accompanied by an asymmetric resting tremor.
Reflexes should be normal to mildly increased, again in a nonfocal pattern.
Focal asymmetries in reflexes suggest the possibility of a diVerent etiology. One
may see frontal release signs (i.e., release of primitive developmental reflexes) such
as a glabellar reflex, a rooting or suck reflex, a grasp reflex, facillatory paratonia
(Mitgehen), and even bilateral Babinski signs can be found in some patients.
Sensory examination can be diYcult to reliably obtain in this patient population,
and many patients in the age group most susceptible to normal pressure hydroceph-
alus also suVer from a multifactorial vibratory sensory loss. Again, we cannot
emphasize enough the importance of asymmetric sensory loss as indicating the
possibility of a diVerent or additional diagnosis than normal pressure hydrocephalus.
There should be no Romberg sign (falling to one side when eyes are closed), otherwise
a unilateral vestibular system abnormality may be contributing to gait disturbance.
Coordination examination may show truncal ataxia and abnormalities of fine
motor activity that resemble ataxia or apraxia, particularly in the lower extremities.
Frank tremor on examination can be seen but it is typically symmetric.
Gait examination, of course, is the most important portion of the examination
in the case of normal pressure hydrocephalus. The most classic finding in idiopathic
normal pressure hydrocephalus is a magnetic apraxia of gait, where the patient has
diYculty with initiation and changes in trajectory of gait, and appears to be literally
‘‘stuck’’ to the floor. The degree of severity of magnetic apraxia can vary, with some
patients only having subtle findings particularly on turns or transfers, whereas
others may be so severe that they cannot even obtain the upright position. Unfor-
tunately, it is not always the classic magnetic apraxia that is the form of gait
disturbance that is manifest in the patient. A significant number of patients can
268 GLEN R. FINNEY
have a mixed magnetic/parkinsonian gait and in some individuals the gait can
appear quite parkinsonian with no clear apraxia. Other forms of gait disturbances
must be assessed for and ruled out. While gross testing of proprioception may be
normal, many elderly patients even without normal pressure hydrocephalus or
overt neurological impairment will have an unsteady gait due to diminution of
multiple sensory modalities (e.g., vision, vestibular, vibratory) as well as general
deconditioning of accessory musculature with decreased activity often seen in the
elderly, particularly seen in deficits in righting reflex when patients get slightly oV
balance. A wide-based gait in frank loss of lower extremity sensation either due to a
peripheral neuropathy or myelopathy impacting posterior columns also should
raise doubts about the diagnosis of normal pressure hydrocephalus.
There is no specific blood test for normal pressure hydrocephalus and any
laboratory serology work-up should be driven by history or examination findings
suggestive of other possible etiologies. It is beyond the scope of this chapter to go
into the laboratory work-up for these alternative etiologies in the diVerential
diagnosis and direct the reader to the chapters on other forms of dementia,
particularly reversible dementias, for further information.
IV. Neuroimaging
Relkin et al. (2005) criteria for probable and possible normal pressure
hydrocephalus
2. Brain imaging
Brain imaging study (CT or MRI) must be after onset and must show
evidence of:
! Ventricular enlargement out of proportion to cerebral atrophy or not
entirely attributable to congenital enlargement (Evans index >0.3 or
comparable measure)
! No macroscopic obstruction of cerebrospinal fluid flow
! At least one of the following supportive features:
– Enlargement of the temporal horns of the lateral ventricles not
attributable to hippocampal atrophy
– Callosal angle of 40" or more
– Evidence of altered brain water content, including periventricular
signal changes on CT and MRI not attributable to microvascular
ischemic changes or demyelination
– Aqueductal or fourth ventricular flow void on T2 MRI
270 GLEN R. FINNEY
3. Clinical
By classic definition:
Findings of gait or balance disturbance must be present plus impairment in
! Cognition
! Urinary incontinence
! Or both
With respect to gait and balance, at least two of the following should be
present and not entirely attributable to other conditions:
! Decreased step height
! Decreases step length
! Decreased cadence
! Increased trunk sway
! Widened standing base
! Toes turned outward on walking
! Retropulsion (spontaneous or provoked)
! En bloc turning (turning requires three or more steps for 180" )
With respect to cognition, there must be documented impairment (adjusted for
age and education) or decrease in performance on a cognitive screening instrument
(such as the Mini-Mental State Examination) or evidence of at least two of the
following on examination that is not fully attributable to other conditions:
! Psychomotor slowing
! Decreased fine motor speed
! DiYculty dividing or maintaining attention
! Impaired recall, especially for recent events
! Executive dysfunction, such as impairment in multistep procedures,
working memory, formulation of abstract/similarities, insight
! Behavioral or personality change
NORMAL PRESSURE HYDROCEPHALUS 271
1. History
Reported symptoms may
! Have a subacute or indeterminate mode of onset
! Begin at any age after childhood
! May have less than 3 months of indeterminate duration
! May follow events, such as mild head trauma, remote history of intrace-
rebral hemorrhage, or childhood and adolescent meningitis, or other
conditions that in the judgment of the clinician are not likely to be
casually related
! Coexist with other neurological, psychiatric, or general medical disor-
ders but in the judgment of a clinician not be entirely attributed to these
conditions
! Be nonprogressive or not clearly progressive
2. Brain imaging
Ventricular enlargement consistent with hydrocephalus but associated with
either of the following (Figs. 1 and 2):
! Evidence of cerebral atrophy of suYcient severity to potentially explain
ventricular size
! Structural lesions that may influence ventricular size
272 GLEN R. FINNEY
3. Clinical
Symptoms of either
! Incontinence or cognitive impairment in the absence of an observable
gait or balance disturbance
! Gait disturbance or dementia alone
4. Physiologic
Opening pressure not available or pressure outside the range of required for
probable idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus.
! No evidence of ventriculomegaly
! Signs of increased intracranial pressure, such as papilledema
! No component of the clinical triad of idiopathic normal pressure
hydrocephalus
! Symptoms explained by other causes (e.g., spinal stenosis)
NORMAL PRESSURE HYDROCEPHALUS 273
Demographics:
Estimates of prevalence in small studies hover around 0.5% (Casmiro et al.,
1989; Trenkwalder et al., 1995).
VI. Treatment
reversible with shunting. In another small study it was discovered that patients
with normal pressure hydrocephalus were more prone to have retrograde jugular
flow during Valsalva maneuver (Kuriyama et al., 2008). Attempts to find better
diagnostic benchmarks for treatment responsiveness in normal pressure hydro-
cephalus continues, with one modest sized study from Norway suggesting a
correlation with treatment response 1 year out from shunting for cerebrospinal
fluid pressure pulsatility after lumbar infusion (Brean et al., 2008), with cerebro-
spinal dynamics after shunting returning to a more normal nature in lumbar
infusion after shunting (Petrella et al., 2008). MRI measurement of CSF spaces at
the high convexities and midline areas of the brain has shown reasonable ability
to distinguish brains with normal pressure hydrocephalus (Sasaki et al., 2008).
Another small study identified the most common changes in gait between lumbar
drainage responders and nonresponders to be walking speed, amount of steps in
turning, and tendency toward falling (Ravdin et al., 2008). One small but sugges-
tive study that suggested the use of MRI cine (phase-contrast MRI) pre- and
postlumbar drainage (at least 50 cc removed) could reliably identify patients who
benefited from shunting from those who did not, with change in the peak flow
velocity through the cerebral aqueduct after lumbar drainage suggesting amena-
bility to shunting (Sharma et al., 2008). In another small study using [(15)O]H(2)O
NORMAL PRESSURE HYDROCEPHALUS 277
References
Adams, R. D. (1966). Further observations on normal pressure hydrocephalus. Proc. R. Soc. Med.
59(11 Part 1), 1135–1140.
Adams, R. D., Fisher, C. M., Hakim, S., Ojemann, R. G., and Sweet, W. H. (1965). Symptomatic
occult hydrocephalus with ‘‘normal’’ cerebrospinal-fluid pressure. A treatable syndrome. N. Engl.
J. Med. 273, 117–126.
Agren-Wilsson, A., Lekman, A., Sjöberg, W., Rosengren, L., Blennow, K., Bergenheim, A. T., and
Malm, J. (2007). CSF biomarkers in the evaluation of idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus.
Acta Neurol. Scand. 116(5), 333–339.
Akiguchi, I., Ishii, M., Watanabe, Y., Watanabe, T., Kawasaki, T., Yagi, H., Shiino, A., Shirakashi, Y.,
and Kawamoto, Y. (2008). Shunt-responsive parkinsonism and reversible white matter lesions in
patients with idiopathic NPH. J. Neurol. 1392–1399.
Bergsneider, M., Black, P. M., Klinge, P., et al. (2005). Surgical management of idiopathic normal
pressure hydrocephalus. Neurosurgery 57(3 Suppl.), S29–S39.
Black, P. M., Ojemann, R. G., and Tzouras, A. (1985). CSF shunts for dementia, incontinence, and
gait disturbance. Clin. Neurosurg. 32, 632–651.
Brean, A., and Eide, P. K. (2008). Assessment of idiopathic normal pressure patients in neurological
practice: The role of lumbar infusion testing for referral of patients to neurosurgery. Eur. J. Neurol.
15(6), 605–612.
Bugalho, P., and Alves, L. (2007). Normal-pressure hydrocephalus: White matter lesions correlate
negatively with gait improvement after lumbar puncture. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 109(9), 774–778.
Bugalho, P., and Guimarães, J. (2007). Gait disturbance in normal pressure hydrocephalus: A clinical
study. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 13(7), 434–437.
Casmiro, M., Benassi, G., Cacciatore, F. M., and D’Alessandro, R. (1989). Frequency of idiopathic
normal pressure hydrocephalus. Arch. Neurol. 46(6), 608.
De Mol, J. (1985). Prognostic factors for therapeutic outcome in normal-pressure hydrocephalus.
Review of the literature and personal study. Acta Neurol. Belg. 85(1), 13–29.
DeVito, E. E., Salmond, C. H., Owler, B. K., Sahakian, B. J., and Pickard, J. D. (2007). Structural
abnormalities in idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Acta Neurol. Scand. 116(5), 328–332.
Fisher, C. M. (1977). The clinical picture in occult hydrocephalus. Clin. Neurosurg. 24, 270–284.
GraV-Radford, N. R. (2007). Normal pressure hydrocephalus. Neurol. Clin. 25, 809–832.
GraV-Radford, N. R., Godersky, J. C., and Jones, M. (1989). Variables predicting surgical outcome in
symptomatic hydrocephalus in the elderly. Neurology 39, 1601–1604.
Greenberg, B. M., and Williams, M. A. (2008). Infectious complications of temporary spinal
catheter insertion for diagnosis of adult hydrocephalus and idiopathic intracranial hypertension.
Neurosurgery 62(2), 431–435.
Hakim, S. (1964). Some Observations on C.S.F. Pressure: Hydrocephalic Syndrome in Adults with
‘‘Normal’’ C.S.F. Pressure. Thesis No. 957 Javeriana University School of Medicine, Bogota,
Colombia, March 10.
Hakim, S. (1973). Hydraulic and mechanical mis-matching of valve shunts used in the treatment of
hydrocephalus: The need for a servo-valve shunt. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 15(5), 646–653.
Hakim, S., and Adams, R. D. (1965). The special clinical problem of symptomatic hydrocephalus with
normal cerebrospinal fluid pressure. Observations on cerebrospinal fluid hydrodynamics.
J. Neurol. Sci. 2(4), 307–327.
Hakim, S., et al. (1973). A critical analysis of valve shunts used in the treatment of hydrocephalus. Dev.
Med. Child Neurol. 15(2), 230–255.
Kilic, K., Czorny, A., Auque, J., and Berkman, Z. (2007). Predicting the outcome of shunt surgery in
normal pressure hydrocephalus. J. Clin. Neurosci. 14(8), 729–736.
280 GLEN R. FINNEY
Klinge, P. M., Brooks, D. J., Samii, A., Weckesser, E., van den HoV, J., Fricke, H., Brinker, T.,
Knapp, W. H., and Berding, G. (2008). Correlates of local cerebral blood flow (CBF) in normal
pressure hydrocephalus patients before and after shunting—A retrospective analysis of [(15)O]H
(2)O PET-CBF studies in 65 patients. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg 110(4), 369–375.
Kubo, Y., Kazui, H., Yoshida, T., Kito, Y., Kimura, N., Tokunaga, H., Ogino, A., Miyake, H.,
Ishikawa, M., and Takeda, M. (2008). Validation of grading scale for evaluating symptoms of
idiopathic normal-pressure hydrocephalus. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 25(1), 37–45.
Kuriyama, N., Tokuda, T., Miyamoto, J., Takayasu, N., Kondo, M., and Nakagawa, M. (2008).
Retrograde jugular flow associated with idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Ann. Neurol.
64, 217–221.
Lenfeldt, N., Hauksson, J., Birgander, R., Eklund, A., and Malm, J. (2008). Improvement after
cerebrospinal fluid drainage is related to levels of N-acetyl-aspartate in idiopathic normal pressure
hydrocephalus. Neurosurgery 62(1), 135–141discussion 141-142.
Mandir, A. S., Hilfiker, J., Thomas, G., Minahan, R. E., Crawford, T. O., Williams, M. A., and
Rigamonti, D. (2007). Extrapyramidal signs in normal pressure hydrocephalus: An objective
assessment. Cerebrospinal. Fluid Res. 4, 7.
Miyamoto, J., Imahori, Y., and Mineura, K. (2007). Cerebral oxygen metabolism in idiopathic-normal
pressure hydrocephalus. Neurol. Res. 29(8), 830–834.
Murakami, M., Hirata, Y., and Kuratsu, J. I. (2007). Predictive assessment of shunt eVectiveness in
patients with idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus by determining regional cerebral blood
flow on 3D stereotactic surface projections. Acta Neurochir. (Wien). 149(10), 991–997.
Petersen, R. C., Mokri, B., and Laws, E. R., Jr. (1985). Surgical treatment of idiopathic hydrocephalus
in elderly patients. Neurology 35(3), 307–311.
Petrella, G., Czosnyka, M., Keong, N., Pickard, J. D., and Czosnyka, Z. (2008). How does CSF
dynamics change after shunting? Acta Neurol. Scand. 118, 182–188.
Pfisterer, W. K., Aboul-Enein, F., Gebhart, E., Graf, M., Aichholzer, M., and Mühlbauer, M. (2007).
Continuous intraventricular pressure monitoring for diagnosis of normal-pressure hydrocephalus.
Acta Neurochir. (Wien). 149(10), 983–990; discussion 990.
Pujari, S., Kharkar, S., Metellus, P., Shuck, J., Williams, M. A., and Rigamonti, D. (2008). Normal
pressure hydrocephalus: Very long term outcome after shunt surgery. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry
79, 1282–1286.
Ravdin, L. D., Katzen, H. L., Jackson, A. E., Tsakanikas, D., Assuras, S., and Relkin, N. R. (2008).
Features of gait most responsive to tap test in normal pressure hydrocephalus. Clin. Neurol.
Neurosurg. 110(5), 455–461.
Relkin, N., Marmarou, A., Klinge, P., Bergsneider, M., and Black, P. M. (2005). Diagnosing idiopathic
normal-pressure hydrocephalus. Neurosurgery 57(3 Suppl.), S4–S16 [discussion: ii-v].
Sahuquillo, J., Arikan, F., Poca, M. A., Noguer, M., and Martinez-Ricarte, F. (2008). Intra-abdominal
pressure: The neglected variable in selecting the ventriculoperitoneal shunt for treating hydro-
cephalus. Neurosurgery 62(1), 143–149; discussion 149-150.
Sakakibara, R., Kanda, T., Sekido, T., Uchiyama, T., Awa, Y., Ito, T., Liu, Z., Yamamoto, T.,
Yamanishi, T., Yuasa, T., Shirai, K., and Hattori, T. (2007). Mechanism of bladder dysfunction
in idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Neurourol Urodyn. 27(6), 507–510.
Sasaki, M., Honda, S., Yuasa, T., Iwamura, A., Shibata, E., and Ohba, H. (2008). Narrow CSF space
at high convexity and high midline areas in idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus detected by
axial and coronal MRI. Neuroradiology 50(2), 117–122.
Sharma, A. K., Gaikwad, S., Gupta, V., Garg, A., and Mishra, N. K. (2008). Measurement of peak
CSF flow velocity at cerebral aqueduct, before and after lumbar CSF drainage, by use of phase-
contrast MRI: Utility in the management of idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Clin.
Neurol. Neurosurg. 110(4), 363–368.
NORMAL PRESSURE HYDROCEPHALUS 281
Trenkwalder, J. C., Schwarz, J., Gebhard, J., Ruland, D., Trenkwalder, P., Hense, H. W., and
Oertel, W. H. (1995). Starnberg trial on epidemiology of Parkinsonism and hypertension in the
elderly. Prevalence of Parkinson’s disease and related disorders assessed by a door-to-door survey
of inhabitants older than 65 years. Arch Neurol. 52(10), 1017–1022.
Zhang, J., Williams, M. A., and Rigamonti, D. (2008). Heritable essential tremor-idiopathic normal
pressure hydrocephalus (ETINPH). Am. J. Med. Genet. A 146A(4), 433–439.
REVERSIBLE DEMENTIAS
I. Introduction
II. Etiologies
III. Nutritional Abnormalities
IV. Endocrine Disorders and Cognition
References
I. Introduction
intravenous drug use increases risk for blood-borne diseases, for example, HIV,
syphilis, and hepatitis. History of immunodeficiency may point to indolent infec-
tions, for example, crytococcal meningitis (CM), though lack of such a history
does not necessarily rule these opportunistic infections out. Remote history of
head trauma may point to a chronic subdural hematoma. Social history is vitally
important. Abuse of recreational substances such as alcohol, tobacco, and other
intoxicants may cause cognitive decline or predispose the patient to several
secondary etiologies. As above, IV drug use may lead to investigation of certain
blood-borne diseases. Suspicion for substance abuse should not be omitted based
on the patient’s appearance. Well-dressed and groomed patients may use sub-
stances covertly. Nutritional habits may reveal unusual or poor diets predisposing
patients to vitamin deficiencies, or more rarely, toxicities. Travel history to areas
both in and outside of the country may reveal exposure to infectious diseases
associated with a particular region to which an illness is known to be endemic,
for example, the northeastern United States for Lyme disease or to Central
America for neurocysticercosis. Family history of heritable medical conditions
(or heritable risk) that might lead to cognitive decline may be prime targets for
exploration of the patient’s own cognitive decline.
Examination: Vital signs, while normal in most cases of indolent reversible
dementias, may reveal an etiology, for example, extraordinarily high resting
blood pressure may underlie a chronic hypertensive encephalopathy. An inspec-
tion of the skin surface can discover signs of infection, neurocutaneous abnorm-
alities, signs of hepatic dysfunction, such as capet medusa, the spidery web of
blood vessels on the surface due to portal hypertension, or evidence of vitamin
deficiency, for example, pellagra. Orientation may be impaired in a myriad
etiologies reflecting alteration of consciousness, most often aVecting orientation
to time and to place. Orientation to self is typically is spared in mild to moderate
forms of altered mental status. Level of consciousness is typically normal in
patients with primary neurodegenerative disorders, though Lewy body dementia
can have a fluctuating level of consciousness. Unlike deliriums which overlap in
diVerential diagnosis with forms of reversible dementias, reversible dementias
tend to have relatively normal to slightly impaired level of consciousness, or at
worst a fluctuating level of consciousness. This is most likely due to the indolent
nature of these perturbations of consciousness which allow for a degree of
compensatory function in arousal systems not seen in acute perturbation as seen
in acute onset delirium. Attention level is often impaired in reversible forms of
dementia and is one of the most common forms of disruption of mental status.
Since higher cortical functions rely on a certain level of attention, a defect in this
portion of the examination can cause disruption in numerous cognitive responses.
Concentration is closely related to attention, but even when working memory and
attention to stimuli may be adequate, concentration requires further executive
function, a role of the frontal lobes, to keep the patient on task and tracking
286 KABASAKALIAN AND FINNEY
PCR. Protein: SPEP, UPEP, albumin, Cyclic AMP (plasma, urine, CSF). Para-
neoplastic (Limbic encephalitis): anti-HU, anti-MA, anti-TA. Autoimmune: ASO
(antistreptolysin antibody), ESR, RF, ANA, lupus anticoagulant assay (tissue
thromboplastin inhibition, anticardiolipin antibody IgG, IgM), Anti-Ro(SS-A),
anti-La (SS-B), antiribosomal ab, anti-Smith ab, anti-RNP, anti-dsDNA, C3
complement, C4 complement, perinuclear antineutrophil cytoplasmic autoanti-
bodies (pANCA), cytoplasmic (cANCA).
II. Etiologies
treatment. The main causes for vitamin B12 deficiency are pernicious anemia and
food-cobalamin malabsorption, a syndrome characterized by the inability to
release cobalamin from food or from its binding proteins, usually resulting from
atrophic gastritis which stresses the urgency for prompt diagnosis and treatment
(Andres et al., 2007). Other risk factors and causes include male gender, age
75 years or more, refraining from milk products (Loikas et al., 2007), blind loop
syndrome, dietary deficiency, gastrectomy, surgical resection of the ileum, defi-
ciency in the exocrine function of the pancreas in chronic gastritis, lymphomas, or
tuberculosis (intestinal), Crohn’s disease, Whipple’s disease, and celiac disease
(Andres et al., 2007). Other neurologic manifestations of cobalamin deficiency
include acroparesthesia (burning and painful sensations in the hands and feet),
sensory ataxia, visual loss (due to optic neuropathy), autonomic dysfunction (e.g.,
sphincter dysfunction, impotence and orthostatic hypotension), loss of position
and vibratory sensation, positive Romberg sign, brisk reflexes (Worrall and
Worrall, 2005). Macrocytic anemia may not be present. MRI and CT may
demonstrate white matter lesions that may be mistaken for changes due to
hypertension or other metabolic causes (Sudo and Tashiro, 1998).
Vitamin D (Calcitriol): Although there are no significant data on the relationship
between vitamin D and dementia, there are some findings supporting a positive
relationship between vitamin D levels and cognitive performance. Przybelski and
Binkley (2007) found a significant positive correlation between performance on
MMSE and serum 25(OH) D. Studies of older adults found a positive relationship
between low levels of vitamin D and poor cognition (Kipen et al., 1995). Stueren-
berg (1996) found that dementia associated with idiopathic hypoparathyroidism
may be eVectively treated with 1, 25-dihydroxy-cholecalciferol. Vitamin D home-
ostatis is also intimately related to PTH (Kipen et al., 1995), and thus to the
homeostasis of several other electrolytes known to aVect cognition, particularly
calcium (Shoback, 2008).
before treatment. Hogervorst et al. (2008) found high TSH, as seen in those with
hypothyroidism, was associated with low cognition, and normal TSH but high
normal free T4 also is associated with poor cognition at baseline and clinically
significant decrease at 2 year follow-up. With regard to etiology and reversibility,
Whalund et al., 2002 found ‘‘little evidence that hypothyroidism causes dementia,
either reversibly or irreversibly.’’ However, there are several cases in which
treatment of both hypothyroid and hyperthyroid function (Fukui et al., 2001)
have restored cognitive function to diVerent degrees (Bono et al., 2004;
Dugbartey, 1998; Mennemeier et al., 1993). Etiologies for derangements in thy-
roid function may be clear, for example, thyroid cancer or treatment with
radiation, or may require more investigation. Anti-thyroid receptor antibodies
and antithyroid stimulating antibodies are found with Grave’s disease, anti-TPO
and antimicrosomal antibodies may be found in Hashimoto’s encephalopathy.
TSH, Free T4, T3 may all be normal in cases of autoimmune thyroid disease,
particularly in Hashimoto’s encephalopathy. Therefore, thorough investigation
demands assessment for antimicrosomal and TPO in conjunction with the
standard thyroid function tests (TFTs).
Hypercortisolemia: Selective memory impairment is seen in Cushing’s syndrome.
Results following surgery are equivocal with some studies demonstrating
improvement in cognitive function and others not (Mauri et al., 1993).
Parathyroid: PTH regulates the homeostatis of calcium, phosphate and vitamin D
activation (Shoback, 2008). PTH released into the blood acts distally at the bone to
increase release of calcium and phosphate into the blood and at the kidney to
promote calcium resorption into the blood and phosphate excretion in the urine. In
the kidney, PTH also facilitates the conversion of inactive vitamin D (25-hydro-
xycalciferol) to active vitamin D (1, 25-dihydroxycalciferol) which subsequently
facilitates increased intestinal absorption of both phosphate and calcium.
Cognitive disturbances are seen in both hypo- and hyperparathyroidism
(Adorni et al., 2005; Chadenat et al., 2008). Cognitive impairment associated
with PTH is most often associated with derangements in free calcium (Shoback
et al., 2008), however, in at least one case of idiopathic hypoparathyroidism and
normal calcium, dementia reversed following treatment with 1,25-dihydroxycal-
ciferol (Stuerenburg et al., 1996). In that same case, MRI had diVuse signal
enhancement of the periventricular frontal and parietal white matter on T2
suggestive of edema that also resolved after treatment with vitamin D. In addition,
magnesium depletion or excess may cause functional hypoparathyroidism
(Shoback, 2008). Hypercalcemia may also result from activity of a PTH-like
substance which may be produced ectopically by several tumor types. The most
common cause of hyperparathyroidism is parathyroid adenoma. Additionally,
hyperparathyroidism and Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease may be mistaken for each
other (Chadenat et al., 2008; Goto et al., 2000). CT in hypoparathyroid will
demonstrate intra parenchymal calcifications (Adorni et al., 2005), and some
REVERSIBLE DEMENTIAS 291
have noted, a positive correlation between the degree of calcification and the
degree of cognitive loss and motor symptoms.
Electrolyte abnormalities: Alterations in mental status resembling dementia may
result from electrolyte abnormalities, particularly sodium, calcium, and
magnesium.
Disorders of calcium homeostasis: Hypocalcemia, in addition to presenting with
cognitive dysfunction, may present with seizures, extrapyramidal signs, papille-
dema and elevated intracranial pressure, neuromuscular hyperreactivity, and
cataracts (Stuerenburg et al., 1996). Hypercalcemia may occur as a paraneoplastic
condition, most often with lung and breast cancer, osteolytic metastases to bone,
as well as in association with hyperparathyroidism. Activated vitamin D, 1,25
(OH)2D3 is normally suppressed when serum calcium levels are high. Elevated
1,25(OH)2D3 in association with hypercalcemia may be seen with granulomatous
disease, non-Hodgkin’s lymphoma, and other hematologic malignancies due to
extrarenal production of 1,25(OH)2D3 (Clines and Guise, 2005). Increased
suspicion for malignancy should be raised in patients greater than 50 years of
age, or progressive pain for greater than 1 month with no relief with bed rest.
Solid tumors may produce other humoral factors resulting in hypercalcemia, for
example, IL-1, IL-6, TGF-alpha, TNF, and granulocyte-CSF (Clines and Guise,
2005).
Disorders of sodium homeostasis: In hypernatremia older patients are at increased
risk of hyperosmolar states. One documented etiology for this is decreased fluid
intake. Decreased thirst has been demonstrated in normal elderly adults. Elderly
patients may be limited in their abilities to act on thirst due medical conditions
limiting their mobility and making them dependent on others to bring them fluids.
In hyponatremia, patients demonstrate lethargy, fatigue, sleep disturbance, mus-
cle cramps, and headaches. Worsening of the condition may result in nausea,
vomiting, confusion, seizures, coma, and death (Flicker and Ames, 2005).
Hepatic encephalopathy: A linear progression of cognitive decline is associated
with severity hepatic dysfunction.
Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD): COPD complicated by hypoxemia
is associated with cognitive impairments, and patients with COPD are shown to
demonstrate anterior cerebral hypoperfusion and directly correlated with impair-
ments on neuropsychological assessment (Incalzi et al., 2003). In a recent literature
review, Kozora et al. (2008) documented some improvement with traditional
therapies, for example, continuous and intermittent oxygen therapy and compre-
hensive pulmonary rehabilitation. Even greater improvement was demonstrated
following lung volume reduction surgery.
Renal failure: Patients with renal failure may develop dialysis encephalopathy
syndrome post-dialysis, which had been proposed to result from aluminum
toxicity (Flicker and Ames, 2005). Pre-dialysis, uremia may result in cognitive
compromise. Neurologic exam may demonstrate asterixis.
292 KABASAKALIAN AND FINNEY
of the brain in primary WD of the CNS may show single or multiple enhancing
lesions (Panegyres et al., 2006).
Crytococcal meningitis and meningoencephalitis: Crytococcal meningitis, usually
thought of as an opportunistic infection in immunocompromised patients
(Mitchell and Perfect, 1995) may occur in immunocompetent patients in whom
diagnosis may be delayed or missed due to similarity of presentation to other
dementias, the insidious nature of onset, lack of risk factors for opportunistic
disease, and the lack of specific symptoms (Butler et al., 2000; Lewis and
Rabinovich, 1972; Vella Zahra et al., 2004). The presence of focal symptoms
which may occur from infarct (Luse, 1967) need not concur with the cognitive
changes. Diagnosis of in immunocompromised patients presents further compli-
cation as immunocompromised patients may not have an inflammatory response
in the spinal fluid. PCR is useful for identification. Imaging studies for CNS
cryptococcal infection have shown deep white matter and basal ganglia lesions
typically interpreted as lacunes, dilated Virchow-Robin spaces, pseudocysts,
masses, hydrocephalus, and radiological meningitis, multiple ring enhancing
lesions, brain edema, hydrocephalus, leptomeningeal enhancement, subdural
eVusions, basal ganglia infarcts, and leukoencephalopathy. Of note, imaging
findings may persist long after eVective treatment and should not be mistaken
for active Cryptococcus (Hospenthal and Bennet, 2000).
Lyme disease: Infection of the brain by Borrelia burgdorferi (neuroborreliosis) may
result in reversible cognitive impairment. However, cognitive impairments may
persist despite treatment and in some cases progress to death. Lyme disease usually
begins with a rash which is usually, but not always, consistent with erythema
migrans. This may be accompanied by fatigue, headache, mild stiV neck, joint
and muscle aches, and fever. Disseminated disease may follow weeks or months
after initial exposure primarily involving neurologic, cardiac, or joint disease.
Neuroborreliosis has a protean presentation and has been reported as presenting
with a frontotemporal dementia syndrome associated with severe associated sub-
cortical atrophy (Waniek et al., 1995), normal pressure hydrocephalus (NPH)
(Danek et al., 1996), and nigrostriatal degeneration (Cassarino et al., 2003) as well
as a ‘‘Lyme encephalopathy,’’ mild to moderate in severity aVecting memory and
learning, sometimes with subtle psychiatric symptoms or somnolence, but usually
without focal neurological signs or abnormalities on MRI (Logigian et al., 1997).
Chronic cognitive deficits may persist posttreatment. Encephalomyelitis and en-
cephalopathy are most often been seen as late manifestations of infection (Wormser
et al., 2006), with rare exceptions (Danek et al., 1996). In untreated patients,
encephalomyelitis is most often monophasic, slowly progressive, and primarily
aVecting white matter and may be confused clinically with an initial manifestation
of multiple sclerosis (Wormser et al., 2006). There is no ‘‘gold standard’’ method to
accurately determine neuroborreliosis. CSF shows a lymphocytic pleocytosis, mod-
erately elevated protein and normal glucose. Serology is performed first by ELISA or
294 KABASAKALIAN AND FINNEY
deficit may persist even after HAART treatment. The most important predictor
of response to treatment was the degree of neurocognitive impairment prior to
initiation of treatment (Tozzi et al., 2007). Therefore, it is important to have a low
threshold of suspicion for possible HIV infection and to screen for it on a regular
basis in subacute dementias. In one study, HIV cognitive impairment patients
were found to have reduced markers of mature neurons and increased markers of
gliosis in the basal ganglia and frontal white matter (Paul et al., 2007), which
correlates with the frontal subcortical dysfunction seen in HIV cases.
Cerebral manifestations of systemic inflammatory disorders: This category includes
such diverse entities as Behcet’s disease, hypereosinophilic syndrome, celiac
sprue CNS vasculitis, Susac’s syndrome, Lupus cerebritis, Sjogren’s syndrome,
Antiphospholipid antibody syndrome, and Neurosarcoidosis. A number of these
inflammatory conditions are associated with vasculopathy of the CNS, by either
inflammatory or coagulopathic mechanisms. In some cases, both of these
mechanisms are observed. A number of mechanisms may be involved in CNS
damage. In SLE, for example, CNS damage may ‘‘fibrinoid necrosis of small
vessels, embolic large- and small-vessel infarction, coagulopathy, vasculitis, anti-
neuronal antibodies and the eVects of cytokines (Ovsiew and Utset, 2002). Anti-
phospholipid antibodies (aPLs) have been associated with hypercoagulability,
recurrent thromboses, transient ischemic attacks and chorea. Some lupus anti-
DNA antibodies may cross-react with NMDA receptor subunits and cause apo-
ptotic cell death. These findings suggest that the integrity of the blood-brain
barrier may be crucial in protecting against the development of cognitive
impairment due to NMDA-binding anti-DNA antibodies in patients with lupus.
Vasculitis may be limited to the CNS or may have associated systemic symptoms
including fever, fatigue, weight loss, rash, neuropathy, or other organ involve-
ment. Workup for vasculitis includes ESR, CRP, C3, C4, Ch-50, ANA, RF, anti-
SSA, anti-SSB, p-ANCA, c-ANCA. Urine evaluation may demonstrate a hemo-
lytic anemia. Lupus anticoagulant may occur independently of SLE and may
present with progressive intellectual decline and has been shown to be reversible
with immune-suppressive therapy. Labs may clarify the diagnosis. Sjogren’s syn-
drome will usually be positive for ANA, SS-A (Anti-Ro), SS-B (Anti-La), though
rarely both SS-A and SS-B may be negative. SLE may demonstrate a positive
ANA, double-stranded DNA, Anti-Smith antibody. Celiac sprue may be diagnosed
with positive antigliaden antibodies. SLE-cerebritis may demonstrate positive anti-
ribosomal and antineuronal antibodies. MRI may show cerebral atrophy, gadolini-
um enhancing T2 hyperintiensities in gray or white matter which are more often
subcortical than periventricular. All may demonstrate elevated ESR and CRP.
In Sjogren’s syndrome, MRI may demonstrate nonenhancing densities on T2
in periventricular and subcortical areas. Reversibility of cognitive impairment in
inflammatory conditions is highly variable (Caselli et al., 1991). Hypercoagulable
states may result from antiphospholipid antibodies. Hyperviscosity syndromes
296 KABASAKALIAN AND FINNEY
Autoimmune disease and the thyroid: Grave’s disease and Hashimoto’s thyroiditis
are autoimmune-mediated conditions of the thyroid. Grave’s disease often pre-
sents with neuropsychiatric and systemic symptoms. Labs demonstrate low TSH,
elevated thyroid hormone levels and demonstration of anti-TSH receptor anti-
body (TSHR). While, memory and cognitive complaints are common in acute
onset Grave’s disease, they do not manifest on formal neurocognitive assessment
and are believed to be manifestations of mood and somatic symptoms experi-
enced by patients with Grave’s, and symptoms resolve with treatment. Hashimo-
to’s thyroiditis (aka corticosteroid-responsive encephalopath associated with
evidence of thyroid autoimmunity (SREAT) is one of several conditions charac-
terized by encephalopathy which responds to treatment with steroids (nonvascu-
litic autoimmune meningoencephalitis (NAIM), has a broad range of clinical
presentations. Onset may be acute or insidious with measurable multiple neuro-
cognitive and neuropsychiatric impairments sometimes accompanied by tremor,
seizures, stroke-like events and systemic symptoms of fatigue, general malaise,
reduced appetite and weight loss (Mocellin et al., 2006; Vernino et al., 2007).
History may include generalized seizures, and neurologic exam may reveal
frontal release signs, brisk reflexes, myoclonus, tremor, and ataxia. Lab evaluation
for Hashimoto’s thyroiditis includes anti-TPO antiobody and antithyroglubulin
antibody (TG). Antimicrosomal antibodies may also be positive. Thyroid hor-
mones may be normal and inflammatory markers, for example, CRP and ESR
may be elevated and some patients may show elevated liver aminotranferase
levels. CSF may show elevated protein with mild lymphocytic pleocytosis.
Neuroimaging is usually normal, however some may show increased white matter
signal and T2-weighted and FLAIR sequences and less commonly dural enhance-
ment. Treatment is with steroids. Of note, though anti-TPO and anti-TG anti-
bodies may be found in Grave’s disease, anti-TSHR antibodies are not observed
in Hashimoto’s thyroiditis. In general, steroid-responsiveness may be the only
diagnostic clue for an autoimmune encephalopathy when serologic work-up is not
revealing. Leger et al. (2004) reported on a woman who presented with changes in
personality and attention with minor associated involuntary movements. In this
patient a PET scan was diagnostic, demonstrating hypermetabolism in the striatum
and CSF exam demonstrated antistriatal antibodies. Steroid nonresponsiveness in
cases suggests a paraneoplastic or neurodegenerative disease (Mocellin et al., 2006).
Wilson’s disease (aka hepatolenticular degeneration): An autosomal recessive disorder
of copper metabolism, resulting in copper toxicity, primarily in the liver and
brain. This presents with psychiatric and movement abnormalities including
personality changes, depression, hyperactivity, dystonia, incoordination, and
tremor. Laboratory assessment should include ceruloplasmin, serum copper,
and liver function tests. Slit lamp exam should be performed to evaluate for
Kayser-Fleisher rings in Descemet’s membrane. Treatment is through chelation
with trientine and supplementation of zinc. In cognitive assessment of treated
298 KABASAKALIAN AND FINNEY
References
Adorni, A., Lussignoli, G., Geroldi, C., and Zanetti, O. (2005). Extensive brain calcification and
dementia in postsurgical hypoparathyroidism. Neurology 65(9), 1501.
Andres, E., Vidal-Alaball, J., Federici, L., Loukili, N. H., Zimmer, J., and Kaltenbach, G. (2007).
Clinical aspects of cobalamin deficiency in elderly patients. Epidemiology, causes, clinical man-
ifestations, and treatment with special focus on oral cobalamin therapy. Eur. J. Int. Med. 18,
456–462.
Bernstein, R., Dowd, C. F., and Gress, D. R. (2003). Rapidly reversible dementia. Lancet 361, 392.
Beyenburg, S., Back, C., Diederich, N., Lewis, M., and Reuber, M. (2007). Is valproate encephalopa-
thy under-recognized in older people? A case series. Age and Ageing 36, 344–346.
Bono, G., Fancellu, R., Blandini, F., Santoro, G., and Mauri, M. (2004). Cognitive and aVective status
in mild hypothyroidism and interactions with L-thyroxine treatment. Acta Neurol. Scand. 110,
59–66.
Butler, D., Sengoz, A., and Schwartz, R. (2000). Potentially reversible cognitive impairment in patients
presenting to a memory disorders clinic. J. Cin. Neurosci. 7, 120–123.
Canto, T. G., and Korek, J. S. (1991). Central nervous system reactions to histamine-2 receptor
blockers. Ann. Intern. Med. 114, 1027–1034.
Caselli, R. J., Scheithauer, B. W., Bowles, C. A., Trenerry, M. R., Meyer, F. B., Smigielski, J. S., and
Rodriguez, M. (1991). The treatable dementia of Sjogren’s syndrome. Ann. Neurol. 30, 98–101.
Cassarino, D. S., Quezado, M. M., Ghatak, N. R., and Duray, P. H. (2003). Lyme-associated
parkinsonism. Arch. Path Lab. Med. 127, 1204–1206.
Chabria, S. B., and Lawrason, J. (2007). Altered mental status, an unusual manifestation of Lyme
disease: A case resport. J. Med. Case Rep. 1, 62–65.
Chadenat, M. L., Dalloz, M. A., D’Anglejean, J., Fineyre, F., and Pico, F. (2008). Primary hyperpara-
thyroidism as a differential diagnosis of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease. Rev. Neurol. (Paris) July [Epub
ahead of print).
Chiu, H. F. K. (1996). Vitamin B12 deficiency and dementia. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiat. 11, 851–858.
Clines, G. A., and Guise, T. A. (2005). Hypercalcemia of malignancy and basic research on mechan-
isms responsible for osteolytic and osteoblastic metastasis to bone. Endoc.-Relat. Cancer 12,
549–583.
Danek, A., Uttner, I., Yousry, T., and Pfister, H. (1996). Lyme neuroborreliosis disguised as normal
pressure hydrocephalus. Neurology 46, 1743–1745.
Das, A. F., Freston, J. W., Jacobs, J., Fox, N. A., and Morton, R. E. (1990). An evaluation of safety in
37,252 patients treated with cimetidine or ranitidine. Intern. Med. 11, 3–14.
Dugbartey, A. T. (1998). Neurocognitive eVects of hypothyroidism. Arch. Int. Med. 13, 1413–1418.
300 KABASAKALIAN AND FINNEY
Feinberg, M. (1993). The problems of anticholinergic adverse eVects in older patients. Drugs Ageing 3,
335–348.
Flicker, L., and Ames, D. (2005). Metabolic and endocrinologic causes of dementia. Int. Psychogeriatr.
17(Suppl.), S79–S92.
Foy, A., O’Connell, D., Henry, D. D., Cocking, S., and Halliday, J. (1995). Benzodiazepine use as a
cause of cognitive impairment in elderly hospital patients. J. Gerontol. 50, M99–M106.
Fukui, T., Hasegawa, Y., and Takenaka, H. (2001). Hyperthyroid dementia: Clinicoradiological
findings and response to treatment. J. Neurol. Sci. 184, 81–88.
Gerard, A., Sarrot-Renauld, F., Liozon, E., Cathebras, P., Besson, G., Robin, C., Vighetto, A.,
Mosnier, J., Durieu, I., Durand, D. V., and Rousset, H. (2002). Neurologic presentation of
Whipple disease: Report of 12 cases and review of the literature. Medicine (Baltimore) 81(6),
443–457.
Goto, F., Kano, S., and Ogawa, K. (2000). Creutzfeld-Jakob disease presenting hyperparathyroidism.
Auris Nasus Larynx 27, 281–283.
Hajjar, E. R., Hanlon, J. T., Artz, M. B., Lindblad, C. I., Pieper, C. F., Sloane, R. J., Ruby, C. M., and
Schmader, K. E. (2003). Adverse drug reaction risk factors in older outpatients. Am. J. Geriatr.
Pharmacother. 1(2), 82–89.
Hirono, N., Yamadori, A., and Komiyama, M. (1993). Dural arteriovenous fistula: A cause of
hypoperfusion-induced intellectual impairment. Eur. Neurol. 33, 5–8 (Abstract).
Hogervorst, E., Huppert, F., Matthews, F. E., and Brayned, C. (2008). Thyroid function and cognitive
decline in the MRC cognitive function and ageing study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 1013–1022.
Hospenthal, D. R., and Bennett, J. E. (2000). Persistence of crytococcomas on neuroimaging. Clin.
Infect. Dis. 31, 1303–1306.
Incalzi, R. A., Marra, C., Giordano, A., Calcagni, M. L., Cappa, A., Basso, A., Basso, S., Pagliari, G.,
and Fuso, L. (2003). Cognitive impairment in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, A neuro-
psychological and spect study. J. Neurol. 250, 325–332.
Jankovic, J., Orman, J., and Janssen, B. (1985). Placebo-controlled study of mesulergine in Parkinson
disease. Neurology 35, 161–165.
Kipen, E., Helme, R. D., Wark, J. D., and Flicker, L. (1995). Bone density, vitamin D nutrition, and
parathyroid hormone levels in women with dementia. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 43(10), 1088–1091.
Kozora, E., Emery, C., Kaplan, R. M., Wamboldt, F. S., Zhang, L., and Make, B. J. (2008). Cognitive
and psychological issues in emphysema. Proc. Am. Thorac. Soc. 5, 556–560.
Lechin, F., van der Dijs, B., and Benaim, M. (1996). Benzodiazepines: Tolerability in elderly patients.
Psychother. Psychosomatics 65(4), 171–182 (Abstract).
Leger, G. C., Johnson, N., Horowitz, S. W., Dale, R. C., Church, A. J., and Mesulam, M. M. (2004).
Dementia-like presentation of striatal hypermetabolic state with antistriatal antibodies responsive
to steroids. Arch. Neurol. 61, 754–757.
Lewis, J. L., and Rabinovich, S. (1972). The wide spectrum of cryptococcal infections. Am. J. Med. 53,
315–322.
Libois, A., De Witt, S., Poll, B., Garcia, F., Florence, E., Del Rio, A., Sanchez, P., Negredo, E.,
Vandenbruaene, M., Gatell, J. M., and Clumeck, N. (2007). HIV and Syphilis: When to perform a
lumbar puncture. Sex. Transm. Dis. 34, 141–144.
Logigian, E. L., Johnson, K. A., Kijewski, M. F., Kaplan, R. F., Becker, J. A., Jones, K. J.,
Garada, B. M., Holman, B. L., and Steere, A. C. (1997). Reversible cerebral hypoperfusion in
Lyme encephalopathy. Neurology 49, 1661–1670.
Loikas, S., Koskinen, P., Irjala, K., Lopponen, M., Isoaho, R., and Kivela, S. -L. (2007). Pelliniemi.
Vitamin B12 deficiency in the aged: A population-based study. Age Ageing 36, 177–183.
Luse, S. A. (1967). Pathology of chronic inflammation in the nervous system. Clin. Neurosurg. 14,
227–238.
Marth, T., and Raoult, D. (2003). Whipple’s disease. Lancet 361, 239–246.
REVERSIBLE DEMENTIAS 301
Mauri, M., Sinforioani, E., Bono, G., Vignati, F., Berselli, M. E., Attanasio, R., and Nappi, G. (1993).
Memory impairment in Cushing’s disease. Acta Neurol. Scand. 87(1), 52–55.
Mennemeier, M., Garner, R. D., and Heilman, K. M. (1993). Mood, memory and measurement in
hypothyroidism. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 15(5), 822–831 (abstract).
Miller, R. R., and Jick, H. (1978). Clinical eVects of meperidine in hospitalized medical patients. J. Clin.
Pharmacol. 18, 180–189.
Mitchell, T. G., and Perfect, J. R. (1995). Crytococcus in the era of AIDS-100 years after the discovery
of Cryptococcus neoformans. Clin. Microbiol. Rev. 8, 515–548.
Mocellin, R., Lubman, D. I., Lloyd, J., Tomlinson, E. B., and Velakoulis, D. (2006). Reversible
dementia with psychosis: Hashimoto’s encephalopathy. Psychiat. Clin. Neurosci. 60, 761–763.
Montejo, M., Ruiz-Irastorza, G., Aguirrebengoa, K., Onate, J., and Aurrekoetxea, J. (1995). Neuor-
osyphilis as a cause of dementia. Does it still exist? J. Infect. 30, 186–187.
Moore, A. R., and O’Keefe, S. T. (1999). Drug-induced cognitive impairment in the elderly. Drugs
Ageing 15(1), 15–28.
Morgan, A. E., Smith, W. K., and Levenson, J. L. (2003). Reversible dementia due to thalidomide
therapy for multiple myeloma. New Eng. J. Med. 348, 1821–1822.
Nakajima, D., Fukushima, K., and Yamanouchi, H. (2006). Neurological complications during and
after the treatment of acute lymphoblastic leukemia. No To Hattatsu 38, 195–200.
Ovsiew, F., and Utset, T. (2001). Neuropsychiatric aspects of rheumatic disease. In ‘‘The American
Psychiatric Publishing Textbook of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences’’ (S. C. Yudovsky
and R. E. Hales, Eds.), 4th editon, pp. 813–850. American Psychiatric Publishing, Washington.
Panegyers, P. K., Eids, R., Beaman, M., and Fallon, M. (2006). Primary Whipple’s disease of the brain:
Characterization of the clinical syndrome and molecular diagnosis. Q. J. Med. 99, 609–623.
Paul, R. H., Yiannoutsos, C. T., Miller, E. N., Chang, L., Marra, C. M., Schifitto, G., Ernst, T.,
Singer, T., Singer, E., Richards, T., Jarvik, G. J., Price, R., et al. (2007). Proton MRS and
neuropsychological correlates in AIDS dementia complex: Evidence of subcortical specificity.
J. Neuropsych. Clin. Neurosci. 19, 283–292.
Prousky, J. E. (2003). Pellagra may be a rare secondary complication of anorexia nervosa: A systematic
review of the literature 8, 180–185.
Przybelski, R. J., and Binkley, N. C. (2007). Is vitamin D important for preserving cognition? A positive
correlation of serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D concentration with cognitive function. Arch. Biochem.
Biophys. 460, 202–205.
Rogers, T. K., and Bowman, C. E. (1990). Cognitive impairment associated with beta-blockade in the
elderly. Postgraduate Med. 66(782), 1050–1052.
Roos, K. L., and Berger, J. R. (2007). Is the presence of antibodies in CSF sufficient to make a definitive
diagnosis of Lyme disease? Neurology 69, 949–950.
Rossi, T., Haghighipour, R., Haghighi, M., Paolini, S., and Scarpino, O. (2004). Cerebral Whipple’s
disease as a cause of reversible dementia. Clin. Neurol. Neurosur. 107, 258–261.
Schofield, P. (2005). Dementia associated with toxic causes and autoimmune diseases. Int. Psychogeriatr.
17(Suppl.), S129–S147.
Shoback, D. (2008). Hypoparathyroidism. New Engl. J. Med. 359, 391–403.
Singh, S., and Kumar, A. (2007). Wernicke encephalopathy after obesity surgery: A systematic review.
Neurology 68, 807–811.
Smith, S. J., and Kocen, R. S. (1988). A Creutzfeldt-Jakob like syndrome due to lithium toxicity.
J. Neurol. Neurosur. Psychit. 51, 21–23.
Stuerenburg, H. J., Hansen, H. C., Thie, A., and Kunze, K. (1996). Reversible dementia in idiopathic
hypoparathyroidism associated with normocalcemia. Neuorology 47, 474–476.
Sudo, K., and Tashiro, K. (1998). Cerebral white matter lesions associated with vitamin B12
deficiency. Neurology 51, 325.
302 KABASAKALIAN AND FINNEY
Timmermans, M., and Carr, J. (2004). Neurosyphilis in the modern era. J. Neruol. Neurosurg. Psychiat.
75, 1727–1730.
Tozzi, V., Balestra, P., Bellagamba, R., Corpolongo, A., Salvatori, M. F., Visco-Comandini, U.,
Vlassi, C., Giuliannelli, M., Galgani, S., Antinori, A., and Narciso, P. (2007). Persistence of
neuropsychological deficts despite long-term highly active antiretroviral therapy in patients with
HIV-related neurocognitive impairment. J. Acq. Immun. Def. Synd. 45, 174–182.
Trimble, M. R. (1987). Anticonvulsant drugs and cognitive function: A review of the literature.
Epilepsia 28(Suppl. 3), S37–S45.
Ueda, K., Takada, D., Mii, A., Tsuzuku, Y., Saito, S. K., Kaneko, T., Utsumi, K., Iino, Y., and
Katayama, Y. (2006). Severe thiamine deficiency resulted in Wernicke’s encephalopathy in
chronic dialysis patient. Clin. Exp. Nephrol. 10, 290–293.
Vella Zahra, L., Mallia Azzopardi, C., and Scott, G. (2004). Cryptococcal meningitis in two apparently
immunocompetent Maltese patients. Mycoses 47, 168–173.
Vernino, S., Geschwind, M., and Boeve, B. (2007). Autoimmune encephalopathies. Neurologist 13,
140–147.
Vernino, S., Geschwind, M., and Boeve, B. (2007). Autoimmune encephalopathies. The Neurologist 13,
140–147.
Waniek, C., Prohovnik, I., Kaufman, M. A., and Dwork, A. J. (1995). Rapidly progressive frontal-type
dementia associated with Lyme Disease. J. Neuropsych. Clin. Neurosci. 7, 345–347.
Whalund, L., Basum, H., and Waldemar, G. Reversible or arrestable dmentias. In ‘‘Evidence-based
Dementia Practice’’ (N. Qizilbash et al., Eds.), pp. 330–340. Blackwell Science, Oxford
Wormser, G. P., Dattwyler, R. J., Shapiro, E. D., Halperin, J. J., Steere, A. C., Klempner, M. S.,
Krause, P. J., Bakken, J. S., Strle, F., Stanek, G., Bockenstedt, L., Fish, D., et al. (2006). The clinical
assessment, treatment, and prevention of Lyme disease, human granulocytic anaplasmosis, and
babeiosis: Clinical practice guidelines by the infectious diseases society of America. Clin. Infect. Dis.
43, 1089–1134.
Worrall, B. B., and Worrall, L. P. (2005). In ‘‘Nutritional disorders: Malnutrition, malabsorption, and
B12 and other vitamin deficiencies in Merritt’s Neurology’’ (L. P. Rowland, Ed.), 11th Edition
New York.
Wright, A. L., and Persad, C. (2007). Distinguishing between depression and dementia in older
persons: Neuropsychologi al and neuropathologica correlates. J. Geriatr. Psychiat. Neurol. 20,
189–198.
Zaret, B. S., and Cohen, R. A. (1986). Reversible valproic acid-induced dementia: a case report.
Epilepsia 27, 234–240.
INDEX
303
304 INDEX
Vascular cognitive impairment, 21, 49, 58. Vascular mild cognitive impairment
See also Cognitive impairment (cont.) (VaMCI), 59, 62–63
diagnostic neuroimaging criteria, 65–66 VCI. See Vascular cognitive impairment
hereditary, 69–70 Visual hallucinations, 216, 219, 221
histopathology, 64 Visuospatial abnormalities, 232
imaging findings and pathology, 73 Visuospatial deficits, 234
neuroimaging techniques, 67 Vitamin B12, 138, 247, 249, 288
conventional structural MRI, 67–70 Vitamin E, 286
functional imaging, 72–73
metabolic imaging, 71–72
nonconventional techniques, 70–71 W
primary prevention, 66
Vascular dementia, 43, 59, 65, 106, 116 Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome, 267
cholinesterase inhibitors for, 66 Whipple’s disease (WD), 255, 286, 292
classification, 22 White matter atrophy, 205
diagnostic criteria, 66 White matter hyperintensities (WMH),
1
H MRS findings, 116–117 64, 67–68
NINDS-AIREN criteria, 24 White matter ischemia, 24
treatment, 66 Wilson’s disease, 297
Vascular diseases, 23, 250–252
CONTENTS OF RECENT VOLUMES
311
312 CONTENTS OF RECENT VOLUMES
Modulation of Amino Acid-Gated Ion Channels Calcium Antagonists: Their Role in Neuro-
by Protein Phosphorylation protection
Stephen J. Moss and Trevor G. Smart A. Jacqueline Hunter
Use-Dependent Regulation of GABAA Sodium and Potassium Channel Modulators:
Receptors Their Role in Neuroprotection
Eugene M. Barnes, Jr. Tihomir P. Obrenovich
Synaptic Transmission and Modulation in the NMDA Antagonists: Their Role in Neuroprotection
Neostriatum Danial L. Small
David M. Lovinger and Elizabeth Tyler Development of the NMDA Ion-Channel
The Cytoskeleton and Neurotransmitter Blocker, Aptiganel Hydrochloride, as a Neuro-
Receptors protective Agent for Acute CNS Injury
Valerie J. Whatley and R. Adron Harris Robert N. McBurney
The Role of MST Neurons during Ocular Brain Development and Generation of Brain
Tracking in 3D Space Pathologies
K. Kawano, U. Inoue, A. Takemura, Gregory L. Holmes and Bridget McCabe
Y. Kodaka, and F. A. Miles
Maturation of Channels and Receptors: Conse-
Visual Navigation in Flying Insects quences for Excitability
M. V. Srinivasan and S.-W. Zhang David F. Owens and Arnold R. Kriegstein
Neuronal Matched Filters for Optic Flow Neuronal Activity and the Establishment of
Processing in Flying Insects Normal and Epileptic Circuits during Brain
H. G. Krapp Development
A Common Frame of Reference for the Analysis John W. Swann, Karen L. Smith, and Chong L. Lee
of Optic Flow and Vestibular Information The Effects of Seizures of the Hippocampus of
B. J. Frost and D. R. W. Wylie the Immature Brain
Optic Flow and the Visual Guidance of Ellen F. Sperber and Solomon L. Moshe
Locomotion in the Cat Abnormal Development and Catastrophic
H. Sherk and G. A. Fowler Epilepsies: The Clinical Picture and Relation to
Stages of Self-Motion Processing in Primate Neuroimaging
Posterior Parietal Cortex Harry T. Chugani and Diane C. Chugani
F. Bremmer, J.-R. Duhamel, S. B. Hamed, and Cortical Reorganization and Seizure Generation
W. Graf in Dysplastic Cortex
Optic Flow Analysis for Self-Movement G. Avanzini, R. Preafico, S. Franceschetti,
Perception G. Sancini, G. Battaglia, and V. Scaioli
C. J. Duffy Rasmussen’s Syndrome with Particular Refer-
Neural Mechanisms for Self-Motion Perception ence to Cerebral Plasticity: A Tribute to Frank
in Area MST Morrell
R. A. Andersen, K. V. Shenoy, J. A. Crowell, Fredrick Andermann and Yuonne Hart
and D. C. Bradley Structural Reorganization of Hippocampal
Computational Mechanisms for Optic Flow Networks Caused by Seizure Activity
Analysis in Primate Cortex Daniel H. Lowenstein
M. Lappe Epilepsy-Associated Plasticity in gamma-
Human Cortical Areas Underlying the Percep- Amniobutyric Acid Receptor Expression,
tion of Optic Flow: Brain Imaging Studies Function and Inhibitory Synaptic Properties
M. W. Greenlee Douglas A. Coulter
What Neurological Patients Tell Us about the Synaptic Plasticity and Secondary Epilepto-
Use of Optic Flow genesis
L. M. Vaina and S. K. Rushton Timothy J. Teyler, Steven L. Morgan,
Rebecca N. Russell, and Brian L. Woodside
index Synaptic Plasticity in Epileptogenesis: Cel-
lular Mechanisms Underlying Long-Lasting
Synaptic Modifications that Require New Gene
Expression
Volume 45 Oswald Steward, Christopher S. Wallace, and
Paul F. Worley
Mechanisms of Brain Plasticity: From Normal Cellular Correlates of Behavior
Brain Function to Pathology Emma R. Wood, Paul A. Dudchenko, and
Philip. A. Schwartzkroin Howard Eichenbaum
316 CONTENTS OF RECENT VOLUMES
Clinical Implications of Circulating Neuroster- Processing Human Brain Tissue for in Situ
oids Hybridization with Radiolabelled Oligonucleo-
Andrea R. Genazzani, Patrizia Monteleone, tides
Massimo Stomati, Francesca Bernardi, Louise F. B. Nicholson
Luigi Cobellis, Elena Casarosa, Michele Luisi,
In Situ Hybridization of Astrocytes and Neurons
Stefano Luisi, and Felice Petraglia
Cultured in Vitro
Neuroactive Steroids and Central Nervous L. A. Arizza-McNaughton, C. De Felipe,
System Disorders and S. P. Hunt
Mingde Wang, Torbjörn Bäckström,
In Situ Hybridization on Organotypic Slice
Inger Sundström, Göran Wahlström,
Cultures
Tommy Olsson, Di Zhu, Inga-Maj Johansson,
A. Gerfin-Moser and H. Monyer
Inger Björn, and Marie Bixo
Quantitative Analysis of in Situ Hybridization
Neuroactive Steroids in Neuropsychopharma-
Histochemistry
cology
Andrew L. Gundlach and Ross D. O’Shea
Rainer Rupprecht and Florian Holsboer
Current Perspectives on the Role of Neuroster- Part II: Nonradioactive in Situ hybridization
oids in PMS and Depression Nonradioactive in Situ Hybridization Using Alka-
Lisa D. Griffin, Susan C. Conrad, and line Phosphatase-Labelled Oligonucleotides
Synthia H. Mellon S. J. Augood, E. M. McGowan, B. R. Finsen,
B. Heppelmann, and P. C. Emson
index
Combining Nonradioactive in Situ Hybridization
with Immunohistological and Anatomical
Techniques
Volume 47 Petra Wahle
Nonradioactive in Situ Hybridization: Simplified
Introduction: Studying Gene Expression in Procedures for Use in Whole Mounts of Mouse
Neural Tissues by in Situ Hybridization and Chick Embryos
W. Wisden and B. J. Morris Linda Ariza-McNaughton and Robb Krumlauf
Part I: In Situ Hybridization with Radiolabelled
index
Oligonucleotides
In Situ Hybridization with Oligonucleotide
Probes
Wl. Wisden and B. J. Morris
Cryostat Sectioning of Brains Volume 48
Victoria Revilla and Alison Jones
Processing Rodent Embryonic and Early Post- Assembly and Intracellular Trafficking of
natal Tissue for in Situ Hybridization with GABAA Receptors Eugene
Radiolabelled Oligonucleotides Barnes
David J. Laurie, Petra C. U. Schrotz, Subcellular Localization and Regulation of
Hannah Monyer, and Ulla Amtmann
GABAA Receptors and Associated Proteins
Processing of Retinal Tissue for in Situ Hybrid- Bernhard Lüscher and Jean-Marc Fritschy D1
ization Dopamine Receptors
Frank Müller Richard Mailman
Processing the Spinal Cord for in Situ Hybridiza- Molecular Modeling of Ligand-Gated Ion
tion with Radiolabelled Oligonucleotides Channels: Progress and Challenges
A. Berthele and T. R. Tölle Ed Bertaccini and James R. Trudel
318 CONTENTS OF RECENT VOLUMES
Alzheimer’s Disease: Its Diagnosis and Patho- The Treatment of Infantile Spasms: An
genesis Evidence-Based Approach
Jillian J. Kril and Glenda M. Halliday Mark Mackay, Shelly Weiss, and
DNA Arrays and Functional Genomics in O. Carter Snead III
Neurobiology ACTH Treatment of Infantile Spasms: Mechan-
Christelle Thibault, Long Wang, Li Zhang, and isms of Its Effects in Modulation of Neuronal
Michael F. Miles Excitability
K. L. Brunson, S. Avishai-Eliner, and
index T. Z. Baram
Neurosteroids and Infantile Spasms: The Deox-
ycorticosterone Hypothesis
Volume 49 Michael A. Rogawski and Doodipala S. Reddy
Are there Specific Anatomical and/or Transmit-
What Is West Syndrome? ter Systems (Cortical or Subcortical) That
Olivier Dulac, Christine Soufflet, Should Be Targeted?
Catherine Chiron, and Anna Kaminski Phillip C. Jobe
The Relationship between encephalopathy and Medical versus Surgical Treatment: Which
Abnormal Neuronal Activity in the Developing Treatment When
Brain W. Donald Shields
Frances E. Jensen
Developmental Outcome with and without
Hypotheses from Functional Neuroimaging Successful Intervention
Studies Rochelle Caplan, Prabha Siddarth,
Csaba Juhász, Harry T. Chugani, Gary Mathern, Harry Vinters, Susan Curtiss,
Ouo Muzik, and Diane C. Chugani Jennifer Levitt, Robert Asarnow, and
Infantile Spasms: Unique Sydrome or General W. Donald Shields
Age-Dependent Manifestation of a Diffuse Infantile Spasms versus Myoclonus: Is There a
Encephalopathy? Connection?
M. A. Koehn and M. Duchowny Michael R. Pranzatelli
Histopathology of Brain Tissue from Patients Tuberous Sclerosis as an Underlying Basis for
with Infantile Spasms Infantile Spasm
Harry V. Vinters Raymond S. Yeung
Generators of Ictal and Interictal Electroenceph- Brain Malformation, Epilepsy, and Infantile
alograms Associated with Infantile Spasms: Spasms
Intracellular Studies of Cortical and Thalamic M. Elizabeth Ross
Neurons
M. Steriade and I. Timofeev Brain Maturational Aspects Relevant to Patho-
physiology of Infantile Spasms
Cortical and Subcortical Generators of Normal G. Auanzini, F. Panzica, and
and Abnormal Rhythmicity S. Franceschetti
David A. McCormick
Gene Expression Analysis as a Strategy to
Role of Subcortical Structures in the Patho- Understand the Molecular Pathogenesis of
genesis of Infantile Spasms: What Are Possible Infantile Spasms
Subcortical Mediators? Peter B. Crino
F. A. Lado and S. L. Moshé
Infantile Spasms: Criteria for an Animal Model
What Must We Know to Develop Better Carl E. Stafstrom and Gregory L. Holmes
Therapies?
Jean Aicardi index
CONTENTS OF RECENT VOLUMES 319
Volume 52
Volume 53
Neuroimmune Relationships in Perspective
Frank Hucklebridge and Angela Clow Section I: Mitochondrial Structure and Function
Sympathetic Nervous System Interaction with Mitochondrial DNA Structure and Function
the Immune System Carlos T. Moraes, Sarika Srivastava,
Virginia M. Sanders and Adam P. Kohm Ilias Kirkinezos, Jose Oca-Cossio,
Mechanisms by Which Cytokines Signal the Brain Corina van Waveren, Markus Woischnick,
Adrian J. Dunn and Francisca Diaz
Section III: Secondary Respiratory Chain The Mitochondrial Theory of Aging: Involve-
Disorders ment of Mitochondrial DNA Damage and
Friedreich’s Ataxia Repair
J. M. Cooper and J. L. Bradley Nadja C. de Souza-Pinto and
Vilhelm A. Bohr
Wilson Disease
C. A. Davie and A. H. V. Schapira index
Hereditary Spastic Paraplegia
Christopher J. McDermott and Pamela J. Shaw
Cytochrome c Oxidase Deficiency Volume 54
Giacomo P. Comi, Sandra Strazzer,
Sara Galbiati, and Nereo Bresolin Unique General Anesthetic Binding Sites Within
Distinct Conformational States of the Nicotinic
Section IV: Toxin Induced Mitochondrial
Acetylcholine Receptor
Dysfunction
Hugo R. Ariaas, William, R. Kem,
Toxin-Induced Mitochondrial Dysfunction James R. Truddell, and Michael P. Blanton
Susan E. Browne and M. Flint Beal
Signaling Molecules and Receptor Transduction
Cascades That Regulate NMDA Receptor-
Section V: Neurodegenerative Disorders
Mediated Synaptic Transmission
Parkinson’s Disease Suhas. A. Kotecha and John F. MacDonald
L. V. P. Korlipara and A. H. V. Schapira
Behavioral Measures of Alcohol Self-Administra-
Huntington’s Disease: The Mystery Unfolds? tion and Intake Control: Rodent Models
Åsa Petersén and Patrik Brundin Herman H. Samson and Cristine L. Czachowski
Mitochondria in Alzheimer’s Disease Dopaminergic Mouse Mutants: Investigating
Russell H. Swerdlow and Stephen J. Kish the Roles of the Different Dopamine Receptor
Contributions of Mitochondrial Alterations, Subtypes and the Dopamine Transporter
Resulting from Bad Genes and a Hostile Envi- Shirlee Tan, Bettina Hermann, and
ronment, to the Pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s Emiliana Borrelli
Disease Drosophila melanogaster, A Genetic Model System
Mark P. Mattson for Alcohol Research
Mitochondria and Amyotrophic Lateral Douglas J. Guarnieri and Ulrike Heberlein
Sclerosis
index
Richard W. Orrell and Anthony H. V. Schapira
Section VII: Defects of " Oxidation Including Section I: Virsu Vectors For Use in the Nervous
Carnitine Deficiency System
Defects of " Oxidation Including Carnitine Non-Neurotropic Adenovirus: a Vector for Gene
Deficiency Transfer to the Brain and Gene Therapy of
K. Bartlett and M. Pourfarzam Neurological Disorders
P. R. Lowenstein, D. Suwelack, J. Hu,
Section VIII: Mitochondrial Involvement in X. Yuan, M. Jimenez-Dalmaroni,
Aging S. Goverdhama, and M.G. Castro
322 CONTENTS OF RECENT VOLUMES
Peter J. Snyder, Feng Gao, Tom Stiger, Rene L. Olvera, David C. Glahn, Sheila C. Caetano,
Christian Rohlff, Athula Herath, Trey Sunderland, Steven R. Pliszka, and Jair C. Soares
Karen Putnam, and W. Frost White
Chemosensory G-Protein-Coupled Receptor
Section III: Informatics Signaling in the Brain
Geoffrey E. Woodard
Proteomic Informatics
Steven Russell, William Old, Katheryn Resing, and Disturbances of Emotion Regulation after Focal
Lawrence Hunter Brain Lesions
Antoine Bechara
Section IV: Changes in the Proteome by
The Use of Caenorhabditis elegans in Molecular
Disease Neuropharmacology
Proteomics Analysis in Alzheimer’s Disease: New Jill C. Bettinger, Lucinda Carnell, Andrew G. Davies,
Insights into Mechanisms of Neurodegeneration and Steven L. McIntire
D. Allan Butterfield and Debra Boyd-Kimball
index
Proteomics and Alcoholism
Frank A. Witzmann and Wendy N. Strother
Proteomics Studies of Traumatic Brain Injury Volume 63
Kevin K. W. Wang, Andrew Ottens,
William Haskins, Ming Cheng Liu, Mapping Neuroreceptors at work: On the Def-
Firas Kobeissy, Nancy Denslow, inition and Interpretation of Binding Potentials
SuShing Chen, and Ronald L. Hayes after 20 years of Progress
Influence of Huntington’s Disease on the Human Albert Gjedde, Dean F. Wong, Pedro Rosa-Neto, and
and Mouse Proteome Paul Cumming
Claus Zabel and Joachim Klose Mitochondrial Dysfunction in Bipolar Disorder:
From 31P-Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopic
Section V: Overview of the Neuroproteome Findings to Their Molecular Mechanisms
Proteomics—Application to the Brain Tadafumi Kato
Katrin Marcus, Oliver Schmidt, Heike Schaefer, Large-Scale Microarray Studies of Gene Expres-
Michael Hamacher, AndrÅ van Hall, and Helmut sion in Multiple Regions of the Brain in Schizo-
E. Meyer phrenia and Alzeimer’s Disease
Pavel L. Katsel, Kenneth L. Davis, and Vahram
index Haroutunian
Regulation of Serotonin 2C Receptor PRE-
Volume 62 mRNA Editing By Serotonin
Claudia Schmauss
index Volume 66
Combined EEG and fMRI Studies of Human W. Gordon Frankle, Mark Slifstein, Peter S. Talbot,
Brain Function and Marc Laruelle
V. Menon and S. Crottaz-Herbette
index
index
Volume 68
Volume 67
Fetal Magnetoencephalography: Viewing the
Distinguishing Neural Substrates of Heterogen- Developing Brain In Utero
eity Among Anxiety Disorders Hubert Preissl, Curtis L. Lowery, and Hari Eswaran
Jack B. Nitschke and Wendy Heller Magnetoencephalography in Studies of Infants
Neuroimaging in Dementia and Children
K. P. Ebmeier, C. Donaghey, and Minna Huotilainen
N. J. Dougall Let’s Talk Together: Memory Traces Revealed
Prefrontal and Anterior Cingulate Contributions by Cooperative Activation in the Cerebral
to Volition in Depression Cortex
Jack B. Nitschke and Kristen L. Mackiewicz Jochen Kaiser, Susanne Leiberg, and Werner
Lutzenberger
Functional Imaging Research in Schizophrenia
H. Tost, G. Ende, M. Ruf, F. A. Henn, and Human Communication Investigated With
A. Meyer-Lindenberg Magnetoencephalography: Speech, Music, and
Gestures
Neuroimaging in Functional Somatic Syn-
Thomas R. Knösche, Burkhard Maess, Akinori
dromes
Nakamura, and Angela D. Friederici
Patrick B. Wood
Combining Magnetoencephalography and
Neuroimaging in Multiple Sclerosis
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Alireza Minagar, Eduardo Gonzalez-Toledo,
Klaus Mathiak and Andreas J. Fallgatter
James Pinkston, and Stephen L. Jaffe
Beamformer Analysis of MEG Data
Stroke
Arjan Hillebrand and Gareth R. Barnes
Roger E. Kelley and Eduardo Gonzalez-Toledo
Functional Connectivity Analysis in Magnetoen-
Functional MRI in Pediatric Neurobehavioral
cephalography
Disorders
Alfons Schnitzler and Joachim Gross
Michael Seyffert and F. Xavier Castellanos
Human Visual Processing as Revealed by Mag-
Structural MRI and Brain Development
netoencephalographys
Paul M. Thompson, Elizabeth R. Sowell,
Yoshiki Kaneoke, Shoko Watanabe, and Ryusuke
Nitin Gogtay, Jay N. Giedd, Christine N. Vidal,
Kakigi
Kiralee M. Hayashi, Alex Leow, Rob Nicolson,
Judith L. Rapoport, and Arthur W. Toga A Review of Clinical Applications of Magne-
toencephalography
Neuroimaging and Human Genetics
Andrew C. Papanicolaou, Eduardo M. Castillo,
Georg Winterer, Ahmad R. Hariri, David Goldman,
Rebecca Billingsley-Marshall, Ekaterina Pataraia,
and Daniel R. Weinberger
and Panagiotis G. Simos
Neuroreceptor Imaging in Psychiatry: Theory
and Applications index
CONTENTS OF RECENT VOLUMES 327
Volume 71
Volume 70
Art, Constructional Apraxia, and the Brain Transmitter Release at the Neuromuscular
Louis Caplan Junction
Section V: Genetic Diseases Thomas L. Schwarz
ID, Ego, and Temporal Lobe Revisited Evidence for Neuroprotective Effects of Antipsy-
Shirley M. Ferguson and chotic Drugs: Implications for the Pathophysiology
Mark Rayport and Treatment of Schizophrenia
Xin-Min Li and Haiyun Xu
Section II: Stereotaxic Studies
Neurogenesis and Neuroenhancement in the
Olfactory Gustatory Responses Evoked by Elec-
Pathophysiology and Treatment of Bipolar
trical Stimulation of Amygdalar Region in Man
Disorder
Are Qualitatively Modifiable by Interview Con-
Robert J. Schloesser, Guang Chen, and
tent: Case Report and Review
Husseini K. Manji
Mark Rayport, Sepehr Sani, and Shirley M. Ferguson
Neuroreplacement, Growth Factor, and Small
Section III: Controversy in Definition of Behavioral
Molecule Neurotrophic Approaches for Treating
Disturbance
Parkinson’s Disease
Pathogenesis of Psychosis in Epilepsy. The ‘‘See- Michael J. O’Neill, Marcus J. Messenger,
saw’’ Theory: Myth or Reality? Viktor Lakics, Tracey K. Murray, Eric H. Karran,
Shirley M. Ferguson and Mark Rayport Philip G. Szekeres, Eric S. Nisenbaum, and
Kalpana M. Merchant
Section IV: Outcome of Temporal Lobectomy
Using Caenorhabditis elegans Models of Neuro-
Memory Function After Temporal Lobectomy
degenerative Disease to Identify Neuroprotective
for Seizure Control: A Comparative Neuropsy
Strategies
chiatric and Neuropsychological Study
Brian Kraemer and Gerard D. Schellenberg
Shirley M. Ferguson, A. John McSweeny, and
Mark Rayport Neuroprotection and Enhancement of Neurite
Life After Surgery for Temporolimbic Seizures Outgrowth With Small Molecular Weight Com-
Shirley M. Ferguson, Mark Rayport, and pounds From Screens of Chemical Libraries
Carolyn A. Schell Donard S. Dwyer and Addie Dickson
index
Appendix I
Mark Rayport
Appendix II: Conceptual Foundations of Studies Volume 78
of Patients Undergoing Temporal Lobe Surgery
for Seizure Control
Neurobiology of Dopamine in Schizophrenia
Mark Rayport
Olivier Guillin, Anissa Abi-Dargham, and
index Marc Laruelle
Serotonin and Dopamine Interactions in The CD8 T Cell in Multiple Sclerosis: Suppressor
Rodents and Primates: Implications for Psych- Cell or Mediator of Neuropathology?
osis and Antipsychotic Drug Development Aaron J. Johnson, Georgette L. Suidan,
Gerard J. Marek Jeremiah McDole, and Istvan Pirko
Cholinergic Circuits and Signaling in the Patho- Immunopathogenesis of Multiple Sclerosis
physiology of Schizophrenia Smriti M. Agrawal and V. Wee Yong
Joshua A. Berman, David A. Talmage, and
Lorna W. Role Molecular Mimicry in Multiple Sclerosis
Jane E. Libbey, Lori L. McCoy, and
Schizophrenia and the #7 Nicotinic Acetylchol- Robert S. Fujinami
ine Receptor
Laura F. Martin and Robert Freedman Molecular ‘‘Negativity’’ May Underlie Multiple
Sclerosis: Role of the Myelin Basic Protein
Histamine and Schizophrenia Family in the Pathogenesis of MS
Jean-Michel Arrang Abdiwahab A. Musse and George Harauz
Microchimerism and Stem Cell Transplantation
Cannabinoids and Psychosis
in Multiple Sclerosis
Deepak Cyril D’Souza
Behrouz Nikbin, Mandana Mohyeddin Bonab, and
Involvement of Neuropeptide Systems in Schizo- Fatemeh Talebian
phrenia: Human Studies
The Insulin-Like Growth Factor System in
Ricardo Cáceda, Becky Kinkead, and
Multiple Sclerosis
Charles B. Nemeroff
Daniel Chesik, Nadine Wilczak, and
Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor in Schizo- Jacques De Keyser
phrenia and Its Relation with Dopamine
Cell-Derived Microparticles and Exosomes in
Olivier Guillin, Caroline Demily, and
Neuroinflammatory Disorders
Florence Thibaut
Lawrence L. Horstman, Wenche Jy, Alireza Minagar,
Schizophrenia Susceptibility Genes: In Search of Carlos J. Bidot, Joaquin J. Jimenez,
a Molecular Logic and Novel Drug Targets for a J. Steven Alexander, and Yeon S. Ahn
Devastating Disorder
Joseph A. Gogos Multiple Sclerosis in Children: Clinical, Diagnostic,
and Therapeutic Aspects
index Kevin Rostásy
Risk and Predictability of Drug Interactions in The Role of Astrocytes and Complement System
the Elderly in Neural Plasticity
Milos Pekny, Ulrika Wilhelmsson, Yalda Rahpeymai
René H. Levy and Carol Collins
Bogestål, and Marcela Pekna
Outcomes in Elderly Patients With Newly
New Insights into the Roles of Metalloprotei-
Diagnosed and Treated Epilepsy
nases in Neurodegeneration and Neuroprotec-
Martin J. Brodie and Linda J. Stephen tion
Recruitment and Retention in Clinical Trials of A. J. Turner and N. N. Nalivaeva
the Elderly
Relevance of High-Mobility Group Protein
Flavia M. Macias, R. Eugene Ramsay, and A. James
Box 1 to Neurodegeneration
Rowan
Silvia Fossati and Alberto Chiarugi
Treatment of Convulsive Status Epilepticus
Early Upregulation of Matrix Metalloproteinases
David M. Treiman
Following Reperfusion Triggers Neuroinflam-
Treatment of Nonconvulsive Status Epilepticus matory Mediators in Brain Ischemia in Rat
Matthew C. Walker Diana Amantea, Rossella Russo, Micaela Gliozzi,
Vincenza Fratto, Laura Berliocchi, G. Bagetta,
Antiepileptic Drug Formulation and Treatment
G. Bernardi, and M. Tiziana Corasaniti
in the Elderly: Biopharmaceutical Consider-
ations The (Endo)Cannabinoid System in Multiple
Barry E. Gidal Sclerosis and Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis
Diego Centonze, Silvia Rossi, Alessandro Finazzi-
index Agrò, Giorgio Bernardi, and Mauro Maccarrone
Chemokines and Chemokine Receptors: Multi-
Volume 82 purpose Players in Neuroinflammation
Richard M. Ransohoff, LiPing Liu, and Astrid E.
Inflammatory Mediators Leading to Protein Cardona
Misfolding and Uncompetitive/Fast Off-Rate Systemic and Acquired Immune Responses in
Drug Therapy for Neurodegenerative Disorders Alzheimer’s Disease
Stuart A. Lipton, Zezong Gu, and Tomohiro Markus Britschgi and Tony Wyss-Coray
Nakamura
Neuroinflammation in Alzheimer’s Disease and
Innate Immunity and Protective Neuroinflam-
Parkinson’s Disease: Are Microglia Pathogenic in
mation: New Emphasis on the Role of Neuroim-
Either Disorder?
mune Regulatory Proteins
Joseph Rogers, Diego Mastroeni, Brian Leonard,
M. Griffiths, J. W. Neal, and P. Gasque
Jeffrey Joyce, and Andrew Grover
336 CONTENTS OF RECENT VOLUMES
Cytokines and Neuronal Ion Channels in Health Retinal Damage Caused by High Intraocular
and Disease Pressure-Induced Transient Ischemia is Pre-
Barbara Viviani, Fabrizio Gardoni, and Marina vented by Coenzyme Q10 in Rat
Marinovich Carlo Nucci, Rosanna Tartaglione, Angelica Cerulli,
Cyclooxygenase-2, Prostaglandin E2, and Micro- R. Mancino, A. Spanò, Federica Cavaliere, Laura
glial Activation in Prion Diseases Rombol, G. Bagetta, M. Tiziana Corasaniti, and
Luigi A. Morrone
Luisa Minghetti and Maurizio Pocchiari
Evidence Implicating Matrix Metalloproteinases
Glia Proinflammatory Cytokine Upregulation as in the Mechanism Underlying Accumulation of
a Therapeutic Target for Neurodegenerative IL-1" and Neuronal Apoptosis in the Neocortex
Diseases: Function-Based and Target-Based
of HIV/gp120-Exposed Rats
Discovery Approaches Rossella Russo, Elisa Siviglia, Micaela Gliozzi,
Linda J. Van Eldik, Wendy L. Thompson, Diana Amantea, Annamaria Paoletti,
Hantamalala Ralay Ranaivo, Heather A. Behanna,
Laura Berliocchi, G. Bagetta, and M.
and D. Martin Watterson
Tiziana Corasaniti
Oxidative Stress and the Pathogenesis of Neuro-
Neuroprotective Effect of Nitroglycerin in a
degenerative Disorders Rodent Model of Ischemic Stroke: Evaluation
Ashley Reynolds, Chad Laurie, R. Lee Mosley, and of Bcl-2 Expression
Howard E. Gendelman
Rosaria Greco, Diana Amantea, Fabio Blandini,
Differential Modulation of Type 1 and Type 2 Giuseppe Nappi, Giacinto Bagetta, M. Tiziana
Cannabinoid Receptors Along the Neuro- Corasaniti, and Cristina Tassorelli
immune Axis
Sergio Oddi, Paola Spagnuolo, Monica Bari, Antonella index
D’Agostino, and Mauro Maccarrone
Effects of the HIV-1 Viral Protein Tat on Cen- Volume 83
tral Neurotransmission: Role of Group I Meta-
botropic Glutamate Receptors Gender Differences in Pharmacological
Elisa Neri, Veronica Musante, and Anna Pittaluga Response
Gail D. Anderson
Evidence to Implicate Early Modulation of Inter-
leukin-1" Expression in the Neuroprotection Epidemiology and Classification of Epilepsy:
Afforded by 17"-Estradiol in Male Rats Under- Gender Comparisons
gone Transient Middle Cerebral Artery Occlusion John C. McHugh and Norman Delanty
Olga Chiappetta, Micaela Gliozzi, Elisa Siviglia, Hormonal Influences on Seizures:
Diana Amantea, Luigi A. Morrone, Laura Berliocchi, Basic Neurobiology
G. Bagetta, and M. Tiziana Corasaniti Cheryl A. Frye
A Role for Brain Cyclooxygenase-2 and Prosta- Catamenial Epilepsy
glandin-E2 in Migraine: Effects of Nitroglycerin Patricia E. Penovich and Sandra Helmers
Cristina Tassorelli, Rosaria Greco, Marie Therèse
Armentero, Fabio Blandini, Giorgio Sandrini, and Epilepsy in Women: Special Considerations
Giuseppe Nappi for Adolescents
Mary L. Zupanc and Sheryl Haut
The Blockade of K+-ATP Channels has Neuro-
protective Effects in an In Vitro Model of Brain Contraception in Women with Epilepsy:
Ischemia Pharmacokinetic Interactions, Contraceptive
Robert Nisticò, Silvia Piccirilli, L. Sebastianelli, Options, and Management
Giuseppe Nisticò, G. Bernardi, and N. B. Mercuri Caryn Dutton and Nancy Foldvary-Schaefer
CONTENTS OF RECENT VOLUMES 337