Jain 2019
Jain 2019
www.emeraldinsight.com/1940-5979.htm
Evaluation of
Evaluation of behavioral biases behavioral
affecting investment decision biases
Abstract
Purpose – Research in the area of behavioral finance has demonstrated that investors exhibit irrational behavior
while making investment decisions. Investor behavior usually deviates from logic and reason, and consequently,
investors exhibit various behavioral biases which impact their investment decisions. The purpose of this paper is
to rank the behavioral biases influencing the investment decision making of individual equity investors from the
state of Punjab, India. This research would provide valuable insight into the different behavioral biases to
investors and other participants of the capital market and help them in improving investment decisions.
Design/methodology/approach – The research is conducted on the individual equity investors of Punjab,
India. Fuzzy analytic hierarchy process was applied to rank the factors influencing the decision making of
individual equity investors of Punjab. The primary factors considered for the study are overconfidence bias,
representative bias, anchoring bias, availability bias, regret aversion bias, loss aversion bias, mental
accounting bias and herding bias.
Findings – The three most influential criteria were herding bias, loss aversion bias and overconfidence bias.
The five most influential sub-criteria were “I readily sell shares that have increased in value (C61),” “News
about the company (Newspapers, TV and magazines) affects my investment decision (C84),” “I invest each
element of my investment portfolio separately (C71)” and “I usually hold loosing stock for long time, expecting
trend reversal (C52).”
Research limitations/implications – Although sample survey conducted in the present study was based
on a limited sample selected from a particular area that truly represented the total population, it is considered
as the limitation of this study.
Practical implications – The outcome of this research provides investors with a better understanding of
behavioral biases that influence their decision making. This study provides them a guideline on different
behavioral biases that they should consider while making investment decisions.
Originality/value – The research model is based on the available literature on behavioral finance and the
research results and findings would add value to the existing knowledge base.
Keywords Investment decisions, Behavioural biases, Fuzzy analytical hierarchy process (F-AHP),
Irrational behaviour
Paper type Research paper
Overconfidence bias
Overconfidence is a cognitive bias. Overconfidence is the tendency of people to overestimate
own skills, cognitive abilities and precision of information (DeBondt and Thaler, 1995) for
achieving his/her goals by underestimating the future uncertainties. Overconfident people
believe that their judgment is more reliable than others ( Jain et al., 2015).
Representative bias
This bias is also known as familiarity bias. When there is a lack of information, neural
connections in the brain process information using shortcuts, for achieving desired objectives.
Information is usually processed on the basis of past experience. Individuals buying a house, Evaluation of
usually compare the price with prices of other houses in a similar location for accessing the behavioral
risk of the property and future value of the investment. biases
Anchoring bias
It is the tendency of the investors to consider logically the irrelevant price level of the stock
as a base while taking their decision. Investors suffering from this bias have a tendency of 299
fixing the price for buying and selling of shares based on past information. This way
investors may actually be timing badly and thus may buy shares at a high price or may sell
shares at a low price. It may also happen that the price which is fixed by the investors for
buying or selling the security, which may not be reached, may result in missing good
investment opportunities.
Availability bias
In availability bias, a decision maker relies upon readily available knowledge, rather than
examining other alternatives ( Javed et al., 2017). When decision makers give preference to
the recent events and information they have come across, they are believed to be suffering
from availability bias. Recent events, which were personally observed, are usually more
memorable. This is because memorable events are likely to be more magnified and cause an
emotional reaction. Investor preferences are based on available information and as a result
of even irrelevant information sometimes influences the investment decision (Steen, 2002).
Investors suffering from this bias usually invest in local stocks and also prefer to invest in
stocks evaluated by experts.
Representative bias
Representativeness is the extent to which the situations and instances are similar to the
population (DeBondt and Thaler, 1995). Tversky and Kahneman (1983) stated that people
usually predict the future value of a script based on representativeness. Representativeness is
generally employed, while making judgments under uncertainty. This leads the investors to
analyze the companies on the basis of its characteristics like its management, products,
publicity and returns, and investment is generally centered on these characteristics (Onsomu,
2014). Representativeness can lead toward the biases in decision making because of the reason
that due to representativeness individuals tend to value more recent events and overlook
long-term events (Ritter, 2003). Individuals suffering from representative bias, sometimes refer
to very few samples, resulting into sample size neglect (Luu, 2014). Grether (1980) and Chen
et al. (2007) mentioned that representativeness bias is more prominent in unsophisticated
investors. Javed et al. (2017) found that representative bias is having positive significant
impact on perceived investment performance.
Anchoring bias
Anchoring bias is also associated with representativeness because it states that the
investors’ decisions are based on recent experiences, and they are more optimistic during the
rising market trend and more pessimistic during the falling market trend (Waweru et al.,
2008). While making an investment, investors rely on anchors (initial reference point), e.g.
52 week low price of a share. Kristensen and Tommy (1997) established the hypothesis that
people use anchoring and adjustment process during negotiations for making counter
offers, and change in the reference point has an influence on these counter offers. Lee et al.
(2013) found that anchoring bias is more evident in females than males. The study
conducted by Kengatharan and Kengatharan (2014) found that anchoring has very high
impact on investors’ decision making.
Availability bias
The availability heuristics states that events which can be easily recalled are supposed to
occur with higher probability. Investor preference changes as per available information and
consequently sometimes irrelevant information influences the decision making (Harris and
Raviv, 2005). Waweru et al. (2008) found that availability bias affects the decision making of
institutional investors. In a study conducted by Javed et al. (2017), it was found that
availability bias is having positive significant impact on perceived investment performance
which is against the findings of Khan (2017) and Rehan and Umer (2017) showing that
availability bias is negatively related to investment decision making.
The criteria and sub-criteria defined for the study are listed in Figure 1.
Research methodology
In order to weight the criteria and sub-criteria identified in Figure 1, Fuzzy AHP is used.
Fuzzy AHP is a logical approach incorporating the concepts of fuzzy set theory and
hierarchical structure. Decision makers can express their inclinations by using verbal terms
or numerical values toward the significance of each criterion and sub-criterion (Ip et al.,
2012). The questionnaire was designed in the form of a pair-wise evaluation based on the
RBF I am Confident about my own ability to do better than others (C11)
12,3 Overconfidence I am Confident about time to entre in the market and exit from market (C12)
Bias I Possess Specific skills and experience for making investments (C13)
(C1)
I Possess Complete knowledge about various investment avenues (C14)
I am Satisfied regarding past investing decision making (C15)
302 I usually invest in familiar stocks (C21)
I evaluate Past Price trends for predicting future price (C22)
Representative
Bias I buy hot stocks and avoid stocks having poor performance in past (C23)
(C2)
I buy stocks on the basis of present performance (C24)
I buy the new equity offering of the same company, in which I have already
invested. (C25)
I usually rely on Past experience in the market for next investment (C31)
Anchoring I usually buy stocks, which have fallen considerably from previous closing or
Bias all time high (C32)
Behavioral biases affecting
(C3) I usually consider the purchase price of stock as reference point for trading
Investment Decision
(C33)
I usually forecast the future stock prices based on recent stock price (C34)
hierarchical structure shown in Figure 1. The primary data have been collected through the
structured questionnaire from the individual equity investors, who reside in the state of
Punjab. Universe for the study is Punjab state and population for the study is individual
equity investors, who invest in the capital market. Data have been collected from 165
individual equity investors by applying the snow ball sampling method.
F-AHP inserts the fuzzy concept to basic analytic hierarchy process (AHP), which was
established by Saaty, 1980. AHP is a method for multi-criteria decision making that helps
decision makers to select a choice between alternatives. The AHP mathematical model is a
technique that generated weighting significance (Gupta et al., 2018). An alternative of the
AHP, called FAHP, is applied in order to overcome the compensatory style and the
The inverse of
NPCM Fuzzy triangular fuzzy triangular
(Saaty scale) Linguistic scale for Importance scale scale
M l(y) M r(y)
Figure 2.
Triangular
membership function
x of fuzzy numbers
0 l m u
RBF incapability of the AHP in handling linguistic variables. F-AHP has become the projecting
12,3 tool for handling inaccuracy or ambiguity aiming at tractability, heftiness, and low-cost
solutions for real-world problems. According to Ip et al. (2012), human findings are often
ambiguous and unclear; therefore, the use of fuzzy analytical hierarchy process theory,
rather than precise numbers, enables us to capture decision-makers’ uncertainty. Kilincci
and Onal (2011) said that AHP does not include ambiguity for personal judgments; it has
304 been amended by benefiting from the fuzzy logic approach. In F-AHP, the pairwise
comparisons of both criteria and the sub-criteria are performed through the linguistic
variables, which are represented by triangular numbers (as shown in Table I). Kabir and
Hasin (2012) mentioned that it is difficult to reflect the decision makers’ ambiguous
preferences through crisp values. Therefore, the FAHP is suggested to relieve the vagueness
of the AHP method, where the fuzzy evaluations ratios are used.
According to Table I, linguistic terms and the corresponding triangular fuzzy numbers, if
the decision maker states “Criteria 1 (C1) is strongly more important than Criteria 2 (C2),”
then it takes the fuzzy triangular scale as (4, 5, 6). On the opposing, in the fuzzy pairwise
comparison matrix of the criteria, the evaluation of C2 to C1 will take the fuzzy triangular
scale as (1/6, 1/5, 1/4).
According to Kahraman et al. (2003), Ml(y) and Mr(y) indicate the left side symbol and the
right side symbol of a fuzzy number, respectively.
The algorithm for evaluating behavioral biases affecting investment decision by the
fuzzy analytical hierarchy process is summarized as follows:
• Step 1: define the evaluative behavioral biases criteria and sub-criteria for investment
decisions.
• Step 2: to develop a hierarchical structure, the decision makers are asked to determine
the relative weights of each criterion.
• Step 3: develop, normal pair-wise comparison matrix (NPCM) as per Saaty’s (1980)
scale by applying AHP for finding out the weights of each criterion and sub-criterion.
The NPCM is presented in Equation (2).
• Step 4: after setting up the order and normal pair-wise comparison matrices for all the
criteria, values for all the criterion are find out by using fuzzy AHP (as shown in
Equation (3)).
Normal Pair-Wise Comparison Matrix:
2 3
a11 a1n
6 ^ & ^ 7
4 5: (2)
a31 a3n
Fuzzy Pair-Wise Comparison Matrix:
2 3
a11l a11m a11u a1nl a1nm a1nu
6 7
4 ^ & ^ 5: (3)
a31l a31m a31u a3nl a3nm a3nu
• Step 5: if there is more than one decision maker, preferences of each decision maker
are consolidate as shown in the following equation:
n o 1X k n o
aij ¼ Mink akij ; bij ¼ bkij ; cij ¼ Maxk ckij : (4)
k k¼I
• Step 6: the geometric mean of fuzzy comparison values of each criterion and Evaluation of
sub-criterion is calculated (Buckley, 1985) (as shown in the following equations): behavioral
~ A2
A1 ~ An
~ ¼ fl1 m1 u1g fl2 m2 u2g fln mn ung biases
¼ ðl1 l2 lnÞ; ðm1 m2 mnÞ; ðu1 u2 unÞ: (5)
l W~ i þmW~ i þuW~ i
ðW iÞ ¼ : (8)
3
• Step 9: (Wi) obtained is a non-fuzzy number; therefore, it is to be normalized by the
following equation:
ðW i Þ
ðN wiÞ ¼ P : (9)
ðwiÞ
These nine steps are executed to find the normalized weights of both criteria and sub-
criteria. Then, by multiplying each sub-criterion weight with related criterion, the weight for
each alternative is calculated.
Research findings
After the assessment of pair-wise comparison matrix among the criteria of behavioral
biases, the fuzzy AHP method was implemented. The crisp values are converted to the fuzzy
pair-wise comparison matrix as shown in the calculation steps of Fuzzy AHP. According to
Step 5 (presented above), Tables II and III demonstrate the aggregate fuzzy pair-wise
comparison matrices related to criteria and sub-criteria.
Results of the present study are consistent with the available literature which suggests
that different behavioral biases impact the decision making of individual equity investors,
globally. Present research has applied fuzzy AHP to conclude the relative importance of
criteria affecting behavioral biases. With respect to the significance/weights for the criteria
Indicators C1 C2 C3 C4 C5 C6 C7 C8
C1 1, 1, 1 1/6, 43/8, 9 1/9, 7/4, 6 1/9, 3, 9 1/6, 1/7, 1/8 1/6, 17/7, 8 1/6, 43/8, 9 1/9, 33/7, 8
C2 1/9, 17/7, 6 1, 1, 1 1/9, 3/4, 3 1/4, 2/7, 1/4 1/9, 1/6, 1/6 1/4, 8/7, 4 1/6, 33/7, 8 1/9, 1/6, 1/6
C3 1/6, 6, 9 1/3, 31/6, 9 1, 1, 1 1/6, 27/8, 6 1/6, 1/7, 1/8 1/9, 7/5, 5 1/6, 1/6, 1/6 1/9, 1/6, 1/6 Table II.
C4 1/9, 40/7, 9 4, 11/3, 4 1/6, 5/2, 6 1, 1, 1 1/9, 1/6, 1/6 1/6, 1/7, 1/8 1/6, 27/8, 6 1/9, 12/5, 8 Aggregate fuzzy
C5 8, 7, 6 6, 19/3, 9 8, 7, 6 6, 19/3, 9 1, 1, 1 1/9, 1/9, 1/9 1/2, 25/9, 6 1/9, 1, 4 pairwise comparison
C6 1/8, 33/7, 6 1/4, 31/9, 4 1/5, 6, 9 8, 7, 6 9, 9, 9 1, 1, 1 1/6, 33/7, 8 1/6, 13/9, 5 matrix of various
C7 1/9, 17/7, 6 1/8, 17/7, 6 6, 17/3, 6 1/6, 5/2, 6 1/6, 7/6, 2 1/8, 17/7, 6 1, 1, 1 1/9, 1/8, 1/8 factors affecting
C8 1/8, 3, 9 6, 19/3, 9 6, 19/3, 9 1/8, 6, 9 1/4, 49/9, 9 1/5, 4, 6 8, 25/3, 9 1, 1, 1 investment decision
RBF level shown in Table IV, among the criteria of behavioral biases affecting
12,3 investment decision, “Herding” was ranked as highest (Weight ¼ 0.255), followed by
“Loss Aversion” (Weight ¼ 0.203), “Overconfidence Bias” (Weight ¼ 0.140), “Regret
Aversion” (Weight ¼ 0.127), “Mental Accounting” (Weight ¼ 0.099), “Availability Bias”
(Weight ¼ 0.077), “Anchoring Bias” (Weight ¼ 0.052) and “Representative Bias”
(Weight ¼ 0.046). The radar chart indicating the distribution of weight values for the
306 eight main behavioral biases is shown in Figure 3. Through arranging, each criterion and
sub-criterion were allotted a local weight, but to understand well the ranking of criteria and
sub-criteria, global significance and global ranking were calculated by multiplying each
sub-criterion weight with the leading criteria weight. Figure 4 represents that weight score
of sub-criteria of behavioral biases affecting the investment decision. According to the
global ranking of behavioral biases sub-criteria, decision makers considered “I readily sell
shares that have increased in value (C61),” “I seek advice from brokers, while investing
(C81),” and “News about the company (Newspapers, TV and magazines) affects my
investment decision (C84)” as the most important sub-criterion affecting investment
decisions. “I buy the new equity offering of the same company, in which I have already
invested (C25),” “I usually invest in familiar stocks (C21),” “I usually forecast the future stock
prices based on recent stock price (C34)” were observed as the least important behavioral
biases sub-criteria. The impact of different behavioral biases has been shown in terms of
percentage ranging from 0.33 to 17.50 percent. Separate colors have been used (Figure 4) in
order to differentiate one criterion from the other. I seek advice from brokers, while
investing (C81) ranked 2 in the global ranking whereas effect of News about the company
(Newspapers, TV and magazines) affects my investment decision (C84) ranked 3. Both of
these variables represent herding bias, which states that the investor’s do not think
independently while taking investment decision rather depend on the opinion of brokers
and news from various media. In the present study herding, overconfidence and loss
aversion bias have emerged as important behavioral biases impacting investors decision
making. In few studies, overconfidence bias has been ranked as (no. 1) behavioral bias
affecting investment decision (Antony and Joseph, 2017), whereas in the present study
herding bias has emerged as most influential behavioral bias (ranked no. 1) followed by
overconfidence (ranked no. 2) and loss aversion bias (ranked no. 3).
Conclusion
Unlike previous studies, this study has used inter-disciplinary approach for evaluating
behavioral biases. Present study has adopted the MCDM technique for evaluating the
behavioral biases affecting investment decisions. Fuzzy AHP has been applied for evaluating
behavioral biases affecting individual equity investor’s decision making. By integrating the
relevant literature review, 23 indicators were selected as being suitable for the behavioral
biases affecting equity investor’s decision making that were grouped into eight behavioral
biases, namely, overconfidence bias, representative bias, anchoring bias, availability bias,
regret aversion bias, loss aversion bias, mental accounting bias and herding bias. Results of
the fuzzy AHP revealed that herding bias, loss aversion bias, overconfidence bias, and the
Figure 3.
The radar chart Availability
Loss Aversion
Bias
indicating the 0.203
0.077
distribution of
weight values Regret Aversion
0.127
regret aversion are the most important criteria that affects the decision making of individual
equity investors. Results of the study can benefit the equity market participants and other
decision makers while taking an investment decision.
This research has also been confined to the eight behavioral biases affecting investor’s
decision making. The scope of the study can be enhanced by taking more behavioral biases
and expanding the geographical area beyond the state of Punjab. Another limitation of this
study was the use of a snow ball sampling approach for data collection.
References
Antony, A. and Joseph, A.I. (2017), “Influence of behavioural factors affecting investment decision – an
AHP Analysis”, Metamorphosis, Vol. 16 No. 2, pp. 107-114.
Barber, M. and Odean, T. (2001), “Boys will be boys: gender, overconfidence, and common stock
investment”, The Quarterly Journal of Economics, Vol. 116 No. 1, pp. 261-292.
Barberis, N. and Huang, M. (2001), “Mental accounting, loss aversion, and individual stock returns”,
Journal of Finance, Vol. 56 No. 4, pp. 1247-1292, available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-1082.00367
Bashir, T., Azam, N., Butt, A.A., Javed, A. and Tanvir, A. (2013), “Are behavioral biases influenced by
demographic characteristics & personality traits? Evidence from Pakistan”, European Scientific
Journal, Vol. 9 No. 29, pp. 277-293.
Blavatskyy, P. and Pogrebna, G. (2008), “Risk aversion when gains are likely and unlikely: evidence
from a natural experiment with large stakes”, Theory and Decision, Vol. 64 Nos 2-3, pp. 395-420,
available at: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11238-007-9056-0
RBF Buckley, J.J. (1985), “Fuzzy hierarchical analysis”, Fuzzy Sets and Systems, Vol. 17 No. 3, pp. 233-247,
12,3 available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-0114(85)90090-9
Caparrelli, F., D’Arcangelis, A.M. and Cassuto, A. (2004), “Herding in the Italian stock market: a case of
behavioral finance”, Journal of Behavioral Finance, Vol. 5 No. 4, pp. 222-230.
Chan, F.T.S. and Kumar, N. (2007), “Global supplier development considering risk factors using fuzzy
extended AHP-based approach”, Omega, Vol. 35 No. 4, pp. 417-431, available at: https://doi.org/
310 10.1016/j.omega.2005.08.004
Chandra, A. (2008), “Decision making in the stock market: incorporating psychology with finance”,
National Conference: FFMI 2008 IIT Kharagpur.
Chandra, P. (2016), Behavioural Finance, 1st ed., McGraw-Hill, Noida.
Chen, G., Kim, K.A., Nofsinger, J.R. and Rui, O.M. (2007), “Trading performance, disposition effect,
overconfidence, representativeness bias, and experience of emerging market investors”,
Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, Vol. 451, February, pp. 425-451, available at: https://doi.
org/10.1002/bdm
Daniel, K., Hirshleifer, D. and Subrahmanyam, A. (1998), “Investor psychology and security market
under- and overreactions”, The Journal of Finance, Vol. LIII No. 6, pp. 1839-1885.
De Bondt, W. and Thaler, R.H. (1995), “Financial decision-making in markets and Örms: a behavioral
perspective”, in Jarrow, R.A., Maksimovic, V. and Ziemba, W.T. (Eds), Finance, Handbooks in
Operations Research and Management Science, Vol. 9, North Holland, Amsterdam, pp. 385-410.
Dennis, P.J. and Strickland, D. (2002), “Who blinks in volatile markets, individuals or institutions ?”,
The Journal of Finance, Vol. LVII No. 5, pp. 1923-1949.
Grether, D.M. (1980), “Bayes rule as a descriptive model: the representativeness heuristic”,
The Quarterly Journal of Economics, Vol. 95 No. 3, pp. 537-557, available at: https://doi.org/10.2
307/1885092
Grinblatt, M. and Han, B. (2005), “Prospect theory, mental accounting, and momentum”, Journal of Financial
Economics, Vol. 78 No. 2, pp. 311-339, available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfineco.2004.10.006
Grinblatt, M. and Keloharju, M. (2009), “Sensation seeking, overconfidence, and trading activity”,
The Journal of Finance, Vol. LXIV No. 2, pp. 549-578.
Gupta, S., Dawar, V. and Goyal, A. (2018), “Enhancing the placement value of professionally qualified
students in marketing: an application of the analytic hierarchy process”, Academy of Marketing
Studies Journal, Vol. 22 No. 3, pp. 1-10.
Harris, M. and Raviv, A. (2005), “Allocation of decision-making authority”, Review of Finance, Vol. 9
No. 3, pp. 1-47.
Hassan, T.R., Khalid, W. and Habib, A. (2014), “Overconfidence and loss aversion in investment
decisions: a study of the impact of gender and age in Pakistani perspective”, Research Journal of
Finance and Accounting, Vol. 5 No. 11, pp. 148-158.
Ip, C., Law, R. and Lee, H.A. (2012), “The evaluation of hotel website functionality by fuzzy analytic
hierarchy process”, Journal of Travel and Tourism Marketing, Vol. 29 No. 3, pp. 263-278.
Jain, R., Jain, P. and Jain, C. (2015), “Behavioral biases in the decision making of individual investors”,
IUP Journal of Management Research, Vol. 14 No. 3, pp. 7-27, available at: https://search.
proquest.com/docview/1703647114?accountid=32499
Javed, H., Bagh, T. and Razzaq, S. (2017), “Herding effects, over confidence, availability bias and
representativeness as behavioral determinants of perceived investment performance: an
empirical evidence from Pakistan stock exchange (PSX )”, Journal of Global Economics, Vol. 6
No. 1, pp. 1-13, available at: https://doi.org/10.4172/2375-4389.1000275
Kabir, G. and Hasin, A.A.M. (2012), “Modified Fuzzy analytical hierarchy process for multiple criteria
inventory classification”, Suranaree Journal of Science and Technology, Vol. 18 No. 4, pp. 255-265.
Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J.L. and Thaler, R.H. (1991), “Anomalies the endowment effect, loss aversion ,
and status quo bias”, Journal of Economic Perspective, Vol. 5 No. 1, pp. 193-206.
Kahraman, C., Cebeci, U. and Ulukan, Z. (2003), “Multi-criteria supplier selection using Fuzzy AHP”, Evaluation of
Logistics Information Management, Vol. 16 No. 6, pp. 382-394, available at: https://doi.org/10.110 behavioral
8/09576050310503367
biases
Kaufmann, A. and Gupta, M.M. (1991), Introduction to Fuzzy Arithmetic: Theory and Application,
International Thomson Computer Press, London, available at: www.find.org.tw/find/home.aspx
Kengatharan, L. and Kengatharan, N. (2014), “The influence of behavioral factors in making
investment decisions and performance: study on investors of Colombo Stock Exchange, 311
Sri Lanka”, Asian Journal of Finance & Accounting, Vol. 6 No. 1, pp. 1-23, available at: https://doi.
org/10.5296/ajfa.v6i1.4893
Khan, M.Z.U. (2017), “Impact of availability bias and loss aversion bias on investment decision making,
moderating role of risk perception”, Journal of Modern Developments in General Management &
Administration, Vol. 1 No. 1, pp. 17-28.
Kilincci, O. and Onal, S.A. (2011), “Fuzzy AHP approach for supplier selection in a washing machine
company”, Expert Systems with Applications, Vol. 38 No. 8, pp. 9656-9664.
Kristensen, H. and Tommy, G. (1997), “Acta psychologica adoption of cognitive reference points in
negotiations”, Acta Psychologica, Vol. 97, pp. 277-288.
Kumar, S. and Goyal, N. (2015), “Behavioral biases in investment decision making – a systematic
literature review”, Qualitative Research in Financial Markets, Vol. 7 No. 1, pp. 88-108, available
at: https://search.proquest.com/docview/1647135679?accountid=32499
Lee, K., Miller, S., Velasquez, N. and Wann, C. (2013), “The effect of investor bias and gender on”,
The International Journal of Business and Finance Research, Vol. 7 No. 1, pp. 1-16.
Lee, Y., Liu, Y. and Roll, R. (2004), “Order imbalances and market efficiency: evidence from the Taiwan
stock exchange”, Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, Vol. 39 No. 2, pp. 327-341.
Lim, L.C. (2012), “The relationship between psychological biases and the decision making of investor in
Malaysian share market”, unpublished Paper, International Conference on Management,
Economics & Finance (ICMEF 2012) Proceeding.
Luong, L.P. and Hu, D.T.T. (2011), “Behavioral factors influencing individual investors’ decision
making and performance: a survey at the ho chi minh stock exchange”, master thesis, Umeâ
School of Business, pp. 1‐114.
Luu, T.B. (2014), “Behavior pattern of individual investors in stock market”, International Journal of
Business and Management, Vol. 9 No. 1, pp. 1-16.
Muermann, A. and Volkman, J. (2006), “Regret, pride, and the disposition effect”, PARC Working Paper
Series WPS 06-07, pp. 1‐26, available at: https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.930675
Nofsinger, J.R. and Sias, R.W. (1999), “Herding and feedback trading by institutional and individual
investors”, The Journal of Finance, Vol. LIV No. 6, pp. 2263-2295.
Odean, T. (1999), “Do investors trade too much?”, The American Economic Review, Vol. 89 No. 5,
pp. 1279-1298, available at: https://search.proquest.com/docview/233034236?accountid=32499
Onsomu, Z.N. (2014), “The impact of behavioural biases on investor decisions in Kenya: male vs
female”, Impact Journals, Vol. 2 No. 6, pp. 87-92.
Rehan, R. and Umer, I. (2017), “Behavioral biases and investor decisions”, Market Forces, Vol. 12 No. 2,
pp. 12-20.
Ritter, J.R. (2003), “Differences between European and American IPO markets”, European Financial
Management, Vol. 9 No. 4, pp. 421-434, available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-036X.00230
Saaty, T.L. (1980), The Analytic Hierarchy Process, McGraw-Hill, New York, NY.
Shefrin, H. and Statman, M. (1985), “The disposition to sell winners too early and ride losers too long”,
Journal of Finance, Vol. 40, pp. 777-792.
Statman, M., Thorley, S. and Vorkink, K. (2006), “Investor overconfidence and trading volume”,
The Review of Financial Studies, Vol. V19 No. 4, pp. 1531-1565, available at: https://doi.org/10.10
93/rfs/hhj032
RBF Steen, E.V. (2002), “Skill or luck? Biases of rational agents”, MIT Solan Working Paper No. 4255-02,
12,3 pp. 1‐39.
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1983), ProbabilisticReasoning, Vol. 1131 No. 1974, pp. 1124-1131.
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1992), “Advances in prospect theory: cumulative representation of
uncertainty”, Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, Vol. 5 No. 4, pp. 297-323, available at: https://doi.
org/10.1007/BF00122574
312 Waweru, N.M., Munyoki, E. and Uliana, E. (2008), “The effects of behavioural factors in investment
decision-making: a survey of institutional investors operating at the Nairobi stock exchange”,
International Journal of Business and Emerging Markets, Vol. 1 No. 1, pp. 24-41.
Wu, Y.-L., Chang, M.C.S. and Yang, P.-C. (2008), “Using fuzzy AHP to evaluate the content of 3G mobile
communication value-added service”, Journal of Statistics and Management Systems, Vol. 11
No. 5, pp. 951-979, available at: www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/09720510.2008.10701352
Zadeh, A. (1965), “Fuzzy sets”, Information and Control, Vol. 8, pp. 338-353.
Zaidi, F.B. and Tauni, M.Z. (2012), “Influence of investor’s personality traits and demographics on
overconfidence bias”, Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research, Vol. 4 No. 6, pp. 730-746.
Further reading
Agnew, J.R. (2006), “Do behavioral biases vary across individuals? Evidence from individual level 401
(k) data”, Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, Vol. 41 No. 4, pp. 939-962, available at:
https://doi.org/10.1017/s0022109000002702
Antonides, G. and Van Der Sar, N.L. (1990), “Individual expectations, risk perception and preferances in
relation to investment decision making”, Journal of Economic Psychology, Vol. 11 No. 2, pp. 227-245.
Barber, B.M. and Odean, T. (2000), “Trading is hazardous to your wealth: the common stock
investment performance of individual investors”, The Journal of Finance, Vol. 55 No. 2,
pp. 773-806, available at: https://search.proquest.com/docview/194716328?accountid=32499
Berkelaar, A.B., Kouwenberg, R. and Post, T. (2004), “Optimal portfolio choice under loss aversion”,
Review of Economics and Statistics, Vol. 86 No. 4, pp. 973-987, available at: https://doi.org/10.
1162/0034653043125167
Bloomfield, R.J. (2009), “Traditional vs behavioral finance”, Johnson School Research Paper Series
No. 222010, Vol. 55 No. 3, pp. 348-361.
Bosch-Domènech, A. and Silvestre, J. (2010), “Averting risk in the face of large losses: Bernoulli vs
Tversky and Kahneman”, Economics Letters, Vol. 107 No. 2, pp. 180-182.
Boulding, W., Kalra, A., Staelin, R. and Zeithaml, V.A. (1993), “Journal of marketing research”,
Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 30 No. 1, pp. 7-27.
Bromiley, P. (1987), “Do forecasts produced by organizations reflect anchoring and adjustment?”, Journal
of Forecasting, Vol. 6 No. 3, pp. 201-210, available at: https://doi.org/10.1002/for.3980060306
Butt, M., Jamil, N. and Nawaz, R. (2015), “The mediating role of risk perception among cognitive biases
towards decision to start a new venture”, International Letters of Social and Humanistic Sciences,
Vol. 54 No. 6, pp. 88-95, available at: https://doi.org/10.18052/www.scipress.com/ilshs.54.88
Craig, E. (2006), “Behavioral finance: back on the road again”, Journal of Behavioral Finance, Vol. 7
No. 1, pp. 58-58, available at: https://doi.org/10.1207/s15427579jpfm0701
Dhar, R. and Zhu, N. (2006), “Up close and personal: investor sophistication and the disposition effect”,
Management Science, Vol. 52 No. 5, pp. 726-740, available at: https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.1040.0473
Filip, A., Pochea, M. and Pece, A. (2015), “The herding behaviour of investors in the CEE stocks
markets”, Procedia Economics and Finance, Vol. 32 No. 15, pp. 307-315.
Fisher, K.L. and Statman, M. (2002), “Blowing bubbles”, Journal of Psychology and Financial Markets,
Vol. 3, pp. 53-65.
Garcia-Retamero, R. and Cokely, E.T. (2014), “The influence of skills, message frame, and visual aids on Evaluation of
prevention of sexually transmitted diseases”, The Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, Vol. 27 behavioral
No. 1, pp. 179-189.
biases
Genesove, D. and Mayer, C. (2010), “Loss aversion and seller behaviour: evidence from the housing
market”, The Quarterly Journal of Economics, Vol. 116, No. 4, pp. 1233-1260.
Gervais, S. and Odean, T. (2001), “Learning to be overconfident”, Review of Financial Studies, Vol. 14
No. 1, pp. 1-27. 313
Grether, D.M. (1992), “Testing bayes rule and the representativeness heuristic: some experimental
evidence”, Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, Vol. 17 No. 1, pp. 31-57, available at:
https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-2681(92)90078-P
Grinblatt, M. and Keloharju, M. (2000), “The investment behaviour and performance of various investor
types: a study of Finland’s unique data set”, Journal of Financial Economics, Vol. 55, pp. 43-67.
Hamid, F.S. and Rangel, G.J. (2013), “The relationship between risk propensity, risk perception and
risk-taking behaviour in an emerging market”, The International Journal of Banking and
Finance, Vol. 10 No. 1, pp. 134-146.
Hirshleifer, D. and Hong Teoh, S. (2003), “Herd behaviour and cascading in capital markets: a review
and synthesis”, European Financial Management, Vol. 9 No. 1, pp. 25-66.
Hsin-Hue, C. (2010), “An investigation of psychological factors influencing investment decision
making”, Indonesian Capital Market Review, Vol. 2 No. 2, pp. 73-86.
Hutton, J.G., Manager, P. and Vista, C. (1993), Abstract, No. 2, pp. 623-632.
Hvide, H.K. (2002), “Pragmatic beliefs and overconfidence”, Journal of Economic Behaviour and
Organization, Vol. 48 No. 1, pp. 15-28.
Immanuel, D. and Selvaraj, M. (2016), “Asian research consortium a study on entrepreneurial qualities”,
Orientation and Self-Employment Intentions among Students, Vol. 6 No. 7, pp. 1753-1756.
Jenna, F. (2012), “A study of gender effects on investing decisions”.
Kallinterakis, V., Munir, N. and Radovic-Markovic, M. (2010), “Herd behaviour, illiquidity and extreme
market states: evidence from Banja Luka”, Journal of Emerging Market Finance, Vol. 9 No. 3,
pp. 305-324.
Keil, M., Wallace, L., Turk, D., Dixon-Randall, G. and Nulden, U. (2000), “Investigation of risk perception
and risk propensity on the decision to continue a software development project”, Journal of
Systems and Software, Vol. 53 No. 2, pp. 145-157.
Kirchler, E., Maciejovsky, B., Weber, M., Kirchler, E., Maciejovsky, B. and Weber, M. (2005),
“Framing effects, selective information, and market behaviour: an experimental analysis”, Vol. 6
No. 2, pp. 90-100.
Kübilay, B. and Bayrakdaroglu, A. (2016), “An empirical research on investor biases in financial
decision-making, financial risk tolerance and financial personality”, International Journal of
Financial Research, Vol. 7 No. 171, pp. 1923-4023.
Kumar, S. and Goyal, N. (2016), “Evidence on rationality and behavioral biases in investment decision
making”, Qualitative Research in Financial Markets, Vol. 8 No. 4, pp. 270-287, available at: https://
doi.org/10.1108/QRFM-05-2016-0016
Lao, P. and Singh, H. (2011), “Herding behaviour in the Chinese and Indian stock markets”, Journal of
Asian Economics, Vol. 22 No. 6, pp. 495-506.
Massa, M. and Simonov, A. (2005), “Behavioral biases and investment”, Review of Finance, Vol. 9 No. 4,
pp. 483-507.
Mbaluka, P., Muthama, C. and Kalunda, E. (2012), “Prospect theory: test on framing and loss aversion
effects on investors decision-making process at the Nairobi Securities Exchange”, Kenya, Vol. 3
No. 9, pp. 31-42.
Nofsinger, J.R. (2003), “Social mood and financial economics”, The Journal of Behavioral Finance, Vol. 6
No. 3, pp. 129-143.
RBF Pompian, M. (2006), Behavioral Finance and Wealth Management: How to Build Optimal Portfolios for
12,3 Private Clients, John Wiley Sons, New York, NY.
Rau, H.A. (2014), “The disposition effect and loss aversion: do gender differences matter?”, Economics
Letters, Vol. 123 No. 1, pp. 33-36.
Riaz, L. and Hunjra, A.I. (2015), “Relationship between psychological factors and investment decision
making: the mediating role of risk perception”, Pakistan Journal of Commerce and Social
Sciences, Vol. 9 No. 3, pp. 968-981.
314
Riaz, L., Hunjra, A.I. and Rawalpondi, P. (2012), “Impact of psychological factors on investment
decision making mediating by risk perception: a conceptual study”, Middle-East Journal of
Scientific Research, Vol. 12 No. 6, pp. 789-795.
Ricciardi, V. (2008), “The psychology of risk: thebehavioral finance perspective”, The Handbook of
Finance, pp. 85-111.
Salem, R. (2017), “Evaluating the investment behaviour of women in Arab Capital Markets: a case
study of Saudi Arabia and Jordan”, A Thesis in partial fulfilment of the requirement of Anglia
Ruskin University for the degree of a Doctor of Philosophy in Behavioural Finance, April.
Shefrin, H. (2002), Beyond Greed and Fear: Understanding Behavioral Finance and the Psychology of
Investing, Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Shiller, R.J. (2000), “Measuring bubble expectations and investor confidence”, Journal of Psychology and
Financial Markets, Vol. 1, pp. 49-60.
Sindhu, K. and Kumar, R. (2014), “Influence of risk perception of investors on investment decisions: an
empirical analysis”, Journal of Finance and Bank Management, Vol. 2 No. 2, pp. 15-25.
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1991), “Loss aversion in riskless choice: a reference-dependent model”,
The Quarterly Journal of Economics, Vol. 106 No. 4, pp. 1039-1061.
Wang, W., Zhao, J., Zhang, W. and Wang, Y. (2015), “Conceptual framework for risk propensity,
risk perception, and risk behaviour of construction project managers”, in Raidén, A.B. and
Aboagye-Nimo, E. (Eds), Procs 31st Annual ARCOM Conference, Association of Researchers in
Construction Management, Lincoln, pp. 165-174.
Weber, M. and Camerer, C.F. (1998), “The disposition effect in securities trading: an experimental
analysis”, Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, Vol. 33 No. 2, pp. 167-184, available at:
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-2681(97)00089-9
Wen, Y.-F. (2010), “Capital investment decision, corporate governance, and prospect theory”, Procedia –
Social and Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 5 No. 2, pp. 116-126.
Zhao, D. and Fang, Y. (2014), “Representation bias, return forecast, and portfolio selection in the stock
market of China”, Mathematical Problems in Engineering, Vol. 2014 No. 1, pp. 1-8, available at:
https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/686201
Corresponding author
Sanjay Gupta can be contacted at: [email protected]
For instructions on how to order reprints of this article, please visit our website:
www.emeraldgrouppublishing.com/licensing/reprints.htm
Or contact us for further details: [email protected]