Conditioning and Learning
Conditioning and Learning
Learning
By Mark E. Bouton
University of Vermont
窗体顶端
Share this URL
窗体底端
Associative learning
Classical conditioning
Instrumental learning
Learning theory
Operant conditioning
Pavlovian learning
Learning Objectives
Distinguish between classical (Pavlovian) conditioning and instrumental (operant)
conditioning.
Understand some important facts about each that tell us how they work.
Understand how they work separately and together to influence human behavior in the
world outside the laboratory.
Students will be able to list the four aspects of observational learning according to Social
Learning Theory.
Although Ivan Pavlov won a Nobel Prize for studying digestion, he is much more
famous for something else: working with a dog, a bell, and a bowl of saliva. Many
people are familiar with the classic study of “Pavlov’s dog,” but rarely do they
understand the significance of its discovery. In fact, Pavlov’s work helps explain why
some people get anxious just looking at a crowded bus, why the sound of a morning
alarm is so hated, and even why we swear off certain foods we’ve only tried once.
Classical (or Pavlovian) conditioning is one of the fundamental ways we learn about
the world around us. But it is far more than just a theory of learning; it is also
arguably a theory of identity. For, once you understand classical conditioning, you’ll
recognize that your favorite music, clothes, even political candidate, might all be a
result of the same process that makes a dog drool at the sound of bell.
The Pavlov in All of Us: Does your dog learn to beg for food
because you reinforce her by feeding her from the table? [Image:
David Mease, https://goo.gl/R9cQV7, CC BY-NC
2.0, https://goo.gl/FIlc2e]
Around the turn of the 20th century, scientists who were interested in understanding
the behavior of animals and humans began to appreciate the importance of two very
basic forms of learning. One, which was first studied by the Russian physiologist Ivan
Pavlov, is known as classical, or Pavlovian conditioning. In his famous experiment,
Pavlov rang a bell and then gave a dog some food. After repeating this pairing
multiple times, the dog eventually treated the bell as a signal for food, and began
salivating in anticipation of the treat. This kind of result has been reproduced in the
lab using a wide range of signals (e.g., tones, light, tastes, settings) paired with many
different events besides food (e.g., drugs, shocks, illness; see below).
We now believe that this same learning process is engaged, for example, when
humans associate a drug they’ve taken with the environment in which they’ve taken
it; when they associate a stimulus (e.g., a symbol for vacation, like a big beach towel)
with an emotional event (like a burst of happiness); and when they associate the flavor
of a food with getting food poisoning. Although classical conditioning may seem
“old” or “too simple” a theory, it is still widely studied today for at least two reasons:
First, it is a straightforward test of associative learning that can be used to study other,
more complex behaviors. Second, because classical conditioning is always occurring
in our lives, its effects on behavior have important implications for understanding
normal and disordered behavior in humans.
Another example you are probably very familiar with involves your alarm clock. If
you’re like most people, waking up early usually makes you unhappy. In this case,
waking up early (US) produces a natural sensation of grumpiness (UR). Rather than
waking up early on your own, though, you likely have an alarm clock that plays a tone
to wake you. Before setting your alarm to that particular tone, let’s imagine you had
neutral feelings about it (i.e., the tone had no prior meaning for you). However, now
that you use it to wake up every morning, you psychologically “pair” that tone (CS)
with your feelings of grumpiness in the morning (UR). After enough pairings, this
tone (CS) will automatically produce your natural response of grumpiness (CR).
Thus, this linkage between the unconditioned stimulus (US; waking up early) and the
conditioned stimulus (CS; the tone) is so strong that the unconditioned response (UR;
being grumpy) will become a conditioned response (CR; e.g., hearing the tone at any
point in the day—whether waking up or walking down the street—will make you
grumpy). Modern studies of classical conditioning use a very wide range of CSs and
USs and measure a wide range of conditioned responses.
Now, once the rat recognizes that it receives a piece of food every time it presses the
lever, the behavior of lever-pressing becomes reinforced. That is, the food pellets
serve as reinforcers because they strengthen the rat’s desire to engage with the
environment in this particular manner. In a parallel example, imagine that you’re
playing a street-racing video game. As you drive through one city course multiple
times, you try a number of different streets to get to the finish line. On one of these
trials, you discover a shortcut that dramatically improves your overall time. You have
learned this new path through operant conditioning. That is, by engaging with your
environment (operant responses), you performed a sequence of behaviors that that
was positively reinforced (i.e., you found the shortest distance to the finish line). And
now that you’ve learned how to drive this course, you will perform that same
sequence of driving behaviors (just as the rat presses on the lever) to receive your
reward of a faster finish.
Operant conditioning research studies how the effects of a behavior influence the
probability that it will occur again. For example, the effects of the rat’s lever-pressing
behavior (i.e., receiving a food pellet) influences the probability that it will keep
pressing the lever. For, according to Thorndike’s law of effect, when a behavior has a
positive (satisfying) effect or consequence, it is likely to be repeated in the future.
However, when a behavior has a negative (painful/annoying) consequence, it is less
likely to be repeated in the future. Effects that increase behaviors are referred to as
reinforcers, and effects that decrease them are referred to as punishers.
A classical CS (e.g., the bell) does not merely elicit a simple, unitary reflex. Pavlov
emphasized salivation because that was the only response he measured. But his bell
almost certainly elicited a whole system of responses that functioned to get the
organism ready for the upcoming US (food) (see Timberlake, 2001). For example, in
addition to salivation, CSs (such as the bell) that signal that food is near also elicit the
secretion of gastric acid, pancreatic enzymes, and insulin (which gets blood glucose
into cells). All of these responses prepare the body for digestion. Additionally, the CS
elicits approach behavior and a state of excitement. And presenting a CS for food can
also cause animals whose stomachs are full to eat more food if it is available. In fact,
food CSs are so prevalent in modern society, humans are likewise inclined to eat or
feel hungry in response to cues associated with food, such as the sound of a bag of
potato chips opening, the sight of a well-known logo (e.g., Coca-Cola), or the feel of
the couch in front of the television.
In a similar way, flavors associated with stomach pain or illness become avoided
and disliked. For example, a person who gets sick after drinking too much tequila may
acquire a profound dislike of the taste and odor of tequila—a phenomenon
called taste aversion conditioning. The fact that flavors are often associated with so
many consequences of eating is important for animals (including rats and humans)
that are frequently exposed to new foods. And it is clinically relevant. For example,
drugs used in chemotherapy often make cancer patients sick. As a consequence,
patients often acquire aversions to foods eaten just before treatment, or even aversions
to such things as the waiting room of the chemotherapy clinic itself (see Bernstein,
1991; Scalera & Bavieri, 2009).
Another interesting effect of classical conditioning can occur when we ingest drugs.
That is, when a drug is taken, it can be associated with the cues that are present at the
same time (e.g., rooms, odors, drug paraphernalia). In this regard, if someone
associates a particular smell with the sensation induced by the drug, whenever that
person smells the same odor afterward, it may cue responses (physical and/or
emotional) related to taking the drug itself. But drug cues have an even more
interesting property: They elicit responses that often “compensate” for the upcoming
effect of the drug (see Siegel, 1989). For example, morphine itself suppresses pain;
however, if someone is used to taking morphine, a cue that signals the “drug is
coming soon” can actually make the person more sensitive to pain. Because the
person knows a pain suppressant will soon be administered, the body becomes more
sensitive, anticipating that “the drug will soon take care of it.” Remarkably,
such conditioned compensatory responses in turn decrease the impact of the drug on
the body—because the body has become more sensitive to pain.
This conditioned compensatory response has many implications. For instance, a drug
user will be most “tolerant” to the drug in the presence of cues that have been
associated with it (because such cues elicit compensatory responses). As a result,
overdose is usually not due to an increase in dosage, but to taking the drug in a new
place without the familiar cues—which would have otherwise allowed the user to
tolerate the drug (see Siegel, Hinson, Krank, & McCully, 1982). Conditioned
compensatory responses (which include heightened pain sensitivity and decreased
body temperature, among others) might also cause discomfort, thus motivating the
drug user to continue usage of the drug to reduce them. This is one of several ways
classical conditioning might be a factor in drug addiction and dependence.
A final effect of classical cues is that they motivate ongoing operant behavior
(see Balleine, 2005). For example, if a rat has learned via operant conditioning that
pressing a lever will give it a drug, in the presence of cues that signal the “drug is
coming soon” (like the sound of the lever squeaking), the rat will work harder to press
the lever than if those cues weren’t present (i.e., there is no squeaking lever sound).
Similarly, in the presence of food-associated cues (e.g., smells), a rat (or an overeater)
will work harder for food. And finally, even in the presence of negative cues (like
something that signals fear), a rat, a human, or any other organism will work harder to
avoid those situations that might lead to trauma. Classical CSs thus have many effects
that can contribute to significant behavioral phenomena.
Blocking and other related effects indicate that the learning process tends to take in
the most valid predictors of significant events and ignore the less useful ones. This is
common in the real world. For example, imagine that your supermarket puts big star-
shaped stickers on products that are on sale. Quickly, you learn that items with the big
star-shaped stickers are cheaper. However, imagine you go into a similar supermarket
that not only uses these stickers, but also uses bright orange price tags to denote a
discount. Because of blocking (i.e., you already know that the star-shaped stickers
indicate a discount), you don’t have to learn the color system, too. The star-shaped
stickers tell you everything you need to know (i.e. there’s no prediction error for the
discount), and thus the color system is irrelevant.
Classical conditioning is strongest if the CS and US are intense or salient. It is also
best if the CS and US are relatively new and the organism hasn’t been frequently
exposed to them before. And it is especially strong if the organism’s biology has
prepared it to associate a particular CS and US. For example, rats and humans are
naturally inclined to associate an illness with a flavor, rather than with a light or tone.
Because foods are most commonly experienced by taste, if there is a particular food
that makes us ill, associating the flavor (rather than the appearance—which may be
similar to other foods) with the illness will more greatly ensure we avoid that food in
the future, and thus avoid getting sick. This sorting tendency, which is set up by
evolution, is called preparedness.
There are many factors that affect the strength of classical conditioning, and these
have been the subject of much research and theory (see Rescorla & Wagner,
1972; Pearce & Bouton, 2001). Behavioral neuroscientists have also used classical
conditioning to investigate many of the basic brain processes that are involved in
learning (see Fanselow & Poulos, 2005; Thompson & Steinmetz, 2009).
Psychologists must accept one important fact about extinction, however: it does not
necessarily destroy the original learning (see Bouton, 2004). For example, imagine
you strongly associate the smell of chalkboards with the agony of middle school
detention. Now imagine that, after years of encountering chalkboards, the smell of
them no longer recalls the agony of detention (an example of extinction). However,
one day, after entering a new building for the first time, you suddenly catch a whiff of
a chalkboard and WHAM!, the agony of detention returns. This is called spontaneous
recovery: following a lapse in exposure to the CS after extinction has occurred,
sometimes re-exposure to the CS (e.g., the smell of chalkboards) can evoke the CR
again (e.g., the agony of detention).
This does not mean that extinction is a bad treatment for behavior disorders. Instead,
clinicians can increase its effectiveness by using basic research on learning to help
defeat these relapse effects (see Craske et al., 2008). For example, conducting
extinction therapies in contexts where patients might be most vulnerable to relapsing
(e.g., at work), might be a good strategy for enhancing the therapy’s success.
Most of the things that affect the strength of classical conditioning also affect the
strength of instrumental learning—whereby we learn to associate our actions with
their outcomes. As noted earlier, the “bigger” the reinforcer (or punisher), the stronger
the learning. And, if an instrumental behavior is no longer reinforced, it will also be
extinguished. Most of the rules of associative learning that apply to classical
conditioning also apply to instrumental learning, but other facts about instrumental
learning are also worth knowing.
As you know, the classic operant response in the laboratory is lever-pressing in rats,
reinforced by food. However, things can be arranged so that lever-pressing only
produces pellets when a particular stimulus is present. For example, lever-pressing
can be reinforced only when a light in the Skinner box is turned on; when the light is
off, no food is released from lever-pressing. The rat soon learns to discriminate
between the light-on and light-off conditions, and presses the lever only in the
presence of the light (responses in light-off are extinguished). In everyday life, think
about waiting in the turn lane at a traffic light. Although you know that green means
go, only when you have the green arrow do you turn. In this regard, the operant
behavior is now said to be under stimulus control. And, as is the case with the traffic
light, in the real world, stimulus control is probably the rule.
Stimulus-control techniques are widely used in the laboratory to study perception and
other psychological processes in animals. For example, the rat would not be able to
respond appropriately to light-on and light-off conditions if it could not see the light.
Following this logic, experiments using stimulus-control methods have tested how
well animals see colors, hear ultrasounds, and detect magnetic fields. That is,
researchers pair these discriminative stimuli with those they know the animals already
understand (such as pressing the lever). In this way, the researchers can test if the
animals can learn to press the lever only when an ultrasound is played, for example.
These methods can also be used to study “higher” cognitive processes. For example,
pigeons can learn to peck at different buttons in a Skinner box when pictures of
flowers, cars, chairs, or people are shown on a miniature TV screen (see Wasserman,
1995). Pecking button 1 (and no other) is reinforced in the presence of a flower
image, button 2 in the presence of a chair image, and so on. Pigeons can learn the
discrimination readily, and, under the right conditions, will even peck the correct
buttons associated with pictures of new flowers, cars, chairs, and people they have
never seen before. The birds have learned to categorize the sets of stimuli. Stimulus-
control methods can be used to study how such categorization is learned.
Another thing to know about operant conditioning is that the response always requires
choosing one behavior over others. The student who goes to the bar on Thursday
night chooses to drink instead of staying at home and studying. The rat chooses to
press the lever instead of sleeping or scratching its ear in the back of the box. The
alternative behaviors are each associated with their own reinforcers. And the tendency
to perform a particular action depends on both the reinforcers earned for it and the
reinforcers earned for its alternatives.
To investigate this idea, choice has been studied in the Skinner box by making two
levers available for the rat (or two buttons available for the pigeon), each of which has
its own reinforcement or payoff rate. A thorough study of choice in situations like this
has led to a rule called the quantitative law of effect (see Herrnstein, 1970), which
can be understood without going into quantitative detail: The law acknowledges the
fact that the effects of reinforcing one behavior depend crucially on how much
reinforcement is earned for the behavior’s alternatives. For example, if a pigeon learns
that pecking one light will reward two food pellets, whereas the other light only
rewards one, the pigeon will only peck the first light. However, what happens if the
first light is more strenuous to reach than the second one? Will the cost of energy
outweigh the bonus of food? Or will the extra food be worth the work? In general, a
given reinforcer will be less reinforcing if there are many alternative reinforcers in the
environment. For this reason, alcohol, sex, or drugs may be less powerful reinforcers
if the person’s environment is full of other sources of reinforcement, such as
achievement at work or love from family members.
Modern research also indicates that reinforcers do more than merely strengthen or
“stamp in” the behaviors they are a consequence of, as was Thorndike’s original view.
Instead, animals learn about the specific consequences of each behavior, and will
perform a behavior depending on how much they currently want—or “value”—its
consequence.
Things can get more complicated, however, if the rat performs the instrumental
actions frequently and repeatedly. That is, if the rat has spent many months learning
the value of pressing each of the levers, the act of pressing them becomes automatic
and routine. And here, this once goal-directed action (i.e., the rat pressing the lever for
the goal of getting sucrose/food) can become a habit. Thus, if a rat spends many
months performing the lever-pressing behavior (turning such behavior into a habit),
even when sucrose is again paired with illness, the rat will continue to press that lever
(see Holland, 2004). After all the practice, the instrumental response (pressing the
lever) is no longer sensitive to reinforcer devaluation. The rat continues to respond
automatically, regardless of the fact that the sucrose from this lever makes it sick.
Habits are very common in human experience, and can be useful. You do not need to
relearn each day how to make your coffee in the morning or how to brush your teeth.
Instrumental behaviors can eventually become habitual, letting us get the job done
while being free to think about other things.
Classical and operant conditioning are usually studied separately. But outside of the
laboratory they almost always occur at the same time. For example, a person who is
reinforced for drinking alcohol or eating excessively learns these behaviors in the
presence of certain stimuli—a pub, a set of friends, a restaurant, or possibly the couch
in front of the TV. These stimuli are also available for association with the reinforcer.
In this way, classical and operant conditioning are always intertwined.
The figure below summarizes this idea, and helps review what we have discussed in
this module. Generally speaking, any reinforced or punished operant response (R) is
paired with an outcome (O) in the presence of some stimulus or set of stimuli (S).
The figure illustrates the types of associations that can be learned in this very general
scenario. For one thing, the organism will learn to associate the response and the
outcome (R – O). This is instrumental conditioning. The learning process here is
probably similar to classical conditioning, with all its emphasis on surprise and
prediction error. And, as we discussed while considering the reinforcer devaluation
effect, once R – O is learned, the organism will be ready to perform the response if
the outcome is desired or valued. The value of the reinforcer can also be influenced by
other reinforcers earned for other behaviors in the situation. These factors are at the
heart of instrumental learning.
Second, the organism can also learn to associate the stimulus with the reinforcing
outcome (S – O). This is the classical conditioning component, and as we have seen, it
can have many consequences on behavior. For one thing, the stimulus will come to
evoke a system of responses that help the organism prepare for the reinforcer (not
shown in the figure): The drinker may undergo changes in body temperature; the eater
may salivate and have an increase in insulin secretion. In addition, the stimulus will
evoke approach (if the outcome is positive) or retreat (if the outcome is negative).
Presenting the stimulus will also prompt the instrumental response.
The third association in the diagram is the one between the stimulus and the response
(S – R). As discussed earlier, after a lot of practice, the stimulus may begin to elicit
the response directly. This is habit learning, whereby the response occurs relatively
automatically, without much mental processing of the relation between the action and
the outcome and the outcome’s current value.
The final link in the figure is between the stimulus and the response-outcome
association [S – (R – O)]. More than just entering into a simple association with the R
or the O, the stimulus can signal that the R – O relationship is now in effect. This is
what we mean when we say that the stimulus can “set the occasion” for the operant
response: It sets the occasion for the response-reinforcer relationship. Through this
mechanism, the painter might begin to paint when given the right tools and the
opportunity enabled by the canvas. The canvas theoretically signals that the behavior
of painting will now be reinforced by positive consequences.
The figure provides a framework that you can use to understand almost any learned
behavior you observe in yourself, your family, or your friends. If you would like to
understand it more deeply, consider taking a course on learning in the future, which
will give you a fuller appreciation of how classical learning, instrumental learning,
habit learning, and occasion setting actually work and interact.
Observational Learning
Not all forms of learning are accounted for entirely by classical and operant
conditioning. Imagine a child walking up to a group of children playing a game on the
playground. The game looks fun, but it is new and unfamiliar. Rather than joining the
game immediately, the child opts to sit back and watch the other children play a round
or two. Observing the others, the child takes note of the ways in which they behave
while playing the game. By watching the behavior of the other kids, the child can
figure out the rules of the game and even some strategies for doing well at the game.
This is called observational learning.
Bandura theorizes that the observational learning process consists of four parts. The
first is attention—as, quite simply, one must pay attention to what s/he is observing in
order to learn. The second part is retention: to learn one must be able to retain the
behavior s/he is observing in memory.The third part of observational
learning, initiation, acknowledges that the learner must be able to execute (or initiate)
the learned behavior. Lastly, the observer must possess the motivation to engage in
observational learning. In our vignette, the child must want to learn how to play the
game in order to properly engage in observational learning.
While reinforcement was not required to elicit the children’s behavior in Bandura’s
first experiment, it is important to acknowledge that consequences do play a role
within observational learning. A future adaptation of this study (Bandura, Ross, &
Ross, 1963) demonstrated that children in the aggression group showed less
aggressive behavior if they witnessed the adult model receive punishment for
aggressing against Bobo. Bandura referred to this process as vicarious
reinforcement, as the children did not experience the reinforcement or punishment
directly, yet were still influenced by observing it.
Conclusion
We have covered three primary explanations for how we learn to behave and interact
with the world around us. Considering your own experiences, how well do these
theories apply to you? Maybe when reflecting on your personal sense of fashion, you
realize that you tend to select clothes others have complimented you on (operant
conditioning). Or maybe, thinking back on a new restaurant you tried recently, you
realize you chose it because its commercials play happy music (classical
conditioning). Or maybe you are now always on time with your assignments, because
you saw how others were punished when they were late (observational learning).
Regardless of the activity, behavior, or response, there’s a good chance your
“decision” to do it can be explained based on one of the theories presented in this
module.
Outside Resources
Discussion Questions
1. Describe three examples of Pavlovian (classical) conditioning that you have seen in your own
behavior, or that of your friends or family, in the past few days.
2. Describe three examples of instrumental (operant) conditioning that you have seen in your
own behavior, or that of your friends or family, in the past few days.
3. Drugs can be potent reinforcers. Discuss how Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental
conditioning can work together to influence drug taking.
4. In the modern world, processed foods are highly available and have been engineered to be
highly palatable and reinforcing. Discuss how Pavlovian and instrumental conditioning can
work together to explain why people often eat too much.
5. How does blocking challenge the idea that pairings of a CS and US are sufficient to cause
Pavlovian conditioning? What is important in creating Pavlovian learning?
6. How does the reinforcer devaluation effect challenge the idea that reinforcers merely “stamp
in” the operant response? What does the effect tell us that animals actually learn in operant
conditioning?
7. With regards to social learning do you think people learn violence from observing violence in
movies? Why or why not?
8. What do you think you have learned through social learning? Who are your social models?
Vocabulary
Blocking
In classical conditioning, the finding that no conditioning occurs to
a stimulus if it is combined with a previously conditioned stimulus
during conditioning trials. Suggests that information, surprise
value, or prediction error is important in conditioning.
Categorize
To sort or arrange different items into classes or categories.
Classical conditioning
The procedure in which an initially neutral stimulus (the conditioned
stimulus, or CS) is paired with an unconditioned stimulus (or US).
The result is that the conditioned stimulus begins to elicit a
conditioned response (CR). Classical conditioning is nowadays
considered important as both a behavioral phenomenon and as a method
to study simple associative learning. Same as Pavlovian conditioning.
Context
Stimuli that are in the background whenever learning occurs. For
instance, the Skinner box or room in which learning takes place is
the classic example of a context. However, “context” can also be
provided by internal stimuli, such as the sensory effects of drugs
(e.g., being under the influence of alcohol has stimulus properties
that provide a context) and mood states (e.g., being happy or sad).
It can also be provided by a specific period in time—the passage of
time is sometimes said to change the “temporal context.”
Discriminative stimulus
In operant conditioning, a stimulus that signals whether the response
will be reinforced. It is said to “set the occasion” for the
operant response.
Extinction
Decrease in the strength of a learned behavior that occurs when the
conditioned stimulus is presented without the unconditioned stimulus
(in classical conditioning) or when the behavior is no longer
reinforced (in instrumental conditioning). The term describes both
the procedure (the US or reinforcer is no longer presented) as well
as the result of the procedure (the learned response declines).
Behaviors that have been reduced in strength through extinction are
said to be “extinguished.”
Fear conditioning
A type of classical or Pavlovian conditioning in which the
conditioned stimulus (CS) is associated with an aversive
unconditioned stimulus (US), such as a foot shock. As a consequence
of learning, the CS comes to evoke fear. The phenomenon is thought to
be involved in the development of anxiety disorders in humans.
Goal-directed behavior
Instrumental behavior that is influenced by the animal’s knowledge
of the association between the behavior and its consequence and the
current value of the consequence. Sensitive to the reinforcer
devaluation effect.
Habit
Instrumental behavior that occurs automatically in the presence of a
stimulus and is no longer influenced by the animal’s knowledge of
the value of the reinforcer. Insensitive to the reinforcer
devaluation effect.
Instrumental conditioning
Process in which animals learn about the relationship between their
behaviors and their consequences. Also known as operant conditioning.
Law of effect
The idea that instrumental or operant responses are influenced by
their effects. Responses that are followed by a pleasant state of
affairs will be strengthened and those that are followed by
discomfort will be weakened. Nowadays, the term refers to the idea
that operant or instrumental behaviors are lawfully controlled by
their consequences.
Observational learning
Learning by observing the behavior of others.
Operant
A behavior that is controlled by its consequences. The simplest
example is the rat’s lever-pressing, which is controlled by the
presentation of the reinforcer.
Operant conditioning
See instrumental conditioning.
Pavlovian conditioning
See classical conditioning.
Prediction error
When the outcome of a conditioning trial is different from that which
is predicted by the conditioned stimuli that are present on the trial
(i.e., when the US is surprising). Prediction error is necessary to
create Pavlovian conditioning (and associative learning generally).
As learning occurs over repeated conditioning trials, the conditioned
stimulus increasingly predicts the unconditioned stimulus, and
prediction error declines. Conditioning works to correct or reduce
prediction error.
Preparedness
The idea that an organism’s evolutionary history can make it easy to
learn a particular association. Because of preparedness, you are more
likely to associate the taste of tequila, and not the circumstances
surrounding drinking it, with getting sick. Similarly, humans are
more likely to associate images of spiders and snakes than flowers
and mushrooms with aversive outcomes like shocks.
Punisher
A stimulus that decreases the strength of an operant behavior when it
is made a consequence of the behavior.
Reinforcer
Any consequence of a behavior that strengthens the behavior or
increases the likelihood that it will be performed it again.
Renewal effect
Recovery of an extinguished response that occurs when the context is
changed after extinction. Especially strong when the change of
context involves return to the context in which conditioning
originally occurred. Can occur after extinction in either classical
or instrumental conditioning.
Social models
Authorities that are the targets for observation and who model
behaviors.
Spontaneous recovery
Recovery of an extinguished response that occurs with the passage of
time after extinction. Can occur after extinction in either classical
or instrumental conditioning.
Stimulus control
When an operant behavior is controlled by a stimulus that precedes
it.
Vicarious reinforcement
Learning that occurs by observing the reinforcement or punishment of
another person.
References