1 PB
1 PB
Website: http://journal.uinjkt.ac.id/index.php/jece
Permalink/DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.15408/jece.v2i1.15511
JECE, 2 (1), Juni 2020, 1-20
Abstract
Habituation is the most common used form of moral education in early childhood education
in Indonesia. However, this method has been found to be insufficient in contributing fully to
young children’s moral development. An alternative method currently being promoted is the
narrative method which involves the use of stories and storytelling. These two techniques
provide children with justification to do good deeds and allow them to be critical of their
actions. This research examined the best practices of teachers in the use of narrative method
for the Moral Education of young children. Best practices show how we can use them to
improve the teaching of Moral Education among young children and can serve as useful
alternative to habituation. The findings revealed that there are five ways in which the
kindergarten use stories and storytelling in teaching and learning moral education for young
children, namely: to repeat the story; to include clear example; to explain the story; to
dramatize the story; and to modify the story.
Abstrak
Pembiasaan adalah bentuk pendidikan moral yang paling umum digunakan dalam pendidikan anak
usia dini di Indonesia. Akan tetapi, metode ini ternyata masih kurang memberikan banyak kontribusi
dalam perkembangan moral Anak. Salah satu metode alternatif yang saat ini sedang banyak
dikenalkan adalah metode naratif yang melibatkan penggunaan cerita dan bercerita (Storytelling).
Dua tehnik ini memberikan dasar pembenaran pada anak-anak untuk melakukan perbuatan baik dan
menjadikan mereka mampu bersikap kritis terhadap tindakan yang mereka lakukan. Penelitian ini
menguji Pengalaman terbaik para guru dalam penggunaan metode naratif untuk pendidikan moral
pada anak. Pengalaman terbaik ini menunjukan bagaimana metode tersebut dapat digunakan untuk
meningkatkan pendidikan moral pada anak dan juga dapat digunakan sebagai salah satu alternatif
proses pembiasaan pada anak. Hasil temuan menunjukan bahwa ada lima cara yang digunakan
Taman Kanak-kanak (TK) dalam menggunakan cerita untuk proses belajar mengajar pendidikan
moral pada anak-anak, yaitu dengan cara : mengulang cerita, memasukkan contoh yang jelas,
menjelaskan cerita, mendramatisir cerita dan memodifikasi cerita
Kata kunci : Pendidikan Moral, Bercerita, Pendidikan Anak Usia Dini, Pengalaman guru
Introduction
Indonesia became a signatory to the Millennium Development Goals (MDGs) in
2000. One of the objectives of the deal is to provide equal opportunities for education
for all, and in 2019 Indonesia joined the group of countries with a high human
development index, according to a report published by the United Nations
Development Program or the UNDP on 9 December 2019 (UNDP, 2019). The
Indonesian Government has increased efforts through various policies in education,
economic, politic and social life to improve the quality of life of Indonesians. The
provision of primary education for all is expected to increase welfare and eradicate
poverty across the nation (Eryong & Xiuping, 2018; Johnston, 2016). Education
prepares students for the complexities of life in this era of new technology. Education
should, therefore, be versatile and flexible so that it can be adjusted to respond to any
changes that may occur, both quickly and responsibly. Due to this, education
practitioners and policymakers need to be informed of expected global developments
and tracked to enable the possibility of development. Proper education will generate
valuable human resources required to develop the country further (Webb et al.,
2018).
The urgent needs of this country are not only to provide the workforce with the
appropriate skills and knowledge but also character building. In meeting these goals,
one of the urgent needs is not only to develop workforce who have the relevant and
appropriate knowledge and skills but also the character to ensure that the integrity of
the work is sustained. The shift from knowledge-based and skill-based education to
character-based education is a significant step in creating a responsible and
dependable workforce.
The Education Unit Level Curriculum (KTSP) has been replaced by the 2013
curriculum, which began to be implemented in 2014. The emphasis of the 2013
Curriculum is on simplification and thematic-integrative efforts. The 2013 curriculum
is set to produce a generation that is ready to face the future since the curriculum is
designed to anticipate future developments.
The most recent national curriculum, the 2013 curriculum, has been
implemented to meet future global challenges, namely, to ensure responsible and
capable citizens (Regulation of the Minister of Education and Culture of the Republic
of Indonesia Number 37 of 2018). The 2013 Education Curriculum promotes the
development of learners optimally and provides the foundation for an Indonesian
human being who can live as an individual and a citizen who is loyal, efficient,
imaginative, inventive, and productive and capable of contributing to social,
regional, state and world civilization.
good behavior, which stems from religious and moral values and comes from
community life in the context of the play.
However, this method does not seem to be sufficient to equip the child's moral
development fully. The argument is that morality is not only about acting correctly; it
is also about reasoning and feeling (May & Kumar, 2018; Saunders, 2016; Sherman &
Guyer, 1995; Tangney et al., 2007). The habituation method only teaches the child to
do things. Still, it is clear that not every child thinks about or understands their
actions. Children need to learn how to think critically about the consequences of their
actions. Children need to learn to act independently and not just to memorize the
rules that may be dictated to them. As a result of this, alternatives to moral education
are now commonly being offered through storytelling to teach children more than
simply to be righteous people, but also to be responsible for their actions. Stories are
believed to be able to explain why they need to do good deeds, and can even make
children more critical of their actions.
Figure 6. Habituation & Narrative Methods for Early Childhood Moral Education
Extensive research and theories exist on how stories profess examples of the
moral life (Gunnestad et al., 2015; McGinn, 1997; Rahim & Rahiem, 2013; Thambu,
2017; Thompson, 2011; Winston, 2005; Woodard, 2005) Narrative techniques such as
stories and storytelling in Moral Education guide young children to distinguish
between what is right and what is wrong. Children's literature, such as fables, folk
tales, and fairy tales, helps to interpret the underlying rules of society and guides the
child to distinguish between right and wrong. Nevertheless, there remains a gap in
the existing theory; empirical knowledge and practice that has prompted the
effectiveness of the use of narratives in the teaching and learning of moral education
for young children. There is a lack of empirical data in how teachers see and practice
it. This research is expected to provide information on the existing practices of
teachers in using narratives in teaching and learning moral education for young
children and the techniques they have used the narrative in the teaching and learning
of moral education in kindergarten.
In early childhood settings, particularly in the learning of values through
storytelling, children are still very dependent on teachers in the learning. Most
children in kindergarten are unable to read, and they need their teachers to read the
stories. Teachers also need to help them understand the meaning of the stories.
Narvaez argued that children develop moral literacy, not just by reading or listening
to moral stories. Her arguments are: first, readers interpret texts in different ways
because of their individual reading skills and the context of the information. Second,
moral claims are also interpreted differently based on variations such as moral
schema. Besides, readers have explained and weighted moral texts not in the same
way, and this has been affected by their moral traditions. Finally, children usually
understand a moral theme from the story, but this is not always the same moral as
was initially intended by the author (Narvaez, 2002).
The ways teachers deliver stories affect the success of using storytelling for
moral education. Specifically, young children's understanding of moral values in a
story is influenced by the knowledge of the teachers on what the stories are used for
in values education and the way the teacher tells the stories for that purpose, which
is essential in facilitating the students to learn from the storytelling (Rahiem et al.,
2017).
It is, therefore, necessary to examine the best practices of teachers in the use of
stories and storytelling for moral education. This research is expected to provide
information on the kindergarten teachers’ insights and observations on the best
practices of the use of narrative technique in teaching and learning moral education
for young children. Understanding the insights and observations of teachers on the
technique can be useful to better integrate the narrative method to other methods of
early childhood moral education (such as habituation) and to core competencies of
the curriculum in order to address the demands of the 2013 curriculum.
Methods
Given the objectives of the study and the nature of its research questions,
qualitative research is the method of investigation for this research. In quantitative
research, researchers try to detach themselves as much as possible from research,
while in qualitative research, researchers are intimately involved in the research
(Nahid, 2003) The role of the researchers in this study is primarily that of the data
collector. As a result, it will be possible to see the entire research process and also the
research findings through the researchers' lens. The researchers played a pivotal role
in the development of the research questions and the completion of the data
collection, data analysis, and interpretation. The researchers used their values, but
despite this, they always tried to remain as objective as possible. The researchers
presented the entire research process as precisely and accurately as possible.
To better understand a particular phenomenon, purposive sampling was used
in this research. The participants were selected according to the criteria or essential
attributes embedded in the research questions (S. R. Jones, 2002). The participants of
this study were selected for their sensitivity, knowledge, and insight into their
experience. Although the decision to participate was strictly voluntary, the
participants who chose to participate in this study appeared to match the defined
criteria. The participants' criteria were that the teachers had been teaching for more
than two years and used storytelling in their classrooms. They were well established
in early childhood teaching and, therefore, were deprived of the nature of the
interview questions posed by the researcher.
The total number of participants is 14 teachers from two kindergartens located
in the South Jakarta area. Both kindergartens have been chosen as research sites
because they have an excellent reputation within the community as a creative and
innovative kindergartens. They have consistently and regularly incorporated
storytelling activities into the student curriculum, while teachers use the storytelling
method to learn. Teachers in both schools, therefore, have extensive experience of
using storytelling as teaching tools. All participants use storytelling in their
classrooms and have a favorable view of the use of storytelling in the teaching-
learning process.
Figure 8. Samples
Coded names conceal the names of the teachers in order to preserve their
identities and increase their confidence in talking about their knowledge,
understanding, and practices in the use of stories and storytelling for moral
education. T1 up to T14 are the coded names used. The actual names of the two
kindergartens were also deliberately obscured in order to ensure further anonymity.
The study used several data collection methods to obtain a rich and deeper
insight into the studied phenomenon. Semi-structured, in-depth interviews, and
focus groups were the primary method of data collection. More information was then
also gathered through observation and document analysis.
The primary sources of analysis of the data in this study were spoken words,
observations, and documents. Data analysis begins when data collection is started,
not after all the data has been collected, as is the case in quantitative studies. Data
analysis occurs simultaneously and interactively with data collection, data
interpretation, and reporting (Creswell, 2002; Miles & Huberman, 1984). The data
analysis began by identifying the themes that emerged from the raw data. Strauss
and Corbin, refer to the process as 'open coding.' At the next stage of the analysis, the
researchers re-examined the categories identified in order to see the connection. This
complex process is sometimes referred to as 'axial coding' (Corbin & Strauss, 2014).
Finally, the researchers then organized everything into a short piece of writing to
ensure that the research is a vibrant, well-woven account that gives an accurate
picture of the reality that was being studied.
The qualitative research involved a triangulation analysis, which included
interviews with teachers, collection and analysis of documents, observations of how
teachers selected stories and how they told stories, and an exploration of the teachers'
understanding by having a focus group discussion (FGD) with the three keynote
informants.
All interviews were taped and transcribed to form the basis for subsequent
analysis. These responses were then compared and contrasted in order to detect
overall perceptions, commonalities, and differences and congruence with the
subsidiary questions of this case study. In practice, the data analysis phases were
carried out simultaneously and repeatedly. During axial coding, the researchers had
to change earlier categories several times, re-examine raw data, and also look for
additional data.
The first theme is repetition, what is meant by repetition is that when teachers
use storytelling to teach and practice moral education, they do not only read or say a
story once; they repeat it several times. During the second interview, T1 clarified that
not only does she tell a story once, but she also repeats the story many times to make
children understand and mirror what the characters do. She added that children tend
to forget stories quickly, and they, therefore, understand the narrative better by
repeating the story.
If we tell the children's story, we can't do it only once, it must be repeated often,
we're going to re-tell the story. If we want to inspire children to want to eat
vegetables, not just once, today we're going to say the story once, tomorrow we're
going to tell something else, but a few days later, we're going to repeat the same
theme, but maybe in a different way, so the story will be understood and it will then
help the children to do as the characters do. We've told the story over and over again,
so the kids seem to remember the story, because they're usually so quick to forget a
story. (T1)
Teachers also explained how they need to include concrete examples of actual
conduct, not just by telling stories. The following statements explain how T4
assumed that children had to practice good deeds after hearing a story:
I believe that in the stories that children can learn, children should not only be
instructed, but also practice instantly so that there is a consequence, for example,
morality, several examples, if they have to be respectful to the elderly, so they must
always be respectful to others, such as the assistant teacher. This could be educated
through storytelling, followed by an example. (T4)
T6 explained his views of the best way to use storytelling to teach and teach
young children moral education by telling stories and giving children real examples.
T6 said that children need to experience it directly themselves before it inspires them
to do good deeds. T6 pointed to the example of littering and having to clean the
classroom and the toilet; if they experience cleaning the classroom and the toilet and
know it is tiresome, then they will not be negligent with throwing litter in the future:
So, for moral education, firstly I use a storytelling technique. But, I believe the most
successful approach is through direct experience. And if they've witnessed it
explicitly, some of the students will say, why do we have to dispose of the waste?
Why do we need to sweep up the classroom?” If children feel exhausted after
cleaning the classroom, exhausted after cleaning the bathroom, they will not be
irresponsible. (T6)
The teachers explained that a discussion during and after storytelling about the
moral dimension of the story is necessary to help children understand the story and
to make sure they interpret it as intended by the teacher. T4 defined the importance
of holding a conversation during and after the narrative to help children understand
the story:
We lead them, we help them to understand. I'm building a link with them when they
ask a question, a dialogue, they get a role, and they're acting out some part of the
story. If we want to know whether or not children understand our narrative, we
should have a discussion... both during and after storytelling. (T4)
Teachers in this study said they often dramatized the story with mimicry,
movements, intonation, and using technology. T11 clarified that the usefulness of the
narrative in the propagation of moral values depends on the techniques used in
telling a story. She said it is essential for teachers to tell a story appealingly so that
kids will listen to it. Otherwise, they will not concentrate on the story, and they are
unlikely to then get the message out of the story:
The success of the narrative in the development of moral principles depends on the
manner in which the tale is told. Okay, I guess if the story is told by the teacher in a
boring manner, children would definitely not want to listen and they won't
understand as well. And, in essence, the most important way to do this is how the
teacher tells the story. If teachers use different approaches or creative storytelling,
kids typically react instantly, but if a teacher, sorry, for example, doesn't tell the
story well, it's useless. That's according to my observation, so the main element is
the teacher, if we want to teach moral values by stories, we should relate a story in an
entertaining way, so that children can understand and respond directly. When they
answer, we see how their interpretation is; it's nice if it's in line with the essence of
the story, but if it's not, it's the teachers' responsibility to guide the students. (T11)
Based on the review of the results, the study found that teachers change the
content of the story if they feel it is not suitable for young children. Teachers in this
study change the story in order to comply with religious instructions and to protect
children from violent and sexually explicit material.
T7 argued that when the story is told, the story must be modified when the
story does not adhere to religious teaching. She said that children would find the
Timun Mas story hard to understand since it makes no sense to pray to the giant for
a child and to have a child born of cucumber seeds. It is easier for her to ask them to
pray to God and make the story more realistic:
There are a number of versions of the Timun Mas narrative. In my view, fairy tales
or myths rooted in or originating from Indonesia should be told or read aloud to
children in kindergartens. But sometimes, we're not allowed to read or tell the tale as
it is. There are certain elements in this story that are difficult for children to
comprehend. For starters, the old lady prayed, not to God, but to the giant instead.
This isn't right, she's expected to pray to God. As described in this story, the giant
gave the mother cucumber seeds that grew larger later, and there was a baby inside.
It's perfect for a fairy tale to have a plot like this. Yet if I read or hear this story, I'm
going to change the story (giggling). (T7)
Overall, the research reveals that five key themes have been identified. These
five principles describe the most common ways in which kindergarten teachers use
storytelling for moral education.
The first theme of the results is repetition because teachers do not just read or
say a story once; they repeat it a few times over a more extended period. Teachers
have explained how they repeat the narration many times to ensure that children
understand the story so that they can learn from the story. They also want to direct
children to the correct interpretation or message of the story. It is in line with what
Anderson & Groft (1972) claimed that children could benefit from stories because
stories have experience. Stories are a reflection of life and mind in the form of words.
They said that stories affect the way children think and act. Children want to hear or
read stories over and again, and they remember the specifics in the tales. This
repetition, combined with the children's imagination, makes stories one of the best
ways to stimulate thought. It is also reflected in the observations of Lartz & Mason
(1988), who performed a case study of a preliterate child's retelling of a story several
times over eight weeks. The retellings were audiotaped and evaluated for
improvement. They found that the story became the child's own story after retelling
the story a handful of times.
An important aspect was also found; the teachers said that they would repeat
the story because they wanted the children to have the same understanding of the
principles of the story as the teachers. T8 said that she would not stop telling the
story until the children understood it as she wanted. T10 said that when kids did not
understand, she would direct them right away. T3, T8, and T12 explained that they
want to make sure that children explain what they understand so that they can
clarify what the meanings of the story are. T1 and T14 explained that they wanted
children to understand the story and better understand the meaning of the story.
Such teachers guide and correct children's comprehension. Teachers like to see a
child as a blank slate. They were born just to be morally neutral. Their personality
can develop in infancy, and adults around them might have a very long-lasting effect
on their personality. Many Muslims believe that children are born like a blank slate,
as John Locke said (John Locke, n.d.). If the definition of faith misleads a child, it is the
fault of culture or the social environment. This principle is also used in many popular
parenting books in Indonesia (Noormindhawati, 2014; Rif’ani, 2013; Setyawan, 2015)
The teachers explained that real examples are needed. They said moral
formation is not optimal by using storytelling only. Children at this age like to
imitate things, they need to practice it and repeat the acts. This suggests similarities
to Spiecker (2005), who proposed training, conditioning, and drilling are essential to
early childhood upbringing and moral training. The teachers also described that
kindergarten children need a concrete example because it is easier to grasp the idea,
storytelling is considered abstract thinking. So, real examples help them to
understand better. This statement is similar to what Piaget (2013) proposed in 1932;
that young children are 'pre-moral' beings who are incapable of considering the
perspectives of others or thinking abstractly. Kohlberg, (1976) also agreed with Piaget
that children at the level I struggle to think abstractly, and this limits their ability to
take the perspective of others. (Bebko et al., 1996) said that young children learn by
imitating and modeling on an education role, which means that they observe around
them without the need for instruction or telling. The data also indicated that teachers
felt that kindergarten children needed specific examples because they still think one-
dimensionally, with storytelling considered to be abstract thinking.
These teachers said that moral development is not optimal by narrative usage
only. Kids tend to mimic actions at this age, and they need to practice it and repeat
the activities. This is similar to Spiecker (2005), who suggested that preparation,
conditioning, and drilling are essential for early childhood education and moral
preparation.
The seven teachers, who said they provided practical examples to children,
graduated from early childhood education programs. Does their educational history
affect their practice of providing specific examples to children? If we look at the
conflicting stories exposes children to a real-life situation. The alteration of the stories
lets them concentrate on the good deeds on the stories.
Conclusion
The results showed that there are five ways in which the participants in this study
use storytelling in teaching and learning moral education for young children,
namely: to repeat the narrative, to provide specific examples; to discuss the story; to
dramatize the story; to alter the story. The methods are not the same for each person,
but they are a collection of strategies revealed at the time of the individual interviews
and the FGD. Just one participant did all of the above methods, while the other
participants did around three or four of the above methods. The findings of this
study have generated several recommendations for potential areas for future
research: First, expanding the study of how kindergarten teachers use storytelling for
moral education in other study locations, as it is vital to improving the standard of
moral education in the kindergarten across Indonesia by learning from the best
practices of the experienced teachers. Second, in particular, the results of this study
open up new agendas for future studies. For example, how the role of teachers in
teaching and learning influences children's comprehension and moral development,
research what books are ideally suited to teaching children's values, or the most
suitable storytelling technique for successful moral education. Third, expanding
studies among other study participants, such as moral education teachers in primary
or secondary schools who use storytelling for moral education. Fourth, engaging
kindergarten students as participants in the triangulation analysis in order to achieve
a thorough understanding of and recognition of moral values. Lastly, for researchers
wishing to perform a quantitative analysis, this study may be used as a guide in the
development of instruments to survey a more significant number of samples.
References
Ago, H. C., & Stan, S. (1995). Religious Topics in Children ’ s Literature. XV(1), 91–97.
Anderson, W., & Groft, P. (1972). A new look at children’s literature. Wadsworth
Publishing Co., Inc.
Bebko, J. M., Perry, A., & Bryson, S. (1996). Multiple method validation study of
facilitated communication: II. Individual differences and subgroup results.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02276233
Bettelheim, B. (2010). The uses of enchantment: the meaning and importance of fairy tales.
Vintage Books.
Bruner, J. . (1966). Toward theory of instruction. Harvard University Press.
Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. (2014). Basics of Qualitative Research: Techniques and Procedures
for Developing Grounded Theory (4th ed.). Sage.
Lartz, M. N., & Mason, J. M. (1988). Jamie: One child’s journey from oral to written
language. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-
2006(88)90022-1
May, J., & Kumar, V. (n.d.). Moral reasoning and emotion. In K. Jones, M. Timmons,
& A. Zimmerman (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of moral epistemology (pp. 139–
156). Routledge.
McGinn, C. (1997). Ethics, Evil and Fiction. Oxford University Press.
Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1984). Qualitative Data Analysis : Handout. A
Sourcebook of New Methods. California; SAGE Publications Inc.
Nahid, G. (2003). Understanding reliability and validity in qualitative research. The
Qualitative Report, 8(4), 597–607. http://www.nova.edu/ssss/QR/QR8-
4/golafshani.pdf
Narvaez, D. (2002). Does reading moral stories build character? In Educational
Psychology Review. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014674621501
Noormindhawati, L. (2014). The secret of millionaires to educate children (Rahasia para
miliuner mendidik anak). Elex Media Computindo.
Penn, H. (2008). Understanding early childhood: issues and controversies. McGraw-Hill
Education.
Piaget, J. (2013). The moral judgment of the child. In The Moral Judgment of the Child.
https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315009681
Rahim, H., & Rahiem, M. D. H. (2013). The Use of Stories as Moral Education for
Young Children. International Journal of Social Science and Humanity, 2(6), 454–
458. https://doi.org/10.7763/IJSSH.2012.V2.145
Regulation of the Minister of Education and Culture of the Republic of Indonesia
Number 58 of 2009
Regulation of the Minister of Education and Culture of the Republic of Indonesia
Number 146 of 2014
Regulation of the Minister of Education and Culture of the Republic of Indonesia
Number 37 of 2018
Rif’ani, N. (2013). The Prophet’s Example in Educating Children (Teladan Rasulullah dalam
Mendidik Anak). Semesta Hikmah.
Rosenblatt, L. M. (2019). The Transactional Theory of Reading and Writing. In M. R.
Ruddel & H. Singer (Eds.), Theoretical Models and Processes of Literacy (pp. 451–
479). International Reading Association.
https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315110592-28
Saunders, L. F. (2016). Reason and emotion, not reason or emotion in moral
judgment. Philosophical Explorations, 19(3), 252–267.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13869795.2016.1212395
Setyawan, A. (2015). Children are also human (Anak juga manusia). Noura Books.
Sherman, N., & Guyer, P. (1995). Reasons and Feelings in Kantian Morality.
Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 55(2), 369.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2108554
Spiecker, B. (2005). Habituation and training in early moral upbringing. In Virtue
Ethics and Moral Education. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203978368-27
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral Emotions and Moral
Behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 345–372.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145
Thambu, N. (2017). Storytelling and Story Reading: A Catalyst for Inculcate Moral
Values and Ethics among Preschoolers. International Journal of Academic Research
in Business and Social Sciences, 7(6). https://doi.org/10.6007/IJARBSS/v7-
i6/3143
The basic framework and structure of the 2013 early childhood curriculum—Kerangka
dasar dan struktur kurikulum PAUD 2013, 2014.
Thompson, M. (2011). Developing moral values in children: Observations from a
preschool. IFE PsychologIA, 19(2), 394–411.
https://doi.org/10.4314/ifep.v19i2.69584
Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant Intersubjectivity: Research, Theory, and
Clinical Applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(1), 3–48.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00701
UNDP. (2019). Human Development Report.
http://hdr.undp.org/en/countries/profiles/IDN
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Interaction Between Learning and Development. Harvard University
Press.
VYGOTSKY, L. S. (2019). Interaction between Learning and Development. In Mind in
Society. https://doi.org/10.2307/j.ctvjf9vz4.11
Webb, M., Kuntuova, I., & Karabayeva, A. (2018). The role of education in realising
youths’ human capital: Social philosophical analysis. Ensaio.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-40362018002601727
Winston, J. (2005). Drama, narrative and moral education: Exploring traditional tales
in the primary years. In Drama, Narrative and Moral Education: Exploring
Traditional Tales in the Primary Years. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203975497
Woodard, J. (2005). Head, Heart and Hands: Waldorf Education. Journal of
Curriculum and Pedagogy, 2(2), 84–85.
https://doi.org/10.1080/15505170.2005.10411551