Lecture Eight
Lecture Eight
NONVERBAL COMMUNICATION
Nonverbal Communication
- Nonverbal communication without obligatory ties to language is by far the most diverse
category, and may be vocal, involving sounds originating in the vocal tract, or nonvocal.
Nonvocal signs in turn may draw on sound production and interpretation, as in whistles and
clapping, or may involve any other sense modality – such as vision, olfaction, taste, and
touch, with vision paramount, whether through gesture, dress styles, or architecture.
- Another foundational distinction made in nonverbal communication centers on context,
especially whether face-to-face, more generally interpersonal, or between more remote
actors (Goffman, 1963). Contemporary media have introduced many innovations for
nonverbal communication. Cell-phone use is verbal, leading to some ordinary nonverbal
signs accompanying speech being masked by the medium while others are exaggerated.
Emailing represents the verbal both nonverbally and nonvocally, in writing; yet further
nonvocal and vocal nonverbal expression seeps in through emoticons and yet-to-be
standardized strings of icons, indexes, and symbols.
- Finally, sign languages for the deaf, such as American Sign Language (ASL), are
quintessentially both nonverbal and nonvocal communication (Armstrong et al., 1995).
These constitute, however, genuine languages with full syntactic and productive properties,
which cannot be said for so-called body language (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, [1967] 1975) or for the
communication systems of other animals (Rauch and Carr, 1980).
- Semiotic research on nonverbal communication by linguists, anthropologists, cognitive
psychologists, and others ranges from the experimental (often quantitative, considering
individual, internal, and microunits of analysis) to the naturalistic (usually qualitative,
assessing social, relational, embodied, and emergent units of analysis) (Schere and Ekman,
1982; Poyatos, 1992).
Nonverbal Communication☆
M.L. Patterson, in Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology, 2017
Summary
- Nonverbal communication is pervasive in both face-to-face and mediated communication.
Because much of nonverbal communication operates automatically and often outside of
awareness, it provides an efficient means of regulating our social contacts with others. The
determinants of biology, culture, gender, personality, and the environment shape stable
patterns of nonverbal communication. Nevertheless, the flexibility and utility of nonverbal
communication are evident in several distinct functions, including providing information,
regulating interaction, expressing intimacy, exercising influence, and managing impressions.
Thus, the complementary behavioral and social judgment tracks of nonverbal
communication constitute an indispensable system for navigating our social worlds.
- Nonverbal communication refers to the ways in which beings convey information about
their emotions, needs, intentions, attitudes, and thoughts without the use of verbal language.
- Nonverbal cues serve important functions in human social life, including expressing
emotions; conveying interpersonal attitudes such as friendliness, insult, or dominance;
regulating affect; regulating turn taking between people in conversation; and facilitating
one's own speech production.
- Nonverbal signals are important in many psychological processes, including attachment,
attraction, social influence, deception, self-presentation, and interpersonal self-fulfilling
prophecies.
- Cultural, gender, and other group differences in nonverbal behaviour have been documented
as well as individual differences in usage and in the accuracy of nonverbal cue transmission
and reception.
Language impairment
- Verbal and nonverbal communication deficits are an essential part of the autism triad.
Language generally parallels intelligence. Echolalia, while occasionally seen as a brief
developmental interlude in normal children, and infrequently seen in persistent fashion in
pure DLDs, is common in children on the autism spectrum. (Echolalic speech often portends
the development of more fluent speech, and therefore it is not necessarily a bad sign.) As
previously stated, a thorough assessment of hearing and the evaluation of a skilled speech
and language pathologist are essential.
- In low-functioning children, verbal auditory agnosia, phonologic-syntactic, and lexical–
syntactic language disorders are seen. In higher-functioning children, pragmatic and
semantic deficits are characteristic. This includes deficits in who/what/where/when/how
questions and in language turn-taking. In addition, prosody is frequently impaired, such that
these children speak in monotone rather than in well-modulated speech. Hyperactivity and
inattention relate inversely to language competence in autistic children under the age of
3 years.
- It is the consensus that language competence at 5 or 6 years of age quite accurately predicts
long-term prognosis, since language, as suggested earlier, determines intelligence, which
then relates to functionality. Chances for a child who remains nonverbal at the age of 8 or
9 years becoming linguistically competent are very poor.
-
Communication
Pictures
- Once children are able to use non-verbal communication to indicate choices using real
objects it may be appropriate to introduce pictures for expressive communication. By
replacing real objects with pictures the child will develop the skills to ‘talk’ about things
that are not actually present, in the way that a typically developing child uses spoken words.
Like signing, pictures can be effective in giving communication partners clues if the child's
speech is difficult to understand, or they may serve as an alternative form of communication
if the child does not have speech, but has specific ideas the child wishes to communicate.
- Pictures may also have a role in supporting language understanding. A child may not
understand ‘we're going to the hospital today’, but may recognize a picture of the hospital if
it is a place that the child has visited several times before.
- Pictures can vary in complexity. Initially a child may need to see a photograph of the actual
person, place or object being talked about. The next stage would be recognizing that a line
drawing (colour or black and white) can represent a person, place, object or activity. This
level of abstraction makes it possible to represent concepts that are difficult to photograph,
for example, ‘fast’ and ‘more’. Line drawings can also increase applicability of objects and
events: ‘cuddle’ can be used for a cuddle with mum or dad, granny or a doll.
- Several vocabularies of line drawings have been developed for children with limited speech.
These are usually described as symbol systems and have the advantage of consistency, that
is, the same symbols will be used in the nursery, the school and the therapy clinic. Most
have computer software packages so that parents and professionals without drawing skills
can create communication materials (see websites and addresses at the end of the chapter for
further information on different systems and sources of information).
- A good way to introduce pictures is to make personalized scrapbooks and photograph
albums. These can include photos of people who are important to the child. Leaflets, tickets
and photos from special outings and events can be collected: these could then be used when
discussing what has happened. This will enable the child to ‘talk’ about past events,
something which may not be possible for a child with limited speech. If a child has speech
that is not easily intelligible, people who were not present at the event under discussion may
be helped to understand what the child is trying to say with the aid of the pictures.
Packaging from favourite foods can be used to indicate preferences and to make choices.
Reduced versions of the covers from videos or storybooks can be a more efficient way of
presenting choices rather than having to take all the videos or books off the shelf to find out
which one the child wants. Figure 18.9 shows a chart offering the child a number of choices
for places to go.
- Experimentation may be required in order to discover how best to present picture materials
for an individual child. The size and layout of pictures on a page will need to match a child's
visual and motor skills. Some children will be able to point directly to pictures, others will
have inaccurate fist-pointing or may rely on eye-pointing. Previous experience of eye-
pointing to objects can transfer to using pictures for communication (see sections on joint
attention and gestures above). Positioning will also need careful consideration as the parent
should be able to see both the child and the pictures the child is selecting.
- If a child has limited speech but does have the ability to indicate ‘yes’ and ‘no’ to questions,
it may appear that asking questions may be a more efficient way of communicating.
Although this can undoubtedly be quick and effective in many instances, it also has its
limitations: the child must wait for the carers to ask the right question, and initiating a new
topic will be difficult. Introducing pictures is probably best seen as an initial step in the
process of expanding communication: once the child understands how powerful pictures can
be in conveying specific messages, more extensive personalized communication systems
can be developed. Vocabulary for curriculum topics, specific activities, interests and a wide
range of communication situations can be developed, enabling the child to initiate and
conduct conversations across various settings. This process of expansion of a picture-based
system is likely to take place over many years if the system is to meet the child's changing
needs.
- Printed words always accompany picture symbols, and so can be a useful part of developing
early literacy skills. Parents may question the value of pictures or symbols: why not simply
teach a child who has limited speech to read? Most children are highly competent language
users before they develop literacy skills. Literate adults who are skilled readers can forget
what it is like to be unable to read until they travel to a foreign country and are suddenly
dependent on pictures and visual signs if they cannot read the language. In developmental
terms, pictures can be read and understood several years before text can be deciphered.