Understanding Reliability Vs Validity
Understanding Reliability Vs Validity
Reliability and validity are closely related, but they mean different things. A measurement can be reliable without being
valid. However, if a measurement is valid, it is usually also reliable.
What is reliability?
Reliability refers to how consistently a method measures something. If the same result can be consistently achieved by
using the same methods under the same circumstances, the measurement is considered reliable.
You measure the temperature of a liquid sample several times under identical conditions. The thermometer displays the
same temperature every time, so the results are reliable.
A doctor uses a symptom questionnaire to diagnose a patient with a long-term medical condition. Several different doctors
use the same questionnaire with the same patient but give different diagnoses. This indicates that the questionnaire has
low reliability as a measure of the condition.
What is validity?
Validity refers to how accurately a method measures what it is intended to measure. If research has high validity, that
means it produces results that correspond to real properties, characteristics, and variations in the physical or social world.
High reliability is one indicator that a measurement is valid. If a method is not reliable, it probably isn’t valid.
If the thermometer shows different temperatures each time, even though you have carefully controlled conditions to
ensure the sample’s temperature stays the same, the thermometer is probably malfunctioning, and therefore its
measurements are not valid.
If a symptom questionnaire results in a reliable diagnosis when answered at different times and with different doctors, this
indicates that it has high validity as a measurement of the medical condition.
However, reliability on its own is not enough to ensure validity. Even if a test is reliable, it may not accurately reflect the
real situation.
The thermometer that you used to test the sample gives reliable results. However, the thermometer has not been
calibrated properly, so the result is 2 degrees lower than the true value. Therefore, the measurement is not valid.
A group of participants take a test designed to measure working memory. The results are reliable, but participants’ scores
correlate strongly with their level of reading comprehension. This indicates that the method might have low validity: the
test may be measuring participants’ reading comprehension instead of their working memory.
Validity is harder to assess than reliability, but it is even more important. To obtain useful results, the methods you use
to collect data must be valid: the research must be measuring what it claims to measure. This ensures that
your discussion of the data and the conclusions you draw are also valid.
Types of reliability
Different types of reliability can be estimated through various statistical methods.
Types of reliability
Interrater reliability The consistency of a measure across Based on an assessment criteria checklist,
raters or observers: do you get the five examiners submit substantially
same results when different people different results for the same student
conduct the same measurement? project. This indicates that the assessment
checklist has low inter-rater reliability
Types of reliability
Types of validity
The validity of a measurement can be estimated based on three main types of evidence. Each type can be evaluated
through expert judgement or statistical methods.
Types of validity
Content validity The extent to which the A test that aims to measure a class of
measurement covers all aspects of the students’ level of Spanish contains
concept being measured. reading, writing and speaking
components, but no listening
component. Experts agree that
listening comprehension is an
essential aspect of language ability,
so the test lacks content validity for
measuring the overall level of ability
in Spanish.
Criterion validity The extent to which the result of a A survey is conducted to measure
measure corresponds to other valid the political opinions of voters in a
measures of the same concept. region. If the results accurately
predict the later outcome of an
election in that region, this indicates
that the survey has high criterion
validity.
To assess the validity of a cause-and-effect relationship, you also need to consider internal validity (the design of the
experiment) and external validity (the generalizability of the results).
Ensuring validity
If you use scores or ratings to measure variations in something (such as psychological traits, levels of ability or physical
properties), it’s important that your results reflect the real variations as accurately as possible. Validity should be
considered in the very earliest stages of your research, when you decide how you will collect your data.
Ensure that your method and measurement technique are high quality and targeted to measure exactly what you want to
know. They should be thoroughly researched and based on existing knowledge.
For example, to collect data on a personality trait, you could use a standardized questionnaire that is considered reliable
and valid. If you develop your own questionnaire, it should be based on established theory or findings of previous studies,
and the questions should be carefully and precisely worded.
To produce valid and generalizable results, clearly define the population you are researching (e.g., people from a specific
age range, geographical location, or profession). Ensure that you have enough participants and that they are
representative of the population. Failing to do so can lead to sampling bias and selection bias.
Ensuring reliability
Reliability should be considered throughout the data collection process. When you use a tool or technique to collect data,
it’s important that the results are precise, stable, and reproducible.
Plan your method carefully to make sure you carry out the same steps in the same way for each measurement. This is
especially important if multiple researchers are involved.
For example, if you are conducting interviews or observations, clearly define how specific behaviors or responses will be
counted, and make sure questions are phrased the same way each time. Failing to do so can lead to errors such
as omitted variable bias or information bias.
When you collect your data, keep the circumstances as consistent as possible to reduce the influence of external factors
that might create variation in the results.
For example, in an experimental setup, make sure all participants are given the same information and tested under the
same conditions, preferably in a properly randomized setting. Failing to do so can lead to a placebo effect, Hawthorne
effect, or other demand characteristics. If participants can guess the aims or objectives of a study, they may attempt to act
in more socially desirable ways.
Section Discuss
Literature review What have other researchers done to devise and improve
methods that are reliable and valid?
Methodology How did you plan your research to ensure reliability and
validity of the measures used? This includes the chosen sample
set and size, sample preparation, external conditions and
Reliability and validity in a thesis
Section Discuss
measuring techniques.
Discussion This is the moment to talk about how reliable and valid your
results actually were. Were they consistent, and did they reflect
true values? If not, why not?
Conclusion If reliability and validity were a big problem for your findings,
it might be helpful to mention this here.