The Human Side of Animals
The Human Side of Animals
This ebook is for the use of anyone anywhere in the United States and most
other parts of the world at no cost and with almost no restrictions
whatsoever. You may copy it, give it away or re-use it under the terms of
the Project Gutenberg License included with this ebook or online at
www.gutenberg.org. If you are not located in the United States, you will
have to check the laws of the country where you are located before using
this eBook.
Language: English
NEW YORK
FREDERICK A. STOKES COMPANY
PUBLISHERS
Copyright, 1918, by
Frederick A. Stokes Company
MADE IN U. S. A.
TO
MARCELLUS E. FOSTER
WHO BELIEVED
NOTE
The author wishes to acknowledge his indebtedness to his
fellow-naturalist and friend, Mr. Franklyn Everett Fitch, for
carefully
reading the entire manuscript and making many scholarly and
valuable
criticisms and corrections.
CONTENTS
CHAPTER PAGE
Foreword xiii
IAnimals That Practise Camouflage 1
IIAnimal Musicians 18
IIIAnimals at Play 32
IVArmour-Bearing and Mail-Clad Animals 46
VMiners and Excavators 61
VIAnimal Mathematicians 88
VIIThe Language of Animals 99
VIIIIn Their Boudoirs, Hospitals and Churches 120
IXSelf-Defence and Home-Government 130
XArchitects, Engineers, and House-Builders 150
XIFood Conservers 170
XIITourists and Sight-Seers 181
XIIIAnimal Scavengers and Criminals 199
XIVAs the Allies of Man 210
XVThe Future Life of Animals 234
ILLUSTRATIONS
Recreation is as common among animals as it Frontispiece
is among children (in Colours)
The Indians claim that the mother bison forced 6
her calf to roll often in a puddle of red clay, so
that it might be indistinguishable against its
clay background
The zebra is one of the cleverest of 7
camouflagers. The black-and-white stripes of
his body give the effect of sunlight passing
through bushes
Monkeys are the most musical of all animals. 20
When they congregate for "concerts," as some
of the tribes do, the air is filled with weird
strains of monkey-music
Cats, unlike dogs, are very fond of music. And 21
it has been proved that their music-sense can
be developed to a remarkable degree
A happy family of polar bears. The young 34
cubs wrestle and tumble, as playfully as two
puppies. This play has much to do with their
physical and mental development
Dryptosaurus. The prehistoric animals, too, 35
undoubtedly had their play time, with games
and "setting up" exercises
The mother opossum is never happier than 38
when she has her little ones playing hide-and-
seek over her back
This young fox came from his home in the 39
woods daily to play with a young fox-terrier.
He is now resting after a romp
Naosaurus and Dimetrodon, two extinct 50
armour-bearers who should have been well
able to protect themselves
An armour-bearer of prehistoric times whose 51
shield was an effective protection against
enemy horns
To the polar bear the ice and snow of the Far 84
North means warmth and protection. The
mother bear digs herself into a snowbank,
where lives quite comfortably throughout the
winter
The sharp claws of the ground squirrel are 85
efficacious tools in digging his cosy
underground burrow
The coyote can readily distinguish whether a 94
herd of sheep is guarded by one or more dogs,
and will plan his attack accordingly
The zebu, the sacred bull of India, in spite of 95
its domestication, has an agile body and a
quick, alert mind
Roosevelt's Colobus. These horse-tailed 112
monkeys chatter together in a language
exclusively their own, yet they seem to have
no difficulty in making themselves understood
by other monkey-tribes
A tamed deer of Texas, whose constant 113
companion and playmate was a rabbit dog.
Between the two, there developed, necessarily,
a common language
Water-loving animals, like the beavers, 122
seemingly take great pride in their toilets.
Their fur is always sleek and clean
Great forest pigs of Central Africa. Like the 123
common domesticated hogs, they will seek a
clay bath to heal their wounds
The Rocky Mountain goat has many means of 134
defence, not the least of which is his agility in
climbing to inaccessible places
Wild boars are among the most ferocious of 135
animals. By means of their great strength
alone they are well able to defend themselves
Brontosaurus. The animals that seemed best 144
equipped to defend themselves are the ones
that, thousands of years ago, became extinct
This prehistoric monster was equipped not 145
only with a pair of strong horns but with a
shield back of them as well
The beaver is the greatest of all animal 158
architects. His skill is equalled only by his
patience (in Colours)
The skunk mother tries to keep on hand a good 172
supply of such delicacies as frogs and toads,
so that her young may never go hungry
The porcupine and the hedgehog have a 173
unique method of collecting food for their
young. After shaking down berries or grapes,
they roll in them, then hurry home with the
food attached to their quills
The black bear is not one of the great 188
migrating animals. The thickness of his coat
must therefore change with the seasons
Rabbits seem to have a well-devised system in 189
their road-building, running their paths in and
out of underbrush in a truly ingenious manner
The mongoose, a scavenger of the worst type, 202
feeding on rats and mice and snakes, and even
poultry
Diplodocus. The prehistoric animals, also, 203
undoubtedly had their scavengers and
criminals
The Esquimo-dog is man's greatest friend in 218
the Far North
Chipmunks are among the most easily tamed 219
of man's wild friends, and they even seem
fond of human companionship
Men cruelly take the lives of these denizens of 244
the wildwood, rejoicing in their slaughter, but
the animal soul they cannot kill
Two pals. There is between man and dog a 245
kinship of spirit that cannot be denied
FOREWORD
"And in the lion or the frog—
In all the life of moor or fen—
In ass and peacock, stork and dog,
He read similitudes of men."
More and more science is being taught in a new way. More and more men
are beginning to discard the lumber of the brain's workshop to get at real
facts, real conclusions. Laboratories, experiments, tables, classifications are
all very vital and all very necessary but sometimes their net result is only to
befog and confuse. Occasionally it becomes important for us to cast aside
all dogmatic restraints and approach the wonders of life from a new angle
and with the untrammelled spirit of a little child.
In this book I have attempted to bring together many old and new
observations which tend to show the human-like qualities of animals. The
treatment is neither formal nor scholastic, in fact I do not always remain
within the logical confines of the title. My sole purpose is to make the
reader self-active, observative, free from hide-bound prejudice, and reborn
as a participant in the wonderful experiences of life which fill the universe.
I hope to lead him into a new wonderland of truth, beauty and love, a land
where his heart as well as his eyes will be opened.
In attempting to understand the animals I have used a method a great deal
like that of the village boy, who when questioned as to how he located the
stray horse for which a reward of twenty dollars had been offered, replied,
"I just thought what I would do if I were a horse and where I would go—
and there I went and found him." In some such way I have tried to think
why animals do certain things, I have studied them in many places and
under all conditions, and those acts of theirs which, if performed by
children, would come under the head of wisdom and intelligence, I have
classified as such.
Life is one throughout. The love that fills a mother's heart when she sees her
first-born babe, is also felt by the mother bear, only in a different way, when
she sees her baby cubs playing before her humble cave dwelling. The
sorrow that is felt by the human heart when a beloved one dies is
experienced in only a little less degree by an African ape when his mate is
shot dead by a Christian missionary. The grandmother sheep that watches
her numerous little lamb grandchildren on the hillside, while their mothers
are away grazing, is just as mindful of their care as any human grandparent
could be. One drop of water is like the ocean; and love is love.
The trouble with science is that too often it leaves out love. If you agree that
we cannot treat men like machines, why should we put animals in that
class? Why should we fall into the colossal ignorance and conceit of
cataloging every human-like action of animals under the word "instinct"?
Man delights in thinking of himself as only a little lower than the angels.
Then why should he not consider the animals as only a little lower than
himself? The poet has truly said that "the beast is the mirror of man as man
is the mirror of God." Man had to battle with animals for untold ages before
he domesticated and made servants of them. He is just beginning to learn
that they were not created solely to furnish material for sermons, nor to
serve mankind, but that they also have an existence, a life of their own.
Man has long preached this doctrine that he is not an animal, but a kinsman
of the gods. For this reason, he has claimed dominion over animal creation
and a right to assert that dominion without restraint. This anthropocentric
conceit is the same thing that causes one nation to think it should rule the
world, that the sun and moon were made only for the laudable purpose of
giving light unto a chosen few, and that young lambs playing on a grassy
hillside, near a cool spring, are just so much mutton allowed to wander over
man's domain until its flavour is improved.
It is time to remove the barriers, once believed impassable, which man's
egotism has used as a screen to separate him from his lower brothers. Our
physical bodies are very similar to theirs except that ours are almost always
much inferior. Merely because we have a superior intellect which enables
us to rule and enslave the animals, shall we deny them all intellect and all
feeling? In the words of that remarkable naturalist, William J. Long, "To
call a thing intelligence in one creature and reflex action in another, or to
speak of the same thing as love or kindness in one and blind impulse in the
other, is to be blinder ourselves than the impulse which is supposed to
govern animals. Until, therefore, we have some new chemistry that will
ignore atoms and the atomic law, and some new psychology that ignores
animal intelligence altogether, or regards it as under a radically different
law from our own, we must apply what we know of ourselves and our own
motives to the smaller and weaker lives that are in some distant way akin to
our own."
It is possible to explain away all the marvellous things the animals do, but
after you have finished, there will still remain something over and above,
which quite defies all mechanistic interpretation. An old war horse, for
instance, lives over and over his battles in his dreams. He neighs and paws,
just as he did in real battle; and cavalrymen tell us that they can sometimes
understand from their horses when they are dreaming just what command
they are trying to obey. This is only one of the myriads of animal
phenomena which man does not understand. If you doubt it, try to explain
the striking phenomena of luminescence, hybridization, of eels surviving
desiccation for fourteen years, post-matrimonial cannibalism, Nature's vast
chain of unities, the suicide of lemmings, why water animals cannot get
wet, transparency of animals, why the horned toad shoots a stream of blood
from his eye when angry. If you are able to explain these things to
humanity, you will be classed second only to Solomon. Yet the average
scientist explains them away, with the ignorance and loquaciousness of a
fisher hag.
By a thorough application of psychological principles, it is possible to show
that man himself is merely a machine to be explained in terms of neurones
and nervous impulses, heredity and environment and reactions to outside
stimuli. But who is there who does not believe that there is more to a man
than that?
Animals have demonstrated long ago that they not only have as many
talents as human beings, but that under the influence of the same
environment, they form the same kinds of combinations to defend
themselves against enemies; to shelter themselves against heat and cold; to
build homes; to lay up a supply of food for the hard seasons. In fact, all
through the ages man has been imitating the animals in burrowing through
the earth, penetrating the waters, and now, at last, flying through the air.
When a skunk bites through the brains of frogs, paralysing but not killing
them, in order that he may store them away in his nursery-pantry so that his
babes may have fresh food; when a mole decapitates earth-worms for the
same reason and stores them near the cold surface of the ground so that the
heads will not regrow, as they would under normal conditions, only a
deeply prejudiced man can claim that no elements of intelligence have been
employed.
There are also numerous signs, sounds and motions by which animals
communicate with each other, though to man these symbols of language
may not always be understandable. Dogs give barks indicating surprise,
pleasure and all other emotions. Cows will bellow for days when mourning
for their dead. The mother bear will bury her dead cub and silently guard its
grave for weeks to prevent its being desecrated. The mother sheep will bleat
most pitifully when her lamb strays away. Foxes utter expressive cries
which their children know full well. The chamois, when frightened, whistle;
they might be termed the policemen of the animal world. The sentinel will
continue a long, drawn-out whistle, as long as he can without taking a
breath. He then stops for a brief moment, looks in all directions, and begins
blowing again. If the danger comes too near, he scampers away.
In their ability to take care of their wounded bodies, in their reading of the
weather and in all forms of woodcraft, animals undoubtedly possess
superhuman powers. Even squirrels can prophesy an unusually long and
severe winter and thus make adequate preparations. Some animals act as
both barometers and thermometers. It is claimed that while frogs remain
yellow, only fair weather may be expected, but if their colour changes to
brown, ill weather is coming.
There is no limit to the marvellous things animals do. Elephants, for
example, carry leafy palms in their trunks to shade themselves from the hot
sun. The ape or baboon who puts a stone in the open oyster to prevent it
from closing, or lifts stones to crack nuts, or beats his fellows with sticks, or
throws heavy cocoanuts from trees upon his enemies, or builds a fire in the
forest, shows more than a glimmer of intelligence. In the sly fox that puts
out fish heads to bait hawks, or suddenly plunges in the water and immerses
himself to escape hunters, or holds a branch of a bush over his head and
actually runs with it to hide himself; in the wolverine who catches deer by
dropping moss, and suddenly springing upon them and clawing their eyes
out; in the bear, who, as told in the account of Cook's third voyage, "rolls
down pieces of rock to crush stags; in the rat when he leads his blind
brother with a stick" is actual reasoning. Indeed, there is nothing which man
makes with all his ingenious use of tools and instruments, of which some
suggestion may not be seen in animal creation.
Great thinkers of all ages are not wanting who believe that animals have a
portion of that same reason which is the pride of man. Montaigne admitted
that they had both thought and reason, and Pope believed that even a cat
may consider a man made for his service. Humboldt, Helvitius, Darwin and
Smellie claimed that animals act as a definite result of actual reasoning.
Lord Brougham pertinently observes, "I know not why so much
unwillingness should be shown by some excellent philosophers to allow
intelligent faculties and a share of reason to the lower animals, as if our own
superiority was not quite sufficiently established to leave all jealousy out of
view by the immeasurably higher place which we occupy in the scale of
being."
From the facts enumerated in this book I find that animals are possessed of
love, hate, joy, grief, courage, revenge, pain, pleasure, want and satisfaction
—that all things that go to make up man's life are also found in them. In the
attempt to establish this thesis I have been led mentally and physically into
some of Nature's most fascinating highways and hedges, where I have had
many occasions to wonder and adore. I will be happy if I have at least
added something to the depth of love and appreciation with which most
men look upon the animal world.
Royal Dixon.
New York, April, 1918.
THE HUMAN SIDE OF ANIMALS
I
ANIMALS THAT PRACTISE CAMOUFLAGE
Mimetic resemblances are worked out with great difficulty, except in such
cases as the nocturnal animals, which simply become one with their
surroundings. Mice, rats, moles, and bats wear overcoats that are very
inconspicuous, and when suddenly approached they appear almost
invisible. Some of the North American Indians claimed that buffaloes made
their calves wallow in the red clay to prevent them from being seen when
they were lying down in the red soil.
The kinds of protection from these mimetic resemblances are many and
varied: the lion, because of his sandy-colouring, is able to conceal himself
by merely crouching down upon the desert sands; the striped tiger hides
among the tufts of grass and bamboos of the tropics, the stripes of his body
so blending with the vertical stems as to prevent even the natives from
seeing him in this position. The kudu, one of the handsomest of the
antelopes, is a remarkable animal in several ways. His camouflage is so
perfect that it gives him magnificent courage. With his spiral horns, white
face, and striped coat tinted in pale blue, he is almost invisible when hiding
in a thicket. The perfect harmony of his horns with the twisted vines and
branches, and the white colourings with blue tints in the reflected sunlight
conceal him entirely.
The snow-leopard, which inhabits Central Asia, is stony-grey, with large
annular spots to match the rocks among which he lives. This colouration
conceals him from the sheep, upon which he preys; while the spotted and
blotchy pattern of the so-called clouded tiger, and the peculiarly-barred skin
of the ocelot, imitate the rugged bark of trees, upon which these animals
live.
One of the most unusual and skilled mimics is the Indian sloth, whose
colour pattern and unique eclipsing effects seem almost incredible to those
unfamiliar with the real facts. His home is in the trees, and he has a deep,
orange-coloured spot on his back, which would make him very conspicuous
if seen out of his home surroundings. But he is very clever, and clings to the
moss-draped trees, where the effect of the orange-coloured spot is exactly
like the scar on the tree, while his hair resembles the withered moss so
strikingly that even naturalists are deceived.
Henry Drummond must have known the animal world rather well when he
remarked that "Carlisle in his blackest visions of 'shams and humbugs'
among humanity never saw anything so finished in hypocrisy as the
naturalist now finds in every tropical forest. There are to be seen creatures,
not singly, but in tens of thousands, whose every appearance, down to the
minutest spot and wrinkle, is an affront to truth, whose every attitude is a
pose for a purpose, and whose whole life is a sustained lie. Before these
masterpieces of deception the most ingenious of human impositions are
vulgar and transparent. Fraud is not only the great rule of life in a tropical
forest, but the one condition of it."
Many of the larger cats live in trees, and most of them have spotted or
oscillated skins, which aid them in hiding among foliage plants. The puma
who wears a brown coat is an exception, but it must be remembered that he
does not need the kind of coat his fellow friends wear. He clings so closely
to the body of a tree while waiting for his prey as to be almost invisible.
This phenomenon is true throughout the animal world. Everywhere does
Nature aid in escape and capture. Only those skilled in the ways of the wild
fully realise how conspicuous amidst foliage, for instance, would be a
uniform colouration. A parti-coloured pattern is extremely deceptive and
thus protective, and for this reason one seldom sees in Nature a background
of one colour; and since the large majority of animals need concealment, it
is necessary for them to be clothed in patterns that vary.
These variations are especially noticeable in young animals, and furnish
them with a mantle that is practically invisible to predatory enemies during
the time they are left unprotected by their parents. These protective mantles
often differ strikingly in pattern and colouration from those of their parents,
and indicate that the young animals present the colouration and pattern of
their remote forbears. It might even be said that "the skins of the fathers are
thrust upon the children, even unto the third and fourth generation!" In fact,
it is quite probable that they give through this varying colouration the "life-
history" of their family.
In all hoofed animals—antelope, deer, horses—the protective colouration is
also adapted to habitat and environment. Most deer belong to the forest,
carefully avoiding the open deserts and staying near water. They live
chiefly in the jungle or scrub, and are usually spotted with red and white in
such a way as to be almost invisible to a casual observer; some, however,
that live in the very shady places are uniformly dark so as to harmonise
with their surroundings. The wild horses and asses of Central Asia are dun-
coloured—corresponding exactly to their sandy habitat.
The Shakesperian conception of the human world as a stage may be
paralleled in the animal world. Animals, like human beings, have all a
definite rôle to play in the drama of life. Each is given certain equipment in
form, colour, voice, demeanour, ambitions, desires, and natural habitat.
Some are given much, others but little. Many have succeeded well in the art
of camouflage while endeavouring to make a success in life. This success
has brought the desired opportunity of mating, rearing young, bequeathing
to them their special gifts and living in ease and comfort.
One of the most successful and striking cases of protective colouration in
young animals is found in wild swine. Here there is longitudinal striping
which marks them from head to tail in broad white bands, over a
background of reddish dark brown. The tapirs have a most unique form of
marking. It is similar in the young of the South American and Malayan
species. Their bodies are exquisitely marked in snow-white bars. At their
extremities these bars are broken up into small dots which tend to overlap
each other. During the daytime these young animals seek the shade of the
bushes and as the spots of sunlight fall upon the ground they appear so
nearly one with their environment as to pass unnoticed by their enemies.
The adults, however, vary greatly one from another in colouration. The
American species is self-coloured, while the Malayan has the most unique
pattern known to the animal world. The fore-quarters, the head, and the
hind-legs are black, while the rest of the body from the shoulders
backwards is of a dirt-white colour.
It has been observed by all students of Nature that bold and gaudy animals
usually have means of defending themselves that make them very
disagreeable to their enemies. They either have poisonous fangs, sharp
spines, ferocious claws, or disagreeable odours. There are still others that
escape destruction because of the bad company with which they are
associated by their enemies.
The reptiles offer us many good examples of mimicry. Most arboreal lizards
wear the colour of the leaves upon which they feed; the same is true of the
whip-snakes and the tiny green tree-frogs. A striking example of successful
camouflage is found in the case of a North American frog whose home is on
lichen-covered rocks and walls, which he so closely imitates in colour and
pattern as to pass unnoticed so long as he remains quiet. I have seen an
immense frog, whose home was in a damp cave, with large green and black
spots over his body precisely like the spots on the sides of his home.
Author Note: The word "mimicry" as used here implies a
particular kind of resemblance only, a resemblance in
external appearance, never internal, a resemblance that
deceives. It does not imply voluntary imitation. Both the
words "mimicry" and "imitation" are used to imply
outward likeness. The object of the outward likeness or
resemblance is to cause a harmless or unprotected animal to
be mistaken for the dangerous one which he oftentimes
imitates; or to aid the unprotected animal in escaping
unnoticed among the surroundings he may simulate.
A splendid example of pure bluff is shown in the case of the harmless
Australian lizard, known scientifically under the name of chlamydosaurus
kingii. When he is undisturbed he seems perfectly inoffensive, but when he
becomes angry, he becomes a veritable fiend-like reptile. In this condition
he stands up on his hind legs, opens his gaping mouth, showing the most
terrible teeth, which, by the way, have never been known to bite anything.
Besides this forbidding display he further adds to his terrible appearance by
raising the most extraordinary frill which is exquisitely decorated in grey,
yellow, scarlet, and blue. This he uses like an umbrella, and if in this way he
does not succeed in frightening away his enemy, he rushes at him, and
lashes him with his saw-like tail. Even dogs are terrified at such camouflage
and leave the successful bluffer alone.
In all parts of the tropics are tree-snakes that lie concealed among the
boughs and shrubs. Most of them are green, and some have richly coloured
bands around their bodies which look not unlike gaily coloured flowers, and
which, no doubt, attract flower-seeking insects and birds. Among these may
be mentioned the deadly-poisonous snakes of the genus elaps of South
America. They are so brilliantly provided with bright red and black bands
trimmed with yellow rings that it is not uncommon for a plant collector to
attempt to pick them up for rare orchids!
Wherever these snakes are found, are also found a number of perfectly
harmless snakes, absolutely unlike the dangerous ones in habit and life, yet
coloured precisely the same. The elaps fulvius, for example, a deadly
venomous snake of Guatemala, has a body trimmed in simple black bands
on a coral-red ground, and in the same country and always with him is
found a quite harmless snake, which is coloured and banded in the same
identical manner. The terrible and much-feared elaps lemnicatus has the
peculiar black bands divided into divisions of three by narrow yellow rings,
thus exactly mimicking a harmless snake, the pliocerus elapoides, both of
which live in Mexico. Presumably, the deadly variety assumes the colouring
of the harmless kind in order to deceive intended victims as to his ferocity.
Surely this is sufficient evidence that colouration and pattern-design is a
useful camouflage device of the great struggle for existence. And it is safe
to assert that any animal that has enemies and still does not resort to
protective colouration or mimicry in some form is entirely able to protect
itself either by its size, strength, ferocity, or by resorting to safety in
numbers. Elephants and rhinoceroses, for example, are too powerful to be
molested when grown, except in the rarest cases, and are furthermore
thoroughly capable of protecting their young. Hippopotamuses are
protected by their immense heads, and are capable of defending their young
from crocodiles even when in the water.
The bison and buffalo, which were once so powerful on the plains of North
America, were protected by their gregarious habits, which terrorised their
enemies—the wolves. Their nurseries were a feature of their wisdom. These
were circular pens where the tall grass was tramped down by expectant
mothers for the protection of their young. This natural nursery was
protected from the inside by sentinels who went round and round the pen
constantly guarding the young not only from the attack of wolves but also
from venturing forth alone too early into the open unprotected plains. In a
similar way the snow-pens of the moose of the Far North serve to protect
them from the hungry hordes of wolves of which they live in constant
danger. This indicates that the annihilation of the bison and buffalo was due,
not to lack of wisdom, but to man's inhumanity; for, taking advantage of
their nurseries, the men crouched near and concealing themselves in the
grass killed not only the mothers for food but even the young in their
savage sport.
The large majority of monkeys are protectively coloured with some shade
of brown or grey, with specially marked faces. Entire packs of Ceylonese
species will, at the slightest alarm, become invisible by crouching on a
palm-tree. One of the most strikingly coloured African monkeys is jet black
with a white bushy tail, and a face surrounded by a white ring, or mantle of
long silky hair. He thus simulates so strikingly the hanging white lichens
upon the trees that he is rarely seen by his enemies.
A book might be written upon the various ways that animals, when closely
associated with other animals or human beings, imitate them. Darwin says
that "two species of wolves, which had been reared by dogs, learned to
bark, as does sometimes the jackall," and it is well known that certain dogs,
when reared by cats, imitate their habits, even to the licking of their feet and
the washing of their faces. If a mongrel dog associates with a trained dog
for any period of time it is remarkable the progress he will make. For this
same reason young dogs are carried on hunting trips with trained dogs that
they may learn by imitation the art of hunting.
In the whole realm of Nature there is nothing more wonderful than this
matter of protective colouration. Animals do not monopolise the art. It
extends through the whole world of living creatures. The fact that
individual animals have no voluntary control over their own colour is
eloquent testimony as to the existence of mysterious life forces and racial
evolutions which are still far beyond the grasp of man's understanding. To
see a tiny chameleon adapt his colouring to his environment, be it red,
green, or yellow, in the twinkling of an eye, is to have seen an argument for
God Himself.
II
ANIMAL MUSICIANS
—Watson.
The great thinkers of the age believe that the world is one marvellous
blending of innumerable and varied voices. This unison of sound forms the
great music of the spheres, which the poets and philosophers have written
so much about. Even from a purely scientific point of view, there is no
denying that this music exists. Aviators tell us that when they listen from a
distance to the myriads of noises and sounds that arise over a great city,
these are all apparently lost in a modulated hum precisely like the vibrations
of an immense tuning-fork, and appearing as but a single tone. Thus the
immense noise going from our world is musically digested into one tone,
and the aviator soaring above the earth hears only the one sound—the
music of the spheres.
The deep appreciation that animals have for music is becoming a generally
known fact among those who have studied them closely. Every one must
admit that there is much truth in the old saying that "music hath charms to
soothe the savage breast." Music is composed of vibrations, which act with
great power upon the nervous system of men and animals alike. Each is
affected according to his particular physical and mental development.
Professor Tarchanoff has made a careful study of the influence of music
upon men and animals. He has demonstrated, by means of a machine which
carefully registers the various activities of the hands and fingers, that when
the hands are so tired and fatigued that they cannot make any marks except
a straight line on the cylinder which registers the movements, music will so
stimulate the nerves as to cause all fatigue to disappear. And as soon as the
fingers again touch the cylinder, they begin to draw lines of various kinds
and heights, thus proving that the music had rested the fingers and placed
them under control. Various kinds of music were used: that of a melancholy
nature had precisely the opposite effect to that of a lively, cheerful
character; the nerves of the hands could either be contracted or expanded
according to the nature of the music.
Like all real scientists, Professor Tarchanoff does not claim to give any
positive explanation of these facts. He believes, however, that the voluntary
muscles act in the same relation to the music as the heart—that is, that
cheerful, happy music affects the excito-motor nerves, sets up a vibration in
those nerves which produces cheer and good feeling; while sad, morbid
music plays along the depressant nerves and produces sadness and
depression.
In view of these facts, it is easy to see how animals, with their nervous
temperaments and ready response to outside stimuli, are greatly influenced
by various kinds of music. It is scientifically recognised that music tends to
increase the elimination of carbonic acid and increases not only the
consumption of oxygen, but even the activities of the skin. There is no
doubt that good music at meal time aids the digestion.
American Museum of Natural History, New York
Cats have a species of unbeautiful music all their own, generally produced
at late hours of the night on the house tops, garden walls, and in the alleys
of our dwellings. Miss Cat's songs are far too chromatic to be appreciated
by human ears; as a result her concertos and solos are rarely spoken of by
human critics. However, Nature does sometimes produce a Tetrazzini, Alice
Neilson, or Caruso, in the form of a cat, which really delights in
harmonious combinations of sound. I know, for instance, of a cat called
"Nordica" owned by Presson Miller, who apparently takes the greatest
delight in hearing good vocal and instrumental music. Another well-
educated musical cat belongs to a friend who plays a guitar. This cat
delights in touching the strings with his dainty, soft paws, and springs with
delight as the notes are produced.
The Animal World speaks of five musical cats, which were carried to
various parts of the world and exhibited as "bell-ringers," and their owner
made a fortune out of their concerts. Five bells were suspended from a
hoop, which hung above the stage, and to each bell was attached a small
rope. At a given signal, each cat would seize a bell and give it a pull. This
was done with such perfect time and spirit that one might well believe it
was the work of human musicians and not of cats.
Cows are responsive to certain kinds of music. A funeral march makes them
sad, and ragtime so disturbs them that they give but little milk. The
newspapers claim that Charles W. Ward, who owns a ranch near Eureka,
California, says that the right kind of music will increase the production of
milk, and that he uses a phonograph in the dairy barn.
A friend, who has travelled much, tells the story of a musical cow. He, in
company with two other friends, was coming up a river in a small boat
singing. Just as they turned a bend, they saw a small brown cow, suckling
her calf, along with several other cows in a nearby pasture. The cow seemed
so fascinated with the music that she plunged into the water and waded up
to her head trying to reach the boat. As they rowed along, she ran up and
down the bank, cutting capers in a most astonishing manner and lowing and
bellowing in testimony of her delight in the music. She would leap, skip,
roll on the grass, paw up the earth, like an angry bull, and chase off like a
playful kitten, always with a low plaintive bellow as a final farewell. These
friends often rowed up the river just to see if the musical cow was there,
and she always greeted them in the usual appreciative manner.
Lions and tigers are proverbially fond of music. Professional trainers tell us
that these animals, when tamed, will not do their stunts without the
accompaniment of music. The story is told of a group of tigers which
recently refused to perform, because the musicians, while the performance
was going on, went on a strike. At once when the music ceased, the animals
returned to their respective seats and no amount of encouragement would
induce them to continue their performance. No amount of threats would
induce them to work without music. The trainer dared not punish them too
severely, yet he feared that if they were not forced to perform, they might
continue to strike. But such was not the case, for on the morrow when the
musicians returned they acted as never before.
Sheep, both tame and wild, are exceedingly fond of music, and the
shepherds of Scotland have used it with their sheep for ages. When the
shepherd plays upon his flute or bagpipe, they gather around him and listen
apparently with great satisfaction; when the music ceases, they wander out
to feed, and in the evening he leads them home by the single strains of his
flute.
Circus horses are not only fond of music, but are partial to certain tunes,
and demand that these be played while they are doing their turn. If for any
reason the band changes the tune during a performance, they immediately
refuse to go on with their stunts.
The original fountain of all music was based on the various voices and
sounds of animals—and each musical instrument was originally devised to
imitate these sounds. For all instruments—the bass drum, flute, clarinet,
trombone, trumpet, violin, and even pipe organ—an animal may be
mentioned that owns the fundamental tones in its voice, and which man has
imitated. Castanets, for example, were imitations of the rattlesnakes; the
first musical instruments of any savage tribe of men are made so as to
represent the voices of the chief animals of that particular locality.
Every animal of the higher order, with the exception of a few mute dogs
that belong to very hot or cold climates, is possessed of some sort of
musical tone, expressive of pain or joy, and by means of which he can
express certain emotions. Darwin claimed that the voice of the gibbon,
while extremely loud, was very musical; and Waterhouse said that this
musician sang the scale with considerable accuracy, at least sufficiently
well for a trained violinist to accompany him.
Often when dogs hear music they howl, or attempt to sing. Some show a
decided preference for certain kinds of music, and actually try to imitate it.
Gross tells of a friend of his who had a dog with which he often gave
performances. The dog would accompany his master, when he sang in
falsetto, with howls that were unmistakably attempts at singing, and which
readily adapted themselves to the pitch of the tone. This was a musical
accomplishment of which he was very proud.
On a subject of which so little is known, there are, of course, diverse
opinions. Scheitlin believed that music is actually disagreeable to a dog, but
he says that it may be questioned whether or not the dog does not in some
way accompany it. And Romanes, the great animal authority, thought the
same thing. He had a terrier, which accompanied him when he sang, and
actually succeeded in following the prolonged notes of the human voice
with a certain approximation to unison. Dr. Higgins, a musician, claimed
that his large mastiff could sing to the accompaniment of the organ.
Alix gives such positive examples that they are really marvellous: "Pere
Pardies cites the case of two dogs that had been taught to sing, one of them
taking a part with his master. Pierquin de Gembloux also speaks of a poodle
that could run the scale in tune and sing very agreeably a fine composition
of Mozart's My Heart It Sings at Eve." All the scientists in Paris, according
to the same authority, went to see the dog belonging to Dr. Bennati, and
hear it sing the scale, which it could do perfectly.
Monkeys and apes most nearly approximate human musicians. In central
Africa these animal tribes have musical centres where they congregate
regularly for "concerts." Prof. Richard S. Garner, the noted authority on
apes and monkeys, believes that the time has already come for the
establishment of a school for their education. He would have the courses
beginning with a kindergarten and advancing through as many grades as the
students required. Prof. Garner furthermore believes that we have little
understanding of the gorilla, and points out that these animals have a very
happy and harmonious home life, the father being highly domestic and
delighting in the company of his wife and children. It is not uncommon to
find five or six generations in a certain district of the jungle.
Their near kin, the chimpanzees, are equally clannish, but more musical.
They come down from the branches of the trees, seating themselves on the
dry leaves and assembling like an orchestra. After all are ready, they begin
beating the leaves with their hands, at first very slowly, like the quiet
prelude to a symphony, and gradually increasing in tempo until the grand
crescendo is reached. Then, as if by the direction of an invisible leader, the
music suddenly ceases. To deny that this is to them a real concert would
lead us into extreme absurdities. In this connection it is interesting to note
that when a baby is expected in the village, all music ceases until after its
birth, when they again resume their periodic musical festivals. Hensel
verifies this observation, and tells us of having seen apes come from their
shelter in the early morning and congregate for a musical concert. "They
repair," he says, "to the shelter of some gigantic monarch of the forest
whose limbs offer facilities for walking exercises. The head of the family
appropriates one of these branches and advances along it seriously, with
elevated tail, while the others group themselves about him. Soon he gives
forth soft single notes, as the lion likes to do when he tests the capacity of
his lungs. This sound, which seems to be made by drawing the breath in and
out, becomes deeper and in more rapid succession as the excitement of the
singer increases. At last, when the highest pitch is reached, the intervals
cease and the sound becomes a continuous roar, and at this point all the
others, male and female, join in, and for fully ten seconds at a time the
awful chorus sounds through the quiet forest. At the close the leader begins
again with the detached sounds."
Perhaps the most remarkable evidence of animals showing a comprehensive
intelligence of musical pitch is demonstrated by cavalry horses. That they
thoroughly understand it is clearly demonstrated by the fact that they will
obey the calls of the bugle for cavalry evolutions without a moment's
hesitation and with no suggestion from outside sources. These bugle calls
are produced by a combination of four notes, each of a different pitch, and it
is rarer to find a horse making a mistake in the musical orders given than it
is for their masters.
Rats and mice have a decided liking for music, as is attested by the fact that
they appear as uninvited guests and also come as near the performer as
possible. Mice, one would believe, love church music, for they often build
their nests in pipe organs, thus being able to rear their children in both a
musical and religious atmosphere! There is more truth than imagination in
the story of the Pied Piper of Hamelin, which illustrates how they respond
to the simple charms of music.
Even donkeys betray tendencies toward musical efforts, and seem to be
aroused by music at least temporarily to a higher mental plane than Balaam
was inclined to ascribe to his wise ass. Not all of them sing equally well,
but in Arizona the donkey is known as the "desert canary." If you were to
spend a few glorious days in the Hopi village of Araibi, you would hear
through the still, silent night their long nasal bray or song, and you would
be convinced that the term is quite appropriate. You may not exactly like
the tune, but you will concede that they sing!
Society is just awakening to the joy and the significance of community art.
This is everywhere indicated by the great growing group of people who
come together for a common music, either as a chorus or an orchestra or
both. But in this field man has not yet attained such unity of communal
effort as have the frogs. In the great swamps of the world myriads of them
gather from miles around, conscious of one purpose, and by a marvellous
understanding and co-operation create for themselves a symphony with
beauties and harmonies of its own, and such as to stand unrivalled in man's
musical world. In the great chorus are voices from the lowest bass of the
croaking bullfrog, squatting in the marshes, to the myriads of tiny green tree
tenors, between which are millions of altos, contraltos, sopranos,
coloraturas and other voices not yet in our musical vocabulary. These are
accompanied by all the sounds of our orchestra and innumerable others of
such delicate shades and gradations as to defy the ear of man. If we listen to
one of these concerts, we will quickly recognise the tones of every familiar
instrument, such as the drum, pipe, horn, trombone, oboe, piccolo, 'cello,
and violin. The greatest of these musical festivals directly precedes the
mating season, and is a dramatic instance of a manifestation of an inner
rhythm which corresponds to an external periodicity.
Among the oldest traditions of the Eastern world are those of snake-
charming by means of music. I have long been interested in this strange
phenomenon of Nature, and in company with a brilliant young violinist
visited a zoological park recently, and after securing permission from the
head keeper, entered the snake-house. The violinist began by playing a few
most sympathetic chords, first delicate and soft, then sad, then gay, slow or
tremulous. Near us, coiled in his immense cage, was a large cobra—the
snake which all legend claims is most easily influenced by music. Almost
immediately after the music began, the cobra raised himself in a listening
attitude, steadily gazed at us as though he were viewing the future, spread
his immense hood, and slowly began to shake his head from side to side, as
if he were trying to keep time to the music. As soon as the music would
change, his attitude changed accordingly. Only after the music had ceased
did he resume his normal position.
The Indians agree that under the influence of various musical instruments,
especially bagpipes, snake-charmers are able to get the snakes to come out
from their homes among the old rocks and walls, and when they appear
they seem perfectly dazed so that they can be easily captured.
It is not well to have any kind of musical instrument played, when in a
forest at night where there are dangerous snakes, lest they come to hear it.
Snake-hunters always carry with them some kind of musical instrument,
depending upon the kind of snakes they wish to capture. It seems that all are
not equally fascinated by it. I have experimented with little effect upon a
large rattler; it may have been that he was deaf. But he gave little evidence
of being interested.
We need not feel humiliated, then, for our animal kinspeople with their
primitive music: we were monkeys, and before them we were reptiles,
birds, fishes, even worms. But that was ages ago, and we have grown up
and become better musicians. Evolution has chosen us as its favourites and
given us every advantage in the struggle up the ladder of life. Our musical
rivals of yesterday are as chorus people compared to Metropolitan Opera
stars, with us. On this earth we reign supreme, we have conquered the earth,
air, and water, annihilating time and distance. What more is there for us to
learn of Nature's secrets? Only an understanding of our lower brothers, the
animals.
III
ANIMALS AT PLAY
—Paradise Lost.
That "one touch of Nature makes the whole world kin" is shown in no
clearer way than by the games and play of animals. Recreation is as
common among them as it is among our own children; and they seem
always to be artistic and even skilled in their play. Young goats and lambs
skip, jump, run races, throw flips in the air, and gambol; calves have
interesting frolics; young colts and mules have biting and kicking games;
bears wrestle and tumble; puppies delight in biting and tussling; while
kittens chase everything from spools of thread to their own tails.
But animal children grow up, and stop playing to a certain extent as age
advances, precisely as human children do. Each settles down into a more
practical condition of life. They dislike to have their games and play
disturbed, and if the mother dog growls because her playful son has
continuously tumbled over her while she was sleeping, or the cat-mother
slaps her kitten because he plays with her tail—it is a display of the same
kind of emotion that prompts a human mother to rebuke her child in the
nursery for making too much noise, or for throwing toys out of the window.
Animals, like ourselves, feel every sensation of joy, happiness, surprise,
disappointment, love, hope, ambition, and through their youthful games an
entire index of their future lives may be obtained.
This play has much to do with the physical and mental development of the
animals; and it is strange indeed that so few writers have considered the
subject of play in the animal world. Most of those who have noticed the
subject at all, drop it with a few remarks, to the effect that it is "highly
amusing," or "very funny," or "unbelievable," or "so like the play of
children," without even a word of explanation of the whys and wherefores
of it.
All animals have some kinds of play. Plutarch speaks of a trained elephant
that often practised her steps when she thought no one was looking. No one
who has ever visited a zoological park and seen the crowded monkey and
baboon cages can have failed to note the wonderful play of these animals.
Seals seem never to tire of chasing one another through the water; while
even the clumsy hippopotamuses have diving games.
Kittens begin to tumble and play before they are two weeks old. They will
roll and toss a ball, hunting it from the dark corners, lay in silent wait for
each other, and suddenly spring upon an unsuspecting fellow-cat-baby's
back, just as they will do later in life, when seeking their prey. I have seen
them play with a catnip mouse for hours at a time, just as the mother cat
plays with a real mouse.
Brehm says that this is noticed in their earliest kittenhood, and that the
mother cat encourages it in all ways possible, even to becoming a child with
her children from love of them, as a human mother does in the nursery with
her child. The mother cat begins the play by slowly moving her tail. Gesner
considered her tail as the indicator of her moods. The kittens, while they
may not understand what this means, are greatly excited by the movement,
their eyes sparkle, their ears stand erect, and slowly one after another
clutches after the moving tail. Suddenly, one springs over the mother's back,
another grabs at her feet, while a third playfully slaps her in the face with
his tiny, soft, cushioned paw. She, patiently and mother-like, lovingly
submits to all this treatment, as it is only play.
DRYPTOSAURUS. THE PREHISTORIC ANIMALS, TOO,
UNDOUBTEDLY HAD THEIR PLAY TIME, WITH GAMES
AND "SETTING UP" EXERCISES.
American Museum of Natural History, New York
Many scientists have claimed that this so-called instinct should not be
classed as real play. However, such an authority as Darwin thought it was
play, and Scheitlin said that the cat let the mouse loose many times in order
that she might have the experience of catching it each time. No mercy is
shown the helpless mouse, which is the same to her as the toy ball—in the
same way as a real beetle and a toy beetle are the same to a small child.
Evidently the cat does not play with the mouse for the delight in torturing it,
but purely for practice that she may become skilled in the art of catching it.
The cat also exercises in springing movements, and by studying the mouse's
probable movements, learns to acquire a knowledge and skill in mouse-
ways otherwise impossible.
The same cruel practice is found among leopards, panthers, and wild cats.
Brehm verifies the observation that many members of the cat family
practise torturing their victims in a horrible manner, pretending to liberate
them, until the poor creatures at last die from their wounds. Lenz tells of a
marten that would play with its prey for hours when not hungry. Especially
was this true when marmots chanced to be his victims, and around these he
would leap and spring, dealing them terrific blows first with one paw and
then with the other. When hungry, however, he proceeded differently,
devouring them at once from teeth to tail.
All the cat family, it seems, are fond of human companionship, and take
almost as much delight in playing with human beings as with their own
kind. This is especially true of the puma. Brehm tells of a tame one that
delighted in hiding at the approach of his master and springing out
unexpectedly, just as the lion does. Hudson claimed that the puma, with the
exception of the monkey, was possibly the most playful of all animals.
Travellers tell many interesting tales of the play of these animals, especially
on the Pampas of South America.
Gross relates the experience of an Englishman who was compelled to spend
the night outdoors on the Pampas of the La Plata. At about nine o'clock, on
a bright moonlight night, he saw four pumas coming toward him, two adult
animals and two young ones. He well knew that these animals would not
attack him, so he quietly waited. In a short time they approached him,
chasing one another and playing hide-and-seek like little kittens; and finally
leaped directly over the man several times. The mother cat would run
ahead, calling to the little ones to follow her. But she never disturbed him.
At times an animal at play with another uses the same tactics and methods
employed on its prey. Of course, the value of such practice for the tasks of
later-life is evident. Dogs play hide-and-seek, tag, and various chasing
games for hours without resting. Among the negroes of the South it is not
uncommon to see a hound playing hide-and-seek with the little
pickaninnies. I have seen a hound peeping in and out among a pile of brush
to discover where the little ones were hiding, and at the first sight of a little
black face, he would lay low in anticipation of a playful spring, or a sudden
dash-away, with the expectation of being chased by his friends. At times he
would suddenly disappear toward his home, and slyly slip around and
approach the playground from an opposite direction.
Every one who has owned fox terriers knows how they will crouch in the
open grass and remain motionless, with quivering expectation for the other
playfellow to arrive, and when the one in ambush sees the other coming he
springs toward him, as though he were going to destroy him! And when the
two come together, they attempt to seize each other by the necks, as they
would do in a real conflict. A wrestle and tussle ensues and when utterly
exhausted from this play, the tired dogs, like two fatigued children, run to
their homes.
Dogs are fond of playing ball, and will readily bring a ball or stick to their
master when he has thrown it. They will also go into the water to bring out
sticks that may have been tossed in for amusement. Eugene Zimmerman
had a young fox terrier that would set a ball in motion, when there was no
one to pitch it for him, by seizing it in his mouth and tossing it up in the air.
Monkeys and jaguars will also play ball, and tame bears take great delight
in wrestling, playing ball, and fighting mock battles.
American Museum of Natural History, New York
It is well to remember that the pangolins and armadillos are the last
survivors of a great and ancient family of armour-bearers. Many of their
remote ancestors have been found in the rocks and hills of South America,
and all of their representatives of to-day are small animals—the last of a
doomed race—creatures of yesterday. The glyptodon is known to have been
more than eleven feet in length, and his near-kinsman, the chlamydothere,
was even larger. He was nearly the size of our present-day rhinoceros.
These extinct giants carried on their backs huge domes of bony plates, that
must have rivalled our much-feared tanks, of trench war fame. One would
think they were invulnerable, yet the glyptodon and the chlamydothere,
with many other equally well protected creatures, have long ago
disappeared from the earth, but how and why nobody knows. This total
disappearance of these marvellously protected giants, which seemed
capable of defending themselves against any and all kinds of enemies that
might have arisen, is one of the strangest and most unsolvable problems of
science.
Another mail-clad animal of importance is the armadillo of the tropical and
temperate regions of South America. He is nocturnal in habits, sleeping in
his underground home during the day, and coming out at night to seek for
food. This underground home is rather large, and the nursery is well
protected from enemies by its location. In it the mother armadillo rears her
young until they are large enough to care for themselves.
All species of the armadillos are powerful burrowers, and they are well
equipped for their tunnelling in the earth with strong fore limbs. They feed
upon all kinds of insects and animal substances. It is claimed that the giant
armadillo is a veritable grave-robber and sometimes digs up dead bodies for
the purpose of eating them.
These animals are plentiful upon the savannas of South America, and they
feast upon the bodies of dead cattle. So hard are their coats of armour that
the Gauchos sharpen their Spanish knives, which they always carry, upon
them. Should the armadillo be attacked by a man on horseback, he will
burrow so rapidly that only by the quickest movements of the man can he
be caught; and if he is, watch out for his terrible claws!
No animal is better protected by nature from its enemies than the
pichiciago, whose scientific name is chlamyphorus truncatus. This strange
little mantle-bearer wears a coat of mail which is as flexible as the human-
made coats of armour of olden times, and he is as safe under its cover,
which allows him perfect freedom, as if he were under the ground. He is
about the size of the ordinary mole, and his general habits are not unlike
those of the mole. He is an underground-dweller, with enormous fore-paws,
palm-shaped, upon which are five powerful claws. These he uses to great
advantage in digging in the earth for insects and for building his home. He
has a small snout, reminding one of that of a pig; while his piercing little
eyes are deeply hidden in his fur. He is a native of Chile, and because of his
shy nature and subterranean habits is rarely seen.
The most interesting feature about this little creature is the cuirass which so
perfectly protects his body. Its formation and arrangement is quite unusual;
it appears like a number of squared plates of horn, tightly united to short
strips of tape, which are sewed together. The cuirass is not connected with
the entire body of the animal, but only on the top of the head and along the
spine. It covers the entire back, and when it reaches the tail, turns
downward, forming a perfect flap, which protects the hindquarters.
The various species of manis are famed for their powerful coats of armour.
They, also, belong to the great group of burrowers, and their coats of mail
assume both offensive and defensive characters. These mail-bearers are
covered with numerous sharp-edged scales, like miniature horns, which
entirely overlap one another, like shingles on a house. They are of great
hardness, and form a belt which no animal of their regions can penetrate. A
revolver shot will produce not the slightest effect upon the body of this
iron-protected animal.
These animals are plentiful in India, and when they are molested, they
deliberately wind themselves up, coil their tails over their bodies, and
remain in conscious security against the fruitless blows of their enemies,
who soon weary of the wounds caused from the prickly scales of
impenetrable armour.
Instead of wearing heavy coats of mail, certain animals, such as the
hedgehog and porcupine, prefer to wear coats covered with needles and
pins. Of course, a coat of spines is used purely for protection. And against
the attacks of such enemies as dogs, it proves all-sufficient, but it is a well-
known fact that pumas and leopards will kill and eat porcupines at all times,
paying small attention to their spines, as is shown by the number which are
sometimes found sticking in the body of a porcupine-eating animal.
There are several species of this great spine-bearing family; and many of
them, especially the true porcupines and the echidnas, have burrows in the
ground and thus have a double means of protecting themselves. But others,
such as the hedgehog, depend for their protection upon their ability to roll
up into a ball, thus presenting a barbed wire protection. Still others live
largely in the trees and seek by other means to protect themselves.
One of the most interesting coats of armour is that worn by the porcupine
ant-eater—oft-times erroneously called porcupine or hedgehog. He is a
native of Australia, and is a powerful burrower. He is marvellously
protected by means of a coat of needles or spines which inflict painful
wounds on the dog or other enemy that ventures to attack him. In case of
danger, he curls himself up into a ball, and defies any one to come near. Not
only does he possess the coat of prickles with which he defends himself, but
he also has a large perforated claw or spur on each hind foot through which
pours an ill-smelling liquid, and these also aid in protecting him. There are
several varieties of porcupines which inhabit Asia, Africa, Southern Europe
and America.
When a porcupine wishes to attack an enemy, he rushes at it backwards, and
usually leaves the enemy literally covered, like a living pin-cushion, with
his spines. These animals have convex skulls, short tails, and live chiefly in
the warmer regions of the Old World. Those of America are different in one
particular—the soles of their feet are covered with hard, bone-like
tubercles, instead of being soft and smooth; there are also a number of hairs
that are intermingled with the spines. The Canada porcupine has more hairs
than the American, and a shorter and stumpier tail.
Another animal whose methods of defence are by means of his spines, is
the hedgehog. His spines do not terminate in sharp points, like those of the
porcupine, but end in tiny knobs. These are placed beneath the skin, and are
like pins stuck through a cushion. The hedgehog, like the porcupine, rolls
himself into a ball when attacked by enemies, and he has the additional
ability of throwing himself down a hillside, like a rolling ball, and thus
escaping his enemies without injury to himself. It would seem that the
hedgehog, rolled into a ball and covered with prickles, would be protected
from all enemies. But this is not true, for the clever fox knows just how to
make him unroll. This one secret of the hedgehog's weakness very often
causes his loss of life. His weakness is a terror of being wet or dropped into
water; and when the fox finds him all rolled up, he carefully rolls him into a
pond of water and, when he unrolls, quickly drowns him. Notwithstanding
the shortness of the hedgehog's spines, he is the most highly specialised of
all spine-bearing animals. In the lower order of animals there are spiny mice
and spiny rats, and even the horned toad uses his horns as a means of
protection against his enemies.
One of the most peculiarly armoured animals is the horned lizard,
commonly known as the "horned toad" of America. His body is covered
with small spiny scales, while the chisel-shaped head has a circlet of
miniature horns. These he uses when attacked by enemies to shield himself
against bites and knocks. The Indians claim that if a snake swallows the
horned lizard whole, the lizard will immediately work his way through the
snake. This would not be without a parallel, however, for it is generally
known that box-fishes, when swallowed by sharks, bite their way out!
Nature has been especially kind to horned lizards, and that is the reason
there are so many of them. They well know the secret of the Gyges ring,
and can put on the garment of invisibility in a very short time. They
especially frequent the desert regions of the South and West; and those that
dwell in black sandy regions are black; those of red clay regions are red;
those of grey regions, grey; those from the variously coloured regions of
blue and red are precisely the colour of the earth. But not satisfied with all
their protections of armour and camouflage, they actually, when hard-
pressed by an enemy, feign death, like an opossum! And if the enemy
persists in his attack, and Mr. Lizard cannot escape, as a final effort he
spurts tears of blood from his eyes. The Mexicans call him the "sacred
toad." The phenomenon of blood-shooting has been explained in various
ways, all of which seem equally unsatisfactory. So far it is one of Nature's
secrets. Perhaps some day we may understand it.
The tortoises are among the best examples of creatures which to-day protect
themselves with armour. They are, of course, reptiles, yet in the general
formation of their armour, they are strikingly like armadillos. The tortoise
has his armour so arranged over his body that it forms one big box. He
draws his head and limbs into this whenever danger is near. In Texas
recently I found a small land terrapin, and as soon as I came near, he closed
his house. I picked him up, and then carefully laid him upside down on the
ground, and stepped behind some nearby bushes to see what he would do.
Immediately he poked his head out, and then his feet, and then he began to
wave his feet wildly in air, and finally threw himself in the right position
and hastened away through the grass.
The turtle protects himself in the same way, and draws his head, feet, and
tail under his own house-roof where nothing can get him.
Lobsters and crabs are excellent types of armour-bearing animals. Lobsters
wear marvellous coats of mail, very similar to those worn by human
warriors during the age of chivalry. Their jointed structure assures them
perfect ease and security. Crabs, however, believe, as the tortoise, in the
strong-box protection. When resting, crabs tuck their legs beneath them, so
as to shelter themselves under the hard covering. Upon crabs Nature has
bestowed twin protective characteristics: namely, they are armoured, and
also mimic their surroundings. The latter protection is especially needful,
because certain big fishes, like the cod, are in the habit of swallowing crabs
whole. In this case the armour is of no use, while the protective
resemblance saves the crab.
To discuss in detail all the various kinds of armour and mail that the
different groups of animals have used and developed for offensive and
defensive purposes since the days of the prehistoric gigantic armadillos to
the present, would require a book of itself. It is sufficient to know that
armour and mail and spines are among Nature's most common forms of
protection, and that each age develops new and ever more efficient methods
of defence. This simply means that the age-long drama of evolution is
always changing. Everything that is came out of that which was, and
throughout the ages the ever-evolving organisms have been developing out
of the past, that they might ever be new.
V
MINERS AND EXCAVATORS
"When the cold winter comes and the water plants die,
And the little brooks yield no further supply,
Down in his burrow he cosily creeps,
And quietly through the long winter sleeps."
There is no danger that the bears will stifle for air under the snow, because
the warmth of their breath always keeps a small hole open at the top of the
snow-cell. This snow-house increases as time goes on, the heat exhaled
from their bodies gradually melting the snow. Often Mrs. Bear's home is
discovered by means of the tiny hole in the roof around which is collected
quantities of hoar frost.
Hibernation is one of the strangest phenomena of the animal world, and
bears, especially the white bear of the polar regions, the black bear of North
America, and the brown bear of Europe, agree in the curious habit of semi-
hibernation. In the late fall of the season, the bears begin to eat heavily and
soon become enormously fat, preparatory for the long winter of semi-sleep.
During the winter, at least for three months, the polar bear takes no food,
but lives entirely upon the store of fat which her body had accumulated
before she went into retirement. The same is true of many hibernating
animals, but in case of the bears it is more remarkable because the mother
bear must not only support herself but nourish her young for a long period
without taking any food for herself.
Another good example of a ground-dweller is the aard vark of Southern
Africa. He is as curious as his name, and scoops out immense quantities of
earth to form his home. This dwelling might be termed a cave, as he heaps
up the earth in the shape of a mammoth artificial ant-hill; on one side is the
entrance, which is so skilfully formed that it looks far more like the work of
man than of an animal.
His name is Dutch and means earth-hog. It is applied to him because his
head looks somewhat like that of a pig. His claws are powerful and
enormous, and with them he is able to dig into the hardest soil, and to
destroy the giant ant-hills which are dotted over the plains of South Africa,
and which can withstand the weight of a dozen men.
This strange creature sleeps during the day, and comes forth at evening to
seek his food. The first thing he does is to burst a hole in the stony side of
an ant-hill, to the utter dismay of its tiny inhabitants. As they run among the
ruins of their fallen city, he throws out his slimy tongue and catches them
by the hundreds. In a short time only the shell of a half-destroyed wall
remains.
These once stately ant-homes metamorphosed into caves, form homes for
the jackals and large serpents of the plains. The Kaffirs of Africa use them
as vaults into which are thrown their dead. The aard vark outrivals, with his
great claws, the most skilled burrowing tools of man. These animals are
therefore rarely captured. It is not uncommon for a horse to fall into their
excavations and be killed.
Miners, excavators, and underground dwellers teach us the great lesson that,
while many of them sought the ground as a protection, and found there
many difficulties to overcome, they not only have won in the great struggle
of life but have so skilfully adapted themselves to their environment and
surroundings as to become entire masters, even artists, in their methods of
living.
VI
ANIMAL MATHEMATICIANS
—Prior.
Among the special senses of animals none seems more human than their
knowledge of mathematics. A recognition of this quality in animals is
encouraging because the new scientists are earnestly trying to build up a
true knowledge of animal behaviour by studying them in the light of the
new psychology. This will fill the place of the vast amount of
misinformation which those skilled only in book-knowledge, without really
knowing the ways of Nature, have builded. It will also record all the strange
and curious facts about animals and their ways without insisting too much
on rigid explanation. These new scientists are far different from their
predecessors who tried to explain everything they did not understand about
an animal's behaviour in terms of the scanty information gained by studying
a few museum specimens. We might as well attempt to explain human
nature from the study of an Egyptian mummy. The new method is simply to
give the facts about an animal, and frankly admit that in many cases, such
as are found in their knowledge of counting and numbers, we must leave
complete explanation to the future when we shall have a greater fund of
scientific data on which to base our conclusions.
It is an established fact that some animals can count, and that they have the
faculty of close observation and keen discrimination. They learn to count
quickly, but they do not fully appreciate the value of numerical rotation.
Most of the arithmetical feats of trained animals are hoaxes regulated by
their sense of smell, sight, touch and taste. But no one doubts their ability to
count. I have known a monkey that could count to five. He played with a
number of marbles, and I would ask for two marbles, one marble, four
marbles, as the case might be, and he would quickly hand the number
requested.
Another incident that will illustrate the point is the case of a mule owned by
an old negro near Huntsville, Texas. The regular routine work of this mule
was to cart two loads of wood to the town every day. One day the negro
wished to make a third trip, but was unable to do so. When asked the
reason, he replied, "Dat fool mule, Napoleon, done decided we had hauled
enough wood fo' one day!"
Prantl claims that the time-sense is totally absent in animals, and that it
belongs only to man, as one of the attributes of his mental superiority.
However, many facts go to show that animals have not only a specific time-
sense, but also a sense of personal identity which reaches back into the past.
Time-sense is very highly developed in dogs, cats, hogs, horses, goats, and
sheep. They apparently are able to keep an accurate account of the days of
the week and hours of the day and night, and even seem to know something
of numerical succession and logical sequence. A friend in Texas had an old
coloured servant, whose faithful dog had been trained to know that just at
noon each day he was expected to carry lunch to his master. I have seen the
dog on more than one occasion playing with children in the streets,
suddenly break away without any one calling him, or any suggestion on our
part as to the time, and rush for the kitchen just at the proper moment. No
one could detain him from his duty. This same dog, however, would on
Sundays continue to play at the noon hour. Surely, if any explanation is to
be offered in such a case as this, it will imply as strict a sense of time as it
does of duty.
A friend relates a case of a dog that went each evening to meet a train on
which his master returned from the city. On one occasion the train was
delayed two hours, and it was exceedingly cold, but the devoted companion
remained until his master arrived. Innumerable instances of such all-
absorbing affection, showing at the same time a sense of time, might be
cited.
Dr. Brown gives a most remarkable example of a dog's ability to distinguish
time. The story is of a female dog, though named Wylie, which was
purchased by Dr. Brown when he was a young man, from an old shepherd
who had long been in his employment. Wylie was brought to his father's,
"and was at once taken," he says, "to all our hearts; and though she was
often pensive, as if thinking of her master and her work on the hills, she
made herself at home, and behaved in all respects like a lady.... Some
months after we got her, there was a mystery about her; every Tuesday
evening she disappeared; we tried to watch her, but in vain; she was always
off by nine P. M., and was away all night, coming back next day wearied,
and all over mud, as if she had travelled far. This went on for some months,
and we could make nothing of it. Well, one day I was walking across the
Grass-market, with Wylie at my heels, when two shepherds started, and
looking at her, one said, 'That's her; that's the wonderful wise bitch that
naebody kens.' I asked him what he meant, and he told me that for months
past she had made her appearance by the first daylight at the 'buchts' or
sheep-pens in the cattle-market, and worked incessantly, and to excellent
purpose, in helping the shepherds to get their sheep and lambs in. The man
said in a sort of transport, 'She's a perfect meeracle; flees about like a
speerit, and never gangs wrang; wears, but never grups, and beats a' oor
dowgs. She's a perfect meeracle, and as soople as a mawkin'.' She continued
this work until she died."
Another most striking instance, showing animals' sense of time, is that
related by Watson in which he tells of two friends, fathers of families, one
living in London and the other at Guilford. For many years it was the
custom of the London family to visit their friends in Guilford, always
accompanied by their spaniel, Cæsar. After some years a misunderstanding
arose between the two families. The usual Christmas visits were
discontinued; not, however, so far as the spaniel was concerned. His visits
continued as before. On the eve of the first Christmas following the
misunderstanding, the Guilford family were astonished to find at their door
their London friend, Cæsar. Naturally, they expected that he had come in
advance of the family, and were happy in the thought of this unexpected
reconciliation. All evening they awaited their friends, but none arrived. Nor
did they the next day. Cæsar had come of his own accord at the accustomed
time, and remained with his friends for the usual number of days. This
naturally led to a correspondence between the families, who thereupon
resumed their former friendly relations. We do not believe, of course, that
this dog counted the exact number of days to know when to start to
Guilford, but he doubtless saw something to remind him of the past.
Sir John Lubbock once related before the British Association at Aberdeen
how cards bearing the ten numerals were arranged before a dog, and the
dog given a problem, such as to state the square root of nine, or of sixteen,
or the sum of two numbers. He would then point at each card in succession,
and the dog would bark when he came to the right one. The dog never made
a mistake. If this was not evidence of a mentality at least approaching that
of men, we do not know what to call it.
If there is any difference between an animal and a human mathematician, it
depends upon special training. The animal never has the same opportunities
to learn as the man. Many savages, for example, cannot count beyond three
or four. Sir John Lubbock gives an anecdote of Mr. Galton, who compared
the arithmetical knowledge of certain savages of South Africa and a dog.
The comparison proved to the advantage of the dog.
There is no reason that a dog should not be taught arithmetic. And if one
wishes to do so, it might be well to begin by making the dog distinguish one
from two, allowing him to touch both once at the word one, and twice at the
word two. Then he might pass on to six or seven. After he had progressed to
ten, he might begin addition. At least the experiment would be interesting
and conducive to learning the truth. Surely a knowledge of mathematics is
no more wonderful than that of the ordinary pointer dog's ability to
distinguish different kinds of birds. Certain of those wise dogs are trained to
hunt only quail, while others hunt several varieties of game.
It should be remembered that all degrees of arithmetical aptitude are found
in the human races, from the genius of a Newton and a Laplace to the
absolute inability of certain of the Hottentots to count to three. These
inequalities in the mathematical notions of different people should make us
very cautious about saying that animals cannot count and have no sense of
numbers. It is extremely probable that if we had a way of choosing those
animals with a special gift for arithmetic, they would surprise us with their
learning.
THE COYOTE CAN READILY DISTINGUISH WHETHER A
HERD OF SHEEP IS GUARDED BY ONE OR MORE DOGS,
AND WILL PLAN HIS ATTACK ACCORDINGLY.
THE ZEBU, THE SACKED BULL OF INDIA, IN SPITE OF ITS
DOMESTICATION, HAS AN AGILE BODY AND A QUICK,
ALERT MIND.
No one denies that animals are capable of distinguishing relative sizes and
even quantities. They are not so skilled as the average human being in
making these distinctions, yet when mentally compared to the state of
Bushmen, Tasmanians, and Veddahs, who can count only two, and call it
many, there is not such a vast gulf between them and mankind.
The zebu, or sacred bull of India, shows his mathematical qualities to a
pronounced degree. When he grows attached to a small group of his kin, he
will often refuse to leave them unless the entire group accompany him.
When driven from his pen, if by chance one of his party is left behind he
refuses to go—thus indicating that he is able to tell that the exact number is
not with him. His affectionate and gentle disposition, not to mention his
love of his offspring, would entitle him to rank among the most human of
animals. No wonder he is worshipped in India, where the human side of
animal life is understood and appreciated to a degree quite unknown to the
Western world!
The fox and the wolf, and even the coyote, can readily distinguish whether
a herd of sheep or cattle is guarded by three or four dogs, and whether there
is one herdsman or two. They cannot tell the exact number of sheep,
however; neither could a man without first counting them. Their knowledge
of geometry is remarkable. They can orient themselves to the surrounding
woods, measure distances, figure out the safest way of escape, and the
power of the enemy even better than savage man. Yet in most of these
problems, definite notions of number or figures have little part. A dog,
when hunting, for example, on a prairie where he has to leap over ditches or
quickly turn around a large tree, is able by a second's thought to do so
without danger. He clears the wire fence, leaps the ditch, dashes through a
closing gate, or escapes an infuriated enemy at a moment's notice. This
natural wisdom is exercised spontaneously in him, it is the result of inborn
theorems of which he may not even be aware, but which he uses with a
sureness that defies the book-learning of all our teachers of mathematics.
He uses speed, force, space, mass, and time with so small an effort, and by
the quickest and shortest routes.
Suppose a wolf or a wild hog could not tell how many dogs were attacking
it? There would be no way for it to defend itself. If four dogs attack it, they
are counted and the tactics used that would be useless in other cases. If four
dogs attack, two on each side, it retreats, with face toward the enemy. If a
dozen dogs are in the attacking force, the hog becomes confused, loses all
idea of number, and wildly bites at any enemy that comes nearest. Man in a
similar condition would use practically the same tactics.
Cats undeniably count their kittens. If the mother loses one of three or four,
she searches for it immediately. When dogs are chasing a hare, if they raise
another, they become very confused, as if they did not know which to
follow. Many shepherd dogs know if a sheep is missing from the flock and
go to hunt it.
The efforts of scientific investigators, who work with so many learned
theories, have been less successful in discovering the real facts about
animals than of laymen, largely because the scientists have not yet learned
that arithmetical notions are more difficult than geometrical ones. Our
industrial civilisation has caused us to lose the idea of the insignificance
that number has in animal life compared to the idea of size. Most animals
have a remarkable sense of size; they measure time and distance better than
civilised man. A hyena, for example, knows just how near he dare approach
an unarmed man.
A sense of time is common among animals that daily eat at fixed hours. A
donkey was accustomed to being fed at six o'clock in the morning, and
when on one occasion his master did not appear on time, he deliberately
kicked in the door to the barn and proceeded to feed himself.
Animals are capable of measuring lapses of time in which they are
particularly interested. Houzeau claims that a female crocodile remains
away from her eggs in the sand for twelve to twenty days, according to the
species, but returns to the place exactly on the day they hatch.
Although we should hesitate to affirm that all animals have an extensive
knowledge of figures and numbers, yet it can hardly be denied that the
elephant, donkey, horse, dog, and cat, if given the proper training, become
good mathematicians. It is undeniable that they have a love of mental
acquisition, and it seems that the Creator has given to every animal, as a
reward for its limitations in other respects, a definite innate knowledge and
desire to advance educationally. There is in the breast of every animal an
irresistible impulse which urges it to advance in the scale of knowledge.
Where the animal is blessed with other mental powers, there is found a
perfect harmony—of tact, intuition, insight, and genius—all that man
himself possesses.
VII
THE LANGUAGE OF ANIMALS
Near our country home in Texas my sister found a very young red deer one
morning just outside the garden, and bringing it into the yard, soon had a
wonderful pet in this dainty spotted child of the woods. We knew that its
mother was not far away, and so we placed salt and food just where the
baby was found, to attract the mother's attention. In a few days, we saw the
mother, and shortly afterwards five grown deer were seen eating the food
we had placed for the mother. Evidently the news had been carried through
the pine forests that it was safe for deer to come near our home. My sister's
pet grew rapidly, and became a great friend of our yard dog. They often
played by running races together, the deer would leap over the fence and the
dog would chase him with great delight. Surely, they must have had a
spoken common language!
No one claims that in the language of animals there are principles of
construction such as we find in the human languages. The term Barbarian
means those whose language is only a "bar-bar," and this is really all that
the sound of an unknown tongue implied to the cultured Athenians. The
neighing of horses, the howling of dogs and wolves, the mewing of cats, the
bleating of sheep, the lowing of cows, the chattering of monkeys and
baboons is nothing more nor less than their language. And it is quite as
intelligible to us as is the chattering of the Hottentots of Africa. Because we
do not speak the languages of our animal friends does not take away from
the genuineness of the languages; we might as well claim that because our
horse does not comprehend what we are saying, that we are not speaking a
language!
Animals and men, under normal conditions, have been friends and
companions since the beginning of time; and in order that they may convey
ideas to each other, it is necessary for them to have some sort of means of
communication.
As a matter of fact, animal language is quite often intelligible to man. Their
language might be likened to that of a young child that cannot pronounce
distinctly the words we commonly use; and yet we get the meaning from
the intonation and gesture.
Any man who has ever owned a horse understands the meanings of his
various actions and vocal expressions. There is the neigh of joy, upon
returning home after a hard day's work, the neigh of distress, when he has
strayed from his companions, the neigh of salutation that passes between
two horses when they meet, and the neigh of terror when enemies are near.
There is also the neigh of affection that is often given to his master when
they first meet in the morning. Thus, spoken words are not necessary to
express elemental feelings.
Elephants readily understand most of the words uttered by their masters.
Menault tells of an elephant that was employed to pile up heavy logs. The
manager, suspecting the keeper of stealing the grain set aside for the
elephant, accused him of theft, which he denied most vehemently in the
presence of the elephant. The result was remarkable. The animal suddenly
laid hold of a large wrapper which the man wore round his waist, and
tearing it open, let out some quarts of rice which the fellow had stowed
away under the voluminous covering.
Animals have the power to make themselves understood by man, especially
when they are in distress and wish man to help them. And they often
combine to help one another. I was on a sheep ranch in western Texas once
when one of the sheep came bleating up to the camp late in the afternoon.
She uttered the most distressing calls. A friend, whom I was visiting,
assured me that something unusual was wrong. Together we followed the
sheep back to where she had been feeding in the pasture, she going forward
in short spurts and continually looking back to see if we were coming. She
finally led us to an old well, and we heard the plaintive voice of her young
lamb that had fallen in. As the well had no water in it, and was only about
six feet deep, we secured a ladder and in a few minutes the lamb was
restored to its mother. She seemed delighted at the successful outcome of
the accident. She had come and told us her troubles and got aid.
Cats are gifted linguists. By mewing they can just as plainly express a
desire to have a door opened or closed as if they requested it in so many
words. A friend has furnished me with an interesting account of her cat's
ability to make herself understood. It seems that the cat, with her three
small kittens, at one time slept in a box prepared for her in the kitchen. But
one night when it was particularly cold, some one left the kitchen window
open, and late in the night the cat went to her mistress's bed and mewed
continuously until her mistress arose and went to the kitchen and closed the
window. The cat was perfectly satisfied, as she had made her great need
understood.
The ability that animals have to make their own language understood by
man is not the only linguistic power they possess; as already mentioned,
they are also capable of understanding something of human speech. There
is no doubt that all domesticated animals understand the human language;
the horse, dog, ox, and sheep comprehend a large part of what is said to
them, though of course they may not understand the precise words used.
I once owned a rabbit dog, "Nimrod," and if he never understood another
word of the English language, there is no doubt that he knew what the word
"rabbit" meant. No matter in what manner or way I used the word, Nimrod
was ready for a hunt, and yelped with glee at the thought of the chase that
he was to have. I tested him over and over again by saying "rabbit hunt"
gently; it thrilled him with delight, and while he was not very well educated
in other things, he always lived up to his name.
The Rev. J. G. Wood speaks of the great individuality of character which he
has observed in dogs, and that they unquestionably understand the human
language. "There was in my pet greyhound 'Brenda,' there was in my dear
lurcher 'Smoker,' and there is now in my dear lurcher 'Bar,' and in my three
setters 'Chance,' 'Quail,' and 'Quince,' a refinement of feeling and sagacity
infinitely beyond that existing in multitudes of the human race, whether
inhabiting the deserts or the realms of civilisation.
"I cannot better define it than by saying that, if I give these dogs a hastily
angered word in my room, though they have never been beaten, they will,
with an expression of the most dejected sorrow, go into a corner behind
some chair, sofa, or table, and lie there. Perhaps I may have been guilty of a
hasty rebuke to them for jogging my table or elbow while I was writing, and
then continued to write on. Some time after, not having seen my
companions lying on the rug before the fire, I have remembered the
circumstance, and, in a tone of voice to which they are used, I have said,
'There, you are forgiven.' In an instant the greyhound Brenda would fly into
my lap, and cover me with kisses, her heart tumultuously beating. After she
grew old, her joy at my return home after a long absence has at times nearly
killed her; and when I was away, the bed she loved best was one of my old
shooting-jackets, but never when I was at home."
The impassable gulf which the writers of old created between mankind and
the animal kingdom was based mainly upon the belief that animals had no
language, but this has been proved a mistake and no longer exists. In the
light of modern knowledge and a better understanding of the marvellous
theory of evolution, we are thoroughly convinced that there is no break
whatever in the long chain of living beings. Man has no art, has developed
no thing whatever, no mode of language or communication, that is not to be
found in some degree among animals. They are capable of feeling the same
emotions as human beings, and are therefore subject to the same general
laws of life. No science has been more beneficial than psychology in
proving that they are human in all ways; no discovery made by the human
mind is so poetical and of such value as that which leads mankind to
recognise some part of himself in every part of Nature, even in the language
of animals.
This knowledge of all life is recognised by thinking men the world over,
removing forever that artificial barrier by which, in his ignorance and
prejudice, he has separated himself from his lower brothers, the animals,
denying unto them even a means of intelligent communication. This
recognition of the existence of a common language will go far toward
establishing the universal brotherhood of all living creatures.
VIII
IN THEIR BOUDOIRS, HOSPITALS AND CHURCHES
Animals know better than man what kind of food they need, for the simple
reason that their tastes are natural, while man has allowed his to become
perverted. In times of sickness absurd practices have been observed. Ice-
cream and buttermilk, for example, were for ages refused to typhoid fever
patients, while to-day they are generally used under such circumstances.
But the natural desire for sour and cold things was always in evidence;
animals have always depended upon these desires.
Among them are skilled dietitians, who restrict their diet in case of illness,
keep quiet, avoid all excitement, seek restful places where there is plenty of
fresh air and clean water. If a dog loses his appetite, he eats "dog grass,"
while a sick cat delights in catnip. Deer, goats, cows, and sheep, when sick
seek various medicinal herbs. When deer or cattle have rheumatism, they
invariably seek a health resort where they may bathe in a sulphur spring and
drink of the healing mineral waters. They also know the full value of lying
in the warm sun.
Cats are skilled physicians, and have various home remedies, such as
dipping a feverish foot into cold water, or lying before a warm fire, if they
have a cold. Many animals know how to treat a sore eye—by lying in the
dark, and repeatedly licking their paws and placing them over the afflicted
member.
How wonderful would the human race become, if it had the strength of a
lion, the power of a bear, the wisdom of an elephant, the cleverness of a fox,
and the health of the wild boar! But these qualities are found chiefly among
the animals because of the marvellous knowledge of the laws of health and
self-preservation.
John Wesley claimed, in his directions on the art of keeping well, that many
of the medicines which were used among the common people of his time
were first discovered by watching animals in their medical practices to cure
their ills and pains. "If they heal animals, they will also heal men," he
claimed. The American Indians learned most of their cures from watching
animals, especially the cure of such diseases as fever, rheumatism,
dysentery, and snake-bites. A rheumatic old wolf would bathe in the warm
waters of a sulphur spring; a sick and feverish deer would eat the fresh
leaves of healing ferns, while a wounded hog or bear would always seek a
red-clay bath to heal the wounds. Sick dogs will invariably eat certain
weeds, and an unwell cat will seek healing mints and grasses.
Old hunters tell us that a deer after having been chased for several hours by
dogs, and after having escaped them by swimming a cold stream, will, upon
reaching safety, lie down in the ice and snow. If a man did such a thing, he
would immediately die. But not so with the deer, for he will arise about
every hour and move around to exercise himself, and on the morrow he is
perfectly well. The same animal, shut up in a warm barn for the night, as
has many times been demonstrated with circus animals, will be dead by
morning.
From this natural method of healing, mankind may learn much, and
especially as it pertains to the treatment of extreme heat, cold, exhaustion,
and paralysis of the muscles, and most especially sores and wounds. I have
seen a wounded hog that had been badly bitten by a dog, wallow in rich red
mud to stop the flow of blood.
It is a common practice for a raccoon actually to amputate a diseased leg, or
one that has been wounded by a gunshot, and wash the stub in cool flowing
water. When it is healing, he licks it with his tongue to massage it, and also
to stop the pain and reduce the swelling. This wisdom is often classed by
the unknowing under the term instinct, whereas it displays no less skill and
knowledge than that of our modern surgery. The intelligence of the raccoon
stands very high in the animal world.
Foxes, when caught in a trap, will very often gnaw off a limb. This requires
a special power and a moral energy that few men possess.
William J. Long, in the Outlook, tells of an unusual proof of animal surgery
in the case of an old muskrat that had cut off both of his forelegs, probably
at different times, and had grown very wise in avoiding man-made traps,
and when found, had covered the wound with a sticky vegetable gum from
a pine tree. "An old Indian who lives and hunts on Vancouver Island told
me recently," said Mr. Long, "that he had several times caught beaver that
had previously cut their legs off to escape from traps, and that two of them
had covered the wounds thickly with gum, as the muskrat had done. Last
spring the same Indian caught a bear in a deadfall. On the animal's side was
a long rip from some other bear's claw, and the wound had been smeared
thickly with soft spruce resin. This last experience corresponds closely with
one of my own. I shot a bear years ago in northern New Brunswick that had
received a gunshot wound, which had raked him badly and then penetrated
the leg. He had plugged the wound carefully with clay, evidently to stop the
bleeding, and then had covered the broken skin with sticky mud from the
river's brink, to keep the flies away from the wound and give it a chance to
heal undisturbed. It is noteworthy here that the bear uses either gum or clay
indifferently, while the beaver and muskrat seem to know enough to avoid
the clay, which would be quickly washed off in the water."
Animals not only know how to doctor themselves when they are sick, but
some of them, such as the fox, have learned how to make artificial heat by
covering green leaves with dirt. And while they do not make fire, their
homes are often heated in this practical way, and thus sickness avoided.
Domestic horses and dogs wear hats in summer, and possibly in the future
they will learn the enormous importance of wearing clothes! Trained
monkeys already take great delight in dressing up, and dogs like smart suits.
Monkeys show the greatest interest and brotherly love when one of their
number is injured. Watson tells of a female monkey that was shot and
carried into a tent. Several of her tribe advanced with frightful gestures, and
only stopped when met with a gun. The chief of the tribe then came
forward, chattering and remonstrating vigorously. But as he came nearer,
there was every evidence of grief and supplication for the body. As he was
given the body, he affectionately took it in his arms and slowly moved to
his companions, and like a silent funeral procession they all walked away.
Nor does their interest cease with life, for we are told by no less authority
than Col. Theodore Roosevelt of a large grizzly bear that was discovered
lying across the trail in the woods. The hunter shot her as she was preparing
to charge him, and later he examined the spot where she was lying, and
found that it was the newly made grave of her cub. Evidently some animal
had killed the cub in her absence, and she, in her grief, was determined to
avenge the wrong by lying in wait for the enemy.
Public meetings for civic council and religious worship are not confined to
man alone. In Macgrave's History of Brazil we are told of a species of South
American monkey known as the ouraines, which the natives call preachers
of the woods. These highly intelligent creatures assemble every morning
and evening, when the leader takes a place apart from the rest and addresses
them from his pulpit or platform, Having taken his position, he signals to
the others to be seated, after which he speaks to them in a language loud
and rapid, with the gestures of a Billy Sunday, the audience listening in
profound silence. He then signals again with his paws, when all cry out
together in apparently confused noises, until another signal for silence
comes from their leader. Then follows another discourse, at the close of
which the assembly disperses. Macgrave attempts no explanation as to the
object of these addresses; but if his accounts be true, surely they must have
as much meaning for the monkeys as many of our public lectures and
church services have for us! No doubt much of the advice imparted
concerns the personal and collective welfare of the tribe members.
IX
SELF-DEFENCE AND HOME-GOVERNMENT
A most unique fighter is the giraffe. He has neither claws nor sharp teeth
with which to defend himself; so, if he gets angry with one of his kind, he
deliberately uses his long neck like a pile driver would use a sledge
hammer. Swinging it round and round, he lets his head descend upon his
adversary like a heavy ax! The two animals use the same kind of tactics,
and bracing themselves so as to stand the blows, they fight until one has to
give in. Their heads are furnished with two small knob-like horns which
only protect them from the heavy blows without serving as offensive
weapons.
Most singular and amusing of all methods of self-defence are those which
entirely depend for their efficiency upon bluff, or pretence. The chameleon,
for example, erects his snake-like hood, though he is harmless, and at the
most could scarcely injure the smallest animal. Equally curious are the
methods of skunks and polecats, which project against enemies a highly
disagreeable fluid.
Passive modes of defence are as many and varied as are the active; one of
the strangest and most inexplicable of these is that known as spontaneous
amputation, technically termed autotomy. The lizard, for example, when
captured, will abruptly break loose his tail in order to escape; and certain
wood rats, when caught, loosen the skin on their tails and deliberately slip
away. Autotomy not only permits flight, but also defends the animal against
the most adverse conditions. Nearest akin to this—defence by means of
amputation—is the practice of bears and raccoons of amputating their limbs
when caught in steel traps.
Mimicry, which is treated under another chapter, comes under the head of
passive defence, and form and colour play an important part in it. Strangely
enough, animals which have never resorted to mimicry as a means of
protection, when associated with others who practice it, take on the habit
themselves. This may possibly be due to the fact that new enemies are
constantly arising.
As human sharpshooters dress in garments of the same colour as the woods
in which they hunt, so many animals use this principle of imitation. The
colour of most animals is very similar to their surroundings. This enables
them to lie in wait for prey, a practice as old as the hillsides with animals.
They have learned the extreme value of silence, and that they must remain
at times motionless. This is especially noticeable with crocodiles, which
wait for whole days without moving, concealed in the water or deep grass,
until their prey comes within striking distance, when they pounce upon it.
The same is true of the python snake, which hangs from a tree so
immovable that he appears like a vine or a branch of the tree. If an animal
attempts to pass, he drops upon it.
Perhaps the most unique and successful method of passive defence is the
feigning of death, or "playing 'possum" met with in several animals, such as
the red fox, the opossum, occasionally the elephant, and several of the
snakes. On many occasions I have been 'possum hunting in the South and
found my dog barking at an apparently dead 'possum. As soon as these
animals are approached by larger and stronger enemies, they drop
absolutely motionless on the ground and close their eyes as though they
were dead. Here they remain until the enemy either destroys them, carries
them away, or leaves them alone. If left alone for a few moments, they
immediately spring to their feet and make their escape.
Elephants often feign death when captured, in order to gain their liberty.
Animal catchers tell many interesting tales of elephants feigning weakness
from which they fall to the earth and later apparently die. In many instances
the fastenings are removed from their legs and head and the carcass is
abandoned as useless, when to the utter astonishment of all—before the
captors get out of sight—the animal springs up and dashes away to the
forest, screaming with joy at the triumph of its deception.
Many animals deliberately assume a frightful, terrifying or grotesque
appearance. This they do by inflating their bodies, by erecting hair, skin, or
folds, or by unusual poses. Darwin speaks of the hissing of certain snakes,
the rattle of the rattle-snake, the grating of the scales of the echis, each of
which serves to frighten or terrify the enemy.
Bluffing is another form of defence that many animals use. The cobra, for
example, when disturbed, raises its immense hood in a most terrifying
attitude! Many of the lizards use the same tactics; while the horned toads of
America when disturbed actually eject blood from their eyes. Every one is
familiar with the cat's habit of raising the fur on his back when molested by
a dog. All bluffing animals, when in danger, try to assume a pose that will
make them look most dangerous and impressive to their enemies, and there
is little doubt that in most cases they succeed very well, for we have all seen
a dog slink away from a menacing cat.
The elk or moose, whose home is in the northern part of America and
Europe, is a powerful and large animal, sometimes seven feet in height, and
is able to endure much cold. He has many enemies among animals and
mankind, and during the summer season he is quite able to protect himself,
but in winter there is considerable danger from hordes of wolves. This is
especially true just after a heavy snowstorm, if the snow is wet and melting.
When it is dry and frozen, he can travel over it with great speed, and this he
does by a most unusual trot which carries him along much faster than the
trotting gait of a horse. Thus he is able to escape the hungry, carnivorous
wolves, whose courage increases with appetite. If crowded too close, he is
able also to protect himself by the most terrific blows of his fore-feet.
But when the spring weather sets in, and the snows begin to melt
underneath, leaving the upper crust sufficiently strong to support the weight
of lighter and smaller animals, such as wolves, especially when they travel
swiftly, he is in great danger. For with every step he sinks to the belly in the
snow, while his enemies can walk right up to his head and shoulders
without his being able to strike or paw them with his dangerous hoofs. The
advantage seems to be with the wolves, and if ever they bring the moose to
bay in the snow, his life is doomed. For they care little for his arrow-like
horns, but boldly jump at his throat and kill him. Herein comes the elk's
wisdom—he deliberately sets to work, before the snow melts, and builds for
himself and family an elk-yard, which is nothing more than a large space of
ground on which the snow is smoothed or trampled down until it becomes a
hard surface on which he can walk; it is also surrounded by a high wall of
snow, through which are certain exits that allow him to pass out, if he
desires. All the enclosed space is not smoothed down, but parts of it only
are cut up into roads through which he may pass very swiftly. Woe unto the
daring wolves that enter his snowy fortification—his "No Man's Land"—-
for sure death awaits them!
A sense of law, order, government; the sacredness of family ties—all these
aid in the protection of animals. Family life with them originated just as it
did in the human world. The social instinct and the moral sentiments which
arise from social relations in man and animal are the same. Moral
obligations, especially in relation to family ties and conjugal unions of
animals, are in many cases sacred binders to such ties. The bear, for
example, is proverbial for his conjugal faithfulness. The married life of
most animals is strictly moral, and most of them are monogamists and have
reached the highest form of family association and life.
In those places where they live promiscuously, it gives them the same
protection in herds as it does among our lower savages. Cattle, sheep, and
horses unite for mutual protection; wolves band together in packs; and after
they have been domesticated there is still not only a strong desire to band
together for social purposes, but also to hold courts of justice. It sometimes
happens that an angered husband takes the law in his hands, like uncivilised
men, and beats his wife.
In the development and organisation of social and civil life the horse and
the goat hold the foremost position. It corresponds to that of man among the
lower animals. They do not believe in monarchies, but strictly in republics,
or rather, a democracy where all power comes from the working class. The
claims of the working class to the exercise of supreme control in all
political affairs are practically realised. Among a herd of wild Arabian
horses, the leading stallion, or so-called king, is really only the father of the
tribe; his functions are paternal rather than regal. If he may be said to reign
in a certain sense, the true workers rule, and his scouts and sentinels obey
his wishes which the workers have influenced and formulated.
The existence of but one king leaves no room for dynastic troubles and
rivalries which disturb, so often, our human countries and empires with
such dreadful results. If two rival kings arise at the same time in a herd of
horses, instead of forming factions in the state which end in civil war, they
fight it out personally until one of them is killed or defeated. Once in a great
while the other horses intervene, and drive the less desirable, or the false-
claimant of power, away from the herd and its grazing territory. In these
troubles the real king has little or no power, all activities are carried on by
the workers.
If by chance he dies or is captured, another king, chosen by the herd,
immediately assumes the kingship. It is a well-known fact that if the king of
a herd of wild horses is caught, it is not uncommon for his herd to remain as
near him as possible, and in their attempt to release him are often trapped
themselves. The king has no heirs, either apparent or presumptive, and no
right of succession is recognised. Any member of the herd, provided the
workers choose him, may become the king, as every American school boy
is a possible president of the United States.
Among many animals there is a perfect social and industrial organisation in
which the division of labour is far better adjusted than in many human
organisations. This, of course, is the result of gradual growth and evolution
just as it is in the human species. This can easily be proved among animals
by their more primitive and savage habits. Monkeys, for example, in
civilised monkey communities, differ very greatly from those of wilder and
less trained districts. They are constantly changing their habits, becoming
more and more civilised by improving their methods of work and their
moral and religious life as well. In many cases they have ceased to kill
members of their own tribe for small offences for which they used to kill,
and the cleanness and beauty of their home lives seem to increase with the
years.
It oftentimes happens, however, that powerful ape and baboon colonies
relapse into barbarism, and roam, plunder, rob and murder, like a pack of
uncivilised wolves or hyenas. They seem all at once to forget their peaceful
industries and lose all desire for clean and right living. And strangely
enough, when they once turn bad, they seldom reform. Some naturalists
believe that they are led astray by a wicked king or ruler who comes into
power; the natives believe the evil spirits have suddenly taken possession of
them.
There is unquestionably, in the life of many tribal animals, a definite
historical connection between the mother tribe and its colonies. This
relation extends to the tribes of tribes, and thus there is an international
relationship between the various members of a large number of tribes.
These communities share the same likes, dislikes, hatreds, and aspirations.
A missionary friend told of his experience with monkey folk, and how
once, when hunting, his gun was accidentally discharged, instantly
wounding a large semi-tame baboon near his home. He hastened to help the
injured animal, but saw that the relatives had crowded around and were
terrorised, as they thought it was intentional. They not only followed him to
his home, but returned in the night and actually tore his fence down. For
months he was afraid to leave his wife alone during the day. And the natives
reported that large tribes of monkey folk immediately came into the
community from remoter regions and were distinctly on the war path. It was
evident that their unjust antipathy was extended to all the kinspeople.
This is evidence of hereditary enmity, such as is common among families,
tribes, and clans, and it often takes the form of feuds, which are still in
vogue in the mountainous counties of the South. The baboons had suffered
wrongs and never forgot it, and it was transmitted to their offspring.
The ability to use weapons, tools, and war instruments is not exclusively
human. Even fish are capable of reaching their prey at a long distance. The
toxotes jaculator, which lives in the rivers of India, and feeds upon insects,
cannot afford to wait until the insects which thrive upon the leaves of
aquatic plants fall into the water. So as he cannot leap high enough to catch
them, he fills his mouth with water and squirts it at an insect with such aim
and force that he rarely fails to knock the insect into the water where he can
easily catch it. Many other animals squirt various liquids, occasionally in
attack, but most times in defence. The fish makes a veritable squirt-gun of
his mouth.
Beavers use sticks, chips, and even stones in building their dams; and their
engineering abilities are astounding. They are also capable of meeting
emergencies, as shown by the following incident. A farmer in Michigan
discovered one morning, just after a flood, that all his potato sacks, which
had been hung on a back fence to dry, had suddenly disappeared. A few
days later he found them in a nearby beavers' colony, used in rebuilding
their dam, which had suddenly overflowed. The beavers wasted no time,
when they discovered their danger, in meeting the emergency by using the
sacks to prevent the destruction of their home.
Monkeys make skilled use of clubs and stones in capturing their prey and
fighting their enemies.
The skill with which some of them throw pebbles would lead us to believe
they have already reached the degree of civilisation that many tribes of
savages had reached only a few years ago, when they learned to use the
boomerang and lasso. Some naturalists claim that monkeys actually set
pitfalls for their enemies and lie in wait for them to be caught, just as a
hunter would do.
Elephants also know the value of clubs in warfare, and will often use a
broken limb of a dead tree as a weapon of defence. The story is told and
vouched for by Mr. William B. Smith that on his farm, near Mount
Lookout, a few years ago a donkey grazed in the same pasture with a
ferocious bull. He was frequently attacked by the bull, and always got the
worst of the fight. His feet were no match for the bull's horns, but one day
the mule grabbed a long pole in his mouth, and, whirling it about, almost
killed the bull, and henceforth the two lived on the best of terms in the same
pasture.
I have a friend who owns a cow that knows exactly how to lift an iron latch
to the barn door with her tongue and open the door. Innumerable times she
has opened a gate in the same way to permit her calf to go free with her. So
skilled is she in the manipulation of doors and latches that we are tempted
to believe in some previous state of existence she was a professional lock-
picker!
Cats and dogs are famed for their ability to open doors by pulling latch-
strings. And not a few cats show a strong desire to study music by walking
up and down the keyboard of a piano!
Monkeys who live near the seashore show wonderful aptness in opening
oysters and shell-fish with sharp stones, exactly as a man would do.
Monkeys have already reached the degree of civilization where they select
the stones best suited for their work, and from their progress in the past it is
reasonable to believe that in the near future they will not only be able to
make their own tools—thus placing themselves on a mental footing with
our flint-chipping ancestors of the early stone age,—but will also learn the
use of fire and eventually the use of guns and ammunition, which marks
one of the most important epochs in the evolution of the human species.
The chimpanzees, gorillas, and apes of the African forests have many times
been observed in the act of piling brushwood upon the fires left by
travellers, and though they do not know how to kindle a fire, they have
learned how to keep it burning. The tame ones soon learn how to ignite
matches, and often do great harm by starting forest fires.
But they show quite as much intelligence about the use of fire as the
average small child. In fact, it has been thought by a number of great
scholars that man had not yet made his appearance upon the earth in the
miocene age, and that all the marvellous chipped flints of that age belong to
semi-human pithecoid apes of wonderful intelligence. There is surely
nothing in the facts of natural history, nor in Darwin's theory of evolution,
that makes such a supposition unbelievable.
Baboons use poles as levers, stones as hammers, and seem to understand
the more simple mechanical devices. Prantl claims that man is the only
animal capable of using fire but not a few baboons know how to strike a
match, heap dried leaves over the blaze to make it burn, and then heap on
dead wood to feed the fire. This knowledge with them, exactly as with
primitive peoples, is a product of long experience and does not show any
mathematical truths or principles any more than making a direct cut across
a field implies "knowledge of the relation of a hypothenuse to the two other
sides of a right-angled triangle." This is what Prantl calls "spontaneous
mathematical thinking."
I knew of a tame ape in Chicago that learned to swing from the end of a
clothes-line and seemed to enjoy it very much. The line was just the right
length and properly hung so as to allow the ape to swing out from a kitchen
window and touch the ground. Just for fun, some one cut a piece from the
line so that he could not reach the ground; immediately the ape hunted
another piece of cord, tying it to the end of his line so as to increase its
length, and much to his delight, continued to swing on the line.
The distinctive features of animal protection and home government,
especially in the higher groups, may compare favourably with any of the
methods used by civilised man. This is true both of their offensive and
defensive contrivances and for their monarchies and republics. They use
shells, scales, plates of every kind, with innumerable modifications for
various purposes—spines and allied armaments—all shapes and sizes;
poisonous secretions, deadly odours, strong claws and teeth wielded by
strong muscles, and form colonies that are more than a gregarious
association. In most cases, they have communities composed of individuals
living individual lives, yet which act in cases of need as one unit.
X
ANIMAL ARCHITECTS, ENGINEERS, AND HOUSE
BUILDERS
The beaver huts, or "lodges" as they are usually called, look not unlike
beehives, somewhat broader at the base, with thick walls and roof, four to
six feet in thickness. They are formed of numbers of poles, twigs, and small
branches of trees, woven together and plastered with mud, in the same way
that the dams are made. Inside the house are circular chambers formed of
mud, which have been smoothed and polished like waxed floors by the feet
of the occupants. Around the outer border of each polished floor is dry
grass used for Mrs. Beaver's nursery, and here the young beavers sleep and
play.
From the outside these beaver huts resemble Esquimaux snow-houses,
being almost circular in form, and domed. The walls are quite thick enough
to keep out the cold, but with all the beaver's ingenuity, he is helpless
against trappers. Summer and winter they are hunted, until now they are
fast becoming extinct. How few people seem fully to realise and care what
is being done to wild animals! They do not seem to know that it is a crime
to take the life of a being unnecessarily. Only human life is sacred to them!
To realize the wonderful work of beavers, and then to act as we do toward
them is unworthy of our civilisation.
An interesting cousin of the beaver, the musquash or muskrat, and called by
the Indians the beaver's "little brother," is also a house-builder and engineer
of no mean abilities. He is at home throughout the greater part of North
America, and, like the beaver, frequents the regions of slowly flowing
streams and large, reed-bordered ponds. Here he mingles in groups of his
own kin, and together they build houses, work and play, dive and swim,
with almost as much skill as their big beaver brothers.
The muskrat is a skilled engineer, and delights in tunnelling. His home
consists of a large rounded chamber which is reached by a long burrow
from the side of a stream. From his main living-room are oftentimes found
a number of smaller chambers or galleries, and these are used to store food
in the form of delicate roots and bits of bark. Some of the more ambitious
muskrats build large houses on piles of mud which rise out of the water.
These houses are usually made of heaps of dead grass and weeds which are
cemented together with mud and clay; at other times they contain no mud or
clay, and seem to be only piles of tender roots and swamp grasses to be
used for food during the long, cold winters.
From his physical appearance, the muskrat is well prepared to do his work:
he is stoutly built, with a body about a foot in length, not including the tail;
has small eyes, and tiny ears, partly covered with fur. In the winter, as food
gets scarce, he begins to eat even the walls of his house, and by the time his
home is gone—spring has arrived!
A most unusual family of skilled house-builders are the brush-tailed rat-
kangaroos, or Jerboa kangaroos of Australia and Tasmania. They are no
larger than an ordinary rabbit, but they have cousins who are as large as a
man. These rat-kangaroos have most interesting tails, covered with long
hair which forms itself into a crest near the tip. Their homes are found
among small grassy hills, where there are a few trees and bushes. They
scratch out a small hole in the ground, near a tuft of tall grass, and so bend
the grass as to form a complete roof to the house, which is rather poorly
constructed, and whose chief interest lies in the unusual way the kangaroos
have of carrying all the building materials, like tiny bundles of hay, held
compactly in their tails. There is no other workman among the animals that
employs quite this method of transporting materials.
The rat-kangaroos have a dainty little brown cousin that lives in Africa, and
who is occasionally seen jumping around on the ground, underneath bushes,
and near damp springs. He is very small, not over three inches in length,
and is like a miniature kangaroo, except for his long tail. Like their great
cousins—the kangaroos—Mrs. Jerboa often carries her babies on her back
when she goes out to seek food.
In the Great Sahara Desert, parched and dry, are found numerous cities of
these little animals. With the exception of a few birds, reptiles, jackals and
hyenas, they are the only inhabitants of this barren and desolate land. From
the Arabs we learn that these little animals have extensive and intricate
burrows, consisting of innumerable passages tunnelled out in the hard, dry
soil. And these tunnels are the result of combined labour on the part of the
entire community. The least alarm causes them to scuffle away into their
underground homes.
One of the larger species of Central Asia employs a stratagem that is
remarkable. Like their cousins of Africa, they live in a great underground
city which is a perfect network of burrows which end in a large central
chamber. From this chamber a long winding tunnel terminates very near the
surface of the ground, and it is a long distance from the other burrows. No
sign of its existence appears from above the surface of the earth, but if an
enemy invades the burrow, away the jerboas rush for this secret exit and
break through to the surface out of reach of the trouble, and escape.
These African jerboas are exceedingly odd in appearance, and they are two-
legged in their habits of walk, and never go on all-fours. They walk by
placing one hind foot alternately before the other; and they run in the same
way. They can leap an extraordinary distance.
Frogs and toads, as a class, are not so skilled in house-building as some of
their higher relations, but there is one of their number—the Hyla faber—
that is remarkably gifted in building mud houses. He lives in Brazil, and the
natives call him the ferreiro, or smith, and he is indeed the master-builder of
his family. Mrs. Hyla is really the gifted member of the tribe, and it is
during the breeding season that she diligently dives underneath the water,
digs up handfuls of mud, and builds on the bottom a small circular wall,
which encloses a space about ten to fourteen inches in diameter. This wall is
continued until it reaches about four inches above the surface of the water.
It looks not unlike a small volcano, and the inside is skilfully smoothed.
This has been done by Mrs. Frog's artistic hands. When the house is entirely
completed, Mrs. Frog lays a great number of eggs, and here they are quite
safe from enemies both as eggs and baby tadpoles.
Mr. Frog seems little concerned in the building of the home, but he does
take pleasure in croaking for Mrs. Frog while she works. Perhaps this is to
her heart genuine music, and his faithful attention to their children makes
up for his love of idleness!
Perhaps the strangest animal engineer in the world is found in Madagascar
and Australia. It is the duckbill or duckmole, and is scientifically known as
the Ornithorhynchus paradoxus. The natives of Australia call it by several
names: Mallangong, Tambreet, and not a few call it, Tohunbuck.
This odd little aquatic engineer digs long tunnels of great intricacy in the
bands of lazy rivers, and because of its paradoxical nature and appearance
has caused many strange stories to originate about its habits and methods of
propagation. It has the beak of a duck and waddles not unlike this bird, but,
like other mammals, it gives birth to its young, and does not lay eggs, as is
so often claimed for it. When swimming it looks like a bunch of floating
weeds or grass.
Its home is always on the banks of a stream, and is always provided with
two entrances: one below the surface of the water, and the other above. This
insures escape in case of enemies. The main tunnel or road to the home is
sometimes fifty feet in length, and no engineer could devise a more
deceptive approach; it winds up and down like a huge serpent, to the right,
and to the left, and is so annoyingly variable in its sinuous course that even
the natives have great trouble in digging the duckbill out of its nest.
The nest is oval in form, and is well-carpeted with dry weeds and grass.
Here the young reside on soft beds until they are large enough to care for
themselves. There are from one to four in each nest.
There are no greater architects in the universe than may be found among the
coral-polypes. These interesting little animals of the deep have been much
misunderstood, and have sometimes had the erroneous designation of
"insect" bestowed upon them. The word "insect" has been applied in a very
loose and general sense in other days; but naturalists and scientists should
see to it that the use of this term be corrected in reference to these
wonderful coral-architects, and that no informed person refer to them
except as animals. Even poets have been guilty of propagating the most
erroneous ideas about the nature and works of these sea-builders.
Montgomery, in his Pelican Island, makes statements that are shocking to
an intelligent thinker, and which no scientist can excuse on the ground of
poetical license. "The poetry of this excellent author," says Dana, "is good,
but the facts nearly all errors—if literature allows of such an incongruity."
Think of coral-animals as being referred to as shapeless worms that "writhe
and shrink their tortuous bodies to grotesque dimensions"! These deep-sea
builders manufacture or secrete from their own bodies the coral substance
out of which the great reefs are built. It is a part of their life work and
nature, as a flower produces its own colours and shapes; it is amusing to
know that it has only been about one hundred and fifty years since it was
discovered not to be a plant but an animal! Even Ovid states the popular
belief of the classic period when he speaks of the coral as a seaweed "which
existed in a soft state as long as it remained in the sea, but had the curious
property of becoming hard on exposure to the air."
These strange coral-producing animals of the deep demand two especially
important conditions only under which they will thrive: namely, a certain
depth of water and a certain temperature. Thus it is seen that the warmth of
the sea determines the distribution of the corals; the geography of these
animals is defined by degrees of temperature. Only in equatorial seas may
reef-building corals be found; and if we select the "Equator as a natural
centre of the globe, and measure off a band of 1800 miles in breadth on
each side of that line," we will find that it will include the chief coral
regions of the earth.
The work of the corals is most interesting. Small as are these tiny workmen,
each and every one does his bit and, speck by speck, adds his minute
contribution to the growing mass of coral until entire islands are surrounded
by extensive reefs. Tahiti, for example, is surrounded by a barrier reef
which is really an immense wall. The large barrier reef on the northeast
coast of Australia extends in a continuous line for 1,000 miles, and varies
from 10 to 90 miles in breadth. Some reefs are mere fringes which simply
skirt the coast lands, and seem to be mere extensions of the beach. Still
another variety of reef is known as the "atoll" or "lagoon" reef. This latter
form is seen in circular rings of coral of various breadths which enclose a
body of still water—the lagoon. There are many of these coral islands in the
Indian and Pacific Oceans. Keeling or Cocos Atoll, of the Indian Ocean, is
9½ miles in its greatest width; Bow Island is 30 miles in length, and 6 miles
wide; while in the Maldive Archipelago one island measures 88
geographical miles in length, and in some places is 20 miles wide. When
one beholds a large coral ring, covered with rich soil and tropical
vegetation, and "protecting a quiet lake-haven from the restless ocean
without, it is little to be wondered at that the earlier voyagers recorded their
surprise that the apparently insignificant architects of such an erection are
able to withstand the force of the waves and to preserve their works among
the continual attacks of the sea." As Pyrard de Laval truly said, "It is a
marvel to see each of these atollons surrounded on all sides by a great bank
of stone—walls such as no human hands could build on the space of earth
allotted to them.... Being in the middle of an atollon, you see all around you
this great stone bank, which surrounds and protects the island from the
waves; but it is a formidable attempt, even for the boldest, to approach the
bank and watch the waves roll in, and break with fury upon the shore."
As to the explanation of the modes of formation of these coral-reefs, the
scientists have long been propounding theories which are sometimes
amusing. Strangely enough they have nearly all explained that coral-
polypes aggregate themselves in the forms of atolls and barrier-reefs by a
mysterious "instinct," mediocrity's only term for screening its ignorance,
and which is also given as the cause for their secreting lime. Flinders says
that they form a great protecting reef in order that they may be protected by
its shelter, and that the leeward aspect of the reef forms a nursery for their
infant colonies.
Thus we see that these same scientists are accrediting these little architects
with the possession of a great intelligence, and they are thought to co-
operate together in a manner expressive of the greatest degree of efficiency
and brotherly feeling. Each of these scientists gives a theory that leaves
untouched the essential question of the causes for coral-reefs assuming their
various shapes; and it is reasonable to believe that they work according to a
divine wisdom and plan, and that mankind does not yet understand their
strange ways, which give us a higher conception of the universe than that
held by the ancients. Science has come to the point where it must recognise
the perfect unity of all life, and that our fellow-architects, engineers, and
house-builders in the animal world also fill an important place in Nature's
great scheme.
XI
FOOD CONSERVERS
—Coleridge.
It can almost be said that there is no industry or profession of the human
world that is not carried on with equal skill in the animal world. This is
especially true of merchandising and store-keeping; animals, however, have
different methods of merchandising than men, although these methods are
none the less real. They give and take instead of buy and sell and have co-
operative shops which they operate with great success. They unite for a
desired end, and demonstrate their ability to work together in a common
enterprise in a way that might teach man a good lesson.
Food and shelter are the first needs of animals. In order to obtain these, they
group themselves into foraging parties in the most ingenious manner. Like
mankind, they sometimes co-operate for dishonest ends; they form "trusts"
and organise into gangs for purposes of mutual aid.
Deer, monkeys, rabbits, foxes, and numerous others conduct their dining-
rooms on a co-operative principle. Some watch and wait while others dine.
The same is true where they go to watering places to drink and bathe.
Perhaps the most unique and clever food conserver is the American polecat.
He not only provides for himself, but prepares a larder for his young, so that
they will have plenty of food. The nursery is usually comfortably embedded
in a cave, and is lined with soft, dry grass. Adjoining this nursery is a larder,
which often contains from ten to fifty large frogs and toads, all alive, but so
dexterously bitten through the brain as to make them incapable of escaping.
Mr. and Mrs. Pole-cat can then visit or hunt as they please, so long as their
children have plenty of fresh meat at home!
Another interesting food conserver is the chipping squirrel, or chipmunk, so
named because his cry sounds like the chirp of little chickens. His method
of dress is most unusual; he is brownish grey in colour, with five stripes of
black and two of pale yellow running along the back of his coat; the throat
and lower part of his body is snowy white. These colours occasionally vary,
when the grey and yellow are superseded by black.
His home is underground, usually under an old wall, near a rock fence, or
under a tree; his burrow is so long and winding that he can easily escape
almost any enemy, except the weasel, which is not easily outwitted. His
nursery and living-room is quite pretentious, but his lateral storeroom is a
marvel! He is a miser indeed, and stores up every acorn and nut he can find,
even many times more than he can ever eat. His variety of food is almost
unending—he loves buckwheat, beaked nuts, pecans, various kinds of grass
seeds, and Indian corn. In carrying food to his home he first fills his
pouches to overflowing and then takes another nut in his mouth; he thus
reminds the classical reader of Alemæon in the treasury of Crœsus.
The hedgehog is a regular Solomon in her methods of collecting fruit.
Plutarch had a very high opinion of her. He says that when grapes are ripe,
the mother hedgehog goes under the vines and shakes them until some of
the grapes fall; she then literally rolls over them until many are attached to
her spines, and marches back to her babies in the cave. "One day," says
Plutarch, "when we were all together, we had the chance of seeing this with
our own eyes—it looked as if a bunch of grapes was shuffling along the
ground, so thickly covered was the animal with its booty."
American Museum of Natural History, New York
Alpine mice not only form comfortable winter homes in the earth, but
combine into small winter colonies, each colony numbering about ten to
twelve inhabitants, all of whom are under the direction of a leader. Thus
organised, they proceed to lay up provisions for the winter. They use their
mouths as scythes and their paws as rotary machines. Surely their wisdom
and foresight call forth our greatest admiration. The jerboas or jumping
mice are not only skilled athletes in the art of jumping, but they are gifted
food conservers and producers as well. They lay up complete storehouses of
food, which they do not consume altogether as their appetite may direct; but
conserve it carefully for the times when nothing can be obtained from the
fields. Then, and then only, do they open the closed magazines. Such acts of
intelligence cannot be recorded under the head of "instinct"! They
demonstrate the ability to plan for the future, and meet all emergencies.
Certain food hoarders and robbers, like the vole, are so very greedy and
become such misers that they often threaten total destruction to large areas
of grain. They were so plentiful in the classic land of Thessaly, the vale of
Tempe, and the Land of Olympus that the old Greeks established what they
called an Apollo Smintheus, the Mouse-destroying God. In the early spring,
according to Professor Loeffler, who has made a special study of their
invasions, they begin to come down from their homes in the hills to the
cultivated fields. They seem to follow regular roads, and often travel along
the railroad embankment. They travel very slowly, and when at home live
somewhat on the order of prairie dogs, that is, in underground dwellings
with numerous winding passages and tunnels.
These wise little food conservers are nocturnal in habit, and are rarely seen
except by careful observers. When they once determine to rob a field, they
do it with amazing rapidity and completeness. In a single night hordes of
these workers go into a cornfield and by daylight not a stalk of corn
remains. The field is as empty as if a cyclone had struck it. They work with
great system, and while a part of their number cut the stalks down, others
cut it up into movable sizes, while still others superintend its systematic
removal. Storehouses are usually provided before the grain is even cut.
They make long voyages throughout a country, storing away tons of grain
and food in these various granaries. To these they come for supplies
whenever necessary. All poverty-stricken voles are also fed from these
storehouses, since it is the product of the community as a whole. Aristotle
wrote at length about their wise and destructive ways.
Not the least ingenious of food conservers are the hamsters, members of the
great rodent family. They have made their dwellings most comfortable and
even luxurious in arrangement and furnishings. Like wealthy farmers, they
are not satisfied with comfortable dwellings only, but they too must have
spacious barns adjoining their homes. Their home, or burrow proper,
consists of two openings: one, which is used as an entrance, and which
sinks vertically into the ground; the other, which is used as an exit, with a
winding slope. The central room is beautifully carpeted with straw, moss,
and dry leaves, which makes it a very pleasant living-room and bedroom. A
third small winding tunnel leads from this room to the barns and storehouse.
Thus, Mr. and Mrs. Hamster and the children have no need to go forth in
the cold and wet weather to seek food—they can remain at home perfectly
protected and well-fed. They are very liberal, and in case of need or
poverty, will always share their food with their neighbours.
I once found the nest of a harvest mouse, which was woven of plaited
blades of straw of the oats and wheat. It was perfectly round, with the
aperture so ingeniously closed that I could scarcely tell to what part of the
nest it belonged. It was as round as a marble and would actually roll when
placed on a table, although within its walls were six tiny mice, naked and
blind. As they increased in size day by day, the elastic wall of their small
home expanded, and thus served their need until such time as they were old
enough to live independent of this specially provided shelter.
There is a larger animal, known as a "rat-hare" or the harvest rat, which
gathers piles of hay for winter use, sometimes to the height of six or eight
feet in diameter. They begin harvesting in the early part of August, and after
having cut the grass, they carefully spread it out to dry before placing it in
their barns. These barns are usually located in holes or crevices of
mountains. They are found in immense numbers in the Altai Mountains.
The California woodrat is not only a food hoarder but a notable thief and
robber. A nest was found that was a veritable tool chest and pawn shop! It
contained fourteen knives, three forks, six small spoons, one large soup
spoon, twenty-seven large nails, hundreds of small tacks, two butcher
knives, three pairs of eye-glasses, one purse, one string of beads, one rubber
ball, two small cakes of soap, one string of red peppers, several boxes of
matches, with numerous small buttons, needles, and pins. Apparently these
woodrats are as ambitious for unnecessary and useless possessions as is
man himself. Their big storeroom did, however, contain a larder in which
they had some of their favourite food, such as seeds and nuts.
Some animals have learned not only to acquire, but also to defend and
protect, all their property. We see in the human world how strong is the
impulse to collect, and children will invariably collect anything from
pebbles to peach-pits, if they see other children doing the same thing.
Most animals that do not hoard are those that forage for food, or fish, and
rarely have permanent homes. The orang-outangs, for example, are regular
gipsies, and go from place to place wherever food is plentiful. They take
life easy, and sometimes during their journeys select a suitable spot near the
seashore and have a real picnic. A scout has already discovered the right
spot for getting big oysters, of which they are exceedingly fond, and when
they have assembled, certain ones proceed to dig up the oysters, which they
hand to others on the shore and they, in turn, place them on big stones, and
proceed to open them for the feast. If one of the fishermen-monkeys
discovers an oyster open, he will not insert his hand to remove the meat
until first placing a stone between the valves. This assures him protection
against the closing of the oyster. In most cases, they open the oysters by
first placing them on stones and then using another stone as a hammer.
These facts are vouched for by no less authorities than Gamelli Carreri,
Dampier, and Wafer.
It is only a matter of time until many animals will understand the use of
man-made tools. Some have already learned to use such tools as they make
and shape for themselves. Monkeys and apes are already gifted in this art.
Of course, under domestication, they use knives, forks, spoons, and dishes
not so much from intelligence as from imitation. This, however, might be
said of many human beings. I have seen an immense chimpanzee sit in a
chair, set his own dinner table, use his knife and fork correctly when eating,
and take great delight in the use of his napkin, which he always carefully
refolded when his meal was over.
The human-like qualities of apes and monkeys, however, need scarcely be
told. They are so very similar to man in most ways that there are few things
they cannot do. Aelian tells of an ape which learned to drive horses
skilfully. He knew just when and how to use the whip, how much slack to
allow in the reins, and when to tighten them! They greatly resent any
intrusion on their hunting-grounds, and make use of sticks and clubs to
protect them. The chief is always armed with a club, and is thoroughly
skilled in the use of it. It sometimes happens that an elephant will come to
the same tree to seek food that apes frequent, and although they have no
enmity towards each other, they like the same kind of food. As soon as the
ape sees the elephant reaching his trunk among the branches, he
immediately slips near the elephant, and when an opportunity presents
itself, he whacks him over the trunk with his club! The infuriated elephant
runs away in terror!
A story is told of a party of foraging apes who went into a cornfield with
the purpose of robbing it, and discovered two men. They immediately
rushed upon them and attempted to poke their eyes out with sticks and
would have succeeded but for the intervention of two other men who
chanced to be near. The extreme cleverness of apes in applying their reason
and judgment is shown in Vosmaer's account of the female orang-outang,
who tried to open the padlock of her chain with a small stick. She had seen
her master open it with a key, and she exactly imitated the motion of his
hands in the attempt.
Man shows a disposition to deny animals all traits and characteristics which
are similar to his own. This reminds us of a remark that Cardinal Newman
once made that men know less of animals than they do of angels. Why
should we show such foolish pride and delusion, and try to baffle one of
God's great facts? When men attempt to extinguish the idea of animal
intelligence and sentiment by referring to it as instinct, we are reminded of
the desert ostrich, which buries its head in the sand and thinks it cannot be
seen. We should proudly acknowledge the wonderful human-like methods
of these food conservers of the animal world, and recognise in all this a
guiding Providence who provides for and protects all his creatures, be they
great or small.
XII
TOURISTS AND SIGHT-SEERS
The members of these co-operative tours take life tickets, and each tour
lasts about one year. One of the most unusual instances of such
These are animals of the mouse tribe, which live in the mountainous
districts. They live upon roots and grasses. They breed very rapidly. At
certain times they go from the centre of Norway to the east and west,
crossing valley, hill, and river in great masses. Many are destroyed by birds
and beasts of prey, but finally the survivors reach the Atlantic on the Gulf of
Bothnia and, for some strange unknown reason, plunge in and die. Only
enough remain from one season to another to propagate the species. It is an
immense co-operative suicide society.
Rivers and valleys are sometimes effectual barriers. On the plains of the
Amazon great numbers of animals are found on one side of the river only;
these have not been able to cross to the other. On the north side of the Rio
Negro are two varieties of monkeys, the brachiurus conxion and the
jacchus bicolor, which are unknown on the south side. Of course, water-
loving animals, such as seals, whales, and porpoises are at home in the
water and can swim for days without stopping. Quite a few animals can
swim for a short distance, but comparatively few for long distances. In the
early days in North America it was not uncommon for buffalo to swim
across the Mississippi River. Rats and squirrels often migrate in great
numbers. It oftentimes happens that Arctic animals travel from one place to
another on floating ice. In the South American waters it is a common sight
to see floating islands covered with plants and trees upon which there are
live animals; and while these animals are likely to perish, they are
oftentimes carried safely to land. Eagles have often been instrumental in
bringing new species of animals to islands where they had previously been
unknown, their purpose being to provide food for their own young. Some of
these animals would escape and henceforth become citizens of their new
habitation.
An interesting division of migrants is that of the casual travellers, like the
men and women who always remain at home except when special business
calls them away. Sudden climatic changes, or the scarcity of food, often
cause stay-at-home animals to make tours into new
territories. As a good instance, I might cite the case of three wolves, which I
saw entering Jackson Park in Chicago, during very severe weather when
Lake Michigan was frozen over. The morning papers stated that because of
forest fires in Michigan, and the extreme cold, which not only made food
scarce for the wild animals of Michigan, but froze the Lake, many of them
had come across the ice into the great Chicago parks seeking food and
shelter.
The subject of animal travel is full of interesting and difficult problems, and
not the least interesting nor the least difficult is the question of just how
they find their way to and from various places.
Many naturalists tell us that these animals are led by inherited instinct along
the migration lines followed by their forefathers. But even if this were true,
what made them originally follow such a course?
invariably have a leader, and while we do not know how he obtains his
position, nor how he directs his followers, we do know he is highly
successful in his efforts.
No act in the animal world bespeaks more intelligence than that of placing
sentinels, especially during a journey. Horses show striking skill and
ingenuity in the choosing and placing of their sentinels. Any one who has
been fortunate enough to have seen them travelling in the forests of South
America, where the wild horses are gregarious, and travel in herds of five
hundred to a thousand, has noticed that
sentinels are always stationed around the herd. These animals are not well
prepared for fighting, and experience has taught them that their greatest
safety is in flight, and so, when they graze or sleep,
sentinels are always on the look-out for enemies. If a man approaches, the
sentinel at first walks toward him, as if to make sure what the enemy is, and
what he desires, if the man goes nearer to the herd, the sentinel neighs in a
most peculiar tone. Immediately the herd is aroused, and gallops away, not
in confusion, but perfect order, as though its members were human soldiers.
The African apes have an interesting way of sending their sentinel to the top
of an adjacent rock or tree, that he may look over the
A few years ago, many of the sheep in the northern part of Wales had
become quite wild, and they usually grazed in parties of twelve to twenty,
always having a sentinel so stationed as to command a prominent view of
the surrounding territory. If any animal or person came near, he would give
a peculiar hiss or whistle, repeating it two or three times, at which the
whole herd would scamper away to places of safety.
One of the most striking facts about migration is its never-failing regularity
and success. Most animals migrate at the recurrence of the breeding season.
Of these, the great sea-turtle, which seeks the shallow water and deep sandy
hills when ready to lay her eggs, is well known.
The love of their original homes is one of the most striking features of
certain animal travellers. The fierce struggle for existence and the territory
required for an animal's home largely determine the amount of effort they
make to seize and hold certain possessions. A pair of wildcats, for example,
require a comparatively small hunting ground. But this they will defend
against invasion even to the point of death. There are many more evidences
showing the animals' love of home, and that they also know the meaning of
home-sickness.
Not a few animals have learned definitely to lay out and obtain recognition
for the boundaries of their respective ranging-grounds. This is amply
proven by their respect and recognition of rights of way. Animals of certain
farms seem to know the exact boundaries of their grazing lands and
pastures, and to teach this knowledge to their young. In addition they often
police their lands and pastures against intruders. Woe unto any traveller
found on the wrong highway! It is not uncommon for the transgressor to be
pushed from a right of way to the rocks below. More than once a court's
decision regarding disputable territory has been based on the sheep's
recognition of boundary; those sheep slain in battle or otherwise injured
while trying to invade the questionable territory have been paid for by the
owner of the transgressing sheep.
It is easy to understand how sheep can recognise their rights of way, but
somewhat difficult to account for their knowledge of boundaries. Sheep and
goats have for ages been the greatest mountain-path and road-makers.
Whether or not they have engineers, we are not sure, but they seem to select
the shortest, easiest, and best route across the trackless hills, and never seem
to change the way. In these localities, the sheep are almost in a primitive
condition, and "not the least interesting feature of their conduct in this
relapse to the wild life is that, in spite of the highly artificial condition in
which they live to-day, they retain the primitive instincts of their race."
That this "peremptory and path-keeping" instinct is shown by the habits of
the musk-ox, is clear. He is as much akin to the sheep as to cattle, and in
habits more like those of the great prehistoric sheep as we imagine these to
have been. The musk-ox naturally assembles in large flocks, and is
migratory, just as the domesticated flocks of Spain are, and those of Thrace
and the Caspian steppe. These flocks always return from the barren lands in
the far north by the same road, and cross rivers by the same fords. Nothing
but too persistent slaughter at these points by the enemies who beset them,
induces them to desert their ancient highways. Pictures and anecdotes of the
migrations of these animals, and of the bison in former days, represent them
as moving on a broad front across the prairie or tundra. The examples of all
moving multitudes suggest that this was not their usual formation on the
march, and their roads prove that they moved on a narrow front or in file.
On the North American prairie, though the bison are extinct, their great
roads still remain as evidence of their former habits. These trails are paths
worn on the prairie, nearly all running due north and south (the line of the
old migration of the herds), like gigantic rabbit tracks. They are hard, the
grass on them is green and short, and, if followed, they generally lead near
water, to which a diverging track runs from the highway.
How interesting must have been the life on this great animal highway,
before the Indian made the deadly arrow to destroy these nature-loving
travellers! There is no doubt but that, in their own way, these animals felt
all the emotions known to a human traveller; that they enjoyed the flowery
road, rested and played when weary, looked forward with joy to their
favourite watering and bathing places, and recognised old watering places
that they had visited for years.
The great roads and highways made by graminivorous animals, from those
which the hippopotamus cuts through the mammoth canes and reeds of the
African streams, to the smaller rabbit highways of England and America, all
tell their own story of how these animals live and travel. The principal
roads of rabbits over hills are as permanent as sheep and buffalo roads.
These roads, however, should not be confused with the little trails that lead
to their play and feeding grounds.
My friend and fellow-naturalist, Ralph Stuart Murray, in writing to me from
Quebec, says: "In speaking of animal road builders, I might say that the
rabbit or hare of the north woods deserves much attention, for greatly
interesting are his highways. The life of the north woods brings one
constantly in touch with these roads, which, after generations upon
generations of constant use, are worn deep and smooth into the moose grass
and muskeg through which they run. At places, several distinct paths
intersect, and it is curious to note that while these roads wind in and out
underneath the low hanging evergreens, the 'cross-roads' will invariably be
located in a clear open space, often on the top of some small hillock.
"The great age of these roads is very evident when compared with the
newer, shallower paths of more recent years. So deep are the old ones, in
fact, that the quiet watcher in the woods will occasionally see two large,
upright ears—unmistakably those of a rabbit, seemingly sticking out of a
hole in the ground—yet moving at a rapid pace, and all the while no rabbit
in view. For all the world these vertical ears belonging to an unseen owner
resemble in use and appearance the periscope of a submarine—the
difference being that the rabbit uses his 'periscopes' for hearing, in order to
locate and avoid his foe, the submarine its periscope to locate and attack its
enemy."
The sheep terraces, which are so common on the sides of hills, though made
by sheep, are not roads, but feeding grounds. Sheep, when walking on a
hillside, invariably graze on the upper side, as they cannot reach the lower
grass. Therefore they walk backwards and forwards on the slope, just as a
reaping machine is driven over a hillside wheat-field. As the sheep takes a
"neck's length" each time, the little ridges or roads correspond exactly with
the measurements of the sheep's neck.
There are as many kinds of roads and terminals in the animal world as there
are in the human, and lest our pride make us forget, we should remember
that even the Panama Canal is dug according to the plan of a crawfish's
canal, such as may be seen near any muddy stream. It is strange that no
animal has learned to build elevated roads, though animals that live in trees,
like flying squirrels, monkeys, and flying foxes, are very skilled in going
from one tree to another. They have regular aerial highways, and some of
the tree frogs are veritable wonders in the accuracy of their leaps from tree
to tree. Even more skilled than these are the agamid lizards of India, whose
chief means of travel is a folding parachute, which at a moment's notice can
be erected and carry to another tree its lucky possessor. In Borneo is an
aviator tree-snake which is able to so spread his ribs and inflate his body
that he can actually sail from branch to branch in the tree-tops.
There are night travellers as well as day travellers; in fact, there are more
animals that roam around in a great forest at night than in the daytime. They
sleep during the day, when the day animals are roaming about, and go forth
to roam when it is night. It is then they seek for prey, and are much feared
by day animals. They see well in the dark, and travel so lightly that their
footsteps cannot be heard.
On the Island of Java are found a family of strange, dwarfish little beings,
which are called by the natives malmags, or hobgoblins. And they are well
named, for they look like creatures of a distorted imagination more than
real, living animals. They travel only at night, and so superstitious are the
natives of their evil influence that if one of these uncanny little creatures
appears near their rice fields, the plantation is immediately abandoned.
However, these small creatures are no larger than squirrels, and are
perfectly harmless. They are very rare even in their native lands—the
Oriental Archipelago and the Philippine Islands. They rear their young in
the hollow roots of bamboo trees, and to disturb their nests means to incur
the evil of all the land.
Night animals do not go forth to travel and seek prey until the night is far
advanced, and their prey is soundly sleeping. They seem to know the exact
time of the night, as if they had watches or clocks, and they usually go forth
to hunt about midnight and return to their homes about four o'clock. Only in
cases of extreme hunger do they vary from this rule.
How marvellously skilled are they in finding their way! They pass through
a crowded forest as though it were daytime, and strangely enough know just
how to return to their lairs. This special sense or gift is not possessed by
man; he must have marks and signs to return to a definite place.
These night-travellers number among their lot bats, flying squirrels,
leopards, and prowling snakes.
Bats are not only the most interesting of the night-travellers, but by far the
most curious and wonderful animals in the world. They are hideously ugly,
reminding one more of a miniature, closed-up umbrella than an animal!
They are coarse, awkward, when not in flight, and repellent; yet they have
such highly developed senses that they have no rivals in the animal world.
They excel most birds in flight, are able to make long nightly journeys, in
which they use their wings not only for flight, but as air-bags in which they
catch all kinds of flying insects. Their sense of touch as we know it is really
a combination of touch, sight, and hearing.
A bat is a paradox par excellence! Nature seems to have started to make a
little bear or fox, and suddenly forgot how and changed it into a winged
freak, with tail, claws, fur, sharp teeth, small ears that stand up, and tiny,
half-buried eyes. Its queer angular-edged wings look like an umbrella, with
the cloth stretched over steel ribs; but in the case of the bat, this framework
is made of delicate bones which are covered with a thin skin. The skin
contains numerous little sense organs dotted over its surface, which give the
bat his strange power.
Bats look more like mice than they do like birds, and they are sometimes
called flittermice. But they are mammals, and the young are fed with milk
by the mother, just as a cow feeds her calf. There is no danger that a bat will
ever fly against you in the dark; for they can avoid all mishap even when
their eyes are put out. They have special sense organs that tell them when
they are nearing an object, and can fly at headlong speed with the accuracy
of a rifle bullet directly into a small opening. This power is all due to the
mysterious sense located in their wings and ears, which causes even man to
consider his senses weak in comparison.
Bats are sociable creatures and huddle together and sleep in vast numbers
during the day, but when night comes on they come forth for their nocturnal
travels and sport by the millions. I have seen them leaving caves just at
dusk in such numbers as to look like one immense volume of smoke,
twenty to thirty feet wide, and lasting for more than five minutes. Mrs. Bat
often takes her babies with her on these nightly travels. I found one with
two young clinging to her breast. How they must enjoy these lovely trips!
There are many kinds and varieties of bats, ranging in size from the flying
foxes of the tropical world, with wings five feet in length, to the wood bat
of North America, which is not over six inches long. These interesting
friends of man are his greatest scavengers of the air. They are doing much
to check the mosquitoes throughout the regions of the world, and in more
civilized communities man makes shelters for them, that they may eradicate
mosquitoes.
XIII
ANIMAL SCAVENGERS AND CRIMINALS
Not the least among the uses of war dogs is the curious practice of sending
them into the enemies' lines of cavalry to convey fire in order to terrorise
the horses and throw them into confusion. This practice has been quite
common in the past. Each dog is dressed in a cuirass of leather and on his
back is carefully strapped a pot of boiling, blazing tar. Nothing so terrorises
horses as the sight of approaching fire.
A small but valuable ally to man is the ferret. This little creature has come
into prominence more particularly during recent years, when the rat infested
trenches have made his services invaluable. These Hun-like rats, devouring
and devastating in their thirst for human blood, would have forced the
abandonment of many a front line trench but for the aid of these trained
ferrets, thousands of which have been daily employed on the battle fronts.
The immense services rendered by carrier pigeons in the battle of the
Marne, not only to the military authorities, but also to the public at large,
will cause the civilised world to pay more attention to the importance of
these birds in the future. They carried all kinds of messages to and from
Paris during this memorable battle; in fact, they have been used in all the
battles as invaluable messengers.
Small animals, such as mice, canary birds, guinea pigs and rabbits are used
in trench warfare, because they are more sensitive than man to poisonous
gases. It sometimes happens that hundreds of men must be rescued from a
trench by three or four men. Each rescuer carries with him a canary bird in
a small cage attached to his shoulder. And as long as these birds show no
signs of distress the men are safe from gas poison. The birds soon become
attached to their masters and seem to like the adventure of the trenches.
As time goes on, it is to be hoped that we will understand our animal
brothers better, and that our old attitude toward the so-called "brutes" will
be entirely changed. Heretofore we have greatly abused the zebra, for
example, because of his wild disposition, ferocious humour, distrust of all
power except that in his own legs, and his pronounced aversion to work.
Why should we reproach him for his wildwood philosophy? It is perfectly
natural that any animal of his experience with man, and with sufficient
brains, would have only contempt for all mankind. His native home is in
Africa, and his human associates, if they are human, have been the
Hottentots, the Namaquois or the Amazoulons—the most impossible and
hideous people on the earth. Since his babyhood days he has seen nothing
but cannibalism and carnage among the savages; and since his
transportation to Europe by a strange occurrence of horrible circumstances,
he has been the subject for all kinds of barbarous punishments which man
has seen well to heap upon him. The zebra is not of the mental calibre to be
suddenly seized with love for the human species and its civilisations! And
the human species is astounded and thinks the zebra stupid and wicked. He
may be both, but his wisdom is undeniable when it comes to trusting
humanity, and his wickedness is small in comparison to man's terrible
cruelties. He should be awarded a medal for wisdom! For man is far the
greater ass of the two!
He roams the wild prairies where the fields need no ploughing. There he
finds an abundance of grass and fresh water along the streams. No loud
cursing and swearing ever greets his ears, nothing but the sweet song of the
wild birds. And his children romp and play with him, free as the winds that
blow. Of course, he has enemies even there, and so he uses camouflage by
painting himself in attractive stripes, so no one can see him at a distance.
Even Solomon should have praised his wisdom!
In the beginning God created man, and not long after gave him as his
policeman, the dog. And the obedience, friendship and devotion of the dog
to his master has been unending. The dog discusses no questions of right or
wrong, his only duty is to obey. This he does without a murmur. He is the
greatest testimony to man's civilisation, the first and the greatest element of
human progress. Through his co-operation man was elevated from the
savage to the state of the civilised. He made the herd possible. Without him
there could have been no herd, no assured subsistence of food and clothing,
no time to study and improve the mind, no astronomical observations, no
science, no arts, no automobiles, no airships, no wireless telegraphy—
nothing. The East is the home of civilisation, because the East is the home
of the dog.
A young hound knows more about tracking game or scenting the enemy
after six months' practice than the most skilled savage after fifty years of
study. The dog has so aided mankind as to give him more time for study
and self-improvement. Thus began the arts and sciences. An interesting, and
we believe original observation, of the influence of the dog on peoples is
that wherever the dog is found, especially among the shepherd peoples,
such as the Chaldeans, Egyptians, Arabs, Tartars, and Mongols, cannibalism
is unknown. This is due to the fact that the dog enables them to maintain the
herds which supply them with milk, food, and clothing, thus preserving
them from the criminal temptation of hunger.
The Indians of North America never refrained from roasting their enemies
until they made allies of the horse and dog. Humboldt proves the lively
regret held by one of the last surviving chief lieutenants of the war-like
Tecumseh whom he asked about a certain American officer who took part
in the fight. "Uh!" replied the Indian, "I eat some of him." "Do you still eat
your enemies?" asked Humboldt. "No," replied the Indian. "Big dog catch
heap meat for me!"
Surely no animal could be more uncivilised or cannibalistic in its desires
than man! Spinoza believed, however, that benevolence in animals
consisted only in their kindliness and friendly feeling for each other and
that we should expect nothing more of them. A good cow, so he thought,
was one that was kind to her calf, however ferocious she might be toward
human children. But we do not accept this standard of goodness, nor
believe that animals' kindness extends only to their own tribes. Their lowest
standard of life is no worse than the cannibalism existing among the lower
tribes of uncivilised man, which is one of the highest ideals of tribal life.
The greatest hero among our savages is the one that can put the most
enemies to death.
Many animals seem to have a social instinct and a moral sentiment toward
man. They try to break the old bonds of distrust between their master and
themselves. This is especially true of the puma, second to the largest of the
big cats of the Americas, which seems to love the society of man, and seeks
not only to be near him, but to protect him from the attacks of the much-
dreaded jaguar. A civil engineer tells the story of an experience he had
while journeying up one of the big South American rivers by boat. At their
nightly encampments one of the passengers on board was an old miner who
insisted on sleeping in a hammock suspended between two small trees. His
weight was sufficient to bring the hammock almost to the ground at its
lowest curve. One morning, his friends inquired how he had slept, and he
complained that "the frogs and small animals had made so much noise
under the hammock that he could not sleep." One of the Indian servants
roared with laughter, as he said, "Uh, 'tiger' sleep with old man last night.
He watch him!"—tiger being the Indian term for the puma. Careful
searching revealed the footprints of an immense puma, and that he had
evidently lain directly under the hammock. The noise which had kept the
old man from sleeping was the purring of the animal, pleased over the
privilege of sleeping so near a man. These Guiana Indians know the ways
of the forests, and have a special liking for wild animals. This entire
absence of fear in the puma is the same as exhibited by the tame house cat.
Many animals seem fond of human companionship, and are easily tamed.
My sister raised a small red deer in Texas, and he became so perfectly tame
that he would follow her wherever she went, and would even take food
from her hand. In Yellowstone Park the deer are so tame they will come into
the yards to get food, while the brown bears approach the hotels like
tramps, and many of the smaller animals are perfectly fearless. At the
Bronx Zoological Gardens, and the London Zoo, the animals have lost all
fear. They seem to realise that they have no power to escape and depend
entirely upon man for their daily food. But, of course, their conditions are
artificial, hence such conclusions as we may draw as to their normal attitude
toward man do not necessarily indicate the innate character of their wild
kinsmen. We occasionally find, for instance, that in unsettled regions like
parts of Mexico and South America, where animals are plentiful and man's
influence largely absent, they are found to be particularly ferocious, yet
even then lions and leopards rarely attack men unless disturbed in some
unusual way.
Quite a few naturalists and scientists believe that the animals' love for man
was acquired and not natural. But if this be true, how did the very early
tribes of men escape destruction at the hands of the wild beasts which were
far more numerous than at present? The animal kingdom was evidently
impressed by the power of man at a very early stage of its development, but
in just what manner or what period of time this came to pass is not known.
If we regard the conflict as merely between two great groups of animals,
surely the animals should have won, and man would have disappeared from
the face of the earth. The fact that he did not, and that he became master of
the animals, is presumptive evidence that man exceeded the animals in
intelligence.
Primitive man could have lived in no other way than by "his wits." For he
was not nearly so well equipped for defence as are the monkeys of to-day.
Their greatest power is in the ability to use their arms and hands in
swinging rapidly from branch to branch. This gives them an advantage over
all tree-climbing cats. They are very proficient in throwing stones and other
missiles. This is dumbfounding to other animals. Of course, their intelligent
and quick-witted methods of defence, menace, guard-duty, and loyalty to
tribe makes them great warriors, and enables them to survive even the
onslaughts of their greatest enemy and nightmare of every non-carnivorous
animal—the harpy eagle!
Through the necessary adjustments growing out of the close relationships of
men to animals, the mental faculties of both have been greatly stimulated
and advanced. The least developed races seem to be in such places as Tierra
del Fuego, where there are no savage animals, and, therefore, no
inducement for man to arm and defend himself. The Pygmies of Central
Africa are mighty hunters, otherwise they could not survive. Even the
Esquimaux are masters of the great polar bears and other northern animals.
In the wilds of Africa, where animals have had a terrible struggle for
existence, not only against disagreeable climatic conditions, but all kinds of
fellow-foes as well, we find the nkengos have attained a civilisation that
almost equals that of our savage brothers. And these pale-faced little beings,
with their wrinkled, care-worn, parchment-like skins, remind one of ill-
treated, white, human-dwarfs. Their name, nkengo, means wild animal-
men, and when tamed they actually make excellent family servants for men.
These closest allies of man live in tall bamboo trees, and are so curiously
human that when seen walking around hunting berries, nuts, and fruits,
talking in guttural, chattering tones, like old fisher-women, no one could
doubt even their kinship to man.
Their children assemble in groups to romp and play under the guardianship
of either one of their mothers or grandmothers; while the men forage for
food, and watch for enemies. It is not uncommon to see an aged, half-
decrepit nkengo lying on a bed of sticks in a tall tree. Here he eats only
green leaves and bits of fruit brought him by some kind friend, being far too
weak to hunt for food himself, and furthermore, fearing an attack from his
mortal enemy, the leopard.
If the colony decides to move to other territory, either because of enemies or
the scarcity of food, they all assemble and hold a farewell gathering in
which there is much mourning and apparent grief at forever leaving their
aged kin to the fate of the wilds. If they are possibly able to walk, they are
given patient assistance in travelling along. Sometimes, when they are
deserted, sympathetic friends return for days with berries and koola nuts,
until at last the colony has gone so far away that none dare return alone, in
which event these helpless superannuated members are left to die in their
lone tree-top beds.
Many of these beds are as well made as the tree-beds of human beings, and
even better than the beds of the savage Dyaks of Borneo. They are usually
located in tall trees, inaccessible to leopards and out of reach of their most
dreaded of all enemies, the terrible hordes of war-ants. From these nothing
escapes—not even elephants and tigers.
The arrival of a baby to these nkengos is of far more importance in their
tree-top village, than in a human city. Each of the female relatives, and also
the aged males, takes special interest in the new-comer, and they chatter
around his little grape-vine cradle with much enthusiasm, shaking their
heads and delicately handling his tiny hands and toes as though he were the
baby of a king.
This baby is much stronger and quicker to learn than human babies; for
when he is only two days old he is able to cling to his mother, so that she
can carry him with her on her hunting trips. If he becomes too noisy from
sheer delight when she is travelling through the forest with him, she slaps
him, in an attempt to quiet him, lest the leopards get him.
At night he sleeps snugly by his mother's side in the great tree-bed, and she
never allows him to crawl out of her arms for fear that he fall to the depths
below. She loves him dearly, and watches with human eagerness for his first
tooth. He loves his mother and will stand for hours while she dresses his
hair; or lie on her breast as she rubs his little back.
These wild-children are always ill-tempered and self-willed. No human
mother has to show more patience and love than does the nkengo mother.
She takes the greatest delight in his first efforts at climbing and hunting, and
for hours she and his admiring relatives will watch him attempting to climb
a cocoanut tree. Sometimes she will climb just behind him to catch him if
he falls or becomes frightened.
His arms soon become very powerful, for he is constantly swinging,
climbing, and exercising by hanging from a bough with one hand while he
pulls himself up with the great power of his muscles. He is able to gather
koola nuts long before his jaws are strong enough to crack them; so his fond
mother cracks them for him until his hands and mouth are stronger. Like all
babies, his ambition is to be big and strong like his father.
Some of the apes are most intelligent and human, and, as allies to man, are
more desirable than certain of the human savages. Dr. Livingstone, in his
Last Journals, describes one he first discovered. "Their teeth," he says, "are
slightly human, but their canines show the beast by their large development.
The hands, or rather the fingers, are like those of the natives. They live in
communities consisting of about a dozen individuals, and are strictly
monogamous in their conjugal relations, and vegetarian, or rather
frugivorous, in their diet, their favourite food being bananas." The natives
where these apes live are cannibals, and Dr. Livingstone says, "they are the
lowest of the low." One of their number, who had committed a great
murder, offered his grandmother "to be killed in expiation of his offence,
and this vicarious punishment was accepted as satisfactory."
Thus it is evident that certain of these wild-creatures—like the sokos—have
a more correct conception of justice than their human associates, the
savages. At least the animals do not make the innocent suffer for the guilty,
and give their lives unjustly. Should a soko try to take another's wife he is
publicly punished by the tribe. These animals have a great sense of humour
and fully enjoy a practical joke. Strangely enough, they never attack women
and children, but if any man approaches them with a spear or gun, they try
to rush upon him, often at the expense of their own life, and wrest the
weapon from him. Most of them are exceedingly kind and civilised in their
actions, and natives always say, "Soko is a man, and nothing bad in him."
Often they kidnap babies and carry them up into trees. But these are never
harmed and the apes are ever ready to exchange them for bananas. The
robbery is, no doubt, for the purpose of extortion. If perchance one of their
children is stolen, the entire forest sets up a scream and wail until it is
returned. Old hunters and travellers say that they would rather steal the
child of a native savage than to take one of the sokos. If one of the soko
children disappears, and they do not know what became of it, they
immediately send out detectives throughout the country to seek for it. And
woe be the home where a stolen soko baby is found!
But man has one great power—a far more potent ally than he has in his
animal friends—the use of fire. Unquestionably to the minds of animals it is
a supernatural power. They cannot create it, understand it, and it is very
doubtful if they can yet use it to advantage. How marvellous is this thing—
fire! That great blazing pillar of cloud that destroys all, and leaves nothing
to show where it has taken its enemies! To animals it springs up wherever
man rests his head, and protects him while he sleeps. It is always with him,
and its presence for untold ages has brought terror to all of them.
Not a few reports tell us that certain of our animal allies among the
monkeyfolk of South Africa use fire. This may not be true; but it is
probable that the time is near at hand when the wild baboon-men of the
woods will learn to make and use fire just as we have done.
Enough instances could be shown illustrating animals as man's allies to fill
an entire book, but a sufficient number have been adduced to show how
truly they are our allies, helpers, and protectors just as we are theirs, only
their mode of manifesting it is different. We have shown the absolute
fallacy of the old belief that animals lack mentality, and that all their acts of
kindness are based upon self-love and personal gain, and have seen that in
proportion to their opportunities in life, they have quite as much mentality
and brotherly love for each other and mankind as is found among our lower
savages. We have seen that among animals as among men, individuals will
give their lives for their fellows, serve the weak and timid, and demonstrate
the highest and holiest feelings of which true souls can be capable, and
always share equally with man the burdens that fall upon themselves and
their human allies. And the time is already here when man should protect
his animal friends more, and teach them through human kindness not to fear
him. But this can only be done when he is willing to treat them as fellow
beings only a little below him in the scale of existence.
CHAPTER XV
THE FUTURE LIFE OF ANIMALS
To the readers of this book who furnish name and address (a postal card will
do), we will gladly send, free of charge, announcements of our new
publications. Our illustrated holiday pamphlets with colored picture covers
are unusually attractive. Books may then be ordered through your local
bookshop.
We employ no agents or canvassers
FREDERICK A. STOKES COMPANY
447-448 Fourth Avenue, New York, N. Y.
*** END OF THE PROJECT GUTENBERG EBOOK THE HUMAN
SIDE OF ANIMALS ***
Updated editions will replace the previous one—the old editions will be
renamed.
Creating the works from print editions not protected by U.S. copyright law
means that no one owns a United States copyright in these works, so the
Foundation (and you!) can copy and distribute it in the United States
without permission and without paying copyright royalties. Special rules,
set forth in the General Terms of Use part of this license, apply to copying
and distributing Project Gutenberg™ electronic works to protect the
PROJECT GUTENBERG™ concept and trademark. Project Gutenberg is a
registered trademark, and may not be used if you charge for an eBook,
except by following the terms of the trademark license, including paying
royalties for use of the Project Gutenberg trademark. If you do not charge
anything for copies of this eBook, complying with the trademark license is
very easy. You may use this eBook for nearly any purpose such as creation
of derivative works, reports, performances and research. Project Gutenberg
eBooks may be modified and printed and given away—you may do
practically ANYTHING in the United States with eBooks not protected by
U.S. copyright law. Redistribution is subject to the trademark license,
especially commercial redistribution.
1.D. The copyright laws of the place where you are located also govern
what you can do with this work. Copyright laws in most countries are in a
constant state of change. If you are outside the United States, check the
laws of your country in addition to the terms of this agreement before
downloading, copying, displaying, performing, distributing or creating
derivative works based on this work or any other Project Gutenberg™
work. The Foundation makes no representations concerning the copyright
status of any work in any country other than the United States.
1.E.1. The following sentence, with active links to, or other immediate
access to, the full Project Gutenberg™ License must appear prominently
whenever any copy of a Project Gutenberg™ work (any work on which the
phrase “Project Gutenberg” appears, or with which the phrase “Project
Gutenberg” is associated) is accessed, displayed, performed, viewed, copied
or distributed:
This eBook is for the use of anyone anywhere in the United States and
most other parts of the world at no cost and with almost no restrictions
whatsoever. You may copy it, give it away or re-use it under the terms
of the Project Gutenberg License included with this eBook or online at
www.gutenberg.org. If you are not located in the United States, you
will have to check the laws of the country where you are located
before using this eBook.
1.E.2. If an individual Project Gutenberg™ electronic work is derived from
texts not protected by U.S. copyright law (does not contain a notice
indicating that it is posted with permission of the copyright holder), the
work can be copied and distributed to anyone in the United States without
paying any fees or charges. If you are redistributing or providing access to a
work with the phrase “Project Gutenberg” associated with or appearing on
the work, you must comply either with the requirements of paragraphs
1.E.1 through 1.E.7 or obtain permission for the use of the work and the
Project Gutenberg™ trademark as set forth in paragraphs 1.E.8 or 1.E.9.
1.E.6. You may convert to and distribute this work in any binary,
compressed, marked up, nonproprietary or proprietary form, including any
word processing or hypertext form. However, if you provide access to or
distribute copies of a Project Gutenberg™ work in a format other than
“Plain Vanilla ASCII” or other format used in the official version posted on
the official Project Gutenberg™ website (www.gutenberg.org), you must, at
no additional cost, fee or expense to the user, provide a copy, a means of
exporting a copy, or a means of obtaining a copy upon request, of the work
in its original “Plain Vanilla ASCII” or other form. Any alternate format
must include the full Project Gutenberg™ License as specified in paragraph
1.E.1.
1.E.7. Do not charge a fee for access to, viewing, displaying, performing,
copying or distributing any Project Gutenberg™ works unless you comply
with paragraph 1.E.8 or 1.E.9.
1.E.8. You may charge a reasonable fee for copies of or providing access to
or distributing Project Gutenberg™ electronic works provided that:
• You pay a royalty fee of 20% of the gross profits you derive from the
use of Project Gutenberg™ works calculated using the method you
already use to calculate your applicable taxes. The fee is owed to the
owner of the Project Gutenberg™ trademark, but he has agreed to
donate royalties under this paragraph to the Project Gutenberg Literary
Archive Foundation. Royalty payments must be paid within 60 days
following each date on which you prepare (or are legally required to
prepare) your periodic tax returns. Royalty payments should be clearly
marked as such and sent to the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive
Foundation at the address specified in Section 4, “Information about
donations to the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation.”
• You provide a full refund of any money paid by a user who notifies
you in writing (or by e-mail) within 30 days of receipt that s/he does
not agree to the terms of the full Project Gutenberg™ License. You
must require such a user to return or destroy all copies of the works
possessed in a physical medium and discontinue all use of and all
access to other copies of Project Gutenberg™ works.
• You comply with all other terms of this agreement for free distribution
of Project Gutenberg™ works.
1.F.
1.F.4. Except for the limited right of replacement or refund set forth in
paragraph 1.F.3, this work is provided to you ‘AS-IS’, WITH NO OTHER
WARRANTIES OF ANY KIND, EXPRESS OR IMPLIED, INCLUDING
BUT NOT LIMITED TO WARRANTIES OF MERCHANTABILITY OR
FITNESS FOR ANY PURPOSE.
1.F.6. INDEMNITY - You agree to indemnify and hold the Foundation, the
trademark owner, any agent or employee of the Foundation, anyone
providing copies of Project Gutenberg™ electronic works in accordance
with this agreement, and any volunteers associated with the production,
promotion and distribution of Project Gutenberg™ electronic works,
harmless from all liability, costs and expenses, including legal fees, that
arise directly or indirectly from any of the following which you do or cause
to occur: (a) distribution of this or any Project Gutenberg™ work, (b)
alteration, modification, or additions or deletions to any Project
Gutenberg™ work, and (c) any Defect you cause.
The Foundation’s business office is located at 809 North 1500 West, Salt
Lake City, UT 84116, (801) 596-1887. Email contact links and up to date
contact information can be found at the Foundation’s website and official
page at www.gutenberg.org/contact
While we cannot and do not solicit contributions from states where we have
not met the solicitation requirements, we know of no prohibition against
accepting unsolicited donations from donors in such states who approach us
with offers to donate.
Please check the Project Gutenberg web pages for current donation methods
and addresses. Donations are accepted in a number of other ways including
checks, online payments and credit card donations. To donate, please visit:
www.gutenberg.org/donate.
Project Gutenberg™ eBooks are often created from several printed editions,
all of which are confirmed as not protected by copyright in the U.S. unless a
copyright notice is included. Thus, we do not necessarily keep eBooks in
compliance with any particular paper edition.
Most people start at our website which has the main PG search facility:
www.gutenberg.org.