Notes
Notes
Unit 1
INTRODUCTION
● Cognition: Area within psychology that examines how we acquire, store, transform and
use knowledge. (Matlin, 2013).
● Cognitive Psychology is part of an active interdisciplinary area known as cognitive
science. A branch of Psychology which involves scintific study of cognition.
● Cog Psy influences other areas like clinical, social, educational etc.
● Cognitive approach/perspective
● Prejudice: psychoanalytic, behavioral, cognitive approach
● Central in human life
ORIGINS OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY (margret textbook, pg 4 to 6)
● Mother discipline of Psychology: Philosophy
● Early Greek Philosophers: Aristotle: perception, memory, imagery and how these
processes & acquire knowledge.
● In 1868 a Dutch psychologist Donders did first cognitive experiment (Reaction time).
Perceiving light. Press L or R when light was illuminated
● Hermann Ebbinghaus (1850–1909), was the first person to scientifically study human
memory (Baddeley et al., 2009; Schwartz, 2011). Ebbinghaus examined a variety of
factors that might influence performance, such as the amount of time between two
presentations of a list of items. He frequently chose nonsense syllables (e.g., DAX),
rather than actual words. This precaution reduced people’s previous experience with the
material. He chose nonsense syllables because he did not want
meaning to shade his results. He assumed that meaningful
syllables would be more memorable
● Meanwhile, in the United States, similar research was being conducted by psychologists
such as Mary Whiton Calkins (1863–1930). For example, Calkins reported a memory
phenomenon called the recency effect (Schwartz, 2011). The recency effect refers to the
observation that our recall is especially accurate for the final items in a series of stimuli.
In addition, Calkins emphasized that psychologists should study how real people use their
cognitive processes in the real world, as opposed to the psychology laboratory. USA:
Calkins: (1863-1930) Recency effect. First woman president of APA.
● https://www.mindtools.com/pages/article/forgetting-curve.htm#:~:text=The%20Forgettin
g%20Curve%20is%20an,learned%20sooner%20rather%20than%20later.
Cognitive psychologists generally agree that the birth of cognitive psychology can be listed as
1956 (Eysenck & Keane, 2010; G. Mandler, 2002; Thagard, 2005). During this prolific year,
researchers published numerous influential books and articles on attention, memory, language,
concept formation, and problem solving. By the 1960s, the methodology, approach, and attitudes
had changed substantially (Shiraev, 2011).
Many researchers interested in memory had shifted from animal learning to human memory
(Baddeley et al., 2009; Bower, 2008) It was difficult to explain complex human behavior using
only behaviorist concepts such as observable stimuli, responses, and reinforcement (G. Mandler,
2002; Neisser,1967). The behaviorist approach tells us nothing about numerous psychologically
interesting processes, such as the thoughts and strategies that people use when they try to solve a
problem (Bechtel et al., 1998).
● Emergence -
● Mid 20th century: Dissatisfaction with behaviorism. According to the principles of
behaviorism, psychology must focus on objective, observable reactions to stimuli in the
environment, rather than introspection.
● Behaviorists also argued that researchers could not objectively study mental
representations, such as an image, idea, or thought.
● behaviorists emphasized the importance of the operational definition, a precise definition
that specifies exactly how a concept is to be measured.
● Similarly, cognitive psychologists in the 21st century need to specify exactly how
memory, perception, and other cognitive processes will be measured in an experiment.
● Behaviorists also valued carefully controlled research, a tradition that is maintained in
current cognitive research (Fuchs & Milar, 2003).
● Noam Chomsky: said that Inborn capacity to master complicated aspects of language.
Criticized behaviorism. Noam Chomsky (1957), who emphasized that the structure of
language was too complex to be explained in behaviorist terms (Pickren & Rutherford,
2010; Pinker, 2002). Chomsky and other linguists argued that humans have an inborn
ability to master all the complicated and varied aspects of language.
● Piaget Swiss psychologist: According to Piaget, children actively explore their world in
order to understand important concepts (Gregory, 2004b). Children’s cognitive strategies
change as they mature, and adolescents often use sophisticated strategies in order to
conduct experiments about scientific principles.
● Info processing approach: 1960s: info passes through cognitive sys in a series of
stages/steps. This information-processing approach argued that
● (a) our mental processes are similar to the operations of a computer, and
● (b) information progresses through our cognitive system in a series of stages, one step at
a time. (Gallistel & King, 2009; Leahey, 2003; MacKay, 2004).
● Atkinson & Shiffrin model. Richard Atkinson and Richard Shiffrin (1968) developed an
information processing model that became extremely popular within the emerging field
of cognitive psychology (Baddeley et al., 2009; Rose, 2004).
● External stimuli from the environment first enter sensory memory. Sensory memory is a
storage system that records information from each of the senses with reasonable accuracy
(Schwartz, 2011)
● The model proposed that information is stored in sensory memory for 2 seconds or less,
and then most of it is forgotten. For example, your auditory memory briefly stores the last
words of a sentence spoken by your professor, but this memory disappears within about 2
seconds. Atkinson and Shiffrin’s model proposed that some material from sensory
Ecological Validity - measures how generalizable experimental findings are to the real world, such as situations or
settings typical of everyday life
memory then passes on to short-term memory.
● Long-term memory has an enormous capacity because it contains memories that are
decades old, in addition to memories of events that occurred several minutes ago.
● Atkinson and Shiffrin’s model proposed that information stored in long-term memory is
relatively permanent, compared to the information stored in working memory
Current/contemporary areas
● Cognitive Neuroscience: Brain lesions, EEG, MRI, FMRI, PET, ERP.
● Cognitive neuroscience combines the research techniques of cognitive psychology with
various methods for assessing the structure and function of the brain (Marshall, 2009).
● In humans, the term brain lesions refers to the destruction of an area in the brain, most
often by strokes, tumors, blows to the head, and accidents. The study of brain lesions has
definitely helped us understand the organization of the brain. However, the results are
often difficult to interpret. For example, a brain lesion is not limited to just one specific
area. As a result, researchers typically cannot associate a cognitive deficit with a specific
brain structure.
● In a positron emission tomography (PET scan), researchers measure blood flow in the
brain by injecting the participant with a low dose of a radioactive chemical just before this
person works on a cognitive task. This chemical travels through the bloodstream to the
parts of the brain that are activated during the tasks. While the person works on the task, a
special camera makes an image of the accumulated radioactive chemical in various regions
of the brain.PET scans can be used to study such cognitive processes as attention, memory,
and language. PET scans require several seconds to produce data, so this method is not
very precise.
● ERP: fast, precise, electrode cap, group of neurons, bad spatial resolution; the
event-related potential (ERP) technique records the very brief fluctuations in the brain’s
electrical activity, in response to a stimulus such as an auditory tone. The ERP technique
provides a reasonably precise picture about changes in the brain’s electrical potential while
people perform a cognitive task.
● (Pg 17 to 21)
● AI: language, problem solving, decision making face recognition etc; computational
metaphor; computer simulation, emotion recognition/expression.
● BCI
● consciousness:
● Consciousness means the awareness that people have about the outside world and about
their perceptions, images, thoughts, memories, and feelings (Chalmers, 2007; Revonsuo,
2010; Zeman, 2004). In recent years, cognitive psychologists have been especially
interested in three interrelated issues concerned with consciousness: (1) our inability to
bring certain thoughts into consciousness; (2) our inability to let certain thoughts escape
from consciousness; and (3) blindsight, which reveals that people with a specific visual
disorder can perform quite accurately on a cognitive task, even when they are not
conscious of their accuracy.
● A more general phenomenon, called mind wandering, occurs when your thoughts shift
from the external environment in favor of internal processing (Barron et al., 2011; McVay
& Kane, 2010; Smilek et al., 2010). Again, you may not be conscious that your mind has
wandered to another topic.
● Altered states, Inattentional blindness, Change blindness,
Absent-mindedness,Mindwandering,daydreaming,Impairment of
consciousness....vegetative state, amnesia, coma, blindsight
● Imagery
● User experience
● Social navigation.
Top-down processing
● Top down processing emphasizes how a person’s concepts, expectations, and memory
can influence object recognition.
● This top-down process then combined together with the specific physical information
about the stimulus from bottom-up processing. As a result, you could quickly and
seamlessly identify the object
● Cognitive psychologists believe that both bottom-up and top-down processing are
necessary to explain the complexities of object recognition (Riddoch & Humphreys,
2001).
● For example, you recognize a coffee cup because of two almost simultaneous processes:
○ (1) Bottom-up processing forces you to register the component features, such as
the curve of the cup’s handle; and
○ (2) the context of a coffee shop encourages you to recognize the handle on the cup
more quickly, because of top-down processing.
CAT - example
● According to the word superiority effect, we can identify a single letter more accurately
and more rapidly when it appears in a meaningful word than when it appears alone by
itself or else in a meaningless string of unrelated letters (Dahan, 2010; Palmer, 2002;
Vecera & Lee, 2010). For instance, you can recognize the letter p more easily if it appears
in a word such as plan than if it appears in a nonword such as pnla.
● The same shape—an ambiguous letter—can sometimes be perceived as an H and
sometimes as an A. In this demonstration, you began to identify the whole word ‘‘THE,’’
and your tentative knowledge of that word helped to identify the second letter as an H.
● One important principle in gestalt psychology is that humans have basic tendencies to
organize what they see; without any effort, we see patterns, rather than random
arrangements (I. E. Gordon, 2004; Schirillo, 2010).
● We distinguish figure from the ground on the basis of the following characteristics:
○ 1. Figure has a definite form, while the background is relatively formless.
○ 2. Figure is more organised as compared to its background.
○ 3. Figure has a clear contour (outline), while the background is contourless.
○ 4. Figure stands out from the background, while the background stays behind the
figure.
○ 5. Figure appears more clear, limited, and relatively nearer, while the background
appears relatively unclear, unlimited, and away from us.
● In an ambiguous figure-ground relationship, the figure and the ground reverse from time
to time.
● At first, you see a white vase against a blue background, but a moment later, you see two
blue faces against a white background. Even in this ambiguous situation, our perceptual
system imposes organization on a stimulus, so that one portion stands out and the
remainder recedes into the background.
● We are so accustomed to the certainty of the figure-ground relationship that we are
surprised when we encounter a situation where the figure and the ground exchange
places.
● In illusory contours (also called subjective contours), we see edges even though they are
not physically present in the stimulus.
Recognizing Faces
● Visual processing: Primary Visual Cortex but for facial recognition- temporal lobe/
inferotemporal cortex.
● The occipital lobe, at the back of your brain, is the location in the part of the cortex that is
responsible for the initial, most basic visual processing. Information then travels from the
occipital lobe to numerous other locations throughout the brain.
● The location most responsible for face recognition is the temporal cortex, at the side of
your brain (Farah, 2004; Kanwisher et al., 2001; Sinha et al., 2010). The specific location
is known as the inferotemporal cortex, in the lower portion of the temporal cortex.
● Researchers have also tested monkeys, using neuroscience recording techniques. They
report that certain cells in the inferotemporal cortex respond especially vigo a photo of
another monkey’s face (Rolls & Tovee, 1995; Wang et al.,1996).
● Farah, 2004; Gazzaniga et al., 2009- Various parts of a person’s face appear independent
of one another, instead of forming a unified, complete face.
Speech Perception
During speech perception, auditory sys records sound vibrations, translates these vibrations into
a sequence of sounds that are perceived as meaningful speech. We must distinguish the sound
pattern of 1 word from tens of thousands of irrelevant words stored in memory. Also, voice of
the speaker has to be separated from background noise (Brown & Sinnot, 2006; Mattys & Liss,
2008)
● Categorical perception: (When a smooth continuum of sounds b/w ‘b’ and ‘p’ was
presented, s’s could not hear a sound part way between a ‘b’ and a ‘p’ but reported
hearing a clear-cut ‘b’ or ‘p’) but non speech sounds were heard as a smooth continuum
(Liberman & Mattingly, 1989). However new research shows humans also exhibit
categorical perception for non-speech sound (Esgate & Groome, 2005).
● General mechanism approach: speech perception can be explained without any special
phonetic module.
● Humans use the same neural mechanisms to process both speech sounds and non speech
sounds.
● Speech perception is a learned ability and not an innate one.
● ERP studies show adults show the same sequence of shifts in brain’s electrical potential
whether listening to speech or to music (Patel et al., 1998).
● If speech perception can be influenced by visual perception (e.g. Mcgurk effect), we can’t
conclude that a special phonetic module handles all aspects of speech perception.
Attention
Attention: Focusing on an object/stimulus of interest carrying out desired/necessary action while
ignoring other irrelevant stimuli
Ability to actively process specific info while tuning out other details.
● ability to focus on a task
● ability to concentrate
● refers to the allocation of processing resources (Anderson, 1995) (assumes limited
resources)
Types of attention:
● Selective attention - Selective attention is the process of focusing on a particular object
in the environment for a certain period of time. Attention is a limited resource, so
selective attention allows us to tune out unimportant details and focus on what matters.
● Divided attention - In a divided-attention task, you try to pay attention to two or more
simultaneous messages, responding appropriately to each message. In many cases, both
your speed and your accuracy suffer. These problems are especially likely if the tasks are
challenging, for instance, if two people are talking quickly to you at the same time
Divided Attention
● Hyman (2010): College students walk more slowly when they are talking on cell phones.
Furthermore, the research shows that college students read their textbooks significantly
more slowly when they are responding to instant messages.
● (Bowman et al., 2010): Students also earn lower grades when they are tested on the
material they had been reading while multitasking .
● Collet et al. (2009): Reaction times of people talking on handheld phones were about
20% slower than others without cell phone conversations.
● Strayer et al. (2003): In heavy-traffic conditions, people in the hands-free cell phone
group took significantly longer to apply the brakes, compared to those in a control group.
● In further testing, Strayer and his colleagues discovered that the participants who used
cell phones showed a form of inattentional blindness
● Alternating - Alternating attention refers to the ability to switch between tasks; to stop
one task to participate in another and then be able to return to the initial task.
● There are 4 main types of attention that we use in our daily life and they are selective
attention, divided attention, sustained attention, and executive attention.
● Examples include listening carefully to what one is saying amidst a group of people or
among some big noise like talking to a person in a party(cocktail party effect). Talking to
someone on the phone while driving a car – selective attention. When we study, we need
to be awake and attentive to whatever we are reading or trying to learn. – sustained
attention. Jamming with a song playing on the radio while driving a car. – divided
attention.
● You may try to use divided attention, for example, concentrating on both your professor’s
lecture and a nearby whispered conversation between two students. You’ll discover,
though, that you cannot accurately attend to both categories of stimuli simultaneously. In
reality, you are likely to use some form of selective attention, by trying to focus your
attention. The remaining four items in this list require this selective attention
Conjunction search
● When you look at a red circle, you actually analyze its red color separately from its round
shape. In other words, your visual system sometimes has a binding problem because it
does not represent the important features of an object as a unified whole. Focused
attention acts like a form of glue, so that an object’s color and its shape can stick together.
● When you use focused attention to look at the apple, you will accurately perceive an
integrated figure—a red, round object.Focused attention allows the binding process to
operate (Bouvier & Treisman, 2010
● Research shows that our visual system can create an illusory conjunction from verbal
material also (Treisman, 1990; Wolfe, 2000).
● For example: 2 nonsense words are shown, dax and kay. You might report seeing the
English word day. When we cannot use focused attention, we sometimes form illusory
conjunctions that are consistent with our expectations. Top-down processing helps us
screen out inappropriate combinations. As a result, we are more likely to perceive
familiar combinations (Treisman, 1990).
● I. Broadbent’s filter does not allow through unattended messages, whereas Treisman’s
filter allows unattended messages through, but in an attenuated form.
● II. Broadbent’s is a simple single filter model, whereas Treisman’s can be thought of as a
two-stage filtering process: firstly, filtering on the basis of incoming channel
characteristics, and secondly, filtering by the threshold settings of the dictionary
● units.
● III. Both models are “early selection”models, in which selection occurs prior to pattern
recognition.
Late Selection theory:
● Deutsch & Deutsch (1963) model is called a late selection model because they claim that
all information (attended and unattended) is analysed for meaning in order to select an
input for full awareness. Whether or not information is selected is dependent on how
relevant it is at the time.
● More support comes from Moray (1969), who paired electric shocks with a word to
condition a galvanic skin response (GSR) when the word was spoken. A GSR was
produced even when the word was presented to the unattended ear and the participants
were unaware of it.
● In contrast to the early-selection assumption that information is filtered or blocked prior
to recognition, Norman’s theory is that all stimuli are processed for meaning, which
happens in LTM but only the meaning of the attended stimulus reaches awareness.
○ Information that is more pertinent (relevant/important) is most likely to be
selected.
○ Information is kept in short term memory and is quickly forgotten unless it is
pertinent.
● This theory holds that all messages are routinely processed for at least some aspects of
meaning-the selection of which message to respond happens “late” in processing.
MacKay (1973)
❖ Ambiguous sentence in attended ear
❖ “They were throwing rocks at the bank.”
❖ Bias word in unattended ear (money/river)
❖ Result
❖ Bias word influences sentence meaning
❖ Conclusion
❖ Unconscious processing of meaning of unattended information
❖ Support for late selection
Stroop Effect:
● The Stroop effect is named after James R. Stroop (1935), who created this well known
task.
● According to the Stroop effect, people take a long time to name the ink color when that
color is used in printing an incongruent word (100 secs); in contrast, they can quickly
name that same ink color when it appears as a solid patch of color.(60 secs)
● Selective attention: People take longer to pay attention to a color when they are distracted
by another feature of the stimulus, namely, the meaning of the name itself (Styles, 2006).
Researchers have examined a variety of explanations for the Stroop effect.
● Some have suggested that it can be explained by the connectionist or parallel
distributed processing (PDP) approach,
● According to this explanation, the Stroop task activates two pathways at the same time.
One pathway is activated by the task of naming the ink color, and the other pathway is
activated by the task of reading the word. Interference occurs when two competing
pathways are active at the same time. As a result, task performance suffers.
● Another explanation is that adults have had much more practice in reading words than in
naming colors. The more automatic process (reading the word) interferes with the less
automatic process (naming the color of the ink). As a result, we automatically—and
involuntarily—read the words that are printed
● For instance, many clinical psychologists have used a related technique called the
emotional Stroop task (C. MacLeod, 2005; C. M. MacLeod, 2005).
● On the emotional Stroop task, people are instructed to name the ink color of words that
could have strong emotional significance to them. These individuals often require more
time to name the color of the stimuli, presumably because they have trouble ignoring
their emotional reactions to the words themselves (Most, 2010).
● For example, suppose that someone appears to have a phobic disorder, which is an
excessive fear of a specific object. A person with a fear of spiders would be instructed to
name the ink colors of printed words such as hairy and crawl. People with phobias are
significantly slower on these anxiety-arousing words than on control words. In contrast,
people without phobias show no difference between the two kinds of words (Williams et
al., 1996). These results suggest that people who have a phobic disorder are hyper-alert to
words related to their phobia, and they show an attentional bias to the meaning of these
stimuli.
● An attentional bias describes a situation in which people pay extra attention to some
stimuli or some features. In the emotional Stroop task, for example, the participants pay
less attention to the ink color of the words. In addition, adults who showed an attentional
bias toward suicide-related words are more likely than other adults to make a suicide
attempt within the following 6 months (Cha et al., 2010).
Visual Search:
● In visual search, the observer must find a target in a visual display that has numerous
distractors. In some cases, our lives may depend on accurate visual searches.
● For instance, airport security officers search travelers’ luggage for possible weapons, and
radiologists search a mammogram to detect a tumor that could indicate breast cancer.
Examples of visual search:
● Searching for keys on a messy table
● Searching for your wallet
● Searching for tea bags in the supermarket
● Searching for your name on a list of names
● Searching your roll no in competition results
● Searching for your friend in a crowd
Research on Visual Search:
● Wolfe and his colleagues (2005) found that people are much more accurate in identifying
a target if it appears frequently. If the target appears—in a visually complex
background—on 50% of the trials, participants missed the target 7% of the time. When
the same target appeared in this same complex background on only 1% of the trials,
participants missed the target 30% of the time.
● Role of bottom-up & top-down processes (Wolfe, 2010)
Combined/conjunctive feature
The feature-present/feature-absent effect..:
● Treisman and Souther (1985) found that people performed rapid searches for a feature
that was present whether the display contained zero irrelevant items or numerous
irrelevant items. When people are searching for a feature that is present, the target item in
the display usually captures their attention automatically (Franconeri et al., 2005;
Matsumoto, 2010; Wolfe, 2000, 2001). In fact, this ‘‘pop-out’’ effect is automatic, and
researchers emphasize that locating the target is strictly a bottom-up process (Boot et al.,
2005)
● When people search for a feature that is absent, they typically examine every item, one
item at a time. They therefore must use a kind of attention that emphasizes both
bottom-up processing and top-down processing. This task is substantially more
challenging, as Wolfe has also found in his extensive research on the
feature-present/feature-absent effect (Wolfe, 2000, 2001; Wolfe et al. 2009)
● Rapid movement of the eyes from one spot to the next is known as saccadic eye
movement. The purpose of a saccadic eye movement during reading is to bring the center
of your retina into position over the words you want to read. The eye must be moved so
that new words can be registered on the fovea.
● Researchers have estimated that people make between 150,000 and 200,000 saccadic
movements every day (Irwin, 2003). We cannot process much visual information when
your eyes are moving (saccades) but a fixation occurs during the period between two
saccadic movements.
● During each fixation, your visual system pauses briefly in order to acquire information
that is useful for reading. Eye movement while reading is not smooth but the eyes are
actually alternating between jumps and pauses.
● Most of the research has been done on English language reading. In English, each
saccade moves your eye forward by about 7 to 9 letters (Wolfe et al., 2009).
● Few researchers have determined that Chinese readers move their eyes only 2 to 3
characters in a saccade. The reason behind this finding can be the fact that each character
in the Chinese written language is more densely packed with information, compared to
each letter in written English (Rayner, 2009; Shen et al., 2008).
● When the eye jumps forward in a saccadic movement, it usually moves toward the center
of a word, rather than to a blank space between words or between sentences (Engbert &
Krugel, 2010). The eye also jumps past short words, words that appear frequently in a
language, and words that are highly predictable in a sentence
● In contrast, the size of the saccadic movement is small if the next word in a sentence is
misspelled or if it is unusual
Characteristics of good readers.:
● Good readers make larger jumps. They are also less likely to make regressions, by
moving their eyes backward to earlier material in the sentence. The good reader also has
shorter pauses before moving onward (Castelhano & Rayner, 2008).
● CConsciousness
Definitions:
● Consciousness is the sum of processes we are aware of.
● Consciousness: “current awareness of external or internal circumstances”
● “the awareness that people have about the outside world and about their perceptions,
images, thoughts, memories, and feelings” (Chalmers, 2007; Revonsuo, 2010; Zeman,
2004).
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Types of consciousness: Block 1995: 4 types:
● i. Monitoring: One’s ability to reflect on one’s own thinking process. Appraisal
of one’s own internal states. (Metacognition)
● ii. Self conscious: It refers to one’s general knowledge about self. Awareness of
self; components, strengths, weaknesses, emotional awareness, body awareness, etc.
● iii. Access:
● Iv. Phenomenal:
● Access Consciousness: In A-conscious state, a person is able to use/retrieve previously
stored info but is not subjectively aware about it. Eg: blind sight, automaticity in driving,
consciousness of animals etc. Eg: A zombie or robot may have A consciousness but not P
conscious.
● During mind wandering, your thoughts shift from the external environment in favor of
internal processing (Barron et al., 2011; McVay & Kane, 2010; Smilek et al., 2010)
without being conscious about it.
● Mind-wandering has been associated with beneficial processes as goal-directed thinking
(Gorgolewski et al., 2014), planning (Baird et al., 2011), and creativity (Baird et al.,
2012).
● On the other hand, it correlates with such costly outcomes as attenuated processing of the
environment (Smallwood et al., 2008), driving accidents (Yanko & Spalek, 2014),
disruptions to learning [Wammes et al, 2016; Seli et al., 2016), affective dysfunction
[Smallwood et al., 2003)], and impaired performance in daily life (Mc Vay et al., 2009).
● There is a lot of inconsistency in defining mind wandering phenomena itself (Seli et al.,
2018)
Research on Thought suppression
● Wegner (1997b, 2002) uses the phrase ironic effects of mental control to describe how
our efforts can backfire when we attempt to control the contents of our consciousness.
● Wegner et al. (1987) instructed one group of students not to think about a white bear
during a 5-minute period, and then they were allowed to think about a white bear during a
second 5-minute period. These students were very likely to think about a white bear
during the second period. In fact, they thought about bears more often than students in a
control group. Students in this control group had been instructed to think freely about a
white bear—without any previous thought-suppression session.
● In other words, initial suppression of specific thoughts can produce a rebound effect
(Wegner et al., 2002; Purdon et al., 2005).
● Furthermore, this rebound effect is not limited to suppressing thoughts about white bears
and other relatively trivial ideas. For example, when people are instructed not to notice a
painful stimulus, they are likely to become even more aware of the pain.
● Similar ironic effects—which occur when we try to suppress our thoughts—have been
documented when people try to concentrate, avoid movement, or fall asleep (Harvey,
2005; Wegner, 1994).
Research on Blindsight
● Most of the information that is registered on the retina travels to the visual cortex.
However, a small portion of this retinal information travels to other locations in the
cerebral cortex that are located outside the visual cortex (Weiskrantz, 2007; Zeman,
2004).
● A person with blindsight can therefore identify some characteristics of the visual
stimulus—even with a damaged primary visual cortex—based on information registered
in those other cortical locations.
Process of remembering
Encoding:
Retrieval
● Retrieval refers to bringing the stored information to her/his awareness so that it can be
used for performing various cognitive tasks such as problem solving or decision-making.
● It may be interesting to note that memory failure can occur at any of these stages.
● You may fail to recall an information because you did not encode it properly, or the
storage was weak so you could not access or retrieve it when required.
● Bringing to mind information that has been stored in memory
● In retrieval, we pull out or use information stored in memory.
Recall
● Recall in memory refers to the mental process of retrieval of information from the past.
Along with encoding and storage, it is one of the three core processes of memory.
● There are three main types of recall: free recall, cued recall and serial recall.
● Psychologists test these forms of recall as a way to study the memory processes of
humans and animals
Types of Recall
Free recall:
● Free recall describes the process in which a person is given a list of items to remember
and then is tested by being asked to recall them in any order.
○ Hardest type of recall
○ Least environmental support
○ Free recall often displays evidence of primacy and recency effects.
○ Primacy effects are displayed when the person recalls items presented at the
beginning of the list earlier and more often.
○ The recency effect is when the person recalls items presented at the end of the list
earlier and more often
Cued recall:
● Sensory Memory: Sensory memory- Short term memory/ WM: Short capacity
memory storage system which receives information from sensory memory and holds it
for short periods of time for further processing
● Long term memory: Memory system in which information is retained for a very long
period of time
Sensory Memory
● External stimuli from the environment first enter sensory memory. Sensory memory is a
storage system that records information from each of the senses with reasonable accuracy
● shortest element of memory. Its the ability to retain impressions of sensory information
(for a brief period of time) after the original stimuli have ended. (Iconic and echoic)
Iconic memory
● It involves the brief persistence of visual impressions that “makes them briefly available
for processing even after the stimulus has been terminated”.
● Whole Method : Sperling flashed an array of letters and numbers on a screen for a mere
50 milliseconds. Participants were asked to report the identity and location of as many of
the symbols as they could recall. 12 items were presented. The subjects felt that they had
stored all 12 items in their memory but most of them were lost at the moment of verbal
report. 4-5 items recalled.
● Partial report- Sperling (1960): Chart is displayed v briefly. S’s instructed to recall one
row items only
● Subjects hear a tone: high pitch: recall I row; moderate pitch: II row; Low pitch: III row
● S’s could report more than 3 items per line and saw between 9-10 items but the image of
these 9 items fades so rapidly that the person can report only 4 or so…
● To estimate the duration of iconic memory, Sperling manipulated the interval between the
display and the tone. The range of the interval was upto 1.0 second after the offset of the
display
● Conclusion: Icon persisted for 200-400 milliseconds i.e less than half a sec. Iconic
memory capacity- abt 9 items
● Half a sec-1 sec
Haptic Memory
https://courses.lumenlearning.com/boundless-psychology/chapter/types-of-memory/
● Sensory receptors all over the body detect sensations like pressure, itching, and pain,
which are briefly held in haptic memory before vanishing or being transported to
short-term memory.
● This type of memory seems to be used when assessing the necessary forces for gripping
and interacting with familiar objects.
● Haptic memory seems to decay after about two seconds. Evidence of haptic memory has
only recently been identified and not as much is known about its characteristics compared
to iconic memory.
● Section summary: Working memory (originally called short-term memory) is the very
brief,immediate memory for material that we are currently processing.
2. In 1956, George Miller proposed that we can hold about seven chunks of information
in short-term memory.
3. The Brown/Peterson & Peterson technique, which prevents rehearsal, shows that
people have only limited recall for items after a brief delay. The recency effect in a
serial-position curve is also used in measuring the limited capacity of short-term memory.
4. Word meaning can also influence the number of items we store in short-term memory;
when the semantic category changes between adjacent trials, our recall for the new
material increases.
5. According to the Atkinson-Shiffrin (1968) model, the items that we store in short-term
memory can be lost within about 30 seconds unless they are repeated; people can use
rehearsal and other control processes to improve their short-term memory.
The original form of this model focused on the role of short-term memory in learning and
memory. This model did not explore how short-term memory plays an important role
when we perform other cognitive tasks (Roediger et al., 2002).
Working memory
● Working memory is the brief, immediate memory for the limited amount of material that
you are currently processing; part of working memory also actively coordinates your
ongoing mental activities.
● In other words, working memory lets you keep a few items active and accessible, so that
you can use them for a wide variety of cognitive tasks (Baddeley, 2007; Baddeley et al.,
2009; Hassin, 2005; Pickering, 2006b).
● In the current research, the term working memory is more popular than a similar but older
term, short-term memory (Schwartz, 2011;Surprenant & Neath, 2009).
● Holds on items for a brief period of time
● The short-term store also has some control processes available that regulate the flow of
information to and from the long-term store, where we may hold information for longer
periods.
● Typically, material remains in the short-term store for about 30 seconds, unless it is
rehearsed to retain it.
● Wickelgren (1975): Information is stored acoustically (by the way it sounds) rather than
visually (by the way it looks).
● Part of the memory system in which info is retained for v long period of time. LTM may
last for days, months, years, lifetime.
● We can design experiments to tax the limits of short-term memory, but we do not know
how to test the limits of long-term memory and thereby find out its capacity.
● Some theorists have suggested that the capacity of long-term memory is infinite, at least
in practical terms (Bahrick, 2000; Brady, 2008).
● Permastore effect: The term refers to the very long-term storage of information, such as
knowledge of a foreign language (Bahrick, 1984a, 1984b; Bahrick et al., 1993) and of
mathematics (Bahrick & Hall, 1991).
● Schmidt and colleagues (2000) studied the permastore effect for names of streets near
one’s childhood homes. Indeed, the author just returned to his childhood home of more
than 40 years ago and perfectly remembered the names of the nearby streets. These
findings indicate that permastore can occur even for information that you have passively
learned.
Types of Memory
● There are a number of different types of long-term memory. two main divisions of LTM
are explicit memory and implicit memory.
● Explicit memory also called conscious memory or declarative memory,
● Episodic memory, memory for personal experiences, and
● Semantic memory, stored knowledge and memory for facts.
● Episodic and semantic memories are illustrated by two memories that Cliff, the student is
experiencing. When he remembers talking with Gail yesterday about meeting to study for
the cognitive psychology exam, he is having an episodic memory.
● When he remembers some facts about theories of attention that he learned in his
cognitive psychology class, he is having a semantic memory.
● Both of these types of memory are called explicit, because their contents can be described
or reported (Smith & Grossman, 2008).
● The other division of long-term memory, implicit memory, Implicit memories also
called nondeclarative memory or unconscious memory are memories that are used
without awareness, so the contents of implicit memories cannot be reported
● One type of implicit memory that has influenced Cliff’s behavior is priming—a change in
response to a stimulus caused by the previous presentation of the same or a similar
stimulus.
● An example of priming would be finding it easier to recognize words that are familiar or
that he has recently seen compared to words that he has rarely encountered.
● Another type of implicit memory is procedural memory, also called skill memory, which
is memory for doing things.
● When Cliff is typing notes into his computer, his ability to type is procedural memory.
Finally, classical conditioning is another form of implicit memory.
● Classical conditioning occurs when pairing an initially neutral stimulus with another
stimulus results in the neutral stimulus taking on new properties. For example, about a
week ago Cliff had a frightening accident in which a red SUV smashed into his car. He
escaped without serious injury, but was emotionally shaken.
● Now, when he sees a red SUV or even red cars, he begins to feel anxious, just as he felt
immediately after the accident. Because of classical conditioning, the previously neutral
cars have taken on new properties.
● Atkinson- Shiffrin: proposed that memory involves a sequence of separate steps; in each
step, information is transferred from one storage area to another.
● External stimuli from the environment first enter sensory memory.
Sensory memory
● Sensory memory is a storage system that records information from each of the senses
with reasonable accuracy (Schwartz, 2011).
● During the 1960s and 1970s, psychologists frequently studied both visual sensory
memory and auditory sensory memory (e.g., Darwin et al., 1972; Parks, 2004; Sperling,
1960).
● The model proposed that information is stored in sensory memory for 2 seconds or less,
and then most of it is forgotten.
● For example, your auditory memory briefly stores the last words of a sentence spoken by
your professor, but this memory disappears within about 2 seconds.
● Atkinson and Shiffrin’s model proposed that some material from sensory memory then
passes on to short-term memory.
Short-term memory
● According to the model, only a fraction of the information in short-term memory passes
on to long-term memory (Leahey, 2003)
● Long-term memory has an enormous capacity because it contains memories that are
decades old, in addition to memories of events that occurred several minutes ago.
● Atkinson and Shiffrin’s model proposed that information stored in long-term memory is
relatively permanent, compared to the information stored in working memory.
● The original form of this model focused on the role of short-term memory in learning and
memory.
● The levels of processing theory holds that memory is not three-staged which separates it
immediately from the stage theory model.
● levels-of-processing approach argues that deep, meaningful processing of information
leads to more accurate recall than shallow, sensory kinds of processing. (This theory is
also called the depth-of-processing approach.)
● The levels-of-processing approach predicts that your recall will be more accurate when
you use a deep level of processing, in terms of meaning.
● In contrast, you will be less likely to recall a word when you consider its physical
appearance (e.g., whether it is typed in capital letters) or its sound (e.g., whether it
rhymes with another word).
● In general, then, people achieve a deeper level of processing when they extract more
meaning from a stimulus.
● Craik and Lockhart argue that stimulus information is processed at multiple levels
simultaneously (not serially) depending on characteristics, attention and meaningfulness.
● New information does not have to enter in any specific order, and it does not have to pass
through a prescribed channel.
● They further contend that the more deeply information is processed, the more that will be
remembered (Kearsley, 2001b).
● Craik and Tulving (1975) performed a typical levels-of-processing investigation.
Participants were presented with a series of questions about particular words. Each word
was preceded by a question, and participants were asked to respond to the questions as
quickly as possible; no mention was made of memory or learning. Any learning that is
not in accord with the participant’s purpose is called incidental learning.
● In one experiment, three kinds of questions were used. One kind asked the participant
whether the word was printed in capital letters. Another asked if the target word rhymed
with another word. The third kind asked if the word fit into a particular sentence (for
example, “The girl placed the _____ on the table”). The three kinds of questions were
meant to induce different kinds of processing. To answer the first kind of question, you
need look only at the typeface (physical processing). To answer the second, you need to
read the word and think about what it sounds like (acoustic processing). To answer the
third, you need to retrieve and evaluate the word’s meaning (semantic processing).
● Presumably, the “depth” of the processing needed is greatest for the third kind of question
and least for the first kind of question. As predicted, Craik and Tulving (1975) found that
on a surprise memory test later, words processed semantically were remembered best,
followed by words processed acoustically.
● Self referenced effect: According to the self reference effect, you will remember more
information if you try to relate that information to yourself (Burns, 2006; Gillihan &
Farah, 2005; Schmidt, 2006). Self reference tasks tend to encourage especially deep
processing.
● Experimental support: Burgess & Weaver (2003):
○ Showed participants photos of faces and asked them questions about the persons
of the photo to induce either deep or shallow processing.
○ Faces that were deeply processed were better recognized on a subsequent test than
those that were studied at a lower level of processing.
○ Meta-memory condition (do you think you’ll be able to remember this face?)
■ Personality (What do you think about the personality? Honest?)
■ Self referenced (Would you like to talk/study with/go out etc.)
■ Physical features (Does this face has a big nose?)
■ Following instructions about the levels of processing manipulation,
students saw the study set of 80 faces at the rate of 8 sec. per face. They
were then tested on the recognition set of 160 faces at the rate of 6.5 sec.
per face.
● Working memory model: Baddley
○ The term working memory is often used interchangeably with short-term memory,
although technically working memory refers more to the whole theoretical
framework of structures and processes used for the temporary storage and
manipulation of information, of which short-term memory is just one component.
○ WM is not a unitary storage concept. Like STM, it has a limited capacity sys
containing transient info.
○ Unlike STM, function of WM is less a matter of storage pathway to LTM than of
holding info used for other cognitive work.
○ WM is a part of many imp activities like problem solving, reasoning,
comprehension etc.
○ Its an active sys which constantly handles, combines and transforms material
drawn from sensory memory and LTM
Goldstein
Central Executive:
● The central executive is where the major work of working memory occurs.
● The central executive pulls information from long term memory and coordinates the
activity of the phonological loop and visuospatial sketch pad by focusing on specific
parts of a task and switching attention from one part to another.
● One of the main jobs of the central executive is to decide how to divide attention between
different tasks.
● For example, imagine you are driving in a strange city, and a friend in the passenger seat
is reading your directions to a restaurant while the news is being broadcast on the car
radio.
● As your phonological loop takes in the verbal directions, your sketch pad is helping you
visualize a map of the streets leading to the restaurant , and the central executive is
coordinating and combining these two kinds of information.
● In addition, the central executive might be helping you ignore the messages from the
radio, so you can focus your attention on the directions.
● The visuospatial sketch pad handles visual and spatial information and is therefore
involved in the process of visual imagery—the creation of visual images in the mind in
the absence of a physical visual stimulus. The following demonstration illustrates an
early visual imagery experiment by Roger Shepard and J. Metzler (1971).
● When Shepard and Metzler measured participants’ reaction time to decide whether pairs
of objects were the same or different, they obtained the relationship shown in ● Figure
5.19 for objects that were the same. From this function, we can see that when two shapes
were separated by an orientation difference of 40 degrees
● (like Figure 5.18a), it took 2 seconds to decide that a pair was the same shape, but for a
difference of 140 degrees (like Figure 5.18b), it took 4 seconds. Based on this finding that
reaction times were longer for greater differences in orientation, Shepard and Metzler
inferred that participants were solving the problem by rotating an image of one of the
objects in their mind, a phenomenon called mental rotation. This mental rotation is an
example of the operation of the visuospatial sketch pad because it involves visual rotation
through space.
● Lee Brooks (1968) did some experiments in which he demonstrated how interference can
affect the operation of the visuospatial sketch pad. The following demonstration is based
on one of Brooks’s tasks.
● Most people find that the pointing task is more diffi cult. The reason is that holding the
image of the letter and pointing are both visuospatial tasks, so the visuospatial sketch pad
becomes overloaded. In contrast, saying “Out” or “In” is an articulatory task that is
handled by the phonological loop, so speaking didn’t interfere with visualizing the F.
Phonological loop: It briefly holds inner speech for verbal comprehension and for acoustic
rehearsal.
● The phonological similarity effect is the confusion of letters or words that sound similar.
● Remember Conrad’s experiment, shows that in a memory test people often confuse
similar sounding letters, such as “F” and “S.”
● Conrad interpreted this result to support the idea of auditory coding in STM. In
present-day terminology,
● Conrad’s result would be described as a demonstration of the phonological similarity
effect, which occurs when words are processed in the phonological store part of the
phonological loop.
● Memory suffers for similar items because they are confused with one another.
Episodic buffer:
● According to the PDP model, the key to knowledge representation lies in the connections
among various nodes, or elements, stored in memory, (Feldman & Shastri, 2003).
● Activation of one node may prompt activation of a connected node. This process of
spreading activation may prompt the activation of additional nodes
● In this model, activation spreads through nodes within the network.
● A prime is a node that activates a connected node. A priming effect is the resulting
activation of the node.
● Some evidence supports the notion that priming is due to spreading activation. But not
everyone agrees about the mechanism for the priming effect.
● Working memory comprises the activated portion of long-term memory and operates
through at least some amount of parallel processing.
● Spreading activation involves the simultaneous (parallel) activation (priming) of multiple
links among nodes within the network.
● Meyer & Schvanveldt (1971): performed a series of experiments that elaborated the
semantic network model. If related words are stored close by one another and are
connected to one another in a semantic network, then whenever one node is activated or
energized, energy spreads to the related nodes. Participants saw 2 words at a time, and
had to decide if both strings were words or not..if one of the string was a real word
(bread), participants were faster to respond if the other string was a semantically
associated word (butter) than if it was an unrelated word (chair) or a non-word (rencle).
This was called spreading activation. (excitation spreads along the connections of nodes
Memory Processes
● The procedure is actually quite simple. First, you arrange items into different groups. Of
course one pile may be sufficient, depending on how much there is to do. If you have to
go somewhere else due to lack of facilities that is the next step; otherwise, you are pretty
well set. It is important not to overdo things. That is, it is better to do too few things at
once than too many. In the short run this may not seem important but complications can
easily arise. A mistake can be expensive as well. At first, the whole procedure will seem
complicated. Soon, however, it will become just another facet of life. It is difficult to
foresee any end to the necessity for this task in the immediate future, but then, one can
never tell. After the procedure is completed one arranges the materials into different
groups again. Then they can be put into their appropriate places. Eventually they will be
used once more and the whole cycle will then have to be repeated. However, that is part
of life. (Bransford & Johnson, 1972-recall the steps)
● Gross & Eagle (1970): S’s learned a list of 41 words. Had to complete a recognition test
having distractors after 5 mins. 9 distractors-semantically related. 9 not. S’s falsely
recognized an avg of 1.83 synonyms but only an avg of 1.03 unrelated words
● Bousfield (1953): showed 60 words including 15 (animals, professions, names,
vegetables). Free recall was taken.Participants tended to recall more from same category
more frequently
● Levels of processing model: Also shows semantic coding to reach LTM. Indication:
Semantic coding in LTM
● Visual as well as semantic coding:
● Frost (1972: )Participants in a study received 16 drawings of objects, including 4 (items
of clothing, animals, vehicles, and items of furniture
● The investigator manipulated not only the semantic category but also the visual category.
The drawings differed in visual orientation. Four were angled to the left, four angled to
the right, four horizontal, and four vertical.
● Items were presented in random order. Participants were asked to recall them freely. The
order of participants’ responses showed effects of both semantic and visual categories.
These results suggested that participants were encoding visual as well as semantic
information.
Decay
● The inability to retrieve a memory is one of the most common causes of forgetting.
● One possible explanation of retrieval failure is known as decay theory.
● According to this theory, a memory trace is created every time a new theory is formed.
Decay theory suggests that over time, these memory traces begin to fade and disappear. If
the information is not retrieved and rehearsed, it will eventually be lost.
● One problem with this theory, however, is that research has demonstrated that even
memories which have not been rehearsed or remembered are remarkably stable in
long-term memory
● Research also suggests that the brain actively prunes memories that become unused, a
process that is known as active forgetting. As memories accumulate, those that are not
retrieved eventually become lost
Interference: RI, PI
Failure in consolidation
● Losing information has less to do with forgetting and more to do with the fact that it
never made it into long-term memory in the first place.
● Encoding failures sometimes prevent information from entering long-term memory.
Retrieval
● Sternberg (1966): Participants were shown a short (one to six items) list of numbers and
asked to memorize them. After putting them to memory, a probe number was shown.
● For eg. 4,1,9,3,5,2: probe item: 9
● The probe number was either one of the numbers in the list or a new number. The
participant was to respond as quickly as possible, indicating whether the probe number
was in the list or not (Y/N). The response time of the participant should reflect the time
spent searching STM to determine whether the probe number is part of the list. By
varying the number of items in the list, Sternberg hypothesized that he could test several
theories of STM search.
● If memory search requires consideration of each item in succession – a serial search – the
response times should increase with memory set size because the participant will, on
average, have to search through more items for larger set sizes.
● In contrast to a serial search, it is possible that there is a parallel search. If such a search
took place in STM, the prediction is that response times would not vary as the memory
set size increased.
● Sternberg's data were consistent with the successive or serial search.
● Specifically, he found that response times grew linearly with increases in memory set
size. For each additional item in the memory set, participants took (on average) an
additional 38 ms to make their responses. Thus, it seems the probe item is compared
one-by-one with each item in STM, and each comparison takes approximately 38
milliseconds.
● When he compared response times for probe "Present" and "Absent" trials (probe item
was in the memory set or not, respectively), Sternberg found no differences in response
times.
● An "Absent" response can be made only after all items in STM have been searched and
found not to match the probe item. At first glance, it seems that a "Present" trial could
terminate as soon as the probe item is matched with the appropriate item in STM. With a
self-terminating search, one would expect "Present" trials to be faster, but the data
contradict this hypothesis.
● The counterintuitive finding from Sternberg's study is that search of STM is serial
exhaustive
● Associative learning: When you learn something new about a new kind of stimulus (that
is, an extra stimulus).
● Non-associative learning is when you're not pairing a stimulus with a behavior.
Non-associative learning can be either habituation or sensitization.
● Habituation is when repeated exposure to a stimulus decreases an organism's
responsiveness to the stimulus. Eg:to noise, to pollution
● Sensitization is kind of the opposite. It's learning that occurs when stimulus is repeated,
and each time your response to it increases as it goes on and on. So what's an example of
sensitization in real life? Eg: aggression/frustration increases with time….
● EXPLICIT MEMORY
○ Declarative memory: Episodic
○ Tulving: Episodic memory involves ‘mental time travel’. However, the recall is
not necessarily accurate
○ Tulving describes the experience of episodic memory as ‘self knowing’ or
‘remembering’.
○ Declarative memory: Semantic
○ Tulving: Exp of semantic memory involves accessing knowledge abt the world
which can be facts, vocabulary, numbers and concepts.
○ Tulving describes the experience of semantic memory as ‘knowing’ without time
travel
○ Rosenbaum et al. (2005): Case of K.C., age 30 yrs. Suffered severe damage to
hippocampus after an accident. Lost episodic memory but not semantic. Eg: he
remembered 2 yrs ago his brother died but he didn’t remember any event/episode
related to it. When and how he heard the news, what happened then etc.
○ Levine et al. (2004): had participants keep diaries of audiotaped descriptions of
everyday events (“It was the last night of our Salsa dance class. . . . People were
dancing all different styles of Salsa. . . .”), and facts drawn from their world
knowledge (“By 1947, there were 5,000 Japanese Canadians living in
Toronto”).
○ When the participants later listened to these descriptions while in an MRI scanner,
the everyday experiences elicited retrieval of episodic memories, and the facts
elicited retrieval of semantic memories.
○ Conclusion: Retrieving episodic and semantic memories causes overlapping but
different patterns of brain activity.
○ Other research comparing brain activity during episodic and semantic retrieval
has also found differences between the areas activated by episodic and semantic
memory (Cabeza & Nyberg, 2000)
● Connections between episodic and semantic memories
○ Episodic memory can be lost, leaving only semantic:
○ “Morphing” from episodic to semantic memory can occur for personal
experiences. Eg: highschool farewell; episodic details can fade over time and only
semantic information can remain
○ Eg: year of highschool, institution name etc.
○ Semantic Memory Can Be Enhanced If Associated With Episodic Memory:
○ personal semantic memories, are easier to remember than semantic memories that
are not personally significant. Eg. knowledge about the facts associated with your
high school would be personal semantic memories
○ Westmacott and Moscovitch (2003) showed that participants have better recall for
names of public figures such as actors, singers, and politicians whom they
associated with personal experiences. For example, you would be more likely to
recall the name of a popular singer in a memory test if you had attended one of his
or her concerts
○ Semantic Memory Can Influence Our Experience (episodic memory) by
Influencing Attention:
○ Eg: If you have sound knowledge about a subject, you will be able to pick more
from an event related to that subject. Eg: You did a dissertation on a topic. Later
in a conference, you are able to attend related studies but your friend is not. And
later you can give a rich description of the whole episode
○ Eg: A football player is able to pay more attention to the details of game played as
compared to a non-player
○ Method of loci
Unit 3- Margret + PP
● Mental Imagery (Imagery) - mental representation of stimuli when those stimuli are not
physically present (Kosslyn et al.,2010).
● Imagery relies exclusively on top-down processing because sensory receptors do not
receive an input when you create a mental image.
● Diff. b/w Imagery and Perception:
○ Perception uses previous knowledge to gather and interpret the stimuli registered
by the senses. It requires you to register information through the receptors in your
sensory organs, such as your eyes and ears.
○ As a result, perception requires both bottom-up and top-down processing.
○ Visual Imagery is the most common form of imagery.
TYPES OF IMAGERY -
HISTORY OF MENTAL IMAGERY - Wundt proposed that images were one of the
three basic elements of consciousness, along with sensations and feelings. He also
proposed that because images accompany thought, studying images was a way of
studying thinking.
● This idea of a link between imagery and thinking gave rise to the imageless
thought debate, with some psychologists taking up Aristotle’s idea that “thought is
impossible without an image,” and others contending that thinking can occur
without images.
● Evidence supporting the idea that imagery was not required for thinking was
Francis Galton’s (1883) observation that people who had great difficulty forming
visual images were still quite capable of thinking.
● Other arguments both for and against the idea that images are necessary for
thinking were proposed in the late 1800s and early 1900s, but these arguments
and counterarguments ended when behaviorism toppled imagery from its central
place in psychology
● The behaviorists branded the study of imagery as unproductive because visual
images are invisible to everyone except the person experiencing them. The
founder of behaviorism, John Watson, described images as “unproven” and
“mythological” (1928), and therefore not worthy of study.
● The dominance of behaviorism from the 1920s through the 1950s pushed the
study of imagery out of mainstream psychology.
● Cognitive Revolution: (1950s and 1960s) cognitive psychologists developed ways
to measure behavior that could be used to infer cognitive processes.
● Alan Paivio’s (1963) showed that it was easier to remember concrete nouns, like
truck or tree, that can be imaged, than it is to remember abstract nouns, like truth
or justice, that are difficult to image.
● One technique Paivio used was paired-associate learning. participants are
presented with pairs of words, like boat–hat or car–house, during a study period.
They are then presented, during the test period, with the first word from each pair.
Their task is to recall the word that was paired with it during the study period.
Thus, if they were presented with the word boat, the correct response would be
hat. -
○ To explain this result, Paivio proposed the conceptual peg hypothesis.
According to this hypothesis, concrete nouns create images that other
words can “hang onto.” (For example, if presenting the pair boat-hat
creates an image of a boat, then presenting the word boat later will bring
back the boat image, which provides a number of places on which
participants can place the hat in their mind )
○ Roger Shepard and J. Metzler (1971) inferred cognitive processes by
using mental chronometry, determining the amount of time needed to
carry out various cognitive tasks. Their study was one of the first to apply
quantitative methods to the study of imagery and to suggest that
imagery and perception may share the same mechanisms.
DUAL CODE THEORY (Paivio) - We use both pictorial and verbal codes for representing
information - These two codes organize information into knowledge that can be acted on, stored
somehow, and later retrieved for subsequent use.
● According to Paivio, mental images are analog codes. Analog codes resemble the
objects they are representing. The mental images we form in our minds are analogous to
the physical stimuli we observe.
● Mental representations for words chiefly are represented in a symbolic code. A symbolic
code is a form of knowledge representation that has been chosen arbitrarily to stand for
something that does not perceptually resemble what is being represented.
● Paivio noted that verbal information seems to be processed differently than pictorial
information. - For example, in one study, participants were shown both a rapid sequence
of pictures and a sequence of words (Paivio, 1969).
● They then were asked to recall the words or the pictures in one of two ways. One way
was at random, so that they recalled as many items as possible, regardless of the order in
which the items were presented. The other way was in the correct sequence.
● Participants more easily recalled the pictures when they were allowed to do so in any
order. But they more readily recalled the sequence in which the words were presented
than the sequence for the pictures, which suggests the possibility of two different systems
for recall of words versus pictures.
● - Hypothesis: actual visual perception could interfere with simultaneous visual imagery.
Similarly, the need to produce a verbal response could interfere with the simultaneous
mental manipulation of words.
● - (Brooks, 1968). Tested and confirmed this hypothesis. Participants performed either a
visual task or a verbal task. The visual task involved answering questions requiring
judgments about a picture that was presented briefly. The verbal task involved answering
questions requiring judgments about a sentence that was stated briefly. Participants
expressed their responses verbally (saying “yes” or “no” aloud), visually (pointing to an
answer), or manually (tapping with one hand to agree and the other to disagree). There
were two conditions in which Brooks expected interference: a visual task requiring a
visual (pointing) response and a verbal task requiring a verbal response. This prediction
assumed that both task and response required the same system for completion.
Interference was measured by slow-downs in response times.
ALTERNATIVE THEORY TO DUAL CODE PROCESSING, THE PROPOSITIONAL
THEORY (Anderson and Bower, Phylyshyn) -
● Propositional theory suggests that we do not store mental representations in the form of
images or mere words.
● We may experience our mental representations as images, but these images are
epiphenomena—secondary and derivative phenomena that occur as a result of other more
basic cognitive processes.
● According to propositional theory, our mental representations (sometimes called
“mentalese”) more closely resemble the abstract form of a proposition. Limitations of
propositional theory -
IMAGERY DEBATE -
Shepard and Metzler’s Research: (In support of the Analog code approach)
● Reasoning - Suppose that you're holding a physical, geometric object in your hands, and
you decide to rotate it. It will take you longer to rotate this physical object by 180 degrees
than to rotate it only 90 degrees. Now suppose that our mental images operate the same
way that physical objects operate.Then it will take you longer to rotate this mental image
180 degrees, instead of 90 degrees.
● Shepard and Metzler (1971) asked eight extremely dedicated participants to judge 1,600
pairs of line drawings like these. They were instructed to pull a lever with their right hand
if they judged the figures to be the same,and to pull a different lever with their left hand if
they judged the figures to be different. In each case, the experimenters measured the
amount of time required for a decision. Here, DV = Reaction Time. (A- requires 2D
rotation, B requires 3D rotation)
● Results - People’s decision time was strongly influenced by the amount of mental rotation
required to match a FIgure with its mate. For example, rotating a figure 160 degrees
requires much more time than rotating it a mere 20 degrees.
● The participants in this study performed a three-dimensional rotation almost as quickly as
a two-dimensional rotation.
● The relationship between rotation and reaction time is a straight line. This research
supports the analog-code perspective, because you would take much longer to rotate an
actual physical object 160 degrees than to rotate it a mere 20 degrees. In contrast, a
propositional code would predict similar reaction times for these two conditions; the
language-like description for the gure would not vary with the amount of rotation.
● Other Research: Alphabet rotation, similar results were observed.
● Kotaro Takeda and his coauthors (2010) asked the participants in their study to look at
pictures of a human hand and to identify whether they were viewing a left hand or a right
hand. Right-handers recognized a right hand faster than a left hand. In contrast,
left-handers recognized right and left hands equally quickly. However, both groups
recognized upright pictures faster—and more accurately—than upside-down pictures.
This particular finding is consistent with the earlier research.Afterall,people take less
time to rotate an image 0 degrees, rather than 180 degrees
● Elderly people perform more slowly than younger people on a mental-rotation task. In
contrast, age is not consistently correlated with other imagery skills, such as sense of
direction or the ability to scan mental images (Beni et al., 2006; Dror & Kosslyn, 1994).
● Deaf individuals who are uent in American Sign Language (ASL) are especially skilled
in looking at an arrangement of objects in a scene and mentally rotating that scene by 180
degrees
● Kosslyn, Thompson, and their coauthors (2001) examined whether people use their
motor cortex when they imagine themselves rotating one of the geometric figures in the
figure above.
● Researchers instructed one group of participants to rotate—with their own hands—one of
the geometric figures that had been used in Shepard and Metzler’s (1971) study. They
instructed a second group of participants to simply watch as an electric motor rotated this
same gure.
● Participants who had originally rotated the original geometric figure with their hands
showed activity in their primary motor cortex. The same part of the brain that had
been active when they had rotated the figure with their hands.
● In contrast, participants who had originally watched the electric motor as it rotated the
figure.On the mental-rotation task, these people now showed no activity in the primary
motor cortex. Without the ‘‘hands on’’ experience, their primary motor cortex was not
active. - When people received the standard instructions to rotate the gure, their right
frontal lobes and their parietal lobes were strongly activated.
● Researchers modified the instructions in a second condition where the participants were
instructed to imagine rotating themselves so that they could ‘‘see’’ the figure from a
different perspective. These instructions produced increased activity in the left temporal
lobe, as well as in a part of the motor cortex.
● Implication : a relatively subtle change in wording can make a dramatic change in the
way that the brain responds to a mental-imagery task.
● Application : Such imagery tasks can be beneficial as ‘exercises’ for people recovering
from stroke. When people rotate a visual image, a large rotation takes them longer, just as
they take longer when making a large rotation with a physical stimulus.
● Kosslyn and his colleagues (1978) showed that people took a long time to scan the
distance between two widely separated points on a mental image of a map that they had
created. In contrast, they quickly scanned the distance between two nearby points on a
mental image of that map .Later research confirms that there is a linear relationship
between the distance to be scanned in a mental image and the amount of time required to
scan this distance.
● Possible problem w/this research: experimenter expectancy: the researchers’ biases and
expectations influence the outcomes of the experiment.
● To counter this criticism, Jolicoeur and Kosslyn repeated the mental-map experiment.
However, these researchers made certain that the two research assistants—who actually
administered the new study—were not familiar with the research on mental imagery.
They did not know about the typical linear relationship found in the previous research.
Instead, the assistants were given an elaborate and convincing (but incorrect) explanation
about visual imagery. This incorrect explanation described how the participants’ results
should show a U-shaped relationship between visual-imagery distance and scanning time.
● Interestingly, the research assistants did not obtain the U-shaped curve that they were
told they would find. Instead,their results demonstrated the standard linear relationship.
People make distance judgments in a similar fashion for visual images and for physical
stimuli.
● Allan Paivio (1978) asked participants to make judgments about the angle formed by the
two hands on an imaginary clock.
● Results: The high-imagery participants made decisions much more quickly than the
low-imagery participants.
● Participants in both groups made decisions very slowly when they compared the angle
formed by the hands at 3:20 with the angle of the hands at 7:25.
● Their decisions were relatively fast if the two angles were very different in size, perhaps
3:20 and 7:05.
● Implication: According to Paivio (1978), this study demonstrates strong support for the
proposal that people use analog codes, rather than propositional codes.
● 1 more study in support of this- Shepard and Chipman (1970) asked participants to
construct mental images of the shapes of various U.S.states, such as Colorado and
Oregon. Then the participants judged the similarity between the two mental images, with
respect to their shapes. For example—without looking at a map—do you think that
Colorado and Oregon have similar shapes? How about Colorado and West Virginia?
These same participants also made shape-similarity judgments about pairs of states while
they looked at an actual physical sketch of each state (rather than only its name). The
results showed that the participants’ judgments were highly similar in the two conditions.
Once again, people’s judgments about the shape of mental images are similar to their
judgments about the shape of physical stimuli.
People make decisions about shape in a similar fashion for visual images and for physical
stimuli. This conclusion holds true for both simple shapes (angles formed by hands on a clock)
and complex shapes (geographic regions, like Colorado or West Virginia).
● Segal and Fusella(1970). In part of this study, they asked participants to create a visual
image, for example, a visual image of a tree. As soon as the participant had formed the
requested image, the researchers presented a real physical stimulus, for example a small
blue arrow. The researchers then measured the participants’ ability to detect the physical
stimulus.
● Results showed that people had more problems detecting the physical stimulus when the
mental image was in the same sensory mode. For example, when the participants had
been imagining the shape of a tree, they had trouble detecting the small blue arrow. The
mental image interfered with the real visual stimulus. In contrast, when they had been
imagining the sound of an oboe,they had no trouble reporting that they saw the arrow. -
● Mast and his colleagues (1999) told participants to create a visual image of a set of
narrow parallel lines. Next, they were instructed to rotate their mental image of this set of
lines, so that the lines were in a diagonal orientation. Meanwhile, the researchers
presented a physical stimulus, a small segment of a line.The participants were told to
judge whether this line segment had an exactly vertical orientation.
● Results showed that the imagined set of lines and the real set of lines produced similar
distortions in the participants’ judgments about the orientation of that line segment.
● When people create a mental image of an ambiguous gure—they sometimes use analog
codes and sometimes propositional codes.
● Stephen Reed (1974, 2010) tested people’s ability to decide whether a specific visual
pattern was a portion of a design that they had seen earlier.
● Specifically, Reed presented a series of paired figures. For example, people might first
see a pattern like the Star of David in Demonstration 7.3, and then this figure
disappeared.
● Next, after a brief delay, people saw a second pattern, such as a parallelogram with
slanted right and left sides. In half the trials, the second pattern was truly part of the rst
one (for example, a parallelogram). In the other half, it was not (for example, a
rectangle).
● The participants in Reed’s(1974) study were correct only14% of the time on the
star/parallelogram example. Across all stimuli, they were correct only 55% of the time,
hardly better than chance.
● Reed (1974) argued that people could not have stored a visual image for gures like the
Star of David, given the high error rate on items like this one. Instead, Reed proposed that
people sometimes store pictures as descriptions, using a kind of propositional code.. -
Reed’s (1974) research supports the verbal propositional-code approach, rather than the
analog-code approach.
● Chambers and Reisberg (1985) asked participants to create a clear mental image of this
figure.
● Next, the researchers removed the figure. The participants were then asked to give a
second, different interpretation of that particular figure. None of the 15 people could do
so. In other words, they apparently could not consult a stored mental image.
● Then, the participants were asked to draw the figure from memory. All of them looked at
the figure they had just drawn, and all 15 were able to supply a second interpretation. -
Implication: a strong verbal propositional code—such as ‘‘a duck that is facing
left’’—can overshadow a relatively weak analog code. - It’s often easy to reverse a visual
stimulus while you are looking at a physical picture that is ambiguous. In contrast, it’s
usually more difficult to reverse a mental image (Reisberg & Heuer, 2005).
● Conclusion: People often use an analog code when they are thinking about fairly simple
figures (like the two hands of a clock).
○ People may use a propositional code when the figures are more complex, as in
the case of the research by Reed (1974) and Chambers and Reisberg (1985). Our
memory has a limited capacity for visual imagery. We may therefore have
difficulty storing complex visual information in an analog code and then making
accurate judgments about these mental images.
○ Verbal labels (and a propositional code) may be especially helpful if the visual
stimulus is complex.
● Ishai and Sagi (1995) showed that people can see a visual target more accurately if they
create mental images of vertical lines on each side of the target. (masking effect)
● This study on the masking effect is especially important because of a research methods
issue called ‘‘demand characteristics.’’ Demand characteristics are all the cues that
might convey the experimenter’s hypothesis to the participant.
● Some critics of the analog approach have proposed that the experimental results in
imagery experiments might be traceable to one or more of these demand characteristics.
participants may be able to guess the results that the experimenter wants. Perhaps they
might guess that a visual mental image is supposed to interfere with visual perception. -
● The masking effect is virtually unknown to people who have not completed a psychology
course in perception. The participants in the study by Ishai and Sagi (1995) would not
know that visual targets are especially easy to see if they are surrounded by masking
stimuli.Therefore, demand characteristics cannot account for the masking effect with
mental images.
● Visual imagery really can produce the masking effect, just as visual perception can
produce the masking effect. Visual imagery can indeed resemble visual perception.
● He asked participants to imagine animals next to each other, such as an elephant and a
rabbit, and told them to imagine that they were standing close enough to the larger animal
so that it fills most of their visual field.
● He then posed questions such as “Does a rabbit have whiskers?” and asked his
participants to find that part of the animal in their mental image and to answer as quickly
as possible. When he repeated this procedure but told participants to imagine a rabbit and
a fly next to each other, participants created larger images of the rabbit, as shown in
Figure 10.9b. The result of these experiments, shown alongside the pictures, was that
participants answered questions about the rabbit more rapidly when it filled more of the
visual field.
● Participants were to imagine that they were walking toward their mental image of an
animal.
● Their task was to estimate how far away they were from the animal when they began to
experience “overflow”—when the image fi lled the visual field or when its edges started
becoming fuzzy.
● The result was that participants had to move closer for small animals (less than a foot for
a mouse) than for larger animals (about 11 feet for an elephant), just as they would have
to do if they were walking toward actual animals. This result provides further evidence
for the idea that images are spatial, just like perception.
● Perky asked her participants to “project” visual images of common objects onto a
screen, and then to describe these images. Perky was back-projecting a very dim
image of this object onto the screen. Thus, when participants were asked to create
an image of a banana, Perky projected a dim image of a banana onto the screen. -
Result: the participants’ descriptions of their images matched the images that
Perky was projecting. Not one of Perky’s 24 participants noticed that there was an
actual picture on the screen. They had apparently mistaken an actual picture for a
mental image.
● Farah (1985) instructed her participants to imagine either the letter H or T on a
screen. Once they had formed clear images on the screen, they pressed a button
that caused two squares to flash, one after the other. One of the squares contained
a target letter, which was either an H or a T. The participants’ task was to indicate
whether the letter was in the first square or the second one.
● The results indicate that the target letter was detected more accurately when the
participant had been imagining the same letter rather than the different letter.
● Farah interpreted this result as showing that perception and imagery share
mechanisms. Many other experiments have demonstrated similar interactions
between perception and imagery
● One of the early brain imaging experiments to study imagery was carried
out by LeBihan and coworkers (1993), who demonstrated that both
perception and imagery activate the visual cortex.
● Activity in the striate cortex increased both when a person observed
presentations of actual visual stimuli (marked “Perception”) and when the
person was imagining the stimulus (“Imagery”).
● Giorgio Ganis and coworkers (2004) used fMRI to measure activation
under two conditions, perception and imagery.
● For the perception condition, participants observed a drawing of an
object, like a tree For the imagery condition, participants were told to
imagine a picture that they had studied before, when they heard a tone. -
For both the perception and imagery tasks, participants had to answer a
question such as “Is the object wider than it is tall?”
● Results of Ganis’s experiment show activation at three different locations
in the brain.
● There is almost complete overlap of the activation caused by perception
and imagery in the front of the brain, but some differences near the back of
the brain.
● Kosslyn and coworkers (1999) did an experiment using a technique called transcranial
magnetic stimulation (TMS).
● Kosslyn and coworkers performed TMI to the visual area of the brain while participants
were carrying out either a perception task or an imagery task.
● Result indicated that stimulation caused participants to respond more slowly, and that this
slowing effect occurred both for perception and for imagery.
● Based on this result, Kosslyn concluded that the brain activation that occurs in response
to imagery is not an epiphenomenon and that brain activity in the visual cortex plays a
causal role in both perception and imagery.
The Imagery Debate : The analog perspective v/s the propositional perspective
● According to the analog perspective, you create a mental image of an object that closely
resembles the actual perceptual image on your retina
● According to the propositional perspective, mental images are stored in an abstract,
language-like form that does not physically resemble the original stimulus. Zenon
Pylyshyn (2003, 2004, 2006) has been the strongest supporter of this perspective.
Pylyshyn agrees that people do experience mental images; it would be foolish to argue
otherwise.
● However, Pylyshyn says that these images are not a necessary, central component of
imagery. it would be awkward—and perhaps even unworkable—to store information in
terms of mental images. For instance, people would need a huge space to store all the
images that they claim to have.
● He also emphasizes the differences between perceptual experiences and mental images.
For example, you can re-examine and re-interpret a real photograph. However, on a task
like the rabbit/duck gure, people typically cannot reinterpret a mental image (Chambers
& Reisberg, 1985).
● At present, the analog code apparently explains most stimuli and most tasks. However,
for some kinds of stimuli and several specific tasks, people may use a propositional code.
✅
● Visual Imagery relies only on top- down processes (Perception- bottom up + top down) -
Does not activate rods or cones (Perception )
● Visual imagery activates between about 70% and 90% of the same brain regions that are
activated during visual perception.
● Some individuals with brain damage cannot distinguish between (1) the colors registered
during visual perception and (2) the visual imagery created in a mental image.
● People who have prosopagnosia cannot recognize human faces visually, though they
perceive other objects relatively normally. The research shows that these individuals also
cannot use mental imagery to distinguish between faces -
● This neuroscience evidence about the similarity between visual imagery and visual
perception is especially persuasive,because it avoids the demand-characteristics problem.
● As Farah (2000) pointed out, people seldom know which parts of their brain are typically
active during vision. Therefore, when you create a mental image of a bow tie, you cannot
voluntarily activate the relevant cells in your visual cortex!
● Research on cognitive maps is a part of a larger topic called “spatial cognition” (thoughts
abt cognitive map, how we remember the navigation, how we keep track of objs in a
spatial array)
● Individual differences in spatial-cognition skills are quite large (Smith & Cohen, 2008;
Wagner, 2006). However, people tend to be accurate in judging their ability to find their
way to unfamiliar locations (Kitchin & Blades, 2002)
● Spatial-cognition scores are also correlated with performance on the spatial tasks like
mental rotation
● People often use heuristics (general problem-solving strategy that usually produces a
correct solution ... but not always) in making judgments about cognitive maps. As a
result, they tend to show systematic distortions in distance, shape, and relative position.
Distance Estimates -
● We tend to construct cognitive maps in which the shapes are more regular than they are in
reality.
● Angles: Moar and Bower (1983), who studied people’s cognitive maps of Cambridge,
England. All the participants in the study had lived in Cambridge for at least five years.
Moar and Bower asked people to estimate the angles formed by the intersection of two
streets, without using a map. The participants showed a clear tendency to ‘‘regularize’’
the angles so that they were more like 90-degree angles. (Systematic Distortion) -
● 90-degree-angle heuristic- they represent angles in a mental map as being closer to 90
degrees than they really are.
● Curves: people tend to use a symmetry heuristic; we remember gures as being more
symmetrical and regular than they truly are.
● The small inconsistencies of geographic reality are smoothed over, so that our cognitive
maps are idealized and standardized.
UNIT 4- Language
● Linguistics
● Neurolinguistics
● Sociolinguistics
● Computational linguistics
COMPONENTS OF WORDS
● Phonemes :
○ When you say words, you produce sounds called phonemes.
○ A phoneme is the shortest segment of speech that, if changed, changes the
meaning of a word.
○ Thus, the word bit contains the phonemes /b/, /i/, and /t/ (phonemes are indicated
by phonetic symbols that are set off with slashes), because we can change bit into
pit by replacing /b/ with /p/, to bat by replacing /i/ with /a/, or to bid by replacing
/t/ with /d/.
○ Because different languages use different sounds, the number of phonemes varies
in different languages. There are only 11 phonemes in Hawaiian, about 47 in
English, and as many as 60 in some African dialects.
● Morphemes
○ Morphemes are the smallest units of language that have a definable meaning or
a grammatical function.
○ For example, “truck” consists of a number of phonemes, but only one morpheme,
because none of the components that create the word truck mean anything.
○ Similarly, even though “table” has two syllables, “tabe” and “ul,” it also consists
of only a single morpheme, because the syllables alone have no meaning. In
contrast “bedroom” has two syllables and two morphemes, “bed” and “room.”
○ Endings such as “s” and “ed,” which contribute to the meaning of a word, are
morphemes. Thus even though “trucks” has just one syllable, it consists of two
morphemes, “truck” (which indicates a type of vehicle) and “s” (which indicates
more than one).
Structure of language:
● Noam Chomsky, a pioneering linguist and a professor at MIT, put forth an idea called the
language acquisition device or LAD.
● The LAD is a hypothetical tool hardwired into the brain that helps children rapidly learn
and understand language.
● Chomsky used it to explain just how amazingly children are able to acquire language
abilities as well as accounting for the innate understanding of grammar and syntax all
children possess. The LAD is a theoretical concept.
● Just as humans are genetically programmed to walk, they are programmed to acquire and
use language.
● Chomsky concluded that despite the wide variations that exist across languages, the
underlying basis of all language is similar.
● Two types of structures:
○ 1. Surface structure: Words that are actually spoken or written
○ 2. Deep structure: Underlying more abstract meaning of the sentence
○ Eg: Sentences with different surface structures but same deep structures: ● Sara
threw the ball. The ball was thrown by Sara.
○ Similar surface structure but different deep structure:
○ Mike is easy to please ● Mike is eager to please
○ Even the surface structures of two sentences can be identical yet deep structure
different. These sentences are called “ambiguous sentences”
○ I want to go to the bank (financial institution) I want to go to the bank (river) Or I
saw a man on a hill with a telescope.
● Chomsky (1975) also proposed that language is modular
● In contrast to Chomsky’s theory, the standard cognitive approach argues that language is
not modular. Instead, it is interconnected with other cognitive processes such as working
memory
● The surface structure is represented by the words that are actually spoken or written. In
contrast, the deep structure is the underlying, more abstract meaning of a sentence
(Garnham, 2005; Harley, 2008).
● People use transformational rules to convert deep structure into a surface structure that
they can speak or write.Brain areas related to language:
● Broca’s area ● Wernicke’s area
Reading
● Reading requires recognition of letters, moving eyes across the page, using WM, recall
earlier info from LTM, use metamemory, metacomprehension etc
Dyslexia
▪ Dyslexia—difficulty in deciphering, reading, and comprehending text
1. Phonological awareness, which refers to awareness of the sound structure of
spoke language. Measured by phoneme deletion task, phoneme counting.
2. Phonological reading, which entails reading words in isolation. (word decoding”
or “word attack“) Measured by reading words in isolation.
3. Phonological coding in working memory. This process is involved in
remembering strings of phonemes that are sometimes confusing. Measured by
comparing working memory for confusable versus non-confusable phonemes
4. Lexical access refers to one’s ability to retrieve phonemes from long-term
memory.
▪ Developmental dyslexia, which is difficulty in reading that starts in childhood and
typically continues throughout adulthood.
▪ Acquired dyslexia, caused by traumatic brain damage.
Perceptual Issues in Reading
▪ A very basic but important step in reading is the activation of our ability to recognize
letters. Somehow manage to perceive the correct letter when it is presented in a wide
array of typestyles and typefaces
▪ Translate the letter into a sound, creating a phonological code
▪ Manage to translate all those visual symbols into sounds, you must sequence those
sounds to form a word
▪ Need to identify the word and figure out what the word means
▪ Move on to the next word and repeat the process all over again.
▪ When learning to read, novice readers must master 2 basic kinds of processes: lexical
processes & comprehension processes.
- Lexical processes are used to identify letters and words. They also activate relevant
information in memory about these words.
- Comprehension processes are used to make sense of the text as a whole
● Readers usually encounter standardized, error-free input, whereas listeners often need to
cope with variability, grammatical errors, sloppy pronunciation, and interfering stimuli.
● Readers can see discrete boundaries between words, whereas listeners often encounter
unclear boundaries in spoken language.
● Readers encounter only the stimuli on a page, whereas listeners encounter both nonverbal
cues and auditory cues, such as emphasized words and variations in pace.
● Beginning in the 1960s, psychologists began to study how several linguistic factors can
influence language comprehension. In general, people have difficulty understanding
sentences in these four conditions:
1. If they contain negatives, such as not.
a. If a sentence contains a negative word, such as no or not, or an implied negative
(such as rejected), the sentence almost always requires more processing time than
a similar, affirmative sentence (Williams, 2005).
b. In a classic study, Clark and Chase (1972) showed a picture of a star above a plus
sign. Then they asked people to verify statements, such as the following:
c. Star is above plus.+ ∗ The participants responded quickly in this case, when the
sentence was affirmative. They responded more slowly if the sentence contained
the negative form isn’t (for example, ‘‘Plus isn’t above star’’). The participants
also made fewer errors with affirmative sentences than with negative sentences.
d. Our cognitive processes handle positive information better than negative
information.
2. If they are in the passive rather than the active voice.
a. The active form is more basicand easier to understand. For example, we need to
add extra words we want to create the passive form of a sentence.
b. As you might guess, the English language uses the active voice much more often
than the passive voice (Fiedler et al., 2011).
c. For example, Ferreira and her coauthors (2002) asked participants to determine
whether each sentence in a series was plausible.
d. The participants were highly accurate in responding ‘‘No’’ to sentences in the
active voice, such as, ‘‘The man bit the dog.’’
e. In contrast, their accuracy dropped to about 75% when the same sentences were
converted to the passive voice, for example, ‘‘The dog was bitten by the man’’ (p.
13)
3. If they have complex syntax.
a. In a nested structure, one phrase is embedded within another phrase. Readers
often experience a memory overload when they try to read a sentence that has a
nested structure.
b. nested phrase that appears between the two dashes. Your working memory needs
to maintain the first part of this sentence, while you navigate the nested phrase.
Then you must relate the last part of this sentence to the first part.
4. If they are ambiguous
a. Psychologists have designed several methods of measuring the difficulty of
understanding a sentence with an ambiguous word or phrase (Harley, 2010;
MacDonald, 1999; Rodd et al., 2002).
b. For example, one method measures the amount of time that the reader pauses on a
word before moving his or her eyes to the next words in the sentence People
typically pause longer when they are processing an ambiguous word, for example,
when they are completing a questionnaire (Lenzner et al., 2010).
c. When people encounter a potential ambiguity, the activation builds up for all the
well-known meanings of the ambiguous item. Furthermore, people are likely to
select one particular meaning
i. (1) if that meaning is more common than the alternate meaning and
ii. (2) if the rest of the sentence is consistent with that meaning (Hurley,
2011; Morris & Binder, 2001; Sereno et al., 2003).
Reading Processes
● We recognize letters, create a phonological code (sound out) and sequence them to form
words, identify them and then access their meaning. With each word, we do the same
process and then combine words into a sentence. In English, translating the letter into
sound is relatively difficult.
● When learning to read, novice readers must master 2 basic kinds of processes: lexical
processes & comprehension processes.
○ Lexical processes are used to identify letters and words. They also activate
relevant information in memory about these words.
○ Comprehension processes are used to make sense of the text as a whole.
● Saccadic eye movements, fixation & reading speed:
○ Readers fixate for a longer time on longer words than on shorter words.
○ They also fixate longer on less familiar words (i.e., words that appear less
frequently in the English language) than on more familiar words (i.e., words of
higher frequency).
○ The last word of a sentence also seems to receive an extra long fixation time. This
can be called “sentence wrap-up time”
○ Readers fixate up to about 80% of the content words in a text. These words
include nouns, verbs, and other words that carry the bulk of the meaning.
● Saccadic movements leap an average of about seven to nine characters between
successive fixations.
● When students speed-read, they show fewer and shorter fixations.
● But apparently their greater speed is at the expense of comprehension of anything more
than just the gist of the passage
● Lexical access: the identification of a word that allows us to gain access to the meaning
of the word from memory.
● It combines information of different kinds, such as the features of letters, the letters
themselves, and the words comprising the letters
4. Factor 3: Vocabulary
● In children, vocabulary size is positively related to performance on a number of
semantic-understanding tasks, including retelling, ecoding ability, and the ability to draw
inferences across sentences (Hagtvet, 2003).
● People with larger vocabularies are able to access lexical information more rapidly than
are those with smaller vocabularies
● Studies have found that people with large or small vocabularies (high verbal/low verbal)
learn word meanings differently.
● High-verbal participants perform a deeper analysis of the possibilities for a new word’s
meaning than do low-verbal participants. In particular, the high-verbal participants used a
well-formulated strategy for figuring out word meanings.
● The low-verbal participants seemed to have no clear strategy at all (van Daalen-Kapteijns
& Elshout-Mohr, 1981
5. Factor 4: Reader’s point of view & Context Comprehending Text Based on Context
and Point of View:
● Anderson & Pichert (1978): A passage about a wealthy person’s house was given. 2
conditions: think from the perspective of a burglar and of a prospective home buyer ●
● Passage mentioned the condition of the house including a leaky roof, a musty basement
etc. as well as contents of the house including jewellery, silverware etc.
● Ps from condition 1 remembered more about house condition while Ps from condition 2
remembered more about contents of the house from the passage
● Passages of text that lead unambiguously to a single mental model are easier to
comprehend than are passages that may lead to multiple mental models
● Bridging inference: This is an inference a reader or listener makes when a sentence seems
not to follow directly from the sentence preceding it.
● In essence, what is new in the second sentence goes one step too far beyond what is given
in the previous sentences.
● Read the following two pairs of two sentences:
○ 1. John took the picnic out of the trunk. The beer was warm. 2. John took the beer
out of the trunk. The beer was warm.
● Readers took about 180 milliseconds longer to read the first pair of sentences than the
second. Haviland and Clark suggested a reason for this greater processing time. It was
that, in the first pair, information needed to be inferred (the picnic included tea) that was
directly stated in the second pair. Factors affecting Inferences
○ Large working memory capacity
○ Good metacomprehension skills
○ Expertise about the topic
● One kind of higher-level inference is based on our own preferences about the way we
want a story to turn out.
● The research shows that readers who are involved in a story do develop strong mental
preferences for a particular outcome read.
● Readers integrate material into a cohesive unit, and they are puzzled if they encounter
something that contradicts the inferences they drew. Eg: unhappy ending vs happy ending
● Inferences may be relatively rare in scientific texts and relatively common in novels. In
novels, our own preferences may interfere with text comprehension.
Language Production
● Producing language is another one of those cognitive processes we achieve rapidly and
easily, but which is actually extremely complex.
● The act of speaking involves assembling strings of words that have been rapidly retrieved
from memory (more than 3 words per second for normal conversation)—and is drawn
from a lexicon of more than 50,000 words.
● Not only are the correct words rapidly retrieved, but they are produced in the correct
order and combined with other words to create a grammatically correct sentence
● Psychologists who study language production often examine how we retrieve
grammatical, semantic, and phonological information.
● Some researchers argue that speakers retrieve all three kinds of information at the same
time (Damian & Martin, 1999;Saffran & Schwartz,2003).
● According to this approach,for example,you look at an apple and simultaneously access
the grammatical properties of apple, the meaning of apple, and the phonemes in the word
apple.
● Other researchers argue that we access each kind of information independently, with little
interaction among these three components (Ferreira & Slevc,2009; Meyer & Belke,2009
● Miranda van Turennout and her colleagues (1998), who conducted research with
Dutch-speaking individuals.Dutch resembles languages such as Spanish,French,and
German,because Dutch nouns have a grammatical gender.
● These researchers presented pictures of objects and animals, and the participants tried to
name the object as quickly as possible
● Using the event-related potential technique, researchers demonstrated that speakers
access the grammatical gender of the word about 40 milliseconds before they access the
word’s phonological properties.
● These results suggest that we do not acquire all the different kinds of information at the
exact same moment. Instead, we literally use split-second timing.
Speech errors
● Researchers have been particularly interested in the kind of speech errors called
slips-of-the-tongue.
● “Slips of the tongue,” were made famous by Sigmund Freud, who suggested that slips of
the tongue reflected the speaker’s unconscious motivations
● Although slips of this kind, which have been called Freudian slips, do occur, there is little
to support Freud’s idea that all slips are caused by unconscious motivation.
● Rather than focusing on unconscious motivation, speech researchers have used speech
errors to provide insights into basic mechanisms of language
● One of the challenges of studying speech errors is to identify them as they occur only
about 1 or 2 times out of every 1,000 words in normal conversation
● Slips-of-the-tongue are errors in which sounds or entire words are rearranged between
two or more different words.
● These slips of the tongue are informative because they reveal our extensive knowledge
about the sounds, structure, and meaning of the language we are speaking (Dell et al.,
2008; Traxler, 2012).
● Types of Slip-of-the-Tongue Errors. Gary Dell and his coauthors propose that three kinds
of slips-of-the-tongue are especially common in English (Dell, 1995; Dell et al., 2008):
○ 1. Sound errors, which occur when sounds in nearby words are exchanged—for
example, snow flurries → flow snurries.
○ 2. Morpheme errors, which occur when morphemes (the smallest meaningful
units in language, such as -ly or in-) are exchanged in nearby words—for
example, self-destruct instruction → self-instruct destruction.
○ 3. Word errors, which occur when words are exchanged—for example, writing a
letter to my mother → writing a mother to my letter
● Furthermore, we are likely to create a word (e.g., leading), rather than a nonword (e.g.,
londing) when we make a slip-of-the-tongue error (Griffin & Ferreira, 2006; Rapp &
Goldrick, 2000).
● Finally, we seldom create a word that begins with an unlikely letter sequence. For
example, English speakers rarely create a slip-of-the-tongue such as dlorm when trying to
say dorm
● These two principles reflect the importance of our knowledge about the English
language's emphasis on top-down processing. In almost all cases, the errors occur across
items from the same category.
● The pattern of these errors suggests that the words we are currently pronouncing are
influenced by both the words we have already spoken and the words we are planning to
speak (Dell, Burger, & Svec, 1997).
● Dell and his colleagues propose a comprehensive theory for speech errors that is similar
to the connectionist approach and includes the concept of spreading activation
● When you are about to speak, each element of the word you are planning to say will
activate the sound elements to which it is linked.
● The words in the tongue twister ‘‘She sells seashells’’ might activate each of the six
sounds in the last word, seashells
● Usually, we utter the sounds that are most highly activated, and usually these sounds are
the appropriate ones.
● However, each sound can be activated by several different words. Notice, for example,
that the sh sound in the sound-level representation of seashells (that is, seshelz) is highly
‘‘charged’’ because it receives activation from the first word in the sentence, she, as well
as the sh in seashells.
● As Dell (1995) emphasizes, incorrect items sometimes have activation levels that are just
as high as (or higher than) the correct items.
● The activation level for sh is just as high as the level for s. By mistake, a speaker may
select an incorrect sound in a sentence, such as ‘‘She sells seashells.’’
Gestures:
● Gestures are visible movements of any part of your body, which you use to communicate
(Hostetter & Alibali, 2008;
● The same intentional gestures may convey different meanings in different cultures
● For example, suppose that you make a circle with your thumb and your first finger. This
gesture signifies ‘‘money’’ in Japan, and ‘‘perfect!’’ in France.
● However, in Malta—an island off the coast of Italy—this same gesture is an obscene
insult.
● Use of gestures in day to day life:
○ Define the word spiral
○ Give directions on how to walk from your current location to another location
about 10 minutes away.
○ Describe how you peel a carrot.Gestures:
● Gestures can also influence how you retrieve words(Goldin-Meadow & Beilock, 2010).
● For example, the spontaneous motor movements of your hands can sometimes help you
remember the word you want to produce
● ● In a representative study, Frick-Horbury and Guttentag (1998) read the definitions for
50 low-frequency, concrete English nouns.
● Then the researchers asked each participant to identify the target word. For example, the
definition ‘‘a pendulum-like instrument designed to mark exact time by regular ticking’’
was supposed to suggest the noun metronome.
● In this study, however, half of the participants were instructed to hold a rod with both
hands; therefore, their hand movements were restricted.
● The average score for these individuals was 19 words out of 50. In contrast, the
participants with unrestricted hand movements earned an average score of 24 words out
of 50.
● According to other similar research works, when our verbal system cannot retrieve a
word, a gesture can sometimes activate the relevant information (Brown, 2012).
● We frequently produce gestures when we speak, especially when we want to discuss a
concept that is easier to describe with body movements than with words (Ambady &
Weisbuch, 2010).
● Embodied cognition emphasizes that people use their bodies to express their knowledge
(Hostetter & Alibali, 2008).
● There is an ongoing connection between our motor system and the way we process
spoken language, for eg, when we make gestures or indicate some kind of motion
(Hostetter & Alibali, 2008; Tomasello, 2008).
● The recent attention to embodied cognition has convinced many psychologists that we
frequently think nonverbally (Ambady & Weisbuch, 2010).
● Amber Hostetter (2011) conducted a meta-analysis of the 63 studies that addressed this
question. She found that gestures actually increase the listener’s understanding, especially
when the speaker is describing concrete actions
Producing sentences:
Producing Discourse:
Conversations
Producing discourse/conversations:
● Although language can be produced by a single person talking alone, as when a person
recites a monologue or gives a speech, the most common form of language production is
conversation—two or more people talking with one another.
● While conversing, we don’t know what the other person would say. Still we are able to
respond efficiently.
● One way that people deal with these difficulties is that they coordinate their conversations
on both semantic and syntactic levels
Semantic coordination:
● When people are talking about a topic, each person brings his or her own knowledge to
the conversation, and conversations go more smoothly when the participants bring shared
knowledge to a conversation.
● But even when everyone brings similar knowledge to a conversation, it helps when
speakers take steps to guide their listeners through the conversation.
● One way this can be achieved is by following the given–new contract.
● The given–new contract states that the speaker should construct sentences so they include
two kinds of information:
○ Given information—information that the listener already knows; and
○ New information—information that the listener is hearing for the first time
(Haviland & Clark, 1974).
○ Example: 1. John was given a toy aeroplane for his birthday.
○ Given information (from previous conversation): John had a birthday. New
information: He got a toy aeroplane. The aeroplane was his favorite present.
○ Given information (from sentence 1): John got a toy aeroplane.
○ New information: It was his favorite present.
● Susan Haviland and Herbert Clark (1974): demonstrated the consequences of not
following the given–new contract by presenting pairs of sentences and asking participants
to press a button when they felt they understood the second sentence in each pair.
● They found that it took longer for the participants to comprehend the second sentence in
pairs like this one:
● We checked the picnic supplies. The beer was warm. than it took to comprehend the
second sentence in pairs like this one:
● We got some beer out of the trunk. The beer was warm. (Bridging inference)
Syntactic coordination
● When two people exchange statements in a conversation, it is common for them to use
similar grammatical constructions.
● Kathryn Bock (1990) provides the following example, taken from a recorded
conversation between a bank robber and his lookout, which was intercepted by a radio
operator as the robber was removing the equivalent of a million dollars from a bank vault
in England.
○ Robber: “. . . you’ve got to hear and witness it to realize how bad it is.”
○ Lookout: “You have got to experience exactly the same position as me, mate, to
understand how I feel.”
● This copying of form reflects a phenomenon called syntactic priming—hearing a
statement with a particular syntactic construction increases the chances that a sentence
will be produced with the same construction.
● Syntactic priming is important because it can lead people to coordinate the grammatical
form of their statements during a conversation.
● Holly Branigan and coworkers (2000) illustrated syntactic priming to set up a
give-and-take between two people.
● In a syntactic priming experiment two people engage in a conversation, and the
experimenter determines whether production of a specific grammatical construction by
one person increases the chances that the same construction will be used by the other
person.
● Participants in Branigan’s experiment were told that the experiment was about how
people communicate when they can’t see each other (Branigan et al. 2000).
● They thought they were working with another participant who was on the other side of a
screen. In reality, the person on the other side of the screen was a confederate who was
working with the experimenter.
● The confederate began the experiment by making a priming statement. This statement
was in one of the following two forms:
○ “The girl gave the book to the boy.”
○ “The girl gave the boy the book.
● The participant had two tasks:
○ (1) find the matching card on the table that corresponded to the confederate’s
statement, as shown on the right of Figure 10.12a;
○ (2) describe one of the response cards to the confederate.
● We can conclude that syntactic priming has occurred if the form of the participant’s
statement to the confederate matches the form of the confederate’s original statement.
● Branigan found that on 78 percent of the trials, the form of the participant’s statement
matched the form of the confederate’s priming statement.
● Thus, if the participant heard the confederate say “The girl gave the boy the book,” this
increased the chances that the participant would describe a response card: “The father
brought his daughter a present” (instead of “The father brought a present for his
daughter” or some other construction).
● This coordination of syntactic form between speakers reduces the computational load
involved in creating a conversation because it is easier to copy the form of someone
else’s sentence than it is to create your own form from scratch.
Pragmatics
Neurolinguistics:-
● Neurolinguistics is the discipline that examines how the brain processes language. It
demonstrates that the neurological basis of language is very complicated
Aphasia:
● A person with aphasia has difficulty communicating, caused by damage to the speech
areas of the brain. This damage is typically caused by a stroke or a tumor (Gazzaniga et
al., 2009)
● Brain areas related with language: Broca’s area and Wernicke’s area
● Broca’s area: language production; Wernicke’s area: language comprehension
Broca’s Aphasia:
● Broca’s area is one of the locations of the brain that manages motor movement.To
produce speech,you must move your lips and tongue. Therefore, it makes sense that these
individuals have trouble with speech production.
● However, people with Broca’s aphasia may also have some minor trouble with language
comprehension
Wernicke’s Aphasia:
Hemispheric specialization:
● Left hemisphere: Mostly its role is implicated in language processing for majority of
people. However for about 5% of right-handers and about 50% of left-handers, language
is either localized in the right hemisphere or is processed equally by both hemispheres.
● Right hemisphere: For many years, people thought that the right hemisphere did not play
a major role in language processing.However, we now know that this hemisphere does
perform some tasks. For example, the right hemisphere is active when you are paying
attention to the emotional tone of a message.
● It also plays a role in appreciating humor (Harley, 2010). In general, then, the right
hemisphere is responsible for more abstract language tasks
● The left and right hemispheres often work together on tasks such as interpreting subtle
word meanings, resolving ambiguities, and combining the meaning of several sentences
SOMETIMES I WAKE UP GRUMPY. OTHER TIMES I LET HIM SLEEP IN.
Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis:
● The concept relevant to the question of whether language influences thinking is linguistic
relativity.
● Linguistic relativity refers to the assertion that speakers of different languages have
differing cognitive systems and that these different cognitive systems influence the ways
in which people think about the world.
● Thus, according to the relativity view, the Garo would think about rice differently than
we do. Conclusion: language may not determine thought, but that language certainly may
influence thought. Our thoughts and our language interact in myriad ways
Writing
● When writing their first draft, writers have numerous opportunities to make mistakes
(Kellogg, 1998).
● Revision task will include checking for organization and coherence, assessing whether
writer has achieved his goal
● The most effective writers use flexible revision strategies, and they make substantial
changes if their paper doesn’t accomplish its goal (Harley, 2001).
● However, college students typically devote little time to revising a paper (Mayer, 2004).
● Metacognition inaccurate Hayes and his colleagues (1987) conducted a classic study,
comparing how first-year college students and expert writers revised a poorly written
two-page letter.
● Most first-year students revised the text one sentence at a time.
● They fixed relatively minor problems with spelling and grammar, but they ignored
problems of organization, focus, and transition between ideas.
● Proofreading: According to Daneman & Stainton (1993): You can proofread someone
else’s writing more accurately than your own.
● When you are very familiar with a paper that you’ve just written, you often overlook the
errors in the text. (Top-down processing)
Bilingualism:
● A bilingual speaker is someone who is fluent in two different languages (Harley, 2008;
Schwartz & Kroll, 2006).
● Simultaneous bilingualism: bilinguals learn two languages simultaneously during
childhood,
● Sequential bilingualism: their native language is referred to as their first language, and
the non-native language that they acquire is their second language
● The world has between 6,000 and 7,000 languages (Ku, 2006; Segalowitz, 2010). Even
so, almost all of the research on bilingualism includes English as one of the two
languages (Bassetti & Cook, 2011).
Background of bilingualism:
● During the early 1900s, theorists proposed that bilingualism produced cognitive deficits
because the brain must store two linguistic systems (Erwin-Tripp, 2011; De Groot, 2011).
● However, in the 1960s, researchers controlled for factors such as age and social class.
They discovered that bilingual children actually scored higher than monolinguals on a
variety of tasks.
Advantages of bilingualism:
● Bilinguals actually acquire more expertise in their native (first) language For example,
English-speaking Canadian children whose classes are taught in French gain greater
understanding of English-language structure
● Bilingual children are also more likely to realize that a word such as rainbow can be
divided into two morphemes, rain and bow (Campbell & Sais, 1995).
● Bilinguals are more aware that the names assigned to concepts are arbitrary (Cromdal,
1999; De Groot, 2011; Hakuta, 1986). For example, many monolingual children cannot
imagine that a cow could just as easily have been assigned the name dog.
● A number of studies have examined metalinguistics, or knowledge about the form and
structure of language. On many measures of metalinguistic skill—but not all of
them—bilinguals outperform monolinguals
● Bilinguals excel at paying selective attention to relatively subtle aspects of a language
task, while ignoring more obvious linguistic characteristics. For example, Bialystok and
Majumder (1998) gave third-grade children some sentences like: ‘‘Apples grow on
noses’’ Is this grammatically correct?
● Bialystok and Majumder (1998) gave third-grade children some sentences that were
grammatically correct but semantically incorrect (for example, ‘‘Apples grow on
noses’’).The bilingual children were more likely than the monolingual children to
recognize that the sentence was grammatically correct
● Bialystok (2009) also reported that bilingual individuals perform better on the Stroop
Test, a task that requires people to emphasize an item’s color and ignore its meaning.
Bialystok (2005) proposes that these experiences with selective attention may facilitate
the development of a portion of the frontal lobe, labeled ‘‘executive attention network’’
● Bilingual children are better at following complicated instructions and performing tasks
where the instructions change from one trial to the next (Bialystok, 2005, 2009).
Bialystok and Martin (2004) asked preschoolers to sort some cards that featured either a
blue circle, a red circle, a blue square, or a red square. The researchers first instructed
them to sort the cards on one dimension (e.g., shape).
● Later, the researchers instructed them to sort the cards on the other dimension (e.g.,
color). Bilingual children were much faster than monolingual children in switching to the
new dimension.
● Bilinguals perform better on concept-formation tasks and on tests of non-verbal
intelligence that require reorganization of visual patterns (Peal & Lambert, 1962).
● Bilinguals also score higher on problem-solving tasks that require them to ignore
irrelevant information (Bialystok, 2001; Bialystok & Codd, 1997)Advantages of
bilingualism:
● Bilingual adults who have dementia typically develop signs of dementia later than
monolingual adults with dementia (Bialystok, 2009; Bialystok et al., 2007). ● For
example, Bialystok and her coauthors (2007) examined the medical history of 184 people
at a memory clinic. All of them had a medical diagnosis of dementia. However, the
bilinguals had received this diagnosis at the average age of 75.5, in contrast to an average
age of 71.4 for the monolinguals.
Disadvantages of bilingualism:
● People who use two languages extensively may subtly alter how they pronounce some
speech sounds in both languages (Gollan et al., 2005).
● Bilingual individuals may also process language slightly more slowly, in comparison to
monolinguals.
● Bilingual children may have somewhat smaller vocabularies for words that are used in a
home setting (Bialystok, 2009; Bialystok et al., 2010). ●
● however, these disadvantages are far outweighed by the advantages of being able to
communicate effectively in two languages (Michael & Gollan, 2005).
● Age of acquisition refers to the age at which you learned a second language.
● According to the critical period hypothesis, your ability to acquire a second language is
strictly limited to a specific period of your life.
● Specifically, the critical period hypothesis proposes that individuals who have already
reached a specified age—perhaps early puberty—will no longer be able to acquire a new
language with native-like fluency.
● Second-Language Proficiency as a Function of Age of Acquisition:
● Fortunately, however, the current research evidence does not support a clear-cut,
biologically based ‘‘deadline’’ for learning a second language (Bialystok 2001; Birdsong,
2006; De Groot, 2011; Wiley et al., 2005).
● Phonology. The research suggests that age of acquisition does influence the mastery of
phonology, or the sounds of a person’s speech.
● Specifically, people who acquire a second language during childhood are more likely to
pronounce words like a native speaker of that language.
● In contrast, those who acquire a second language during adulthood are more likely to
have a foreign accent when they speak their new language (Bialystok, 2001; Flege et al.,
1999; MacKay et al., 2006).
● For example, Flege and his coauthors (1999) tested people who had immigrated to the
United States from Korea when they were between the ages of 1 and 23 years. At the
time of the study, all participants had lived in the United States for at least eight years.
● To test phonology, Flege and his colleagues asked their participants to listen to an English
sentence, and then repeat it. The phonology of each sentence was later judged by
speakers whose native language was English.
● Korean immigrants who had arrived in the United States during childhood typically had
minimal accents when speaking English; you can see that most have scores of 7 or 8.
● Those who had arrived as adolescents or adults usually had stronger accents, with scores
of 2 to 4.
● However, notice the fairly smooth decline with age of acquisition, rather than the abrupt
drop predicted by the critical period hypothesis (Bialystok, 2001). In later research,
MacKay and his coauthors (2006) found similar results in phonology, with people who
had emigrated from Italy.
● Problem occurs when there is an obstacle between a present state and a goal and it is not
immediately obvious how to get around the obstacle
● You use problem solving when you want to reach a specified goal; however, the solution
is not immediately obvious because you are missing important information and/or it is
not clear how to reach the goal
● The nature of these problems may differ. However, every problem includes three
components: Newel and Simon
○ (1) the initial state,- The initial state describes the situation at the beginning of the
problem
○ (2) the goal state- You reach the goal state when you solve the problem
○ (3) the obstacles.- The obstacles describe the restrictions that make it difficult to
proceed from the initial state to the goal state (Thagard, 2005).
● Problem representation refers to the way you translate the elements of the problem into a
different format. If you choose an appropriate representation, you are more likely to reach
an effective solution to the problem.
● Your representation of the problem must show the essential information that you need in
order to solve it. Some of the most effective methods of representing problems include
symbols, matrices, diagrams, and visual images
Symbols.
● Sometimes the most effective way to represent an abstract problem is by using symbols,
as you learned to do in high school algebra
● A major challenge is that problem solvers often make mistakes when they try to translate
words into symbols (Mayer, 2004).
● One common error is that they reverse the roles of the two variables (Fisher et al., 2011).
● An additional error may occur when problem solvers try to translate sentences into
symbols: They may oversimplify the sentence, so that they misrepresent the information
(Mayer, 2004).
Matrices.
● You can solve some problems effectively by using a matrix, which is a grid consisting of
rows and columns; it shows all possible combinations of items (Hurley & Novick, 2010).
● A matrix is an excellent way to keep track of items, particularly if the problem is
complex and if the relevant information is categorical (Halpern, 2003). For example, you
can solve
Diagrams.
● Diagrams allow you to represent abstract information in a concrete fashion. They also let
you discard unnecessary details (Bassok & Novick, 2012; Reed, 2010; Reif, 2008;
Schneider et al., 2010).
● Diagrams can also be useful when you want to represent a large amount of information
and can represent complicated information in a clear, concrete form. As a result, you have
more ‘‘mental space’’ in your working memory for solving other parts of the problem
(Halpern, 2003; Hurley & Novick, 2006).
Visual Images.
● A visual image allows us to escape from the boundaries of traditional, concrete
representations.
● Good visual-imagery skills also provide an advantage when a problem requires you to
construct a figure (Gorman, 2006; Pylyshyn, 2006).
Types of Problems:
Gestaltist view:
● In contrast, when you work on a noninsight problem, you solve the problem gradually,
by using your memory, reasoning skills, and a routine set of strategies(Davidson, 1995) ●
Top-down processing may prevent you from solving an insight problem. In contrast,
noninsight problems— such as straightforward algebra problems—typically do benefit
from top-down processing (McCormick, 2003)
● Non- Insight problems: Eg: Mathematical calculations (⅕)x+10= 25 ● Insight problems:
two string problem, Duncker’s candle problem
● Failure to solve insight problems: role of top-down processes; functional fixedness and
other obstacles
Ill structured problem example: A woman who lived in a small town married 20 different men in
that same town. All of them are still living, and she never divorced any of them. Yet she broke no
laws. How could she do this?
Ill structured problem example: (Duncker’s candle problem; 1945) You are in a room with a
cork board on the wall. You are given some candles, matches in a matchbox, and some thumb
pins. Your task is to mount a burning candle on the corkboard so it will burn without dripping
wax on the floor
Duncker’s radiation problem (1945): Suppose you are a doctor faced with a patient who has a
malignant tumor in his stomach. You cannot operate on the patient because of the severity of
tumor, but unless the tumor is destroyed the patient will die. High intensity X rays can be used to
destroy the tumor. Unfortunately, the intensity of the X rays needed to destroy the tumor also will
destroy the health tissue through which the rays must pass. X rays of lower intensity will spare
the tissue but they will be insufficiently powerful to destroy the tumor. What type of procedure
might be used to destroy the tumor with the rays and at the same time avoid destroying the
healthy tissue?
The military problem: A small country was ruled from a strong fortress by a dictator. The
fortress was situated in the middle of the country, surrounded by farms and villages. Many roads
led to the fortress through the countryside. A rebel general vowed to capture the fortress. The
general knew that an attack by his entire army would capture the fortress. He gathered his army
at the head of one of the roads, ready to launch a full-scale direct attack. However, the general
then learned that the dictator had planted mines on each of the roads. The mines were set so that
small bodies of men could pass over them safely, since the dictator needed to move his troops
and workers to and from the fortress. However, any large force would detonate the mines. It
therefore seemed impossible to capture the fortress. What should the general do?
Solution of the military problem: However, the general devised a simple plan. He divided his
armies into small groups and dispatched each group to the head of a different road. When all
was ready, he gave the signal and each group marched down a different road. Each group
continued down its road so that the entire army arrived together at the fortress at the same time.
In this way, the general captured the fortress and overthrew the dictator.
Heuristics
1. Analogy approach:
● Participants who received the military problem with the convergence solution and then
were given a hint to apply it in some way to the radiation problem.
● About 75% of the participants reached the correct solution to the radiation problem.
● This figure compared with less than 10% of the participants who did not receive the
military story first but instead received no prior story or only an irrelevant one (Holyaak,
1983).
● When participants were asked to memorize the military story under the guise that it was a
story-recall experiment and then were given the radiation problem to solve Only 30% of
participants produced the convergence solution to the radiation problem.
● Positive transfer was seen when an isomorphic problem was given and its solution told to
the participants.
● The investigators also found that positive transfer improved if two, rather than just one,
analogous problems were given in advance of the radiation problem. Problem solving
strategies:
● When you use the analogy approach in problem solving, you employ a solution to a
similar, earlier problem to help you solve a new problem (Benjamin & Ross, 2011;
Leighton & Sternberg, 2003)
● Research clearly shows that people often fail to see the analogy between a problem they
have solved and a new problem isomorph that has similar structural features (e.g., Barnett
& Ceci, 2002) as they focus only on surface features rather than structural features.
● People often have trouble solving the same problem in a new setting; they fail to transfer
their knowledge.
● Similarly, they have trouble solving the same problem when it is ‘‘dressed up’’ with a
superficially different cover story (Bassok, 2003).
● People who have limited problem-solving skills and limited metacognitive ability are
especially likely to have difficulty using analogies
● Expertise
● Mental set
● Functional fixedness
● Gender stereotype threat
● Emotions and motivation
● Irrelevant/misleading information
● Assumptions
● Knowledge Base. Experts and novices differ substantially in their knowledge base and
schemas
● As Michelene Chi (1981) found in her classic study of physics problem solving, the
novices simply lacked important knowledge about the principles of physics.
● Memory: Experts differ from novices with respect to their memory for information
related to their area of expertise (Chi, 2006; Robertson, 2001).
● The memory skills of experts tend to be very specific. For example, expert chess players
have much better memory than novices for various chess positions (Chi, 2006; Gobet &
Simon, 1996a).
● Factors influencing problem solving: When experts encounter a novel problem in their
area of expertise, they are more likely than novices to use the means-ends heuristic
effectively (Sternberg & Ben-Zeev, 2001). ●
● They are also more likely to approach a problem systematically, whereas novices are
more likely to have a haphazard approach (Reif, 2008).
● Speed and accuracy: Experts are much faster than novices, and they solve problems
very accurately (Chi, 2006; Ericsson, 2003b; Ericsson & Towne, 2010).
● Their operations become more automatic, and a particular stimulus situation also quickly
triggers a response (Bransford et al., 2000; Glaser & Chi, 1988; Robertson, 2001).
● On some tasks, experts may solve problems faster because they use parallel processing,
rather than serial processing. (Eg: solving anagram problems)
● Metacognitive skills: Experts are better than novices at monitoring their problem
solving.
● For example, experts seem to be better at judging the difficulty of a problem, and they
are more skilled at allocating their time appropriately when solving problems (Bransford
et al., 2000).
● According to a study of people who are the expert inventors skillfully monitor ideas, to
see that they are useful, as well as creative (Mieg, 2011). Experts can also recover
relatively quickly when they realize that they have made an error (Feltovich et al., 2006).
Mental Set: A common problem-solving obstacle is known as a mental set, which is the
tendency people have to only use solutions that have worked in the past rather than looking for
alternative ideas.
● A mental set can often work as a heuristic, making it a useful problem-solving tool.
However, mental sets can also lead to inflexibility, making it more difficult to find
effective solutions.
● If you have a mental set, you close your mind prematurely, and you stop thinking about
how to solve a problem effectively (Kruglanski, 2004; Zhaoetal.,2011).
● Mental set represents overactive top down processing- Eg: Abraham Luchins’s (1942)
water-jar problem
● If you have a fixed mindset, you believe that you possess a certain amount of intelligence
and other skills, and no amount of effort can help you perform better. You give up on
trying to discover new ways to improve your abilities.
● In contrast, if you have a growth mindset, you believe that you can cultivate your
intelligence and other skills. You challenge yourself to perform better,
Functional Fixedness: This term refers to the tendency to view problems only in their
customary manner.
● Functional fixedness prevents people from fully seeing all of the different options that
might be available to find a solution.
● Mental set refers to our problem-solving strategies, whereas functional fixedness refers to
the way we think about physical objects. Specifically, functional fixedness means that we
tend to assign fixed functions to an object.
● As a result, we fail to think about the features of this object that might be useful in
helping us solve a problem ● Eg: Maier’s two string problem; Duncker’s candle problem
(Duncker, 1945).
● Functional fixedness can also be demonstrated in cultures with little experience using
manufactured objects.
● For example, German and Barrett (2005) showed some simple kitchen objects to
adolescents living near the Amazon River Ecuador.
● If the adolescents saw a spoon being used to stir rice,they later had difficulty imagining
that the spoon could also serve as a bridge between two other objects.
Gender stereotypes ● Our top-down processes may be overactive because stereotypes can
influence our beliefs about our own abilities (Walton & Dweck, 2009)
● The most widely researched topic is gender stereotypes that focus on problem solving in
mathematics. A typical gender stereotype is that men are more skilled than women in
solving mathematics problems.
● Janet Hyde e al. (2008) analyzed scores on standardized mathematics tests for 7,200,000
U.S. students. They found consistent gender similarities for students of all ages, from
second grade through eleventh grade.
● In one study, the researchers found gender similarities, even when the test required
students to solve complex math problems.
● The same pattern of gender similarities has been replicated.
● Gender Stereotype threat: If you belong to a group that is hampered by a negative
stereotype—and you think about your membership in that group—your performance may
suffer (Smith et al., 2007).
● Research with Asian American females: In North America, one stereotype is that Asian
Americans are ‘‘good at math,’’ compared to those from other ethnic groups. Shih et
al.(1999) divided these Asian American women into three different conditions.
● Ethnicity-emphasis condition: One group of participants were asked to indicate their
ethnicity and then answer several questions about their ethnic identity. Then they took a
challenging math test. These women answered 54%of the questions correctly. 2.
● Control-group condition: A second group of participants did not answer any questions
beforehand. They simply took the challenging math test. These women answered 49% of
the questions correctly.
● Gender- emphasis condition: A third group of participants were asked to indicate their
gender and then answer several questions about their gender identity. Then they took the
challenging math test. These women answered only 43% of the questions correctly.
● When Asian American women are reminded of their ethnicity, they perform relatively
well. However, when Asian American women are reminded of their gender, they may
experience stereotype threat, and their problem-solving ability can decline.
● Nalini Ambady et al. (2001) demonstrated this same pattern among Asian American girls
enrolled in elementary and middle school.
● Research with European American Females: The effects of stereotype threat have also
been replicated in samples where most of the women are European American (O’Brien &
Crandall, 2003; Quinn & Spencer, 2001).
● Explanation of gender stereotype Threat
● Two factors probably contribute to the problem.
○ One factor is that stereotype threat can produce high arousal. High arousal is
likely to interfere with working memory, especially on difficult tasks. Research
shows that people may ‘‘choke under pressure’’ on a challenging math test.
○ A second factor is that females who are taking a difficult math test may work hard
to suppress the thought that they are supposed to perform poorly. Thought
suppression requires great effort, which might reduce the capacity of working
memory even further.
● ● Quinn and Spencer (2001) proposed that these factors decrease women’s abilities to
construct problem-solving strategies.
● Researchers have examined numerous gender comparisons in mathematical problem
solving.
● In general, these studies show gender similarities in problem-solving skills (Quinn and
Spencer; 2001).
● The studies also show that female students may earn lower scores if they receive
messages that females are less competent than males in mathematics.Factors influencing
problem solving:
Irrelevant or Misleading Information: When you are trying to solve a problem, it is important
to distinguish between information that is relevant to the issue and irrelevant data that can lead to
faulty solutions.
● Assumptions: When dealing with a problem, people often make assumptions about the
constraints and obstacles that prevent certain solutions. Eg: the surgeon problem, twin
problem
● Confirmation bias: Tendency to look for evidence that is in synchronicity with one’s
beliefs (Religious people/atheism) Tendency to selectively look for information that
conforms to our hypothesis and to overlook information that argues against it. This effect
was demonstrated by Wason (1960)