Developmentalpsychologyresearch SAchapter
Developmentalpsychologyresearch SAchapter
net/publication/267573786
CITATION READS
1 9,612
1 author:
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Catriona Ida Macleod on 31 October 2014.
CHAPTER
??.1 Introduction
The questions that form the basis for this chapter are:
• What are the theoretical frameworks utilised by South African
researchers and authors in developmental psychology?
• How are these theories put to work to highlight issues in people’s
lives in the South African context?
• What are some of the criticisms that could be levelled at the
theories used?
These questions are important in the light of the dominance of Euro-American
research in our textbooks and many developmental psychology courses.
It must be made clear from the outset, however, that this chapter is not
intended as a comprehensive review of developmental psychology research
and literature in South Africa, for two reasons. Firstly, putting boundaries
around what counts as developmental psychology, and what does not, proves
to be difficult. For example, collecting research on children only is not
satisfactory, given the life-span developmental theories. Focussing on work
that specifically studies individual development is also not adequate, given the
emphasis on the meso-, exo- and chrono-systems of Bronfenbrenner’s (1979)
approach. Asking the questions ‘Which studies concerning children,
adolescents, adults, and the aged are relevant, and which not?’ and ‘Which
studies on the family, the school, race, class, cultural issues etc. are relevant
and which not?’ becomes a tedious, and perhaps not very useful task.
Secondly, researchers in South Africa, contrary to popular belief, are relatively
prolific. Collating and summarising all the research in developmental
psychology (whatever the boundaries decided upon) exceeds our current
scope. In this chapter, therefore, we shall take a broad view of the field in the
last fifteen years, discussing the main theoretical trends and illustrating each
with examples of research or theoretical writing.
In structuring this chapter I have utilised Overton and Reese’s (1973, cited in
Widdershoven 1997) distinction between mechanistic and organismic
models of development as well as Lerner’s (1986) and Widdershoven’s (1997)
extension of this to the contextual and narrative models respectively.
In 1973, Overton and Reese identified two basic metaphors or models that
underlay developmental psychology theorising of the time. What they meant
by this is that all the theories of human development could broadly be divided
into two categories in terms of their underlying philosophical assumptions
about the nature of development and the nature of the developing person.
These two categories they called mechanistic and organismic (more detail
concerning what is meant by each of these is supplied in the relevant sections
below). In response to further developments in the field, Lerner (1986)
introduced a further category, the contextual model. At a later stage,
Widdershoven (1997) discusses a narrative approach to developmental
psychology. Although Widdershoven (1997) introduces an important new
element to the broad understanding of the basic models underlying
developmental theorising, his use of the word ‘narrative’ is underinclusive.
Narrative theory is just one approach within many broadly identified with the
social constructionist movement in psychology. Thus, for the fourth model
we shall propose a social constructionist model.
FIGURE 1 HERE
The mechanistic model can lend itself to treating socio-political issues merely
as variables that should be measured in terms of their impact on the
individual’s development. It is possible, within this model, for the complexities
of these issues to be glossed over. Race, class, gender, ethnicity and so forth
may become homogenised and essentialised through being measured as a
variable.
Some South African authors using this model have, however, attempted to
grapple with contextual and political issues. For example, Panday et al.
(2007, 208), in justifying their use of ethnicity as a variable, state that ‘the
history of Apartheid in SA (sic) means that poverty and inequality continue to
exhibit strong spatial and racial biases … Consequently ethnicity has become
a proxy for social, economic, spatial and cultural differences when these
factors are difficult to estimate’. They found that the strength of the
‘determinants’ of smoking differed amongst ethnic groups. This led them to
call for further research to understand the influence of differing social,
economic and cultural contexts on smoking onset, but also to acknowledge
the political difficulties potentially associated with their research. They
conceded that ‘the history of racial segregation and discrimination in SA
makes recommendations for ethnic-specific school-based programmes
undesirable’ (Panday et al 2007, 215). As we shall see throughout this
chapter, how contextual issues are conceptualised and theorised in
Developmental Psychology is something that needs to be carefully
considered.
This type of stage theorising has a number of implications. The first is that it
allows for the development of tools to measure the appropriateness of a
particular individual’s development as calibrated against the norms of others
in the same developmental stage. For example, Herbst and Huysamen
(2000) report on their development and validation of a set of developmental
scales for what they call ‘environmentally disadvantaged’ pre-school children.
The purpose of the scales is to assess these children’s ‘mastery of selected
cognitive and motor developmental tasks’ (Herbst and Huysamen 2000,19).
One of the potential difficulties with organismic models is that, because of the
emphasis on internal factors, researchers may ignore the political, social,
gendered and cultural context within which development is taking place. This
has certainly been the case in some earlier South African research (see, for
example, Ackerman 1990). However, this type of de-contextualisation is not
necessarily a feature of research that utilises organismic frames. For
example, Swartz (2007, 361), using psychoanalytic theory, argues that the
Oedipal stage is pivotal in terms of children’s awareness of ‘racial differences
and their effects on class, privilege and custom’. She emphasizes the
variability of developmental pathways (recall the principle of equifinality
discussed above) and ‘their construction in powerful social, economic and
political contexts’.
Organismic models that take context into account inevitably bump up against
the universalism versus relativism debate. For example, Tudin, Straker and
Mendolsohn (1994) investigate the relationship between Kohlberg’s stages of
moral development and exposure to political and social complexity amongst a
group of South African university students. They accept Kohlberg’s assertion
that there are universal principles that guide moral reasoning and that there
are basic, invariant developmental stages in moral reasoning, although their
research revolves around the influence of social context on the development
of this moral reasoning. Ferns and Thom (2001, 38), on the other hand,
present their findings in a study on the moral development of white and black
teenagers as ‘evidence against cultural universality in Kohlberg’s theory’.
They argue that the influence of norms and values, parenting styles and
historical and political effects mitigate against a stage-like progression and
identical endpoints of morality.
We shall now discuss approaches that fall under the broad banner of
contextual models. The first is a positivist approach. In many respects
positivism may be classed as mechanistic as it isolates various elements and
explores the relationship between them. However, the research I discuss
here all has one key feature, and that is a commitment to understanding the
influence of contextual issues on children’s responses. Other approaches
discussed include a developmental-contextual approach, cultural psychology,
a public health perspective, and a political approach.
Positivist research allows for comparative work. This is different to the notion
of generalisability discussed above. Researchers conducting comparative
research are not necessarily interested in establishing universal laws or
truths. They may rather want to provide an in-depth description of two groups
identified as different in some ways and as similar in others. A good example
of this is Liddell’s (1996) research on the interpretations of six pictures by 80
South African and 80 British children in their second and third year of
schooling. Liddell (1996) starts her paper by exploring how pictures may: 1)
provide a bridge into literacy for children, 2) enrich the meaning of texts, 3)
provide contextual information in text, and 4) assist children in retaining
information. She reviews the literature from developed countries in which it is
shown that children follow a predictable developmental sequence in their
picture interpretation skills. But then she asks the question whether the same
patterns ‘manifest themselves in children from homes where literacy skills
amongst parents are poorly developed, where picture books do not exist,
where teacher:child ratios mean that one teacher assists 40 to 50 children in
the classroom, and where children at school are exposed to – at most – four
illustrated readers in a year’ (Liddell 1996, 356). This question is important as
it provides the framework within which the comparative research is located.
Here Liddell (1996) invokes structural-contextual issues, not to explain the
differences noted, but to frame her question. Her results show differences in
the way rural South African and rural British children interpret pictures, as well
as different patterns of change as the two groups progress in school. She
discusses this in the light of the possible different functions of literacy in the
two communities.
One of the potential difficulties with positivist research is that too little critical
analysis of how terms are operationalised is entered into by researchers.
For example, Cherian and Malehase (2000) investigate the relationship
between parental control and children’s scholastic achievement. They state
that an ‘objective estimate’ of parental control was obtained via a
questionnaire. The questionnaire items, we are informed, measured ‘parental
order and control of children, parental supervision of daily activities of
children, parental involvement in proper control and supervision of school
tasks, parental time spent on children’s school work, and parental
communication with their children’ (2000, 666). The actual items of the
questionnaire are not provided so we are not able to judge exactly what
questions elicited responses in these various areas. Nevertheless, there is no
indication that the choice of words such as ‘proper control and supervision’,
‘parental order’, ‘parental involvement’ is political, and implies the valuing of
particular parental activities over others. Instead these practices take on the
aspect of naturalness - correct and good parental actions. This point is taken
up by Rose (1989), who points out that scientific and professional descriptions
of good parental practices gain their power by appearing to be universally
valid and natural.
This point is clearly illustrated in two papers about related issues - child
neglect and child abuse and their respective effects in terms of child
development. In the first, Du Preez, Naudé and Pretorius (2004) research the
influence of neglect on language development. They found that the neglected
children in their sample had delays in terms of verbal development. They
postulate that this is owing to a lack of interaction and communication
between the parents or caregivers and children. However, exactly how this
external event leads to the internal one is left untheorised. What are the
cognitive, emotional, social and neurological processes involved in moving
from the one to the other?
It is this static and essentialist view of culture that cultural psychology has
attempted to counter, while still maintaining the explanatory power that may
be gained by considering cultural issues in developmental psychology.
Gilbert, Van Vlaenderen & Nkwinti (1995), for example, locate their research
within cultural psychology. They study the role of local knowledge in the
process of socialisation in rural families. They define local knowledge as ‘the
presuppositions used to interpret immediate experience borne out of action in
the local environment’ (p. 229). This conceptualisation illustrates the dynamic
nature of a cultural psychology perspective, in that local knowledge is a
product of day-to-day actions and hence is constantly being constructed and
re-constructed while still having a historicity. Contrast this to the notion of
‘traditional knowledge’, which has the connotation of stasis and preservation.
Given the above, there is a strong public health focus in many South African
writers’ work in developmental psychology. This acknowledges the fact that
general health issues and children’s development are strongly interconnected.
For example, Richter (2004) provides a thorough review of the psychosocial
impact of HIV/Aids on children’s development and adjustment.
Public health shifts the definition of health away, firstly, from an individual
focus and, secondly, from something attended to by medical practitioners in
clinics and hospitals. Instead, public health ‘targets all points where matter,
energy, and information are exchanged between people and their human,
social, and physical environments, for it is through this exchange that
individual and group health status is determined’ (Butchart & Kruger, 2001, p.
215). Duncan (1997), for example, illustrates how the causes of malnutrition,
a condition linked to poor developmental outcomes, should be located not in
individuals’ shortcomings (e.g. parental ignorance concerning nutrition), but
rather in broader social processes. Combating malnutrition will, according to
Duncan (1997), require broad-ranging interventions, including employment
generation programmes and projects aimed at making diversified nutrition and
basic health facilities available to all.
Much of the public health debate is framed within the human rights discourse.
Authors draw on documents such as the United Nations Declaration of
Children’s Rights (Duncan, 1997), and the World Health Organisation and
South African government documents on disability rights (Van Niekerk, 1997).
Strong arguments for the recognition of the rights and aspirations of groups
marginalised by developmental psychology and government policy decisions
(such as children with mental handicap – Parekh & Jackson, 1997) are made.
In an edited collection, entitled ‘Monitoring child well-being’ and containing
chapters on a wide range of issues relating to children, a strong rights-based
approach is taken (Dawes, Bray and van der Merwe 2007). The editors
explain that there are three stages of measurement required in this approach.
Firstly, the specification of rights and what the state and others are duty
bound to deliver; secondly, provision through policy and programmes to
deliver these rights; and thirdly, the measurement of child outcomes in relation
to a minimum standards, models of cause and effect, and the opinions of
children and their carers and service providers (Bray and Dawes 2007). Thus
a rights-based approach is intricately linked to advocacy for delivery of
services and interventions, and the evaluation of these services.
The risk versus resilience debate has, mostly, been framed within a public
health discourse. On the risk side of the debate, factors that put children at
risk for the development of particular problems are analysed. This is
frequently done with the aim of prevention (one of the fundamental purposes
of primary health care). The rationale is that if we can identify risk factors,
then we can, perhaps, do something to prevent them. Van der Merwe and
Dawes (2007) take this approach. They review the risk factors for the
development of violent and antisocial behaviour, as well as the developmental
pathways along which violent and antisocial behaviour may manifest itself.
Arguing that interventions need to based on theoretical and empirical
evidence, the authors review the common characteristics of effective violence-
prevention and treatment interventions.
A strictly risk approach was questioned in the mid-80s (Rutter, 1985). Since
then, the idea of resilience in the face of adversity has become popular. The
key reasoning here is that some children, despite difficult circumstances,
manage to cope well and do not develop any problems. For example,
Henderson (2006, 303) argues that ‘too narrow a focus on the vulnerabilities
of AIDS orphans obscures the ways in which they share similar circumstances
with other poor children, as well as the strengths they bring to bear on their
circumstances’.
The resilience thesis has gained a fair amount of credence in South Africa in
the light of the poor socio-economic conditions and the political violence that
characterises many South African children’s young lives. Instead of seeing
children as victims of their circumstances (i.e. at risk for the development of a
range of psycho-social and physical disorders), the resilience hypothesis
allows researchers to emphasise positive aspects of children’s environments
as well as the children’s agency in developing coping mechanisms.
While these sorts of papers are important, there are also potential dangers.
For example, Stevens and Lockhat’s (1997) conclusion rests on an uncritical
usage of Erikson’s theory. They indicate that Erikson’s concept of
adolescence as a psychosocial moratorium (a period in which society allows
adolescents to experiment with various identities) does not apply to the
majority of black adolescents. Furthermore, in choosing between capitulation
and assimilation into the dominant (white) culture versus radicalisation, the
authors see black adolescents as experiencing what Erikson called identity
foreclosure. Instead of problematising the theoretical concepts of
psychosocial moratorium and identity foreclosure as linked to particular socio-
historical circumstances, the authors accept their legitimacy and utilise them
to suggest that there are ‘potentially negative psychological consequences’
(1997, 252) and a ‘long-term impact’ (1997, 253) associated with the lack of a
psychosocial moratorium and with identity foreclosure. The net result is the
(probably unintended) pathologisation of black adolescents. Aware of this
potential, the authors spend some time discussing the debate on risk versus
resilience in South Africa, stating that adolescents should not merely be seen
as victims. However, their own theorising allows for little more, and merely
asserting resilience does not do the trick.
Examples of the first contribution are to be found Parekh and Jackson (1997)
and Shefer (1997). Parekh and Jackson (1997, 41) argue that ‘children with
mental handicap are subjected to, constrained and marginalised by
psychological developmentalist talk’. They question the assumption that
mental handicap means the same thing for one group as for another, as well
as the prioritisation of the cognitive and intellectual as hallmarks of childhood
development. Shefer (1997) discusses how developmental psychology’s
approach to gender ignores the social, historical and political context of
gendered identity development and presents development from a male
perspective. She uses a social constructionist perspective to analyse how
gender (as a social construction) has an impact on our development from the
moment we are identified as male or female.
??.6 Conclusion
From the above it is clear that a range of theoretical approaches and models
have informed developmental psychology research in South Africa. Two
important questions are: Why is it important to take stock of our theoretical
orientations? Why is it vital that a forum for the discussion of theory in
developmental psychology remains open when there are clearly pressing
issues facing children and adolescents (as well as parents, adults and the
elderly) in South Africa? I hope that the answers to these questions are at
least partially provided in this chapter. But to summarise, the basic
philosophical and theoretical assumptions that we make have implications in
terms of 1) how we view the nature of the developing person; 2) what factors
we consider in our research and how we conceptualise their linkages; 3) the
questions we ask in conducting our research; 4) how we undertake our
research; 5) the usages we envisage for our research; and finally 6) how
interventions in the lives of children, adolescents, and parents proceed. This
latter point is made very strongly in a book edited by Donald, Dawes and
Louw (2000), in which they discuss various community-based programmes
that have attempted to address adversity in children’s lives.
2). Do you think that South African developmental psychologists should take
an overtly political stance in their work, or do you think that there is a place for
scientific neutrality and objectivity? Is there a middle ground? Do you think
there is a way of integrating the two stances, and if so, how?
3). Do you think that ‘culture’ should be included in our thinking about
developmental psychology? If so, how should it be conceptualised?
Acknowledgements
Thank you to Tracy Morison for invaluable assistance in gathering the
literature for this chapter and for organising the references.
References
Aase, H. Meyer, A. & Sagvolden, T. (2006), Moment-to-moment dynamics of
ADHD behaviour in South African children. Behavioral and Brain Functions,
2(11). Retrieved 01/25/08 from
http://www.behavioralandbrainfunctions.com/content/2/1/11
Ackerman, C. J. (1990). Identiteitsontwikkeling tydens adolessensie: ‘n groep
sekondêre skool-leerlinge se evaluering van die belangrikheid van
lewensterreine. South African Journal of Education, 10,(4), 283-290.
Amod, Z., Cockcroft, K. Soellaart, B. (2007). Use of the 1996 Griffiths Mental
Development Scales for infants: a pilot study with a Black, South African
sample Journal of Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 19(2), 123–130.
Barbarin, O.A. & Richter, L. (2001). Economic status, community danger and
psychological problems among South African children. Childhood: A Global
Journal of Child Research, 8(1), 115-133.
Cherian, V.I. & Malehase, M.C. (2000). The relationship between parental
control and scholastic achievement of children from single- and two-parent
families. The Journal of Social Psychology, 140 (5), 665-667.
Crossley, M.L. (2000). Introducing narrative psychology: self, trauma and the
construction of meaning. Buckingham: Open University Press.
Dawes, A., Bray, R. and van der Merwe, A. (eds) (2007) Monitoring child
well-being: A South African rights-based approach. Cape Town: HSRC
Press.
Dawes, A., Bray, R. and van der Merwe, A. (2007). Monitoring child well-
being: A South African rights-based approach. Cape Town: HSRC Press.
Ferns, I. & Thom, D. (2001). Moral development of black and white South
African adolescents: evidence against cultural universality in Kohlberg's
theory. South African Journal of Psychology, 31(4), 38 – 47.
Hook, D., Mkhize, N., Kiguwa, P., Collins, A., Burman, E. & Parker, I. (eds)
(2004). Critical Psychology. Lansdowne: UCT Press.
Kritzas, N., & Grobler, A.A. (2005). The relationship between perceived
parenting styles and resilience during adolescence. Journal of Child and
Adolescent Mental Health, 17(1), 1–12
Laubscher, L. & Klinger, J. (1997). Story and the making of the self. In C. De
la Rey, N. Duncan, T. Shefer & A. Van Niekerk (Eds), Contemporary issues in
human development: a South African focus (pp. 58-79). Johannesburg:
International Thomson Publishing.
Liddell, C. (1996). Every picture tells a story: South African and British
children interpreting pictures. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
14, 355-363.
Liddell, C., Lycett, J. & Gordon, R. (1997). Getting through Grade 2: predicting
children’s early school achievement in rural South African schools.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21(2), 331-348.
Mathews, C., Aarø, L.E., Flisher, A.J., Mukoma, W., Wubs, A.G., & Schaalma
H. (2008). Predictors of early first sexual intercourse among adolescents in
Cape Town, South Africa. Health Education. Retrieved 10/25/08 from
http://her.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/reprint/cym079v1.
Meyer, A., Eilertsen, D., Sundet, J.M., Tshifularo, J., & Sagvolden, T. (2004).
Cross-cultural similarities in ADHD-like behaviour amongst South African
primary school children. South African Journal of Psychology, 34,(1), 122-138
Panday, S., Reddy, S.P., Ruiter, R.A.C., Bergstrom, E., & De Vries, H. (2007).
Determinants of smoking among adolescents in the southern Cape-Karoo
region, South Africa. Health promotion international, 22(3):207-217.
Parkes, J. (2002). “Children also have rights, but then who wants to
listen to our rights?” Children’s Perspectives on Living with
Community Violence in South Africa. Educate, 2(2), 59 – 71.
Richards, K.A.M., Pillay, Y., Mazodze, O., Govere, A.S.M. (2005). The impact
of colonial culture in South Africa and Zimbabwe on identity development.
Journal of Psychology in Africa, 15(1), 41-51.
Rose, N. (1989). Governing the soul: The shaping of the private self.
London: Routledge.
van der Merwe, A., & Dawes, A. (2007). Youth violence: A review of risk
factors, causal pathways and effective intervention. Journal of Child and
Adolescent Mental Health, 19(2), 95–113
Outcome beliefs
X Attitude to the
Outcome behaviour
evaluation
Control beliefs
X Perceived
Perceived power behavioural
of control factor control
Source: Modified from Ajzen & Madden (1986) by Morojele (1997, p. 223)