Psychopathy and Attachment
Psychopathy and Attachment
PII: S1359-1789(18)30043-0
DOI: doi:10.1016/j.avb.2018.09.002
Reference: AVB 1234
To appear in: Aggression and Violent Behavior
Received date: 18 February 2018
Revised date: 15 August 2018
Accepted date: 14 September 2018
Please cite this article as: Marion van der Zouwen, Machteld Hoeve, Anne M. Hendriks,
Jessica J. Asscher, Geert Jan J.M. Stams , The association between attachment and
psychopathic traits. Avb (2018), doi:10.1016/j.avb.2018.09.002
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As
a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The
manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before
it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may
be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the
journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Running head: ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS
PT
RI
The association between attachment and psychopathic traits
SC
Marion van der Zouwen, Machteld Hoeve, Anne M. Hendriks, Jessica J. Asscher,
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to examine the association between attachment and
independent samples), containing 133 effect sizes based on in total 1,876 participants. Results
overall association of r = .18, indicating that insecure attachment was positively related to
PT
psychopathic traits. We also tested moderator effects; six significant moderators were found.
RI
First, the magnitude of the effect size depended on which psychopathic trait was investigated;
SC
the largest effect size was found for callous-unemotional traits (r = .23), and no effect was
found for antisocial behavior, narcissism and impulsivity. Second, parent- and caregiver
NU
reports on psychopathic traits resulted in stronger associations compared to self-reports and a
composite of multiple sources. Third and fourth, the association varied across attachment and
MA
psychopathic traits instruments. Fifth, if both attachment and psychopathic traits were
reported by the same informant, the association was stronger than when attachment and
D
psychopathic traits were reported by different informants. Sixth, the association differed
E
across sample setting, with the strongest association for a clinical setting followed by a
PT
secured setting and no association for both community or combined settings. No moderating
CE
effects were found for the attachment categories, attachment to whom and the informant of
attachment, publication year, and sample characteristics, such as gender, age, and sample size.
AC
We discuss the implications of our findings for future research and clinical practice.
Nowadays, attachment theory and John Bowlby are inseparable (e.g., Craig, Gray, &
Snowden, 2013; MacDonald, Berlow, & Thomas, 2013; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2012; Savage,
2014). Development of attachment theory started during World War II, when he conducted
his first empirical study (Bowlby, 1944). In that study, Bowlby examined forty-four juvenile
thieves at the London Child Guidance Clinic. Through detailed examination, he was able to
PT
link their histories of maternal deprivation and separation to their symptoms. Fourteen of
RI
these thieves were described by Bowlby as affectionless, indicating that they lacked normal
SC
affection or warmth of feeling for anyone, shame or sense of responsibility. Bowlby
discovered that none of them had ever shown normal affection to anyone, and that they were
NU
unresponsive, solitary and undemonstrative. Many of their parents and foster parents
remembered that nothing they said or did to the children made any difference, because they
MA
responded neither to punishment nor to kindness. The children themselves all had a very
similar history of early separations from their (foster) mothers. Attachment theory was later
D
conceived, among other factors, to explain the relation between lack of warm and continuous
E
Almost at the same time, Hervey Cleckley was among the first to attempt to formalize a
CE
maltreatment and poor parenting were not causes of psychopathy, but Cleckley’s delineation
AC
of the psychopath was comparable to Bowlby’s description of the affectionless. In his book
“The Mask of Sanity”, Cleckley (1941; p. 24, 1988) described psychopathy as a disorder with
relations”, and ”pathologic egocentricity and incapacity for love”. Despite the seemingly
opposite views of Bowlby and Cleckley about the role of a disruptive early environment in the
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 4
origins of both psychopathy and insecure attachment, decades ago both scholars assumed a
Attachment
This theory assumes that the etiology of psychopathy may be related to poor attachment
(Bailey & Shelton, 2014; Craig et al., 2013; Schimmenti et al., 2014). Attachment is a
PT
biologically-based developmental system that regulates proximity of the child to its
RI
caregiver(s) from infancy on. It serves to protect children from danger and to provide children
SC
with the opportunity to safely develop and explore their environment.
attachment behaviors of the child, e.g., crying) provides the child with a safe haven and a
secure base. In return, the child can explore the environment and seek comfort and contact
D
with its caregiver in times of distress, a so-called secure attachment (Ainsworth, Blehar,
E
Waters, & Wall, 1978). When caregivers are insensitive or inconsistently sensitive, they will
PT
not be perceived as a secure base and a safe haven. Their children are described as insecurely
CE
in their response to their infant’s distress. They seem to be more attuned to their own needs
AC
than to their child‘s needs. Anxious/ resistant children seem unable to be comforted by their
caregiver in times of distress. On the contrary, avoidantly (A) attached children seem
indifferent or ignorant to their caregivers and do not seem to show any need of comfort. This
minimization of emotions seems to be a response to the consistent rejection by the parent. The
avoidant behavior allows the child to maintain sufficient proximity to the caregiver in times of
Some children behaved so inexplicably and contradictory during the SSP that the
observers were unable to classify the attachment pattern in one of the above categories.
(Cicchetti & Doyle, 2016; Guttmann-Steinmetz & Crowell, 2006). In addition to this four
PT
(i.e., anxiety and avoidance) model, albeit still leading to the same four quadrants as
RI
mentioned above.
SC
The association between attachment difficulties and mental disorders in general is well
documented (Beeney et al., 2015; Cicchetti & Doyle, 2016; Bogaerts, Buschman, Kunst, &
NU
Winkel, 2010; Fearon, Bakermans- Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn, Lapsey, & Roisman, 2010;
Mikulincer & Shaver, 2012; Timmerman & Emmelkamp, 2006). Meta-analyses confirm
MA
Fearon, Van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Roisman, 2016; MacDonald, Berlowb, &
D
Thomas, 2013; Madigan, Atkinson, Laurin, & Benoit, 2013; Madigan, Brumariu, Villani,
E
Atkinson, & Lyons-Ruth, 2016). Meta-analyses and reviews examining the relation between
PT
attachment and externalizing disorders show also an association (Fearon et al., 2010; Groh et
CE
al., 2017; Hoeve et al., 2012; Madigan et al., 2016; Savage, 2014; Schimmenti et al., 2014),
but not always as consistent as for internalizing disorders (Fearon & Belsky, 2011).
AC
Psychopathic traits
First of all, it is important to notice that there is a difference between psychopaths and
individuals with psychopathic traits. In the current study, psychopathic traits include
antisocial behavior, narcissism, impulsivity and callous-unemotional traits, based on the factor
analyses performed by Hare and colleagues (e.g., 2003). They concluded that the construct of
psychopathy consists of two main, partially independent, dimensions. One dimension includes
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 6
the emotional and interpersonal traits, such as superficial charm (also called as narcissism),
lack of guilt and empathy, and affective deficits, the so-called callous-unemotional (CU)
traits. The other dimension captures impulsive and antisocial behavior (Dadds, Jambrak,
Pasalich, Hawes, & Brennan, 2011; Dadds & Salmon, 2003; Salekin, 2002). The concurrent
presence of both dimensions differentiates those with psychopathy from people suffering from
Antisocial Personality Disorder, which includes only the latter, and people with psychopathic
PT
traits but without antisocial behavior. Only individuals with psychopathic traits who also
RI
exhibit antisocial behavior are considered to be psychopaths (Blair, 2001; Farrington, 2005).
SC
Coid, Yang, Ullrich, Roberts and Hare (2009) found a prevalence of 29.2 % of people
exhibiting one or more psychopathic traits in a general population, whereas only 0.6% of their
NU
sample could be labeled as being a psychopath.
Furthermore, with regard to children and adolescents, the term psychopathy should
MA
personality pattern, whereas children are still in the progress of character development (Kerig
D
& Wenar, 2006). Therefore, in the present study, psychopathic traits comprise both overall
E
Research on psychopathic traits has suggested a strong and robust link between
CE
behavior later in life (Asscher et al., 2011; Fite, Greening, & Stoppelbein, 2008; Fontaine,
AC
McCrory, Boivin,Moffitt, & Viding, 2011; Wilkinson, Waller, & Viding , 2016). This link
has stimulated a growing body of research examining causes and correlates of psychopathic
traits as early as possible in child development (e.g., Carlson. Oshri, & Kwon, 2015;
Risk factors for psychopathic traits are diverse. Sometimes, the development of CU
al., 2015; Daversa, 2010; Gao, Raine, Chan, Venables, & Mednick, 2010; Hicks et al., 2012;
Kimonis, Fanti, Isoma, & Donghue, 2013; Kolla et al., 2013), negative parental discipline
(Fontaine et al., 2011), low parental warmth (Bisby, Kimonis, & Goulter, 2017; Kimonis,
Cross, Howard, & Donoghue, 2012; Kroneman, Hipwell, Loeber, Koot, & Pardini, 2011),
parental indifference (Bailey & Shelton, 2014), insensitive parenting (Wagner, Mills-Koonce,
Willoughby, Zvara, & Cox, 2015), harsh parenting (Waller et al., 2012), emotional abuse
PT
(Schimmenti, Di Carlo, Passanisi, & Caretti, 2015), negative parenting (Waller, Gardner, &
RI
Hyde, 2013), or attachment difficulties (Fite et al., 2008; Frodi, Dernevik, Sepa, Philipson, &
SC
Bragesjö, 2001).
Tuvblad, Bezdjian, Raine, & Baker, 2014), because of the heritability of CU traits (Bezdjian,
MA
Raine, Baker, & Lynam,2011; Hicks et al., 2012; Larsson, Viding, & Plomin, 2008; Viding et
al., 2013) and maternal prenatal risks (Barker, Oliver, Viding, Salekin, & Maughan, 2011).
D
Also neurological alterations of the amygdala and the surrounding structures (Beaver, Vaugh,
E
DeLisi, Barnes, & Boutwell, 2012; Blair, Colledge, Murray, & Mitchell, 2001; Boccardi et
PT
al., 2011; Craig et al., 2009; Herpers, Scheepers, Bons, Buitelaar, & Rommelse, 2014), are
CE
mentioned as possible risk factors, for instance caused by early adverse parenting (Daversa,
2010), and leading to emotion recognition dysfunction, especially for the recognition of
AC
negative emotions of others (Bowen, Morgan, Moore, & Van Goozen, 2013). Subsequently,
the dispositional risks could explain the fact that the CU dimension of psychopathy has been
found to be fairly stable over time from infancy to adulthood (Andershed, 2010; Beaver et al.,
2012; Lynam et al., 2009 ). Furthermore, after early childhood, their effects persist regardless
of the quality of parenting (Beaver, Rowland, Schwartz, & Nedelec, 2011; Farrington, 2005;
parent-child attachment, contemporary research on this theme is scarce. McCord and McCord
(1956) concluded that emotional deprivation, parental rejection, and lack of affection lead to
the development of a psychopathic personality in general and to the core affective deficits in
particular. Furthermore, Bowlby (1970) emphasized the importance of rejecting mothers and
PT
maternal deprivation for the development of psychopathic traits. Since then, several studies
RI
have documented an association between attachment and psychopathy and different potential
SC
explanations for this association have been put forward. For instance, delinquents high on
psychopathy report a childhood with separations, maltreatment, or abuse and are subsequently
NU
insecurely attached (Bailey & Shelton, 2014; Frodi et al., 2001; Schimmenti et al., 2014).
Nevertheless, it could also be the other way around. Parent-child relationships, such as
attachment, develop as a result of complex interactions between parents and their children.
D
CU traits are likely to impede these relationships with caregivers, so attachment difficulties
E
could also be seen as child-driven (Larsson et al., 2008; Hawes, Dadds, Frost, & Hasking,
PT
2011). For example, researchers found that children with CU traits fail to attend to the eyes of
CE
attachment figures in general (Dadds, Allen, et al., 2014; Dadds et al., 2011), due to a
dysfunction in the neural systems necessary for emotional recognition (Blair, Peschardt,
AC
Budhani, Mitchell, & Pine, 2006; Bowen et al., 2013; Dawel, O’Kearney, McKone, &
Children with CU traits are seen as relatively fearless and insensitive to punishment
(Barker et al., 2011; Dadds & Salmon, 2003; Hawes, et al., 2011). Subsequently, these traits
pose a difficult parenting task: too gentle parenting will not be noted (Kochanska, 1995), and
increasing the amount of applied power will only promote resentment and anger, and external
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 9
attributions for conduct problems (Dadds & Salmon, 2003; Kochanska, Barry, Stellern, &
O’Bleness, 2009). Both ways will not lead to internalization of rules nor stimulate conscience-
driven behavior and in all probability to the formation of an insecure attachment. Moreover,
children high on CU traits are found to be at increased risk for experiencing disruptions in
parent-child attachment (Pasalich, Dadds, Hawes, & Brennan, 2012). In line with these two
suppositions, Hawes and colleagues (2011) found a bidirectional effect for both CU traits and
PT
parenting. CU traits uniquely accounted for changes in inconsistent discipline, punishment,
RI
and parental involvement (e.g., also Larsson et al., 2008). Likewise, positive parenting,
SC
parental involvement and poor monitoring uniquely predicted change in CU traits.
resulted in inconsistent findings. For example, Fite and colleagues (2008) found a relation
MA
between attachment difficulties and CU and narcissistic traits. Some scholars have found
insecure attachment to be associated with only the affective factor of psychopathy (Catchpole,
D
2009), or psychopathy in general (Mack, Hackney, & Pyle, 2011), whereas others concluded
E
that they could not find any relation at all between attachment and psychopathy (Brennan &
PT
Shaver, 1998; Frodi et al., 2001; Holmqvist, 2008; Russell, 2004) or only for attachment to
CE
fathers, but not for attachment to mothers (Flight & Forth, 2007). Thus, the discussion about
the relation between attachment difficulties and psychopathic traits has not been resolved yet.
AC
a meta-analysis could shed light on this association and on potential explanations for the
variety in findings. However, apart from the literature reviews by Saltaris (2002), and Bailey
and Shelton (2014), a systematic review or meta-analysis that relates attachment specifically
to psychopathic traits has not been conducted yet. Several studies have found that the
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 10
children with antisocial behavior, but without CU traits (e.g.,Wilkinson et al., 2016). It is thus
aim to improve interventions that target both CU traits and antisocial behavior.
The main aim of the current meta-analysis was to examine the association between
psychopathic traits and attachment, and because the literature distinguishes between
PT
psychopathic traits and psychopathy, to shed light on this distinction and its clinical
RI
implications. A second aim was to examine moderators that might influence the relation
SC
between attachment and psychopathic traits. A meta-analysis is designed to reveal findings
upon which studies agree and eventually divest spurious findings (Dawel et al., 2012).
NU
Advantages of a meta-analysis are more reliable estimates of effect sizes and increased
statistical power. Because of the inconsistency in the literature about the role of attachment in
MA
the development of psychopathic traits and vice versa, we aim to disentangle patterns
Method
E
Sample of Studies
PT
We aimed to include all studies on the relation between attachment and psychopathic
CE
traits. The included studies were found in three consecutive steps. The only inclusion criterion
was that the study should describe an association between psychopathy or psychopathic traits
AC
and attachment to parents/caregivers. All sample types were included. The first step was to
search the database of Robert Hare. He has collected over 2000 studies that all focus on
psychopathy. The second step was a search through the computerized databases Web of
Premier, and Google Scholar for articles, books, chapters, paper presentations, dissertations
and reviews. Combinations of the following key words were used: psychopathy, psychopathic
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 11
traits, callous-unemotional and CU, cross-referenced with attachment. All found titles,
abstracts, and full-text articles were screened and excluded if they violated the inclusion
criterion. The literature search included all studies that were found until May, 2016. In the
third step, studies were located using the snowball method, that is, inspection of the reference
sections of the articles, the narrative reviews and book chapters. Finally, 15 studies met the
inclusion criterion that studies had to examine the association between attachment to
PT
parents/caregivers and psychopathy or psychopathy-alike disorders.
RI
On closer inspection, three studies had to be excluded: One study measured teenage
mothers’ (insecure) attachment to their own caregivers and CU traits in their children
SC
(Phillips, 2004). Barb (2007) compared two groups of juveniles, with psychopathy and
NU
attachment both as independent discrete variables, but did not report the association between
these variables. Schimmenti and colleagues (2014) investigated the relation between
MA
unpublished, between 2001 and 2015, which examined 133 associations between attachment
E
were included, as were all kinds of different sample types, such as community samples,
CE
incarcerated juvenile offenders, adult and child treatment groups, juveniles in group homes, et
cetera. In total, data of 1,876 participants were analyzed in the present study. Sample sizes
AC
ranged from 14 (Frodi et al., 2001) to 957 (Buck, 2015) participants, with an average of 170
participants per dataset. The age of the participants ranged from age 0-48 with a mean age of
8.9 years. Table 1 presents the included studies and their main characteristics.
Potential moderators of the association between attachment and psychopathic traits were
assessment characteristics. Publication year and impact factor of the journal in which the
gender (males, females, or both), percentage of boys, sample setting (incarcerated, clinical,
Study characteristics for the associations between psychopathic traits and attachment
included all reported associations between psychopathic traits and attachment scores. For
PT
attachment we reported overall attachment, attachment coded as a ratio between avoidance
RI
and anxiety or coding in the categories A, B, C and D. Attachment measurements were coded
SC
as categorical (e.g., secure or insecure) or continuous (more or less insecure). With whom the
attachment relationship had been measured was categorized as follows: with father, mother,
NU
or parents /caregivers. For psychopathy we reported overall psychopathy, CU traits,
narcissistic traits, impulsivity, and antisocial behavior. Important to mention here is that only
MA
some of the studies that used the different versions of the Psychopathy Checklist (e.g., the
PCL: Youth Version (PCL:YV; Forth, Kosson, &Hare, 2003), or PCL-Screening Version
D
(PCL-SV; Hart, Cox, Hare, 1997)) included antisocial behavior in the construct of overall
E
psychopathy. For the other instruments, overall psychopathy was composed of the scores on
PT
instruments were coded: IPPA (Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment; Armsden &
AC
George, Kaplan, & Main, 1985) or other. See Table 1 for more information about the
measures. The eight different psychopathy instruments were coded: PCL (all versions),
Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD; Frick & Hare, 2001), or other. The assessment
methods for attachment were coded as self-reported, parent reported or reported by clinicians.
For psychopathic traits, some studies used different sources for the measurement, such as an
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 13
integration of self-reports, official data and observations, coded as a composite, next to self-
reports and parent/caregiver reports. Finally, we coded whether the same informants were
used to gain information on psychopathic traits and attachment (same informant bias).
developed. Possible moderators were coded by the first author according to this coding
scheme. In total, 38 % of the articles were randomly selected to be double coded both by the
PT
second and the third author to check for interrater reliability. The average intraclass
RI
correlation measuring correspondence between the coders was .95 on the coded variables. The
SC
lowest intraclass correlations between the three raters were for the variables attachment coded
as categories (ABCD) or dimensional, gender and sample setting; their values were .79, .87,
NU
and .87 respectively.
Analyses
MA
Publication bias. To test for publication bias, an Egger’s test was performed to
inspect whether the effect sizes were distributed symmetrically (Egger, Davey-Smith,
D
Schneider, & Minder, 1997). Next, a trim and fill analysis was performed to investigate
E
whether adding effect sizes to the right or the left side of the distribution influenced the mean
PT
estimate (e.g., underestimation or overestimation) of the overall effect size (Duval &
CE
Tweedie, 2000a, 2000b). These analyses were performed with a two-level random-effects
model.
AC
Overall effect size. To correct for dependency because of multiple effect sizes derived
from the same studies, a three-level random effects model was used for the analyses, so there
were three levels of variance (Field, 2001). The first level of variance was the sampling
variance around the estimated population effect size. The second level of variance was
between effect sizes within studies. The third level of variance was across effect sizes
between studies (Van den Noortgate, López-López, Marín-Martínez, & Sánchez-Meca, 2012).
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 14
We applied the procedure and R script described by Wibbelink and Assink (2015). Parameters
were estimated using a Restricted Maximum Likelihood method. The first step was to
calculate an overall estimate of the effect size. Of this overall estimate it was investigated
whether the variances at level 2 (within studies) and level 3 (between studies) significantly
deviated from zero by performing two log-likelihood ratio tests, to compare whether
constraining either the level 2 or level 3 variance deteriorated model fit compared to the
PT
unconstrained model. Next, it was established which percentage of the total variance was
RI
accounted for at each level. In addition, a check for possible outliers of the Fischer z scores of
SC
the effect sizes was conducted, but no outliers were found so just all raw data were used.
psychopathic traits. Furthermore, the best fit and likelihood of all possible 63 models was
MA
examined by checking the corrected AIC’s (Akaike Information Criterion) and the Akaike
weight (Anderson, 2007). The Akaike weight for a particular model can be regarded as the
D
probability that that model is the best model (Viechtbauer, 2016). Of the final model, it was
E
investigated whether the variance at level 2 and 3 significantly deviated from zero. Next, it
PT
was again established how much of the total variance was found at each level. Finally, it was
CE
calculated how much more of the proportion of variance was explained by adding moderators,
using the formulas of Cheung (2014). All analyses were performed in R with the metafor and
AC
Results
The mean estimate of the overall effect size of the association between attachment and
psychopathic traits was .18 and significantly deviated from zero, SEz = .06, t(132) = 3.04, p <
.001, 95% CI = .06 - .31. This estimate indicated that there was a small-to-medium but
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 15
significant association. There was a significant amount of heterogeneity in the effect sizes not
accounted for by the model, Q(132) = 861.55, p < .001. The variance at level 2, or the
variance between effect sizes within studies, was .02. Setting this variance to zero
significantly improved model fit compared to the unconstrained model, χ2(1) = 216.57, p <
.001, indicating a significant amount of variance at level 2. The variance at level 3, or the
variance between effect sizes, was .03. Setting this variance to zero also significantly
PT
improved model fit compared to the unconstrained model, χ2(1) = 75.06, p < .001, indicating a
RI
significant amount of variance at level 3. Of the total variance, 19.98% was at level 1, 36.58%
SC
was at level 2, and 43.43% was at level 3. Because the residual heterogeneity was significant,
Publication Bias
MA
The Egger’s test revealed that the distribution of the effect sizes was asymmetrical, z = -
3.67, p = .000. Performing a trim and fill analysis led to adding effect sizes on the right side
D
of the distribution. The effect size after the trim and fill analysis (z = 0.22) was further away
E
from zero than the effect size before the trim and fill analysis (z = 0.14), thereby posing no
PT
threat for possible overestimation. Figure 1 displays the original funnel plot of the effect sizes,
CE
whereas Figure 2 displays the funnel plot after the trim and fill procedure, therefore
visualizing how the distribution of the effect sizes would be if it was symmetrical.
AC
Moderator Analysis
The QM statistics for whether a moderator was significant and the QE statistics for the
residual heterogeneity of the discrete moderators are displayed in Table 2. The following
moderators were not significant: year of publication, impact factor, gender, percentage of
boys, age, sample size, attachment categories, attachment scale, attachment to whom and
attachment reported by whom. The moderators which psychopathic trait, psychopathic traits
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 16
reported by whom, attachment instrument, psychopathy instrument, sample setting and same
informant bias were significant. Coefficients for the significant univariate moderators are
displayed in Table 3.
Due to the fact that we found six significant moderators, with 63 possible combinations,
we used an information-theoretic approach to select the best fitting model (Anderson, 2007).
PT
The “best” model (the model with the lowest AICc, -124.49, and the highest Akaike weight,
RI
0.87) turned out to be a multivariate model, including all six significant univariate moderators
SC
(sample setting, psychopathic traits, attachment instrument, psychopathy instruments,
psychopathy reported by whom and informer bias). Effects of all moderators remained
NU
significant, QM (14) = 9.15, p <.001. A significant amount of residual heterogeneity remained
not accounted for by the model, QE(118)= 201.69, p <.001. Level-2 variance of the final
MA
model was .01. Setting this variance to zero significantly deteriorated model fit, χ2(16) =
43.52, p < .001, indicating that the level-2 variance significantly deviated from zero. Level-3
D
variance was .01. Setting this variance to zero did not deteriorate model fit, χ2(16) =.89, p =
E
.346, indicating that the variance did not deviate from zero. Of the total variance, 42.59% was
PT
at level 1, 23.89% was at level 2, and 33.52% was at level 3. Compared to the model without
CE
the moderators, at level 1 there was an increase of 22.60% in variance explained by the final
variance were explained by the final model. According to Cheung (2014), these negative
values should be truncated to zero, indicating that adding the moderators did not explain more
Discussion
The purpose of the present meta-analysis was to investigate the strength of the
moderators influenced the strength of this association. The present study is, to our knowledge,
the first secondary analysis to focus on the association between these constructs. On the basis
of theoretical models (e.g., Farrington, 2005; Saltaris, 2002), we expected that psychopathy
would be associated with attachment. However, on the basis of the literature no prior
hypotheses could be formulated regarding the association between specific psychopathic traits
and attachment categories and the effects of potential moderators. Therefore, the current study
PT
was mainly explorative.
RI
Overall, we found a significant association between attachment and psychopathic traits
SC
of .18, meaning that psychopathy in general and attachment are weakly related to each other,
following the criteria of Cohen (1992) about the interpretation of the magnitude of effect
NU
sizes. However, examining the different categories of psychopathic traits, significant
differences in the attachment-psychopathic traits link were found; the strongest link was
MA
found for CU traits, followed by psychopathy in general, while nonsignificant links were
found for narcissism, antisocial behavior, and impulsivity. This suggests that those with CU
D
traits in particular were more likely to have insecure attachments. This finding is in line with
E
prior research, in which CU traits were seen as the most impeding psychopathic trait for
PT
interpersonal relationships in general (Larsson et al., 2008; O’Connor et al., 2016). Moreover,
CE
the heritability of CU traits is strong (Bedzjian et al., 2011; Larsson et al., 2008), meaning that
children with CU traits are more likely to have parents with CU traits, thus the forming of a
AC
secure attachment may be impeded by both parents and offspring (Dadds, Allen et al., 2014).
the setting. Moderate to large associations were found in clinical and secure setting, but no
significant effects sizes were found when a study did combine two different settings, such as a
forensic and a mental health clinic (e.g., Catchpole, 2009; Frodi et al., 2001). Also, for the
community samples we did not find any significant effect. An explanation may be that
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 18
psychopathy (e.g, Coid, et al., 2009) and CU traits (e.g., Rowe, Maughan, Moran, Ford,
Briskman, & Goodman, 2010) are relatively rare in general population samples. Samples in
PT
instrument, a medium effect size was found when the scores on the IPPA were reported, and
RI
no effect was found for any other attachment instrument. Possibly, this could be due to the
SC
fact that the IPPA measures the overall strength of attachment compared to for instance the
AAI, which focuses more specifically on the different attachment categories. Moreover, the
NU
IPPA and the BSQ (Behavioral Systems Questionnaire) were the only questionnaires
compared to the interviews and observations used for the other attachment assessments.
MA
Likewise, for the psychopathy instruments, a medium to large effect size was found for
the APSD, and nonsignificant effect sizes were found for the other instruments. The APSD
D
was an adaptation of the PCL- which was based on the PCL-R for adults (Patrick, 2005,
E
p.396), and aimed to be more sensitive to the developmental stage of children and
PT
adolescents, but in contrast to PCL-versions, the APSD captured only the CU, narcissistic and
CE
impulsive traits, and not antisocial behavior. This could be in line with the finding that the
only significant effect size was found for CU traits, and not for narcissistic, impulsive or
AC
antisocial traits. The fact that most of the parent-reported instruments included only CU traits,
might explain that the only significant effect size found across informants was for parent-
reported psychopathic traits (i.e., not self-reports and composites of different sources), might
also be in line with the largest effect size found for CU. Finally, stronger associations between
insecure attachment and psychopathic traits were found for studies relying on the same
informant reporting on both attachment and psychopathic traits than for studies using data
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 19
from different informants. At least partly this may be explained by common method variance
We did not find any differences between attachment categories, suggesting that the
associations between psychopathic traits and secure attachment on the one hand, and the
attachment on the other hand, are similar. Nevertheless, because of the low number of studies
PT
that met our inclusion criteria, these results need to be interpreted with caution. Furthermore,
RI
the association between attachment and psychopathic traits was also independent of
SC
participant characteristics, as this association was similar across gender and age groups. This
former finding is in line with the literature (e.g., Blanchard & Lyons, 2016), and the latter
NU
finding could be ascribed to the high genetic component of CU traits, and thus a small
Likewise, we did not find differences between to whom the attachment was reported,
associated with psychopathic traits. This particular finding may possibly be explained by the
E
fact that only six of the twelve included studies reported separate father and mother scores on
PT
attachment. A moderating effect may be expected, because research did show differences
CE
between mothers and fathers for the association between CU traits in boys and impaired eye
contact with their attachment figures. This impairment in eye contact was associated with
AC
psychopathic traits in their fathers, whereas the mothers did not show any impairments, and
the quality of their attachment-behaviors was independent of the level of eye contact of their
sons (Dadds et al., 2011; Dadds, Allen et al., 2014). Subsequently, attachment with mothers
would be less impeded and therefore would show a weaker correlation with psychopathic
traits. Nevertheless, the lack of an effect of this moderator could be due to the number of
studies, and therefore should be interpreted with caution and needs further investigation.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 20
attachment and psychopathy as self-evident, even without any exclusion criteria, we did find
only 12 studies based on empirical findings that met our criteria for the current meta-analysis.
To our knowledge, the current study is the first to investigate this relation at a meta-analytic
level. Due to the correlational nature of this study, we cannot make statements about a causal
PT
relation between attachment and psychopathy. Given the limitations of the present study,
RI
which is based on only 12 studies, more research on the role of attachment in the development
SC
of psychopathy and the different traits in particular, but also on the role of psychopathic traits
attachment and psychopathic traits, with the significant moderators explaining a unique
MA
proportion of the variance of effect sizes, the present study yielded implications for future
Despite these limitations, a strength is that this study has found evidence for the
E
association between attachment and psychopathy, and more particular the association between
PT
study the association between attachment and the different psychopathic traits, with the aim to
obtain a more refined picture of the options and (im)possibilities of intervention and treatment
AC
seems to be highly dependent on age, therefore stressing the importance of early interventions
(Högström, Enebrink, & Ghaderi, 2013; Hyde et al., 2013; Ribeiro da Silva, Rijo, & Salekin,
2013; Savage, 2014, Waller Gardner, & Hyde, 2013) and the need for interventions to be
tailored specifically for youth high on CU traits, regardless of any comorbid behavioral
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 21
difficulties (Högström et al., 2013; Hyde et al., 2013; Manders, Deković, Asscher, Van der
Laan, & Prins, 2013; McDonald, Dodson, Rosenfield, & Jouriles, 2011).
Future perspectives Two attachment related lines of research may be relevant when
implementing the findings of the current meta-analysis into interventions. First, the findings
of research showing that attention to fearful faces predicts attachment security (Peltola,
Forssman, Puura, Van IJzendoorn, & Leppänen, 2015), linked with the finding that CU traits
PT
are seen as a consequence of the inability to recognize fearful faces (e.g., Dadds et al., 2006),
RI
as a result of an impairment in attention to the eye region (Dadds et al., 2011), resulted in
SC
Emotion Recognition Training (ERT). For children with high levels of CU traits, ERT and not
TAU produced significant improvements in conduct problems and affective empathy. So ERT
NU
could be used as an intervention for children with high CU-traits, although the change
associated with ERT seemed not to occur through improvements in emotion recognition, but
MA
The second recommended line of inquiry based on the found association between CU
E
traits and attachment is more pharmacogenetically orientated. Oxytocin, a hormone known for
PT
its role in bonding between parents and their offspring (Buchheim et al., 2009; Liu, McErlean,
CE
& Dadds, 2012; Strathearn, 2011; Strathearn, Fonagy, Amico, & Montague, 2009; Swain et
al., 2014), might also be involved in the development of psychopathy. Variations in oxytocin
AC
receptors and/or oxytocin blood levels were found to be associated with CU-traits (Beitchman
et al, 2012; Dadds, Moul, et al., 2014). Furthermore, administration of oxytocin to adults
(Guastella, Mitchell, & Dadds, 2008), fathers (Weisman, Zagoory-Sharon, & Feldman, 2014)
or children (Guastella et al., 2010) showed to increase not only their focus on the eye region
of the face and therefore their emotion recognition (Guastella et al., 2010), but also the quality
of the parent-child social behaviors such as positive affect (Weisman et al., 2014). So both
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 22
oxytocin) seem to have in common that they both target not only the CU-trait but also
examine the associations between CU traits and different attachment categories, given the
PT
For example, secure attachment priming resulted in more attachment security and less
RI
psychopathy (Allen, 2015). Additionally, research findings indicate that formation of a more
SC
secure attachment, although complicated by the insensitivity of the child, could serve as a
protective factor for a child with callous-unemotional traits (Kochanska, 1995; Nakash-
NU
Eisikovits et al., 2002). Albeit the association between attachment and psychopathic traits in
particular turned out to be weak, more investigation in the correlations between psychopathic
MA
traits and other constructs concerning parental behavior, such as parental bonding (Blanchard
& Lyons, 2016; Craig et al., 2013), positive parenting (Hawes et al., 2011) or parental warmth
D
(Bisby et al., 2017; Kimonis et al., 2013) is highly recommended in general, and more
E
specifically for treatment possibilities, given the devastating effects psychopathic traits can
PT
have on both individuals with psychopathic traits themselves, but also their environment.
CE
Conclusion
AC
Although this study provides insight into the strength of the association between
attachment and psychopathic traits, it does not –nor did it intend to– provide insight in what
comes first, attachment difficulties or psychopathic traits. So, the apparent paradox between
Bowlby (1944), who stated that psychopathy should be seen as a result of a bad early
environment, and Cleckley (1941), who thought that bad parenting could not be the regular
cause, still exists. However, this study has demonstrated that there is an association between
attachment and psychopathic traits. It might be that both Cleckley and Bowlby were right
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 23
about the nascence of psychopathy and that it only depends on where the starting point for
any assessment is put. Nonetheless, children with neurological alterations due to genetic
factors and/or early childhood experiences do form a specific parenting challenge, and are
therefore at a greater risk for a distorted relationship with their caregivers. It is therefore
in younger children.
PT
RI
SC
NU
MA
E D
PT
CE
AC
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 24
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A
T. Salekin & D. T. Lynam (Eds.), Handbook of child and adolescent psychopathy (pp.
PT
233–250). New York: The Guilford Press.
RI
Anderson, D.R. (2007). Model based inference in the life sciences: A primer on evidence.
SC
New York: Springer.
*Asscher, J. J., Dekovic, M., Manders, W. A., Prins, P. J. M., & Van der Laan, P. H. (2010).
NU
Dutch MST randomized Controlled Trial [Data file and Codebook]. Author
Asscher, J. J., Van Vugt, E. S., Stams, G. J. J. M., Deković, M., Eichelsheim, V. I., & Yousfi,
Bailey, C., & Shelton, D. (2014). Self-reports of faulty parental attachments in childhood and
CE
review. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing, 21, 365–374. doi:
AC
10.1111/jpm.12086
Barker, E. D., Oliver, B. R., Viding, E., Salekin, R. T., & Maughan, B. (2011). The impact of
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test
doi: 1.1037//0022-3514.61.2.226
Beaver, K. M., Rowland, M. W., Schwartz, J. A., & Nedelec, J. L. (2011). The genetic origins
PT
of psychopathic personality traits in adult males and females: Results from an
RI
adoption-based study. Journal of Criminal Justice, 39, 426-432. doi:
SC
1.1016/j.jcrimjus.2011.07.002
Beaver, K. M., Vaughn, M. G., DeLisi, M., Barnes, J. C., & Boutwell, B. B. (2012). The
NU
neuropsychological underpinnings to psychopathic personality traits in a nationally
1.1007/s11126-011-9190-2
Beeney, J. E., Stepp, S. D., Hallquist, M. N., Scott, L. N., Wright, A. C., Ellison, W. D., . . .
D
doi:10.1037/per0000110
Beitchman, J. H., Zai, C. C., Muir, K., Berall, L., Nowrouzi, B., Choi, E., & Kennedy, J. L.
AC
012-0240-6
Bezdjian, S., Raine, A., Baker, L. A., & Lynam, D. R. (2011). Psychopathic personality in
Bisby, M. A., Kimonis, E. R., & Goulter, N. (2017). Low maternal warmth mediates the
juvenile offenders. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26, 1790-1798. doi:
10.1007/s10826-017-0719-3
PT
727-731. doi 10.1136/jnnp.71.6.727
RI
Blair, R. J. R., Colledge, E., Murray, L., & Mitchell, D. G. V. (2001). A selective impairment
SC
in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic
Blair, R. J. R., Peschardt, K. S., Budhani, S., Mitchell, D. G. V., & Pine, D. S. (2006). The
MA
276. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01596.x
D
Blanchard, A., & Lyons, M. (2016). Sex differences between primary and secondary
E
Blonigen, D. M., Hicks, B. M., Krueger, R. F., Patrick, C. J., & Iacono, W. G. (2005).
Psychopathic personality traits: Heritability and genetic overlap with internalizing and
AC
1.1017/S0033291704004180
Boccardi, M., Frisoni, G. B., Hare, R. D., Cavedo, E., Najt, P., Pievani, M., . . . Repo-
Bogaerts, S., Buschman, J., Kunst, M., & Winkel, F. (2010). Intra- and extra-familial child
Bowen, K. L., Morgan, J. E., Moore, S. C., & Van Goozen, S. H. (2014). Young offenders’
PT
psychopathic traits, conduct disorder and offense severity. Journal of Psychopathology
RI
and Behavioral assessment, 36, 60-73. doi: 10.1007/s10862-013-9368-z
SC
Bowlby, J. (1944). Forty-four juvenile thieves: Their character and home-life. International
*Brennan, K. A., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Attachment styles and personality disorders: Their
Bretherton, I. (1992). The origins of Attachment theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth.
PT
Brody, Y., & Rosenfeld, B. (2002). Object relations in criminal psychopaths. International
Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 46, 400-411. doi:
AC
10.1177/0306624X02464003
Buchheim, A., Heinrichs, M., George, C., Pokorny, D., Koops, E., Henningsen, P., . . .
*Buck, K. A. (2015). Understanding adolescent psychopathic traits from early risk and
protective factors: Relations among inhibitory control, maternal sensitivity, and
attachment representation. Journal of Adolescence, 44, 97-105. doi:
1.1016/j.adolescence.2015.07.009
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 28
Burgess, K. B., Marshall, P. J., Rubin, K. H., & Fox, N. A. (2003). Infant attachment and
1.1111/1469-761.00167
Carlson, M., Oshri, A., & Kwon, J. (2015). Child maltreatment and risk behaviors: The roles
of callous/unemotional traits and conscientiousness. Child Abuse & Neglect, 50, 234-
PT
243. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.07.003
RI
*Catchpole, R. E. H. (2009 ). Attachment to caregivers and psychopathic characteristics
SC
among adolescents at risk for aggression. Dissertation Abstracts International:
doi:10.1037/a0032968
Christian, E. J., Meltzer, C. L., Thede, L. L., & Kosson, D. S. (2016). The relationship
D
adolescent detainees. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 48, 260-269. doi
PT
10.1007/s10578-016-0638-3
CE
Cicchetti, D., & Doyle, C. (2016). Child maltreatment, attachment and psychopathology:
Cleckley, H.,M. (1988). The Mask of Sanity. (5th ed.). Scanned facsimile produced for non-
10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 29
Coid, J., Yang, M., Ullrich, S., Roberts, A., & Hare, R. D. (2009). Prevalence and correlates
Colonnesi, C., Draijer, E. M., Stams, G. J. J. M., Van der Bruggen, C. O., Bögels, S. M., &
Noom, M. J. (2011). The relation between insecure attachment and child anxiety: A
meta-analytic review. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 40, 630-
PT
645. doi: 1.1080/15374416.2011.581623
RI
Craig, M. C., Catani, M., Deeley, Q., Latham, R., Daly, E., Kanaan, R., . . . Murphy, D. G. M.
(2009). Altered connections on the road to psychopathy. Molecular Psychiatry, 14,
SC
946-953. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.40
Craig, R. L., Gray, N. S., & Snowden, R. J. (2013). Recalled parental bonding, current
NU
attachment, and the triarchic conceptualisation of psychopathy. Personality and
Dadds, M. R., Jambrak, J., Pasalich, D., Hawes, D. J., & Brennan, J. (2011). Impaired
1.1111/j.1469-761.201.02323.x
Dadds, M. R., Allen, J. L., McGregor, K., Woolgar, M., Viding, E., & Scott, S. (2014).
CE
Dadds, M. R., Cauchi, A. J., Wimalaweera, S., Hawes, D. J., & Brennan, J. (2012). Outcomes,
1.1016/j.psychres.2012.04.033
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 30
Dadds, M. R., El Masry, Y., Wimalaweera, S., & Guastella, A. J. (2008). Reduced eye gaze
explains “fear blindness” in childhood psychopathic traits. Journal of the American
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 455-463. doi:
10.1097/CHI.0b013e31816407f1
Dadds, M. R., Moul, C., Cauchi, A., Dobson-Stone, C., Hawes, D. J., Brennan, J., & Ebstein,
R. E. (2014). Methylation of the oxytocin receptor gene and oxytocin blood levels in
PT
doi:10.1017/S0954579413000497
RI
Dadds, M. R., Perry, Y., Hawes, D. J., Merz, S., Riddell, A. C., Haines, D. J., . . .
SC
Abeygunawardane, A. I. (2006). Attention to the eyes and fear-recognition deficits in
Dadds, M. R., & Salmon, K. (2003). Punishment insensitivity and parenting: Temperament
MA
and learning as interacting risks for antisocial behavior. Clinical Child and Family
Dawel, A., O’Kearney, R., McKone, E., & Palermo, R. (2012). Not just fear and sadness:
Meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal
AC
Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000a). Trim and fill: A simple funnel‐plot–based method of
testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta‐analysis. Biometrics, 56, 455-463.
doi: 1.1111/j.0006-341X.200.00455.x
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 31
Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000b). A nonparametric “trim and fill” method of accounting for
Egger, M., Smith, G.D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected
by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315, 629-634. Retrieved from
http://www.jstor.org/stable/25175671
PT
Farrington, D. P. (2005). The importance of child and adolescent psychopathy. Journal of
RI
Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 489–497. doi: 1.1007/s10802-005-5729-8
SC
Fearon, R. P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Lapsley, A.-M., &
Fearon, R. P., & Belsky, J. (2011). Infant–mother attachment and the growth of externalizing
org/1.1037/1082-989X.6.2.161
Fite, P. J., Greening, L., & Stoppelbein, L. (2008). Relation between parenting stress and
AC
psychopathic traits among children. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 26, 239-248.
doi: 1.1002/bsl.803
*Flight, J. I., & Forth, A. E. (2007). Instrumentally violent youths. Criminal Justice and
Fontaine, N. M., McCrory, E. J., Boivin, M., Moffitt, T. E., & Viding, E. (2011). Predictors
Fossati, A., Feeney, J. A., Donati, D. D., Donini, M., Novella, L., Bagnato, M., . . . Maffei, C.
sample: A multivariate study. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 191, 30-
PT
37. doi: 1.1097/00005053-200301000-00006
RI
*Frodi, A., Dernevik, M., Sepa, A., Philipson, J., & Bragesjö, M. (2001). Current attachment
SC
representations of incarcerated offenders varying in degree of psychopathy.
Groh, A. M., Fearon, R. M., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans‐ Kranenburg, M. J., &
D
Roisman, G. I. (2017). Attachment in the early life course: Meta‐ analytic evidence for
E
doi: 10.1111/cdep.12213
CE
Guastella, A. J., Einfeld, S. L., Gray, K. M., Rinehart, N. J., Tonge, B. J., Lambert, T. J., &
Hickie, I. B. (2010). Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with
AC
10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.020
Guastella, A. J., Mitchell, P. B., & Dadds, M. R. (2008). Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye
10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.026
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 33
1.1097/01.chi.0000196422.42599.63
Hare, R. D. (2003). Hare Psychopathy Checklist -Revised (PCL-R) (2nd ed.). North
PT
Hawes, D. J., Dadds, M. R., Frost, A. D. J., & Hasking, P. A. (2011). Do childhood callous-
RI
unemotional traits drive change in parenting practices? Journal of Clinical Child &
SC
Adolescent Psychology, 40, 507-518. doi: 1.1080/15374416.2011.581624
Herpers, P., Scheepers, F. E., Bons, D., Buitelaar, J. K., & Rommelse, N. N. (2014). The
NU
cognitive and neural correlates of psychopathy and especially callous–unemotional
Hicks, B. M., Carlson, M. D., Blonigen, D. M., Patrick, C. J., Iacono, W. G., & MGue, M.
D
Hoeve, M., Stams, G. J. J. M., Van der Put, C., E., Semon Dubas, J., Van der Laan, P. H., &
Högström, J., Enebrink, P., & Ghaderi, A. (2013). The moderating role of child callous-
Hyde, L. W., Shaw, D. S., Gardner, F., Cheong, J., Dishion, T. J., & Wilson, M. (2013).
doi:10.1017/S0954579412001101
*Isaak, R. (2012). Early antisocial markers and attachment styles: their influence on
psychopathic characteristics in high-risk boys and girls (Master’s thesis, Simon Fraser
PT
University, Canada Arts & Social Sciences: School of Criminology. Retrieved from:
RI
http://summit.sfu.ca/item/12126
SC
Kerig, P. K., & Wenar, C. (2006). Developmental psychopathology; from infancy through
with or at risk for developmental delay. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23, 437-
Kimonis, E. R., Cross, B., Howard, A., & Donoghue, K. (2012). Maternal care, maltreatment
E
and callous-unemotional traits among urban male juvenile offenders. Journal of Youth
PT
Kimonis, E. R., Fanti, K. A., Isoma, Z., & Donoghue, K. (2013). Maltreatment profiles among
doi: 10.1177/1077559513483002
*Kochanska, G., Barry, R. A., Stellern, S. A., & O'Bleness, J. J. (2009). Early attachment
organization moderates the parent-child mutually coercive pathway to children's
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 35
Kolla, N. J., Malcolm, C., Attard, S., Arenovich, T., Blackwood, N., & Hodgins, S. (2013).
doi:10.1177/070674371305800901
PT
*Kosson, D. S., Cyterski, T. D., Steuerwald, B. L., Neumann, C. S., & Walker-Matthews, S.
RI
(2002). The reliability and validity of the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version (PCL
SC
doi: 1.1037/1040-359.14.1.97 NU
Kroneman, L. M., Hipwell, A. E., Loeber, R., Koot, H. M., & Pardini, D. A. (2011).
Larsson, H., Viding, E., & Plomin, R. (2008). Callous-unemotional traits and antisocial
E
Liu, J. C.J., McErlean, R.A., & Dadds, M.R. (2012). Are we there yet? The clinical potential
doi:10.2174/157340012798994902
Lynam, D. R., Charnigo, R., Moffitt, T. E., Raine, A., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M.
MacDonald, K., Berlowb, R., & Thomas, M. L. (2013). Attachment, affective temperament,
Mack, T. D., Hackney, A. A., & Pyle, M. (2011). The relationship between psychopathic
Madigan, S., Atkinson, L., Laurin, K., & Benoit, D. (2013). Attachment and internalizing
doi: 10.1037/a0028793
PT
Madigan, S., Brumariu, L. E., Villani, V., Atkinson, L., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2016).
RI
Representational and questionnaire measures of attachment: A meta-analysis of
SC
relations to child internalizing and externalizing problems. Psychological Bulletin,
adolescents treated for antisocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41,
Mann, F. D., Briley, D. A., Tucker-Drob, E. M., & Harden, K. P. (2015). A behavioral genetic
E
McCord, W. M., & McCord, J. (1956). Psychopathy and delinquency. New York, NY: Grune
& Stratton.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 37
McDonald, R., Dodson, M. C., Rosenfield, D., & Jouriles, E. N. (2011). Effects of a parenting
PT
Moran, P., Rowe, R., Flach, C., Briskman, J., Ford, T., Maughan, B., . . . Goodman, R.
RI
Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 1079-1084.
SC
doi: 1.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b766ab NU
Nakash-Eisikovits, O., Dutra, L., & Westen, D. (2002). Relationship between attachment
10.1097/01.CHI.0000020258.43550.BD
D
*Pasalich, D. S., Dadds, M. R., Hawes, D. J., & Brennan, J. (2012). Attachment and callous-
E
Peltola, M. J., Forssman, L., Puura, K., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Leppänen, J. M. (2015).
AC
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method
9010.88.5.879
Rowe, R., Maughan, B., Moran P., Ford, T., Briskman, J., & Goodman, R. (2010). The role of
callous and unemotional traits in the diagnosis of conduct disorder. Journal of Child
PT
psychopathy construct in a college sample. Dissertation Abstracts International:
RI
Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 65(8-B), 4303.
SC
Salekin, R. T. (2002). Psychopathy and therapeutic pessimism: Clinical lore or clinical
Salekin, R. T., & Lochman, J. E. (2008). Child and adolescent psychopathy : The search for
MA
1.1177/0093854807311330
D
Savage, J. (2014). The association between attachment, parental bonds and physically
Schimmenti, A., Di Carlo, G., Passanisi, A., & Caretti, V. (2015). Abuse in childhood and
Schimmenti, A., Passanisi, A., Pace, U., Manzella, S., Di Carlo, G., & Caretti, V. (2014). The
PT
Strathearn, L. (2011). Maternal neglect: oxytocin, dopamine and the neurobiology of
RI
attachment. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 23, 1054-1065. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-
SC
2826.2011.02228.x
Strathearn, L., Fonagy, P., Amico, J., & Montague, P. R. (2009). Adult attachment predicts
NU
maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34,
2655-2666. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.103
MA
Swain, J., Kim, P., Spicer, J., Ho, S., Dayton, C., Elmadih, A., & Abel, K. (2014).
Approaching the biology of human parental attachment: Brain imaging, oxytocin and
D
coordinated assessments of mothers and fathers. Brain Research, 1580, 78-101. doi:
E
10.1016/j.brainres.2014.03.007
PT
Taylor‐ Colls, S., & Fearon, R. M. P (2015). The effects of parental behavior on infants'
CE
10.1111/cdev.12348
AC
Tuvblad, C., Bezdjian, S., Raine, A., & Baker, L. A. (2014). The heritability of psychopathic
Van den Noortgate, W., López-López, J. A., Marín-Martínez, F., & Sánchez-Meca, J. (2015).
Viding, E., Price, T. S., Jaffee, S. R., Trzaskowski, M., Davis, O. S. P., Meaburn, E. L., . . .
PT
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of
RI
Statistical Software, 36, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v036.i03
SC
Viechtbauer, W. (2016). Model selection using the glmulti Package. Retrieved from
NU
http://www.metafor-project.org/doku.php/tips:model_selection_with_glmulti
Wagner, N. J., Mills-Koonce, W. R., Willoughby, M. T., Zvara, B., & Cox, M. J. (2015).
MA
doi:10.1037/a0039353
E
Waller, R., Gardner, F., & Hyde, L. W. (2013). What are the associations between parenting,
PT
Waller, R., Gardner, F., Hyde, L. W., Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J., & Wilson, M. N. (2012). Do
AC
harsh and positive parenting predict parent reports of deceitful‐ callous behavior in
doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02550.x
Wibbelink, C.J.M. & Assink, M. (2015). Handleiding voor het uitvoeren van een drie-level
Wilkinson, S., Waller, R., &Viding, E. (2016). Practitioner Review: Involving young people
PT
RI
SC
NU
MA
E D
PT
CE
AC
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 42
Tables
PT
(parentsa) reported) group (clinical)
Buck, 2015 957 unknown 15,0 BSQ: Self-report: Combination YPTI Adolescents from the
secure attachment & WAI (Self- NICHD Study of Early
RI
(parents) report ) Child Care and Youth
SC
Development
(community)
Catchpole, 109 50 15.4 AAI: Semi- PCL:YV ( Semi- Adolescents (boys and
NU
2009 structured structured girls sample) for
interview: interview and a assessment of severe
adolescent review of collateral behavioral problems in
MA
interview and a
review of files) (incarcerated).
PT
Frodi et al., 14 100 34.0 AAI: Semi- PCL:SV ( trained Incarcerated, 10 violent
2001 structured assessor, structured index offence, 4 drug-
CE
(combination)
Isaak, 2012 110 58 15.4 CAPAI: PCL-YV High risk adolescents (boy
Interview (integration of & girl sample) in forensic
c
(mother ) self-reports, and mental health setting
Kosson eal., 114 100 14.5 IPPA: Self-report PCL:YV; Adjudicated delinquent
2009 (parentsa) (Composite of self- adolescents
report, observation supervised by court
and collateral counselors/ in short-term
sources) detention centre
(combination)
Mazzarello, 42 0 23.4 AAP: analysis of APSD –CU (Self- Adolescent mothers at
2007 narratives report) Montreal children’s
PT
Pasalich et 55 100 6.3 MCAST: Story SDQ & APSD Boys referred to
al. 2012 stem completion pooled ( Mother, university psychology
RI
task (motherb) father and teacher clinic for assessment and
report) treatment of conduct
SC
problems (clinical)
Smith, 2011 60 50 15.7 IPPA Self-report ICU (Self-report) High-risk young offenders
a
(father, mother ) serving custodial services
NU
in a Secure Training
Centre (incarcerated)
MA
=dimensional measure of attachment, IPPA = Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment, BSQ
E
al., 2009) or Strange Situation Paradigm (Willoughby et al., 2014), AAP = Adult Attachment
Projective, MCAST = Manchester Child Attachment Story Task, APSD = Antisocial Process
AC
Table 2 The QM statistics for whether a moderator was significant and the QE statistics for the
QM (df) p QE (df) p
Publication characteristics
PT
Sample characteristics
RI
Gender 0.54 (2) .586 771.98 (130) < .001
SC
Percentage boys 0.05 (1) .816 645.38 (129) < .001
Study characteristics
MA
Assessment characteristics
PT
Psychopathic traits reported by whom 28.04(2) < .001 645.80 (130) < .001
Psychopathic traits.
psychopathy
PT
Narcissism 4 26 .08 .07 1.17 -0.059 0.230 .245 .08
RI
Impulsivity 5 27 .11 .07 1.60 -0.027 0.255 .113 .11
SC
Antisocial 2 14 .14 .08 1.62 -0.027 0.255 .106 .13
NU
Psychopathic traits reported by whom.
Psychopathy instruments.
CE
Attachment instruments.
PT
Other 6 27 .06 .06 0.97 -0.064 - 0.187 .333 .06
RI
SC
Sample setting
Informant bias
Same informant 6 91 .23 .07 3.44 0.097 - 0.359 < .001 .22
informant
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 47
Figure 1.
Funnel plot with Fisher’s z transformed correlations. On the y-axis are the standard
errors of the studies, with smaller standard errors representing larger sample sizes. On the x-
PT
RI
SC
NU
MA
E D
PT
CE
AC
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ATTACHMENT PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS 48
Figure 2
Funnel plot of the effect sizes. On the y-axis are the inverses of the standard errors, on the x-
PT
RI
SC
NU
MA
E D
PT
CE
AC
Figure 2 displays the funnel plot after the trim and fill analysis. It visualizes how the
distribution of the effect sizes would be if it was symmetrical. The black dots represent the
effect sizes, the white dots represent the imputed effect sizes. As is visible in Figure 2, all the
(AVB_2018_37)
PT
Only in clinical and secured settings this relation was present
RI
relation
SC
NU
MA
E D
PT
CE
AC