Andrew 2016
Andrew 2016
This study aimed to investigate prospective predictors and health-related outcomes of positive body
image in adolescent girls. In so doing, the modified acceptance model of intuitive eating was also
examined longitudinally. A sample of 298 girls aged 12 to 16 years completed a questionnaire containing
measures of body appreciation, potential predictors, and a range of health outcomes, at 2 time points
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
separated by 1 year. Longitudinal change regression models showed that perceived body acceptance by
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
others (positively), self-objectification and social comparison (negatively), and body appreciation (pos-
itively) prospectively predicted intuitive eating 1 year later, consistent with the acceptance model of
intuitive eating. Perceived body acceptance by others was the only proposed predictor to prospectively
predict an increase in body appreciation over time. Time 1 body appreciation prospectively predicted a
decrease in dieting, alcohol, and cigarette use, and an increase in physical activity 1 year later. In
particular, girls with low body appreciation were more likely than girls with high body appreciation to
take up alcohol and cigarette use between time points. The results highlight body appreciation as an
important target for interventions designed to prevent or delay the uptake of alcohol and cigarette
consumption among girls. More broadly, they suggest that a positive body image can confer considerable
benefit for adolescent girls.
Keywords: adolescent girls, positive body image, body appreciation, prospective study, intuitive eating
Body image is recognized as a complex and multifaceted con- media messages depicting narrow beauty ideals (Avalos et al.,
struct that affects cognitive, behavioral, and emotional functioning 2005). Body appreciation has been shown to relate to adaptive
(Pruzinsky & Cash, 2002). Despite this broad conceptualization of psycho-social indicators of well-being including self-esteem, op-
body image, the majority of body image research has focused on timism (e.g., Avalos et al., 2005), and self-compassion (Albertson,
understanding and investigating negative aspects of body image Neff, & Dill-Shackleford, 2015; Wasylkiw, MacKinnon, & Ma-
(Tylka, 2011). Recently, however, there has been an increase in cLellan, 2012). In experimental studies, body appreciation has
examinations of positive and adaptive components of body image. been shown to protect against media-induced body image distur-
Positive body image is broadly described as the love, respect, and bance (Andrew, Tiggemann, & Clark, 2015a; Halliwell, 2013). To
acceptance of one’s appearance and physical function (Wood- date, the majority of research examining body appreciation has
Barcalow, Tylka, & Augustus-Horvath, 2010), and is distinct from used samples of adult women. Given that a considerable propor-
body satisfaction or a lack of negative body image (Tylka, 2011;
tion of adolescent girls report marked body dissatisfaction
Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015a; Wood-Barcalow et al., 2010).
(Thompson, Heinberg, Altabe, & Tantleff-Dunn, 1999), it is im-
Qualitative studies have revealed common characteristics of peo-
portant that development of positive body image be investigated
ple with positive body image, including body acceptance, taking a
before adulthood.
functional view of the body, filtering appearance information in a
protective manner, and advocating a broad conceptualization of Adolescence is a time associated with significant social, phys-
beauty (Bailey, Gammage, van Ingen, & Ditor, 2015; Frisén & ical, and emotional change (Eccles, 1999). For girls, puberty
Holmqvist, 2010; Holmqvist & Frisén, 2012; McHugh, Coppola, brings about physical transitions in body composition, including
& Sabiston, 2014; Wood-Barcalow et al., 2010). increased fat deposits on areas such as the hips (Wertheim &
Positive body image is most commonly measured by the Body Paxton, 2011), that move appearance further away from cultural
Appreciation Scale (Avalos, Tylka, & Wood-Barcalow, 2005). definitions of beauty (e.g., thinness). Although the study of neg-
This scale assesses acceptance, respect and attention toward bodily ative body image in adolescent girls is extensive, as yet only a
needs, favorable opinions toward one’s body and rejection of small number of studies have investigated positive body image in
adolescents. In a sample of Spanish adolescent girls, body appre-
ciation correlated positively with self-esteem and adaptive coping,
and negatively with eating disorder symptomology (Jáuregui
This article was published Online First January 4, 2016.
Lobera & Bolaños Ríos, 2011; Lobera, Ríos, Fernández, & Casals
Rachel Andrew, Marika Tiggemann, and Levina Clark, School of Psy-
chology, Flinders University.
Elsa Sánchez, 2011). In Australian adolescent girls, positive rela-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Rachel tionships were found with perceived body acceptance by others
Andrew, School of Psychology, Flinders University, GPO Box 2100, Adelaide and intuitive eating (Andrew, Tiggemann, & Clark, 2015b). The
5001, South Australia, Australia. E-mail: [email protected] current study aims to expand upon these limited findings by
463
464 ANDREW, TIGGEMANN, AND CLARK
investigating a range of potential predictors and outcomes of body a more rigorous test of proposed causal relationships within a
appreciation in adolescent girls. model.
poses that perceived body acceptance by others indirectly pre- (as postulated in the acceptance model of intuitive eating). This has
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
dicts body appreciation and intuitive eating through decreased been shown cross-sectionally to relate to body appreciation in both
self-objectification (the adoption of an observer’s perspective women (e.g., Avalos & Tylka, 2006) and adolescent girls (Andrew
of the body, Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). People who feel et al., 2015b), but has not yet been investigated longitudinally. The
others accept their body should feel less need to monitor their second type of predictor was consumption of various forms of
outward appearance (Avalos & Tylka, 2006). Reduced self- media, and the third was engagement in different types of activi-
objectification is proposed to predict increased body apprecia- ties. Both of these aimed to assess the impact of girls’ engagement
tion and intuitive eating, as those who pay attention to body in everyday behaviors on body appreciation.
function should appreciate their body for its abilities, instead of In general, girls’ media consumption has been shown to have
focusing on perceived flaws, and should have more resources to negative effects on body image. For example, a recent meta-
attend to physical hunger cues (Avalos & Tylka, 2006; analysis concluded that there was a relationship between exposure
Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). Body appreciation directly pre- to mass media emphasizing the thin-ideal and vulnerability to body
dicts intuitive eating because people with higher body appreci- image disturbances (Grabe, Ward, & Hyde, 2008). Thus, we pro-
ation should have a heightened awareness of bodily needs and pose that adolescent girls who consume less appearance-based
act with respect toward their body (Avalos & Tylka, 2006). media will show greater body appreciation, as a result of decreased
The acceptance model of intuitive eating has been tested and levels of exposure to messages emphasizing the importance of
confirmed cross-sectionally in undergraduate women (Avalos & appearance. Likewise, greater exposure to nonappearance media
Tylka, 2006), college athletes (Oh, Wiseman, Hendrickson, Phil- that highlight ability (e.g., cooking shows) or that engage girls
lips, & Hayden, 2012) and in young and middle-aged adult women intellectually (e.g., documentaries) may enhance body apprecia-
(Augustus-Horvath & Tylka, 2011). More recently the model was tion. In addition, Menzel and Levine (2011) have proposed that
shown to hold in a sample of Australian adolescent girls (Andrew engaging in embodying activities, that is, activities which involve
et al., 2015b). These authors also reported that reduced social a mind-body integration (Piran, 2001, 2002), is key to positive
appearance comparison was an additional (negative) mediator in body image. Accordingly, we propose that playing sports or other
the positive relationships between perceived body acceptance by physical activities that place emphasis on body function (e.g.,
others and both body appreciation and intuitive eating. Thus, their soccer) should enhance body appreciation. We also expected that
modified acceptance model of intuitive eating included both self- participating in hobbies and other engaging activities, such as
objectification and social appearance comparison as mediators playing a musical instrument, may also foster body appreciation
(Andrew et al., 2015b). because, as suggested by Halliwell (2015), such activities empha-
The current study sought to longitudinally examine the modified size one’s physical capabilities and the mind-body connection.
acceptance model of intuitive eating in adolescent girls, a group
undergoing a period of development associated with eating attitude Outcomes of Body Appreciation
formation (Graber, Brooks-Gunn, Paikoff, & Warren, 1994) and One facet of positive body image is investment in the care of the
heightened risk of disordered eating (Paxton et al., 1991). All body and its physical needs (Tylka, 2011, 2012), and thus, Tylka
previous tests of the acceptance model of intuitive eating have and Wood-Barcalow (2015a) have suggested that positive body
been limited to correlational designs. Correlational studies cannot image may be protective of physical health. To our knowledge,
confidently determine the direction of causality between variables. there have been only two studies, both correlational, investigating
For example, while the acceptance model postulates that body the health-related outcomes of body appreciation outside of intu-
appreciation leads to intuitive eating, the converse causal direction itive eating. In the first, Andrew, Tiggemann, and Clark (2014)
is also plausible. That is, engaging in an adaptive and intuitive found that body appreciation was positively associated with sun
eating style might lead to greater body appreciation. A minimum protection, skin screening, and seeking medical attention when
requirement for causality is temporal precedence (Menard, 1991). needed, and negatively related to weight-loss behavior in adult
This can be tested using longitudinal methodology by examining women. In the second, Gillen (2015) reported that body appreci-
whether a proposed cause (e.g., body appreciation) is temporally ation was positively correlated with intentions to protect skin from
antecedent to (occurs before) the proposed effect (e.g., intuitive sun damage and negatively related with dieting in young adult men
eating). Thus, the use of a longitudinal research design allows for and women.
PREDICTORS AND OUTCOMES OF GIRLS’ POSITIVE BODY IMAGE 465
As yet, potential health outcomes of positive body image (other measures of body appreciation, self-objectification, social appear-
than intuitive eating) have not been examined in adolescents; nor ance comparison, and perceived body acceptance (in that order),
has there been any longitudinal study of health outcomes in any followed by health behaviors including intuitive eating, and gen-
age group. The adolescent period is associated with the develop- eral demographic information.
ment of personal autonomy (Eccles, 1999), including increased Body appreciation. Body appreciation was measured by the
choices around behaviors that impact on health. In addition, Body Appreciation Scale (BAS) of Avalos and colleagues (Avalos
healthy habits developed during this period can have benefits into et al., 2005). Participants rate 13 items (e.g., “Despite my flaws, I
adulthood (Aarts, Paulussen, & Schaalma, 1997). In the current accept my body for what it is”) on a 5-point scale ranging from
study, we examined a range of health behavior outcomes, both 1 ⫽ never to 5 ⫽ always. Items are averaged with higher scores
adaptive and harmful. The adaptive health behaviors included reflecting higher levels of body appreciation. The BAS has dem-
consumption of the recommended intake of fruit and vegetables, onstrated 3-week test–retest reliability (r ⫽ .90), construct and
sun protection, adequate sleep, and seeking medical attention. discriminant validity (Avalos et al., 2005), and has been shown to
Potentially harmful behaviors examined were dieting, consump- have high internal reliability (␣ ⫽ .88) in an adolescent sample
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
tion of takeaway food, and alcohol and cigarette use. (Lunde, 2013). For the present sample, internal reliability was high
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
(Avalos & Tylka, 2006). Participants rate items relating to percep- ever been on a diet to lose weight, on a scale ranging from 1 ⫽
tion of how accepting important others are of their weight and never to 5 ⫽ always.
shape on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 ⫽ never to 5 ⫽ always. Fruit, vegetable, and takeaway consumption. Girls re-
The original 10-item scale assessed body acceptance by family, sponded to three questions regarding their food intake. In Austra-
friends, dating partners, society, and the media. In the current lia, dietary guidelines recommend the consumption of two serves
study, these questions were asked in relation to family and friends. of fruit and five serves of vegetables daily (National Health and
The resulting scale had four items (e.g., “I’ve felt acceptance from Medical Research Council, 2013), with this quantity advertised
my family regarding my shape and/or weight”). Items were aver- widely through TV and print campaigns that aim to assist parents
aged with higher scores indicative of greater perceived body in increasing their children’s intake of fruit and vegetables (Com-
acceptance by others. The original measure has been shown to monwealth of Australia, 2008). The first question asked partici-
have good internal consistency (␣ ⫽ .91), 3-week test–retest pants to indicate (on average) how many days during the week
reliability (r ⫽ .85), and construct validity in college women (i.e., 0 to 7) they consume at least two serves of fruit. The second
(Avalos & Tylka, 2006). In the present sample, internal reliability question asked how many days (on average) they consume at least
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
was acceptable at Time 1 (␣ ⫽ .74) and Time 2 (␣ ⫽ .79). five serves of vegetables. The final question asked how many days
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Self-objectification. Self-objectification was measured by the in an average week participants consume a takeaway (i.e., “fast
Body Surveillance Subscale of the Objectified Body Conscious- food”) meal.
ness Scale (McKinley & Hyde, 1996). The subscale examines Sun protection. Sun protection was examined with five items
monitoring of one’s body and perceiving it in terms of appearance adopted from the Cancer Council Australia’s National Sun Pro-
as opposed to function (McKinley & Hyde, 1996). The scale tection Survey (see Dobbinson et al., 2008) used previously with
consists of eight items (e.g., “I rarely think about how I look”). an adult female sample (Andrew et al., 2014). These questions
Participants rate each item on a scale ranging from 1 ⫽ strongly examined specific behaviors such as wearing sunscreen and stay-
agree to 7 ⫽ strongly disagree, with a not applicable option coded ing in the shade during peak UV hours in summer. Participants
as missing. Scores are averaged and higher scores reflect higher rated how often they performed each sun protection behavior on a
self-objectification. This measure has been shown to be reliable 5-point scale ranging from 1 ⫽ never to 5 ⫽ always. The five
items were summed to create a total sun protection score (ranging
and valid in adults (McKinley & Hyde, 1996), and internally
from 5 to 25). Internal reliability for this scale was acceptable
consistent in a sample of Australian adolescent girls (␣ ⫽ .86,
(␣ ⫽ .70) at Time 1, and a little short of acceptable (␣ ⫽ .60)
Slater & Tiggemann, 2002). In the current study, internal reliabil-
at Time 2.
ity was acceptable at Time 1 (␣ ⫽ .79) and Time 2 (␣ ⫽ .84).
Alcohol consumption. Participants were asked how many
Social comparison. Social appearance comparison was as-
times they had ever consumed alcohol with a choice of five
sessed using the Physical Appearance Comparison Scale (Thomp-
responses ranging from 1 ⫽ zero to 5 ⫽ 10 or more. This question
son, Heinberg, & Tantleff-Dunn, 1991). Participants rate five
was considered the most appropriate assessment of alcohol con-
items (e.g., “In social situations, I sometimes compare my figure to
sumption for this sample, given that participants were under the
the figures of other people”) on a 5-point scale from 1 ⫽never to
age of legal alcohol consumption in South Australia (18 years old)
5 ⫽ always. This scale has demonstrated high 3-week test–retest
and legally should not have consumed any alcohol.
reliability (r ⫽ .78) in adolescent girls (Schutz, Paxton, & Cigarette smoking. Participants were asked how many ciga-
Wertheim, 2002), and was highly correlated with upward and rettes they had smoked in their lifetime with four possible re-
downward appearance comparison in college students (O’Brien et sponses 1 ⫽ none, 2 ⫽ 1 to 2, 3 ⫽ 2 to10, and 4 ⫽ 10 or more.
al., 2009). At Time 1, the internal reliability fell short of acceptable Because of the small number of participants who had ever smoked
(␣ ⫽ .66). Item-scale correlations indicated that low internal any cigarettes, responses were dichotomized to none or some
reliability was because of the one negatively worded item (Item 4), categories.
which has been previously reported in adolescent samples (Tigge- Weekly sleep. Participants were asked to indicate what time
mann & Miller, 2010). Therefore, Item 4 was removed from Time they fell asleep and wake up on an average day during the week
1 and Time 2 scales, and the remaining four items were summed and on the weekend. Weekly sleep was calculated by multiplying
with scores ranging from 4 to 20. The resulting internal reliability weekday sleep by five, and weekend sleep by two, and summing.
was acceptable at Time 1 (␣ ⫽ .80) and Time 2 (␣ ⫽ .81). Seeking medical attention. One question asked how often
Intuitive eating. Intuitive eating was measured with the Intu- participants go to a doctor when they feel ill, rated on a 5-point
itive Eating Scale for Adolescents (Dockendorff, Petrie, Greenleaf, scale ranging from 1 ⫽ never to 5 ⫽ always.
& Martin, 2012). This scale aims to assess unconditional permis- Demographics. The girls were asked to provide their age,
sion to eat, eating for physical rather than emotional reasons, and year level at school and ethnicity, and reported their weight and
awareness and trust in internal satiety and hunger cues. Seventeen height measurements.
items (e.g., “I trust my body to tell me how much to eat”) are rated
on a 5-point scale from 1 ⫽ strongly disagree to 5 ⫽ strongly
Procedure
agree. Scores are averaged with higher scores reflecting greater
intuitive eating. The scale has demonstrated validity (Dockendorff The protocol used in the current study was approved by the
et al., 2012). Internal reliability was acceptable at Time 1 (␣ ⫽ .80) relevant institutional research ethics board, the Department for
and Time 2 (␣ ⫽ .83) in the current sample. Education and Child Development Research Unit and the Catholic
Dieting behavior. Frequency of dieting was assessed with one Education Office. The Principal of each participating school was
question asking participants to rate the extent to which they have contacted for permission to take part in the study. After permission
PREDICTORS AND OUTCOMES OF GIRLS’ POSITIVE BODY IMAGE 467
was granted, parental consent was obtained for both Time 1 and time. All correlations between Time 1 and Time 2 ranged from
Time 2 participation through a package sent home with each moderate to strong (r ⫽ .47 to r ⫽ .78).
eligible girl containing a Letter of Introduction, Information Sheet,
and Consent Form. Girls who were given parental consent also
Longitudinal Test of the Modified Acceptance Model
gave their own assent to participate on the day of data collection.
Participants completed both Time 1 and Time 2 questionnaires in of Intuitive Eating
normal class time in groups. At each school, girls were offered the Table 2 displays the correlations between variables contained
opportunity to enter into a raffle to win one of several $20 within the modified acceptance model of intuitive eating at Time
vouchers for a clothing store as a gesture of appreciation for their 1, at Time 2, and cross-lagged (between Time 1 and Time 2). As
time. can be seen, all variables were significantly correlated with each
other at all time points. In particular, in line with the modified
Results acceptance model, the cross-lagged correlations showed that per-
ceived body acceptance by others at Time 1 was negatively related
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Table 1
Mean and SDs, and t Values for Study Variables
Main variable
Body appreciation 3.39 (.80) 3.49 (.80) 2.84ⴱ
Predictors
Appearance media 25.24 (5.40) 24.00 (5.25) ⫺5.50ⴱⴱ
Nonappearance media
Nonfashion magazines 1.80 (.96) 1.86 (.97) .10
Cooking and construction TV 2.69 (1.04) 2.74 (1.08) .82
Information based television 2.63 (1.05) 2.76 (1.04) 2.30ⴱ
Activities
Sports and physical activity 483.73 (483.04) 515.44 (487.02) 1.17
Hobbies 380.89 (522.70) 370.03 (472.46) ⫺.37
Perceived body acceptance by others 4.04 (.84) 4.15 (.83) 2.65ⴱ
Self-objectification 4.54 (1.08) 4.52 (1.12) ⫺.16
Social comparison 12.19 (3.44) 11.98 (3.35) ⫺1.27
Outcomes
Intuitive eating 3.22 (.55) 3.20 (.57) ⫺.52
Dieting behavior 2.48 (1.44) 2.58 (1.38) 1.46
Fruit consumption 5.03 (1.88) 4.97 (1.78) ⫺.41
Vegetable consumption 3.56 (2.02) 3.44 (2.18) ⫺.65
Takeaway consumption 1.09 (1.05) 1.06 (1.13) ⫺.73
Sun protection 13.85 (3.93) 14.08 (3.51) 1.70
Alcohol consumption 1.68 (1.03) 2.20 (1.29) 7.81ⴱⴱ
Cigarette smoking 1.05 (.21) 1.12 (.33) 4.45ⴱⴱ
Weekly sleep (hours) 62.36 (6.24) 59.91 (6.83) ⫺6.41ⴱⴱ
Medical attention 3.29 (1.13) 3.26 (1.12) ⫺.55
ⴱ ⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .05. p ⬍ .001.
468 ANDREW, TIGGEMANN, AND CLARK
Table 3
Results (Standardized Regression  Coefficients) of Regression Table 4
of Change in Intuitive Eating on Perceived Body Acceptance by Correlations Within and Between Time for Predictor Variables
Others, Self-Objectification, Social Comparison, and and Body Appreciation
Body Appreciation
Cross-lagged: T1
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Predictor variable Within T1 Within T2 predictor, T2 BA
prospectively predicted an increase in body appreciation 1 year ies that have shown body appreciation to ameliorate negative
later. Finally, body appreciation itself predicted decreased dieting effects on body image resulting from exposure to thin-ideal media
behavior, and alcohol and cigarette consumption, and increased (Andrew et al., 2015a; Halliwell, 2013). Similar to media con-
physical activity, 1 year later. sumption, participation in sporting activities was correlated with
The first aim of the present study was to longitudinally examine body appreciation within time but not over time.
the modified acceptance model of intuitive eating in adolescent The final aim of the present study was to examine a range of
girls. Consistent with the postulated causal model, we found that potential health outcomes of body appreciation over 1 year. The
initially higher perceived body acceptance by others, lower self- only two other studies to have examined health outcomes of body
objectification and social comparison, and greater body apprecia- appreciation (Andrew et al., 2014; Gillen, 2015) were cross-
tion predicted higher intuitive eating over time. Our findings sectional in nature and sampled young adults. In our sample of
support previous correlational tests of the model in adult female adolescent girls, we found that body appreciation predicted de-
samples (Augustus-Horvath & Tylka, 2011; Avalos & Tylka, creased dieting over a 1 year period, and that this relationship was
2006; Oh et al., 2012). However, the longitudinal research design bidirectional. Our prospective findings extend previous correla-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
of the current study has enabled confirmation of the postulated tional results indicating a negative association between body ap-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
temporal sequence of variables in the model, consistent with their preciation and dieting behavior in adult women (Andrew et al.,
proposed causal role. Thus, the findings highlight factors impor- 2014; Gillen, 2015). Although we conceptualized participation in
tant in the development of a healthy and adaptive eating style in physical activity as a potential predictor of body appreciation, we
adolescent girls. found instead that greater initial body appreciation in our sample
To examine potential predictors of body appreciation, we mea- of girls was associated with increased participation in sports and
sured several variables that we reasoned might have a positive physical activity 1 year later. Future research might usefully ex-
impact on body image. However, of the postulated predictors amine more precisely the nature of the relationship between body
tested, only perceived body acceptance by others prospectively appreciation and engagement in physical activity in samples of
predicted an increase in body appreciation. Specifically, girls who different ages.
initially felt more weight and shape acceptance from their family More important, we found a significant prospective relationship
and peers expressed greater appreciation for their body 1 year later. between body appreciation and alcohol and cigarette consumption.
This confirms longitudinally the association that has been shown In particular, body appreciation was found to predict uptake over
in a number of cross-sectional studies (Augustus-Horvath & Tylka, time. Specifically, girls with high body appreciation were less
2011; Avalos & Tylka, 2006; Kroon Van Diest & Tylka, 2010; Oh likely to take up smoking cigarettes or drinking alcohol between
et al., 2012). It is likely that the girls in our study who perceived time points than their counterparts with low body appreciation.
that their body was accepted by those important to them, possibly This is a key finding of the current study because alcohol and other
through subtle nonjudgemental messages about appearance, may drug use can have a negative impact on adolescent development in
have been less likely to experience preoccupation with appearance both cognitive and social domains, and such use also increases the
and instead feel respect and appreciation for their body (Tylka & risk of accidental injury (Hawkins, Catalano, & Miller, 1992).
Wood-Barcalow, 2015a). In addition, prediction in the reverse Collectively, these results provide empirical support to the sug-
direction was also found, indicating that the relationship between gestion that individuals with body appreciation will engage in
perceived body acceptance by others and body appreciation was self-care behaviors that benefit physical health, and actively avoid
bidirectional. Thus, girls with greater body appreciation may have behaviors that could physically harm their body (Tylka, 2011,
intentionally surrounded themselves with peers who were body 2012).
positive and communicated body acceptance (Tylka, 2011, 2012).
This type of reciprocal relationship has been suggested as a core
Practical Implications
feature of positive body image, and one that should serve to
strengthen and maintain positive body image (Tylka, 2011, 2012; Our results suggest that body appreciation has important impli-
Wood-Barcalow et al., 2010). cations for adolescent girls. Indeed, body appreciation may have
Although neither of our proposed classes of predictors regarding benefits for girls across a wide range of domains. In particular, the
media or sports was found to prospectively predict body appreci- findings from the current study suggest that fostering body appre-
ation, Time 1 appearance media consumption (negatively) and ciation may be beneficial and protective for girls’ health, in terms
reading nonfashion magazines (positively) were related to Time 1 of decreased dieting, increased physical activity, and protection
body appreciation. Given the decreasing levels of appearance against alcohol and cigarette use. Given the high rates of dieting
media consumption found between Time 1 and Time 2, it may be among adolescent girls (e.g., Paxton et al., 1991), our finding in
that any effect of media consumption on body appreciation has relation to decreased dieting suggests that enhancing body appre-
already been established by early adolescence, perhaps as a result ciation might be one way to tackle the development of maladaptive
of media saturation and transmission of beauty ideals in Western eating behaviors. The observed reciprocal relationship between
society (Levine & Chapman, 2011). Furthermore, it may not be the body appreciation and dieting also suggests that encouraging girls
amount of media viewed by girls that is critical, but rather how it to take a balanced and nonrestrictive approach to food intake might
is viewed. For example, girls with higher body appreciation may help foster their body appreciation. Thus, interventions and public
process appearance media more critically, by analyzing its content health programs targeted at adolescents that focus on controlling
and consciously “filtering” messages in a way that is protective weight and diet may negatively impact on their body appreciation.
and strengthens body image (Tylka, 2011; Wood-Barcalow et al., More holistic programs such as Health at Every Size (Bacon,
2010). This is consistent with results from two experimental stud- Stern, Van Loan, & Keim, 2005) or BodiMojo (Franko, Cousineau,
PREDICTORS AND OUTCOMES OF GIRLS’ POSITIVE BODY IMAGE 471
Rodgers, & Roehrig, 2013) that focus on a healthy lifestyle, body girls might be trained in mindfulness and acceptance techniques
acceptance, and challenging weight stigma are likely to be more that emphasize observation, but not judgment or modification of
beneficial for physical and psychological health (Tylka et al., thoughts, emotions, or physical sensations (Baer, 2003).
2014). Although we only found one major significant prospective pre-
Our results indicate that enhancing body appreciation may also dictor of body appreciation in the current study, there may be other
improve girls’ participation rates in sports and physical activity. potential ways in which positive body image can be fostered in
Australian figures show that participation in sporting activities adolescent girls. One possibility is to encourage engagement in
declines across adolescence at a considerably faster rate for girls more embodying activities (see Menzel & Levine, 2011). Apart
than boys (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2013). This decrease from yoga, we did not measure other mindfulness-based activities
may have a broad impact on the well-being of adolescent girls, as like mindful meditation, which emphasize the mind-body connec-
physical activity has a wide range of demonstrated psychological tion and might be more likely to enhance body appreciation.
and physical health benefits (e.g., Warburton, Nicol, & Bredin, Increasing self-compassion may be another way to enhance body
2006). Thus, enhancing girls’ body appreciation may assist in appreciation. One study has shown self-compassion to protect
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
increasing their participation in physical activity throughout ado- body appreciation against body image-related threats such as ap-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
body appreciation over several more time points would also be Bacon, L., Stern, J. S., Van Loan, M. D., & Keim, N. L. (2005). Size
useful in ruling out third variable explanations. More generally, acceptance and intuitive eating improve health for obese, female chronic
longitudinal examination of predictors and outcomes of body dieters. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 105, 929 –936.
appreciation should also be extended to adult female samples. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2005.03.011
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A
conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Psychology: Science and
Conclusions Practice, 10, 125–143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bpg015
Bailey, K. A., Gammage, K. L., van Ingen, C., & Ditor, D. S. (2015). “It’s
Despite the above limitations, the current study has contributed all about acceptance”: A qualitative study exploring a model of positive
to our understanding of positive body image in adolescent girls. As body image for people with spinal cord injury. Body Image, 15, 24 –34.
one of few studies to examine body appreciation in an adolescent http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.04.010
population and the first to do so longitudinally, the findings show Beck, A. T. (1979). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. Ox-
that it is possible to detect prospective predictors and outcomes of ford, England: International Universities Press.
positive body image in adolescent girls over a relatively short time Beck, J. S. (2011). Cognitive behavior therapy: Basics and beyond. New
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
period of 1 year. The results provide longitudinal support for the York, NY: Guilford Press.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
interventionists in secondary schools. Body Image, 14, 1– 4. http://dx.doi weight loss behaviors in adolescent girls and boys. Journal of Youth and
.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.02.002 Adolescence, 20, 361–379. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01537402
Hawkins, J. D., Catalano, R. F., & Miller, J. Y. (1992). Risk and protective Piran, N. (2001). Re-inhabiting the body from the inside out: Girls trans-
factors for alcohol and other drug problems in adolescence and early form their school environment. In D. L. Tolman & M. Brydon-Miller
adulthood: Implications for substance abuse prevention. Psychological (Eds.), From subjects to subjectivities: A handbook of interpretative and
Bulletin, 112, 64 –105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.64 participatory methods (pp. 218 –238). New York, NY: New York Uni-
Holmqvist, K., & Frisén, A. (2012). “I bet they aren’t that perfect in versity Press.
reality:” Appearance ideals viewed from the perspective of adolescents Piran, N. (2002). Embodiment: A mosaic of inquiries in the area of body
with a positive body image. Body Image, 9, 388 –395. http://dx.doi.org/ weight and shape preoccupation. In S. M. Abbey (Ed.), Ways of knowing
10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.03.007 in and through the body: Diverse perspectives on embodiment (pp.
Homan, K. J., & Tylka, T. L. (2015). Self-compassion moderates body 211–214). Welland, Canada: Soliel.
comparison and appearance self-worth’s inverse relationships with body Piran, N. (2015). New possibilities in the prevention of eating disorders:
appreciation. Body Image, 15, 1–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim The introduction of positive body image measures. Body Image, 14,
.2015.04.007 146 –157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.03.008
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Jáuregui Lobera, I., & Bolaños Ríos, P. (2011). Spanish version of the Pruzinsky, T., & Cash, T. F. (2002). Understanding body images: Histor-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Body Appreciation Scale (BAS) for adolescents. The Spanish Journal of ical and contemporary perspectives. In T. F. Cash & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.),
Psychology, 14, 411– 420. http://dx.doi.org/10.5209/rev_SJOP.2011.v14 Body image: A handbook of theory, research, and clinical practice (pp.
.n1.37 3–12). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Kroon Van Diest, A. M., & Tylka, T. L. (2010). The Caregiver Eating Schutz, H. K., Paxton, S. J., & Wertheim, E. H. (2002). Investigation of
Messages Scale: Development and psychometric investigation. Body body comparison among adolescent girls. Journal of Applied Social
Image, 7, 317–326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.06.002 Psychology, 32, 1906 –1937. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816
Levine, M. P., & Chapman, K. (2011). Media influences on body image. In .2002.tb00264.x
T. F. Cash & L. Smolak (Eds.), Body image: A handbook of science, Slater, A., & Tiggemann, M. (2002). A test of objectification theory in
practice, and prevention (pp. 101–109). New York, NY: Guilford Press. adolescent girls. Sex Roles, 46, 343–349. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:
Lobera, I. J., Ríos, P. B., Fernández, M. J. S., & Casals Elsa Sánchez, O. G. 1020232714705
(2011). Perception of weight and psychological variables in a sample of Stice, E. (2002). Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology: A
Spanish adolescents. Diabetes. Metabolic Syndrome and Obesity: Tar- meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 825– 848. http://dx
gets and Therapy, 4, 245–251. .doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.5.825
Lunde, C. (2013). Acceptance of cosmetic surgery, body appreciation, Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L. J., Altabe, M. N., & Tantleff-Dunn, S.
body ideal internalization, and fashion blog reading among late adoles- (1999). Exacting beauty: Theory, assessment and treatment of body
cents in Sweden. Body Image, 10, 632– 635. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j image disturbance. Washington, DC: American Psychological Associa-
.bodyim.2013.06.007 tion. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10312-000
McHugh, T. L. F., Coppola, A. M., & Sabiston, C. M. (2014). “I’m Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L. J., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (1991). The Physical
thankful for being Native and my body is part of that”: The body pride Appearance Comparison Scale (PACS). Behavior Therapist, 14, 174.
experiences of young Aboriginal women in Canada. Body Image, 11, Tiggemann, M., & Miller, J. (2010). The internet and adolescent girls’
318 –327. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.05.004 weight satisfaction and drive for thinness. Sex Roles, 63, 79 –90. http://
McKinley, N. M., & Hyde, J. S. (1996). The Objectified Body Conscious- dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9789-z
ness Scale: Development and validation. Psychology of Women Quar- Tribole, E., & Resch, E. (1995). Intuitive eating: A recovery book for the
terly, 20, 181–215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1996 chronic dieter. New York, NY: St. Martin’s Press.
.tb00467.x Tylka, T. L. (2006). Development and psychometric evaluation of a mea-
Menard, S. (1991). Longitudinal research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. sure of intuitive eating. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 226 –240.
Menzel, J. E., & Levine, M. P. (2011). Embodying experiences and the http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.53.2.226
promotion of positive body image: The example of competitive athletics. Tylka, T. L. (2011). Positive psychology perspectives on body image. In
In R. M. Calogero, S. Tantleff-Dunn, & J. K. Thompson (Eds.), Self- T. F. Cash & L. Smolak (Eds.), Body image: A handbook of science,
objectification in women: Causes, consequences, and counteractions practice, and prevention (2nd ed., pp. 56 – 64). New York, NY: Guilford
(pp. 163–186). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Press.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/12304-008 Tylka, T. L. (2012). Positive psychology perspectives on body image. In
National Health and Medical Research Council. (2009). Australian guide- T. F. Cash (Ed.), Encyclopedia of body image and human appearance
lines to reduce health risks from drinking alcohol. Canberra: Common- (Vol. 2, pp. 657– 663). Oxford, England: Academic Press. http://dx.doi
wealth of Australia. .org/10.1016/B978-0-12-384925-0.00104-8
National Health and Medical Research Council. (2013). Australian dietary Tylka, T. L., Annunziato, R. A., Burgard, D., Daníelsdóttir, S., Shuman, E.,
guidelines. Canberra: Commonwealth of Australia. Davis, C., & Calogero, R. M. (2014). The weight-inclusive versus
O’Brien, K. S., Caputi, P., Minto, R., Peoples, G., Hooper, C., Kell, S., & weight-normative approach to health: Evaluating the evidence for pri-
Sawley, E. (2009). Upward and downward physical appearance com- oritizing well-being over weight loss. [Advance online publication].
parisons: Development of scales and examination of predictive qualities. Journal of Obesity, 2014, 983495.
Body Image, 6, 201–206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.03 Tylka, T. L., & Kroon Van Diest, A. M. (2013). The Intuitive Eating
.003 Scale-2: Item refinement and psychometric evaluation with college
Oh, K. H., Wiseman, M. C., Hendrickson, J., Phillips, J. C., & Hayden, women and men. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 60, 137–153.
E. W. (2012). Testing the acceptance model of intuitive eating with http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0030893
college women athletes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 36, 88 –98. Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. L. (2015a). What is and what is not
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0361684311433282 positive body image? Conceptual foundations and construct definition.
Paxton, S. J., Wertheim, E. H., Gibbons, K., Szmukler, G. I., Hillier, L., & Body Image, 14, 118 –129. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.04
Petrovich, J. L. (1991). Body image satisfaction, dieting beliefs, and .001
474 ANDREW, TIGGEMANN, AND CLARK
Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. L. (2015b). The Body Appreciation adolescent girls. In T. F. Cash & L. Smolak (Eds.), Body image: A
Scale-2: Item refinement and psychometric evaluation. Body Image, 12, handbook of science, practice, and prevention (2nd ed., pp. 76 – 84).
53– 67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.09.006 New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Warburton, D. E., Nicol, C. W., & Bredin, S. S. D. (2006). Health benefits Wood-Barcalow, N. L., Tylka, T. L., & Augustus-Horvath, C. L. (2010).
of physical activity: The evidence. Canadian Medical Association Jour- “But I Like My Body”: Positive body image characteristics and a holistic
nal, 174, 801– 809. http://dx.doi.org/10.1503/cmaj.051351 model for young-adult women. Body Image, 7, 106 –116. http://dx.doi
Wasylkiw, L., MacKinnon, A. L., & MacLellan, A. M. (2012). Exploring .org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.01.001
the link between self-compassion and body image in university women.
Body Image, 9, 236 –245. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.01 Received June 12, 2015
.007 Revision received November 16, 2015
Wertheim, E. H., & Paxton, S. J. (2011). Body image development in Accepted November 19, 2015 䡲
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.