0% found this document useful (0 votes)
12 views27 pages

Coll-Martín, T., Carretero-Dios, H., & Lupiáñez, J. (2021)

The document discusses attentional difficulties in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). It reviews literature on measuring three attentional networks and two components of vigilance using tasks like the Attention Network Test. The study aims to characterize these attentional domains as a function of ADHD symptoms in adults, using a modified task. Results provided partial support for hypotheses about relationships between symptoms and processes.

Uploaded by

Tao Cm
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
12 views27 pages

Coll-Martín, T., Carretero-Dios, H., & Lupiáñez, J. (2021)

The document discusses attentional difficulties in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). It reviews literature on measuring three attentional networks and two components of vigilance using tasks like the Attention Network Test. The study aims to characterize these attentional domains as a function of ADHD symptoms in adults, using a modified task. Results provided partial support for hypotheses about relationships between symptoms and processes.

Uploaded by

Tao Cm
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 27

1053

British Journal of Psychology (2021), 112, 1053–1079


© 2021 The Authors. British Journal of Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
on behalf of British Psychological Society

www.wileyonlinelibrary.com

Attentional networks, vigilance, and distraction as


a function of attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder symptoms in an adult community sample
Tao Coll-Martın1,2* , Hugo Carretero-Dios1,2 and
Juan Lupia~
nez1,3
1
Mind, Brain, and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, Spain
2
Department of Behavioral Sciences Methodology, University of Granada, Spain
3
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Granada, Spain

Attentional difficulties are a core axis in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).


However, establishing a consistent and detailed pattern of these neurocognitive
alterations has not been an easy endeavour. Based on a dimensional approach to ADHD,
the present study aims at comprehensively characterizing three key attentional domains:
the three attentional networks (alerting, orienting, and executive attention), two
components of vigilance (executive and arousal vigilance), and distraction. To do so, we
modified a single, fine-grained task (the ANTI-Vea) by adding irrelevant distractors. One
hundred and twenty undergraduates completed three self-reports of ADHD symptoms
in childhood and adulthood and performed the ANTI-Vea. Despite the low reliability of
some ANTI-Vea indexes, the task worked successfully. While ADHD symptoms in
childhood were related to alerting network and arousal vigilance, symptoms in adulthood
were linked to executive vigilance. No association between ADHD symptom severity and
executive attention and distraction was found. In general, our hypotheses about the
relationships between ADHD symptoms and attentional processes were partially
supported. We discuss our findings according to ADHD theories and attention
measurement.

Attentional difficulties are one of the core axes in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder


(ADHD). However, establishing a consistent and detailed pattern of these alterations at the
neurocognitive level has not been an easy endeavour, with rather inconsistent and null
findings (Huang-Pollock & Nigg, 2003; Huang-Pollock, Nigg, & Carr, 2005; Wilding, 2005).
In the development of translational science, identifying such neurocognitive mechanisms
underlying ADHD symptoms is crucial to enhance the approach to the disorder
(Castellanos & Tannock, 2002; Luo, Weibman, Halperin, & Li, 2019; Sonuga-Barke &
Halperin, 2010). Moreover, recent advances towards a dimensional model of ADHD have
led to an interest in studying the neurocognitive correlates of ADHD symptoms in non-
clinical community samples (Hilger & Fiebach, 2019; Hilger et al., 2020). Before

This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial License, which permits use,
distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited and is not used for commercial purposes.
*Correspondence should be addressed to Tao Coll-Martın, Departamento de Metodologıa de las Ciencias del Comportamiento,
Facultad de Psicologıa, Universidad de Granada, Campus Universitario de Cartuja s/n, 18071 Granada, Spain (email:
[email protected]).

DOI:10.1111/bjop.12513
1054 Tao Coll-Martın et al.

introducing this dimensional framework underpinning the present study, we will


describe the literature on attentional functioning in ADHD, which is mostly built upon
case–control designs.
Neurocognitive research on attention in ADHD should be grounded on theoretical
frameworks that consider the distinct aspects of attention along with their neurobiolog-
ical substrates (Booth, Carlson, & Tucker, 2007; Bush, 2010). Different theories have
emphasized different aspects of attention, giving rise to a diversity of attentional
phenomena that have even led some authors to question the very existence of attention as
a consistent phenomenon (Hommel et al., 2019). Alternatively, the three attentional
networks model by Posner and colleagues (Petersen & Posner, 2012; Posner & Petersen,
1990) tries to solve this problem by considering the attentional system as three
independent, albeit interactive, networks, each one implementing a different attentional
function. First, the alerting network regulates the level of arousal and activation for both
momentary readiness to imminent events (phasic alertness) and sustained performance
over long time periods (tonic alertness or vigilance). This network involves noradrenergic
innervations from the locus coeruleus towards frontal and parietal lobes of the right
hemisphere. The second subsystem is the orienting network, responsible for prioritizing
sensory inputs by selecting a modality or spatial location or object. It comprises cortical
regions such as parietal cortices and frontal eye fields, and the subcortical structures of
pulvinar nuclei and superior colliculi. Finally, the executive network is in charge of
monitoring performance and prioritizing goal-oriented responses in conflict situations.
This third subsystem includes the anterior cingulate and prefrontal regions.
Several tasks have been developed to simultaneously measure these three components
of attention, the most common being the Attention Network Test (ANT; Fan, McCandliss,
Sommer, Raz, & Posner, 2002; see de Souza, Faria, & Klein, 2021, for a review). This
computerized task and other variants like the ANTI (Attention Network Test for the
~ ez, & Tudela, 2004) presents a sequence of visual stimuli that
interaction; Callejas, Lupian
combines a spatial cueing (Posner, 1980) and warning signal task with a flanker paradigm
(Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974). Subtractions between the tasks conditions resulting from
specific manipulations of warning, cueing, and flankers provide the effects of alerting,
orienting, and congruency (an index of the executive network), respectively. Different
from the ANT, the use of a different cue for measuring alertness and orienting in the ANTI
also allows the measure of the interaction between the three attentional networks.
Extensive research has used the ANT/ANTI or some of its variants to analyse the
attentional networks in ADHD. A recent meta-analysis including the ANT and the ANT
child version (Rueda et al., 2004) compared 491 ADHD children with 402 typical
developing controls in nine studies (Arora, Lawrence, & Klein, 2020). They found the
functioning of the alerting and executive networks – but not orienting – to be impaired in
ADHD. Moreover, Mullane, Corkum, Klein, McLaughlin, and Lawrence (2011) reported
similar group differences using the ANTI. These results support Berger and Posner’s
(2000) original predictions regarding attentional networks in ADHD. In the same vein,
impaired alerting and executive processes fall in line with energetic (Sergeant, 2000,
2005) and executive (Barkley, 1997) accounts of ADHD (Martella, Aldunate, Fuentes, &
Sanchez-Perez, 2020), respectively.
Notwithstanding the numerous studies using the ANT as a tool to characterize the
attentional profile of ADHD, some concerns with this literature motivated our work. First,
compared to children, the amount of research on ADHD adults and the ANT is somewhat
limited (Vazquez-Marrufo, Garcıa-Valdecasas Colell, Galvao-Carmona, Sarrias-Arrabal, &
Tirapu-Ustarroz, 2019). Moreover, this body of research offers mixed evidence about
Attention, vigilance, and distraction in ADHD 1055

ADHD deficits in alerting and executive networks (Bueno et al., 2015; Hasler et al., 2016;
Lampe et al., 2007; Oberlin, Alford, & Marrocco, 2005), with those studies of greater
statistical power failing to find differences between ADHD and controls individuals
(Lundervold et al., 2011). Thus, the functioning of the attentional networks in relation to
adult ADHD symptomatology remains unclear. The two remaining issues concern the role
of vigilance and distraction in the ANT/ANTI as well as in the literature of attentional
processes in ADHD. The next two sections will address each of them.

Measuring vigilance in ADHD: a novel ANT version


Vigilance, understood as the attentional capacity to maintain performance over time, is
one of the most widely studied phenomena in the ADHD literature (Huang-Pollock,
Karalunas, Tam, & Moore, 2012; Schoechlin & Engel, 2005; Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone,
& Pennington, 2005). The variety of terms and measures linked to vigilance have led some
researchers to deem it as a multicomponent concept (Langner & Eickhoff, 2013; Luna,
Marino, Roca, & Lupian ~ez, 2018; Sturm et al., 1999).
On the one hand, vigilance tasks often consist in detecting an infrequent target among
non-target stimuli (e.g., Test of Variables of Attention [TOVA], Greenberg & Waldman,
1993), in line with the Continuous Performance Test (CPT) paradigm, suggesting
executive aspects of vigilance (Luna et al., 2018). Substantial research has shown that both
ADHD children (Huang-Pollock et al., 2012, 2020) and adults (Advokat, Martino, Hill, &
Gouvier, 2007; Barkley & Murphy, 2011; Nikolas, Marshall, & Hoelzle, 2019; Riccio &
Reynolds, 2006; Salomone, Fleming, Bramham, O’Connell, & Robertson, 2020) exhibit
worse performance in numerous CPT indexes (i.e., reaction time [RT] mean and
variability, hits, false alarms, and d0 ). However, most of these studies only compare overall
performance, rather than vigilance decrement over time (i.e., group-by-time interaction),
the defining feature of vigilance (Esterman & Rothlein, 2019; Huang-Pollock et al., 2012;
Tucha et al., 2017). Indeed, research examining such change over time has often failed to
demonstrate a greater vigilance decline in ADHD individuals (Cohen & Shapiro, 2007;
Epstein, Conners, Sitarenios, & Erhardt, 1998; Epstein, Johnson, Varia, & Conners, 2001;
Johnson et al., 2001; Solanto, Etefia, & Marks, 2004; Tucha et al., 2009). Only a few studies
found that, compared to controls, ADHD participants displayed over time higher
variability (Marchetta, Hurks, De Sonneville, Krabbendam, & Jolles, 2008; Weyandt,
Oster, Gudmundsdottir, DuPaul, & Anastopoulos, 2017), more false alarms (Tucha et al.,
2017), and lower reaction time (Weyandt et al., 2017) or fewer hits (Gmehlin et al., 2016).
Alternatively, vigilance has been operationalized as reactivity to the environment,
reflecting tonic arousal levels (Luna et al., 2018; Oken, Salinsky, & Elsas, 2006), and
measured with tasks demanding fast reactions to stimuli without exerting much control
(i.e., without response selection; e.g., the Psychomotor Vigilance Test, Dinges & Powell,
1985). When these tasks are extremely short (≤20 trials), no differences between ADHD
and controls have been found (Tucha et al., 2006, 2008, 2009). Nonetheless, as tasks are
longer, some evidence indicates that both children and adults with ADHD show slower RT
and higher variability of response (Mary et al., 2016; Tucha et al., 2017). Similar to CPT,
only a few studies have measured performance over time for this type of vigilance, with
ADHD adults exhibiting a greater increase in variability – in terms of standard deviation or
lapses, but not in mean RT (Gmehlin et al., 2016; Tucha et al., 2017).
Although some efforts have been made to obtain measures of vigilance from the ANT/
ANTI in the ADHD literature (Ad olfsdottir, Sørensen, & Lundervold, 2008; Bueno et al.,
2015; Lundervold et al., 2011), these tasks cannot provide a direct measure of such
1056 Tao Coll-Martın et al.

construct (Roca, Castro, L opez-Ramon, & Lupian~ez, 2011). A novel version of the ANT has
been developed: the ANT for Interactions and Vigilance – executive and arousal
components (ANTI-Vea; Luna et al., 2018). Grounded on the aforementioned distinction,
the ANTI-Vea is suitable to measure the two independent aspects of vigilance besides the
three attentional networks and their interactions. To assess executive vigilance (EV), the
flanker task is embedded in a CPT structure where participants have to detect a rare target.
For its part, arousal vigilance (AV) is measured with a salient stimulus (i.e., a red down
counter) that participant must stop as fast as possible. Worthy of note, the length of the
task (~33 min) enables the analysis of the decrement of both types of vigilance across the
six blocks with sufficient precision and adequate reliability for using the task in
experimental designs (Luna, Roca, Martın-Arevalo, & Lupian ~ ez, 2020).
Research on the ANTI-Vea has focused on providing empirical dissociation of and task
sensitivity to both vigilance components. In this vein, EV decrement – but not AV – is
mitigated by high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation over the right frontal
and parietal cortices (Luna, Roman-Caballero, Roman-Caballero, Barttfeld, Lupian ~ez, &
Martın-Ar
evalo, 2020) or acute moderate exercise (Sanchıs, Blasco, Luna, & Lupian ~ ez,
2020) and modulated by the cognitive task load (Luna, 2019). Conversely, AV decrement –
but not EV – is reduced by acute caffeine intake (Sanchıs et al., 2020) and increased with
fatigue across 8 hr of testing (Feltmate, Hurst, & Klein, 2020). Furthermore, the ANTI-Vea
has been used to study individual differences related to musical (Roman-Caballero, Martın-
~ez, 2021) or sport (Huertas et al., 2019) practice as well as mindfulness
Arevalo, & Lupian
and mind-wandering dispositions (Casedas, Cebolla, & Lupian ~ ez, 2021). No previous
studies have employed this task in the field of ADHD.

Measuring distraction in ADHD: a novel paradigm


Although distraction is central to ADHD symptomatology, evidence of increased
distractor interference in ADHD is rather inconsistent (Albrecht et al., 2008; Brodeur &
Pond, 2001; Chan et al., 2009; Huang-Pollock et al., 2005; Lundervold et al., 2011; Mason,
Humphreys, & Kent, 2004; Wilding, 2005). Forster (2013) pointed out that this literature
failed in the attempt to employ a paradigm with distractors that were entirely irrelevant to
the task. For instance, in the response-competition paradigm (e.g., flanker tasks), although
distractors appear in an irrelevant location where the target is never presented, their
identity is highly relevant to the task, as it is associated with one of the target responses
(i.e., congruent vs. incongruent). This does not reflect the type of distraction that
interferes with people – mostly those with ADHD – in daily life, where the distractor (e.g.,
a mobile notification) is entirely unrelated to the task being performed (e.g., reading a
paper).
Therefore, to measure task-irrelevant distraction, distractors must be presented in an
irrelevant location, unrelated to any task responses, visually dissimilar from the search
stimuli, and irrelevant to any attentional setting for the current task (Forster, 2013). In line
with this, Forster and Lavie (2008) designed the irrelevant-distractor paradigm to
measure the interference associated with the peripheral presentation of a colourful salient
task-irrelevant distractor, typically a well-known character (e.g., Pikachu). Using this
paradigm, ADHD adults exhibited higher irrelevant distraction than controls (Forster,
Robertson, Jennings, Asherson, & Lavie, 2014). Crucially, Forster and Lavie (2016) found
that while interference from irrelevant distractors correlated positively with ADHD
symptoms in non-clinical adults, interference from response-competition distractors did
not.
Attention, vigilance, and distraction in ADHD 1057

Since the ANTI-Vea measures interference by a response-competition paradigm (i.e.,


flanker task), it may be possible that integrating the irrelevant-distractor paradigm could
enhance the task sensitivity to ADHD symptoms.

A dimensional model of ADHD


Classical disease models and diagnostic systems have conceptualized mental disorders as
discrete categories qualitatively different from normality. Nevertheless, converging
evidence at behavioural (Haslam et al., 2006), neurocognitive (Frazier, Youngstrom, &
Naugle, 2007), and genetic (Gjone, Stevenson, & Sundet, 1996) levels supports a
dimensional rather than a categorical structure of ADHD. A dimensional model posits
continuity in symptoms and underlying causes, so that ADHD would be viewed as an
extreme expression of normal variation in the population (Coghill & Sonuga-Barke, 2012;
Sonuga-Barke, 2013). This approach opens up new opportunities to ADHD-related
research.
On the one hand, neurocognitive ADHD theories could serve to explain symptom-level
variation in non-clinical or community samples (Hilger & Fiebach, 2019; Hilger et al.,
2020). Conversely, research on neurocognitive correlates of ADHD symptom severity in
community samples might shed light on processes likely to be altered in ADHD (Coghill &
Sonuga-Barke, 2012). For example, impaired vigilance (Craig & Klein, 2019) and higher
irrelevant distraction (Forster & Lavie, 2016) positively correlated with ADHD symptoms
in non-clinical samples (but see Craig & Klein, 2019, and Zamani Sani et al., 2020, for null
findings on attentional networks). However, unless a substantial number of individuals
with ADHD are included in community samples, these correlational designs might only
offer preliminary or indirect insights about the disorder, which need to be confirmed in
clinically referred samples. Worthy of mention, even subclinical variations in ADHD
symptoms have been associated with negative family impact, psychosocial problems, and
poorer satisfaction with life (Cussen, Sciberras, Ukoumunne, & Efron, 2012; Gudjonsson,
Sigurdsson, Eyjolfsdottir, Smari, & Young, 2009).

The present study


The aim of our study was to investigate the main attentional processes related to ADHD
symptoms through a single, fine-grained task. For that purpose, we integrated the
irrelevant-distractor paradigm into the ANTI-Vea. This allows simultaneous measures of
the attentional networks, vigilance, and distraction, three key domains in the field of
attention and ADHD. To characterize ADHD symptoms, we employed a community
sample of undergraduates, and both childhood and current symptoms were evaluated.
Grounded on the aforementioned literature, we expected higher ADHD symptoms to
predict (a) poorer functioning in alerting and executive networks (i.e., higher effects), but
not in orienting; (2) impoverished EV and AV – crucially in performance over time (i.e.,
vigilance decrement); and (3) a higher irrelevant-distraction effect.

Method
Participants
Following the reference work by Forster and Lavie (2016), we decided to collect data from
120 participants. This sample size allows the detection of a small to medium effect size
(r = .22; smaller than r = .32, observed by Forster & Lavie, 2016) in one-tailed, zero-order
1058 Tao Coll-Martın et al.

correlations with 1 – b = .80 and a = .05, as computed with G*Power 3.1. Therefore, a
sample of 120 undergraduates from a Spanish university participated in the study. They
received extra credit course as a compensation for their voluntary participation. All
participants (97 women, 23 men; age, M = 20.21, SD = 1.91, range 18–28) were Spanish-
speaking and had a normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Two participants reported a
prior diagnosis of ADHD. All participants completed an informed consent form. The study
was conducted in accordance with the guidelines laid down by our institutional ethics
committee, in compliance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki,
and was part of a larger research project approved by our institutional ethics committee.

Instruments
Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV: childhood and current symptoms
The self-reports of the Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV (BAARS-IV; Barkley, 2011)
include two scales to assess ADHD symptoms: retrospectively in childhood (cBAARS-IV)
and concurrently in adulthood (aBAARS-IV). Each scale is composed of 18 items, nine of
inattention (e.g., ‘forgetful in daily activities’) and nine of hyperactivity–impulsivity (e.g.,
‘fidget with hands or feet or squirm in seat’), in a Likert scale ranged from 1 (never or
rarely) to 4 (very often). Since the items are based on the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.; DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association [APA],
1994), we used the Spanish version of the manual for the translation (APA, 1994/1995). In
our sample, reliability was a = .89 and a = .86 for cBAARS-IV and aBAARS-IV,
respectively, close to the a = .95 and a = .92 of the original BAARS-IV (Barkley, 2011).
Barkley proposed the 95th percentile as a cut-off to identify individuals at high risk of
ADHD.

Adult ADHD Self-Report Screening Scale for DSM-5


The Adult ADHD Self-Report Screening Scale for DSM-5 (ASRS-5; Ustun et al., 2017)
specifically assesses the adult presentation of ADHD based on DSM-5 conceptualization
(APA, 2013). It includes six items (e.g., ‘how often do you put things off until the last
minute’) in a 5-point Likert scale (0 = never to 4 = very often). Items 1–4 had been adapted
into Spanish from a previous versions of the ASRS (Sanchez-Garcia et al., 2015). For items 5
and 6, we used the forward translation of the ASRS-5 from a Spanish journal specialized in
health sciences (Redacci on Medica, n.d.). Then, both items were back-translated into
English, where no discrepancies were found. Reliability of ASRS-5 in our sample was
a = .64, which is within the range of the original study (Ustun et al., 2017), in which a
threshold of 14 points was established as preferred for screening purposes.

ANTI-Vea with irrelevant distractors


The original ANTI-Vea (Luna et al., 2018; see online version on https://www.ugr.es/~ne
urocog/ANTI/), which evaluates the three attentional networks (ANTI trials) and two
types of vigilance (EV and AV trials), was modified in order to add the irrelevant-distractor
paradigm on the task (ID trials). Everything was used as in the original task, except that 8
ID trials were added to each of the 6 blocks of trials. These trials were built as ANTI trials
(see below), but with the replacement of non-target arrows by lines, and the inclusion of a
completely irrelevant distractor.
Attention, vigilance, and distraction in ADHD 1059

Procedure
The study was conducted between November 2019 and March 2020 – before COVID-19
preventive measures were implemented in our region. First, participants filled out an
online survey – via LimeSurvey (https://www.limesurvey.org) – composed of question-
naires about attention and distraction dispositions.1 The survey began with the cBAARS-
IV, the aBAARS-IV, and the ASRS-5, in that order, and ended with a question about previous
diagnosis of ADHD. After completing the survey, participants were invited in our
laboratory to conduct the cognitive task.
Upon arrival at the laboratory, participants were individually brought into a
soundproof room adequately illuminated. Participants were sitting at about 60 cm from
a 15-inch computer screen with an aspect ratio of 16:9. Participants were provided with
headphones at 60% sound level of the computer and were asked to turn-off or silence their
mobile phone. Then, the experimenter presented the ANTI-Vea, designed and run in E-
Prime (Version 2.0; Psychology Software Tools & Inc., 2012). The stimuli sequence and
correct responses for each type of trial are depicted in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Attention Network Test for Interaction and Vigilance – Executive and Arousal components
(ANTI-Vea) procedure in our study. Note. Panel A: Temporal sequence in Attention Network Test for
Interaction (ANTI) and Executive Vigilance (EV) trials. Target and flankers could appear above (see
example) or below the fixation point. Visual cue could appear in the same location as the target (valid cue;
see example), in the opposite location (invalid cue), or could not appear (no cue). Panel B: Temporal
sequence in Arousal Vigilance (AV) trials. Panel C: Temporal sequence in Irrelevant distraction (ID) trials.
Target and flankers could appear above (see example) or below the fixation point. Irrelevant distractor
could appear at the top (see example) or at the bottom of the screen; or it could not appear. Distractor
could be Pikachu (see example), SpongeBob, or Mickey Mouse. Panel D: Correct responses for each type
of trial. The five arrows are randomly displaced  2 px to generate noise in ANTI and ID trials, and the
target is displaced by 8 px in EV trials.

1
The full set of questionnaires, which is part of a larger project, is available at a public repository (https://osf.io/k8jdm/).
1060 Tao Coll-Martın et al.

All the trials lasted 4,100 ms and had a fixation point constantly present at the centre of
the screen. The ANTI-Vea comprised four different types of trials: the three from the
original task (ANTI, EV, and AV) and one added to measure irrelevant distraction (ID).
Trials were pseudorandomly presented within their experimental block. In ANTI (~54%;
48 trials per block) and EV trials (~18%; 16 trials per block), an auditory warning signal
sounded in half of the trials (tone condition), whereas in the other half, no warning signal
was presented (no-tone condition). Next, an asterisk (i.e., visual spatial cue) appeared in
two third of the trials, equally presented in the same (valid condition) or the opposite
(invalid condition) location as the upcoming target. A central arrow (i.e., target) with
four flankers appeared 100 ms later either above or below the fixation point. In ANTI
trials, participants had to discriminate the direction of the target (by pressing either ‘c’ for
leftward direction or ‘m’ for rightward direction) while ignoring the direction of the
flanking arrows, which could equally point to the same (congruent condition) or the
opposite (incongruent condition) as the target. In contrast, on EV trials the target
appeared vertically displaced for participants to detect the displacement by pressing the
space bar. In contrast, AV trials (~18%; 16 trials per block) only displayed a red millisecond
down counter at a variable time interval (900–2,100 ms) for participants to stop it by
pressing any key as fast as possible. Finally, ID trials (~9%; 8 trials per block) had the same
structure and correct response as ANTI trials without tone or cue, except (1) non-target
arrows were replaced by lines to reduce perceptual load and (2) in half of the trials, an
irrelevant distractor (SpongeBob, Pikachu, or Mickey Mouse; ~200 px width 9 ~200 px
height) appeared either at the top or at the bottom of the screen (above ~150 px- or below
~290 px- the central arrow) for the same time as the target (distractor present condition),
whereas no distractor was presented in the other half (distractor absent condition).
The ANTI-Vea task started with several phases of progressive practice, as in Luna et al.
(2018), with the addition of 8 ID trials in a last practice block of 48 randomized trials (24
ANTI, 8 EV, 8 AV, and 8 ID) without visual feedback. Before this practice block, the three
type of distractors were shown to participants, who were told to ‘ignore them for being
irrelevant to the task goal’. After this block, participants were given the possibility to
search for and ask any questions to the experimenter, who had left the room at the
beginning of the practice phase. Then, participants started the six seamless experimental
blocks (48 ANTI, 16 EV, 16 AV, 8 ID trials per block). The whole experimental session –
instructions and task – lasted ~50 min.

Data analysis
Behavioural data were treated based on Luna et al. (2018) through an R script. Because of a
computer or experimenter error, ANTI-Vea data from three participants were corrupted
and they could not be analysed. Participants with more than 25% errors in ANTI trials
(n = 4, among them, one of the two participants with ADHD) were excluded from all task
analyses, and those remaining participants with more than 25% errors in the distractor
present condition (n = 11) were excluded from all ID trials analyses.2 For ANTI and ID RT
analyses, trials with incorrect responses (ANTI = 5.75%; ID = 5.68%) and RTs smaller than
200 ms (ANTI = 1.24%; ID = 1.96%) or higher than 1,500 ms (ANTI = 0.45%; ID = 0.78%)
were excluded.

2
This filter for (ID) trials was added in response to the first data analysis, due to the extremely high percentage errors of these
participants in the distractor present condition (Mdn = 0.94). Most of them probably understood that ‘ignore the distractors’
meant ‘do not response when the distractor appears’.
Attention, vigilance, and distraction in ADHD 1061

We extracted several measures from the ANTI-Vea. For mean RT and percentage
errors in ANTI trials, we calculated the overall mean score and difference scores for
alerting (no-tone–tone conditions3), orienting (invalid–valid conditions), and congru-
ency (incongruent–congruent conditions). Following Luna, Barttfeld, Martın-Arevalo, and
Lupian~ ez (2021), EV outcomes included hits (percentage of correct responses in EV
trials), false alarms (percentage of space bar responses in ANTI trials with more than 2 px
from the target to at least one of its two adjacent flankers), and the signal detection theory
metrics of A’ (sensitivity) and B’’ (response bias). AV outcomes compressed the mean RT,
the standard deviation RT, and the percentage of lapses (RTs > 600 ms). Each EV and AV
outcome included both the overall performance and the slope of the regression line –
representing performance over the six experimental blocks. Finally, ID trials provided
interference from irrelevant distractor. As per Forster and Lavie (2016), we computed the
percentage increase in mean RT due to distraction by dividing the difference score
(distraction present – distractor absent conditions) by RT in the distractor absent
conditions. Distraction interference in percentage errors only employed raw difference
scores.
We analysed the quality of the ANTI-Vea measures. First, we checked the task
functioning. To this end, we conducted Student’s t-tests for indexes based on difference
scores. For indexes based on performance over experimental blocks (i.e., EV and AV
slopes), we conducted six-level one-way repeated-measures analyses of variance
(ANOVAs) with planned comparisons to test the polynomial linear component. Where
appropriate, Huynh–Feldt or Greenhouse–Geisser corrections were applied. Second, we
estimated the reliability of each ANTI-Vea outcome. To do so, we used a permutation-
based split-half correlation approach with 10,000 random splits and then applied the
Spearman–Brown correction (for a rationale, see Parsons, Kruijt, & Fox, 2019). These
reliability estimations were computed by adapting an R script that had previously been
used with the original ANTI-Vea (Luna, Roca, et al., 2020).
Finally, we used JASP (Version 0.13; JASP Team, 2020) to test the correlations between
the three questionnaires of ADHD symptoms (i.e., cBAARS-IV, aBAARS-IV, and ASRS-5)
and the 24 ANTI-Vea outcomes (8 ANTI, 8 EV, 6 AV, and 2 ID). Normality was violated for
the vast majority of pairwise comparisons, as assessed by Shapiro–Wilk tests. Therefore,
we used the Kendall’s s rank correlation coefficient, interpreted as per Gilpin (1993): .07 =
small, .21 = medium, .35 = large. We conducted one- or two-sided contrasts according to
whether they were based on directional or non-directional hypotheses. Statistical
significance was set at a = .05.

Results
ADHD self-reports
Figure 2 shows the distribution of ADHD symptoms compared to an estimated normative
sample (for a detailed procedure and statistical report, see Text S1). Taking together,
although ADHD symptom distributions in our sample might slightly differ from the
population, this does not seem to undermine its spread and variability throughout each
scale, as compared to an estimated normative sample.

3
Although the measure exclusively considering the no-cue conditions is a purer measure of alertness, the measure considering all
conditions is more powerful and reliable (de Souza et al., 2021).
1062 Tao Coll-Martın et al.

Figure 2. Distribution of total ADHD symptom scores for each of the three scales compared to an
estimated normative sample. Note. N = 120. ADHD = Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Histogram and blue solid line represent the frequency and density curve of ADHD total scores in the
study sample. Dashed black lines represent the density curve of ADHD total scores in an estimated
normative sample. This normative, equally sized sample was obtained by extracting 120 quantiles form a
large bootstrapped sample (N = 10,000) that fits the percentile values available in Barkley (2011). Vertical
dashed red lines represent the normative 95th percentile, a cut-off to identify individuals at high risk of
ADHD. The vertical dashed orange line represents a threshold for ADHD screening purposes. Panel A:
cBAARS-IV = Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Childhood Symptoms. Panel B: aBAARS-IV = Barkley
Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Current Symptoms. Panel C: ASRS-5 = Adult ADHD Self-Report Screening
Scale for DSM-5.

Unsurprisingly, the cBAARS-IV (M = 29.6, SD = 8.46), the aBAARS-IV (M = 28.4, SD =


7.32), and the ASRS-5 (M = 8.04, SD = 3.54) showed significant positive correlations
among them, with effect sizes from medium to large. Concretely, for the cBAARS-IV with
the aBAARS, r(118) = .51, p < .001, for the cBAARS-IV with the ASRS-5, r(118) = .35, p <
.001, and for the aBAARS-IV with the ASRS-5, r(118) = .70, p < .001. Interestingly, the
correlation between the two measures of symptoms in adulthood was higher than those
between these measures and the one of symptoms in childhood.
Attention, vigilance, and distraction in ADHD 1063

ANTI-Vea
Table 1 shows descriptive statistics, reliability, and correlations with ADHD symptoms for
each of the ANTI-Vea indexes. Correlations among ANTI-Vea indexes are presented in
Table S2.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics, reliability, and Kendall’s rank correlations with ADHD symptoms in
childhood and adulthood for all ANTI-Vea outcomes

Kendall’s s correlation coefficient

ANTI-Vea index M SD rSB cBAARS-IV aBAARS-IV ASRS-5

ANTI outcomes
RT overall 600 95 .99 .06 .08 .03
% errors overall 5.75 4.34 .91 .06 .08 .06
RT alerting 20 23 .47 .13* .10 .03
% errors alerting 2.33 3.79 .51 .15* .09 .05
RT orienting 35 26 .36 .01 .02 .11
% errors orienting 0.65 3.90 .26 .04 .01 .05
RT congruency 40 28 .66 .05 .03 .03
% errors congruency 1.46 4.21 .60 .03 .01 .06
EV outcomes
% hits 68.62 17.29 .94 .001 .05 .01
% false alarms 5.16 5.09 .85 .001 .11 .02
A0 (sensitivity) 0.90 0.04 .88 .01 .02 .02
B″ (response bias) 0.59 0.35 .86 .02 .11 .02
% Hits slope 1.74 3.00 .27 .02 .04 .11*
% False alarms slope 0.42 1.51 .40 .05 .09 .11*
A0 (sensitivity) slope 0.003 0.01 .40 .03 .09 .14*
B″ (response bias) slope 0.04 0.10 .26 .01 .11 .08
AV outcomes
RT mean 504 58 .97 .11* .04 .10
RT standard deviation 97 49 .88 .11* .06 .01
% lapses 12.98 14.35 .96 .11* .07 .08
RT mean slope 5.36 12.47 .75 .17** .06 .01
RT SD slope 6.10 12.62 .54 .08 .02 .05
% lapses slope 1.99 3.74 .78 .18** .06 .01
ID outcomesa
% interference in RT 5.37 7.35 .21 .02 .02 .03
% errors interference 0.61 6.49 .03 .01 .02 .05

Note. n = 113. According to our hypotheses, correlation tests are one-tailed for positive correlations in
all indexes, except (1) orienting (RT and errors; two-tailed); (2) hits and A0 (both overall and slope; one-
tailed for negative correlations); and (3) B″ (only overall; two-tailed), as it is the only index not directionally
associated with performance in vigilance.
aBAARS-IV = Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Current Symptoms; ADHD = Attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder; ANTI = Attention Network Test for Interaction; ASRS-5 = Adult ADHD Self-
Report Screening Scale for DSM-5; AV = Arousal Vigilance; cBAARS-IV = Barkley Adult ADHD Rating
Scale-IV: Childhood Symptoms; EV = Executive vigilance; ID = Irrelevant distraction; rSB = Spearman–
Brown reliability coefficient; RT = Reaction time.
a
n = 102.; *p < .05, one-tailed.; **p < .01, one-tailed. No other p < .05 appeared with exploratory two-
tailed tests.
1064 Tao Coll-Martın et al.

ANTI outcomes
As reported by Luna et al. (2018), ANTI trials revealed effects of alerting, orienting, and
congruency for RTs and, except orienting (p < .077), for percentage errors. Specifically,
RTs were faster in the tone than in the no-tone trials, t(112) = 9.18, p < .001, d = 0.84,
in valid than invalid trials, t(112) = 14.45, p < .001, d = 1.36, and in congruent than
incongruent trials, t(112) = 14.80, p < .001, d = 1.39. Percentage errors were higher in
no-tone than in tone trials, t(112) = 6.54, p < .001, d = 0.62, and in incongruent than
congruent trials, t(112) = 3.69, p < .001, d = 0.35. Reliability of ANTI outcomes ranged
from rSB = .26 to rSB = .99, with the usual higher values for overall than for difference scores
(see Table 1).
In line with our hypotheses, we observed significant positive correlations between the
cBAARS-IV and the magnitude of the alerting effect (i.e., the difference between no-tone
and tone trials) in both RTs, s(111) = .13, p = .021, and percentage errors, s(111) = .15, p =
.013. Such correlations were not significant for the aBAARS-IV (both p > .063) and the
ASRS-5 (both p > .248). Contrary to our predictions, none of the three ADHD symptom
self-reports significantly correlated with the overall scores of RT (all p > .193) or
percentage errors (all p > .186) nor with the congruency effect, either measured with RTs
(all p > .205) or percentage errors (all p > .314). Finally, as expected, orienting indexes of
RT (all but one p > .804) and percentage errors (all p > .085) did not correlate with any
ADHD symptom self-report.

EV outcomes
The four EV indexes of overall performance (i.e., hits, false alarms, A0 , and B″) yielded high
reliability scores, from rSB = .85 to rSB = .94 (see Table 1). However, none of these indexes
showed significant correlations with any of the three ADHD symptom self-reports (all but
one p > .077).
Consistent with Luna et al. (2018), we found a main effect of experimental block for
hits, F(5, 560) = 8.85, p < .001, g2 = .07, false alarms, F(4.51, 505.16) = 2.56, p < .032, g2 =
.02, and B″, F(4.79, 536.58) = 4.13, p < .001, g2 = .04. Planned comparisons revealed a
linear component indicating that, over the six blocks, there was a decrement in the
percentage of hits, t(560) = 6.27, p < .001, and false alarms, t(112) = 2.94, p = .004, as
well as an increase in B″, t(112) = 4.01, p < .001. Different from Luna et al., we also
observed the block effect on A0 , F(4.28, 478.82) = 2.91, p < .019, g2 = .03, yielding a linear
decrease over the blocks, t(112) = 3.13, p = .002. These indexes of slope exhibited a low
reliability, ranging from rSB = .26 to rSB = .40.
Concerning our hypotheses, only the ASRS-5 correlated with three indexes of EV
slopes. Specifically, higher ASRS-5 scores predicted a greater decrement in percentage of
hits, s(111) = .11, p = .044, and A0 (sensitivity), s(111) = .14, p = .017, as well as a more
attenuated decrement in percentage of false alarms, s(111) = .11, p = .044. The
remaining correlations were not significant (all p > .085).

AV outcomes
Similar to EV, we found high reliability for the three AV indexes of overall performance,
oscillating between rSB = .88 and rSB = .97 (see Table 1). As predicted, the cBAARS-IV
exhibited significant positive correlations with the three indexes, namely mean RT, s(111)
= .11, p = .043, standard deviation of the RT, s(111) = .11, p = .044, and percentage of
Attention, vigilance, and distraction in ADHD 1065

lapses, s(111) = .11, p = .041. Neither the aBAARS-IV nor the ASRS-5 significantly
correlated with any AV index (all p > .061).
In line with Luna et al. (2018), there was a main effect of experimental block for mean
RT, F(3.94, 441.51) = 8.47, p < .001, g2 = .07, standard deviation of the RT, F(4.18, 468.41)
= 7.46, p < .001, g2 = .06, and percentage of lapses, F(3.46, 387.16) = 14.38, p < .001, g2 =
.11. All these variables increased linearly across the blocks, namely mean RT, t(112) = 4.56,
p < .001, standard deviation of the RT, t(112) = 5.13, p = .001, and percentage of lapses,
t(112) = 5.68, p < .001. Reliability for the three indexes of slope ranged from rSB = .54 to
rSB = .78.
Like for AV overall performance, only the cBAARS-IV exhibited significant correlations
with indexes of AV slopes, concretely, with the slope of mean RT, s(111) = .17, p = .004,
and the slope of percentage of lapses, s(111) = .18, p = .002; but not with the slope of
standard deviation of the RT (p = .099). No significant correlations were found between
the two other self-reports (i.e., the aBAARS-IV and the ASRS-5) and the three measures of
AV slope (all p > .169).

ID outcomes
In the same vein as Forster and Lavie (2016), participants were slower in the presence
(M = 640, SD = 103) versus in the absence (M = 608, SD = 103) of the irrelevant
distractor, t(101) = 7.14, p < .001, d = 0.71. Nevertheless, both conditions did not
significantly differ in the percentage of errors, t(101) = 0.95, p = .342, d = 0.09. Reliability
for indexes of percentage increase in mean RT (rSB = .21) and percentage errors (rSB = .03)
was found to be low. Contrary to our predictions, none of the three self-reports correlated
with either percentage increase in mean RT (all p > .310) or percentage errors (all
p > .240).

Discussion
This study aimed at analysing the main attentional processes related to ADHD symptoms,
namely attentional networks, executive and arousal vigilance, and distraction. To do so,
we modified a single, fine-grained task (i.e., the ANTI-Vea) to add a distraction component
(Forster & Lavie, 2016). Based on a dimensional model of ADHD, we employed a
community sample of undergraduates and measured retrospective and current subjective
ADHD symptoms. Although the ANTI-Vea worked successfully, the reliability was
reduced for many indexes. A significant relation was observed between ADHD symptoms
and a higher alerting effect, but not orienting or congruency effects. ADHD symptom
ratings also related to a poorer performance over time in EV and to alterations in different
AV measures. No association was found between ADHD symptoms and irrelevant
distraction. Worthy of note, our pattern of results was not consistent across the three
ADHD symptom self-reports or the specific task indexes. Therefore, our hypotheses were
supported only partially. These findings have implications for the neurocognitive
mechanisms of ADHD symptoms and for the role of the ANTI-Vea in this literature.

Attentional networks
In line with our hypothesis, the finding of a higher alerting effect associated with
ADHD symptoms is consistent with Berger and Posner’s (2000) predictions. It also fits
1066 Tao Coll-Martın et al.

the state regulation deficit account of ADHD (Sergeant, 2000, 2005; Sonuga-Barke,
Wiersema, van der Meere, & Roeyers, 2010). From this view, a task context such as
the ANTI-Vea, which has been shown to be suitable to measure vigilance decrement,
would tend to induce underactivation. This state would be especially detrimental for
the tonic arousal or activation in individuals with higher ADHD symptoms. As a
consequence, environmental stimulation, such as warning signals, would compensate
for that underactivated state, thereby bringing performance to normal levels. Although
impaired alerting network is well established in ADHD children (Arora et al., 2020),
this phenomenon has been less frequently reported in adults with ADHD (Oberlin
et al., 2005). Our findings are inconsistent with Zamani Sani et al.’s (2020) report of
no association between alerting network with ADHD symptoms in non-clinical adults,
despite they had higher statistical power than us. Differences in the task length or
difficulty, in the type of warning signal (auditory vs. visual), or in the measure of
ADHD symptoms (childhood vs. adulthood) could help explain these contradictory
findings.
The lack of an association between ADHD symptoms and the orienting effect in our
data is theoretically and empirically consistent with previous literature (Arora et al., 2020;
Berger & Posner, 2000; Lundervold et al., 2011; Zamani Sani et al., 2020). Of note, most
research uses the original ANT, which provides a global index of orienting network.
However, tasks such as the ANTI or the ANTI-Vea specifically assesses exogenous
orienting, which is related to automatic processes (Ishigami et al., 2016). The scarce
research on exogenous orienting in ADHD has failed to find alterations in children
(Casagrande et al., 2012; Mullane et al., 2011), which is consonant with our results with
symptoms in non-clinical adults.
Contrary to our hypothesis, we could not find an association between ADHD
symptoms and the congruency effect. Indeed, executive attention has been
hypothesized to be deficient in ADHD (Berger & Posner, 2000), and evidence using
the ANT in children (Arora et al., 2020) and adults (Lampe et al., 2007; Oberlin et al.,
2005; but see Lundervold et al., 2011) has supported this notion. However, both
Zamani Sani et al. (2020) and us failed to extend those findings to non-clinical
samples. From a dimensional view of ADHD, it could be argued that the association
between executive attention and ADHD symptoms is not sufficiently meaningful in
non-clinical adults. In parallel, we believe that the difference between tasks is highly
relevant in this regard. In the ANT/ANTI, the flanker task is performed as a single task
whose only goal is to respond to the target direction. By contrast, the ANTI-Vea
incorporates a second goal into the mindset, which is simultaneous to the first one –
namely to respond to the vertical displacement of the target. This increase in working
memory load has been found to reduce the flanker interference, leading to a lower
congruency effect (Luna, Telga, Telga, Vadillo, & Lupian ~ ez, 2020). Indeed, the
congruency effect we obtained for RT and percentage errors was less than half of the
usually reported in the ANT in non-clinical adults (MacLeod et al., 2010). This
substantially lower congruency effect probably makes the index less sensitive to
modulation from individual differences, such as ADHD symptoms, which is a concern
about the ANTI-Vea to bear in mind. Alternatively, this result could be interpreted in
the sense that adults with higher ADHD symptom scores, when appropriately
challenged by task demands, as in the ANTI-Vea task, can overcome any putative
executive deficit they might have.
Attention, vigilance, and distraction in ADHD 1067

Executive and arousal vigilance


Partial support for our hypothesis of a poorer EV associated with ADHD symptoms was
limited to indexes of performance over time (i.e., vigilance decrement). This is consonant
with Craig and Klein’s (2019) finding in non-clinical adults. However, this is rather the
opposite pattern as Huang-Pollock et al.’s (2012) meta-analysis with ADHD children, who
found larger deficits in overall performance than in performance over time. Performance
over time is considered the appropriate form to measure vigilance (Huang-Pollock et al.,
2012; Tucha et al., 2017), although numerous tasks used in ADHD research have failed to
measure it (Johnson et al., 2001; Marchetta et al., 2008; Tucha et al., 2017). However, the
ANTI-Vea task has been specifically developed to induce such vigilance decrement.
Further research comparing clinical ADHD with non-clinical controls in the ANTI-Vea is
likely to find larger and more consistent differences in vigilance decrement than
previously reported.
Different from other EV tasks, vigilance decrement in the ANTI-Vea mainly manifests as
a change to a more conservative response criterion, rather than a loss of sensitivity4.
However, our data showed ADHD symptoms to be associated with a decrement of
sensitivity over the task, but not with a more conservative response style – indeed, we
observed the opposite trend. This pattern, consistent with clinical research (Huang-
Pollock et al., 2012, 2020), suggests that EV impairments in ADHD symptoms are more a
matter of sensitivity than a response bias (Thomson, Besner, & Smilek, 2016). However,
the relatively low rate of false alarms in this literature prevents us from ruling out a floor
effect that might be overestimating the role of sensitivity at the expense of underesti-
mating the role of response criterion. Indeed, Luna, Roca, et al. (2020) found a drop in
sensitivity only among those participants with a percentage of false alarms close to the
floor (≤5%) in the first block, but not for the rest of participants. A similar pattern was
observed in our data.
Furthermore, we found ADHD symptoms – only retrospectively reported in childhood
– to be associated with a diminished AV, in both mean RT and response variability (i.e.,
standard deviation and percentage of lapses). These results support our hypothesis and
are consonant with the scarce clinical research comparing adults with ADHD in overall
and over time AV measures of response variability (Gmehlin et al., 2016; Tucha et al.,
2017). However, different from clinical studies, we also found that a greater increment of
mean RT was positively associated with ADHD symptoms. As in the case of EV, the fact
that the ANTI-Vea is the only task of this literature that generates decrement in AV might
account for such discrepancies. Moreover, higher response variability associated with
ADHD is ubiquitous to multiple types of tasks (Epstein et al., 2011; Kofler et al., 2013). Our
data extended this phenomenon to symptoms in non-clinical adults in an AV task that is
embedded in a complex structure (i.e., the ANTI-Vea).
The relationship between EV and AV is also relevant to the field of ADHD. Grounded on
van Zomeren and Brouwer’s (1994) attentional model, Gmehlin et al. (2016) argued that
sustained alertness (strongly related to AV) is a precondition for more complex attentional
functions over time – including processes that could be considered as components of EV.
According to this view, Gmehlin et al. found that, when controlling for the slope of AV
(i.e., change in percentage of lapses across blocks), differences between ADHD and
control groups in EV disappeared. By contrast, there is evidence supporting that EV and

4
Although a loss of sensitivity over the task has been reported in our data as well as in studies with high statistical power (Feltmate
et al., 2020; Luna, Roca, et al, 2020), this effect size seems to be lower than the effect on the response criterion.
1068 Tao Coll-Martın et al.

AV, albeit probably related, constitute independent components of vigilance (Luna, 2019;
Luna, Roman-Caballero, et al., 2020; Sanchıs et al., 2020). In our data, an equivalence test
(Lakens, 2017) showed that the correlation of r(111) = .06 between the slopes of hits
(EV) and lapses (AV) fell below the upper bound of r = .1 (p = .044). This suggests that EV
and AV do not depend on each other in a meaningful way. Furthermore, the partial
correlation between ADHD symptoms (ASRS-5) and the slope of the percentage of hits,
controlling for the percentage of lapses, remained significant, s(110) = .11, p = .037. This
result, inconsistent with Gmehlin et al., does not support the idea of AV as a prerequisite
for EV and could be in line with the notion of ADHD as a heterogeneous condition (Fair,
Bathula, Nikolas, & Nigg, 2012).

Irrelevant distraction
Although we found an acceptable effect of ID on the RT, the lack of correlation with
ADHD symptoms does not support our hypothesis, and it is contrary to Forster and Lavie’s
(2016) findings. In fact, our results are in line with Meier’s (2020) failed attempt to
replicate Forster and Lavie’s results using exactly the same task and a similar sample
composition (i.e., university students). Against the case of a Forster and Lavie’s false
positive, it should be noted that they also found a positive correlation in a second
experiment with a different task as well as in a case–control study comparing ADHD with
controls (Forster et al., 2014). Therefore, the possibility of a true effect is still likely.
Regarding the event of a false negative in Meier’s and our study, assuming the effect found
by Forster and Lavie (r = .32), a very high statistical power was achieved by Meier (.99) and
us (.95). Moreover, Meier found Bayesian evidence favouring the null hypothesis. Of note,
the reliability of the ID index reported by Meier and us was rather low (rSB = .26 and .21,
respectively). This importantly reduces the size of the observed correlation with ADHD
symptoms, leading to the need for a larger sample size and higher reliability scores to reach
the desired power (Parsons et al., 2019). Further studies are warranted not only to
consistently determine the existence of a positive correlation between the ID effect and
ADHD symptoms, but also to test whether this correlation is stronger than those using
task-relevant distractors (e.g., flanker task).

Measuring ADHD symptoms in childhood and adulthood


To gain a better knowledge of ADHD symptomatology, we used three different but
complementary measures: one for symptoms in childhood (cBAARS-IV) and the other two
for symptoms in adulthood (aBAARS-IV and ASRS-5). Characterizing developmental
trajectories in ADHD is important to obtain more homogeneous subgroups and
phenotypes, also at the neurocognitive level (Luo et al., 2019; Sonuga-Barke & Halperin,
2010). In a longitudinal study, Moffitt et al. (2015) found that ADHD in childhood had very
little overlap with the adult-onset form of ADHD. Moreover, at age 38, only participants
with ADHD in childhood showed neuropsychological deficits, including overall perfor-
mance in EV. Although EV was the only domain where we found poorer performance to
be associated with ADHD symptoms in adulthood but not in childhood, our altered EV
indexes were of performance over time. In fact, our general picture of results
differentiated ADHD symptoms in childhood versus in adulthood. While the former
predicted alterations in arousal (i.e., alerting network and AV), the latter were negatively
associated with executive outcomes (i.e., EV decrement). This dissociation is, to some
extent, consonant with Halperin and Schulz’s (2006) neurodevelopmental model of
Attention, vigilance, and distraction in ADHD 1069

ADHD. This model postulates that, while the early onset of the disorder is associated with
subcortical structures involving arousal, the persistence of the ADHD in the adulthood is
related to prefrontal regions which underlie executive processes. The fact that this model
could explain developmental differences in the neuropsychological correlates of non-
clinical symptoms coheres with the dimensional nature of ADHD.
Within ADHD symptoms in adults, it is noteworthy that, while the ASRS-5 yielded some
significant correlations with ANTI-Vea measures, the aBAARS-IV did not. Besides the
possible statistical errors that will be mentioned in the next section, a tentative account is
related to the different form of both self-reports to measure adult ADHD symptoms. The
aBAARS-IV uses the 18 DSM-IV criteria (without examples) as items. The content of these
items is generic for children and adults. By contrast, the ASRS-5 is not only based on DSM-5
criteria, which better reflect the adult presentation, but also include items specifically
designed to detect ADHD in adults (Ustun et al., 2017). Therefore, instead of a lack of
relationship between adult ADHD symptoms and neuropsychological deficits, it might be
that highly sensitive self-reports are needed to accurately capture the adult presentation of
ADHD symptomatology, along with its underlying alterations.

Limitations
We have identified four main caveats in our study. The first one regards the generalization
of our findings. Our community sample consisted of undergraduates, with a majority of
women. Not only are both sociodemographic characteristics unrepresentative of the
general population, but they also are negatively correlated with ADHD symptom severity
(Arnett, Pennington, Willcutt, DeFries, & Olson, 2015; Birchwood & Daley, 2012).
Despite this sampling bias, our statistical analyses do not suggest that the distribution of
ADHD symptoms in our sample is meaningfully more homogeneous – and less sensitive to
correlate with behavioural tasks – than in a representative community sample. Moreover,
only two out of our 120 participants (i.e., 1.6%) had the diagnosis of ADHD. While this
proportion is lower than the estimated worldwide prevalence of the disorder in adults
(3.6%), it is close to the Spanish prevalence (1.2%; Fayyad et al., 2017). Of note, a study
conducted at a Spanish primary care centre found an extremely low prevalence (0.04%) of
registered ADHD diagnoses in adults (Aragones et al., 2010). In any case, our unsubstantial
number of potential participants with ADHD prevents our results from having direct
implications for clinical ADHD research and practice. Therefore, replications of our
findings with more representative samples including a substantial amount of ADHD
individuals are warranted.
The second concern has to do with the construct validity of ADHD symptoms in our
study. We failed to assess relevant symptoms such as depression or anxiety and did not ask
for other psychiatric disorders. Thus, it is not clear to what degree the ratings obtained
from our sample validly reflect an ADHD symptom status rather than a general
psychological distress severity. In fact, symptoms of depression and anxiety have been
linked to ADHD symptoms (Combs, Canu, Broman-Fulks, Rocheleau, & Nieman, 2015),
and ADHD diagnosis requires that its symptoms be not better explained by another
disorder such as mood or anxiety disorders (APA, 2013). Ultimately, we cannot rule out
that the relation found between ADHD symptoms and attentional functioning in our study
might be a by-product of a third construct (e.g., depression, stress, other disorders,
intelligence, sociodemographic factors). Future research should properly assess and
control for these potential confounders as well as incorporate measures of ADHD
symptoms beyond self-reported questionnaires (i.e., other-reports, clinical interviews).
1070 Tao Coll-Martın et al.

Third, the general picture of correlations between attentional processes and ADHD
symptom self-reports shows that, at best, our hypotheses were supported only partially.
That is, no attentional domain exhibited significant correlations with ADHD symptoms
across the three self-reports. Also, for those observed significant correlations the effect
sizes were at most small to medium. Besides the sampling bias discussed above, a more
plausible reason is related to the psychometric properties of the ANTI-Vea indexes.
Although our task reliability scores are similar to the ones reported in Luna, Roca, et al.
(2020), the reliability found for difference scores and slopes tended to be fairly low. This
limitation, which is also inherent to most cognitive tasks (Dang, King, & Inzlicht, 2020;
Hedge, Powell, & Sumner, 2018), could dramatically attenuate the observed correlations
coefficients. Futures studies should either attempt to improve the reliability of their tasks
or use valid methods to correct for low reliability to estimate the true correlation between
ADHD symptoms and attentional processes.
The fourth limitation concerns the control of the type I error rate in our results. Since
our study did not reach a very high statistical power, strict corrections for multiple
comparisons were likely to dramatically increase the rate of false negatives. Following
McDonald’s (2014) suggestion, we conducted an exploratory secondary analysis where
we applied the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995) to our
correlation matrix in order to control for a false discovery rate of 20%. Groups for multiple
comparisons were set according to our hypotheses. The significant findings of this
corrected pattern of correlations are roughly similar to such comparisons before the
correction (see Table S3). In any case, to attain a more proper control of both types of
statistical errors, our study needs to be replicated with a larger sample.

Conclusion
To conclude, our modified version of the ANTI-Vea was useful for measuring the
functioning of the attentional networks, executive and arousal vigilance, and irrelevant
distraction. This fine-grained distinction between attentional processes is relevant to gain
a depth understanding of the mechanisms underlying ADHD symptomatology. In a sample
of undergraduates, we found that subjective ADHD symptoms in childhood were related
to alerting and arousal processes, while symptoms in adulthood were rather associated
with the executive component of vigilance. Different from other neuropsychological
tasks, the ANTI-Vea could successfully induce vigilance decrement. However, compared
to other tasks (e.g., ANT), our index of the executive attentional network (i.e., congruency
effect) was fairly reduced by task demands. Moreover, some of the task indexes (especially
those involving difference scores) exhibited poor reliability. Although replications with
larger and clinical samples are necessary, this thorough approach to the attentional
processes underlying ADHD symptoms might shed light on the search for more
homogeneous subgroups of the disorder.

Acknowledgements
Our work was supported by a predoctoral fellowship (FPU17/06169) awarded to Tao Coll-
Martın from the Spanish Ministry of Education, Culture, and Sport; a research project grant
(PSI2016-79812-P) awarded to Hugo Carretero-Dios from the Spanish Ministry of Economy,
Industry, and Competitiveness; and a research project grant (PSI2017-84926-P) awarded to
Juan Lupian~ ez from the Spanish Ministry of Economy, Industry, and Competitiveness.
Attention, vigilance, and distraction in ADHD 1071

The funders had no role in any stage of the development and publication of this work. We thank
Sophie Forster for her generous availability and valuable intellectual inputs, Fernando Luna for
his kind willingness to help with the reliability analysis, and Mateu Servera for his enthusiastic
help with the instrument selection process.

Conflicts of interest
All authors declare no conflict of interest.

Author contributions
Tao Coll-Martın (Conceptualization; Data curation; Formal analysis; Funding acquisition;
Investigation; Methodology; Software; Visualization; Writing – original draft) Hugo
Carretero-Dios (Formal analysis; Funding acquisition; Project administration; Supervision;
Validation; Writing – review & editing) Juan Lupian ~ez (Conceptualization; Funding
acquisition; Methodology; Project administration; Resources; Supervision; Validation;
Writing – review & editing).

Data availability statement


The data and materials that support the findings of this study are openly available in the Open
Science Framework at https://osf.io/k8jdm/

References
Adolfsd
ottir, S., Sørensen, L., & Lundervold, A. J. (2008). The Attention Network test: A
characteristic pattern of deficits in children with ADHD. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 4,
Article 9. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-4-9
Advokat, C., Martino, L., Hill, B. D., & Gouvier, W. (2007). Continuous Performance Test (CPT) of
college students with ADHD, psychiatric disorders, cognitive deficits, or no diagnosis. Journal
of Attention Disorders, 10(3), 253–256. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054706292106
Albrecht, B., Brandeis, D., Uebel, H., Heinrich, H., Mueller, U. C., Hasselhorn, M., . . . Banaschewski,
T. (2008). Action monitoring in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, their
nonaffected siblings, and normal control subjects: Evidence for an endophenotype. Biological
Psychiatry, 64, 615–625. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.12.016
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1995). Manual diagn ostico y estadıstico de los transtornos
mentales [Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders] (J. J. L opez-Ibor Ali~
no, Dir.; M.
Valdes Miyar, Coord.; 4th ed.). Barcelona, Spain: Masson. (Original work published 1994).
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
Aragones, E., Pi~
nol, J. L., Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., L
opez-Cortacans, G., Caballero, A., & Bosch, R. (2010).
Prevalence in adults of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder using the medical records of
primary care. Revista Espanola ~ 
De Salud Publica, 84, 415–420. https://www.mscbs.gob.es/
biblioPublic/publicaciones/recursos_propios/resp/revista_cdrom/vol84/vol84_4/RS844C_
415ingles.pdf
Arnett, A. B., Pennington, B. F., Willcutt, E. G., DeFries, J. C., & Olson, R. K. (2015). Sex differences in
ADHD symptom severity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56, 632–639. https://
doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12337
1072 Tao Coll-Martın et al.

Arora, S., Lawrence, M. A., & Klein, R. M. (2020). The attention network test database: ADHD and
cross-cultural applications. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article 388. https://doi.org/10.3389/
fpsyg.2020.00388
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions:
Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94. https://doi.org/
10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65
Barkley, R. A. (2011). Barkley adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV (BAARS-IV). New York, NY: Guilford
Press.
Barkley, R. A., & Murphy, K. R. (2011). The nature of executive function (EF) deficits in daily life
activities in adults with ADHD and their relationship to performance on EF tests. Journal of
Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 33(2), 137–158. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s10862-011-9217-x
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful
approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B
(Methodological), 57(1), 289–300. http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/2346101
Berger, A., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Pathologies of brain attentional networks. Neuroscience &
Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(1), 3–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00046-9
Birchwood, J., & Daley, D. (2012). Brief report: The impact of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD) symptoms on academic performance in an adolescent community sample. Journal of
Adolescence, 35(1), 225–231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.08.011
Booth, J. E., Carlson, C. L., & Tucker, D. M. (2007). Performance on a neurocognitive measure of
alerting differentiates ADHD combined and inattentive subtypes: A preliminary report. Archives
of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 423–432. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.017
Brodeur, D. A., & Pond, M. (2001). The development of selective attention in children with attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 229–239. https://doi.
org/10.1023/a:1010381731658
Bueno, V. F., Kozasa, E. H., da Silva, M. A., Alves, T. M., Louz~a, M. R., & Pompeia, S. (2015).
Mindfulness meditation improves mood, quality of life, and attention in adults with attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder. BioMed Research International, 2015, Article 962857. https://
doi.org/10.1155/2015/962857
Bush, G. (2010). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and attention networks.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 278–300. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.120
Callejas, A., Lupian~ ez, J., & Tudela, P. (2004). The three attentional networks: On their
independence and interactions. Brain and Cognition, 54, 225–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.bandc.2004.02.012
Casagrande, M., Martella, D., Ruggiero, M. C., Maccari, L., Paloscia, C., Rosa, C., & Pasini, A. (2012).
Assessing attentional systems in children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 27(1), 30–44. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acr085
~ ez, J. (2021). To be attentive, do not react: Linking dispositional
Casedas, L., Cebolla, A., & Lupian
mindfulness to attentional networks and vigilance performance. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/
10.31234/osf.io/stxen
Castellanos, F. X., & Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:
The search for endophenotypes. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 617–628. https://doi.org/10.
1038/nrn896
Chan, E., Mattingley, J. B., Huang-Pollock, C., English, T., Hester, R., Vance, A., & Bellgrove, M. A.
(2009). Abnormal spatial asymmetry of selective attention in ADHD. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 50, 1064–1072. https://doi.org/10.1111/
j.1469-7610.2009.02096.x
Coghill, D., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2012). Annual research review: Categories versus dimensions in
the classification and conceptualisation of child and adolescent mental disorders: Implications of
recent empirical study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 53,
469–489. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02511.x
Attention, vigilance, and distraction in ADHD 1073

Cohen, A. L., & Shapiro, S. K. (2007). Exploring the performance differences on the flicker task and
the Conners’ Continuous Performance Test in adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention
Disorders, 11(1), 49–63. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054706292162
Combs, M. A., Canu, W. H., Broman-Fulks, J. J., Rocheleau, C. A., & Nieman, D. C. (2015). Perceived
stress and ADHD symptoms in adults. Journal of Attention Disorders, 19, 425–434. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1087054712459558
Craig, C. M., & Klein, M. I. (2019). The abbreviated vigilance task and its attentional contributors.
Human Factors, 61, 426–439. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720818822350
Cussen, A., Sciberras, E., Ukoumunne, O. C., & Efron, D. (2012). Relationship between
symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and family functioning: A community-
based study. European Journal of Pediatrics, 171(2), 271–280. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s00431-011-1524-4
Dang, J., King, K. M., & Inzlicht, M. (2020). Why are self-report and behavioral measures weakly
correlated? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(4), 267–269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.
01.007
de Souza, R., Faria, A., & Klein, R. M. (2021). On the origins and evolution of the attention network
tests. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 126, 560–572. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ne
ubiorev.2021.02.028
Dinges, D. F., & Powell, J. W. (1985). Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple
visual RT task during sustained operations. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments &
Computers, 17(6), 652–655. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200977
Epstein, J. N., Conners, C. K., Sitarenios, G., & Erhardt, D. (1998). Continuous performance test
results of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12,
155–168. https://doi.org/10.1076/clin.12.2.155.2000
Epstein, J. N., Johnson, D. E., Varia, I. M., & Conners, C. K. (2001). Neuropsychological assessment of
response inhibition in adults with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology, 23, 362–371. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.23.3.362.1186
Epstein, J. N., Langberg, J. M., Rosen, P. J., Graham, A., Narad, M. E., Antonini, T. N., . . . Altaye, M.
(2011). Evidence for higher reaction time variability for children with ADHD on a range of
cognitive tasks including reward and event rate manipulations. Neuropsychology, 25, 427–441.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022155
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter
in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149. https://doi.org/10.3758/
BF03203267
Esterman, M., & Rothlein, D. (2019). Models of sustained attention. Current Opinion in Psychology,
29, 174–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.03.005
Fair, D. A., Bathula, D., Nikolas, M. A., & Nigg, J. T. (2012). Distinct neuropsychological subgroups in
typically developing youth inform heterogeneity in children with ADHD. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 6769–6774. https://doi.
org/10.1073/pnas.1115365109
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and
independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340–347.
https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902317361886
Fayyad, J., Sampson, N. A., Hwang, I., Adamowski, T., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Al-Hamzawi, A. . . . WHO
World Mental Health Survey Collaborators. (2017). The descriptive epidemiology of DSM-IV
adult ADHD in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Attention Deficit
and Hyperactivity Disorders, 9, 47–65. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-016-0208-3
Feltmate, B. B. T., Hurst, A. J., & Klein, R. M. (2020). Effects of fatigue on attention and vigilance as
measured with a modified attention network test. Experimental Brain Research, 238, 2507–
2519. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-020-05902-y
Forster, S. (2013). Distraction and mind-wandering under load. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article
283. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00283
1074 Tao Coll-Martın et al.

Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2008). Failures to ignore entirely irrelevant distractors: The role of load.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 73–83. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.
14.1.73
Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2016). Establishing the attention-distractibility trait. Psychological Science,
27(2), 203–212. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615617761
Forster, S., Robertson, D. J., Jennings, A., Asherson, P., & Lavie, N. (2014). Plugging the attention
deficit: Perceptual load counters increased distraction in ADHD. Neuropsychology, 28, 91–97.
https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000020
Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., & Naugle, R. I. (2007). The latent structure of attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder in a clinic-referred sample. Neuropsychology, 21, 45–64. https://doi.org/
10.1037/0894-4105.21.1.45
Gilpin, A. R. (1993). Table for conversion of Kendall’s Tau to Spearman’s Rho within the context of
measures of magnitude of effect for meta-analysis. Educational and Psychological
Measurement, 53(1), 87–92. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164493053001007
Gjone, H., Stevenson, J., & Sundet, J. M. (1996). Genetic influence on parent-reported attention-
related problems in a Norwegian general population twin sample. Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 588–598. https://doi.org/10.1097/
00004583-199605000-00013
Gmehlin, D., Fuermaier, A. B., Walther, S., Tucha, L., Koerts, J., Lange, K. W., . . . Aschenbrenner, S.
(2016). Attentional lapses of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in tasks of
sustained attention. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 31, 343–357. https://doi.org/10.
1093/arclin/acw016
Greenberg, L. M., & Waldman, I. D. (1993). Developmental normative data on the Test of Variables of
Attention (T.O.V.A.). Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 34, 1019–1030.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb01105.x
Gudjonsson, G. H., Sigurdsson, J. F., Eyjolfsdottir, G. A., Smari, J., & Young, S. (2009). The
relationship between satisfaction with life, ADHD symptoms, and associated problems among
university students. Journal of Attention Disorders, 12, 507–515. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1087054708323018
Halperin, J. M., & Schulz, K. P. (2006). Revisiting the role of the prefrontal cortex in the
pathophysiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 560–
581. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.4.560
Haslam, N., Williams, B., Prior, M., Haslam, R., Graetz, B., & Sawyer, M. (2006). The latent structure
of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A taxometric analysis. Australian and New Zealand
Journal of Psychiatry, 40, 639–647. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1614.2006.01863.x
Hasler, R., Perroud, N., Meziane, H. B., Herrmann, F., Prada, P., Giannakopoulos, P., & Deiber, M. P.
(2016). Attention-related EEG markers in adult ADHD. Neuropsychologia, 87, 120–133. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.05.008
Hedge, C., Powell, G., & Sumner, P. (2018). The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do
not produce reliable individual differences. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 1166–1186.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0935-1
Hilger, K., & Fiebach, C. J. (2019). ADHD symptoms are associated with the modular structure of
intrinsic brain networks in a representative sample of healthy adults. Network Neuroscience, 3,
567–588. https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00083
Hilger, K., Sassenhagen, J., K€uhnhausen, J., Reuter, M., Schwarz, U., Gawrilow, C., & Fiebach, C. J.
(2020). Neurophysiological markers of ADHD symptoms in typically-developing children.
Scientific Reports, 10, Article 22460. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-80562-0
Hommel, B., Chapman, C. S., Cisek, P., Neyedli, H. F., Song, J. H., & Welsh, T. N. (2019). No one
knows what attention is. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 81, 2288–2303. https://doi.
org/10.3758/s13414-019-01846-w
Huang-Pollock, C. L., Karalunas, S. L., Tam, H., & Moore, A. N. (2012). Evaluating vigilance deficits in
ADHD: A meta-analysis of CPT performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 360–371.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027205
Attention, vigilance, and distraction in ADHD 1075

Huang-Pollock, C. L., & Nigg, J. T. (2003). Searching for the attention deficit in attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder: The case of visuospatial orienting. Clinical Psychology Review, 23, 801–
830. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(03)00073-4
Huang-Pollock, C. L., Nigg, J. T., & Carr, T. H. (2005). Deficient attention is hard to find: Applying the
perceptual load model of selective attention to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder subtypes.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 1211–1218. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-
7610.2005.00410.x
Huang-Pollock, C., Ratcliff, R., McKoon, G., Roule, A., Warner, T., Feldman, J., & Wise, S. (2020). A
diffusion model analysis of sustained attention in children with attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder. Neuropsychology, 34, 641–653. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000636
Huertas, F., Ballester, R., Gines, H. J., Hamidi, A. K., Moratal, C., & Lupian ~ ez, J. (2019). Relative age
effect in the sport environment. Role of physical fitness and cognitive function in youth soccer
players. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 16, Article
2837–. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16162837
Ishigami, Y., Eskes, G. A., Tyndall, A. V., Longman, R. S., Drogos, L. L., & Poulin, M. J. (2016). The
Attention Network Test-Interaction (ANT-I): Reliability and validity in healthy older adults.
Experimental Brain Research, 234, 815–827. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4493-4
JASP Team. (2020). JASP (Version 0.13) [Computer software]. Retrieved from https://jasp-stats.org/
Johnson, D. E., Epstein, J. N., Waid, L. R., Latham, P. K., Voronin, K. E., & Anton, R. F. (2001).
Neuropsychological performance deficits in adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, 587–604. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/16.6.587
Kofler, M. J., Rapport, M. D., Sarver, D. E., Raiker, J. S., Orban, S. A., Friedman, L. M., & Kolomeyer, E.
G. (2013). Reaction time variability in ADHD: A meta-analytic review of 319 studies. Clinical
Psychology Review, 33, 795–811. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2013.06.001
Lakens, D. (2017). Equivalence tests: A practical primer for t tests, correlations, and meta-analyses.
Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8, 355–362. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1948550617697177
Lampe, K., Konrad, K., Kroener, S., Fast, K., Kunert, H. J., & Herpertz, S. C. (2007).
Neuropsychological and behavioural disinhibition in adult ADHD compared to borderline
personality disorder. Psychological Medicine, 37, 1717–1729. https://doi.org/10.1017/
S0033291707000517
Langner, R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2013). Sustaining attention to simple tasks: A meta-analytic review of
the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 870–900. https://doi.
org/10.1037/a0030694
Luna, F. G. (2019). Redes atencionales y vigilancia ejecutiva y de activaci on [Attentional
networks and executive and arousal vigilance] [Doctoral dissertation, University of Granada].
Digibug. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/10481/56866
Luna, F. G., Barttfeld, P., Martın-Arevalo, E., & Lupian ~ ez, J. (2021). The ANTI-Vea task: Analyzing the
executive and arousal vigilance decrements while measuring the three attentional networks.
Psicologica, 42, 1–26. https://doi.org/10.2478/psicolj-2021-0001
Luna, F. G., Marino, J., Roca, J., & Lupian~ ez, J. (2018). Executive and arousal vigilance decrement in
the context of the attentional networks: The ANTI-Vea task. Journal of Neuroscience Methods,
306, 77–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2018.05.011
Luna, F. G., Roca, J., Martın-Arevalo, E., & Lupian ~ ez, J. (2020). Measuring attention and vigilance in
the laboratory vs. online: The split-half reliability of the ANTI-Vea. Behavior Research Methods.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01483-4. [Epub ahead of print]
Luna, F. G., Roman-Caballero, R., Barttfeld, P., Lupian ~ ez, J., & Martın-Arevalo, E. (2020). A high-
definition tDCS and EEG study on attention and vigilance: Brain stimulation mitigates the
executive but not the arousal vigilance decrement. Neuropsychologia, 142, Article 107447.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107447
Luna, F. G., Telga, M., Vadillo, M. A., & Lupian ~ ez, J. (2020). Concurrent working memory load may
increase or reduce cognitive interference depending on the attentional set. Journal of
1076 Tao Coll-Martın et al.

Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 46, 667–680. https://doi.


org/10.1037/xhp0000740
Lundervold, A. J., Adolfsdottir, S., Halleland, H., Halmøy, A., Plessen, K., & Haavik, J. (2011).
Attention Network Test in adults with ADHD-the impact of affective fluctuations. Behavioral
and Brain Functions, 7, Article 27. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-7-27
Luo, Y., Weibman, D., Halperin, J. M., & Li, X. (2019). A review of heterogeneity in attention deficit/
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, Article 42. https://doi.
org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00042
MacLeod, J. W., Lawrence, M. A., McConnell, M. M., Eskes, G. A., Klein, R. M., & Shore, D. I. (2010).
Appraising the ANT: Psychometric and theoretical considerations of the Attention Network
Test. Neuropsychology, 24, 637–651. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019803
Marchetta, N. D., Hurks, P. P., De Sonneville, L. M., Krabbendam, L., & Jolles, J. (2008). Sustained and
focused attention deficits in adult ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 11, 664–676. https://
doi.org/10.1177/1087054707305108
Martella, D., Aldunate, N., Fuentes, L. J., & Sanchez-Perez, N. (2020). Arousal and Executive
Alterations in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Frontiers in Psychology, 11,
Article 1991. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01991
Mary, A., Slama, H., Mousty, P., Massat, I., Capiau, T., Drabs, V., & Peigneux, P. (2016). Executive and
attentional contributions to Theory of Mind deficit in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD). Child Neuropsychology, 22, 345–365. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2015.
1012491
Mason, D. J., Humphreys, G. W., & Kent, L. (2004). Visual search, singleton capture, and the control
of attentional set in ADHD. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 661–687. https://doi.org/10.1080/
02643290342000267
McDonald, J. H. (2014). Handbook of biological statistics. Baltimore, MD: Sparky House Publishing.
Retrieved from http://www.biostathandbook.com/HandbookBioStatThird.pdf
Meier, M. E. (2020). Testing the attention-distractibility trait. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/
osf.io/a4cjk
Moffitt, T. E., Houts, R., Asherson, P., Belsky, D. W., Corcoran, D. L., Hammerle, M., . . . Caspi, A.
(2015). Is adult ADHD a childhood-onset neurodevelopmental disorder? Evidence from a four-
decade longitudinal cohort study. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 967–977. https://
doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14101266
Mullane, J. C., Corkum, P. V., Klein, R. M., McLaughlin, E. N., & Lawrence, M. A. (2011). Alerting,
orienting, and executive attention in children with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 15,
310–320. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054710366384
Nikolas, M. A., Marshall, P., & Hoelzle, J. B. (2019). The role of neurocognitive tests in the assessment
of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Assessment, 31, 685–698.
https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000688
Oberlin, B. G., Alford, J. L., & Marrocco, R. T. (2005). Normal attention orienting but abnormal
stimulus alerting and conflict effect in combined subtype of ADHD. Behavioural Brain
Research, 165, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2005.06.041
Oken, B. S., Salinsky, M. C., & Elsas, S. M. (2006). Vigilance, alertness, or sustained attention:
Physiological basis and measurement. Clinical Neurophysiology, 117, 1885–1901. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.clinph.2006.01.017
Parsons, S., Kruijt, A.-W., & Fox, E. (2019). Psychological science needs a standard practice of
reporting the reliability of cognitive-behavioral measurements. Advances in Methods and
Practices in Psychological Science, 2, 378–395. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245919879695
Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after.
Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73–89. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-
150525
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1),
3–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/00335558008248231
Attention, vigilance, and distraction in ADHD 1077

Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of
Neuroscience, 13(1), 25–42. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325
Psychology Software Tools, Inc. (2012). E-Prime (Version 2.0) [Computer Software]. Retrieved from
https://pstnet.com/products/e-prime-legacy-versions/
Redacci on Medica. (n.d.). Un cuestionario de 6 preguntas detecta el TDAH en adultos [A six-items
questionnaire detect ADHD in adults]. Retrieved from https://www.redaccionmedica.com/secc
iones/psiquiatria/un-cuestionario-de-6-preguntas-detecta-el-tdah-en-adultos-1647
Riccio C. A., Reynolds C. R. (2006). Continuous performance tests are sensitive to ADHD in adults
but lack specificity: A review and critiquefor differential diagnosis. Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences, 931, 113–139. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05776.x
Roca, J., Castro, C., L
opez-Ram on, M. F., & Lupian ~ ez, J. (2011). Measuring vigilance while assessing
the functioning of the three attentional networks: The ANTI-Vigilance task. Journal of
Neuroscience Methods, 198, 312–324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.04.014
Roman-Caballero, R., Martın-Arevalo, E., & Lupian ~ ez, J. (2021). Attentional networks functioning
and vigilance in expert musicians and non-musicians. Psychological Research, 85, 1121–1135.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01323-2
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P., & Posner, M. I.
(2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1029–1040.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.012
Salomone, S., Fleming, G. R., Bramham, J., O’Connell, R. G., & Robertson, I. H. (2020).
Neuropsychological deficits in adult ADHD: Evidence for differential attentional impairments,
deficient executive functions, and high self-reported functional impairments. Journal of
Attention Disorders, 24, 1413–1424. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054715623045
Sanchez-Garcia, M., Fernandez-Calderon, F., Carmona-Marquez, J., Chico-Garcia, M., Velez-Moreno,
A., & Perez-Gomez, L. (2015). Psychometric properties and adaptation of the ASRS in a Spanish
sample of patients with substance use disorders: Application of two IRT Rasch models.
Psychological Assessment, 27, 524–533. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000064
Sanchıs, C., Blasco, E., Luna, F. G., & Lupian ~ ez, J. (2020). Effects of caffeine intake and exercise
intensity on executive and arousal vigilance. Scientific Reports, 10, Article 8393. https://doi.org/
10.1038/s41598-020-65197-5
Schoechlin, C., & Engel, R. R. (2005). Neuropsychological performance in adult attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder: Meta-analysis of empirical data. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,
20, 727–744. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2005.04.005
Sergeant, J. (2000). The cognitive-energetic model: An empirical approach to attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(1), 7–12. https://doi.org/
10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00060-3
Sergeant, J. A. (2005). Modeling attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A critical appraisal of the
cognitive-energetic model. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1248–1255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.b
iopsych.2004.09.010
Solanto, M. V., Etefia, K., & Marks, D. J. (2004). The utility of self-report measures and the continuous
performance test in the diagnosis of ADHD in adults. CNS Spectrums, 9, 649–659. https://doi.
org/10.1017/s1092852900001929
Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2013). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Toward a developmental
synthesis. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.), Oxford handbook of developmental psychology (Vol. 2, pp.
549–578). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/
9780199958474.013.0022
Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., & Halperin, J. M. (2010). Developmental phenotypes and causal pathways in
attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Potential targets for early intervention? Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 368–389. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02195.x
Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Wiersema, J. R., van der Meere, J. J., & Roeyers, H. (2010). Context-dependent
dynamic processes in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Differentiating common and
unique effects of state regulation deficits and delay aversion. Neuropsychology Review, 20, 86–
102. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-009-9115-0
1078 Tao Coll-Martın et al.

Sturm, W., de Simone, A., Krause, B. J., Specht, K., Hesselmann, V., Radermacher, I., . . . Willmes, K.
(1999). Functional anatomy of intrinsic alertness: Evidence for a fronto-parietal-thalamic-
brainstem network in the right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia, 37, 797–805. https://doi.org/
10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00141-9
Thomson, D. R., Besner, D., & Smilek, D. (2016). A critical examination of the evidence for sensitivity
loss in modern vigilance tasks. Psychological Review, 123, 70–83. https://doi.org/10.1037/re
v0000021
Tucha, L., Fuermaier, A. B., Koerts, J., Buggenthin, R., Aschenbrenner, S., Weisbrod, M., . . . Tucha,
O. (2017). Sustained attention in adult ADHD: Time-on-task effects of various measures of
attention. Journal of Neural Transmission, 124, 39–53. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-015-
1426-0
Tucha, L., Tucha, O., Laufk€ otter, R., Walitza, S., Klein, H. E., & Lange, K. W. (2008).
Neuropsychological assessment of attention in adults with different subtypes of attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Neural Transmission, 115, 269–278. https://doi.org/
10.1007/s00702-007-0836-z
Tucha, L., Tucha, O., Walitza, S., Sontag, T. A., Laufk€ otter, R., Linder, M., & Lange, K. W. (2009).
Vigilance and sustained attention in children and adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention
Disorders, 12, 410–421. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054708315065
Tucha, O., Walitza, S., Mecklinger, L., Sontag, T. A., K€ ubber, S., Linder, M., & Lange, K. W. (2006).
Attentional functioning in children with ADHD – Predominantly hyperactive-impulsive type and
children with ADHD – Combined type. Journal of Neural Transmission, 113, 1943–1953.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-006-0496-4
Ustun, B., Adler, L. A., Rudin, C., Faraone, S. V., Spencer, T. J., Berglund, P., . . . Kessler, R. C. (2017).
The world health organization adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder self-report
screening scale for DSM-5. JAMA Psychiatry, 74, 520–527. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsyc
hiatry.2017.0298
van Zomeren, A. H., & Brouwer, W. H. (1994). Clinical neuropsychology of attention. Oxford, UK:
Oxford University Press.
Vazquez-Marrufo, M., Garcıa-Valdecasas Colell, M., Galvao-Carmona, A., Sarrias-Arrabal, E., & Tirapu-
Ustarroz, J. (2019). El Attention Network Test en el estudio de los deficits cognitivos de pacientes
con trastorno por deficit de atenci on [The Attention Network Test in the study of cognitive
impairment of ADHD patients]. Revista De Neurologıa, 69, 423–432. https://doi.org/10.33588/
rn.6910.2019202
Weyandt, L. L., Oster, D. R., Gudmundsdottir, B. G., DuPaul, G. J., & Anastopoulos, A. D. (2017).
Neuropsychological functioning in college students with and without ADHD.
Neuropsychology, 31, 160–172. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000326
Wilding, J. (2005). Is attention impaired in ADHD? British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
23, 487–505. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X48972
Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the
executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review.
Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1336–1346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006
Zamani Sani, S. H., Fathirezaie, Z., Sadeghi-Bazargani, H., Badicu, G., Ebrahimi, S., Grosz, R. W., . . .
Brand, S. (2020). Driving accidents, driving violations, symptoms of attention-deficit-
hyperactivity (ADHD) and attentional network tasks. International Journal of
Environmental Research and Public Health, 17, 5238. https://doi.org/10.3390/ije
rph17145238

Received 23 October 2020; revised version received 9 May 2021


Attention, vigilance, and distraction in ADHD 1079

Supporting Information
The following supporting information may be found in the online edition of the article:
Text S1 ADHD symptom distribution in the present study compared to an estimated
normative distribution
Text S2 Irrelevant distractor position and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
symptoms: An exploratory analysis
Table S1 Percentiles values of the cBAARS-IV and the aBAARS-IV in original and
estimated normative samples compared to the sample of the present study
Table S2 Kendall’s rank correlations among ANTI-Vea outcomes
Table S3 Kendall’s rank correlations between ANTI-Vea outcomes and ADHD
symptoms with corrections for multiple comparisons
Figure S1 Q-Q plots comparing ADHD symptom distribution in our sample with
symptom distribution in an estimated normative sample for the cBAARS-IV (Panel A)
and the aBAARS-IV (Panel B)

You might also like