THE ROLE OF CUES AND HEURISTICS IN ONLINE NEWS CREDIBILITY
ASSESSMENTS
by
KATHERINE KEIB
Social Media & Selection
The study of what motivates people to engage with news online has often been centered
on uses and gratifications (U&G) (A. M. Lee, 2013; A. M. Lee & Chyi, 2014; Lin, 1996;
Ruggiero, 2000), on the premise that media selection is driven by an individual’s self-awareness
and expectation that certain media will fulfill those needs, and the prevalence of internet use has
revived its application in communication studies (Ruggiero, 2000). Motivation for internet use
include passing time, interpersonal utility, information seeking, convenience and entertainment
(Papacharissi & Rubin, 2000) as well as problem solving, status seeking, relationship
maintenance, personal insight and persuasion (Flanagin & Metzger, 2001). Regarding news
content specifically, results of a national study of news consumption choices across platforms
revealed four motivations of news consumption: information-motivated, entertainment-
motivated, opinion-motivated, and social-motivated news consumption (A. M. Lee, 2013).
Social media is used prolifically by the public, with 74% of U.S. adults on the platform
(Pew, 2014). People who went beyond just consuming news on social media, but also uploaded
content were found to be motivated by social status and information seeking (C. S. Lee & Ma,
2012). Lee found that when it comes to social media, twitter is used more for entertainment,
opinion and social motivation, Facebook, and twitter are both used least frequently by those with
information motivations (A. M. Lee, 2013). Another study looked at friending journalists and
social media news reception as predictors for sharing news on social media (Weeks & Holbert,
2013). Using a Pew national survey, researchers found that news consumption via social media
and being friends with a journalist did in fact lead to more news sharing, but the behavior was
contingent upon having strong partisan ties. In other words, if the person was consuming the
content regularly, and had strong feelings about it, they were more likely to consume it
positively, and then share it. They also found that people who were news seekers and who got
news via email were more likely to share news socially (Weeks & Holbert, 2013). The sharing
of news via social media is a re-broadcast of the content, therefore it is important for journalists
and researchers to understand how people decide what to share and thus spread further.
One explanation for social sharing is the personal gratification that users gain from
sharing content (C. S. Lee & Ma, 2012b). However, U&G has been criticized because it does
not account for the role of prior experience (C.S. Lee & Ma, 2012b). Prior experience refers to
the past use of social media, in this case, that informs the current and future use of the platform.
In order to overcome this shortcoming, Lee & Ma looked to Social Cognitive Theory to account
for the impact of prior experience on social media use as well as the fact that social media use
may be more passive than active (C. S. Lee & Ma, 2012b). Prior experience, as explained by
SCT, is also used in U&G research to explain the relationship between motivation and self-
efficacy (C. S. Lee & Ma, 2012b). Prior social media experience and socializing have been
found to be the two most important motivators to share content on social media, and led to the
creation of a new U&G model, which combines U&G and SCT (C. S. Lee & Ma, 2012). The
model incorporates prior experience as a predictor between gratifications and intention to share.
The model created by Lee & Ma (2012) was built from previous work that also extended
U&G by applying SCT. One study, for example, addressed behavioral incentives through factor
analysis (LaRose & Eastin, 2004). As people repeatedly use social media, habits are formed,
which can lead to a deficit of self-regulation, and, over time, people may not be as careful in
making decisions (LaRose & Eastin, 2004). The implications of this for shared content here are
that information quickly shared via social platforms may not be carefully processed, and
misinformation can be easily spread, as news sharing intrinsically changes the way users engage
with news (Kümpel, Karnowski, & Keyling, 2015).
Social connections necessarily imply credibility because the source has already been
vetted, whether it is an organization intentionally followed or a friend who is sharing content
they endorse (Metzger, Flanagin, & Medders, 2010b). In addition, users who share news on
social media experience greater involvement with the topic than people who just read the
content, and that involvement increased as people commented on the shared news (Oeldorf-
Hirsch & Sundar, 2015). This greater involvement with the content, coupled with the
credibility automatically given, could result in people mistakenly sharing content that is not
credible. For example, thousands of people shared a false news story about the firing of then
FOX news anchor Megan Kelly in the Fall of 2016 (Ohleiser, 2016). The sharing led to the
story appearing in Facebook’s list of trending stories, which then led to incrementally more
sharing. It was a perfect storm of users acting based on the norms of the platform and the input
from their communities.
Media Credibility
“Journalism is built on credibility” (Cassidy, 2007b)
Conceptualizing the credibility of news information. Credibility, widely considered to
be based on the expertness and trustworthiness of content (Hovland, Janis, & Kelley, 1953), has
been studied in earnest since the 1940s and 1950s. Factors from war time propaganda, to the
growth of television news, and increased advertising, generated research into areas such as who
the public trusted to deliver their news, how citizens decided what was fact and what was hype
and whether people believed advertisers were influencing editorial agendas (Hovland, Janis, &
Kelley, 1953; Metzger, Flanagin, Eyal, Lemus, & McCann, 2003; Roper, 1985). Initially, work
focused on perceptions of the credibility of the message communicator, known as source
credibility (Hovland et al., 1953). The public found information credible if they found the
person delivering the message to be credible, and that assessment could apply beyond the
individual to the organization the person works for or even a medium in general (Hovland et al.,
1953). Over time, the study of credibility became more nuanced, as some critics believed
Hovland’s work was too simplistic (Markham, 1968), and others criticized his lack of theoretical
foundation (Berlo, Lemert, & Mertz, 1969). As scholars undertook source credibility studies,
they began to parse out expertise as a source characteristic and trustworthiness as a characteristic
of the receiver (Metzger, Flanagin, Eyal, Lemus, & McCann, 2003a). Borne of this distinction,
new studies on message and medium credibility began.
Message credibility specifically refers to the media content and includes message
structure, the content itself, language intensity and the delivery of the message (Metzger,
Flanagin, Eyal, Lemus, & McCann, 2003b). Message design characteristics such as font style,
white space, images and color schemes have been shown to influence user’s perceptions of
online credibility (B. Fogg et al., 2003). Assessments of the message influence perceptions of
the source (Kiousis, 2001; Metzger et al., 2003b; Stamm & Dube, 1994). Because of this
relationship, message credibility is often studied along with source or medium credibility. A
new message-specific credibility scale developed by Appelman and Sundar focuses on content
related factors, not design factors as characteristics of message credibility (Appelman & Sundar,
2015).
Another element beyond message credibility or source expertise that has received
attention by researchers is coorientation between the sender and receiver (H. K. Meyer,
Marchionni, & Thorson, 2010; P. Meyer, 1988). Coorientation refers to the relationship between
people and the objects they experience (Avery, Lariscy, & Sweetser, 2010; Newcomb, 1953).
An example would be a journalist, their news story, and the person who reads it. Meyer’s work
revealed that users do feel a connection to the person creating the content, and that this
connection influences judgment about the content (P. Meyer, 1988). The perceived expertise of
the journalist leads the receiver to see the content as more credible precisely because of the
relationship, or coorientation (H. K. Meyer et al., 2010). Considering social media and the
coorientation of journalists and public relations practitioners, researchers found a similar regard
among both groups for the use and importance of social media tools (Avery et al., 2010). As
content has moved online; coorientation suggests there is still a relationship between the source
and the receiver.
In addition, the technological affordances of the internet have refreshed the study of
credibility with inquiry into the influence of message factors such as interactivity and
hypertextuality (Appelman & Sundar, 2015; Chung, Nam, & Stefanone, 2012; Metzger et al.,
2010b; Sundar, Oeldorf-Hirsch, & Xu, 2008). Recent work argues that message credibility is a
separate concept and a new scale was developed and tested for measuring it (Appelman &
Sundar, 2015). In fact, the aspects of online message design are unique to the platform –
hypertextuality is not embedded in messages delivered on television or radio.
The credibility of the medium is regarded as the overall assessment of the credibility of
the platform that delivers the content. For example, content watched on television carries
specific indicators of credibility applicable only to television, such as cues related to the anchor
speaking (Bucy, 2003), that newspaper would not contain. While primarily focusing on the
communicator, Hovland suggested that medium mattered as well as source (Hovland et al.,
1953). Several years later, Westley and Severin came to the same conclusion, stating that
source is distinct from message and this area needed more research (Westley & Severin, 1964).
These early distinctions between content and medium re-enforce the concept that medium
deserves its own study, and more so, that distinct media should be studied by looking at their
unique attributes.
Online credibility assessments. Study of the credibility of the internet as a medium
began in earnest in the 1990s. Much early work looked at how the public and journalists
themselves evaluated the credibility of the internet vs. other media (Flanagin & Metzger, 2000).
The internet quickly earned credibility ratings equal to or higher than traditional news mediums,
which can be explained in part by the reliance on the medium (Johnson & Kaye, 2010). Recent
work has delved into the complications that social media and unknown sources bring to users
facing an overwhelming number of content items delivered daily via Facebook, Twitter, Google
searches and email (Chung et al., 2012; Sundar et al., 2007). Findings show that new features
specific to the online medium are used by people when assessing credibility and should be
studied along with traditional factors (Chung et al., 2012). Metzger and colleagues (2003)
argued that users must be trained or taught in order to understand the tools available online and
the norms of the medium (Metzger et al., 2003b). Online, people assess the webpage itself as
part of a medium assessment, the content itself as message assessment and the publisher or
individual journalist as a source assessment (Metzger et al., 2003a). Lacking formal training,
time and repeated use has taught us how to assess credibility online and these processes may
become automatic, iterative, and reliant on cues that guide our decision-making process (Sundar
et al., 2008; Wathen & Burkell, 2002).
Recent work summarizing credibility in the 21st century stipulates that scholars should
study various levels of credibility (Hilligoss & Reih, 2008; Metzger, Flanagin, Eyal, Lemus, &
McCann, 2003). The interaction between these levels can cause one to influence the other
(Kiousis, 2001), and it is not always in set path; people do move back and forth between levels in
credibility assessments (Hilligoss & Rieh, 2008).
Why online credibility assessments matter. As was brought to light in the 2016 U.S.
presidential election, the credibility of online content can have significant repercussions in
society (Dougherty, 2016) News that was false was spread, largely via social media, in order to
sway voters. Even before this proliferation of fake news, other researchers have studied online
credibility because of concerns that, for example, a lack of reliable information could lead to
dangerous medical, financial and personal decisions (Markov et al., 2014). Online, common
filters for content, such as government regulators, or journalistic gatekeepers, are not able to
weed out all the incorrect, misleading or manipulative content. Following the 2016 election,
several organizations experimented with fake news filters (Oremus, 2016). While some of these
filters exist online, the volume and diverse nature of online content makes it impossible to filter
all content published. Thus, the public is largely left to decide for itself what is credible.
Although certainly some consumers have become literate in how to navigate online
content, their ability to do so varies widely. The main way that consumers learn to discern what
is credible online is through experience (Flanagin & Metzger, 2000), and that experience informs
their credibility assessments (Fico, Richardson, & Edwards, 2004; Johnson & Kaye, 2016).
Consumers themselves report that experience with a site helps them determine the credibility of
content on the site (B. Fogg et al., 2003). Experience is drawn upon when users notice cues or
markers on websites, and assign meaning to them (B. J. Fogg, 2003), which can lead to habits
and less thoughtful attention to content, i.e., habitual news reading (LaRose & Eastin, 2004;
Schudson, 2003). Habitual news reading has been operationalized as a construct of self-
regulation, which is a part of SCT, and habit is seen as a failure of SCT (LaRose & Eastin,
2004). Specifically, habit means that potentially “…we become inattentive to the reasoning
behind our media behavior, our mind no longer devotes attention resources to evaluating it”
(LaRose & Eastin, 2004 p. 363). This behavior can allow people to employ credibility heuristics
on one website, then apply them to a different site with similar design elements (Hilligoss &
Rieh, 2008). These short cuts in the credibility assessment process can lead to misapplied
credibility on the web.
The websites on which people choose to visit can determine the type of content they are
thus continuously exposed to. Search engines remember our past preferences and deliver custom
search results (McEvoy, 2015). Social media news feeds are populated with posts from friends
and brands we have chosen to follow, and the outlets whose posts we click on more often are
thus served more often in our feed. Selective exposure refers to the phenomenon of content
being limited by self-selected choices in exposure, which, critics warn, can lead to polarization
and lack of open-mindedness (Messing & Westwood, 2012; Zillmann & Bryant, 1985).
Although there has been concern about the effects of selective exposure leading to ideological
polarization, several recent studies have cast doubt that online content or social media is
increasing the problem. Users who are experienced at reading political blogs and partisan
websites are more willing to seek out opposing viewpoints online, good news for those who see
the importance between reading a variety of opinions and a strong democracy (Johnson & Kaye,
2013). In addition, cues such as endorsements in social media increase exposure to a variety of
viewpoints and opinions (Messing & Westwood, 2012). As people are exposed to content from
which the source is not familiar, and the content topic may also be out of their wheelhouse,
assessing the content must rely on elements other than source or topic familiarity.
When online credibility assessments occur. Web users form impressions of the
credibility of a website very quickly, in less than 50 milliseconds, in fact (Lindgaard, Fernandes,
Dudek, & Brown, 2006). They notice design elements, even if that noticing is subconscious &
instinctive (Lindgaard et al., 2006), which then trigger mental shortcuts called heuristics.
Assessments happen quickly, and design can lead to an incorrect assessment. Design elements
that consumers use to assess credibility, such as font style, hyperlinks, and length of copy are
easy to mimic, (Metzger, 2007) and websites wishing to gain credibility can mimic credible
styles, using design to mislead consumers (Metzger, 2007). Additionally, individuals bring
preconceived attitudes into the assessment process (Kiousis, 2001) and researchers often account
for these by assessing relevant differences through their study design. Participants are often
asked about items such as experience with a medium (Fico et al., 2004), familiarity with a brand
(Flanagin & Metzger, 2007) or level of knowledge on a topic (Metzger et al., 2003).
A model of the credibility assessment process developed by Hilligoss & Reih (2008)
integrates common aspects of credibility assessment, regardless of the media, type of information
and environment of use, i.e. context. The authors assert that a credibility assessment is the result
of individual credibility judgments, which may take place in no specific order, and may affect
each other. They developed their model through an observation and analysis of credibility
assessments made over time by 24 subjects. The levels within the assessment are called
construct, heuristics and interaction. Construct is the most abstract level, includes the individual
aspects a person uses to define credibility, and is unique to each person. The heuristics level
encompasses all the shortcuts people use to assess credibility, which may come from the media,
source, aesthetics/design or be endorsement based. The interaction level refers to the specific
source and content cues related to the content at hand. These three sets of characteristics can all
influence situational evaluations of the credibility of a message.
This model shares some characteristics of another model, developed by Wathen &
Burkell (2002). Their model, based on theory, not on actual observations, also suggests a
layered process, but one that happens in linear steps (Wathen & Burkell, 2002). They posit that
users make initial judgments about surface characteristics of a website, then, if the website meets
the standards, they continue to consume the content and simultaneously undertake more
credibility judgments. At each step of their process, users will either continue to consume and
assess the site, or will choose to exit and abandon the content. Given this scenario, the stakes are
high for publishers to get the surface and message characteristics right if they want to attract and
maintain an audience. Conversely, sites wishing to attract an audience just to generate clicks and
revenue, as well as propaganda and advertiser based sites, can mimic credibility cues and present
information that the audience finds difficult to accurately assess. This dissertation will explore
the process that takes place, and, although not explicitly testing these models, will perhaps shed
some insight into them. Specifically, Study I will ascertain the commonalities and differences
that individuals self-report about their credibility assessment process. The eye-tracking portion
in Study II will confirm that people do fixate on aspects of the webpage that they told us they
consider.