Learning Theory
Learning Theory
Ebit
Program: MAT-Chemistry
Learning Theory: Pavlov’s Classical Conditioning Theory
Subject: Psychology of Learning for Psychology
Learning is the process by which new knowledge, behaviors, attitudes, and ideas
are acquired. Learning can occur through both unconscious and conscious pathways.
Classical conditioning is one of those unconscious learning methods and is the most
straightforward way in which humans can learn. Classical conditioning is the process in
which an automatic, conditioned response is paired with specific stimuli. Although Edwin
Twitmyer published findings pertaining to classical conditioning one year earlier, the
best-known and most thorough work on classical conditioning is accredited to Ivan
Pavlov, a Russian physiologist born in the mid-1800s. Pavlov had such a great impact
on the study of classical conditioning that it is often referred to as Pavlovian
conditioning.
In the past, some psychology experts believed that classical conditioning (CC)
could explain nearly all aspects of human psychology — including our ability to learn
how to communicate, cooperate with others and control our emotions. While this theory
remains controversial, we do know that classical conditioning is behind many learned
behaviors, both good and bad. In fact, it’s considered the most straightforward way in
which humans can learn.
Learning — the process by which new knowledge, behaviors, attitudes and ideas
are acquired — can occur through both unconscious and conscious pathways, and in
CC it happens below the level of conscious awareness. The broader
term conditioning is a method of learning that occurs through rewards and punishments
for behaviors. This term is used in the field of behaviorism (or behavioral psychology) to
help explain why people act the way they do.
CC deals with responses that are “natural” and involuntary. It works by pairing
two stimuli together to produce a new learned response. CC helps determine
behaviors in both people and animals. This type of learning goes by several other
names too, including Pavlovian conditioning — since Ivan Pavlov, a Russian
physiologist in early 20th century, had such a great impact on the study of CC. It’s also
sometimes referred to as respondent conditioning or type I/type S conditioning.
To test his theory, Pavlov set up an experiment in which he rang a bell shortly
before presenting food to the dogs. At first, the dogs elicited no response to the bells.
However, eventually, the dogs began to salivate at the sound of the bell alone.
Psychologists use specific terms to identify the stimuli and the responses in
classical conditioning. The unconditioned stimulus (US) is something (such as food) that
triggers a naturally occurring response, and the unconditioned response (UR) is the
naturally occurring response (such as salivation) that follows the unconditioned
stimulus. The conditioned stimulus (CS) is a neutral stimulus that, after being repeatedly
presented prior to the unconditioned stimulus, evokes a similar response as the
unconditioned stimulus. In Pavlov’s experiment, the sound of the tone served as the
conditioned stimulus that, after learning, produced the conditioned response (CR),
which is the acquired response to the formerly neutral stimulus. Note that the UR and
the CR are the same behavior — in this case salivation — but they are given different
names because they are produced by different stimuli (the US and the CS,
respectively).
After he had demonstrated that learning could occur through association, Pavlov
moved on to study the variables that influenced the strength and the persistence of
conditioning. In some studies, after the conditioning had taken place, Pavlov presented
the sound repeatedly but without presenting the food afterward. As you can see, after
the initial acquisition (learning) phase in which the conditioning occurred, when the CS
was then presented alone, the behaviour rapidly decreased — the dogs salivated less
and less to the sound, and eventually the sound did not elicit salivation at
all. Extinction refers to the reduction in responding that occurs when the conditioned
stimulus is presented repeatedly without the unconditioned stimulus.
Pauli WM, Gentile G, Collette S, Tyszka JM, O'Doherty JP. Evidence for model-
based encoding of Pavlovian contingencies in the human brain. Nat
Commun. 2019 Mar 07;10(1):1099.
Jarius S, Wildemann B. Pavlov's Reflex before Pavlov: Early Accounts from the
English, French and German Classic Literature. Eur Neurol. 2017;77(5-6):322-
326.
Jarius S, Wildemann B. And Pavlov still rings a bell: summarising the evidence
for the use of a bell in Pavlov's iconic experiments on classical conditioning. J
Neurol. 2015 Sep;262(9):2177-8.
Otto MW, McHugh RK, Kantak KM. Combined Pharmacotherapy and Cognitive-
Behavioral Therapy for Anxiety Disorders: Medication Effects, Glucocorticoids,
and Attenuated Treatment Outcomes. Clin Psychol (New York). 2010 Jun
01;17(2):91-103.
Otto MW, Basden SL, Leyro TM, McHugh RK, Hofmann SG. Clinical
perspectives on the combination of D-cycloserine and cognitive-behavioral
therapy for the treatment of anxiety disorders. CNS Spectr. 2007 Jan;12(1):51-6,
59-61.
Otto MW, Hinton D, Korbly NB, Chea A, Ba P, Gershuny BS, Pollack MH.
Treatment of pharmacotherapy-refractory posttraumatic stress disorder among
Cambodian refugees: a pilot study of combination treatment with cognitive-
behavior therapy vs sertraline alone. Behav Res Ther. 2003 Nov;41(11):1271-6.
Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Health and Behavior: Research,
Practice, and Policy. Health and Behavior: The Interplay of Biological,
Behavioral, and Societal Influences. National Academies Press (US);
Washington (DC): 2001.