Thinking Skills and Creativity in Second Language Education (2020)
Thinking Skills and Creativity in Second Language Education (2020)
CREATIVITY IN SECOND
LANGUAGE EDUCATION
Edited by Li Li
First published 2020
by Routledge
2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon OX14 4RN
and by Routledge
52 Vanderbilt Avenue, New York, NY 10017
Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
© 2020 selection and editorial matter, Li Li; individual chapters, the
contributors
The right of Li Li to be identified as the author of the editorial material,
and of the authors for their individual chapters, has been asserted in
accordance with sections 77 and 78 of the Copyright, Designs and Patents
Act 1988.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or
utilized in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now
known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording, or in
any information storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing
from the publishers.
Trademark notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or
registered trademarks, and are used only for identification and explanation
without intent to infringe.
British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
A catalog record has been requested for this book
Contributors vii
Index 216
CONTRIBUTORS
Maya Defianty is a Lecturer at UIN Syarif Hidayatullah Jakarta where she has
taught several courses in the area related to Teaching English to Speakers of
Other Languages (TESOL). She has an extensive experience in working with
viii Contributors
Xuying Fan (Enjo) is a research fellow at the South China Normal University,
China. She has Bachelor’s degree in Education Studies, a Master’s degree in
TESOL, a Master’s degree in Educational Research, and a Doctoral degree
in Education from the University of Exeter, United Kingdom. She is also an
Associate Fellow of the Higher Education Academy in the United Kingdom. Her
primary research focus is thinking skill in language classrooms, and language
teacher cognition, focusing especially on classroom interaction. She is working
on a project on Thinking Skills and Creativity in Language Classrooms.
Hang Eun Lee is currently working for a Korean American school in California.
She received her PhD from the University of Georgia specializing in Gifted and
Creative Education. She loves to work with creative people and is researching
Contributors ix
and teaching in the gifted and creative education field both locally and
internationally. She has researched bilingualism and creativity for decades and has
several academic publications on bilingualism and creativity. Her work focuses
specifically on the creativity of the Korean American populations. She has served
creative people with multicultural backgrounds internationally, including in
Korea and the United States. She is a specialist in creative problem solving and
conducts various creativity-related projects for educators, scholars, and parents
both locally and internationally.
Mei Lin is Senior Lecturer at Newcastle University in the United Kingdom. She
specializes in teaching pedagogy, thinking skills, and learner autonomy. Lin is
a co-author of Thinking through Modern Foreign Languages (2004). Since then she
devotes much her career to the promotion of integrating teaching thinking into
English as a foreign language curriculum. She has supervised PhD studies on
integrated thinking skills curriculum in Sudan, Taiwan, and China. Lin has a
particular interest in Vygotsky’s mediation, investigating the impact of teacher
behaviours, thinking frameworks or tools on nurturing students’ thinking, and
dispositions, as well as on language competence. Lin is currently consultant
for learning community projects in Shanghai and teacher training projects on
thinking skills for primary students in Chongqing, China.
Introduction
There is a growing global concern with “21st-century skills” (Voogt, Erstad,
Dede, & Mishra, 2013) in the field of education, and research suggests that inte-
grating good thinking skills is beneficial for developing 21st-century learners
(e.g. Mercer, 1996, 2004; Wegerif, Mercer, & Dawes, 1999). In particular, higher-
order thinking skills are important for learning and social practice to develop
global citizens with creativity and innovative capacity (MacDonald, 2005). It is
more so today than ever that we need people equipped with 21st-century skills,
because people need to engage in more global collaboration and communication
to solve problems and create new knowledge, especially with the rapid develop-
ment of digital and networked technologies (Wegerif, 2006). Twenty-first-century
skills include being creative, being able to argue for a position or course of action,
and being willing to concede to stronger arguments and evidence (Li, 2016a).
Over the years, we have witnessed the global movement in curriculum reforms,
with policy reports around the world stressing that exercising critical judgement,
collaboration, problem solving, creativity, and learning to learn together (L2L2)
are crucial for future economic growth (e.g. World Bank, 2011), as well as for
personal and collective well-being in an increasingly globalized world (OECD,
2014). In light of the growing interest in creativity, good judgement, and the
ability to think logically and clearly in diverse work and education contexts, it
is clear that there is a need to develop students’ thinking skills in subject areas
(Avargil, Herscovitz, & Dori, 2012; Li, 2011; Nagappan, 2001; Wegerif, Li, &
Kaufman, 2015).
In many countries, such as England (Qualifications and Curriculum Authority,
1999), Malaysia (Abdullah, Marimuthu, & Liau, 2003), and China (Ministry of
Education [MOE], 2001), thinking skills have been proposed to be a central
2 Li Li
element of the curriculum since the new millennium. In recent years, the cul-
tivation of children’s creativity has increasingly been regarded as an educational
imperative (Skiba, Tan, Sternberg & Grigorenko, 2010), and many countries
have introduced initiatives aimed at fostering it (Craft, 2007) with various foci.
For example, in Great Britain and Georgia (USA), “creative development” is pro-
posed (Department for Children, Schools and Families [DCSF], 2008; Georgia
Department of Early Care and Learning, 2011; QCA/Df EE, 2000), whereas
culture and creativity are closely linked and widely advocated in Norway and
Iceland (MOESC, 2012; NMOER, 2012). In other parts of Europe, “imagining,
feeling, and creating” are the core of the concept of creativity (e.g. in France;
OCED, 2004), whereas in the Far East, creativity is interpreted as “the develop-
ment of creative and aesthetic appreciation” in India (Ministry of Women and
Child Development [MWCD], 2012) and “expression and creativity” are educa-
tional goals in China (MOE, 2012). In the field of language education, thinking
skills are beginning to be integrated into curriculum and pedagogy, given the
close tie between language proficiency and thinking skills (Chapple & Curtis,
2000; Renner, 1996; Tarone, 2005). Critical thinking has always been a crucial
field in second language learning, and in recent years, there has been a subtle
development of creativity in language, and relevant research suggests that lan-
guage learning is no longer focusing only on linguistic knowledge development
(e.g. Carter, 2016; Jones & Richards, 2016).
have become the research focus in recent years. The shift of focus implies that the
view of “thinking” is gradually moving from the individual to the group, from
cognitive to social development (Li, 2016a).
Integrating good thinking skills in teaching has proved to be beneficial for
academic learning, personal development, and social relations. In the language
learning literature, thinking skills and creativity are explored from different per-
spectives, with a particular focus on critical thinking, creativity and creative
thinking, and metacognition (especially in reading and writing). In what fol-
lows, I will provide a brief overview of the research in these areas.
Critical thinking
According to Fisher and Scriven (1997), criticality in thinking involves “skilled
and active interpretation and evaluation of observations and communications,
information and argumentation” (p. 21). However, the concept has been inter-
preted with different foci; for example, Atkinson (1997) associates critical think-
ing with “cultural thinking” (p. 89), highlighting the close tie between thinking
and cultural practice, whereas Lipman (2003) considers it “healthy scepticism”,
the ability to engage in evaluation with reasoning. In spite of differences among
scholars in defining the concept of critical thinking skills, there is consensus
that criticality is important. For example, Li (2016a) claims that “the ability to
recognize, construct, and evaluate arguments, and the skills of analysing, synthe-
sizing, and critiquing material, are essentially the foundation of academic success
in higher education” (p. 268). It is not surprising that the majority of existing
research on critical thinking is carried out in higher education, with a specific
focus on reading and writing skills.
In L2 studies, one aspect of research has examined the effects of critical think-
ing on second language acquisition (e.g. Ghanizadeh & Moafian, 2011; Hashemi
& Ghanizadeh, 2012). When integrated into teaching properly, critical thinking
skills can result in positive linguistic gains (DeWaelsche, 2015). For example,
Rao’s (2007) and Gibson’s (2012) studies show how teaching critical thinking in
an L2 writing class facilitates the production of more critical ideas when focusing
on writing, which also then influences the use of language. Yang, Chuang, Li,
and Tseng (2013) similarly report a positive outcome in speaking and listening
performance. Elsewhere, Wu, Marek, and Chen (2013) report better student
performance in both oral and written tasks when students were taught criti-
cal thinking skills. Interestingly, studies also suggest that the quality of critical
thought depends on the topic content, with a familiar topic generating better
critical thinking (Stapleton, 2001). Clearly, language development and critical
judgement are interrelated and interdependent. Turning to an affective aspect,
Casanave (2010) and Shahini and Riazi (2011) argue that integrating critical
thinking skills in L2 learning can motivate students to take risks. These stud-
ies extended language learning from developing linguistic competence to social
abilities, including being open-minded and prepared to take risks.
4 Li Li
Metacognition
Closely related to critical thinking is the concept of metacognition, and research
suggests that integrating critical thinking into language learning increases stu-
dent motivation to learn about and improve their thinking, in particular with
reading and writing in second language education (Zhang, 2001). This is because
substantive reading and writing require learners to process information in a criti-
cal manner and reflect on their own thinking process. Metacognition, a term first
coined by Flavell (1979), refers to “knowledge concerning ones’ own cognitive
processes and products or anything related to them”, which includes “the active
monitoring and consequent regulation and orchestration of these processes in
relation to the cognitive objects or data on which they bear, usually in the service
of some concrete goal or objective’ (p. 232). Simply put, metacognition includes
one’s knowledge of cognitive resources (knowledge) and their regulation (con-
trol). Most research in this area focuses on learners’ awareness of, and use of,
“Thinking” move in second language education 5
a strategy in relation to their own proficiency levels (Zhang, 2010). Another area
of metacognition research is the discussion on the relationship between metacog-
nition and academic success. There is also evidence that successful learners use
different metacognitive strategies from those employed by less successful learn-
ers (Li & Larkin, 2017; Yayli, 2010; Zhang, 2001, 2010). Li (2016a) summarizes
idiosyncratic metacognitive strategies used by successful L2 learners. These
strategies included translation, the use of background and personal knowledge,
self-questioning techniques, guessing unknown words or phrases and predicting
text content, paying attention to topic sentences, picking out key words, and
compairng and contrasting the L1 and L2 knowledge domains (p. 269).
This body of research suggests that the successful and the less successful
L2 student differ in the amount and quality of the metacognitive knowledge
they possess. However, there are no differences among students from different
socio-cultural contexts (Li & Larkin, 2017). Therefore, understanding one group
of successful learners’ metacognitive knowledge and strategies might have signif-
icant implications for pedagogical improvement across cultures, because teachers
might be able to train students to use particular metacognitive strategies to mon-
itor, control, and regulate learning. This is something that is worth exploring
in the future, perhaps with a design-based research methodology, to determine
whether providing training on metacognition has a positive impact on L2
academic performance.
Creativity
Although there is a long tradition of separating the teaching of critical thinking
from the teaching of creativity (Wegerif et al., 2015), and no consensus exists as
to what creativity is and how best to encourage it (Craft, 2005), it has been rec-
ognized as central to successful teaching and learning ( Jones & Richards, 2016;
Sotto, 1994). As Wegerif et al. (2015) have pointed out, the scholarship on cre-
ativity is a rapidly increasing juggernaut that has brought academic rigour to a
field that is often thought to be “soft” (p. 3). In recent years, “creativity” has
become a significant aspect of language learning and teaching since “the Year
of Creativity and Innovation” (Official Journal of the European Union, 2008).
In language learning, creativity can be referred to as creative language or cre-
ative texts, and the field has acknowledged the importance of “using language
in creative ways to solve problems, to establish or maintain relationships and to
get people to act, think or feel in certain ways” ( Jones & Richards, 2016, p. 5).
That is to say, learning a language is ultimately a creative exercise, because each
user needs to use and combine elements of linguistic and non-linguistic knowl-
edge in new ways to achieve communicative purposes. Of course, creativity
might be viewed as “person” or “process” or “products” (Fisher, 2004). In that
respect, the concept of creativity is a “multifaceted phenomenon” ( Jones &
Richards, 2016, p. 5) or perhaps consists of “conflicting conceptions” (Nelson,
2016). When it is viewed as a product, it might concern a task, an activity, a
6 Li Li
argues that bilinguals’ experience of participation in two cultures makes them see
the world through two different conceptual systems and therefore enhances cog-
nitive flexibility, divergent thinking, and the creative expression of experiences.
Creativity does not just happen; it needs conditions to develop. In a school
environment, developing creativity might need school support, peer support
among teachers, and teachers’ willingness to take risks and adopt innovations,
as well as teacher knowledge and skills. In that respect, Richards and Cotterall
(2016) have described 11 characteristics of creative teachers. In summary, cre-
ative teachers should demonstrate flexibility, willingness to take risks, and rich
academic and pedagogical knowledge. They should also cultivate an ability to
design activities that have creative dimensions and should develop an individual
teaching style. They should display confidence and willingness to make deci-
sions about classroom management, and they should be committed to making
changes in their teaching. When working with colleagues and students, they are
interested in looking for innovative teaching ideas, customizing teaching, and
adopting technology.
This said, creative teachers cannot enhance creativity if students are not moti-
vated to participate and contribute. On that front, Chappell (2016), based on
the work of Csikszentmihalyi (1997b) and Sawyer (2007), proposed 10 condi-
tions that enhance the development of creativity in second language classrooms.
Figure 1.1 below summarizes these conditions. Nevertheless, teachers do not
need to create all these conditions in order to facilitate creativity, and not every
condition is equally important across all contexts. We have to understand that
socio-cultural contexts play a significant role in shaping these conditions.
all participants
support each the task has a
others' language use collaborative
focus
References
Abdullah, A. C., Marimuthu, S., & Liau. M. (2003). A study on the use of higher order think-
ing skills in the teaching of the English language in schools in Penang. In A. Pandian, G.
Chakravarthy, & S. C. Lah (Eds.), English language teaching and literacy: Research and reflections
(pp. 153–164). Serdang: University Putra Malaysia Press.
Atkinson, D. (1997). A critical approach to critical thinking in TESOL. TESOL Quarterly,
31(1), 71–94.
Avargil, S., Herscovitz, O., and Dori, Y. J. (2012). Teaching thinking skills in context-based
learning: Teachers’ challenges and assessment knowledge. Journal of Science Education and
Technology, 21(2), 207–225.
Beghetto, R. A. (2008). Prospective teachers’ beliefs about imaginative thinking in K-12
schooling. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 3, 134–142.
Beghetto, R. A., & Kaufman, J. C. (2007). Toward a broader conception of creativity: A case
for mini-c creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 1, 73–79.
Beghetto, R. A., & Plucker, J. A. (2006).The relationship among schooling, learning, and cre-
ativity: “All roads lead to creativity” or “You can’t get there from here”? In J. C. Kaufman
& J. Bear (Eds.), Creativity and reason in cognitive development (pp. 316–332). Cambridge,
NY: Cambridge University Press.
Carter, R. (2016). Language and creativity.The art of common talk.Abingdon, England: Routledge.
“Thinking” move in second language education 13
Casanave, C. P. (2010). Taking risks? A case study of three doctoral students writing quali-
tative dissertations at an American university in Japan. Journal of Second Language Writing,
19(1), 1–16.
Chappell, P. (2016). Creativity through inquiry dialogue. In R. H. Jones & J. C. Richards
(Eds.), Creativity in language teaching: Perspectives from research and practice (pp. 130–145).
Abingdon, England: Routledge.
Chapple, L., & Curtis, A. (2000). Content-based instruction in Hong Kong: Student responses
to film. System, 28, 419–433.
Cohen, L. M. (1989). A continuum of adaptive creative behaviors. Creativity Research Journal,
2, 169–183.
Craft, A. (2005). Creativity in schools:Tensions and dilemmas. London: Routledge.
Craft, A. (2007). Possibility thinking in the early years and primary classrooms. In A. G. Tan
(Ed.), Creativity: A handbook for teachers (pp. 231–250). Singapore: World Scientific.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding flow. New York: Basic Books.
Department for Children, Schools and Families [DCSF]. (2008). Statutory framework for the
early years foundation stage. Nottingham, England: Author.
DeWaelsche, S. (2015). Critical thinking, questioning and student engagement in Korean
university English courses. Linguistics and Education, 32 (Part B), 131–147.
Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The psychology of the language learner. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Fisher, A., & Scriven, M. (1997). Critical thinking: Its definition and assessment. Norwich,
England: Centre for Research in Critical Thinking, University of East Anglia.
Fisher, R. (2004). What is creativity? In R. Fisher & M. Williams (Eds.), Unlocking creativity:
Teaching across the curriculum (pp. 6–20). New York: Routledge.
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Meta-cognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive
developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.
Georgia Department of Early Care and Learning. (2011). Georgia’s pre-K program content
standards. Retrieved from http://.ga.gov/documents/attachments/Content_Standards.
pdf
Ghanizadeh, A., and Moafian, F. (2011). Critical thinking and emotional intelligence:
Investigating and relationship among EFL learners and the contribution of age and
gender. Iranian Journal of Applied Linguistics, 14(1): 23–49.
Ghonsooly, B., & Showqi, S. (2012). The effects of foreign language learning on creativity.
English Language Teaching, 5(4), 161–167.
Gibson, M. (2012). Integrating higher-order thinking skills into the L2 classroom. Retrieved
from http://dadun.unav.edu/bitstream/10171/27571/1/Gibson.pdf
Hashemi, M. R., & Ghanizadeh, A. (2012). Critical discourse analysis and critical thinking:
An experimental study in an EFL context. System, 40(1), 37–47.
Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Fischer, R., & Christoffels I. K. (2011). Bilingualism and crea-
tivity: Benefits in convergent thinking come with losses in divergent thinking. Frontiers
in Psychology, 2, 1–5.
Jones, R. H., & Richards, J. C. (2016) Creativity and language teaching. In R. H. Jones &
J. C. Richards (Eds.), Creativity in language teaching: Perspectives from research and practice
(pp. 3–15). Abingdon, England: Routledge.
Kaufman, J. C., & Beghetto, R. A. (2009). Beyond big and little: The four C model of
creativity. Review of General Psychology, 13(1), 1–12.
Kharkhurin, A. V. (2007). The role of cross-linguistic and cross-cultural experiences in
bilinguals’ divergent thinking. In I. Kecskes & L. Albertazzi (Eds.), Cognitive aspects of
bilingualism (pp. 175–210). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.
Kim, K. H. (2016). The creativity challenge: How we can recapture American innovation. Amherst,
NY: Prometheus Books.
14 Li Li
Li, L. (2011). Obstacles and opportunities for developing thinking through interaction in
language classrooms. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 6(3), 146–158.
Li, L. (2016a). Thinking skills and creativity in second language education: Where are we
now? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 267–272.
Li, L. (2016b). Integrating thinking skills in foreign language learning: What can we learn
from teachers’ perspectives? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 273–288.
Li, L., & Larkin, S. (2017). The role of metacognition in the success of reading and writing tasks across
cultures (ELT research papers). London: British Council.
Lin, M., Preston, A., Kharrufa, A., & Kong, Z. R. (2016). Making L2 learners’ reasoning
skills visible: The potential of computer supported collaborative learning environments.
Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 303–322.
Lipman, M. (2003). Thinking in education (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Lockley, T. (2013). International history as CLIL: Reflection, critical thinking and making
meaning of the world. Asian EFL Journal, 15(4), 330–338.
MacDonald, S. (2005). A century of arts and design education. Cambridge: Lutterworth Press.
Marin, L. M., and Halpern, D. F. (2011). Pedagogy for developing critical thinking in
adolescents: Explicit instruction produces greatest gains. Thinking Skills and Creativity,
6(1), 1–13.
Maybin, J., & Swann, J. (2007). Everyday creativity in language: Textuality, contextuality and
critique. Applied Linguistics, 28(4), 497–517.
Mercer, N. (1996). The quality of talk in children’s collaborative activity in the classroom.
Learning and Instruction, 6(4), 359–377.
Mercer, N. (2004). Sociocultural discourse analysis: Analysing classroom talk as a social mode
of thinking. Journal of Applied Linguistics, 1(2), 13–168.
Merrill, M. D. (2002). First principles of instruction. Educational technology research and devel-
opment, 50(3), 43–59.
Ministry of Education [MOE]. (2001). Guidelines for compulsory education reform.
Retrieved from http://www.edu.cn/20010926/3002911.shtml (Chinese).
Ministry of Education, Science and Culture [MOESC]. (2012). The Icelandic national cur-
riculum guide for preschools. Retrieved from http://brunnur.stjr.is/mrn/utgafuskra/
utgafa.nsf/xsp/.ibmmodres/domino/OpenAttachment/mrn/utgafuskra/utgafa.nsf/
CA2C880C51C8CE0D00257A230058FCA5/Attachment/adskr_leiksk_ens_2012.pdf
Ministry of Women and Child Development [MWCD]. (2012). Early childhood educa-
tion curriculum framework (draft). Retrieved from http://dietaizawl.weebly.com/
uploads/2/3/5/9/23594870/ecce_curriculum_framework_draft.pdf
Nagappan, R. (2001). Language teaching and the enhancement of higher-order thinking
skills. In W. A. Renandaya & N. R. Sunga (Eds.), Language curriculum and instruction in mul-
ticultural societies – Anthology Series 42. Singapore: SEAMEO Regional Language Centre.
Nelson, C. D. (2016). Cultivating creative teaching via narrative inquiry. In R. H. Jones &
J. C. Richards (Eds.), Creativity in language teaching: Perspectives from research and practice
(pp. 241–255). Abingdon, England: Routledge.
Niu, W., & Sternberg, R. J. (2006). The philosophical roots of Western and Eastern concep-
tions of creativity. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 26, 18–38.
Norwegian Ministry of Education and Research [NMOER]. (2012). Framework plan for the
content and tasks of kindergartens. Retrieved from http://www.udir.no/Upload/barnehage/
Rammeplan/Framework_Plan_for_the_Content_and_Tasks_of_Kindergartens_2011_
rammeplan_engelsk.pdf
OECD. (2014). ESP international report: Skills for social progress. Retrieved from http://
www.oecd.org/site/espforum2014/IssuesPaperESPForum2014.pdf
“Thinking” move in second language education 15
Vygotsky, L. S. (2004). Imagination and creativity in childhood. (M. E. Sharpe, Inc., Trans.).
Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 42, 7–97. (Original work published 1967).
Wang, Y., & Henderson, F. (2014). Teaching content through Moodle to facilitate students’
critical thinking in academic reading. The Asian EFL Journal Quarterly, 16(3), 7–40.
Wegerif, R. (2002). Literature review in thinking skills, technology and learning.
Commissioned by Future Lab. Retrieved from www.futurelab.org.uk/research/reviews/
ts01.htm
Wegerif, R. (2006). A dialogic understanding of the relationship between. CSCL and teach-
ing thinking skills. International Journal of Computer Supported Collaborative Learning, 1(1),
143–157.
Wegerif, R., Li, L., & Kaufman, J. C. (2015). Introduction. In R. Wegerif, L. Li, & J. C.
Kaufman (Eds.), The Routledge international handbook of research on teaching thinking
(pp. 1–8). New York: Routledge.
Wegerif, R., Mercer, N., and Dawes, L. (1999). From social interaction to individual reasoning:
An empirical investigation of a possible socio-cultural model of cognitive development.
Learning and Instruction, 9(6), 493–516.
Wilson, K. (2016). Critical reading, critical thinking: Delicate scaffolding in English for
Academic Purposes (EAP). Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 256–265.
World Bank. (2011). Learning for all: Investing in people’s knowledge and skills to promote devel-
opment. Education sector strategy 2020. Washington, DC: The World Bank. Retrieved from
http://siteresources.worldbank.org/EDUCATION/Resources/ESSU/Education_
Strategy_4_12_2011.pdf
Wu, W.-C.V., Marek, M. W., & Chen, N.-S. (2013). Assessing cultural awareness and linguis-
tic competency of EFL learners in a CMC-based active learning context. System, 4(3),
515–528.
Yang,Y. C., Chuang,Y., Li, L., & Tseng, S. (2013). A blended learning environment for indi-
vidualized English listening and speaking integrating critical thinking. Computers and
Education, 63, 285–305.
Yayli, D. (2010). A think-aloud study: Cognitive and meta-cognitive reading strategies of
EFL department students. Egitim Arastirmalari – Eurasian Journal of Educational Research,
38, 234–251.
Zhang, L. J. (2001). Awareness in reading: EFL students’ meta-cognitive knowledge of reading
strategies in an acquisition-poor environment. Journal of Language Awareness, 10, 268–288.
Zhang, L. J. (2010). A dynamic metacognitive systems account of Chinese university students’
knowledge about EFL reading. TESOL Quarterly, 44, 320–353.
Zhou, J., Jiang, Y., & Yao, Y. (2015). The investigation on critical thinking ability in EFL
reading class. English Language Teaching, 8(1), 83–94.
2
HOW DOES BILINGUALISM
AFFECT CREATIVITY?
Kyung Hee Kim and Hang Eun Lee
FIGURE 2.1 CATs for innovation: Creative climates, attitudes, and thinking skills.
FIGURE 2.2 ION (inbox, outbox, and newbox) thinking: Inbox expertise by knowl-
edge, comprehension, and application.
How does bilingualism affect creativity? 19
FIGURE 2.3 ION (inbox, outbox, and newbox) thinking: Outbox imagination by
fluency, flexibility, and originality.
wheel. Only with outbox imagination does an expert become an innovator who
can combine the wheel with something else, or extend the wheel or its use, to
produce something new and exciting. Third, emerging ideas must be carefully
examined for their usefulness through inbox-critical thinking, a deeper pro-
cessing mode that uses the skills of analysis and evaluation (Figure 2.4). Finally,
selected ideas must then be combined and refined through newbox synthesis.
Newbox synthesis is connecting-dot processing modes between critical think-
ing and outbox imagination, which use the skills of connection and refinement
FIGURE 2.4 ION (inbox, outbox, and newbox) thinking: Inbox-critical thinking by
analysis and evaluation.
20 Kyung Hee Kim and Hang Eun Lee
FIGURE 2.5 ION (Inbox, outbox, and newbox) thinking: Newbox synthesis by
connection and refinement.
(Figure 2.5). Thus, developing children’s ION thinking skills (see details in
Kim, 2017) should be an ultimate goal of education.
(Beardsmore, 2008). Moreover, many of the Nobel laureates (e.g. Berry, 1999;
Kim, 2016; Zuckerman, 1977) and eminent scientists (Berry, 1981; Cole &
Cole, 1973; Feist, 1993; Helson & Crutchfield, 1970; Roe, 1953) have been Jews.
Kim (2016) reported that 194 Nobel laureates of the 860 total individual world-
wide recipients, who account for 22.6% of the Nobel laureates between 1901
and 2014, had at least one Jewish parent. Jews won 8.7% of the Nobel Prizes in
peace, 12.6% in literature, 21.3% in chemistry, 25.6% in physics, 26.6% in phys-
iology/medicine, and 38.7% in economic sciences. Within Nobel Prize awards,
their contribution to the scientific fields is even higher: Jews won 26.3% of the
prizes in scientific fields (chemistry, physics, physiology/medicine, and economic
sciences) worldwide. As of 2015, Jews constituted only 0.2% of the world pop-
ulation, whereas Asian people constituted 23.4% of the world population. But
between 1901 and 2014 only 37 Asian people won a Nobel Prize, accounting
for only 4.3% of the total recipients. Considering the sizes of the populations,
the ratio of winning a Nobel Prize for Jews, per person, is about 115.1, whereas
that for Asian people, per person, is about 0.184. Thus, a Jewish person is about
625 times more likely to win a Nobel Prize than an Asian person (Kim, 2016).
Jews tend to be more creative, on a per person basis, than Christian people
(Arieti, 1976; Hayes, 1989; Veblen, 1919). Jews are also well represented in other
innovations. For example, Jewish musicians are some of the greatest of the 20th
century, accounting for over 25% of the conductors, 40% of the pianists, 50% of
the cellists, and 65% of the violinists (see Kim, 2016). Because traditionally Jews
were bilingual or spoke more than one language, their high rates of innovation
can be considered indirect evidence of the effect of bilingualism on creativity
(Kim, 2016; Simonton, 2008). For example, European Jews were often exposed
to multiple languages, such as Hebrew and/or Yiddish, in addition to the offi-
cial language of the country where they resided (Simonton, 2008). Moreover,
throughout history, Jews were often violently expelled from their homes and/or
countries because of their beliefs (Mohl, 2011). This forced them to be exposed
to different kinds of languages, areas, and cultures (Kim, 2016). However, Jews’
innovations tend to depend on their receiving basic civil rights in the country
where they reside (Simonton, 2008); for example, Jews in Switzerland, where
they had more freedom, won 83 times more Nobel Prizes in the sciences than
did Jews in Russia, where they had less freedom (Berry, 1981).
Small i. Bilingualism may affect different types of creativity tasks differ-
ently (Leikin, 2012; Leikin & Tovli, 2014; Simonton, 2008). Some research
findings have indicated that bilingualism affects verbal creativity through bilin-
guals’ enhanced linguistic and verbal capacity. For example, bilinguals out-
performed monolinguals on verbal tasks (Cummins, 2000; Gollan, Montoya,
& Werner, 2002; Leikin & Tovli, 2014; Okoh, 1980; Rosselli et al., 2000),
and specifically on flexibility (Carringer, 1974; Yousefi, Soleimani, &
Ghazanfariyanpoor, 2017), on originality (Carringer, 1974), and on fluency and
elaboration (Yousefi et al., 2017). In contrast, some research findings have indi-
cated that bilinguals’ limited linguistic skills may cause a deficiency in their
How does bilingualism affect creativity? 23
to concepts that are not emphasized in their native language, which develops
their linguistic sensitivity. They develop alternative coding schemes through
subjunctive grammatical constructions, which also enhances their understand-
ing of the structure, grammar, and vocabulary of their native language. They
establish different associations with familiar concepts and relate concepts from
different categories, which gives them access to richer semantic networks to the
extent that words can trigger more than one set of denotations and connotations
for individuals who are experiencing two different linguistic and conceptual
systems (Lubart, 1999; Simonton, 2008). They learn the arbitrary nature of any
one linguistic representation and the lack of complete semantic correspondence
between two languages, which enhances their capacity for generating numerous
and remote associations for newbox synthesis (Gough, 1976; Mednick, 1962;
Simonton, 2004, 2008).
facilitates the strength of the connections between the conceptual and lexical
systems. Bilinguals can postulate unrelated conceptual representation through
their two languages, which promotes cognitive flexibility (Kharkhurin, 2017).
Scientific research can become a creative thinking process when it is fluid
and dynamic with different perspectives (Stein, 1974). A hypothesis that is never
reconsidered, reformulated, or restated can inhibit creative thinking. Learning
and doing things the way people have always done them may be quick and con-
venient, but once a way of doing something becomes engrained, it is unlikely
that other ways will be sought. Thus cognitive flexibility allows creative indi-
viduals to disregard presumptions and to break through barriers. Increased
flexibility can allow more ideas to enter the mind and enhance more distant
connections or combinations between ideas, so it leads to the creation of original
ideas (Dreisbach & Goschke, 2004).
Metacognition. Compared to monolinguals, bilinguals are more likely
to come up with unique verbal expressions, beyond labelling, thus exhibit-
ing superior skill in meeting cognitive demands, such as abstract thinking
(Kormi-Nouri, Moradi, Moradi, Akbari-Zardkhaneh, & Zahedian 2012;
Lee, 2007). Bilinguals, especially those who acquired their second language
early in life, tend to outperform monolinguals on metalinguistic and metacogni-
tive awareness, which is awareness of one’s own language and thought (Adesope
et al., 2010). Their insight into the abstract features of language and into their
own learning processes further enhances their metacognitive awareness and
regulation, which facilitate newbox synthesis (e.g. Adesope et al., 2010; Kim,
Shoghi, & Ghonsooly, 2015).
of the Jews: They kept their own culture while adapting to the main culture as
cultural outsiders (Bialystok, 2001; Breger, Epstein, Kandil, & Schwartz, 2011;
Fleith et al., 2002; Leung et al., 2008; Simonton, 1994; Smith & Silva, 2011;
Tadmor et al., 2012; Yoon et al., 2013). They opened their minds to other per-
spectives and other ways of doing things (Barel, Van IJzendoorn, Sagi-Schwartz,
& Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2010; Eschleman, Bowling, & Alarcon, 2010;
Eitinger & Major, 2010; Kaplan, 2009).
A bicultural attitude increases creative performance and the use of creativ-
ity-supporting cognitive processes (Leung et al., 2008; Maddux & Galinsky,
2009). For example, biculturals gain familiarity with the new homeland’s dis-
tinct language, customs, history, heritage, and literature and are able to look
at concepts both routine and complex through different lenses efficiently and
to develop an understanding of the culture and worldview prevalent among
the language’s native speakers, which broadens their worldview and helps them
escape the conventions and conformity pressures that limit creative minds and
expressions (Crutchfield, 1962; Maslow, 1959; Simonton, 1999). They develop
cultural sensitivity and flexible thoughts, perspectives, and behaviours (Benet-
Martínez et al., 2006; Maddux et al., 2010; Tadmor et al., 2012). Their new
cultural knowledge also allows them to think differently, to draw on a lot of
alternative ideas, and to form unique combinations of ideas (Leung & Chiu,
2010; Weisberg, 1999). They notice inconsistencies or contrasts between the cul-
tures, grapple with understanding at a deep level, generate complex strategies
to cope, and synthesize seemingly incompatible ideas (Leung & Chiu, 2010;
Tadmor et al., 2012). For example, creative bicultural comedians employ linguis-
tic and cultural idiosyncrasies to express their own cultural identity and concep-
tualizations, which they translate into another language, evoking the seemingly
identical cultural schema to highlight the differences between the two cultures,
which generates a synthesis (e.g. comedy) from the paradoxes (Fallatah, 2017).
Also, creative bicultural writers’ languages are grounded in cultural discourse
deeply rooted in their linguistic and cultural similarities and dissimilarities,
resulting in newbox synthesis (e.g. Albakry & Siler, 2012; Kim, 2016).
Open-minded and complexity-seeking attitudes. The relationship
between bilingualism and creativity may be attributed to bilinguals’ open-
minded and complexity-seeking attitudes, which are nurtured in parallel
with learning a foreign language and/or a foreign culture. Bilinguals out-
perform monolinguals on tasks that represent an open-minded attitude (e.g.
Kharkhurin, 2010a; Kharkhurin & Samadpour Motalleebi, 2008), which
is nurtured by exposure to diversity and diverse experiences and languages
(Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2005). Bilingualism nurtures individuals’
open-mindedness, perhaps because mastering a foreign language requires them
(a) to be open to seemingly strange syntactic structures, illogical idiomatic
expressions or figures of speech, and words that look or sound similar to words
in their native language but have different meanings and/or (b) to deliberately
step out of the comfort zone of the system and structure of their native language
30 Kyung Hee Kim and Hang Eun Lee
Implications
Since the No Child Left Behind (NCLB) Act became law in the United States,
some educators and immigrant parents have forced children to adapt to American
culture only and to focus on learning only English to increase the students’
How does bilingualism affect creativity? 31
References
Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., Hernandez, M., Scifo, P., Keim, R., Cappa, S. F.,
& Costa, A. (2012). Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitor-
ing. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 2076–2086.
Adesope, O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and
meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research,
80, 207–245.
Albakry, M., & Siler, J. (2012). Into the Arab-American borderland: Bilingual creativity in
Randa Jarrar’s “Map of home.” Arab Studies Quarterly, 34, 109–121.
Anderson, J. A. E., Chung-Fat-Yim, A., Bellana, B., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2018). Language
and cognitive control networks in bilinguals and monolinguals. Neuropsychologia 117,
352–363.
Anderson, J. A. E., Grundy, J. G., De Frutos, J., Barker, R. M., Grady, C. & Bialystok, E.
(2018). Effects of bilingualism on white matter integrity in older adults. NeuroImage, 167,
143–150.
Arieti, S. (1976). Creativity:The magic synthesis. New York: Basic Books.
Ayneto, A., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2017). The influence of bilingualism on the preference
for the mouth region of dynamic faces. Developmental Science, 20, e12446.
How does bilingualism affect creativity? 33
Barac, R., Bialystok, E., Castro, D. C., & Sanchez, M. (2014). The cognitive development of
young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29,
699–714.
Barel, E.,Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Sagi-Schwartz, A., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2010).
Surviving the Holocaust: A meta-analysis of the long-term sequelae of a genocide.
Psychological Bulletin, 136, 677–698.
Beardsmore, H. B. (2008). Multilingualism, cognition and creativity. International CLIL
Research Journal, 1(1), 4–19.
Becker,T. M., Prat, C. S., & Stocco, A. (2016). A network-level analysis of cognitive flexibility
reveals a differential influence of the anterior cingulate cortex in bilinguals versus mono-
linguals. Neuropsychologia, 85, 62–73.
Benet-Martínez, V., Lee, F., & Leu, J. (2006). Biculturalism and cognitive complexity:
Expertise in cultural representations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37, 386–407.
Berry, C. (1981).The Nobel scientists and the origins of scientific achievement. British Journal
of Sociology, 32, 381–391.
Berry, C. (1999). Religious traditions as contexts of historical creativity: Patterns of scien-
tific and artistic achievement and their stability. Personality & Individual Differences, 26,
1125–1135.
Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Bialystok, E. (2005). Consequences of bilingualism for cognitive development. In F. Kroll &
A. M. B. De Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approach (pp. 417–432).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience.
Psychological Bulletin, 143, 233–262.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Freedman, M. (2007). Bilingualism as a protection against the
onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 459–464.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger
and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 34,
859–873.
Breger, S., Epstein, N., Kandil, C.Y., & Schwartz, A. E. (2011). A Moment symposium:The origins
of Jewish creativity. Moment, 32–37.
Brito, N., & Barr, R. (2012). Influence of bilingualism on memory generalization during
infancy. Developmental Science, 15, 812–816.
Brito, N., & Barr, R. (2014). Flexible memory retrieval in bilingual 6-month-old infants.
Developmental Psychobiology, 56, 1156–1163.
Brito, N. H., Grenell, A., & Barr, R. (2014). Specificity of the bilingual advantage for mem-
ory: Examining cued recall, generalization, and working memory in monolingual, bilin-
gual, and trilingual toddlers. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1369.
Brito, N. H., Sebastian-Galles, N., & Barr, R. (2015). Differences in language exposure and its
effects on memory flexibility in monolingual, bilingual, and trilingual infants. Bilingualism:
Language and Cognition, 18, 670–682.
Buchanan, D. R., & Bandy, C. (1984). Jungian typology of prospective psychodramatists:
Myers-Briggs type indicator analysis of applicants for psychodrama training. Psychological
Reports. 55, 599–606.
Buchanan, D. R., & Taylor, J. A. (1986). Jungian typology of professional psychodramatists:
Myers-Briggs type indicator analysis of certified psychodramatists. Psychological Reports,
58, 391–400.
Burt, R. S. (2004). Structural holes and good ideas. American Journal of Sociology, 110(2),
349–399.
Carne, G., & Kirton, M. J. (1982). Styles of creativity: Test-score correlations between Kirton
Adaption-Innovation Inventory and Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Psychological Reports,
50, 31–36.
Carringer, D. C. (1974). Creative thinking abilities in Mexican youth: The relationship of
bilingualism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 5, 492–504.
Carter, B. A., Nelson, D. L., & Duncombe, L. W. (1983). The effect of psychological type on
the mood and meaning of two collage activities. American Journal of Occupational Therapy,
37, 688–693.
CB Insights. (2017). The United States of Unicorns: Every US company worth $1B+ in one
map. Retrieved from https://www.cbinsights.com/research/startup-unicorns-us-map/
Center for Immigration Studies [CIS]. (2014). One in five U.S. residents speaks for-
eign language at home, record 61.8 million. 3 Oct 2014. Retrieved from https://cis.org/
One-Five-US-Residents-Speaks-Foreign-Language-Home-Record-618-million
Chamorro-Premuzic,T., & Furnham, A. (2005). Personality and intellectual competence. Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Chang, J. H., Hsu, C. C., Shih, N. H., & Chen, H. C. (2014). Multicultural families and crea-
tive children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45, 1288–1296.
Chang, J. H., Su, J. C., & Chen, H. C. (2015). Cultural distance between parents’ and chil-
dren’s creativity: A within-country approach in Taiwan. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic
Minority Psychology, 21, 477–485.
Cheng, Y., Kim, K. H., & Hull, M. F. (2010). Comparisons of creative styles and personality
types between American and Taiwanese college students and the relationship between
creative potential and personality types. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4,
103–112.
Christensen, P. R., Guilford, J. P., & Wilson, R. (1957). Relations of creative responses to
working time and instructions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53(2), 82–88.
Cole, S., & Cole, J. R. (1973). Social stratification in science. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago
Press.
Costa, A., Foucart, A., Hayakawa, S., Aparici, M., Apestguia, J., Heafner, J., & Keysar, B. (2014).
Your morals depend on your language. PLoS One, 9, e94842.
Crutchfield, R. (1962). Conformity and creative thinking. In H. E. Gruber, G. Terrell, & M.
Wertheimer (Eds.), Contemporary approaches to creative thinking (pp. 120–140). New York:
Atherton Press.
Cummins, J. (2000). Language, power, and pedagogy: Bilingual children in the crossfire. Tonawanda,
NY: Multilingual Matters.
Cushen, P. J., & Wiley, J. (2011). Aha! Voila! Eureka! Bilingualism and insightful problem
solving. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 458–462.
Da Costa, S., Páez, D., Sánchez, F., Garaigordobil, M., & Gondim, S. (2015). Personal factors
of creativity: A second order meta-analysis. Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology,
31, 165–173.
De Dreu, C. K., Nijstad, B. A., Baas, M., Wolsink, I., Roskes, M. J. P., & Bulletin, S. P. (2012).
Working memory benefits creative insight, musical improvisation, and original ideation
through maintained task-focused attention. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38,
656–669.
Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modulates cognitive control:
Reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, 30, 343–353
Eiduson, B. T. (1962). Scientists:Their psychological world. New York: Basic Books.
Eitinger, L., & Major, E. F. (2010). Stress of the Holocaust. In L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz
(Eds.), Handbook of stress. New York: Free Press.
How does bilingualism affect creativity? 35
Engel, J. S. (Ed.) (2014). Global clusters of innovation: Entrepreneurial engines of economic growth
around the world. London: Elgar.
Eschleman, K. J., Bowling, N. A., & Alarcon, G. M. (2010). A meta-analytic examination of
hardiness. International Journal of Stress Management, 17, 277–307.
Fallatah, W. (2017). Bilingual creativity in Saudi stand-up comedy. World Englishes,
36, 666–683.
Fee, A., & Gray, S. J. (2012). The expatriate-creativity hypothesis: A longitudinal field test.
Human Relations, 65, 1515–1538.
Feist, G. J. (1993). A structural model of scientific eminence. Psychological Science,
4, 366–371.
Feist, G. J. (1999). The influence of personality on artistic and scientific creativity. In R. J.
Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity. (pp. 273–296). New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Ferjan Ramírez, N., Ramírez, R. R., Clarke, M., Taulu, S., & Kuhl, P. K. (2017). Speech
discrimination in 11-month-old bilingual and monolingual infants: A magnetoencepha-
lography study. Developmental Science, 20, e12427. doi: 10.1111/desc.12427
Fernandez, M., Tartar, J. L., Padron, D., & Acosta, J. (2013). Neurophysiological marker of
inhibition distinguishes language groups on a nonlinguistic executive function test. Brain
and Cognition, 83, 330–336.
Fleeson, W. (2001). Toward a structure-and process-integrated view of personality: Traits as
density distributions of states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 1011–1027.
Fleith, D. de S., Renzulli, J. S., & Westberg, K. L. (2002). Effects of a creativity training
program on divergent thinking abilities and self-concept in monolingual and bilingual
classrooms. Creativity Research Journal, 14, 373–386.
Fleming, L., Mingo, S., & Chen, D. (2007). Collaborative brokerage, generative creativity, and
creative success. Administrative Science Quarterly, 52, 443–475.
Florida, R. (2002a). Bohemia and economic geography. Journal of Economic Geography, 2(1),
55–71.
Florida, R. (2002b). The economic geography of talent. Annals of the Association of American
Geographers, 92, 743–755.
Florida, R. (2002c). The rise of the creative class and how it’s transforming work, leisure, community
and everyday life. New York: Basic Books.
Florida, R. (2008). Who’s your city? How the creative economy is making where to live the most
important decision of your life. New York: Basic Books.
Florida, R., & Gates, G. (2003). Technology and tolerance: The importance of diversity to
high-technology growth. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution, Center for Urban and
Metropolitan Policy.
Ghonsooly, B., & Showqi, S. (2012). The effects of foreign language learning on creativity.
English Language Teaching, 5(4), 161–167.
Gardner, H. (2011). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York, NY: Basic
Books.
Goertzel, M. G., Goertzel,V., & Goertzel,T. G. (1978). 300 Eminent personalities: A psychosocial
of the famous. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Gold, B. T. (2016). Lifelong bilingualism, cognitive reserve and Alzheimer’s disease: A review
of findings. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6, 171–189.
Gold, B. T., Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., Kryscio, R. J., & Smith, C. D. (2013). Lifelong bilin-
gualism maintains neural efficiency for cognitive control in aging. Journal of Neuroscience,
33, 387–396.
Gollan,T. H., Montoya, R. I., & Werner, G. A. (2002). Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish–
English bilinguals. Neuropsychology, 16, 562–576.
36 Kyung Hee Kim and Hang Eun Lee
Gough, H. G. (1976). Studying creativity by means of word association tests. Journal of Applied
Psychology and Personality, 61, 348–353.
Grady, C. L., Luk, G., Craik, F. I. M., & Bialystok, E. (2015). Brain network activity in mono-
lingual and bilingual older adults. Neuropsychologia, 66, 170–181.
Greenholtz, J., & Kim, J. I. (2009).The cultural hybridity of Lena: A multi-method case study
of a third culture kid. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 33, 391–398.
Gryskiewicz, N. D., & Tullar, W. L. (1995). The relationship between personality type and
creativity style among managers. Journal of Psychological Type, 32, 30–35.
Hall, W. B., & MacKinnon, D. W. (1969). Personality inventory correlates of creativity among
architects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53, 322–326.
Hamers, J. F., & Blanc, M. H. A. (2000). Bilinguality and bilingualism (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Hanna, P. L. (2011). Gaining global perspective: Educational language policy and planning.
International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism,14, 733–749.
Hayes, J. R. (1989). The complete problem solver (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Helson, R. (1971). Women mathematicians and the creative personality. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 36, 210–220.
Helson, R., & Crutchfield, R. S. (1970). Mathematicians: The creative researcher and the
average. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34, 250–257.
Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Fischer, R., & Christoffels, I. (2011). Bilingualism and creativ-
ity: Benefits in convergent thinking come with losses in divergent thinking. Frontiers in
Psychology, 2, 273.
Isaksen, S. G., Lauer, K. J., & Wilson, G.V. (2003). An examination of the relationship between
personality type and cognitive style. Creativity Research Journal, 15, 343–354.
Jacobson, C. M. (1993). Cognitive styles of creativity: Relations of scores on the Kirton
Adaption-Innovation Inventory and the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator among managers
in USA. Psychological Reports, 72, 1131–1138.
Johnson, R. C. (2003). Study of the relationship between cognitive styles of creativity and
personality types of military leaders. Dissertation Abstracts International, 64, (10-A).
Kaplan, R. (2009). Soaring on the wings of the wind: Freud, Jews and Judaism. Australasian
Psychiatry, 17, 318–325.
Karapetsas, A., & Andreou, G. (1999). Cognitive development of fluent and nonfluent bilin-
gual speakers assessed with tachistoscopic techniques. Psychological Reports, 84, 697–700.
Karwowski, M., & Lebuda, I. (2016). The big five, the huge two, and creative self-beliefs: A
meta-analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10, 214–232.
Kéri, S. (2011). Solitary minds and social capital: Latent inhibition, general intellectual
functions and social network size predict creative achievements. Psychology of Aesthetics,
Creativity, the Arts, 5, 215–221.
Keysar, B., Hayakawa, S. L., & Gyu, A. S. (2012). The foreign-language effect: Thinking in a
foreign tongue reduces decision biases. Psychological Science, 23, 661–668.
Kharkhurin, A.V. (2007). The role of cross-linguistic and cross-cultural experiences in bilin-
guals’ divergent thinking. In I. Kecskes & L. Albertazzi (Eds.), Cognitive aspects of bilingual-
ism (pp.175–210). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.
Kharkhurin, A. V. (2009). The role of bilingualism in creative performance on divergent
thinking and invented alien creatures tests. Journal of Creative Behavior, 43, 59–71.
Kharkhurin, A. V. (2010a). Bilingual verbal and nonverbal creative behavior. International
Journal of Bilingualism, 14, 211–226.
Kharkhurin, A.V. (2010b). Sociocultural differences in the relationship between bilingualism
and creativity potential. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 776–783.
How does bilingualism affect creativity? 37
Kharkhurin, A.V. (2017). Language mediated concept activation in bilingual memory facili-
tates cognitive flexibility. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1–16.
Kharkhurin, A. V., & Samadpour Motalleebi, S. N. (2008). The impact of culture on the
creative potential of American, Russian, and Iranian college students. Creativity Research
Journal, 20, 404–411.
Kim, K. H. (2016). The creativity challenge: How we can recapture American innovation. Amherst,
NY: Prometheus Books.
Kim, K. H. (2017). The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking — Figural or Verbal:
Which One Should We Use? Creativity. Theories-Research-Applications, 4, 302–321.
Kim, K. H. (2018). How can parents and teachers cultivate creative climates for children to
become innovators? Childhood Education, 94(2), 10–17.
Kim, K. H. (in press). Demystifying creativity: What creativity isn’t and is. Roeper Review.
Kim, K. H., & Lee, J. (in press). Creative and Gifted Education in Korea: Using the CATs
Theory to Illustrate How Creativity Can Grow into Innovation. In S. Smith, Australasian-
Pacific Handbook on Giftedness and Talent, Dordrecht,The Netherlands: Springer Social and
Behavioural Sciences.
Kim, B. S.,Yang, P. H., Atkinson, D. R.,Wolfe, M. M., & Hong, S. (2001). Cultural value simi-
larities and differences among Asian American ethnic groups. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic
Minority Psychology, 7, 343–361.
Kormi-Nouri, R., Moradi, A.-R., Moradi, S., Akbari-Zardkhaneh, S., & Zahedian, H.
(2012). The effect of bilingualism on letter and category fluency tasks in primary
school children: Advantage or disadvantage? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,
15(2), 351–364.
Kousaie, S., & Phillips, N. A. (2012). Ageing and bilingualism: Absence of a “bilingual
advantage” in Stroop interference in a nonimmigrant sample. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 65, 356–369.
Kovács, A. M., & Mehler, J. (2009). Flexible learning of multiple speech structures in bilingual
infants. Science, 325, 611–612.
Langford, M. (2014/2015 Winter). American in Britain, 22–23. London: Elliott.
Larson, M. J., Clayson, P. E., & Clawson, A. (2014). Making sense of all the conflict: A theoret-
ical review and critique of conflict-related ERPs. International Journal of Psychophysiology,
93, 283–297.
Lassig, C. J. (2013). Approaches to creativity: How adolescents engage in the creative process.
Thinking Skills and Creativity, 10, 3–12.
Lee, H., & Kim, K. H. (2011). Can speaking more languages enhance your creativity?
Relationship between bilingualism and creative potential among Korean American
students with multicultural link. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 1186–1190.
Lee, C. S., Therriault, D. J., & Linderholm, T. A. C. P. (2012). On the cognitive benefits of
cultural experience: Exploring the relationship between studying abroad and creative
thinking. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 768–778.
Lee, H. E. (2007). The relationship between bilingualism and creativity of Korean
Americans. (Doctoral dissertation), Retrieved from https://getd.libs.uga.edu/pdfs/
lee_hang-eun_200708_phd
Leikin, M. (2012). The effect of bilingualism on creativity: Developmental and educational
perspectives. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17, 431–447.
Leikin, M., & Tovli, E. (2014). Bilingualism and creativity in early childhood. Creativity
Research Journal, 26, 411–417.
Leung, A. K., & Chiu, C.Y. (2008). Interactive effects of multicultural experiences and open-
ness to experience on creative potential. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 376–382.
38 Kyung Hee Kim and Hang Eun Lee
Leung, A. K., & Chiu, C.Y. (2010). Multicultural experience, idea receptiveness, and creativ-
ity. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 723–741.
Leung, A., Maddux, W. W., Galinsky, A. D., & Chiu, C. Y. (2008). Multicultural experience
enhances creativity: The when and how. American Psychologist, 63, 169.
Levin, S. G., & Stephan, P. E. (1999). Are the foreign born a source of strength for U.S.
science? Science, 285, 1213–1214.
Li, W., Li, X., Huang, L., Kong, X., Yang, W., Wei, D., … Liu, J. (2015). Brain structure links
trait creativity to openness to experience. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10,
191–198.
Livermore, D. (2016). Driven by difference: How creative companies fuel innovation through diversity.
New York: Amacom.
Lubart, T. I. (1999). Creativity across cultures. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity
(pp.339–350). Boston, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Luk, G., Anderson, J. A. E., Craik, F. I. M., Grady, C., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Distinct neural
correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: Response inhibition versus interfer-
ence suppression. Brain and Cognition, 74, 347–357.
Luk, G., Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Grady, C. L. (2011). Lifelong bilingualism maintains
white matter integrity in older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 16808–16813.
Ma, H.-H. (2009). The effect size of variables associated with creativity: A meta-analysis.
Creativity Research Journal, 21, 30–42.
Maddux, W. W., Adam, H., & Galinsky, A. D. (2010). When in Rome… Learn why the
Romans do what they do: How multicultural learning experiences facilitate creativity.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 731–741.
Maddux, W. W., & Galinsky, A. D. (2009). Cultural borders and mental barriers: The relation-
ship between living abroad and creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96,
1047–1061.
Marinova-Todd, S. H., Marshall, D. B., & Snow, C. E. (2000).Three misconceptions about age
and L2 learning. TESOL Quarterly, 34(1), 9–34.
Maslow, A. H. (1959). Creativity in self-actualizing people. In H. H. Anderson (Ed.), Creativity
and its cultivation (pp. 83–95). New York: Harper & Row.
McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1258–1265.
Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69,
220–232.
Mezias, J. M., & Starbuck, W. H. (2003). What do managers know, anyway? Harvard Business
Review, 81(5), 16–18.
Mohanty, A. K., & Babu, N. (1982). Bilingualism and metalinguistic ability among Kond
tribals in Orissa, India. Journal of Social Psychology, 121, 15–22.
Mohl, A. (2011). The evolution of anti-Semitism: Historical and psychological roots. Journal
of Psychohistory, 39, 115–128.
Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013).Working memory development in monolingual
and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 187–202.
Morales, J.,Yudes, C., Gómez-Ariza, C. J., & Bajo, M. T. (2015). Bilingualism modulates dual
mechanisms of cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 66, 157–169.
Moreno, S., Wodniecka, Z., Tays, W., Alain, C., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Inhibitory control in
bilinguals and musicians: Event related potential (ERP) evidence for experience-specific
effects. PLoS ONE, 9, e94169.
Moulin, L. (1955). The Nobel Prizes for the sciences from 1901–1950: An essay in sociolog-
ical analysis. British Journal of Sociology, 6, 246–263.
How does bilingualism affect creativity? 39
Myers, I. B., McCaulley, M. H., & Most, R. (1985). Manual: A guide to the development and use
of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Okoh, N. (1980). Bilingualism and divergent thinking among Nigerian and Welsh school
children. Journal of Social Psychology, 110, 163–170.
Osborn, A. F. (1963). Applied imagination; principles and procedures of creative problem-solving
(3rd ed.). New York: Charles Schribner’s.
Parnes, S. J. (1961). Effects of extended effort in creative problem solving. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 52, 117–122.
Paulus, P. B., Kohn, N. W., & Arditti, L. E. (2011). Effects of quantity and quality instructions
on brainstorming. Journal of Creative Behavior, 45, 38–46.
Paulus, P. B., Nakui, T., Putman,V. L., & Brown,V. R. (2006). Effects of task instructions and
brief breaks on brainstorming. Group Dynamics:Theory, Research, and Practice, 10, 206–219.
Pierce, A. R., & Kim, K. H. (2014). Third culture kids. In S. Thompson (Ed.), Encyclopedia of
Diversity and Social Justice. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.
Pons, F., Bosch, L., & Lewkowicz, D. J. (2015). Bilingualism modulates infants’ selective atten-
tion to the mouth of a talking face. Psychological Science, 26, 490–498.
Putman, V. L., & Paulus, P. B. (2009). Brainstorming, brainstorming rules and decision
making. Journal of Creative Behavior, 43, 29–40.
Raina, M. K. (1999). Cross-cultural differences. Encyclopedia of Creativity, 1, 453–464.
Ricciardelli, L. A. (1992). Bilingualism and cognitive development: A review of past and
recent findings. Journal of Creative Behavior, 26, 242–254.
Richter, R. H., & Winter, W. D. (1966). Holtzman Inkblot correlates of creative potential.
Journal of Projective Techniques and Personality Assessment, 30(1), 62–67.
Rodríguez-Pujadas, A., Sanjuán, A., Fuentes, P.,Ventura-Campos, N., Barrós-Loscertales, A.,
& Ávila, C. (2014). Differential neural control in early bilinguals and monolinguals during
response inhibition. Brain and Language, 132, 43–51.
Roe, A. (1953). The making of a scientist. New York: Dodd, Mead.
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Araujo, K., Weekes,V. A., Caracciolo,V., Padilla, M., Ostrosky-Solís, F.
(2000). Verbal fluency and repetition skills in healthy older Spanish–English bilinguals. Applied
Neuropsychology, 7(1), 17–24.
Russ, S. W. (1993). Affect and creativity: The role of affect and play in the creative process. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Sebastián-Gallés, N., Albareda-Castellot, B.,Weikum,W. M., & Werker, J. F. (2012). A bilingual
advantage in visual language discrimination in infancy. Psychological Science, 23, 994–999.
Shoghi, S., & Ghonsooly, B. (2015). The nature of metacognitive awareness in foreign lan-
guage learners and its interaction with creativity. Journal of Language Teaching and Research,
6, 1051–1057.
Simonton, D. K. (1975). Sociocultural context of individual creativity: A Transhistorical
time-series analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 1119–1133.
Simonton, D. K. (1994). Greatness:Who makes history and why. New York: Guilford Press.
Simonton, D. K. (1997). Foreign influence and national achievement: The impact of open
milieus on Japanese civilization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 86–94.
Simonton, D. K. (1999). Origins of genius: Darwinian perspectives on creativity. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Simonton, D. K. (2003). Creative cultures, nations, and civilizations: Strategies and results.
In P. B. Paulus & B. A. Nijstad (Eds.), Group creativity: Innovation through collaboration
(pp. 304–325). New York: Oxford University Press.
Simonton, D. K. (2004). Creativity in science: Chance, logic, genius, and zeitgeist. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
40 Kyung Hee Kim and Hang Eun Lee
Yi, X., Hu, W., Scheithauer, H., & Niu, W. (2013). Cultural and bilingual influences on
artistic creativity performances: Comparison of German and Chinese students. Creativity
Research Journal, 25, 97–108.
Yoon, E., Chang, C. T., Kim, S., Clawson, A., Cleary, S. E., Hansen, M., John, P. B., Chan,
T. K. (2013). A meta-analysis of acculturation/enculturation and mental health, Journal of
Counseling Psychology 60(1), 15–30.
Yousefi, R., Soleimani, M., & Ghazanfariyanpoor, S. (2017). Comparison between switch-
ing and creativity among bilingual and monolingual children. Journal of Rehabilitation,
18(1), 1–12.
Zuckerman, H. (1977). Scientific elite. New York: Free Press.
3
PEDAGOGIES FOR SCAFFOLDING
THINKING IN ESL
Integrating first principles of learning
Maya Gunawardena
Introduction
Despite the tremendous growth in the teaching and learning endeavours and the
abundance of learning resources, teachers who teach courses such as English as a
second language (ESL), English as a foreign language (EFL), English for Specific
purposes (ESP), and English for Academic purposes (EAP) face numerous chal-
lenges in engaging their learners in the instructed learning settings. Teaching
reading has been found to be influential because it impacts all language skills
(Birch, 2002). Recent studies on thinking skills in teaching ESL have examined
new ways to increase engagement in students’ learning by minimizing student
inhibition and promoting interaction and active thinking (Cheng, 2010).
Previous research argues that cultural issues and education traditions in some
countries seem to affect students’ learning, particularly how students interact
with their peers and teachers (Li & Wegerif, 2014; Gunawardena et. al., 2017).
Studies on teaching critical thinking have also highlighted cultural differences
in the way students develop thinking dispositions (Arkinson, 1997; Paton, 2010).
Despite these cultural variables, scaffolding has been found to be necessary
and useful for effective learning (Wilson, 2017). In the course of scaffolding
thinking, thinking routines are useful ways of helping students to develop their
thinking styles and dispositions (Harvard University Visible Learning Project).
Therefore, universal principles such as scaffolding have been found to be useful
in the instructional design for all contexts, despite different cultural styles in
learning and teaching.
There is a plethora of literature on how to teach ESL reading, writing, gram-
mar, listening, and speaking, including the pronunciation of English. Such
frameworks and ideas have been found to be useful for teacher education, yet
some are controversial because they were developed and empirically tested in
Pedagogies for scaffolding thinking in ESL 43
the West. Recent research into promoting thinking skills has expanded and
enriched such pedagogies irrespective of the context (Chamot, 1995; Tarvin
& Al-Arishi, 1991). Research into scaffolding thinking in ESL needs further
expansion. The focus on explicit thinking in other subject areas has been found
to be useful for developing more effective learning and teaching by addressing
inherent dilemmas. However, currently teaching thinking is not as significant
in ESL as in other skill-based areas. Wilson (2017) argues that “critical reading
pedagogy can be realized in different ways, but that nurturing students’ critical
dispositions, in particular, requires delicate scaffolding to support their develop-
ment as critical meaning-makers” (p. 256).
Therefore, this chapter aims to develop a framework to scaffold reading in
ESL by incorporating Merrill’s (2002) first principles of learning. Developing a
holistic teaching framework for scaffolding thinking in ESL in different stages
of reading lessons (pre, while, and post) will enhance the process of “nurtur-
ing students’ critical dispositions” (Wilson, 2017, p. 256). The framework has
been given the acronym, KADEI, (see details later in this chapter). This chapter
will also examine two exemplars adopting the KADEI framework to develop
thinking routines for critical reading. This framework aims to negotiate chal-
lenges of developing thinking in ESL where students’ language competencies
are lower and cultural factors may impinge on their learning. The chapter will
also discuss the implications of adopting such an approach to promote thinking
and learning in ESL.
this content obligatory model in the mainstream curriculum has several draw-
backs, such as the potential lack of ESL teachers with specialist subject knowl-
edge and the fact that many students are not acquiring the content due to
their language barriers. Thus content integration is not being widely supported
and implemented in some contexts, and ESL is still often taught as a separate
subject.
An alternative and much more feasible approach is to include content that
interests learners and get them to deeply reflect on content, as an incentive to
learn the language. The information gap between the students and the ESL
teacher would provide an authentic, natural context for interaction (Snow
et al., 1989) between them. It would also allow students to mentally engage with
the content at a deeper level despite their language barriers, and teachers could
act as more knowledgeable persons (Vygotsky, 1978) who can assist them with
language-related problems.
It is then the teachers’ duty to facilitate students’ language-learning and
deep-thinking skills and critical dispositions by adopting appropriate pedago-
gies that will sufficiently equip learners with skills for comprehension, and by
further integrating the content with their real-life experiences (Merrill, 2002).
Teachers may have a different orientation to developing thinking (Gunawardena
et al., 2017) and critical pedagogy (Wilson, 2017); however, scaffolding their
thinking using a macro model would assist all students who are learning the
language and relating it to the world around them.
The Gunawardena et al. (2017) study indicated that teachers are in con-
sistent agreement with the importance of embedding thinking into their
reading lessons; yet their descriptions of the lessons demonstrated that they
used the term scaffolding loosely and referred only to micro-level scaffold-
ing. Merrill’s first principles of learning resonate with teachers, and there-
fore these principles are embedded in the model discussed in this paper,
which seeks to provide macro-level scaffolding where students read texts to
construct knowledge and broaden their ESL language repertoire in a more
engaging environment.
learning. By examining several design theories (p. 45), he identifies five phases
of effective instruction:
TABLE 3.1 KADEI in application: Reading about Queen Victoria (an information piece)
provide two examples of how a teacher might use the KADEI framework in
facilitating reading. The two reading examples are drawn from the Sri Lankan
state school textbook for grade 11, and they are quite self-explanatory in their
content, so that readers from other contexts may be able to understand the les-
son procedure and proposed activities.
Table 3.1 provides the sequence of a reading lesson where KADEI is applied
in scaffolding students’ thinking. The lesson is an information text about Queen
Victoria written by a local panel of teachers. The lesson objective is to increase
students’ basic reading comprehension skills, such as skimming and scanning to
answer a few simple questions provided in the text. The procedure has embedded
Pedagogies for scaffolding thinking in ESL 51
Integration
Expand
Demonstration
Activation
Global Local
Context Knowledge Context
Gap
De
ge f
mo Go
led n o
ns als
ow tio
tra
Students’ Prior
Kn ra
w st
tio
Knowledge
Ne mon
n
of
De
Regional Context
FIGURE 3.1 The KADEI framework for scaffolding students’ thinking in reading lessons.
TABLE 3.2 KADEI in application: Reading about friendship (an opinion piece)
Knowledge gap Teacher introduces the topic Complete the task and
and asks students to write their share their thoughts with
own opinions about friendship other students
Activation Teacher gets students to think Think and write
about different views/questions questions and share their
about friendship questions with the class
Demonstration Before reading the text, teacher Listen, tell, think, and
1. Demonstrate learning provides a few questions for summarize
goals (pre-reading) which they will seek answers
2. Demonstrate learned in the text
knowledge Teacher asks the students to read
(post-reading) the text to help them
summarize the content
Expand learning and Teacher asks students to think Research or talk with
thinking beyond to about how culture might partners, and present
connect influence friendship (teacher their findings to the class
can provide resources help them
arrive at their conclusions)
Integration of Teacher gives students different Students connect themes
knowledge cultural contexts and asks them and topics and have their
to research what friends do on own views about the
different occasions world and the country
to their examination” (T3). “They want the teachers’ help them with the exam by giving
them past questions” (T8). Therefore, the national examination culture seems to
be a crucial obstacle for learner engagement in learning.
The problem of students’ detachment from the content, as some teachers
think, was a significant obstacle for providing students with activities for reading,
thinking, and expanding, which ultimately results in students’ disengagement.
It is also important to note that the time when the KADEI framework was used
may have been the reason why teachers have experienced such dilemmas. One
teacher said: “This is towards the end of the year and students are not very focused,
and students in grade 11 can only think about their examination” (T10). If the model
had been adopted in a different period, the results might have been different.
Therefore, the results can be inconclusive, yet examination can generally be
regarded as a constraint for teacher flexibility in instructional design for ESL in
senior secondary classes.
The above situation, if it relates to any context (perhaps it does to many
ESL contexts in their own countries), contradicts Merrill’s argument for active
learning in a problem-based environment to decrease disengagement and
underachievement. Connecting learners with the content may be much eas-
ier than connecting learners to language structures or vocabulary in isolation
(Cummins, 1980, 1981). However, teachers’ are preoccupied with the upcoming
examination, (“to teach the test”) and they seem to address students’ imme-
diate needs. Students’ expectation of teachers helping them with the potential
examination questions can be justifiable, because in contexts such as Sri Lanka,
passing the examination is essential for entering higher education and to obtain
employment opportunities; their tacit knowledge of content or language is least
important, and the examination result is a critical determiner of their future.
Teachers have also noted potential problems of the model: Steps are long
and difficult to execute in one lesson (40 minutes in this context) (T1, T2, and
T7); students’ language abilities are limited, and using KADEI in a multilevel
classroom is a challenge (T10 and T9); students’ general knowledge is poor, and
therefore they did not engage in conversations as expected (T1, T3, T8, and T6);
they are not used to the culture of asking questions and thinking beyond, and
they waited for the teachers to fill in the gaps (T1). These limitations were noted
consistently across ten teachers — some explicitly and others implicitly, in the
context of similar problems. Such problems as poor language knowledge, time
constraints, multilevel classrooms, and issues with learning and teaching tradi-
tions exist in many contexts.
I argue that the above problems cannot necessarily be attributed to the prob-
lems with the proposed model as they resonate with the problems that the
model attempts to address by embedding Merrill’s principles. Teachers need
to be equipped with resources and skills to mitigate such typical problems
that ESL teachers confront in an instructed setting. One teacher (T5) reported
applying the KADEI procedures without any problems and found the stu-
dents fully engaged with the controversial topic, which was “advantages and
54 Maya Gunawardena
DuFour argues that there is a need for congruity between teachers and leaders of
schools and that they should collaboratively commit to ensuring that all students
learn and are successful in achieving desired results, irrespective of their entry
levels and learning barriers.
This chapter has highlighted the need for a macro model for scaffolding
thinking. If teachers use approaches such as KADEI, students will develop
an automated path to think, research, and expand their understanding of the
world. Often ESL lessons terminate in reading comprehension and building
vocabulary skills (or simply addressing the needs of the education traditions,
such as meeting the examination needs mentioned in this chapter). Through
KADEI, students can extend their learning beyond facts or information and
arrive at divergent thinking, thereby increasing their cognitive and metacog-
nitive skills.
The KADEI model aims to address the challenges inherent in the use of a text-
book for learning by providing guidelines for thinking and extension through
whatever resources are available to learners. Despite the limitations of the use of
textbooks, poor countries can only afford to provide a textbook, which is indeed
better than having no resources for learning. Therefore, teachers need to be
equipped with approaches to use textbooks in the 21st century to develop criti-
cal reading skills. There is also a dire need to change the entire school culture to
produce more informed citizens.
Conclusion
This chapter has presented a macro model to scaffold thinking in teaching
reading in ESL lessons. Despite the limitation of students’ language compe-
tencies, the KADEI framework offers useful ideas for teachers to help students
develop thinking dispositions when learning English. It has incorporated use-
ful principles from Merrill’s paper (2002) to motivate and increase learner
engagement in reading and developing critical reading skills. It provides
examples only to assist teachers in implementing the model, which should
not be used as a prescriptive or rigid model. Teachers can incorporate various
teaching strategies to make reading more interesting for their learners. The
chapter also examined the efficacy of the model, based on a small sample
of teachers executing the model in their reading lessons. However, findings
are inconclusive; the model must be used in many contexts in order for its
effectiveness in all contexts to be evaluated. There is no “one-size-fits-all”
model, and teachers are requested to test the KADEI model and amend it for
their purposes. The main proposition for embedding thinking-based learn-
ing is universally supported in modern education (Chamot, 1995; Harvard
University Zero Project, 2015; Merrill, 2002; Tarvin & Al-Arishi, 1991;
Wilson, 2017). We need to create a culture of thinking to produce successful
learners who can make informed decisions, and they must be ready for the
unpredictable, rapidly changing world.
56 Maya Gunawardena
References
Arkinson, D. (1997). A critical approach to critical thinking in TESOL. TESOL Quarterly,
31(1), 71–94.
Birch, B. M. (2002). English L2 reading: Getting to the bottom. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Campbell, J., & Li, M. (2008). Asian students’ voices: An empirical study of Asian students’
learning experiences at a New Zealand university. Journal of Studies in International
Education, 12(4), 375–396.
Canagarajah, A. S. (1999). Resisting linguistic imperialism in English teaching. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Castaneda-Pena, H. A., Barbosa-Chacon, J. W., & Hormiga-Sanchez, M. (2017). Exploring
the complementarity between information literacy and foreign language competencies.
Revista ESPACIOS, 38(35), 41.
Chamot, A. (1995). Creating a community of thinkers in the ESL/EFL. TESOL Matters,
5(5), 1–16.
Cheng, V. M. (2010). Tensions and dilemmas of teachers in creativity reform in a Chinese
context. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 5(3), 120–137.
Choy, S. C., & Cheah, P. K. (2009). Teacher perceptions of critical thinking among students
and its influence on higher education. International Journal of Teaching and Learning in
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20(2), 198–206.
Cummins, J. (1980). The exit and entry fallacy in bilingual education. NABE Journal, 4(3),
25–59.
Cummins, J. (1981). The role of primary language development in promoting educational
success for language minority students. In J. Cummins, Schooling and language minority
students: A theoretical framework (pp. 3–49). Los Angeles: California State Department
of Education, California State University, National Evaluation, Dissemination and
Assessment Center.
Derewianka, B. (1991). Exploring how texts work. Newtown, New South Wales: PETA.
DuFour, R. (2004). What is a “professional learning community”? Educational Leadership,
61(8), 6–11.
Duran, L. B., & Duran, E. (2004). The 5E instructional model: A learning cycle approach for
inquiry-based science teaching. Science Education Review, 3(2), 49–58.
Etikan, I., Musa, S. A., & Alkassim, R. S. (2016). Comparison of convenience sampling and
purposive sampling. American Journal of Theoretical and Applied Statistics, 5(1), 1–4.
Fisher, D. (2008). Effective use of the gradual release of responsibility model. Author
Monographs, 1–4. Retrieved from https://www.mheonline.com/_treasures/pdf/doug-
las_fisher.pdf
Fisher, D., Frey, N., & Lapp, D. (2008). Shared readings: Modeling comprehension, vocabulary,
text structures, and text features for older readers. The Reading Teacher, 61(7), 548–556.
Gunawardena, M., Sooriyampola, M., & Walisundara, N. (2017). Scaffolding thinking in ESL
lessons: Negotiating challenges. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 24, 279–285.
Hammond, J. (2001). Scaffolding: Teaching and learning in language and literacy education.
Newtown, Australia: Primary English Teaching Association,
Hammond, J., & Gibbons, P. (2005) Putting scaffolding to work: The contribution of scaf-
folding in articulating ESL education. Prospect, 20(1)6–30.
Harvard University Zero Project. http://www.pz.harvard.edu/ accessed and viewed
10/11/17.
Kramsch, C. (1993). Context and culture in language teaching. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Li, L., & Wegerif, R. (2014).What does it mean to teach thinking in China? Challenging and
developing notions of ‘Confucian education’. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 11, 22–32.
Maier, J., & Richter, T. (2014). Fostering multiple text comprehension: How metacognitive
strategies and motivation moderate the text-belief consistency effect. Metacognition and
learning, 9(1), 51-74.
Mercer, N (1994). Neo-Vygotskian theory and classroom education. In B. Steirer & J. Maybin
(Eds.), Language, Literacy and Learning in Educational Practice. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual
Matters.
Merrill, M. D. (2002). First principles of instruction. Educational Technology Research and
Development, 50(3), 43–59.
Paton, M (2010). Asian students, critical thinking and English as an academic lingua franca.
Analytic Teaching and Philosophical Praxis, 32(1) 27–39.
Pearson, P. D., and Gallagher, M. C. (1983).The instruction of reading comprehension. Higher
Education, 20(2), 198–206.
Snow, M. A., Met, M., & Genesee, F. (1989). A conceptual framework for the integration of
language and content in second/foreign language instruction. TESOL Quarterly, 23(2),
201–217.
Snow-Gerono, J. L. (2005). Professional development in a culture of inquiry: PDS teachers
identify the benefits of professional learning communities. Teaching and Teacher Education,
21(3), 241–256.
Tarvin, W., & Al-Arishi, A. (1991). Rethinking communicator language teaching: Reflection
and the ESL classroom. TESOL Quarterly, 25(1), 9–27.
Visible thinking: http://www.visiblethinkingpz.org/VisibleThinking_html_files/01_Visible
ThinkingInAction/01a_VTInAction.html viewed 10/11/17.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society:The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Wilson, K. (2017). Critical reading, critical thinking: Delicate scaffolding in English for
academic purposes (EAP). Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 256–265.
Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89–100.
Introduction
This chapter reports a small-scale action research of my implementation of
the thinking skills pedagogy for teaching theoretical linguistics to Saudi uni-
versity students. The students are specializing in the field of foreign languages
(i.e. English Language major) at Taif University. It is hoped that this chapter will
serve as a research-based practical guide for linguistics instructors teaching in
Middle Eastern higher education sectors who might find the teaching of English
theoretical linguistics to non-natives challenging.
The chapter starts by highlighting the merits of using thinking skills pedagogies
for teaching foreign languages and providing a rationale for teaching theoretical
linguistics through thinking skills pedagogies. The chapter then discusses the meth-
odology adapted for carrying out this study in a classroom of 50 female students
majoring in English Language at Taif University, Saudi Arabia. This is followed by
the Findings section, where illustrations of some extracts from classroom discus-
sions of activities designed for teaching theoretical linguistics are presented and dis-
cussed in relation to literature. Students’ views of the implemented pedagogy were
obtained from a focus group and interviews. The discussion of the main findings
is supported by my reflections, as the class instructor and researcher, on the design,
implementation procedures, and assessment methods used. The chapter ends with
suggested criteria for successfully implementing thinking skills pedagogies that are
designed for teaching linguistics courses but could be adapted to other majors.
emphasize the need for implementing thinking skills into their foreign language
programmes without providing implementation guidance that could be followed by
instructors (Al-Nofaie, 2013). Another reason is that only a limited number of stud-
ies have investigated the merits of implementing thinking skills for teaching foreign
language majors. Available studies shed light on the application of thinking skills
for teaching English as a Foreign Language (EFL), more specifically for teaching
language skills: writing, reading, and speaking skills (e.g. Alwehaibi, 2012; Fairley,
2009; Park, 2011; Rahimi, 2013, 2018). According to the findings of these studies,
thinking skills can help learners master language skills such as writing, reading, and
debate skills. Also, thinking skills can help learners reflect on their learning progress
since it raises their awareness of their learning experiences (Lin & Mackay, 2004).
The majority of available studies, mentioned above, on implementing thinking
skills pedagogies for teaching foreign language skills measure the effects of these
pedagogies on learning via pre- and post-measures. Some of these studies have
incorporated interviews and/or reflective journals which lead to valid findings.
However, the issue of EFL teacher training for implementing thinking skills needs
more investigation. There are studies which conclude that EFL teachers find it
difficult to relate thinking skills to EFL teaching (Beder & Petek, 2018; Li, 2016).
The insufficiency in the number of published detailed descriptions of the design,
application procedures, and evaluations of thinking skills pedagogies will leave
EFL instructors with little research-based guidance. Such detailed descriptions
and evaluations can be better understood from qualitative perspectives. There are
some studies that examine how thinking skills tasks might increase the quality of
discussions in teacher-fronted classrooms, with a particular focus on teacher talk
(e.g. Al-Nofaie, 2013; Li, 2011; Pinkevičienė, 2011). More of these studies that
rely on the qualitative analyses of teacher talk and strategies for infusing thinking
skills are needed to draw a clear picture of how thinking skills pedagogies can be
implemented successfully. It is very important for teachers to know the following:
Methodology
Study design and context
This study was an action research study where I was the researcher and the module
instructor. The context of this study was Taif University, located in the city of Taif,
Saudi Arabia. The university offers a wide range of undergraduate and postgrad-
uate degrees, and it aims to become a well-recognized university in the region in
terms of teaching excellence. For this purpose, Taif University has established a
teaching and learning centre and had signed a contract with the Higher Education
Academy, UK, for training its academic staff. The sample that participated in this
study consisted of 50 undergraduate female students majoring in English Language,
Teaching through thinking skills pedagogies 61
and the module chosen for this study was An Introduction to Linguistics, a module
that familiarizes students with all branches of linguistics (i.e. phonology, syntax,
semantics, … etc.). The module consisted of three hours per a week, and the total
period allotted for conducting this study was three months. The next section intro-
duces the planning and implementation processes of thinking skills pedagogies.
Module outcomes
1. Knowledge
1.1 Memorizing basic concepts and methodologies of linguistic science
1.2 Describing language issues in an informed way in both linguistics and
non-linguistics terms
1.3 Recalling the different general and specific basic features of human language in
terms of phonetics, morphology, syntax, semantics, and traditional linguistic
2. Cognitive skills
2.1 Interpreting critically and analytically linguistic subfields such as phonetics,
morphology, syntax, and semantics
2.2 Developing intelligent curiosity about the linguistic science
3. Interpersonal skills and responsibility
3.1 Demonstrating cooperation with the teacher when performing classroom tasks
3.2 Illustrating proper compliance with ethics when performing tasks given
4. Communication, information technology, numerical
4.1 Showing an ability to collaborate with classmates on classroom assignments
4.2 Showing an ability to engage in online communication
4.3 Showing an ability to use online academic resources
5. Psychomotor
Demonstrating an ability to produce task outcomes
62 Haifa Al-Nofaie
thinking, taking learners to broader and deeper levels (Leat, 2001). I was keen to
ensure that my learning activities help students to apply their HOTS in order to
achieve their potential and the ultimate goal of my teaching philosophy: creating
autonomous learners through enhancing learners’ HOTS and reflective skills.
For this purpose, I found it useful to adapt some activities that match the out-
comes of the module that are specified by the Department of Foreign Languages
at Taif University (Table 4.1). Activities were from David Leat’s book Thinking
through Geography (2001), and Lin and Mackay’s book Thinking through Foreign
Modern Languages (2004). These key guides include various classroom tech-
niques and activities for enhancing learners’ HOTS, such as Venn diagrams,
mind-mapping, and argumentative questions that could be adapted for teaching
any field of study. The following paragraphs provide details on the planning,
implementation procedures, and assessment used in this module.
In order to understand the students’ needs, and before starting the first session
on thinking skills, I gave my 50 students a paper survey that investigated their
knowledge and application of thinking skills. Based on the results, I found that
the knowledge of a majority of my students (48 students) did not go beyond
analysis and making inferences. They related thinking skills to skills used for
analysing literary texts, while 2 students helped me to identify the sorts of think-
ing skills students need to develop and thus adapt activities that targeted HOTS.
I started my first session by introducing the different levels of thinking skills and
reflection and how these skills could be applied. I was explicit about the benefits
of thinking skills for their academic progress and future career in general and
understanding linguistic aspects in particular (Leat, 2001).
In order to facilitate the teaching of HOTS tasks, I incorporated the task-
based teaching approach (TBT) for delivering these HOTS tasks, an approach
that requires learners to interact collaboratively based on task instructions and
produce expected task outcomes (Ellis, 2003). As in any TB lesson, the task is
introduced in three phases:
• Pre-task phase, in which students are given warm-up activities to introduce
the new topic.
• While-task phase, in which students work collaboratively on the task fol-
lowing given instructions.
• Post-task phase, in which students share the task outcomes.
In a thinking skill lesson, the last phase, the post-task phase, is not limited to
the sharing of task outcomes. It goes beyond that to include reflection or what is
called debriefing (Leat, 2001). In this phase, students reflect on their learning and
relate what they have learned to their lives.
I used at least one thinking-skill-based activity for each chapter to ensure that
students practiced thinking skills. Some of the activities were comparison sheets in
which students compare two linguistic items or features. To clarify things for my
students and to enhance their deeper learning, I asked them to compare Arabic and
English linguistic features because this could help them relate learning of the new
Teaching through thinking skills pedagogies 63
language to their mother tongue. For instance, one of the activities asked the stu-
dents to compare the use of prefixes and suffixes in the Arabic and English languages,
using Venn diagrams, aimed at activating students’ analysis skills (Leat, 2001; Lin &
Mackay, 2004; see Appendix A for an example). In the debriefing phase of the post-
task phase, the discussion also asked students to comment on possible similarities
that could be found in languages that were derived from different language families.
Findings
Learners’ engagement into learning
The influence of visual tools versus non-visual tools. The results discussed
here come from tasks that used visual tools, such as using Venn diagrams and
mind-mapping, and those tasks that did not use visual tools, such as argumen-
tative questions. To begin with, the Venn diagram was a helpful learning tool
for some students in terms of understanding linguistics features. The following
extract shows how Venn diagrams helped some students to compare some lin-
guistic concepts in Arabic and English.
Extract 1
31 T: ↑ What did you write for the differences? (0.6)
32 S1: We said uh … both Arabic and English have affixes and-
33 T: So you are talking about the similarities=
34 S1: = Yes (.) similarities
35 T: ↑ Okay go on
36 S1: Also similar to English affixes in Arabic come at the beginning and the
37 end=
38 S2: =Yah like the Prefix [YA] at the beginning of verb to make present tense
39 S3: But in English the present tense -s come last I mean after the verb
40 (.) this should be difference
64 Haifa Al-Nofaie
Extract 2
13 T: ↑o::kay/show me your map looks very nice
14 ((students laughs))
15 T: OK tell me how did you find mind-mapping
16 S1: I like it because I love (•drawing•)
17 T: ↑ Okay what about you
18 S2: It is easy to draw but it is not easy to find related examples
Teaching through thinking skills pedagogies 65
In Extract 2, the students enjoy drawing the maps, but S2 refers to an impor-
tant point, which is the cognitive load that thinking tasks might bear (lines 18,
21, and 22). According to Robinson (2001), complex tasks can contribute to
successful learning. During my observation of this group, I found that the stu-
dents were engaged in discussions and drawing the map and that they managed
to come up with interesting examples. The above conversation was in Arabic as
this could help students to express their views more confidently.
To enhance evaluation skills, debate questions were designed to engage stu-
dents in interactive discussions where there is no right or wrong answer. As a
teacher, I believe that learning through interaction is essential for mastering lan-
guage skills. This belief is underpinned by the sociocultural theory that empha-
sizes the need for learners’ interaction with teachers, peers, and society (Lantolf,
2000). For instance, when teaching phonology, I introduced the question: Is there
a particular age during which a language learner can pick up a foreign accent easily? Explain.
Extract 3
18 S1: I think (.) children learn faster and they co:py the accent=
19 T: =You think so↑
20 S1: Yes
21 T: What about the others (2.3)
22 S2: Yes children uh…fast learn but still may be cannot speak like natives
23 T: why not?
24 S2: Because the influence of mother tongue=
25 S3: =You do not need to sound like natives
26 Ss ( (inaudible chats))
27 T: What do you mean you do not need to sound like natives
28 S3: Yes I me::an if people can understand me this is enough
29 S4: for me (0.2) it is different native like accent is important
30 T: Very interesting discussion going on here can you all please listen↑
31 S1: Doctor last time you said English now is used as a lingua franca
32 T: Yes this is true but maybe there are other students in this class who
33 do not agree with S3 right? (0.4)
34 S5: Yes Doctor I do not agree with S3 because I think if you don’t talk
35 like native speakers miscommunication can happen
people can learn the language better (line 18). I encourage others to take part in
this conversation (line 21). S2 responds to my turn and expresses her opinion that
differs from S1’s opinion (lines 22–24). S3 initiates a turn to disagree with S1’s
opinion and support the view of S2 (line 25). My question in line 27 “What do
you mean you do not need to sound like natives” encourages S3 to expand her turn
in line 28. S4 expresses her view in line 29. These turns of disagreements indicate
that the students are engaged in the class talk. I praise the students’ contributions
to the conversation, and at the same time I try to draw the attention of others to
this conversation (line 30). S1 initiates a turn to build on the view of S3 (line 31).
In this turn (line 31), S1 not only supports the opinion of S3 but also moves the
conversation to a new direction (i.e. from the age of acquiring the language to the
issue of lingua franca). S1 takes the conversation back to a previous lecture they had
about lingua franca, which indicates her ability to link and evaluate information.
In lines (32 and 33), I try to make students aware of the importance of accepting
the views of others. This could encourage students to talk freely about argumen-
tative issues, as reflected in the turn by S5 (lines 34 and 35).
I like to create a space for discussions when students share and evaluate their
views. Sharing ideas help students to evaluate what they hear, think of alternative
answers, and decide on an appropriate answer (Leat, 2001; Lin & Mackay, 2004).
As an instructor, my role is to be a facilitator, and I do not intervene in such stu-
dent-student discussions in order to minimize my influence. I moved around the
tables, listened to students’ discussions, and reminded them that all their classmates’
opinions mattered. The students appreciated interaction based on argumentation.
I noticed that there were some of my shy students who did not contribute much
to class participation. For the purpose of their engagement in learning, I used a
teaching technique that I call RUSH, which means Read-Understand-Share. In
order to enhance understanding and analysis skills, I prefer to give students time to
do a text analysis where they have to read a text, understand its message, and then
share their understanding with the class. This kind of activity might suit students
who prefer to work alone instead of joining pairs or groups. I interviewed one of the
shy students and asked her to evaluate the RUSH technique. She explained that this
technique provides her with some time to think and share her thoughts with others.
Varying classroom activities can help teachers to meet different types of learners.
I always feel shy in the class. During my school years, I was not encouraged
by my teachers to talk in the class. We were asked to answer simple drills
that required writing a word or a simple sentence. I used to watch TV to
improve my English, but to be honest I did not spend much time on that.
Teaching through thinking skills pedagogies 67
Things are different at the Department. I must speak English all the time
with the instructors and classmates. I am not quite confident with my speak-
ing. I cannot speak without preparation. The RUSH technique provides me
with some time to organize my ideas and think about what I am going to
say. (Final interview)
Key issues in learning assessment
This section discusses the implementation of summative and formative assessment
in my module and considers their effectiveness in module objective achievement.
Saudi HE institutions place high emphasis on summative assessment, especially
through examinations. A constraint of this approach is that students often feel
anxious about their marks, and they put their efforts into achieving high marks
rather than taking their learning deeper and beyond their institution’s walls. The
distribution of marks for this module was as follows: 10% for presentations, 10%
for the open book quiz, 20% for the midterm examination, and 60% for the final
examination. For the validity of examination marks, I checked the marking crite-
ria, especially those for marking open-ended questions, with two other colleagues
who used to teach linguistic courses, to see if the marks were distributed logically.
Also, the validity of the examination questions was assessed by colleagues against
the module learning outcomes. In these examinations, I included lower thinking
questions, such as memorization questions, as well as HOTS questions, such as
analysis, evaluation, and reflection questions.
I recognize the importance of trying to balance the effects of examinations as an
assessment strategy that often prioritizes surface learning with the need to support
deeper learning. Since one aim of higher education is to equip learners with lifelong
learning skills, higher education institutions value the concept of assessment for
learning that prioritizes formative assessment (Boud & Falchikov, 2007). Effective
formative assessment is believed to be productive if students participate in setting
assessment goals and criteria. For this purpose, I made the formative assessment aim
and criteria explicit to my students from the first day. Also, I made sure that they
were satisfied with the assessment tools used for this module, and I offered choice by
allowing them to suggest alternative tools. One mutually agreed formative assess-
ment tool was giving immediate feedback on the performance of students in small
groups based on their activity sheets, while another was giving feedback on the
quality of class discussions as a whole. I felt that this would prompt students to think
together about things that needed to be improved for the next time. Regarding
individual feedback, I encouraged the students to attend individual drop-in sessions
in order to talk to them about their progress in more detail. I found the students
responsive to this step, as I managed to meet nearly all of them frequently. Also,
even though examinations were based on marks, I used to write my comments on
the margins of the examination papers, so students could understand the points of
improvement. Based on my observations, I have noticed that my students feel less
anxious when using the non-credit formative assessment tools explained above, a
point that confirms the benefits of formative assessment (Boud & Falchikov, 2007).
68 Haifa Al-Nofaie
The success of the thinking skills pedagogy for teaching linguistics became
more obvious at the end of the module. I have noticed that the students in their
final examinations became conscious of applying their evaluative skills and used
phrases such as “I think … because”, “based on my observations, I can say that …”,
“Unlike what many people might think, I believe that … because …”. Such
answers demonstrated that the students do not take things for granted and that
they are more aware of revealing their voices while learning. The students felt
excited about learning through this new approach. At the end of this module, the
students were required to complete online feedback, which included evaluating
the module design and extracurricular activities. I received between 4/5 and 4.5/5
for these items, which was higher than previous years’ ratings for the same mod-
ule (between 3/5 and 3.5/5). In her email, one student wrote: “I would like to
thank you for your teaching efforts … I have carried thinking skills and reflection
to other courses as well which helped me to improve my marks”. The following
section highlights the point of how the learning environment could support the
implementation of thinking skills pedagogies.
hours and through email. I encourage them to discuss with me any concerns that
they might have about this course. The number of the students who visit me
during my office hours or send me emails shows that students never waste any
opportunity to gain support if I show willingness to help.
Also, I sought to do my best to extend the students’ independent learning
opportunities beyond the class by utilizing BlackBoard. In order to meet my
pedagogic aim (enhancing HOTS for better learning opportunities), I posted
discussion questions that required students to search and add their own perspec-
tives. Some of the questions included their views on using English as a lingua
franca. Such BlackBoard activities help them feel more confident about express-
ing their own views. During some of my discussions with these students, I asked
them about their views of BlackBoard activities. They said that they liked the
kind of opinion questions that do not carry true or false answers. A student said,
“I hate memorization. I like BlackBoard opinion questions because they are all
about my own views, whether people agree with my views or not. This makes
me feel confident”. Another student said, “Opinion questions encourage me to
search for facts and information and think about what I read. I can see that my
searching and evaluation skills have been improved”.
• Teachers need to prepare their students for using their thinking skills through
explicit teaching and training.
• Material design should take into consideration the module outcomes and
students’ needs.
• Teachers need to encourage students to interact actively and use their reflective
skills in thinking lessons.
• In order to assess the effectiveness of thinking skills pedagogies, teachers need
to use different assessment methods.
• Teachers should provide students with a learning environment that encourages
and facilitates their learning (class arrangement, utilizing technology, regular
informal meetings, etc.).
• Teachers need to reflect on their teaching practices in order to enhance their
teaching quality.
as my learners (Warin et al., 2006). Here are some hints for teachers on how to
reflect on their teaching practices.
Conclusion
In this chapter, I share my experience of implementing the thinking skills pedagogy
for teaching English theoretical linguistics to Saudi university students. Teaching
theoretical linguistics could be challenging for Arab learners of English. This small-
scale action research reports how I experienced this pedagogy throughout different
phases: the task design, application, and learning assessment methods. The findings
of this study reveal that many learners prefer learning through visual methods, such
as Venn diagrams and mind-mapping, while shy learners prefer to avoid group
work and engage more with the RUSH technique. Also, the study highlights the
importance of varying learning assessment techniques, particularly when applying
a novice teaching pedagogy. The study suggests some criteria that should be fol-
lowed by instructors interested in applying thinking skills pedagogies. This chapter
could serve as a research-based practical guide for linguistics instructors who might
find the teaching of the theoretical linguistics of English to non-natives a hard task.
References
Al-Nofaie, H. (2013). The implementation of critical thinking as EFL pedagogy (PhD Thesis).
Newcastle University, Newcastle, Tyne and Wear, UK.
Al-Nofaie, H. (2017). Rapport-building strategies: Snapshots from Saudi EFL classrooms.
International Journal of Language Academy, 21, 304–315. http://dx.doi.org/10.18033/ijla.3826
Alwehaibi, H. (2012). Novel program to promote critical thinking among higher education
students: Empirical study from Saudi Arabia. Asian Social Science, 8 (11), 193–204. http://
dx.doi.org/10.5539/ass.v8n11p193
Teaching through thinking skills pedagogies 71
Beder, H., & Petek, E. (2018). An adaptable teacher education framework for critical thinking
in language teaching. Thinking Skills and Creativity Journal, 28, 56–72. doi: https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.tsc.2018.02.008
Biggs, J. B. (2003). Teaching for quality learning at university (2nd ed.). Buckingham: Open
University Press/Society for Research into Higher Education.
Bloom, B. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives: The classification of educational aims, handbook
1, cognitive domain. New York: David McKay.
Bonwell, C. (1996). Enhancing the lecture: Revitalizing a traditional format. New Directions
for Teaching and Learning, 67, 31–44. https://doi.org/10.1002/tl.37219966706
Boud, D., & Falchikov, N. (2007). Rethinking assessment in higher education: Learning for the
longer term. Oxon: Routledge.
Brown, S., & Knight, S. (1994). Assessing students in higher education. London: Kogan Page.
Ellis, R. (2003). Task-based language learning and teaching. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Fairley, M. J. (2009). De-silencing female voices:The use of controversial debate topics in the
EFL classroom. In P. E. Wachob (Ed.), Power in the EFL classroom: Critical pedagogy in the
Middle East (pp. 55–73). Newcastle Upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
Lantolf, J. (2000). Sociocultural theory and second language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Leat, D. (2001). Thinking through geography (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Chris Kington.
Li, L. (2011). Obstacles and opportunities for developing thinking through interaction in
language classrooms. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 6, 146–158. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.tsc.2011.05.001
Li, L. (2016). Integrating thinking skills in foreign language learning: What can we learn
from teachers’ perspectives? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 273–288. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.tsc.2016.09.008
Lin, M., & Mackay, S. (2004). Thinking through modern foreign languages. Cambridge: Chris
Kingston.
Long, M. H. (2016). In defence of tasks and TBLT: Nonissues and real issues. Annual Review
of Applied Linguistics, 36, 5–33. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0267190515000057
Park, Y. (2011). Using news articles to build a critical literacy classroom in an EFL setting.
TESOL Journal, 2(1), 24–51. https://doi.org/10.5054/tj.2011.244134
Pinkevičienė, D. (2011). Triadic dialogue in EFL classroom: Embedded extensions. Studies
about Languages, 18, 97–104. http://dx.doi.org/10.5755/j01.sal.0.18.416
Pollock, P. H., Hamann, K., & Wilson, B. M. (2011). Learning through discussions: Comparing
the benefits of small-group and large-class settings. Journal of Political Science Education,
7(1), 48–64. https://doi.org/10.1080/15512169.2011.539913
Rahimi, M. (2013). Is training student reviewers worth its while? A study of how training
influences the quality of students’ feedback and writing. Language Teaching Research, 17(1),
67–89. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168812459151
Rahimi, M. (2018). Effects of increasing the degree of reasoning and the number of ele-
ments on L2 argumentative writing. Language Teaching Research. https://doi.org/
10.1177/1362168818761465
Robinson, P. (2001).Task complexity, task difficulty, and task production: Exploring interaction
in a componential framework. Applied Linguistics, 22(1), 27–57. https://doi.org/10.1093/
applin/22.1.27
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Warin, J., Maddock, M., Pell, A., & Hargreaves, L. (2006). Resolving identity dissonance
through reflective and reflexive practice in teaching. Reflective Practice, 7(2), 233–245.
https://doi.org/10.1080/14623940600688670
Waring, H. Z. (2011). Learner initiatives and learning opportunities in the language classroom.
Classroom Discourse, 2(2), 201–218. http://doi.org/10.1080/19463014.2011.614053
Appendix A
Appendix B
Teaching through thinking skills pedagogies 73
Appendix C: CA Conventions
The transcription conventions used for transcribing data in this study are
adapted from the Jeffersonian Transcription Notation described in G. Jefferson,
“Transcription Notation,” in J. Atkinson and J. Heritage (eds), Structures of Social
Interaction, New York: Cambridge University Press, 1984, p. xi
Symbol Use
[[ ]] Simultaneous utterances – (beginning [[ ) and ( end]])
= Contiguous utterances
(0.4) the tenths of a second between utterances
(.) A brief pause, usually less than 0.2 seconds.
. or ↓ falling pitch.
? or ↑ rising pitch.
, a temporary rise or fall in intonation.
- an abrupt halt or interruption in utterance.
>text< The enclosed speech was delivered more rapidly than
usual for the speaker.
<text> The enclosed speech was delivered more slowly than
usual for the speaker.
° whisper or reduced volume speech.
ALL CAPS shouted or increased volume speech.
Underline the speaker is emphasizing or stressing the speech.
: Sound extension of a word (more colons demonstrate
longer stretches)
$ $ Smile voice
[BOLD] Arabic words are written in BOLD capitals between
brackets
(?) Intelligible speech
((text )) Analyst’s notes
5
FOSTERING CRITICAL THINKING
THROUGH QUESTIONING IN EFL
An Indonesian study
Introduction
Critical thinking has not often been explored in the literature on second and
foreign language learning, although it has been a widely debated topic in peda-
gogy more generally and in the teaching of disciplines such as science and social
studies. This lack of attention may derive from teachers’ uncertainty about how
critical thinking applies in the field of language learning. As Li (2016) found in
a study of 473 teachers in China, English language teachers may have positive
attitudes towards integrating critical thinking in their classes, but lack awareness
of how to link language learning and thinking skills. In fact, thinking skills play
a key role in learning a second language, both because acquiring language pro-
ficiency requires considerable higher-order thinking (e.g. learners need to make
choices about how to use appropriate expressions in particular contexts), and also
because, as learners gain increasing proficiency in the language, they can use it as
a medium for critical thinking about wider topics.
This chapter argues that critical thinking applies in two ways to language
learning. First, it can be understood as critical thinking ABOUT language. In
particular, language learning is promoted and enhanced when students engage
in thinking skills such as comparing and contrasting, analysing, and evaluating
— skills that are featured in Bloom’s (1984) taxonomy of learning objectives— in
order to better understand the target language, its vocabulary, its grammatical
forms and functions, and how to apply them in social contexts for particular pur-
poses. Second, students can engage in critical thinking THROUGH the target
language. Students may be expected, for instance, to interact critically with writ-
ten or spoken texts in the target language. A good example of such an approach
is presented by Wallace (2003), who describes an English class for migrants in
London, which focused on analysing controversial newspaper articles. Similarly,
Fostering critical thinking by questioning 75
Literature review
Definitions of critical thinking are notoriously controversial. However, Davies
and Barnett (2015) were able to summarize three key perspectives drawn from
the many competing definitions found in the literature. At the most fundamental
level they identified a “thinking skills” perspective characteristic of the cognitive
approach to pedagogy. The most influential theoretical framework underpinning
this approach remains Bloom’s (1984) taxonomy of learning objectives. Bloom
identified a hierarchy of different levels of thinking, among which knowing and
understanding are the most fundamental. He argued that students should go
beyond this to engage in higher-order thinking: Applying knowledge, analysing
information, synthesizing, and evaluating ideas. Bloom’s taxonomy also had a
strong influence on researchers in the EFL field and was reflected in the learning
strategies approach to language learning developed by Oxford (1990), Chamot
and O’Malley (1994), Rubin and Thompson (1994), and others. The strategy
inventories developed by these researchers are extremely detailed and compre-
hensive, so for the sake of this chapter, we will follow the succinct version of
Bloom’s taxonomy shown in Table 5.1, derived from the work of Ozuem and
Lancaster (2015) and Wong (2010). Bloom’s taxonomy can be applied effectively
to thinking ABOUT the language. Language learning, like any other kind of
content learning, can be enhanced by not simply recalling but by applying knowl-
edge (e.g. applying grammar rules), analysing (e.g. comparing grammatical forms
76 Maya Defianty and Kate Wilson
Based on: Ozuem and Lancaster (2015, p. 478) and Wong (2010, p. 38).
animals. Some standard EFL textbooks, such as New Headway (Soars & Soars,
2017) and Interchange (Richards, 2017), provide plenty of opportunities for stu-
dents to engage with such topics and provide prompt questions to initiate crit-
ical exchanges in the classroom.
The third perspective is what Davies and Barnett (2015) call, “the criti-
cal pedagogy perspective”. Some proponents of critical thinking believe that
students should develop a social conscience, becoming acutely aware of pow-
erful, hegemonic forces at play in our world and of the ways in which these
forces promote or disenfranchise participants in society. For theorists such
as Pennycook (1997), Fairclough (1989), and Luke (2002), teachers have a
responsibility to stimulate students’ engagement with issues of colonialism,
racism, and gender inequality, for example. Several practitioners have written
up their attempts to integrate the critical pedagogy approach into language
pedagogy. For example, Pohl (2005) described an innovative EAP course in
an Australian university, which attempted to raise students’ critical awareness
of the sociopolitical power relations embedded in a range of texts, particularly
in relation to international education. Students reflected on their language
learning experiences, their current identities as English language learners,
and their motivations for seeking a Western education. They then read and
discussed texts on linguistic and cultural imperialism. The course achieved
its aim of raising awareness of the sociohistorical context of English language
learning, but Pohl was disappointed by the students’ reluctance to challenge
power relationships.
Teachers can bring these perspectives on critical thinking into their classes
in many different ways, for example by their choice of texts, by the activi-
ties and tasks that they introduce (see Yang & Gamble, 2013, for e.g.), and
in particular by the questions that they pose. Questioning is one of the most
fundamental of all teaching tools and can play an important role in promoting
critical thinking. Socrates famously used questions to push his students to struggle
with ideas. As confirmed by Li (2011), the teacher holds a key role in construct-
ing space for thinking. For example, teachers who ask more referential questions
(questions to which the teacher does not already know the answer — genuine
questions) provide more space for thinking skills than teachers who pose dis-
play questions (questions that simply ask students to display what they have
learned). Teachers use questions for many different purposes, both instruc-
tional and regulative (Christie, 2002): Some questions merely serve to reg-
ulate students’ behaviour (for example “Are we ready now?”), while others
serve to move learning forward. Often, Christie (2002, p. 173) argues, “the
regulative register appropriates or speaks through the instructional register”,
so the teacher’s questions may control the pace of the lesson, manage class-
room routines, and demand students’ attention, as well as having an overt
instructional purpose. The most ubiquitous questioning technique is the IRE
sequence (initiate-respond-evaluate), in which the teacher asks a question, to
which students respond, and the response is evaluated by the teacher before
78 Maya Defianty and Kate Wilson
commencing the next cycle of IRE. The IRE pattern is also seen in the
“chorus” style of classroom discourse, in which the teacher asks a question by
making an unfinished statement ending in a strong upward intonation to which
the students respond in chorus by finishing the statement. For example:
To what extent do senior high school EFL teachers’ questions foster critical
thinking?
Method
The data used here were gathered as part of a multiple case study conducted in
EFL classes in senior high schools in West Java, Indonesia: Three schools under
the Ministry of Education and Culture (MoEC) and four schools under the man-
agement of the Ministry of Religious Affairs (MoRA). The teachers who agreed
to participate were selected based on two main criteria: They had participated
in recent teacher development programmes, and they were recommended by
the relevant teachers’ association and the school principal. These teachers were
teaching in various grades in senior secondary schools from grade 10 to grade 12.
Each participant was observed for a series of 3 to 7 classes, each class lasting for
90 minutes. Class sizes varied from 13 to 42 (Table 5.2).
The data were gathered from classroom observations of English classes during
which teacher-student interactions were audio-recorded, and field notes were
made. Classroom artefacts were also collected to support the data from the obser-
vation. Permission for the study was granted through the University of Canberra
Human Research Ethics Committee. Pseudonyms are used in this chapter to
protect the participants’ identities and those of their students.
Duration of observation
Participant Ministry Teaching grade (minutes) Class size
Knowledge
Thinking skill
Comprehension
Perspective on
critical thinking Critically
Application
Critical pedagogy
Analysis
Thinking about
the L2 Evaluation
Topic
Thinking through
the L2
Questioning
Moving learning
forward
Pedagogical
Refrain
purpose
Self-assessment
Whole Class
Addressee
Individual student
Findings
Critical thinking perspective
The sequences were analysed first to identify the critical thinking perspec-
tives embodied in the sequence: Thinking skills, criticality, and critical ped-
agogy. To some extent, thinking skills and criticality overlapped, especially
where the teacher used higher-order questioning. These sequences were
double-coded.
Overwhelmingly, the classroom sequences focused on thinking skills (78% of
the 110 sequences), with smaller numbers focusing on criticality (15%) and criti-
cal pedagogy (6%). However, the teachers varied in their use of critical thinking
questions. Two of the seven teachers, Evelyn and Daisy, did not use any questions
which could be categorized under “criticality” or “critical pedagogy”, while one
teacher in particular (Wendy) engaged her students somewhat more actively in
critical thinking even though she asked relatively few questions. For example,
we coded the following question by Wendy under “criticality” (see Appendix A
for transcription conventions):
developed into a deeper exploration of global warming and how the picture
positions viewers:
However, this exchange did not progress further, and Brenda’s questions did
not move beyond this basic exchange of information, so the opportunity for
critical pedagogy was missed (Table 5.3).
The sequences which focused on thinking skills were further categorized
to investigate what type of skills were addressed (see Table 5.4). As has been
documented by several previous studies (e.g. Inan & Finda, 2013; Shen &
Yodkhumlue, 2012; Wong, 2010), the vast majority of teachers’ questions dealt
with surface-level “knowing”; that is, they mostly focused on recall of informa-
tion or checking comprehension. In the following example, Evelyn’s class was
learning about personality traits. Students were divided into several groups, and
each group was given a list of vocabulary related to personality traits and was
asked to find the meaning in the dictionary. Afterwards, Evelyn assigned each
group to inform other groups about the meaning of the given vocabulary items.
Although the students introduced the examples, Evelyn kept tight control of the
exchange through the IRE pattern of discourse, asking for a definition and then
confirming and/or informing students about the meaning of each word:
DAISY: So simple present tense means we use it for habitual action, for daily
activity. How about for simple past?
STUDENTS: Verb2
DAISY: Ok, I want you to create a sentence. For example, “What did you do last
night?”
STUDENT 1: I watched a movie.
STUDENT 2: I slept.
STUDENT 3: I ate dinner.
STUDENT 4: I played basketball yesterday.
Further analysis of the data revealed that when questions focused on identify-
ing students’ knowledge, teachers tended to use the IRE cycle. In comparison, if
the participants’ questions were categorized as higher order, as in the example of
application above, it tended to free up the IRE cycle. In the following sequence, in
which Wendy asks her students to compare two posters, Wendy does not evaluate
the students’ answers herself as in the IRE pattern, but asks the students to analyse
the differences between the two posters, and to evaluate which one is better:
Topic
Language General
Thinking ABOUT Thinking THROUGH
Participant the L2 the L2 Total
knowledge of the language, as can be seen from the following exchange while
Adam is checking the attendance list. However, as can be seen, this “general”
content was often very mundane and strongly permeated by code-switching:
Pedagogical purpose
Moving learning
Participant forward Refrain Self-assessment Total
ALFRED: Okay here, “The sun was shining and the sky was blue”. Um, what is
the subject here?
STUDENTS: The sun
ALFRED: How many subject can you find in this?
STUDENTS: Two
ALFRED: The sun and then the? (rising intonation)
STUDENTS: The sky
ALFRED: The sky. (falling intonation)
Questions which aimed at “moving learning forward” made up only 26% of the
participants’ questioning. In some cases, questioning activities began with refrain
questioning and were then followed by attempts at moving learning forward. This
can be seen in the sequence below, in which Adam was discussing questions based
on a reading passage. First, he asked the students about the reading passage, fol-
lowing the questions in the textbook; then, abruptly, Adam tried to move learning
forward by asking individual students an authentic question about themselves:
Both Anne and Wendy were able to move learning forward without relying
on the IRE pattern. For example, in the following sequence, Anne engaged
in a one-to-one discussion with an individual student, opening up space for
the student to interact thoughtfully and ask questions of her own. Rather than
maintaining a strong position as “knower”, she engages the student in discussion
and makes suggestions rather than insisting on a correct answer. In this way, she
encourages the student to take more responsibility and to develop a more critical
disposition:
ANNE: What is the title? “Dancer everyone wants to be the king” Ini ya?
[This is it, isn’t it?] (pointing to the student’s short story, and continuing
to read),
ANNE: No capital letter. Comma.
STUDENT: In the novel I read, it’s capital.
ANNE: Masa’ sih? Hurufnya kecil, terus ini koma, bukan titik. [Really? (it’s in) lower
case, then it should be a comma, not a full stop]
STUDENT: Kan it’s end of sentence masa’ koma [It’s end of a sentence, so it cannot
be a comma]
ANNE: Tapi yang ngomong siapa? [but, who is talking?]
STUDENT: Robyn
ANNE: Iya kan? Iya [That’s right, isn’t it? Yes] Coba deh dicek lagi [Why don’t
you check again]. Kalau percakapan begitu, kasih koma juga [If it’s in a
conversation like this, you should put a comma]. Ini koma nih [This is a
comma here]
STUDENT: Gitu ya? saya gak pernah gitu kalau baca2 [Really…? I’ve never seen it
when I’m reading]
ANNE: Masa’ sih? Coba nanti kita cek lagi, tapi yang betul koma terus huruf kecil.
Di bahasa Indonesia gak diajarin? Kalau kayak gini, ini huruf besar [Really?
Let’s check again, but the correct one is lower case. Haven’t you been
taught this in Bahasa Indonesia classes? If it is like this, it should be
capital]
88 Maya Defianty and Kate Wilson
ANNE: Okay, “There was once a man so lonely, so isolated ….” (cited from
the student’s story). So do you have problem or difficulties in doing this
kind of task?
STUDENT: I think … I tried to write well, not using repeated words. I think the
hardest is finding its diction and find the suitable words for my short story.
WENDY: What’s this? What kind of information you can get from the picture?
What was the event?
STUDENTS: “Pecinta alam” (Several students answer at the same time) [“Nature
lovers”]
WENDY: Is there anyone interested in this?
STUDENTS: Not really
WENDY: So, have you heard about this event? How do you think about this
event actually?
STUDENT: Maybe …
WENDY: Why you say maybe?
(The student who said “may be” remains silent),
WENDY: They love adventure and going one forest to another … What do you
think about this event actually? It is personal interest. Ok, Sarah, what do
you think about this event?
SARAH: I like the event, but I think my parents will not let me.
WENDY: You have to obey your parents yes … this picture tell you about an
event, who is the addressee? For whom is it?
STUDENTS: All students who are interested.
Two more issues in relation to questioning became apparent during the anal-
ysis. First, teachers sometimes asked several questions in a row, making it is
Fostering critical thinking by questioning 89
difficult for students to think, let alone answer the teachers’ questions, as shown
in the sequence below:
DAISY: You can do it, look at the structure of the tense. For example, “We were
so happy”. How to say it in Bahasa Indonesia?
STUDENTS: (no answer)
DAISY: “Kami sangat bahagia.” “We are happy” and “We were happy” are the
same, sama enggak [aren’t they?]
STUDENTS: (no answer)
DAISY: In language (Bahasa Indonesia) if we did something in the past is there
any difference? What’s the difference? So in our language it’s the same, but
in English because of the time is different, the linking verb is different. The
difference is the time.
Second, an interesting finding from the study is that questioning did not
always start from teachers; occasionally, students initiated the questioning
sequences. However, unlike Anne in the sequence above, teachers sometimes cut
off students’ attempts at asking questions, by giving a minimal answer and fail-
ing to elaborate or explain, effectively blocking the student’s attempt at critical
thinking and missing the opportunity to move learning forward. For example, in
the extract below, the students ask “Can we use either: ‘has’ or ‘have’?” and the
teacher dismisses the question with “Of course not” and moves on to the next
item offering no explanation.
ADAM: Nah sekarang kita belajar [Now we are going to learn] “Be or Have”. Itu
bisa menjadi [They can become] “is, am, are”. Tika [Girl’s name] …? (rising
intonation)
STUDENTS: IS …. (in chorus)
ADAM: Oke betul [Ok that’s right], Tika IS … Terus [Then] “have” atau [or] “has”
kan memiliki dua arti ya [has two meanings, right?] Memiliki [have]. Jadi [So]
Tika “have” atau [or] “has”?
STUDENTS: have … has …
ADAM: Ada yang menjawab “have?” ada? [Did anyone answer “have”?]
STUDENTS: Bapak, boleh dua-duanya? [Sir, can we use either “have” or “has”?]
ADAM: Enggak dong, salah satu [Of course not, only one]. Number three. “Mother
and her friends … an idea to celebrate the birthday.” “Has” atau [or] “have”?
STUDENTS: have (in unison)
Discussion
The findings show that teachers in Indonesian secondary schools find multi-
ple occasions to ask their students to think critically ABOUT language. Some
teachers in the study asked questions which called for higher-order thinking
about language: Questions which required students to apply, analyse, synthesize,
and/or evaluate. In some cases, such questions provided an excellent model of
90 Maya Defianty and Kate Wilson
critical thinking which could be said to foster students’ own abilities and disposi-
tions as critical thinkers, as seen in Anne’s questions in one-to-one consultations,
prompting her students to self-assess.
However, the majority of teachers’ questions called for lower-order thinking:
Recalling information or checking comprehension. The “refrain” response is
particularly common in the data explored for this study, and it was nearly always
associated with lower-order questions. In fact, one of the teachers, Evelyn,
asked lower-order questions exclusively, 75% of which were refrain questions.
Intriguingly, other studies have found a similar proportion of lower-order ques-
tions in EFL classroom discourse: Shen and Yodkhumlue (2012) in a classroom in
China identified 79.2% lower order questions, and Wong (2010) identified 88.4%
of questions in the Hongkong classroom that she observed as either knowledge or
comprehension questions. Lower-order refrain questions serve a useful instruc-
tional purpose in recalling, reinforcing, and monitoring what students already
know; they also serve a regulative function holding the class together, providing
a familiar rhythm and routine to classroom discourse, bringing the students up
to a shared level of knowledge, and ensuring that the class are all paying atten-
tion (Christie, 2002). Such questions, as Ozuem and Lancaster (2015) point out,
can also provide an valuable opportunity for formative feedback, allowing the
teacher to monitor students’ progress and reassuring students that they are keep-
ing up. However, the effectiveness of both the instructional and regulative func-
tions of such questions can be called into question. All too often, the students in
this study who were chorusing responses to such questions were observed to be
barely focusing on the question: Present in body but perhaps not in mind. Most
importantly, these refrain questions were nearly all lower order questions which
did not stimulate critical thinking and which did not move learning forward.
Such questions are very familiar to students who have experienced the transmis-
sive style of teaching in which the teacher holds all the power and is the “primary
knower” (Wells, 2002).
In terms of thinking critically THROUGH the language, although questions
concerning general topics made up 36% of the corpus, many of these questions
were of a mundane communicative nature, like Adam’s “Are you sleepy?” At a
higher level, questions like Wendy’s “Which is better – fame or study?” could
have provoked some deeper critical reflection if they had been further pursued.
In addition, students were rarely given the encouragement or opportunity to fol-
low up with their own questions, and when they did occasionally ask questions,
these questions were sometimes ignored or blocked. In fact, the teachers in the
study missed multiple opportunities to foster critical thinking. In some instances,
teachers asked good questions but then failed to give sufficient wait time before
answering the question themselves, a problem highlighted by Wiliam and Leahy
(2015). Moreover, when students occasionally asked their own questions, these
questions were sometimes ignored or blocked.
Two classrooms were characterized by more active critical thinking. Wendy,
in particular, asked a greater proportion of higher-order questions than most
Fostering critical thinking by questioning 91
teachers, although, interestingly, she also asked fewer questions overall. The
data show that Wendy had established a different relationship with her students.
While Brenda and Adam, for example, kept tight control over their classes and
their students’ learning by using multiple IRE exchanges, Wendy was able to
interact with her students on a much more democratic basis. To some extent, this
can be explained by the high level of competence of her students, most of whom
attended private English classes outside school. In addition, she was teaching in a
class for high achievers, with a class size of only 13 students, as opposed to some
of the other teachers with classes of over 30. Like Anne, who had a class size of
35, she positioned herself more as a facilitator than as an authority, which gave
space for the students to contribute more, and more critically, both ABOUT and
THROUGH language. While the lesson content for other teachers focused on
the language presented in the textbook almost exclusively, Wendy’s class talked
about public events and even had the opportunity to interact with students
around the world via blogging and a videoconference on the internet.
It appeared that, unlike other teachers, Anne and Wendy were able to cre-
ate a culture of critical thinking in their classes and that this culture seemed to
breed further critical thinking. Both teachers had the advantage that most of
their students came from privileged families. The students’ exposure to English
was abundant: They had internet access, they had the opportunity to enrol in
out-of-school language courses, and some of them had participated in student
exchanges or been to English-speaking countries for holidays. It could be argued
that critical thinking THROUGH the language is possible only when students
have reached a certain level of competence in the language. DeWaelsche (2015),
for example, argues that Korean students can overcome cultural unfamiliarity
with critical thinking if their level of English is adequate. In addition, students
who have been exposed to wider life experiences are better placed to engage in
critical thinking.
Overall, the data show that teachers in the Indonesian classrooms that we
studied were able to use questioning to promote critical thinking both ABOUT
and THROUGH English to some extent; however, their practice could have
been much richer and more extensive had they used more questions about gen-
eral topics and had they followed through on these questions with a more critical
perspective. By asking fewer refrain questions and interacting with their students
more as facilitators than as instructors, teachers could open up space for their
students to develop into better critical thinkers.
Conclusion
In general, although this study of classroom discourse in senior schools in
Indonesia has provided some excellent examples of how questioning can lead to
critical thinking both ABOUT and THROUGH the language, it was clear that
many opportunities to foster critical thinking were lost. To some extent this may
be because of the lack of attention given to critical thinking in many teacher
92 Maya Defianty and Kate Wilson
In conclusion, the data from this study show that questions that stimulate
critical thinking ABOUT language are common in Indonesian classrooms,
although there is still a strong reliance on lower-order refrain questions as a
way of regulating the class, and teachers still miss many opportunities to foster
a critical disposition. Sadly, the Indonesian teachers in this study rarely took the
opportunity to use the language as a vehicle for critical thinking — thinking
THROUGH the language. Nevertheless, teachers who have the confidence to
reposition themselves as learners alongside their students — to step down from
the role of “sage on the stage” and become the “guide by the side” — may be
more effective in making space for their students to practice critical thinking
more actively and thus to develop what Davies and Barnett (2015) would see as a
critical disposition. Learning to ask, and to be open to, questions which stimulate
critical thinking both ABOUT and THROUGH the language is an important
step towards realizing a pedagogy for critical thinking in EFL classrooms.
References
Alexander, R. (2008). Essays on pedagogy. London: Routledge.
Bloom, B. S. (1984). Taxonomy of educational objectives. Boston, MA: Pearson.
Chamot, A., & O’Malley, J. M. (1994). The CALLA handbook: Implementing the Cognitive
Academic Language Learning Approach. Reading, MA: Addison Wesley.
Christie, F. (2002). Classroom discourse analysis: A functional perspective. London: Continuum.
Clifton, J. (2006). Facilitator talk. ELT Journal, 60(2), 142–150.
Creswell, J. W. (2005). Educational research: Planning, conducting, and evaluating quantitative and
qualitative research. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill/Prentice Hall.
Davies, M., & Barnett, R. (2015). Introduction. In M. Davies & R. Barnett (Eds.), The Palgrave
handbook of critical thinking in higher education (pp. 1–26). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
DeWaelsche, S. A. (2015). Critical thinking, questioning and student engagement in Korean
university English courses. Linguistics and Education, 32, 131–147.
Fairclough, N. (1989). Language and power. London: Longman.
Hammond, J., & Gibbons, P. (2005). Putting scaffolding to work: The contribution of
scaffolding in articulating ESL education. Prospect, 20(1), 6–30.
Li, L. (2011). Obstacles and opportunities for developing thinking through interaction in
language classrooms. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 6, 146–158.
Li, L. (2016). Integrating thinking skills in foreign language learning:What can we learn from
teachers’ perspectives? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 273–288.
Luke, A. (2002). Two takes on the critical. In B. Norton & K. Toohey (Eds.), Critical pedagogy
and language learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Oxford, R. (1990). Language learning strategies: What every teacher should know. Boston, MA:
Heinle and Heinle.
Ozuem, W., & Lancaster, G. (2015). Questioning: A path to student learning experience.
Education and Training, 57(5), 474–491. doi: 10.1108/ET-04-2014-0039
Pennycook, A. (1997). Vulgar pragmatism, critical pragmatism and EAP. English for Specific
Purposes, 16, 253–269.
Pohl, A. (2005). Language learning histories: An introduction to critical thinking. Paper presented at
the 18th Annual EA Education Conference.
Richards, J. (2017). Interchange (4th ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
94 Maya Defianty and Kate Wilson
Rubin, J., & Thompson, I. (1994). How to be a more successful language learner (2nd ed.). Boston,
MA: Heinle and Heinle.
Shen, P., & Yodkhumlue, B. (2012). A case study of teacher’s questioning and students’ critical
thinking in college EFL reading classroom. International Journal of English Linguistics, 2(1),
199–206. doi: 10.5539/ijel.v2n1p199.
Soars, J., & Soars, L. (2017). New headway (4th ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Stake, R. (2006). Multiple case study analysis. New York: Guilford Press.
Wallace, C. (2003). Critical reading in language education. Houndmills: Palgrave Macmillan.
Wells, G. (2002). The role of dialogue in activity theory. Mind, Culture and Activity, 9(1),
43–66.
Wiliam, D., & Leahy, S. (2015). Embedding formative assessment: Practical techniques for K–12
classrooms. West Palm Beach, FL: Learning Sciences International.
Wilson, K. (2016). Critical reading, critical thinking: Delicate scaffolding in English for
academic purposes (EAP). Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 256–265.
Wong, R. (2010). Classifying teacher questions in EFL classrooms: Question types and their
proper use. TESOL in Context 20(1), 37–57.
Yang, Y-T.C., & Gamble, J. (2013). Effective and practical critical thinking-enhanced EFL
instruction. ELT Journal 67(4), 398–412.
Introduction
Recent discussions on students’ ability to think critically (Atkinson, 1997)
argue that students from Asian countries have the ability to think critically,
although this can manifest itself differently from their Western counterparts
(Li & Wegerif, 2014; Matsuda & Tardy, 2007; Stapleton, 2002; Tian & Low,
2011). Some researchers argue that it is not so much lack of ability as “lack of
questing attitude … or inclinations in thinking critically” (Zin & Eng, 2014,
p. 61), or lack of opportunity in the curriculum to develop critical thinking
(CT) and make it explicit (Alagözlü, 2007). Several comparative studies about
Chinese college students conclude that they have the ability to think critically
and can perform as well as their Western counterparts (Wen & Liu, 2006; Wen
et al., 2010). Yet, after four years of university study, Chinese English majors
were left behind, in terms of critical thinking, by their counterparts in other lib-
eral arts majors. Wen and colleagues argue that this lack of further development
in thinking skills occurs because language skills are the main focus of training in
the first and second years of university study. Consequently, the high proportion
of the time spent on memorization, imitation, and translation results in a lack of
training in analytical, evaluating skills both in teaching content and pedagogy
(Ren, 2013).
Wen and Liu’s (2006) analysis of English majors’ argumentative writing
identifies weaknesses in understanding the task, in stating and structuring an
argument, and in supporting it. They propose that, to improve students’ writ-
ing ability, teachers should combine the teaching of basic thinking steps in the
process of writing with systematic training in analytical, evaluative, and logical
thinking. In China, training students’ ability to analyse, synthesize, and solve
problems from multiple perspectives was stated in the college English syllabus
96 Mei Lin and Xiaoting Xiang
When writing, the students must keep in mind their purpose, think about
the facts they will need to select which are relevant to that purpose, and
think about how to organise those facts in a coherent fashion. The process
of learning to write is largely a process of learning to think more clearly
(p. 33).
Hence, to teach CT is to teach students to “think their way through … what they
write” (Paul, 2005, p. 32).
Teaching students to read critically has been recommended as a helpful peda-
gogy to develop CT in writing (Kuek, 2010). Some empirical studies on teach-
ing critical reading at colleges, however, draw our attention to the inadequacy
of teaching mere critical reading. Mehta and Al-Mahrooqi’s (2015) findings on
the effect of critical reading on Indonesian college students’ writing performance
suggest limited transfer, and these investigators emphasize the need to view CT
as a set of teachable skills, reinforced by reflection, discussion, and drafting, and
the need to evaluate the extent to which students have been able to express them-
selves. In the same vein, Wilson (2016) argued that critical reading could be useful
if the observed Austrian teachers of critical reading in EAP courses could nurture
and scaffold students’ critical dispositions, because “such scaffolding pushes stu-
dents to develop deeper skills and criticality, yet enables them to feel secure in the
transcultural contact zones in which they are participating” (p. 264).
100 Mei Lin and Xiaoting Xiang
Methodology
A mediation-based curriculum was designed to address the following questions:
T S
Clarity Precision
Accuracy Signiicance
Relevance Completeness
Must be
Logicalness Fairness applied to
Breadth Depth
T E
Purposes Inferences
Questions Concepts
As we learn Points of view Implications
to develop Information Assumptions
I T
Intellectual Humility Intellectual Perseverance
Intellectual Autonomy Conidence in Reason
Intellectual Integrity Intellectual Empathy
Intellectual Courage Fairmindedness
FIGURE 6.1 Paul and Elder’s framework of EOT and IS (2006, p. 21 with
permission).
An integrated curriculum
An integrated curriculum (Figure 6.2) was designed adopting the following
principles:
Presentation- to
Deinition Explanation Poster-making Writing an essay
design a one-day
paragraph paragraph about climate based on a
trip as a travel
writing (why) change debating TV show
agency
Vygotsky believes that “people make sense out of what they are doing not
in advance of their activity but in their very engagement in, and reflection
on, practical-critical activity itself ” (Lantolf & Thorne, 2006, p. 89). Visual
auxiliary tools and activities were designed to engage students in learning to
thinking critically. A smiling face (see Figure 6.4) was adopted as a visual aid
for the teacher to model and explain how EOT would work in text analysis and
Purpose Question at
issue
Point of
view
Implications
Logic Clarity
Secon
d laye
Breadth
r
Secon aye
r Signi
icance
d laye dl
r on Third layer
Sec r
yer d laye
Second la Fir Secon
Se
co
nd
st
lay er
Third layer relevance
lay er ay
Third layer er tl
rs Second layer
First Fi r
layer d laye
Secon
layer Topic accuracy
Second First layer
Third layer
Third layer
Se
Fi co
layer er rs nd
Second
tl
ay tl
ay
lay
er precision
irs er
er F
Second lay
er Se
lay co
nd
nd lay
co Se
Se co er
nd
lay
er
Depth
Writing tasks were designed on Pigai.com, one of the most popular online
English writing platforms with more than twenty million users in China.
Students could modify their grammar mistakes and expression problems after
class, so that class time could be spent on the structure and content of their
writing. Pigai automatically recorded the frequency and the content of students’
revisions.
Students would give feedback in class after completion of each required
writing task. A peer feedback form based on Paul and Elder’s IS framework
was designed (Appendix 1) to guide students’ evaluation of each other’s writ-
ing, focusing on selected standards at a time. The teacher’s feedback was mainly
offered in class, commenting on students’ evaluation of peers writing with IS,
drawing attention to issues, clarifying misunderstanding of IS, and further mod-
elling of how to use IS to evaluate peers’ work. In the unit “Travel”, the teacher
noticed that in the group presentation, most groups only listed all the informa-
tion without further analysis, and that comments from other groups touched
on the standards rather superficially. The teacher used a popular debating TV
show in China to model and exemplify how to evaluate with the standards. The
chosen episode also served as a lead-in to the new writing topic “negative and
positive attitude towards life”.
Students were to complete self-reflection reports on revision (Appendix 2),
reflecting on how they evaluated peers’ and their own writing, anything they
learned in the evaluation, any improvement in understanding CT, and the iden-
tification of their next learning targets. Students could choose to writing in
English or Chinese. The teacher would read these reports to check students’
understanding and use of the concepts.
Data collections
1. A ll the students were asked to complete an evaluation questionnaire
(Appendix 3) at the end of the course, rating on a 4- or 5-point scale their
perceptions of the possible impact of the course on their CT and writing
behaviour, and offering their feedback on the teaching pedagogy. Eighty-
five percent (79 out of 93) of the students completed this questionnaire.
2. One piece of written work by students from the TT class was collected
before the course, and one piece after it, to identify any impact on their
writing. The writing topics were selected from the British Council IELTS
(International English Language Testing System) task website for writing,
and students’ writing was automatically scored by Pigai. Details of sample
writing are not presented in this chapter due to constraints on the size of
the chapter.
3. The two pieces of written work were also marked to assess students’ perfor-
mance of CT before and after the course with the use of a rubric based on
Elder and Paul’s (2008) IS with some modifications (Appendix 4).
Integrating critical thinking into EFL 105
Findings
Overall, students were very positive about the integration of teaching CT
with the writing. All 79 students said that cultivating CT was important or
very important. Sixty percent of the students replied that integrating CT skills
with English writing lessons was very important, and another 40% rated it quite
important. The majority of the students said that they had made some improve-
ment in their CT ability and writing skills, and as a result, they were more inter-
ested in learning English (Table 6.1 and Figure 6.6).
Reflections
Mind map I used to write an outline before I finished writing. Now I think
what I want to write, my purpose of writing, and I would draw
mind maps before every writing. This becomes a habit. Drawing a
mind map will make your logical thinking clearer. I will know after
I have done this what I need to do next. How can I connect the
two? In the past, there was no connection between the two
paragraphs, or very weak. (Qing)
Six thinking hats What is particularly impressive now is the hat. I feel that I can remind
myself while writing when to use which kind of thinking mode to
write. (Chen)
Standards There are many standards, and we write according to the standards. I
think it is good and it feels like it is still controlled.
Teacher and peer Sometimes when you are thinking too much you can get stuck. Peers
feedback could help you think, and can also find bigger problems in your
essay so as to make your essay more logical. Teacher can spot
problems in the use of language and logical organization of your
essay. Together they make the weaknesses of your essay explicit. Even
if you have a point of view and reasoning evidence, their feedback
gives you ideas how you can structure the essay to make it a better
piece of work.
Q10 P
ay more attention than 1.3 0 5.1 68.4 25.3 100
before to reading the
requirement of a written
task before writing
Q11. Consciously use EOT 1.3 3.8 10.1 65.8 19 100
to analyse the written
task or topic at
pre-writing stage
Q12. Consciously use EOT 1.3 3.8 13.9 65.8 15.2 100
to construct the
content of my writing
at pre-writing stage
Q13. While writing, 1.3 2.5 13.9 65.8 16.5 100
consciously use EOT
to adjust or revise the
content
Q14. While writing, 2.5 5.1 20.3 62 10.1 100
consciously use IS to
evaluate my writing
Q18. At revision stage, 2.5 7.6 15.2 68.4 6.3 100
consciously use EOT
to reflect on my
writing and extend
with additional
content
Q17. At revision stage, 1.3 5.1 13.9 73.4 6.3 100
consciously use IS to
evaluate my writing
Sequences
have standards for things. The standards are to check the things whether
they are good or not” (S19)’. “To think with purpose, to question the issues,
point of view, concepts of objective, assumptions, implications, inference, and
information” (S41).
Some students described CT as considering things from “two sides”.
“Every coin has two sides” (S47) echoes findings in other studies on Chinese col-
lege students’ understanding of CT (Chen, 2017). A comparison of the responses
to “What is CT?” reveals that the “two sides” interpretation was presented in
a more or less black-and-white way (i.e. good or bad, advantages or disadvan-
tages, positive or negative), and at the end of the course, half of them (22 out of
46) emphasized that CT considers issues from different or many perspectives, as
shown in the word cloud (Figure 6.7).
Students began to use “angles”, “aspects”, and “perspectives” rather than
“side” in describing CT.
“a way of thinking which needs or requires you to see the problem or the
things from different aspects” (S44).
“a way of thinking, including different ideas from different angles” (S11).
“think from different angles; think deeply” (S30).
“Thinking something in an objective, all round way, not only considering it
as positive or negative” (S53)
“a logical thinking which views an issue from different angles. It is to
think about an issue in various aspects and show our own thinking critically”
(S29).
FIGURE 6.7 A word cloud of students’ interpretations of CT at the end of the course.
Integrating critical thinking into EFL 111
Conceptualization of CT in writing
All 79 students said, in the final course evaluation questionnaire, that it was
important or very important to cultivate CT ability and integrate it into the
English writing course. Four students from the TT class were interviewed indi-
vidually, and their comments gave insights into their conceptualizing of CT in
writing. Pseudonyms were used.
All the four students who were interviewed talked about a shift in focus from
the use of flowery language to the argument in a piece of work. “In the past, we
were taught to use advanced vocabulary when expressing ideas. You are, like a
porter, simply stacking your essay with beautiful words or expressions. It looks
flowery, but when you sum up what was actually discussed in it, it is in fact very
shallow”. (Yu)
112 Mei Lin and Xiaoting Xiang
Echoing Yu’s focus on the use of beautiful language, Qing described in detail
changes in approaching a piece of reading. “In the past, when I looked at a piece of
work, I used to look at details. Now, I would start from looking at the purpose of
this writing; whether the writer has given his/her point of view at the beginning of
the essay; how or in what way does he/she present his/her point of view, e.g. by giv-
ing examples, through telling a story, or some data. Then I would see how the
writer reasons for his point of view, and check for the logicalness and convincing-
ness of the supporting evidence. Finally I would read the conclusion”. (Qing)
Presenting one’s point of view is not unknown to students. They learned at
high school to include three or four points of views in their writing. Most of
the time, as they recalled, their essays were full of personal opinions, and once
they presented them, the essay was considered completed and the job was done.
Attending this course made them understand the need to make their points of
view convincing.
“You need to check both positive and negative aspects of your point of view.
If the examples you give are refuted by others, what would you do? … When
presenting our views, we should understand how to argue and how to convince
others as well as ourselves. If what is written cannot convince ourselves, the essay
is useless”. (Zhu).
However, searching for convincing evidence not only requires great effort but
also can lead to mental exhaustion. As Chen recalls, when looking for support-
ing points, one has to think about the purpose of doing so, the relevance of the
points found, and possible opposite points of view. “It is very demanding and my
brain cells are almost dead”. These quotations support our argument that CT is
a reflective process and involves constant decision making.
Discussion
The positive outcomes of this study suggest that it is feasible to integrate CT
into a college English reading and writing curriculum and that Paul and Elder’s
frameworks can promote students’ CT in reading and writing. The findings
indicate that more students (60% of the respondents) shifted their foci away from
grammar and expressions to the structure and content of their writing. By the
end of this course, more students could also articulate their understanding about
CT and consciously use EOT and SI to plan and evaluate their writing. Our
findings are especially encouraging in terms of ways for teachers to implement
the new curriculum requirements and, at the same time, fulfil their teaching
objectives. Drawing from these findings, we would argue that (1) it is crucial
to match elements of CT with language teaching objectives; (2) teaching CT
involves developing students’ cognitive and metacognitive skills, and, above all,
helping them construct individualized interpretations of CT; (3) this construct is
initially mediated externally, co-constructed, and gradually internalized towards
the ideal conceptualization of CT in writing; and the development of this con-
struct is also voluntary, requires constant reflection and effort, and is nourished
through mediated learning.
Conclusions
The aim of this research is to investigate the feasibility of integrating CT into
an EFL reading and writing course and its impact on college students’ CT and
writing performance. The findings confirm that an integrated curriculum has
the potential to develop students’ CT and writing competence hand in hand. An
116 Mei Lin and Xiaoting Xiang
Acknowledgement
The authors are thankful to Min Zhou for carrying out interviews and mark-
ing essays as an independent researcher and her useful suggestions for the
curriculum.
References
Alagözlü, N. (2007). Critical thinking and voice in EFL writing. The Asian EFL Journal,
9, 118–136.
Alagözlü, N., & Süzer, S. S. (2010). Language and cognition: Is critical thinking a myth in
Turkish educational system? Procedia – Social and Behavioural Sciences, 2(2), 782–786.
Arapoff, N. (1967). Writing: A thinking process. TESOL Quarterly, 1(2), 33–39.
Atkinson, D. (1997). A critical approach to critical thinking in TESOL. TESOL Quarterly,
31(1), 71–94.
Bloom, B. S., & Krathwohl, D. R. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives: The classification of
educational goals. New York: Longmans Green.
British Council. IELTS TASK 2 writing band descriptors (public version). Retrieved from
https://takeielts.britishcouncil.org/sites/default/files/IELTS_task_2_Writing_band_
descriptors.pdf on 10 December 2016.
Chen, L. (2017). Understanding critical thinking in Chinese sociocultural contexts: A case
study in a Chinese college. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 24, 140–151.
Integrating critical thinking into EFL 117
College Foreign Language Teaching Committee. (2000). College English syllabus. Shanghai:
Shanghai Foreign Language Education Press.
Cottrell, S. (2011). Critical thinking skills: Developing effective analysis and argument (2nd ed.).
New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
Davies, M. (2015). A model of critical thinking in higher education. In M. B. Paulsen
(Ed.), Higher education: Handbook of theory and research (pp. 41–92). Switzerland: Springer
International Publishing.
DeWaelsche, S. A. (2015). Critical thinking, questioning and student engagement in Korean
university English courses. Linguistics and Education, 32, 131–147.
Djiwandono, P. I. (2013). Critical thinking skills for language students. TEFLIN Journal,
24(1), 32–47.
Dong,Y. (2015). Critical thinking in second language writing: Concept, theory and pedagogy (Doctoral
thesis).Vancouver, BC: The University of British Columbia.
Dong,Y. (2017). A rating scale for assessing critical thinking in L2 writing: Exploration and
practice. Foreign Language Education in China, 10(10), 23–29.
Dwyer, C. P., Hogan, M., & Stewart, I. (2014). An integrated critical thinking framework for
the 21st century. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 12, 43–52.
Elder, L., & Paul, R. (2008). The thinker’s guide to intellectual standards:The words that name them
and the criteria that define them. Dillon Beach, CA: The Foundation for Critical Thinking.
Ennis, R. (2011). The nature of critical thinking: An outline of critical thinking dispositions and
abilities. Retrieved from http://faculty.education.illinois.edu/rhennis/documents/
TheNatureofCriticalThinking_51711_000.pdf
Halpern, D. (2014). Thought and knowledge: An introduction to critical thinking. Milton Park,
Didcot: Taylor and Francis.
Higgins, S. (2015). A recent history of teaching thinking. In R.Wegerif, L. Li, & J. C. Kaufman
(Eds.), The Routledge international handbook of research on teaching thinking (pp. 19–28). .
Abingdon, England: Routledge.
Johnson, K. E. (2009). Second language teacher education: A sociocultural perspective (1st ed.). New
York: Routledge.
Ku, K.Y. L., Lee, V. S. L., & Ellis, J. W. (2017). Using artwork as problem context in generic
critical thinking instruction: A strategy for thoughts. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 25,
53–59.
Kuek, M. C. T. (2010). Developing critical thinking skills through integrating teaching of reading and
writing in the L2 writing classroom (PhD thesis). Newcastle University, Newcastle upon
Tyne, U.K.
Kuhn, D. (1999). A developmental model of critical thinking. Educational Researcher, 28(2),
16–25.
Lantolf, J.P., & Poehner, M.E. (2014). Sociocultural theory and the pedagogical imperative in L2
education :Vygotskian praxis and the research/practice divide. London: Routledge.
Lantolf, J. P., & Thorne, S. L. (2006). Sociocultural theory and the genesis of second language devel-
opment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Li., L. (2016). Integrating thinking skills in foreign language learning: What can we learn
from teachers’ perspectives? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 273–288.
Li, L., & Wegerif, R. (2014).What does it mean to teach thinking in China? Challenging and
developing notions of “Confucian education”. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 11, 22–32.
Liaw, M. (2007). Content-based reading and writing for critical thinking skills in an EFL
context. English Teaching and Learning, 31(2), 45-87.
Lin, M., Preston, A., Kharrufa, A., & Kong, Z. (2016). Making L2 learners’ reasoning skills
visible: The potential of Computer Supported Collaborative Learning Environments.
Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 303–322.
118 Mei Lin and Xiaoting Xiang
Matsuda, P. K., & Tardy, C. M. (2007).Voice in academic writing: The rhetorical construc-
tion of author identity in blind manuscript review. English for Specific Purposes, 26(2),
235–249.
Mehta, S. R., & Al-Mahrooqi, R. (2015). Can thinking be taught? Linking critical thinking
and writing in an EFL context. RELC Journal, 46(1), 23–36.
Mok, J. (2010). The new role of English language teachers: Developing students’ critical
thinking in Hong Kong secondary school classrooms. The Asian EFL Journal Quarterly,
12(2), 262–287.
Mosley, D., Baumfield,V., Elliot, J., Gregson, M., Higgins, S., Miller, J., & Newton, D. (2005).
Frameworks for thinking. Cambridge: CUP.
Nieto, C. H. G. (2007). Applications of Vygotskyan concept of mediation in SLA. Colombian
Applied Linguistics Journal, 9, 213–228.
Oatley, K., & Djikic, M. (2008).Writing as thinking. Review of General Psychology, 12(1), 9–27.
Paul, R. (1993). Why students and teachers don’t reason well. Retrieved from http://www.
criticalthinking.org/resources/articles/why-studentsteachers-dont-reason.shtml
Paul, R. (2005). The state of critical thinking today. New Directions for Community Colleges,
130, 27–38.
Paul, R., & Elder, L. (2006). A miniature guide to critical thinking: Concepts and tools. Billon
Beach, CA: Foundation for Critical Thinking Press.
Paul, R., & Elder, L. (2012). Critical thinking: Tools for taking charge of your learning and your life
(3rd ed.). London: Pearson.
Paul, R., Elder, L., & Bartell,T. (1997). California teacher preparation for instruction in critical think-
ing: Research findings and policy recommendations. Sacramento, CA: California Commission
on Teacher Credentialing.
Paul, R. W. (1985). Bloom’s taxonomy and critical thinking intervention. Educational
Leadership, 42(8), 36–39.
Ravenscroft, A., Wegerif, R., & Hartley, R. (2007). Reclaiming thinking: Dialectic, dialogic
and learning in the digital age. British Journal of Educational Psychology, Monograph Series,
Series II, 5, 39–57.
Ren, W. (2013). Revisiting foreign language majors’ critical thinking skills – A case study of
English public speaking course. Foreign Language China, 10(1), 10–17.
Stapleton, P. (2002). Critical thinking in Japanese L2 writing: Rethinking tired constructs.
ELT Journal, 56(3), 250–257.
Tian, J., & Low, G. D. (2011). Critical thinking and Chinese university students: A review of
the evidence. Language, Culture and Curriculum, 24(1), 61–76.
Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wen, Q. & Liu, R. (2006). An exploratory study on features in English majors’ abstract think-
ing in English argumentative compositions. Journal of Foreign Languages, 162(2), 49–58.
Wen, Q., Wang, H., Wang, J., Zhao, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2010). A comparative study of critical
thinking skills between Chinese English and other liberal arts majors. Foreign Language
Teaching & Research, 42 (5), 350–355.
Wilson, K. (2016). Critical reading, critical thinking: Delicate scaffolding in English for
academic purposes (EAP). Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 256–265.
Yang, Y. C., Gamble, J., & Tang, S. S. (2012). “Voice over instant messaging as a tool for
enhancing the oral proficiency and motivation of English-as-a-foreign-language
learners”. British Journal of Educational Technology, 43(3), 448–464.
Zin, Z. M., & Eng, W. B. (2014). Relationship between critical thinking disposition and
critical reading skills of Malaysian ESL learners. Asian EFL Journal, 16(3), 41–68.
Integrating critical thinking into EFL 119
1. You did well on …? (Name at least two things you liked or that the writer
did well in the essay.)
2. These parts have to be changed because …. (Name at least two specific
things that the writer might work on in the revision.)
3. You can improve by …. (Name at least two specific suggestions that would
help the writer revise her/his writing.)
Scale
List sentences
Standards Specific aspects 5 4 3 2 1 to improve
Clarity Demonstrate a clear
Understandable, understanding of the
free from confusion, purpose of writing
and ambiguity Clearly define the key
issues of the writing task
Clearly show the writer’s
own position on the issue
Clearly give further details or
explanations to each point
Clearly provide examples to
support
Clearly show writers’
conclusion on the issue
Accuracy Accurately explain/use the
True, free from key concepts
errors, and Accurately identify
distortions assumptions in the writing
tasks and other people’s
viewpoints
Gather credible information
Provide references in the
text when introducing
other people’s ideas
Relevance The problems imply a close
Relating to the relationship with the task
matter at hand Address all important points
of view relevant to the
writing task
Use concepts and ideas in
ways relevant to their
functions
Focus on relevant
information and present no
irrelevant information
(Continued)
120 Mei Lin and Xiaoting Xiang
Scale
List sentences
Standards Specific aspects 5 4 3 2 1 to improve
Precision The information or data
Relating to data or used to support the point of
information to be view should be as follows:
exact, not vague detailed with exact number
specific instead of broad and
abstract description
Depth Present a full understanding
Containing of the complexities
complexities inherent in the questions
and multiple Make deep rather than
interrelationships; superficial inferences
implies Imply thoroughness in
thoroughness thinking
in thinking
Breadth View the issue from
Comprehensive multiple perspectives
and broad minded Approach problems and issues
in perspective, with a richness of vision
encompassing Reason from points of view
multiple viewpoints that are significantly
different from their own
Note:
5. Consistently does all or almost all of the above.
4. Consistently does most of the above.
3. Consistently does many of the above.
2. Consistently does a few of the above.
1. Consistently does few or almost none of the above.
Writer’s response
The writer’s response
Do you agree or disagree with what your partner said? Do you think the feed-
back she/he gave will be helpful in writing your revision?
Original score on Pigai: ____ ____ Current score on Pigai: _____ ____
How did I evaluate peer’s writing?
Integrating critical thinking into EFL 121
My improvements:
Two things I have learned most about critical thinking in this writing
assignment.
My confusions/problems:
Two most important questions I would like to share with Cora and
classmates.
My goals for next writing:
What do I want to achieve in the next writing assignment?
Appendix 3: Questionnaire
Background information
1. Gender
A. Male B. Female
2. Have you received any training related to critical thinking before?
A. No B. Yes
4. After receiving one semester’s training on critical thinking, I have now paid more
attention than before to reading the requirement of a written task before writing.
A. Strongly disagree
B. Disagree
C. Not sure
D. Agree
E. Strongly agree
5. Before writing, I can now consciously use Elements of Thought to help me
analyse the writing task.
A. Strongly disagree
B. Disagree
C. Not sure
D. Agree
E. Strongly agree
6. Before writing, I can now consciously use Elements of Thought to help me
construct writing contents.
A. Strongly disagree
B. Disagree
C. Not sure
D. Agree
E. Strongly agree
7. During the writing process, I can now consciously use Elements of Thought
to help me adjust writing contents.
A. Strongly disagree
B. Disagree
C. Not sure
D. Agree
E. Strongly agree
8. During the writing process, I can now consciously use Elements of Thought
to evaluate the content of my writing.
A. Strongly disagree
B. Disagree
C. Not sure
D. Agree
E. Strongly agree
9. When giving feedback to peers’ writing, I mainly focus on language prob-
lems (grammar, vocabulary, etc.).
A. Strongly disagree
B. Disagree
124 Mei Lin and Xiaoting Xiang
C. Not sure
D. Agree
E. Strongly agree
10. When giving feedback to peers’ writing, I can use Elements of Thought as a
guide to make suggestions for revisions.
A. Strongly disagree
B. Disagree
C. Not sure
D. Agree
E. Strongly agree
12. When revising, I can now consciously use Elements of Thought to reflect on
and extend the content of my writing.
A. Strongly disagree
B. Disagree
C. Not sure
D. Agree
E. Strongly agree
13. When revising my writing, I mainly revise ______ (sequence the answers
in the order of focus)
A. Content of writing
B. Structure of writing
C. Use of grammar
D. Use of vocabulary
E. Conjunction
F. Others
Teaching evaluation
1. Gradual introduction of single to multiple Elements of Thought and critical
thinking standards is helpful for me to understand these concepts.
A. Strongly disagree
B. Disagree
C. Not sure
D. Agree
E. Strongly agree
2. Teaching from writing single paragraphs to the whole composition is helpful
for me to understand the structure of a composition.
A. Strongly disagree
B. Disagree
C. Not sure
D. Agree
E. Strongly agree
3. Teacher’s constant emphasis on the importance of the structure and content
in writing has led me to pay more attention to them.
A. Strongly disagree
B. Disagree
C. Not sure
D. Agree
E. Strongly agree
4. Different activities provided by the teacher in class (e.g. making posters by
groups, designing traveling routes, brainstorming, writing spontaneously
in class, etc.) are helpful for me to apply Elements of Thought and critical
thinking standards to my writing.
A. Strongly disagree
B. Disagree
C. Not sure
D. Agree
E. Strongly agree
126 Mei Lin and Xiaoting Xiang
5. Graphical demonstrations (e.g. mind map, writing map, etc.) used by the
teacher are helpful for me to apply Elements of Thought and critical think-
ing standards to my writing.
A. Strongly disagree
B. Disagree
C. Not sure
D. Agree
E. Strongly agree
10. Please list at least three things you have gained from teacher’s feedback (it
can be in oral or written form).
13. Please list at least three things you have gained from peer’s feedback (it can
be in oral or written form).
14. I think teacher’s encouragement of revising over and over again ______ to
improve my writing skills ________.
A. is not helpful at all
B. is not that helpful
C. not sure
D. helps to some extent
E. helps a lot
(Continued)
128 Mei Lin and Xiaoting Xiang
Sources: Elder, L., & Paul, R. (2008); British Council, IELTS TASK 2 Writing band descriptors; Cottrell,
S. (2011); and Dong,Y. (2015).
Introduction
Listening is primarily considered to be a complex cognitive process. The con-
struct listening has been explained through bottom-up and top-down informa-
tion processing and the way meaning is understood through mental activities.
The reason behind this assumption is that listening is often considered to be
synonymous with listening comprehension, and its main function in second
language learning is helping language learners to understand spoken discourse
(Richards, 2008).
Cognition, however, is not the only dimension of listening comprehension,
which involves at least two other dimensions, affective (listeners’ attitudes, moti-
vation, self-efficacy, etc.) and behavioural (verbal and nonverbal responses) com-
ponents (Worthington & Bodie, 2018). Therefore, cognitive models of teaching
listening comprehension that concentrate solely on mental aspects of listening
processes cannot guarantee listening skillfulness, and many foreign language
learners who have experienced these types of listening instruction still find lis-
tening the most difficult language skill to master. In trying to portray a successful
language learner in general and an effective listener in particular, the focus of
attention of language educationists, practitioners, and pedagogues shifted from
cognitive models of listening comprehension to metacognitive instruction in the
second half of the 20th century.
132 M. Rahimi, N. N. Kashani, and E. Soleymani
Does incorporating multimedia presentations into the cycle of cognitive and metacog-
nitive listening instructions have any effect on foreign language learners’ development
of listening comprehension?
Listening instruction
The evolution of teaching listening has coincided with the historical devel-
opment of language-teaching methodology. As a part of oral proficiency,
listening has been neglected in traditional methods of language teaching,
such as the grammar translation method. With the Reform Movement,
and as scientific attention was paid to the robust study of the sound system
of language, along with highlights from the psychology of child language
acquisition, listening entered language classes in the context of teaching con-
versation in the Direct Method and later Situational Language Teaching in
the late 19th and early 20th centuries. Through the emergence of scientific
views towards learning and teaching, theoretical bases of English Language
Teaching (ELT) were formed, and teaching language skills began to be han-
dled more systematically. From the 1940s to the 1980s more than 15 teaching
methods emerged, and listening was taught by different types of techniques
and class activities.
More careful scrutiny shows that, based on the psychological and psycho-
linguistic underpinnings, listening instruction can be modeled in at least three
different eras: 50s to 60s, 70s to 80s, and 90s to present (Goh, 2008). Vandergrift
and Goh (2012) name these three phases text-oriented listening instruction,
communication-oriented listening instruction, and learner-oriented listening
instruction, respectively.
In the first phase, text-oriented listening instruction was developed under
the influence of behaviourism and its application in language classes. Therefore,
much attention was given to language forms and written texts with the aim of
training students to listen, imitate, and memorize the stream of language, includ-
ing sounds, voices, and grammatical structures. With the decline of behaviour-
ism and its pedagogical models, most teaching activities of this phase are not used
in language classes any more. Bottom-up activities that are the central techniques
of this phase, however, are adapted into the cycle of teaching listening in the
coming phases.
Later in the 80s, communication-oriented listening instruction came into
the picture. One major improvement of listening instruction in this phase was
including pre-listening activities in the cycle of teaching listening, to help
134 M. Rahimi, N. N. Kashani, and E. Soleymani
learners comprehend the text better by linking the new information to what is
already known or experienced. The aim of the pre-listening stage is to “prepare
learners to listen by using activities that focus on the content of the text and/or
the language in the text” (Goh, 2014, p. 84), to activate listeners’ background
knowledge on the topic using different types of activities (such as brainstorm-
ing, researching, reading, viewing pictures, watching movies, and discussing),
and to present or practise linguistic forms (new vocabulary or grammatical
structures). CLT emphasizes “practicing core listening skills, such as listening for
details, listening for gist, predicting, listening selectively and making inferences”
(Goh, 2008, p. 3). The ultimate goal of teaching listening based on this model
is comprehension and interpretation of the text, and both bottom-up and top-
down processing of information are integrated into this cycle. During listening
the listener should focus on getting the gist of meaning and try to use the textual
clues in future speaking activities and other communication situations. There
might be a post-listening phase wherein listeners’ gain in comprehension would
be assessed by different types of activities.
Although this model of teaching listening has gained popularity all over the
world and in many English classes to teach listening comprehension, it is not
without problems. Vandergrift and Goh (2012) list the following challenges for
language learners (p. 9):
• Listening is often neglected in thematic lessons that integrate the four lan-
guage skills
• Listening is neglected in oral communication activities that focus more on
speaking
• Learners are indirectly assessed for comprehension
Further, this type of instruction cannot arm language learners with the long-
life listening skills required for living in an L2 society. In other words, how to
manage and self-regulate listening situations outside the classroom is not taught
very explicitly in this model. Self-regulation research shows that listeners are
required to be able to monitor their own cognitive processes while listening, to
evaluate the way they handle the listening task, to choose their own listening
tasks based on their set goals, and to keep themselves motivated to do further
listening. In this way, they become the managers of their knowledge, the masters
of their own learning, and learn how to learn.
The findings of experimental studies also supported the effectiveness of
metacognitive experiences in knowledge management and learning, and thus
cognitive psychologists and later educational psychologists became interested
in metacognition and its pedagogical implications. Following these trends, lan-
guage educationists (particularly in foreign and second language learning are-
nas) focused on integrating metacognition theory into teaching and developing
competencies in four language skills, but especially in reading and listening
comprehension.
Effect of multimedia on listening comprehension 135
1. The Coherence Principle: Any material in the multimedia that is not rel-
evant to core information and may distract the attention of the learners
should be removed from the multimedia presentation. Therefore, irrelevant
texts, pictures, and narrations should be excluded from the multimedia.
2. The Signaling Principle: The important materials should be highlighted and
should be presented in a way that they can easily and effectively attract the
attention of the learners. Effects such as underlining and highlighting can be
used in multimedia to emphasize important points.
Effect of multimedia on listening comprehension 137
Managing essential processing includes three principles that address the way
the material is presented:
1. The Segmenting Principle: Learning is achieved more deeply when the pace
at which learners can absorb it is considered and the material is not presented
as a continuous unit. This involves learners in the process of language learn-
ing and escalates learner control and more active learning.
2. The Pre-training Principle: The multimedia materials are more effective
if the learners are familiar with the key concepts of the presentation prior
to the presentation. This emphasizes the pre-phases of instruction in the
cycle of teaching language macro skills (listening, speaking, reading, and
writing).
3. The Modality Principle: The learning is maximized if the words are said
rather than written. Therefore, the spoken and written words should not be
presented at the same time. Narration of the oral input should be presented
first and then the written text.
Fostering generative processing deals with the way motivation of the learners
to pursue the learning task is addressed. This includes four principles:
Method
Participants
The participants in this study were 64 young language learners. The partic-
ipants’ English proficiency was at the level of a basic user in the Council of
Europe’s Common European Framework of Reference for Languages: Learning,
Teaching, and Assessment (2001) at the time of data collection.
The students were organized into four groups: Two experimental and two
control groups, with 16 students in each group. The students’ entry level of
listening comprehension was assessed through the Key English Test (KET) to
check the homogeneity of the groups prior to the study and choose appropriate
data analysis techniques accordingly.
The instruments
In order to assess participants’ listening abilities before and after the study, the lis-
tening section of the KET (2010) was used as the pre- and post-test. KET is part of
a group of examinations developed by Cambridge Exams in English for speakers
of other languages (ESOL), called the Cambridge Main Suite. KET is at the level
of A2 in the Council of Europe’s Common European Framework of Reference for
Languages: Learning, Teaching, and Assessment and is considered the first level of
Cambridge ESOL. The test is designed based on language in real-life situations,
and it recognizes the ability to deal with everyday written and spoken English and
assesses participants’ ability to listen and “understand the meaning of spoken English
at the word, phrase, sentence, paragraph and whole text level” (KET, 2010, p.5).
The listening section of KET has five parts and a total of 25 questions that
should be answered in 30 minutes:
The scoring system of KET is objective, and each correct item is assigned one point
based on the KET’s guideline. Each item in sections 4 and 5 were given a maximum
of two points as they required students to listen and comprehend and write one or
two words to answer the questions. KR21 was used to estimate the reliability of KET,
and the reliability index was found to be 0.76 for the pretest and 0.77 for the post-test.
Procedure
Cognitive listening instruction
Cognitive listening instruction was taught based on the cycle of pre-listening,
while-listening, and post-listening. In the pre-listening phase the students were
familiarized with the topic and theme of the listening tasks. Different types of
activities were used in this phase to activate students’ background knowledge
and/or present linguistic data (such as words and grammatical structures).
In the listening part, students were asked to listen carefully and try to under-
stand the gist of meaning.
In the post-listening phase, students’ understanding of the text and
textual interpretation were assessed by a variety of activities such as question and
answers, fill in the blanks, role-plays, etc.
Teaching materials
Audio files
Twelve listening tasks were prepared for students based on the themes of their
textbooks. The difficulty level of the tasks and their familiarity were checked by
the teacher and three colleagues.
Multimedia presentations
Twelve multimedia presentations of the listening tasks were made, considering
12 principles of multimedia design. The appropriateness of the files was evalu-
ated by the teacher and three colleagues.
Results
Hypothesis 1
In order to test Hypothesis 1 and check whether there was a signif icant
effect for multimedia presentations to develop EFL learners’ listening pro-
f iciency in a cognitive model of teaching listening (in comparison to when
technology is absent), one-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was run.
Participants’ scores on the KET pretest were used as the covariate in the
analysis.
As Table 7.2 shows, the result of ANCOVA revealed that there was a signifi-
cant difference between the two groups in the KET post-test [F(1, 29) = 64.362;
p = 0.000; partial eta squared = 0.689] in favour of the experimental group.
Accordingly, the conclusion can be drawn that cognitive listening instruction
with multimedia has caused a higher level of listening comprehension in com-
parison to cognitive listening instruction without multimedia presentations. The
descriptive statistics of experimental and control groups’ pretest and post-test are
summarized in Table 7.3.
TABLE 7.2 The result of ANCOVA to compare the effect of cognitive listening
instruction with and without multimedia on the development of listening
comprehension
Hypothesis 2
In order to test Hypothesis 2 and check whether there is a significant effect
for multimedia presentations to develop EFL learners’ listening proficiency in a
metacognitive model of teaching listening (in comparison to when technology is
absent), ANCOVA was run. Participants’ scores on the KET pretest were used as
the covariate in the analysis.
As Table 7.4 shows, the result of ANCOVA revealed that there was no sig-
nificant difference between two groups in the KET post-test [F(1, 29) = 0.395;
p = 0.535; partial eta squared = 0.013]. Accordingly, the conclusion can be
drawn that metacognitive listening instruction with and without multimedia
improved listening comprehension to almost the same extent. The descriptive
statistics of experimental and control groups’ pretest and post-test are summa-
rized in Table 7.5.
Hypothesis 3
In order to test Hypothesis 3 and check whether the development of listen-
ing prof iciency would be much higher when multimedia is incorporated
into the cycle of metacognitive instruction in comparison to the cognitive
instruction, a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was run to compare
four groups’ performance on the KET post-test. The result of ANOVA
revealed a signif icant difference between four groups of the participants
(Table 7.6).
The post hoc test to compare the means of the groups revealed significant
differences among the following groups: Metacognitive without multimedia-
Cognitive with multimedia, Metacognitive with multimedia-Cognitive with
multimedia, and Cognitive with multimedia-Cognitive without multimedia
(Table 7.7).
Examining the descriptive statistics revealed that experimental group 1, those
students who experienced cognitive listening instruction with multimedia, had
the best performance on KET post-test. The control group 1, the participants that
experienced cognitive listening instruction (without technology), ranked sec-
ond. Experimental and control groups 2, those who experienced metacognitive
Effect of multimedia on listening comprehension 145
TABLE 7.4 The result of ANCOVA to compare the effect of metacognitive listening
instruction with and without multimedia on the development of listening
comprehension
Mean Difference
(I) group (J) group (I-J) Std. Error Sig.
listening instruction with multimedia and without it, ranked third and fourth
with almost the same performance on the KET post-test (Table 7.8).
Discussion
This study was carried out with the aim of comparing the effect of cognitive
and metacognitive listening instructions integrated with multimedia learning on
EFL learners’ improvement of listening comprehension. To attain this goal, four
groups of basic language learners in an EFL context were selected and experi-
enced four types of listening instructions: Cognitive instruction with multime-
dia, cognitive instruction without multimedia, metacognitive instruction with
multimedia, and metacognitive instruction without multimedia.
The results obtained from the data analysis revealed primarily that integrating
multimedia into the cycle of cognitive listening instruction caused an increase in
the development of listening comprehension of the participants who experienced
listening instruction with multimedia in comparison to those participants who
participated in conventional cognitive listening instruction.
The finding is in agreement with studies done in other contexts and supports
the educational value of multimedia in listening instruction (e.g. Reyes Torres,
Pich Ponce, & García Pastor, 2012). As it is suggested, applying computer tech-
nologies can promote the learning interest of students (Hwang & Chang, 2011),
improve their learning achievement, and help them to become more involved
in the learning situation (Stacey & Hardy, 2011). As for listening, experiencing
listening tasks with multimedia that combines music, audio, video, image, and
graphics helps learners employ the cognitive processes that support learning from
verbal linguistic to spatial, musical, interpersonal, intrapersonal, naturalist, and
kinaesthetic (Lynch & Fleming, 2007). Therefore, memory capacity is enlarged
by allowing learners to classify multimodal input in the right channel and then
bridge the gap between the coming data and what is already saved in long-term
memory.
The findings support Paivio’s dual coding theory (1986) and Mayer’s CTML
(2009), indicating that in order not to overload the limited capacity of short
memory, it is better to use two modes in teaching simultaneously. Animation
or image (visual aids) are useful in learning language skills, especially the lis-
tening skill (Verdugo & Belmonte, 2007), by helping listeners to figure out
the main points of the audio or listening part (Kashani, Sajjadi, Sohrabi, &
Effect of multimedia on listening comprehension 147
Conclusions
The current study examined the effect of multimedia learning integrated with
two well-known L2 listening instructions — that is, comprehension-based
listening instruction and metacognitive listening instruction. The results pri-
marily supported the superiority of integrating multimedia into cognitive
instruction over a metacognitive cycle of teaching. It was also revealed that both
cognitive instructional approaches (with technology and without technology)
were more powerful practices to improve students’ listening comprehension in
comparison to metacognitive listening instruction. Further, it was found that
integrating multimedia presentations into the cycle of metacognitive instruction
cannot modify its effect, as it does significantly with a cognitive cycle, because
metacognitive listening instruction with and without technology has almost the
same effect on both experimental and control groups.
References
Ashcraft, M. H., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math
anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 130(2), 224–237.
Austin, K. (2009). Multimedia learning: Cognitive individual differences and display design
techniques predict transfer learning with multimedia learning modules. Computers &
Education, 53(4), 1339–1354.
Azevedo, R. (2005). Using hypermedia as a metacognitive tool for enhancing student learn-
ing? The role of self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 40(4), 199–209.
Baddeley, A. (2010). Working memory. Current Biology, 20(4), R136–R140 [PMC][10.1016/
j.cub.2009.12.014] [20178752].
Clark, R. C., & Mayer, R. E. (2016). E-learning and the science of instruction (4th ed.). Hoboken,
NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Flavell, J. H. (1976). Metacognitive aspects of problems solving. In: Resnick, L. B. (Ed.), The
nature of intelligence (Vol 12, pp. 231–236). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erbaum.
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring. American Psychologist, 34(10),
906–911.
Goh, C. (2008). Metacognitive instruction for second language listening development theory,
practice and research implications. RELC J. 39(2), 188–213.
Goh, C. (2014). Second language listening comprehension: Process and pedagogy. In M.
Celce-Murcia, D. Brinton, and M. A. Snow, Teaching English as a second or foreign language
(pp. 72–89). Boston, MA: Heinle Cengage Learning.
Hwang, G. J., & Chang, H. F. (2011). A formative assessment-based mobile learning approach
to improving the learning attitudes and achievements of students. Computers & Education,
56(1), 1023–1031.
Kashani, A. S., Sajjadi, S., Sohrabi, M. R., & Younespour, S. (2011). Optimizing visually-
assisted listening comprehension. Language Learning Journal, 39, 75–84.
Effect of multimedia on listening comprehension 149
Introduction
The present study was motivated by anecdotal observations and the Taiwan
government’s promotion of the importance of learning to learn skills. My
(principal author) personal learning and teaching experiences show that, in a
secondary school classroom setting in Taiwan, students often do not learn
about aspects of their own learning, such as goals, strategies, and strengths and
weaknesses. Reflection on the course of my own study in school revealed that
I learned primarily to obtain the highest possible score in the Joint Entrance
Examination. My time in school was often filled with various tests and exam-
inations, with excessive time allotted for mechanical practice to memorize the
subject content. Teacher-directed instruction was the norm, and prescriptive,
teacher-determined answers were considered the “standard” answers. Soon
after I started my teaching career, I began to grow increasingly dissatisfied with
my teaching, because the process appeared to be a reproduction of my own
school experience. My students became dependent on me for learning directions.
For example, they often asked me how to obtain higher scores on English tests,
expecting that I knew the single correct answer. The students asked me ques-
tions such as “Why do I constantly forget the vocabulary I have learned?” and
“How do I find the meaning of a text?” These questions prompted me to start
thinking about how I myself learn. I asked myself how my students would be
affected if they went through an experience similar to mine.
In 2009, the Taiwan Ministry of Education updated the curriculum
guidelines for senior high schools, with a new emphasis on logic and critical
thinking, creativity, reflection, and learners’ self-management (Ministry of
Education, Taiwan, 2009). Numerous researchers and practitioners in Taiwan
(e.g. Chen, 2012; Cheng, Yeh, & Su, 2011; Dai, 2011) have addressed the
152 S. Lin, J. Rattray, and C. Walker-Gleaves
implications of the new guidelines. Despite the emphasis on thinking skills and
the learning process in the guidelines, difficulties in applying them in practice
have been identified (Chen, 2012; Cheng et al., 2011). Furthermore, newspaper
reports (e.g. Chen, 2015) have stated that, years after the implementation of the
curriculum guidelines, secondary school students in Taiwan remain limited in
planning for, monitoring, and reflecting on their own learning practice — they
are not engaging in meta-learning behaviours. With the motivation of alleviat-
ing the aforementioned weaknesses, I conducted this research to contribute to
the principles and practices for encouraging students to become aware of how
they learn and to manage their own learning.
Taiwanese secondary school students, as described above, traditionally do not
take ownership of their learning (e.g. Lee, 2011; Tsai, 2017) in school. Therefore,
the process of becoming aware of and reflecting on their own learning is alien to
them. According to Lemke, Budka, and Gabrys (2013), students who are capable of
meta-learning are able to identify their assets and liabilities regarding the require-
ments of different learning tasks. Meta-learners can adapt themselves to various
tasks and select corresponding strategies. It seems likely that, faced with the chal-
lenges of the rapidly changing knowledge society, students equipped with a range
of cognitive skills and learning strategies are more resourceful in terms of choice
making and problem solving than their peers who passively accumulate content
knowledge (Ertmer & Newby, 1996; Rush, 2008). Thus, the discrepancy between
the long-held conventional view and the “counter-intuitive” contemporary view
must be resolved to engender a transformation in learning and teaching.
This study was conducted at a girls’ school with a history of holding sum-
mer camps for underprivileged children in remote areas of Taiwan. However, it
was not until the development and implementation of this present programme
that the concept of combining community service and learning was officially
incorporated into the school English curriculum as an elective course. This pro-
gramme involved term-time activities and a one-week service learning experi-
ence during summer vacation. The term-time activities included weekly lessons
and tasks, as well as field teaching practice sessions and observations, whereas
during the service learning experience, the students served as teachers, imple-
menting English learning activities planned for underprivileged children in
remote areas of Taiwan. The programme, developed as a way to introduce a new
pedagogical approach, aimed at encouraging secondary school students to think
about their own learning while they assumed the role of a teacher.
Theoretical framework
As outlined in the introduction, meta-learning was identified as an area for
development in Taiwanese secondary students as a counter to the predominant
approach to learning and teaching in Taiwan. Reflection, as a means of facilitat-
ing thinking about how to learn (Winters, 2013), offers a bridge between com-
plex problem-solving experience and meta-learning. The first part of this section
Designing a meta-learning programme 153
FIGURE 8.2 The study’s theoretical framework of pedagogical design (Lin, Rattray, &
Walker-Gleaves, 2017).
The programme
The participants in the programme were tenth-grade secondary school students
(aged 15–16 years) whose meta-learning capacity was examined in the context
of the school’s EFL elective curriculum. These students exhibited a minimum
proficiency in English at the Common European Framework for Languages A2
Level. Specifically, they could understand and communicate in English in the
areas of most immediate relevance. The school personnel who participated in
this project were two administrative staff members and three English teachers.
The programme was designed to encourage Taiwanese secondary school stu-
dents to think about and control how they learn, instead of simply receiving
what their teachers offer. The programme involved term-time activities and a
one-week service learning experience during summer vacation. At an orienta-
tion at the beginning of the school year, the students were provided with a clear
explanation of the course and its requirements. One assignment requirement
of the programme was for each student to maintain a reflective journal with a
partner, who responded to the journal entries and gave feedback. In the journal,
the students responded to question prompts related to the theme of each activity,
reflecting on their in-class or field experiences. In addition, the students were
exposed to different approaches to learning English each week. A movie was
used to illustrate and compare various learning strategies. Goal-setting steps,
lesson planning, assessment, and evaluation techniques were also demonstrated,
and the students then formed groups and planned and performed learning activi-
ties for children. Subsequently, the groups took turns practising teaching skills in
local churches. The teaching practice activities involved team-building exercises
in addition to teaching English lessons for children of various ages. When one
group held a teaching practice class, the other group observed and then provided
feedback during a post-practice discussion session. A list of the question prompts
posed after the term-time activities can be found in Table 8.1. After two semes-
ters of preparation, the participating students, staff members, and I began the
one-week service learning experience. During this week, the students served
as teachers, implementing their planned English-learning activities, including
songs, stories, short plays, and games. Discussions similar to the post-practice
discussions were held every evening, with the students sharing their experiences
of the day and receiving feedback from the other participants.
TABLE 8.1 Description of each term-time activity and its corresponding post-activity
prompts
First semester
TABLE 8.1 Description of each term-time activity and its corresponding post-activity
prompts (Continued)
Second semester
Nomination of a • Students compare and • Describe your role model for
role model contrast their own learning English (e.g. your classmate,
approaches to learning your brother or sister), and talk
English with those of about his/her way of learning.
their role models. • What are some similarities/
• Teacher guides the differences between your own
students to think about learning approaches and those of
the differing approaches your role model?
to learning English. • Based on what you have mentioned,
what do you think may help the
children who attend the summer
camp learn English?
In-class teaching • Students, in groups, apply • What did you learn? What is the
practice (III) what they have learned difference/relationship between the
about planning and giving new idea and what you used to
a lesson to prepare know?
English learning activities • How specifically did you learn it?
for children at church. • Why does this learning matter?
• Teacher gives feedback on • In what ways will you use this
the students’ teaching learning?
practices.
Group 1 field • One group of students For the presentation group:
teaching practice teaching the
practice activities they have • How did you approach the task?
planned for the children • What did you do well? Why do you
at church. think so?
• What could you have done better?
How are you going to improve?
• What effect did the learning
experience this week have on your
learning English?
Group 2 field • The other group of For the observation group:
teaching students observe and
practice examine their peers’ • What did you learn? What is the
teaching practices. difference/relationship between the
• Teacher gives feedback on new idea and what you used to
the students’ teaching know?
practices. • How specifically did you learn it?
• Why does this learning matter?
• In what ways will you use this
learning?
Final evaluation • Teacher guides the • What is the most impressive part of
students to review the this course?
term-time activities. • Why is what you have mentioned
• Students evaluate the above impressive to you?
term-time activities. • How has this learning experience
affected how you learn?
160 S. Lin, J. Rattray, and C. Walker-Gleaves
in-depth interviews were conducted with the students after they had held
summer camps for children in two remote areas in Taiwan. In the inter-
views, the students were asked to describe incidents that were critical to
them during the summer service learning experience, the impacts of such
incidents on their own English-learning processes, and their attitudes and
perceptions about the reflective activities. Additional questions, contingent
on the students’ responses, were posed to encourage them to expand on their
reflections.
The data collected for this study — reflective journal entries and inter-
view transcripts — were written or verbal texts. The interviews in this study
were conducted in Mandarin to facilitate clear and accurate communication.
In addition, the students were free to write their journals in either English or
Mandarin. Initially, most students voluntarily chose to write their journals in
English in order to practice the target language. However, as the school year
progressed, the students’ workloads increased and their time for writing in their
journals decreased; the students then began using their first language for ease of
expression. The linguistic challenges affecting the development of meta-learning
capacity are discussed in Lin et al. (2017).
I searched for cues related to meta-learning components in the data and
interpreted these components by adopting the coding scheme developed by
McCormick (2003) and Tarricone (2011). Tarricone (2011) developed a com-
prehensive taxonomy of metacognition that, on the basis of previous research
(e.g. Baker & Brown, 1984; Brown, 1987; Ertmer & Newby, 1996; Jacobs &
Paris, 1987; Livingston, 2003; Paris & Winograd, 1990; Schraw, 1998; Schraw
& Moshman, 1995; Wenden, 1998), categorizes the construct of metacognition
into two components: Knowledge about cognition and control over cogni-
tion. Knowledge about cognition is further divided into three subcategories:
Declarative, procedural, and conditional aspects of knowledge. Declarative
metacognitive knowledge consists of people’s understanding of themselves
and what factors influence their cognitive processing performance ( Jacobs &
Paris, 1987; McCormick, 2003; Schraw, Crippen, & Hartley, 2006). According
to Flavell (1979, 1981), this subcategory encompasses knowledge of a person
(understanding the characteristics of oneself, the similarities and differences
between oneself and other people, and the universal properties of humans in
learning); knowledge of the task (understanding the task type and demands
as well as their implications); and knowledge of the strategy (knowing what
means, processes, or actions are likely to achieve what goals in what types
of cognitive activities). The second subcategory, procedural metacognitive
knowledge, concerns knowledge about thinking procedures and how to
sequence them. The final subcategory of metacognitive knowledge pertains
to knowledge of why and when to use a specific strategy, which enables a
person to assess the demands of a particular cognitive activity and make a
flexible decision on appropriate strategies to follow ( Jacobs & Paris, 1987;
McCormick, 2003; Schraw et al., 2006).
Designing a meta-learning programme 161
Metalearning
capacity
Knowledge Control
about over
learning learning
Knowledge Knowledge
Knowledge
about about
about task
person strategy
FIGURE 8.4 Frequency of knowledge about learning versus control over learning.
FIGURE 8.5 Frequency of the three subcategories of control over learning in each
journal entry.
Designing a meta-learning programme 163
students were explicitly asked to set a goal for themselves and to decide how
to proceed with term-time activities (Figure 8.5; time points 1 and 5). By
contrast, the occurrence of the reflections categorized as evaluating peaked
at the end of the second semester, when an appraisal of students’ progress
was required. The percentages of reflections categorized as monitoring and
evaluating reached a high point when the students reflected on their final
teaching practice in the field (Figure 8.5; time point 6). The current results
supported the finding of earlier researchers, such as Schraw and Moshman
(1995), that monitoring capacity develops relatively slowly compared with
the other controlling capacities. It was not until after the service learn-
ing experience that the occurrence rate of reflections coded as monitoring
exceeded 20%.
Regarding the three subcategories of knowledge about learning (as compared in
Figure 8.6), declarative knowledge remained the dominant one. Throughout the
term, more than 80% of reflections categorized as knowledge about learning fell
into this subcategory. By contrast, the students’ procedural and decisional knowl-
edge about learning exhibited a low occurrence during the term. Teaching prac-
tices, especially those used in the field, fostered the students’ awareness of why
and when to use a specific learning strategy (Figure 8.6; time points 4 and 6).
Specifically, when the students reflected on these experiences, they became more
aware of their lack of decisional knowledge. A student reported that she “panicked”
(second semester_S17_W2) when faced with contingencies during the teach-
ing practice sessions, and other students remarked that gaining more experience
would be helpful for developing knowledge about coping with contingencies
(e.g. first semester_S04_final).
The plan created by this student drew on the decisional knowledge she gained
during field teaching practice. In addition to the planning phase, decisional
knowledge was apparent during the monitoring of task progression. The follow-
ing is an example of the students’ decisional knowledge guiding their monitoring
practices.
Analysing the interview data revealed that the percentages of response seg-
ments categorized into the subcategories of control over learning — planning (44%),
monitoring (24%), and evaluating (32%) — were relatively close. This suggested that
the service learning experience provided a platform for the students to compre-
hensively apply learning control mechanisms. During the interview, the students
were asked to recall critical incidents at the summer camp, and this discussion
was followed by questions derived from the incidents.
Before we went to teach at the camp, the other team told us they were unable to
follow their plan, so I thought we probably couldn’t either. But I think at least we
were able to carry out the most essential part of our plan. Although we printed
out the lesson plans, we sometimes forgot which activity to do in a particular lesson
because we were nervous. But we couldn’t read the plans in class. We had to immedi-
ately come up with another activity …. After practicing teaching at church, I thought
maybe we could divide the children into smaller groups and have our group members
each lead a group. Learning and competing in groups can make learning more
effective …. If the children worked in groups, they could discuss and share
their leaning together, just as we did in the evening. (S18_interview)
This student demonstrates her capacity for planning through collecting infor-
mation from experienced peers and anticipating events accordingly. Furthermore,
she planned to employ a small-group learning strategy, drawing on her own
field teaching experience at church before teaching at the service learning site.
Despite the plan, the student had to consider alternatives, because her nervous-
ness interfered with the procedures and because “the class size was not as big in
the remote-area school as at church” (S18_interview). This demonstrates her
capacity for monitoring. Finally, in the excerpt, the student commented on the
extent to which she fulfilled her plan. She also explained her rationale for prefer-
ring the group learning strategy.
In summary, evidence drawn from the analysis of interview data showed
that for the students to apply their knowledge about learning, they should assume
“responsible roles that require real-time decisions with actual consequences”
(Lizzio & Wilson, 2004, p. 472). During this innovative programme, ensuring
that the students had choices regarding how to think was essential. For example,
they had the opportunity to develop their own problem-solving approaches,
which is absent in typical English classes. Such opportunities conform to what
Stefanou, Perencevich, DiCintio, and Turner (2004) referred to as a form of
autonomy support — the provision of cognitive choices to students.
Discussion
The meta-learning programme in this study engaged the students in reflecting on
practical experiences through both inner and outer dialogues. Another principle
behind this programme was fostering a break with hierarchical student-teacher
166 S. Lin, J. Rattray, and C. Walker-Gleaves
similarity was noted between the identity of the students’ new teaching role and
that of a real teacher. According to the argument that a role is associated with a
set of behaviours and attitudes recognized by society, Robinson, Schofield, and
Steers-Wentzell (2005) suggested that assuming a teaching role might enable
students to empathize more with their teachers. Upon returning to the role of a
student, they tend to approach learning with the attitudes and behaviours they
expected from their own students, which fosters approaches conducive to learn-
ing. Students who have played the role of a teacher may employ more learning
strategies, pay more attention, and participate more actively in learning activities
(Robinson et al., 2005). For example, one student (S18) became aware that the
children she taught learned more effectively within a group. We infer that she
might be more likely to employ interdependent learning strategies in her own
learning.
Finally, one way for the teacher to scaffold the students’ awareness of their
learning was to draw analogical or contrastive links between various learn-
ing experiences. As indicated in the section “Evidence of changes in students’
meta-learning capacity during the school year” in this chapter, comparing and
contrasting the students’ approaches to learning with those of movie characters
or their role models elicited the students’ awareness of their own strengths or
weaknesses in learning and of effective learning strategies. Furthermore, some
journal questions were used as prompts to unpack the students’ perceptions
of their prior experiences of learning, and then to compare and contrast these
understandings with current tasks in the relevant class or field. The students
found that tasks at different levels of English learning, such as elementary, junior
high, and senior high, differ noticeably in complexity and in how they can be
effectively completed. In the present study, the activities and question prompts in
the journal imposed a structure that facilitated identifying connections between
past and personal learning experiences, between learning events in the present
and future, and between the individual’s own learning experiences and those
of other students. We argue that the mechanisms of comparison and contrast
prevented the students from perceiving their experiences as discrete, isolated
occurrences. Hence, sense-making strategies (e.g. looking within and consider-
ing others’ perspectives) and decision making in transfer contexts are improved
(Harkrider et al. 2013).
References
Alanen, R. (2003). A sociocultural approach to young language learners’ beliefs about
language learning. In P. Kalaja & A. M. Ferreira (Eds.), Beliefs about SLA: New research
approaches (pp. 55–85). Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic.
Baird, J. R., Fensham, P. J., Gunstone, R. F., & White, R. T. (1991). The importance of reflec-
tion in improving science teaching and learning. Journal of Research in Science Teaching,
28(2), 163–182.
Baker, L., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Metacognitive skills of reading. In P. D. Pearson, R. Barr,
& M. L. Kamil (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 353–394). New York: Longman.
Biggs, J. B. (1985). The role of meta-learning in study process. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 55(3), 185–212.
Brown, A. (1987). Metacognition, executive control, self-regulation, and other more myste-
rious mechanisms. In F. E. Weinert & R. H. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition, motivation, and
understanding (pp. 65–116). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.
Designing a meta-learning programme 169
Brown, A. L., Bransford, J. D., Ferrara, R. A., & Campione, J. C. (1983). Learning, remem-
bering, and understanding. In J. H. Flavell & E. M. Markman (Eds.), Handbook of child
psychology.Vol. 3 of cognitive development (pp. 77–166). New York: Wiley.
Chen, C. (2012). Teachers’ and students’ perceptions of the competence indicators in
National Curriculum Guidelines for Senior High School English (Master’s dissertation).
National Taiwan Normal University. Retrieved from http://handle.ncl.edu.tw/11296/
ndltd/80501338550461158108
Chen, Z. (2015, April 7). Guó gāozhōng shēng f ǎnsī bùzú jì huà nénglì ruò [Secondary
school students lack reflection and planning skills]. The United Daily News. Retrieved
from http://udn.com/news/story/6915/819728
Cheng, Y., Yeh, H., & Su, S. (2011). Planning, implementation, and expected impact of the
2010 curriculum guidelines for senior high school English:Teachers’ perspectives. English
Teaching & Learning, 35(2), 91–137.
Conner, L., & Gunstone, R. (2004). Conscious knowledge of learning: Accessing learning
strategies in a final year high school biology class. International Journal of Science Education,
26(12), 1427–1443.
Dai, M. (2011). Constructing an L2 motivational model of planned learning behavior
(PhD thesis). National Taiwan Normal University,Taipei,Taiwan. Retrieved from http://
handle.ncl.edu.tw/11296/ndltd/96675847384362446124
Daniels, H. (2001). Vygotsky and pedagogy. London: Routledge.
Desautel, D. (2009). Becoming a thinking thinker: Metacognition, self-reflection, and class-
room practice. The Teachers College Record, 111(8), 1997–2020.
Dewey, J. (1933). How we think: A restatement of the relation of reflective thinking to the educative
process. Boston, MA: D. C. Heath.
Dewey, J. (1938). Experience and education. New York: Macmillan.
Ertmer, P. A., & Newby, T. J. (1996). The expert learner: Strategic, self-regulated, and reflec-
tive. Instructional Science, 24(1), 1–24.
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-
developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10), 906–911.
Flavell, J. H. (1981). Monitoring social cognitive enterprises: Something else that may
develop in the area of social cognition. In J. H. Flavell, & L. Ross (Eds.), Social cog-
nitive development: Frontiers and possible futures (pp. 272–287). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Freire, P. (2000). Pedagogy of the oppressed (30th anniversary). New York: Continuum.
Giles, D. E., & Eyler, J. (1994). The theoretical roots of service-learning in John Dewey:
Toward a theory of service-learning. Michigan Journal of Community Service Learning,
1(1), 77–85.
Harkrider, L. N., MacDougall, A. E., Bagdasarov, Z., Johnson, J. F., Thiel, C. E., Mumford, M.
D., … Devenport, L. D. (2013). Structuring case-based ethics training: How comparing
cases and structured prompts influence training effectiveness. Ethics & Behavior, 23(3),
179–198.
Harrison, B., & Clayton, P. H. (2012). Reciprocity as a threshold concept for faculty
who are learning to teach with service-learning. Journal of Faculty Development,
26(3), 29–34.
Jacobs, J. E., & Paris, S. G. (1987). Children’s metacognition about reading: Issues in defini-
tion, measurement, and instruction. Educational Psychologist, 22(3–4), 255–278.
Jameson, J. K., Clayton, P. H., & Jaeger, A. J. (2010). Community-engaged scholarship through
mutually transformative partnerships”. In L. M. Harter, J. Hamel-Lambert, & J. Millesen
(Eds.), Participatory partnerships for social action and research (pp. 259–278). Dubuque, IA:
Kendall Hunt Publishing Company.
170 S. Lin, J. Rattray, and C. Walker-Gleaves
Shor, I. (1999). What is critical literacy? Journal for Pedagogy, Pluralism & Practice, 4(1), 1–26.
Stefanou, C. R., Perencevich, K. C., DiCintio, M., & Turner, J. C. (2004). Supporting auton-
omy in the classroom: Ways teachers encourage student decision making and ownership.
Educational Psychologist, 39(2), 97–110.
Tarricone, P. (2011). The taxonomy of metacognition. Hove, NY: Psychology Press.
Tsai, C. C. (2017). Conceptions of learning in technology-enhanced learning environments:
A review of case studies in Taiwan. Asian Association of Open Universities Journal, 12(2),
184–205.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1979). Consciousness as a problem in the psychology of behavior. Journal of
Russian and East European Psychology, 17(4), 3–35.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ward, S. C., Connolly, R., & Meyer, J. H. F. (2013).The enactment of meta-learning capacity:
Using drama to help raise students’ awareness of the self as learner. Innovations in Education
and Teaching International, 50(1), 14–24.
Wells, G. (2000). Dialogic inquiry in education: Building on the legacy of Vygotsky. In
C. D. Lee and P. Smagorinsky (Eds.), Vygotskian perspectives on literacy research (pp. 51–85).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Wenden, A. L. (1998). Metacognitive knowledge and language. Applied Linguistics, 19(4),
515–537.
West, A. (2007). Power relationships and adult resistance to children’s participation. Children,
Youth and Environment, 17(1), 123–135.
Whitebread, D., Coltman, P., Pasternak, D. P., Sangster, C., Grau,V., Bingham, S., … Demetriou,
D. (2009). The development of two observational tools for assessing metacognition and
self-regulated learning in young children. Metacognition and Learning, 4(1), 63–85.
Winters,T. (2013). A framework for facilitating meta-learning as part of subject teaching. The
Future of Education, Conference Proceedings. Retrieved from http://conference.pixel-online.
net/foe2013/common/download/Paper_pdf/166-ITL28-FP-Winters-FOE2013.pdf
Woolfolk, A., Hughes, M., & Walkup, V. (2008). Psychology in education. Harlow, England:
Pearson Longman.
9
TEFL POSTGRADUATE STUDENTS’
PERCEPTION OF CRITICAL THINKING
Conceptualizations, obstacles,
and solutions
Introduction
Outline of the study
The present chapter deals with Teaching English as a Foreign Language (TEFL)
postgraduate students’ perception of critical thinking (CT) in learning English
as a foreign language. It starts with various definitions of the concept, especially
from the two known perspectives on CT, considering CT as a cognitive skill
and regarding it as a disposition. The chapter then continues with the relation-
ship between CT and foreign language learning, which is followed by a section
entitled “Why focus on CT?”. Next, the empirical section of the study presents
and discusses TEFL postgraduate students’ conceptualizations of CT and their
perceptions of the obstacles to their CT development, as well as their suggestions
on how to improve CT skills. The chapter ends with concluding remarks and
presents some implications on how to enhance CT skills.
What is CT?
Critical thinking (CT), which is subsumed under the umbrella term of higher
order thinking skills, entails self-regulation, meaning-imposition, evaluation,
and interpretation (Li, 2016a). The higher order thinking skills include the
power of reasoning, enquiry, and analysis, CT, creative thinking, evaluation,
and complex information processing (Resnick, as cited in Li, 2016a).
As Li (2016b) maintains, although CT has been investigated extensively in
the literature of the field, “it is also a fuzzy term which is difficult to define and
describe” (p. 268). Similarly, Long (2003) maintains that CT is an elusive term to
define, which has been understood differently by various scholars and in different
cultural contexts.
Postgraduate students’ perception of CT 173
power relations (Thompson & Thompson, 2008). This dimension takes a flexible
and open-minded perspective and “acknowledges that there may be no straight-
forward “right” answers and that powerful established voices will often hold
sway over newer, alternative ways of seeing things” (Brechin, Brown, & Eby,
2000, p. Xi). Thompson and Pascal (2012) maintain that there must be an active
interaction and interrelationship between the two aspects of depth and breadth
for CT to function properly, the results of which they would call “critically
reflective practice”, which is open to new perspectives and new challenges and
also values the social context in a non-atomistic holistic manner.
In a nutshell, CT might be conceptualized as goal-directed and self-regulated
mental processes, strategies, and attempts to deeply analyse, evaluate, and
interpret the issue at hand (e.g. a learning point, problem, or situation) from
various perspectives with an open, non-dogmatic mind to make the right
decision, drawing on one’s existing knowledge base and considering the given
context.
Other experts in the field, however, have conceptualized CT in terms of
affective (disposition) factors, which are associated with the motivation and incli-
nation to use CT skills such as open-mindedness, truth-seeking, inquisitive-
ness, systematicity, maturity, and self-confidence (Ennis, 1989; Facione, 2011).
Atkinson (1997) maintains that CT is fundamentally associated with American
and Western modes of thinking and cannot be put into practice in such cultures
as Asian contexts, where it is not commonly practised and valued. For Atkinson
(1997, p. 89) “critical thinking is cultural thinking”, which means that CT is
a covert social practice that cannot be generalized beyond the contexts which
created it. Atkinson is inclined towards the disposition conceptualization of CT,
which enables one to acquire CT through experience in a culture that practises
and values it. In a similar vein, Facione (2000) believes that CT is a disposition,
attitude, or motivation that determines how CT skills are developed and applied
and maintains that CT is the “consistent internal motivation to engage problems
and make decisions” (p. 65). It could thus be argued that, in terms of disposition,
CT includes such concepts as logical thinking awareness, objectiveness, inquisi-
tiveness, and evidence-based judgement (Kusumoto, 2018).
This argument is supported by Ellwood (2000) and Vandermensbrugghe
(2004), especially the latter, who, believing in the disposition dimension of
CT, maintains that Asian learners are unable to think critically. Although
it might be true that Asian learners are somewhat less critical and reflective
thinkers than their Western counterparts, as maintained by Marzban and
Ashraafi (2016), this does not rule out teaching thinking skills in cultures and
contexts where they are not commonly known and practised (Davidson, 1998;
Marin & Halpern, 2011; Soodmand Afshar, Movassagh, & Radi Arbabi, 2017).
Therefore, while such scholars in the field as Atkinson (1997) hold that, since
CT is culture-specific and context-dependent, it cannot be explicitly taught
and transferred, such other experts as Davidson (1998) and Li (2011, 2016b)
believe that CT is teachable.
Postgraduate students’ perception of CT 175
educational system of the country in general and in the EFL education policies
and programmes in particular.
Also, although CT became a subject of second language acquisition (SLA)
research in the late 1990s following the paradigm shift from positivism to
post-positivism ( Jacob & Farrell, 2001) and the emergence of critical applied
linguistics (Pennycook, 2001), thinking skills have not received due attention
in language learning and teaching in general (Li, 2011; Pica, 2000; Soko, Oget,
Sonntag, & Khomenko, 2008).
Thus, even though CT is one of the most widely researched constructs in
education (Fisher, 2011), and despite the fact that promoting learners’ thinking
skills is of crucial importance in second/foreign language learning and teaching
(Li, 2016a), there is still a paucity of research on the barriers to, and ways of,
promoting second/foreign language learners’ higher-order thinking skills, espe-
cially those of postgraduate students of TEFL. Therefore, the following research
questions were formulated for the present study:
Method
Participants
The participants in the study included 50 postgraduate students of TEFL
(39 MA students and 11 PhD candidates) who formed three groups/classes
of PhD candidates and three groups/classes of MA students enrolled in the
department. Before the study began, the informed consent of all the partic-
ipants was obtained, and they were ensured that the results of the CT test
and their remarks in focus-group discussions would be kept confidential and
would be used only for the purposes of the present study. The participants
were also assured that the study results would not affect their final-exam
course scores.
courses out of which the PhD candidates have to take and pass maximally
18 credits of coursework and 18 credits of dissertation compilation (which is
obligatory). In both MA and PhD curricula, some credits are compulsory to
take and some are optional.
Results
As mentioned earlier, the first research question of the study set out to examine
the extent to which the participants were critical thinkers. To this end, they
completed CCTST-Form B, the results of the descriptive statistics of which are
presented here. However, to save space, the results of the descriptive statistics
of the participants’ responses to the individual items of CCTST-Form B are
Postgraduate students’ perception of CT 179
Component N F P M SD
As shown in Table 9.1, overall, the participants were able to answer less than
one-fifth (i.e. nearly 16%) of the CCTST correctly. This shows that Iranian
postgraduate students of TEFL were not high critical thinkers. As for the com-
ponents of CT, the highest and the lowest percentages of responses belonged to
evaluation (25.14%) and inference (9.62%), respectively. According to California
critical thinking skills test: User manual and resource guide (2016), evaluation
refers to the ability to assess whether the sources of information are reliable and
whether the arguments produced are strong or weak. Inference, however, refers
to the capacity to draw evidence-based conclusions and predict the consequences
of a set of propositions, conditions, and facts. Thus, it seems that the latter (i.e. infer-
ence) is comparatively more difficult to achieve and requires more higher-order
cognitive and metacognitive skills.
The second research question explored the participants’ conceptualizations
of CT. Focus-group discussions were conducted with the participants, and the
common codes and recurring themes of the discussions were extracted based on
the grounded-theory approach. The results are presented, in descending order of
frequency of occurrence, in Table 9.2.
As indicated in Table 9.2, the definition given most frequently by the majority
of the participants in all four groups was that CT was equal to “not taking things
for granted”, followed by “looking at things from various perspectives”. The other
definitions given by the participants included “questioning things”, “finding the
best solution (to the given problem) out of a variety of possible solutions and
making appropriate decisions”, “making sound judgements and strong reasoning
based on analysis”, “criticizing others”, “going beyond the framework of the
classroom”, “using more cognitive and especially more metacognitive strategies”,
and “CT being orientation-dependent” and disposition.
It thus becomes evident from Table 9.2 that the Iranian postgraduate students
of TEFL were at least partially familiar with CT theoretically. Thus, why is it
that they were not very critically minded in practice? This urged me to inves-
tigate the obstacles to CT faced by Iranian postgraduate students of TEFL
180 Hassan Soodmand Afshar
(i.e. to answer the third research question of the study). The results of this inves-
tigation are presented, in descending order, in Table 9.3.
Rank Obstacle
1 Educational system
(Teachers, curricula, syllabi, and materials; teaching and testing methods,
gap between theory and practice, etc.)
2 Time
(Lack of time for CT and CT start time being late)
3 Students’ lack of a strong knowledge base in CT, especially knowledge of
how to apply it in practice
4 Sociopolitical and cultural factors
5 Students’ lack of motivation
6 Individual characteristics and personality factors
on the part of the learners. This finding is, in fact, consistent with the results of
Soodmand Afshar and Farahani (2018), who, studying Iranian EFL teachers’ per-
ception of the inhibitors to their students’ reflective thinking (as the root of CT,
as maintained by Dewey), found the educational system (or what they termed
“learning situation inhibitors”) to be one of the main factors, among others such
as “emotional/affective” and “cognitive” inhibitors, that discouraged Iranian EFL
learners from thinking reflectively. Supporting this, one of the participants stated,
I think our educational system kills our motivation, innovation, and crea-
tivity. It is a closed system. It is a kind of dogmatic system. It doesn’t allow
you to be critical, to be innovative and creative.
Another participant stated that the educational system was confined to mem-
orizing some books and preparing for the exams.
The teachers were also blamed by the participants for the problem. It is widely
believed that the teachers usually teach the way they were taught. One of the
participants corroborated this as follows:
Another participant, putting the blame more on the authorities and the edu-
cational system itself, stated,
Curriculum designers are to blame more I think. The curriculum dictates the
teachers and us lots of routine memorization things to do. To be fair, some-
times the teachers are slaves to the educational system and the curriculum.
The above remarks and the like are corroborated by the contents of the MSRT
curricula. Analysing all the curricula of MSRT belonging to postgraduate
courses of TEFL, I found nothing explicit about the incorporation of thinking
182 Hassan Soodmand Afshar
skills, including CT, although I found sporadic mentions of such terms as anal-
ysis, survey, evaluation, etc. That is, the curricula neither aimed for the explicit
incorporation of CT skills in the materials and classes, nor did they encourage
the teachers to provide learners with some opportunities or ways to boost their
thinking skills in general and CT in particular.
Another obstacle that the participants believed impeded their thinking criti-
cally was the separation of theory from practice in the educational system. One
of the participants noting this, explained,
There is a divide between researchers in this field and the teachers because
teachers normally, er… I mean they are, in practice, dealing with students
on daily basis, but the researchers always talk from ivory towers, because
they are not really involved with the practice itself, so, sometimes, their
messages are not really well received by teachers, because they are very
idealistic probably.
Time
The second factor that reportedly prevented postgraduate students from think-
ing and acting critically was not enough time being allotted to CT. The results
of the study in this respect are corroborated by those of Soodmand Afshar and
Farahani (2018) and Moradkhani and Shirazizadeh (2017), who also found that
lack of time interfered with the participants’ efforts to develop their (reflective)
thinking skills. This is also echoed by Thompson and Pascal (2012), who believe
that development of thinking skills requires the allocation of enough time. One
of the participants, a PhD candidate, agreed:
Besides setting aside some time for developing critical and reflective thinking
abilities, the time in their studies when learners start to think critically and reflec-
tively is also of paramount importance. The educational system of the country,
Postgraduate students’ perception of CT 183
I think the teachers who are teaching at elementary schools and levels have
to be more knowledgeable and critical thinkers because at that time the
learners’ personality is shaped, not at PhD level!
The remarks of this participant and those of others show that there is a press-
ing need to start developing the critical and creative thinking skills from the very
beginning at primary schools.
Lack of knowledge
The third factor that postgraduate students of TEFL perceived as inhibiting
their efforts to think critically was lack of a strong knowledge base in CT, and
especially lack of knowledge of how to apply it in actual EFL learning envi-
ronments. This finding is partially corroborated by the results of Moradkhani
and Shirazizadeh (2017) and Soodmand Afshar and Farahani (2018), who both
found that lack of a firm knowledge base in reflection (as the root of CT) dis-
couraged Iranian EFL teachers and learners from acting reflectively. As Coyle
(2002) and Minott (2006) rightly hold, the given problem or situation is eval-
uated, analysed, and explored critically according to our existing knowledge
base. Thus, when students lack a firm knowledge base in CT, they cannot
readily analyse, evaluate, infer, and reason well. Supporting this, one of the
participants stated,
I think if more interaction with students is created in the class, they have
more opportunities to develop their critical thinking abilities.
He added,
The way we think also goes back to society. In our culture, people believe
that questioning the elders, the teachers, etc. somehow undermines their
authority. So, it is [considered a sign of ] disrespect. But, after all, questioning
broadens up the people’s minds!
The sociopolitical and cultural context in which we are engaged affects our
critical thinking. For example, in more democratic and developed socie-
ties, the people automatically grow to be critical thinkers, but in underde-
veloped and developing countries, they are not grown to be very much so.
Lack of motivation
The fifth obstacle to CT mentioned by the participants was lack of motivation
on the part of the postgraduate students of TEFL. Previous research findings
(e.g. Bartlett, 1990; Soodmand Afshar & Farahani, 2018) show that the learn-
ers’ enthusiasm, interest, and motivation can influentially involve them in var-
ious thinking processes and skills, the lack of which might potentially act as an
obstacle to CT skills development. Along the same lines, CT skills can lead to
self-motivation and self-esteem of the learners (Bolitho et al., 2003; de Villa,
2017). One of the participants, corroborating this, said,
I say to myself, I learn this and that. Well, I think critically. So what? Who
cares? If I learn it from different perspectives, so what [happens]?
The last question of the study invited the participants to suggest how to
improve CT skills. To this end, the participants’ responses were subjected to
content analysis, the results of which are presented in descending order in
Table 9.4.
Row Pattern/code
As indicated in Table 9.4, the first suggestion presented by the participants for
improving their CT skills was that a serious reform in the educational system of
the country was needed. Some Asian countries like Japan and China have already
started such efforts, but they are still lacking in Iran. As one of the participants
mentioned,
Another participant stressed the crucial role that curricula, syllabi, and mate-
rials can play:
The curricula provide food for thought. But, the curricula, materials, and
syllabi here are not challenging enough to give us enough chance or impe-
tus to think critically. The design of the curricula, materials, and syllabi
should encourage CT. They should raise students’ consciousness of CT to
enable them to think more deeply.
We are not in the habit of thinking critically. We are just passive recipients
of knowledge, so the teacher or the educational system should teach us how
to think critically.
I don’t know how to think critically, you know, or how to approach it.
Nobody has told me, you know. Maybe it has some criteria, components,
or types. It must be encouraged by teachers or, I don’t know, even directly
taught.
The second suggestion offered by the participants was the need for active
learning, participation, and interaction. Thompson and Pascal (2012), listing the
tenets of reflective thinking, mention active learning and participative learning
as two of the main features of, or prerequisites for, reflective thinking — among
others, such as blending theory with practice and challenging dogma.
One of the participants remarked,
On the one hand, it is believed that thinking skills are acquired through
interaction, participation, and negotiation, as stated earlier. On the other hand,
foreign language development seems to be an indispensable part of active par-
ticipation in today’s world, considering the place of English as an international
language (EIL). This view can also be verified by the constructivists’ view
of language, which holds that knowledge is co-constructed through the acts
of communication, interaction, negotiation, and reflection. This reasoning
could also be justified from the socio-cultural perspective, wherein learning is
defined as negotiation and collaboration rather than as acquisition of knowledge
(Yang & Miller, 2004). That is, as Li (2011, p. 147) maintains, one of the mis-
sions of language is “to develop ideas and to negotiate concepts”, which implies
that “knowledge-based teaching” should be shifted to “skills-based learning”
Postgraduate students’ perception of CT 187
I think one of the best ways to be a critical and reflective thinker is to keep
a diary to record what you read or do.
This key reform, to incorporate thinking skills into the curricula and pro-
grammes, has already taken place in various educational systems throughout the
world. However, to the best of my knowledge, this has not yet happened here in
Iran, nor perhaps in some Asian countries. Therefore, first, EFL education pol-
icy makers and curriculum developers, syllabus designers, and materials writers
especially for postgraduate programmes in TEFL are strongly urged to draw up a
comprehensive plan to incorporate critical, reflective, and creative thinking skills
into their policies, curricula, syllabi, and materials and to teach these skills explic-
itly and practically, as echoed by the participants in the study, if they intend not to
fall any more behind the international trend in other contexts as mentioned earlier.
Second, the EFL teachers would be well advised to provide an open-to-criticism,
tolerant, interactive, active, and participative atmosphere in their classes, so that the
students do not take the things the teacher says or does for granted. Rather, they
should be encouraged to participate in discussions and to freely negotiate meanings,
ideas, and questions with their classmates and teachers so that they can promote
their thinking skills through participation, negotiation, discussion, and interac-
tion. Third, the students themselves should see the need and persevere enough to
question and challenge the ideas and materials presented to them and to make use
of their power of reasoning, argumentation, analysis, inference, evaluation, and
interpretation in the language classroom and beyond, rather than remaining silent
during the whole class period. I have sometimes begged my postgraduate students
to inject their ideas and comments and/or to raise questions about the topic under
discussion, but too often I have been unable to elicit anything from some!
Acknowledgements
I would like to express my deepest thanks and gratitude to my dear postgraduate
students here at the department who kindly completed the CCTST and generously
and enthusiastically provided me with their invaluable comments and discussions. I
also thank my dear family members, my dear wife, and my lovely kids, Mahan and
Mersana, who patiently tolerated and energized and re-energized me throughout
the completion of this study. Thanks also go to my dear colleague here, Dr. Mostafa
Hosseini, who kindly provided me with the translation of Sohrab Sepehri’s work.
Last but not least, my heartfelt thanks go to Dr. Li and the publisher, Routledge,
who provided me with the opportunity to present the work.
References
Abednia, A., Hovassapian, A., Teimournezhad, S., & Ghanbari, N. (2013). Reflective journal
writing: Exploring in-service EFL teachers’ perception. System, 41, 503–514.
Akbari, R. (2007). Reflections on reflection: A critical appraisal of reflective practices in L2
teacher education. System, 35, 192–207.
Anderson, S. R. (1985). Phonology in the twentieth century: Theories of rules and theories of
representations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Atkinson, D. (1997). A critical approach to critical thinking in TESOL. TESOL Quarterly,
31, 9–37.
190 Hassan Soodmand Afshar
Bartlett, L. (1990). Teacher development through reflective teaching. In J. C. Richards & D. Nunan
(Eds.), Second language teacher education (pp. 202–214). Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Bhatia,V. K. (1993). Analysing genre: Language use in professional settings. London: Longman.
Black, B. (2012). An overview of a programme of research to support the assessment of
critical thinking. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 7(2), 122–133.
Bolitho, R., Carter, R., Hughes, R., Ivanič, R., Masuhara, H., & Tomlinson, B. (2003). Ten
questions about language awareness. ELT Journal, 57(3), 251–259.
Brechin, A., Brown, H., & Eby, M. A. (2000). Introduction. In A. Brechin, H. Brown, & M. A.
Eby (Eds.), Critical practice in health and social care (pp. ix–xv). London: Sage.
California critical thinking skills test: User manual and resource guide. (2016). San Jose, CA:
California Academic Press. Retrieved from www.insightassessment.com
Carroll, R.T. (2004). Becoming a critical thinker: A guide for the new millennium (2nd ed.). Boston,
MA: Pearson Custom Pub.
Carter, R. (2004). Language and creativity:The art of common talk. New York: Routledge.
Chamot, A. (1995). Creating a community of thinkers in the ESL/EFL classroom. TESOL
Matters, 5(5), 1–16.
Cottrell, S. (2005). Critical thinking skills. London: Pallgrave Macmillan.
Coyle, D. (2002). The case for reflective model of teacher education in fundamental principles module.
Nottingham: Nottingham University Press.
Davidson, B.W. (1998). A case for critical thinking in the English language classroom. TESOL
Quarterly, 32(1), 119–223.
de Villa, A. (2017). Critical thinking in language learning and teaching. History Research, 7(2),
73–77.
Dewey, J. (1933). How we think. Boston, MA: D. C. Heath.
Dörnyei, Z. (2007). Research methods in applied linguistics: Quantitative, qualitative, and mixed
methodologies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Ellwood, C. (2000). Dissolving and resolving cultural expectations: Socio-cultural approaches
to program development for international students. Proceedings of the national language
and academic skills conference, La Trobe University.
Emami, K. (1997). From the ups and downs of translation: Seven papers. Tehran: Niloofar
Publications.
Ennis, R. H. (1993). Critical thinking assessment. Theory into Practice, 32(3), 179–186.
Ennis, R. H. (1989). Critical thinking and subject specificity: Clarification and needed
research. Educational Researcher 18(3), 4–10.
Facione, P.A. (1992). The California Critical Thinking Skills Test and test manual. Millbrae, CA:
California Academic Press.
Facione, P. A. (2000).The disposition toward critical thinking: Its character, measurement, and
relationship to critical thinking skills. Informal Logic, 20(1), 61–84.
Facione, P. A. (2011). Critical thinking: What it is and why it counts. Millbrae, CA: California
Academic Press.
Fairclough, N. (1992). Discourse and social change. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Farley, M. J., & Elmore, P. B. (1992). The relationship of reading comprehension to critical
thinking skills, cognitive ability, and vocabulary for a sample of underachieving college
freshmen. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52(4), 921–931.
Fisher, A. (2011). Critical thinking: An introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ghonsooly, B., & Showqi, S. (2012). The effects of foreign language learning on creativity.
English Language Teaching, 5(4), 161–167.
Good, J. M., & Whang, P. A. (2002). Encouraging reflection in preservice teachers through
response journals. The Teacher Educator, 37, 254–267.
Postgraduate students’ perception of CT 191
Pica, T. (2000). Tradition and transition in English language teaching methodology. System,
29, 1–18.
Rafi, M. S. (2010). Promoting critical pedagogy in language education. International Research
Journal of Arts & Humanities (IRJAH), 37, 63–73.
Rojas-Drummond, S. M., & Mercer, N. (2003). Scaffolding the development of effective
collaboration and learning. International Journal of Educational Research, 39, 99–111.
Schön, D. A. (1983). The reflective practitioner: How professionals think in action. New York: Basic
Books.
Soko, A., Oget, D., Sonntag, M., & Khomenko, N. (2008). The development of inventive
thinking skills in the upper secondary language classroom. Thinking Skills and Creativity,
3(1), 34–46.
Soodmand Afshar, H., & Farahani, M. (2018). Inhibitors to EFL teachers’ reflective teaching
and EFL learners’ reflective thinking and the role of teaching experience and academic
degree in reflection perception. Reflective Practice, 19(1), 46–67.
Soodmand Afshar, H., & Hamzavi, R. (2014). The relationship among reflective thinking,
listening anxiety and listening comprehension of Iranian EFL learners: Does proficiency
make a difference? Issues in Language Teaching (ILT), 3(2), 237–261.
Soodmand Afshar, H., & Movassagh, H. (2017). On the relationship among critical thinking,
language learning strategy use, and university achievement of Iranian English as a foreign
language majors. The Language Learning Journal, 45(3), 382–398.
Soodmand Afshar, H., Movassagh, H., & Radi Arbabi, H. (2017).The interrelationship among
critical thinking, writing an argumentative essay in an L2 and their subskills. The Language
Learning Journal, 45(4), 419–433.
Soodmand Afshar, H., & Rahimi, M. (2014). The relationship among emotional intel-
ligence, critical thinking, and speaking ability of Iranian EFL learners. TELL, 8(1),
31–59.
Soodmand Afshar, H., & Rahimi, M. (2016). Reflective thinking, emotional intelligence,
and speaking ability of EFL learners: Is there a relation? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 19,
97–111.
Tarvin,W., & Al-Arishi, A. (1991). Rethinking communicative language teaching: Reflection
and the EFL classroom. TESOL Quarterly, 25(1), 9–27.
Thompson, N., & Pascal, E. J. (2012). Developing critically reflective practice. Reflective
Practice, 13, 311–325.
Thompson, S., & Thompson, N. (2008). The critically reflective practitioner. Hampshire: Palgrave
Macmillan.
Tsui, L. (2002). Fostering critical thinking through effective pedagogy: Evidence from four
institutional case studies. The Journal of Higher Education, 73(6), 740–763.
Tumposky, N. (1989). Foreign language learners as critical thinkers. Inquiry: Critical Thinking
Across the Disciplines, 4(4), 13–16. doi: 10.5840/inquiryctnews19894447
Vandermensbrugghe, J. (2004). The unbearable vagueness of critical thinking in the context
of the anglo-saxonisation of education. International Education Journal, 5(3), 417–422.
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wegerif, R., Mercer, N., & Dawes, L. (1999). From social interaction to individual reasoning:
An empirical investigation of a possible socio-cultural model of cognitive development.
Learning and Instruction, 9(6), 493–516. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4752(99)00013-4
Yang, R. F., & Miller, E. R. (2004). Learning as changing participation: Discourse roles in ESL
writing conferences. The Modern Language Journal, 88, 519–535.
Yang, Y. C. (2012). Cultivating critical thinkers: Exploring transfer of learning from preser-
vice teacher training to classroom practice. Teaching and Teacher Education, 28, 1116–1130.
doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2012.06.007
Postgraduate students’ perception of CT 193
You can give examples, narrate memories of your own experience of learning
as related to critical thinking, and say whatever else you think is related to the
topic and the questions mentioned.
Thank you very much for your kind cooperation and contribution in
advance.
194 Hassan Soodmand Afshar
Appendix 2
Descriptive statistics of the responses to individual items of CCTST-Form B
Item no. N F P M SD
Introduction
There is a wealth of research literature on the development of creativity in edu-
cational contexts worldwide, revealing the significance of promoting creativ-
ity in expanding learners’ opportunities to become 21st-century learners and
global citizens (e.g. Li, 2011, 2016; Wegerif, 2010, 2015; Xerri & Vassallo, 2016).
Promoting learners’ creativity has become one essential objective in EFL edu-
cation in many regions, such as China (Ministry of Education [MOE], 2011).
Research studies suggest that creativity can be developed during EFL learn-
ing and that, in return, creativity improves foreign language prof iciency
(e.g. McDonough, Crawford, & Mackey, 2015; Xerri & Vassallo, 2016). As evi-
denced in the literature, higher-order thinking (HOT) such as creative thinking
often takes place to facilitate successful communication (Mercer, 2000; Mercer
& Littleton, 2007).
Language classroom interaction is an effective and dynamic context for prac-
tising the use of the target language for social interaction (Cook, 2000; Ellies,
1999; Li, 2011). This is particularly the case in primary EFL classrooms, where
teachers facilitate learners’ learning by making pedagogical decisions and com-
municating them via instruction, building up interaction patterns, and promot-
ing different forms of talk in class (Myhill, Jones, & Hopper, 2006). Hence,
teachers’ knowledge of creativity, which is implicitly embedded in their talk, can
directly affect children’s development of creativity and eventually influence their
learning outcomes (Borg, 2006; Myhill, 2006; Myhill et al., 2006). Recognizing
the importance of creativity and the significance of the teacher’s role, and taking
mainland China as an example, this chapter sets out to explore teacher cognition
about creativity to map out teachers’ beliefs and practices of creativity that policy
makers can utilize to improve the educational community.
196 Xuying Fan and Li Li
Defining creativity
The definition of creativity is developmental because there are various lenses
to see it through, such as the psychoanalytic perspective (Sternberg & Lubart,
1999), cognitive approach (Albert & Runco, 1999; Sawyer, 2003), behaviourist
approach (Craft, Gardner, & Claxton, 2008), humanistic approach (Craft, Dugal,
Dyer, Jeffrey, & Lyons, 1997), and post-humanizing approach (Chappell, 2018).
Among the different perspectives on creativity, some perceive creativity as a trait,
assuming that a person’s creativity is a matter of personality and that divergent
thinking featuring flexibility and originality is the primary component of cre-
ativity (Ellis, 2016). Viewing creativity as product-oriented is another approach,
assuming that the creative product is both original and useful (Sternberg &
Lubart, 1999) and that the “subjectivity of feeling, and in-depth insights, [are]
often made manifest in creative products, for example in art” (Craft, 2011, p.19).
Others assume that creativity is process-oriented and thus fundamental in all
areas of life (National Advisory Committee on Creative and Cultural Education
[NACCCE], 1999). In this respect, creativity is not only a multifaceted generative
process that is not limited to the generation of novel ideas, but also a construc-
tive behaviour that produces possibilities and new embodiments of knowledge
(Knight, 2002; McGregor, 2007). It is concerned with the process in which one
makes connections with ideas to produce new understanding, new relationships,
and new perceptions of issues.
Because it is difficult to offer a precise definition of creativity, the Four C
conceptual model of creativity was introduced to articulate the nature of this
concept. Kaufman and Beghetto (2009) framed the four C’s as follows: Big-C
creativity is the remarkable contribution and giftedness of an individual (Craft,
2005); Pro-C creativity is consistent with professional expertise; little-c creativity
is everyday creativity that everyone draws on to solve challenges in daily life
(Craft, 2001; Craft, Jeffrey, & Leibling, 2001); and mini-c creativity consists of
the novel and personally meaningful interpretation of experiences, actions and
events” (Beghetto & Kaufman, 2007, p. 73). It is the “dynamic and interpretive
process of structuring personal knowledge and understanding within a particular
sociocultural context” (Kaufman & Beghetto, 2009, p. 3). In this model, mini-c
creativity yields the personal meaning-making that expands the concept of
little-c creativity (Beghetto & Kaufman, 2007).
In relation to education, the notion of little-c creativity is probably most
relevant (Craft, 2001), because it is “a seamless part of everyday curriculum”
(Beghetto & Kaufman, 2010, p. 198) and provides more general and thought-
ful insights into how learners construct and make sense of ideas interperson-
ally (NACCCE, 1999). This category stimulates creative expression for most
learners, and learning new knowledge might lead to creative practice. In foreign
language education, creativity is a strong means to motivate learners to engage
in learning, and to enable teachers to teach effectively ( Jones, 2016). To use
target language for successful communication, one needs to be flexible in order
Creativity cognition in primary EFL classrooms 197
to recontextualize the linguistic resources that one has accumulated, and to use
language to create, conform to, and transform the turns to accomplish the social
interaction (Mercer, 1995). Learners who have little competence in L2 need to
be creative in order to utilize their limited linguistic resources to adjust to the
situational and dialogic context in such a way as to make their meaning under-
stood (Ellis, 2016; Jones, 2016). This leads to the belief that creativity is not just
an individual property, but something that could be developed with others in an
open dialogic space (Wegerif, 2010). Thus, the little-c creativity approach is used
in this chapter to explore teacher cognition of creativity during classroom inter-
action, because it focuses on everyday creativity, including knowledge, technical
skills, personal insights, and attitudes. The social and interactive nature of cre-
ativity addresses socio-cultural and historical factors that influence its expres-
sion (Rudowicz, 2004). Thus, one needs to position the discussion of creativity
within a certain socio-cultural context.
In the Chinese context, it is suggested that “thinking” means reflective
thinking, which involves creativity and criticality (Li & Wegerif, 2014).
Chinese learners are encouraged to engage in deep thinking before they speak
(Li, 2015). Hence, silent engagement could be an indication that learners are
involved in deep thinking — that they are looking at a question from a holistic
point of view and weighing their ideas against those of others. This approach
is similar to the definitions of creativity given by the various research studies,
which show that the process of creative thinking involves generating multi-
ple ideas and then selecting the most useful, effective, or appropriate ideas in
order to have a workable solution to the problem (Sternberg & Lubart, 1999;
Yaqoob, 2012). Engagement of Chinese learners in inner dialogue with multi-
ple voices also seems to be beneficial in generating new and creative thoughts
(Li, 2015).
Consistent with the above definitions of creativity, we can see that originality
is the key feature in creativity. The NACCCE (1999) proposed a definition of
creativity as an imaginative activity that enables learners to produce original
and valuable outcomes. Being original can be divided into a number of differ-
ent levels. It can be the unexpected and highly original contribution that can
change the world (Sternberg, 1999), or it can consist of original possible solutions
for everyday practice. In this study, the enrichment of creativity leads to the
ability to perceive things in a new way, to recognize unforeseen problems, and
to generate unique, original, and effective solutions suitable in different con-
texts (McGregor, 2007). Therefore, it is not a randomized process of thinking:
rather, everyday creativity involves logic and making sense of real-life scenar-
ios. Thus, critical thinking is one of the features that constitute creativity. The
creative process or a creative product needs to be critically evaluated in order
to discover and examine multiple possibilities and determine what works, and
what does not work, to ensure that the creative process is appropriate to the
task at hand, and to assess its value via reflective scepticism (Brookfield, 1987;
NACCCE, 1999).
198 Xuying Fan and Li Li
(2005) found that teachers had multiple misconceptions about creativity, which
affected their attitudes toward creativity and the creative students’ behaviours.
Second, teachers’ beliefs might be either strong or less intense, depending on
socio-cultural factors such as past learning experiences, their knowledge, the
cultural context, and prevailing policy (Borg, 2006). Therefore, teacher cog-
nition is a developmental dimension in that teachers’ beliefs can be formed or
changed through their teacher education programme (Warford & Reeves, 2003;
Li, 2012). Complex beliefs about creativity and teaching practices are evident
in research studies. For instance, Mak’s research (2011) revealed the conflicting
beliefs of an EFL teacher in teaching practice, indicating that some culturally
bound beliefs might override the teacher’s teaching decisions. Forrester and Hui
(2007) revealed that learners’ development of creativity is positively related to
teachers’ creative personality and teaching techniques. One recent study con-
ducted by Li (2016) reported that creative thinking has been perceived as less
important in language classrooms than in other subjects. Other contextual fac-
tors, such as a lack of teaching time, undesirable learner performance in creative
tasks, and a tight and overloaded curriculum, have also been identified as influ-
encing teachers’ beliefs about promoting creativity (Cheng, 2010).
Research design
Informed by the aforementioned literature on the significance of teacher cog-
nition in EFL classrooms, I intend to explore and illuminate the insiders’ views
associated with teachers’ knowledge, beliefs, and teaching practices regarding
creativity in EFL classrooms. Hence, this study sets out to investigate teachers’
conceptions of creativity, their perceptions of implementing creativity and strat-
egies to do so, and how they actually promote creativity in EFL classrooms.
This exploratory case study was conducted in one state primary school in
Guangdong Province, China, and four EFL teachers participated in the study
(see Table 10.1).
Semi-structured interviews and classroom video recordings were used to pro-
vide a fuller picture of EFL teacher cognition on creativity. The interview data
were analysed by using thematic content analysis. The video-recording anal-
ysis mainly focused on identifying how teachers promote learners’ creativity
through an interactive process, using the insights of teachers’ perspectives from
their interviews, principles of dialogic teaching (Alexander, 2017), and the fea-
tures of Exploratory Talk from socio-cultural discourse analysis to identify the
“creative thinking moment” from the teacher-learner interaction (Mercer, 2004).
I have used Jefferson’s system of transcription notation to capture what was said
in these conversations, and to illuminate the details of complex interaction in
EFL classrooms ( Jefferson, 2004; see the Appendix).
Findings
Teachers’ conceptions of creativity
Although all the participants reported that they had little experience in defining
creativity, they expressed their own understanding of it. For example, Han and
Lei expressed their views:
HAN: When a learner masters the sentence structure, he can bring in other words
to make it a new sentence. This is a way to foster creativity. It is also about
flexibility.
LEI: I would also ask them to think of the words which have the same phonet-
ics, and present their ideas in class. In this way, they could develop creative
thinking.
Participant Year group Number of students Students’ age Teaching experience (years)
The bus isn’t going to the beach, what can they do? I would provide instruc-
tions to the learners and tell them that there is more than one answer to this
question. They need to have different ideas and these ideas are related to the
same topic.
Wei expected different ideas from learners, indicated that creative thinking
embraced alternatives and kept options open, and believed that evaluating, chal-
lenging, and exploring ideas are embedded in the creative process, which in similar
to critical thinking in ensuring that the answer is valuable in context (Craft, 2005).
HAN: I would spend more time on teaching linguistic knowledge than any other
aspects as it would be tested in exams.
202 Xuying Fan and Li Li
WEI: The headmistress of the school would come and talk to you if the stu-
dents’ exam results were not good enough. Given that situation, most of
the teachers would just give up teaching thinking but focus on teaching
linguistics which is tested in the exams. This is more efficient than teaching
other things.
LEI: It would be teachable only if parents show understanding and pay less atten-
tion to exam results.
Wei confirmed that teachers puzzled over designing tasks and struggled
with the preparation time. They would either give up teaching thinking skills
because these would not be evaluated in the exams, or stay up late to prepare
their teaching.
Besides this, EFL teachers’ past learning was reflected in their contrasting
beliefs about promoting creativity. Traditional learning methods such as mem-
orizing vocabularies and grammar rules were assumed to be essential for suc-
cessful foreign language learning. For example, Lei understood that creativity
was important for language learners, but her suggestion of the best way to learn
English was through learning grammar, according to her experience of being a
successful language learner and exam-taker in the past.
Only Wei maintained that the ideal way to learn English is through classroom
interaction:
Wei explained that her current teaching belief was influenced by her past
learning experience when she was taught and learned English in a traditional
way. Because being interactive in class is essential for developing children’s crea-
tivity, Wei would provide the space for children to develop their creativity; this
has also been found in her teaching practices (see Extract 1).
Creativity cognition in primary EFL classrooms 203
HAN: If one learner demonstrates an idea in front of the class, others could
observe, listen, and learn from him/her.
LEI: The low-attaining learners might not have the opportunity to express their
thoughts during interaction, but they listen and learn from those who are
more competent. I think this is a way of thinking skills’ development.
WEI: Some learners shared what they have seen or experienced in other places, and
this created an environment which allowed learners to communicate. They
used English to interact and they would reflect their thoughts. Sometimes
learners would learn new vocabulary and generate new ideas for a topic.
HAN: I often show my appreciation for students’ contributions in the class even
when the answers are incorrect because children had made their efforts in
learning and thinking.
WEI: It is a way to allow students to show their personality in class, which makes
them feel that the teacher appreciates their ideas, and their efforts are being
valued. In this way, thinking skills would be developed. Otherwise, they
would stop participating because they would assume that the teacher would
be angry at them if they replied with incorrect answers.
204 Xuying Fan and Li Li
MEI: Iwould ask them to imagine the follow-up stories of a topic as a way to
inspire them to think.
Mei defined this “follow-up stories” activity as creative teaching that allowed
learners to develop their creative thinking skills and opened up a space for them
to challenge and develop each other’s ideas. Additionally, Wei proposed that
using games in language learning is creative teaching and leads to the develop-
ment of creativity. However, creative teaching and teaching for creativity are
two different concepts, and creative teaching practice would not necessarily pro-
mote children’s creativity (see Extract 2).
13. It is crying. OK, nice idea, what else? You try ((Invite S4))
14. S4: The Earth was beautiful before, but it’s ugly now.
15. T: Yes, it was beautiful before, but it’s ugly now. NAME
16. S5: The ((/ðə/)) Earth is
17. T: The ((/ði:/)) Earth
18. S5: The((/ði:/)) Earth is::
19. Ss: was/was/was ((noise))
20. S5: (9.2) The Earth was strong before, it’s weak now.
21. T: Nice idea, I like it. Yes, strong and:: weak 现在很虚弱很脆弱 ((it’s
very weak now)). OK.
22. Strong and weak. ((Unclear))The Earth one two go
23. Ss: The Earth was strong before, but it is weak now.
24. T: Any more? 刚才的同学讲得非常好 ((very good answers from S5)).
Imagination OK.
25. 想象力 ((Chinese translation)).
First, the notion of little-c is applicable here because the creative expressions
that learners shared in the class or in their groups came from their individual
imaginations (Beghetto & Kaufman, 2010). Creativity is process-oriented in that
learners construct the meaning by connecting what they know (the adjectives
and the tenses) to produce new and imaginative ideas, and co-construction of
meaning could be taking place during discussion. In this episode, Wei embraced
different ideas by inviting learners to present their imaginative responses and
encouraged other learners’ willingness to contribute (lines 4–20), which was
consistent with her conception of creativity in the interview. Wei perceived
using metaphor as a way of developing imagination because the teacher provided
feedback on all the responses learners produced that were imaginative (lines 24
and 25), which potentially led to creativity enrichment.
Second, the teacher created time and space for promoting learners’ creativity
in this episode. For one thing, the teacher provided space and time for them to
discuss with their partners the different appearances of the Earth from two pic-
tures (lines 1–3). This is evident in the use of antonyms in describing the pictures
(lines 5, 7, and 11). Learners’ responses suggested creativity was promoted, espe-
cially when using metaphors such as crying, ugly, and weak (lines 11, 15, and 21)
to describe the Earth. These metaphors effectively related the abstract concept of
the status of the Earth to their everyday experience as human beings. Metaphor
is not just about words, it is about thoughts; and language is an important source
of evidence that reflects our thoughts (Lakoff & Johnson, 1980). In other words,
learners used English as a tool to describe an abstract situation by mapping with
their concrete experiences, such as human emotions. These imaginative thoughts
were reasonable because students had generated them through in-depth think-
ing, through contrasting and comparing skills, and through making associations
with their previous experiences (NACCCE, 1999), in order to fit into the current
context. The language they produced was creative and appropriate for this task.
206 Xuying Fan and Li Li
Peer correction and the increase of wait time were used to facilitate stu-
dents’ creativity and language development. A grammar error was found by the
rest of the students (line 19), when S5 was trying to present his idea through
English (line 18). Wei did not provide a direct corrective repair; instead, she
waited for 9.2 seconds to allow S5 to think and generate the right answer.
S5 continued his turn with consideration of the peer feedback and presented
his idea (line 20). Thus, the increase of wait time facilitated the learner’s lan-
guage and creativity development, because S5 provided a logical and imagi-
native response that demonstrated his creativity and linguistic knowledge. To
go further, Wei also showed her tolerance in S5’s incorrect answer that “is” is
missing in his response (line 20). This behaviour echoed her belief in showing
her appreciation for learners’ efforts in learning and thinking. To encourage
learner participation in class, she provided positive feedback on S5’s contribu-
tion (lines 21 and 24).
Although learners were actively engaged and provided imaginative responses,
there are still some potential moments that could have further developed their
creativity and language learning. The feedback Wei provided was repetition of
the learners’ responses (lines 6, 8, 14, and 21), which did not expand learners’
responses into creative thoughts, as she suggested in her interview; instead, she
provided her own explanation (line 9). In line 11, S3 proposed a creative idea
which was grammatically correct. However, the teacher replaced crying with sad
in her feedback (line 12). This indicated that the teacher emphasized the sen-
tence structure by using antonym opposites, although she repeated crying as in
the present continuous tense as a new sentence (line 13). This limited the space
for learners to develop their creativity, because children would receive a message
of producing the right linguistic knowledge. It was obvious that Wei welcomed
more than one possible answer, to help learners develop their imagination and
linguistic knowledge. However, without effective feedback, the quality of the
active participations in an interactive environment cannot be guaranteed (Smith
& Higgins, 2006). This could reduce the opportunities for promoting learn-
ers’ creativity and thus limit their learning potential. Overall, the above results
show that creative thinking is a developmental ability, which can be fostered
through foreign language learning. Learners were encouraged to generate personal
responses in order to develop creativity in the EFL class.
The following extract demonstrates the teacher’s understanding of creative
teaching. In this lesson, children were learning body parts, and they were asked
to create a stationery man from the stationery items supplied by the teacher.
Extract 2 (Han, Year 4)
1. T: Children, we can make a fruit man and we also can make a stationery
2. man. Mr. Stationery. OK.
3. T: Stationeries, can you make a (5.4)
4. Ss:((Whispering))
5. T: What can be the head?
Creativity cognition in primary EFL classrooms 207
6. S:Pencil
7. T: Pencil can be the head?
8. S:Yes/Yes
9. T: OK. Body, body. The book can be the body. OK.
10. Ss: Ruler/ruler
11. T: OK. I have a Mrs. Stationery.
12. Ss: Wow…
13. T: See? What is this one? The book is his body. OK. and the ruler↑::are
14. his [legs
15. Ss: [legs
16. T:The erasers are his::[feet
17. Ss: [Feet/foot/feet
18. T:Feet
19. T: OK. The paper is his head. OK. 不是全部用到, 选择合适的就可以, 有一
20. 些创意 ((You don’t have to use all the stationery, pick those
21. that are suitable and have some creativity)). OK. You can use the
22. rulers, the reading glass. OK. If you have time, make a stationery
23. man like this.
It was clearly stated (lines 19 and 20) that this task was aimed at helping
learners to develop their creativity. The nature of the designed task reflected
the component of playfulness and imagination in that the children were to be
given opportunities to explore the stationery items, and to use their imagination
to create their own stationery man with the given stationery. In relation to EFL
learning, this task could encourage children to engage in this playful activity
that stimulates them to associate stationery items with body parts that they have
learned in order to generate the meaningful English expressions. However, there
is little evidence in this extract that the learners had developed creativity.
First, the children did not have a chance to elaborate their creative thoughts
because the teacher dominated most of the talk. In line 3, the teacher waited
for 5.4 seconds and did not invite any children to express their ideas, although
they were whispering about the task (line 4). Instead of encouraging creative
responses, Han asked the children (lines 5 and 7) to guess, with short answers
(lines 6 and 8). In lines 9 and 11, the teacher proposed her own idea again and
ignored other learners’ contributions (line 10). Second, this task placed more
emphasis on vocabulary and sentence structure than on promoting creativity as
the teacher claimed (lines 19–22). For example, from lines 13 to 19, the teacher
was trying to check learners’ understanding of the vocabulary by demonstrating
the whole sentence herself. The learners did not have the space to produce the
creative ideas nor to practise the sentence structure themselves. Therefore, there
is an indication that Han might have mistakenly assumed that creative teaching
necessarily leads to creativity enrichment. In fact, this task has the potential to
promote learners’ creativity, but her insufficient pedagogical understanding has
hindered her teaching.
208 Xuying Fan and Li Li
Discussion
Fragmented content knowledge about creativity has been found, which is sim-
ilar to Li’s (2016) research on thinking skills in the Chinese EFL secondary
context. Some teachers defined creativity similar to the literature, as a process
for the active use of creative imagination, the initiation of new possibilities, the
expression of original ideas, and making connections to what is already known
to construct new embodiments of knowledge (Knight, 2002; McGregor, 2007;
Craft, 2015). Creativity has also been perceived as a regenerative process, lead-
ing to the reconstruction of existing knowledge where the characteristics of
originality or newness were missing. Craft (2005) points out that the Eastern
perspective entails the reinterpretation of traditional ideas, which contrasts
with the Western perspective of celebrating creativity. However, Li (2015)
argues that reviewing the old knowledge is desired for respecting the tradi-
tion, and it is also for learners to critique the old knowledge and discover new
knowledge from it. Hence, both Eastern and Western approaches to creativity
emphasize newness and originality. Alternatively, Mullet et al. (2016) reported
that in most of the studies they reviewed, teachers’ perceptions of creativity
were based on subject-specific experiences. Therefore, one possible reason for
defining creativity in relation to the reproduction of knowledge could be the
teachers’ subject-specific experience: They themselves may have been taught
that successful language learning is about memorizing vocabulary and gaining
linguistic knowledge, especially in exam-oriented education systems, such as
China’s (Sargent, 2015).
Conflicting beliefs regarding the promotion of creativity in the EFL class
were found due to the exam-oriented system and teachers’ knowledge of crea-
tivity. Overall, teachers realized the importance of integrating creativity in their
teaching, which contradicts Li’s (2016) finding that creative thinking skills were
ranked as the least important skills by EFL teachers. EFL teachers’ insufficient
knowledge of both content and the pedagogy of creativity was discovered, which
echoes the findings of some empirical research studies (Aljughaiman & Mowrer-
Reynolds, 2005; Stapleton, 2011; Li, 2016) that support confidence in promoting
creativity in class. Additionally, the exam-oriented system has heavily influenced
teachers’ decisions in promoting creativity and has created a washback effect on
EFL teaching and learning (Bailey, 1996; Messick, 1996). This points out the
need to investigate the discrepancy between curriculum and assessment, because
the objective of developing creativity is not tested, and thus there is a failure to
determine how effectively learners have mastered this objective (Brown, 1995).
This is not to suggest that creativity should be tested; rather, it is to argue that
test developers should be encouraged to design indirect measures of the construct
in accordance with the curriculum objectives in regards to creativity (Muñoz &
Álvarez, 2010).
Providing feedback (Smith & Higgins, 2006; Diaz-Ducca, 2014), the use of
wait time (Ingram & Elliott, 2016), and creative teaching methods ( Jeffrey &
210 Xuying Fan and Li Li
Craft, 2004) are the ways of promoting creativity that were identified during
the classroom interaction. The use of wait time shows that teachers can influ-
ence the nature of a classroom discussion by manipulating silence (Ingram &
Elliott, 2016). Learners used the silence as a “thinking time” to restructure
the sentence and successfully delivered the creative idea, which reflected their
use of inner reflection (Li, 2015). This also shows that the appropriate use of
wait time is important in facilitating learners’ development of higher cog-
nitive levels (Tobin, 1987). However, Ingram and Elliot (2016) argue that
“the extended pauses can be interpreted as a signal of trouble, which can lead
to participants self-selecting in order to initiate a repair” (p. 50). Therefore, a
closer examination is needed to analyse in what ways teachers could manip-
ulate the wait time or silence appropriately — in combination with other
features in classroom discourse — to promote learners’ learning and creativity
in the EFL context.
It is evident in this study that “creative teaching is not the same as teaching
to develop creativity” (Starko, 2014, pp. 19 and 20). Part of teachers’ creative
teaching seems to echo the suggestion that it is teacher-oriented (Cremin,
2009), through using an imaginative approach in order to make learning
interesting and fun (NACCCE, 1999). Teaching for creativity implies the
empowerment of learners ( Jeffrey & Craft, 2004), yet learners have not been
empowered because they did not voice their creation. The use of creative
teaching in this study indicates that teachers might perceive a lesson as
“a series of plannable mini-episodes”. Without clear guidance for defining and
teaching creativity, creative teaching becomes an exercise in blind faith that
might lead to ignorance of techniques that teachers could use for creativity
enrichment.
Conclusion
To conclude, this study has contributed to the understanding and richness of
teachers’ conceptions, beliefs, and practice of creativity in the EFL context.
Constrained by fragmented content and pedagogical knowledge, as well as
by other contextual factors, teachers’ reversed their positive attitudes towards
promoting creativity. Teachers’ fragmented understanding of creativity influ-
enced their teaching practices, yet effective feedback and increased wait time
have been found useful in promoting learners’ creativity and language devel-
opment. Therefore, more teacher professional development (TPD) in this field
needs to be established, including teacher training for both pre-service teachers
and in-service teachers. Al-Thani (2010) reported that teachers differed signif-
icantly in using creative thinking skills according to their qualifications and
in-service training. A “multilayer concept” of creativity needs to be brought
forward for the “pedagogical consideration” (Li, 2016, p.285) because teachers’
pedagogical knowledge affects their teaching practices (Borg, 2006; Mok, 2010).
For policy makers, the discrepancies between curriculum and practices need to
Creativity cognition in primary EFL classrooms 211
References
Al-Thani, T. (2010). Investigating teachers’ practices of creative thinking skills in Qatari
preschools. The International Journal of Learning, 17(5), 221–230.
Albert, R. S., & Runco, M. A. (1999). A history of research on creativity. In R. J. Sternberg
(Ed.). Handbook of creativity (pp. 16–31). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Alexander, R. J. (2017). Towards dialogic teaching: Rethinking classroom talk. New York: Dialogos.
Aljughaiman, A., & Mowrer-Reynolds, E. (2005). Teachers’ conceptions of creativity and
creative students. Journal of Creative Behavior, 39(1), 17–34.
Bailey, K. M. (1996). Washback in Language Testing. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
Beghetto, R. A., & Kaufman, J. C. (2007). Toward a broader conception of creativity: A case
for “mini-c” creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 1(2), 73–79.
Beghetto, R. A., & Kaufman, J. C. (2010). Broadening conceptions of creativity in the
classroom. In R. A. Beghetto & J. C. Kaufman (Ed.). Nurturing creativity in the classroom
(pp. 191–205). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
212 Xuying Fan and Li Li
Borg, S. (2003). Teacher cognition in language teaching: A review of research on what lan-
guage teachers think, know, believe, and do. Language Teaching, 36(2), 81–109.
Borg, S. (2006). Teacher cognition and language education: Research and practice. London: Bloomsbury.
Brookfield, S. D. (1987). Developing critical thinkers: Challenging adults to explore alternative ways
of thinking and acting. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Brown, J. D. (1995). The element of language curriculum. New York: Heinle & Heinle.
Carter, R. (2004). Language and creativity:The art of common talk. London: Routledge.
Chappell, K. A. (2018). From wise humanising creativity to (post-humanising) creativity.
(online). [accessed 7 December 2018]. Available from: https://link.springer.com/chapter/
10.1007%2F978-3-319-96725-7_13#citeas
Cheng,V. M.Y. (2010). Tensions and dilemmas of teachers in creativity reform in a Chinese
context. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 5(3), 120–137.
Cook, G. (2000). Language play, language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Craft, A. (2001). An analysis of research and literature on creativity in education. Report prepared for
the Qualification and Curriculum Authority. London: QCA.
Craft, A. (2005). Creativity in schools:Tensions and dilemmas. Abingdon: Routledge.
Craft, A. (2011). Creativity and education futures: Learning in a digital age. Staffordshire:Westview
House.
Craft, A. (2015). Possibility thinking. In R. Wegerif, L. Li, & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.). The
Routledge international handbook of research on teaching thinking (pp. 346–375). London:
Taylor and Francis Group.
Craft, A., Dugal, J., Dyer, G., Jeffrey, B., & Lyons, T. (1997). Can you teach creativity?
Nottingham: Anna Craft and Contributors.
Craft, A., Jeffrey, B., & Leibling, M. (2001). Creativity in Education. London: Continuum.
Craft, A., Gardner, H., & Claxton, G. (2008). Nurturing creativity, wisdom and trusteeship in
education: Exploring the role of education. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.
Cremin, T. (2009). Creative teachers and creative teaching. In A. Wilson (Ed.). Creativity in
primary education:Theory and Practice (pp. 36–46, 2nd ed.). Exeter: Learning Matters.
Diaz-Ducca, J. A. (2014). Positive oral encouragement in the EFL classroom, a case study
through action research. Revista de Lenguas Modernas, 21, 325–346.
Ellies, R. (1999). Learning a second language through interaction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Ellis, M. E. (2006). Language learning experience as a contributor to ESOL teacher cognition
(online). [accessed on 7th December 2018]. Available from: http://www.tesl-ej.org/
wordpress/issues/volume10/ej37/ej37a3/
Ellis, R. (2016). Creativity and language learning. In R. H. Jones & J. C. Richards (Eds.).
Creativity in language teaching: Perspectives from research and practice (pp. 32–48). Abingdon:
Routledge.
Forrester,V., & Hui, A. (2007). Creativity in the Hong Kong classroom: What is the contextual
practice? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 2(1), 30–38.
Higgins, S. (2015). A Recent History of Teaching Thinking. In R. Wegerif, L. Li & J.C.
Kaufman (Eds). The Routledge International Handbook of Research on Teaching Thinking
(pp.19-28). Aingdon: Routledge.
Hung, Y. W., & Higgins, S. (2015). Learners’ use of communication strategies in text-based
and video-based synchronous computer-mediated communication environments:
Opportunities for language learning. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 29(5), 901–924.
İnceçay, G. (2010).The role of teacher talk in young learners’ language process. Procedia – Social
and Behavioral Sciences, 2(2), 277–281.
Ingram, J., & Elliott, V. (2016). A critical analysis of the role of wait time in classroom
interactions and the effects on student and teacher interactional behaviours. Cambridge
Journal of Education, 46(1), 37–53.
Creativity cognition in primary EFL classrooms 213
Myhill, D. (2006). Talk, talk, talk: Teaching and learning in whole class discourse. Research
Papers in Education, 21(1), 19–41.
Myhill, D., Jones, S., & Hopper, R. (2006). Talking, listening, learning: Effective talk in the primary
classroom. Berkshire: Open University Press.
National Advisory Committee on Creative and Cultural Education [NACCCE]. (1999).
All our futures: Creativity, culture and education. (online) [accessed on 7 December 2018].
Available from http://sirkenrobinson.com/pdf/allourfutures.pdf
Pajares, M. F. (1992). Teachers’ beliefs and educational research: Cleaning up a messy
construct. Review of Educational Research, 62(3), 307–332.
Richards, J. C., & Rodgers, T. S. (2014). Approaches and methods in language teaching (3rd ed.).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Rudowicz, E. (2004). Creativity among Chinese people: Beyond Eastern perspective. In
S. Lau, A. Hui, & G. Ng (Eds.). Creativity. when East meets West (pp. 55–86). London:
World Scientific Publishing.
Sargent, T. C. (2015). Professional learning communities and the diffusion of pedagogi-
cal innovation in the Chinese education system. Comparative Education Review, 59(1),
102–132.
Sawyer, R. K. (2003). Mergence in creativity and development. In R. K. Sawyer,V. John-Steiner,
S. Moran, R. J. Sternberg, D. H. Feldman, J. Nakamura, & M. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.).
Creativity and development (pp. 12–60). New York: Oxford University Press.
Sinclair J. M., & Coulthard, R. M. (1975). Towards an analysis of discourse: The English used by
teachers and pupils. London: Oxford University Press.
Smith, H., & Higgins, S. (2006). Opening classroom interaction:The importance of feedback.
Cambridge Journal of Education, 36(4), 485–502.
Stapleton, P. (2011). A survey of attitudes towards critical thinking among Hong Kong
secondary school teachers: Implications for policy change. Thinking Skills and Creativity,
6(1), 14–23.
Starko, A. J. (2014). Creativity in the classroom: Schools of curious delight. New York: Routledge.
Sternberg, R. J. (1999). Handbook of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1999). The concept of creativity: Prospects and paradigms. In
R. J. Sternberg (Ed.). Handbook of creativity (pp. 3–15). Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Sun, D. (2012). “Everything goes smoothly”: A case study of an immigrant Chinese language
teacher’s personal practical knowledge. Teaching and Teacher Education, 28(5), 760–767.
Tagg, C. (2012). Scraping the barrel with a shower of social misfits: Everyday creativity in text
messaging. Applied Linguistics, 34(4), 480–500.
Tobin, K. (1987). The role of wait time in higher cognitive level learning. Review of Educational
Research, 57(1), 69–95.
Tsai, K. C. (2012). Play, imagination, and creativity: A brief literature review. Journal of
Education and Learning, 1(2), 15–20.
Walsh, C., Chappell, K., & Craft, A. (2017). A co-creativity theoretical framework to foster
and evaluate the presence of wise humanising creativity in virtual learning environments
(VLEs). Thinking Skills and Creativity, 24, 228–241.
Walsh, S. (2002). Construction or obstruction: Teacher talk and learner involvement in the
EFL classroom. Language Teaching Research, 6(1), 3–23.
Warford, M. K., & Reeves, J. (2003). Falling into it: Novice TESOL teacher thinking. Teachers
and Teaching, 9(1), 47–65.
Wegerif, R. (2010). Mind expanding: Teaching for thinking and creativity in primary education.
Berkshire: Open University Press.
Creativity cognition in primary EFL classrooms 215
(( )) Contextual information
T Teacher
S Unidentified speaker
S1, S2, S3… Identified speaker
Ss Several speakers speak at the same time
NAME Name of a student
↑ Rising tone
↓ Falling tone
[ Overlapping utterance
= No break or gap between the speech
(2.8) Waiting time. The number indicates the length of the elapsed time in seconds
:: Prolongation
/italics/ Phonetics
yes/yes/no Simultaneous speech by more than one person
INDEX
analogical, 10, 167 cognitive skills, 8, 17, 25–26, 32, 61, 96,
arguments, 1, 3, 95, 103, 111–12, 129, 111, 152, 172–73, 175, 180
166–67, 173–74, 177, 179 comprehension, 10, 18, 46, 76, 80, 114,
assessment, formative, 60, 67 134–35
attitudes: bicultural, 27–29; complexity- conceptions, 8, 11, 30, 200–201, 204–5,
seeking, 8, 27, 29–31; open-minded, 208, 210
17, 27, 29–30 conceptualizations, 10, 29, 97–98, 101,
116, 172, 179, 188
Barnett, 75–77, 80, 93 contexts, cultural, 51, 92, 172, 184, 199
beliefs, 10–11, 22, 48, 65, 96, 135, 166, creative attitudes, 20–21, 27
173, 195, 197–200, 208–9 creative teaching, 11, 203–4,
bicultural, 8, 17, 20, 27–28, 31 206–7, 210
biculturalism, 8, 17, 28, 31 creative thinking, 3, 6, 11, 23, 27–28,
bilingualism, 6, 8, 17, 21–26, 28, 31–32, 31–32, 172, 195, 198–201, 204, 206,
34; effect of, 22–23, 26 208, 211; process of, 27, 197–98
bilingualism and creativity, 8, 17, creative thinking skills, 8, 17, 20, 24–25,
23, 27, 29 27–28, 30, 32, 183, 189, 209–10
Bloom’s taxonomy, 49, 75, 78, 80 creativity, 1–8, 10–12, 17, 19, 23–27,
29, 31, 151, 181, 188, 195–97,
California Critical Thinking Skills Test, 199–211; developing, 7–8, 11, 21, 209;
10, 176 development of, 2, 6–7, 24, 28, 195,
classroom discourse, 78, 90–91, 210 199, 202–4, 206, 211; knowledge of,
classroom interaction, 185, 197, 199, 195, 198, 209; perceptions of, 198, 211;
202–3, 210 practices of, 195, 210; promoting, 195,
climates, creative, 17–18, 21 199, 201–4, 207–10; teacher cognition
CLT (communicative language teaching), of, 195, 197–98
134, 139 creativity enrichment, 197, 203, 205,
cognition, 131, 135, 147, 160 207, 210
cognitive flexibility, 7, 25–27, 30 critical thinkers, 10, 79, 90, 97,
cognitive listening instruction, 10, 141, 100, 175–76, 178, 183–84,
143–44, 146–48 187–88, 212
cognitive processes, 4, 134–36, critical thinking questions, 75, 81, 92
138, 146 critical thinking skills test, 177, 179
Index 217
CT (critical thinking), 3–5, 10, 19, language classrooms, 12, 92, 189, 199
42, 74–77, 79–81, 88–93, 95–102, language competencies, 43, 52, 55
104–6, 108–16, 172–77, 179–88, language development, 3–4, 45, 206, 210
197, 201, 214 language proficiency, 2, 23, 25, 74, 100
Cummins, 22–23, 45, 53 language skills, 42, 95, 131, 134, 139
learning environments, 46, 68, 198
design, instructional, 8, 42, 46–47, 53 learning processes, 11, 27, 152–53,
Dewey, 154, 173, 181 164, 166
dispositions, 42, 90, 97–98, 116, 172, 174, Li, 1–6, 8–10, 12, 59, 74, 77–78, 92, 96,
179; critical, 4, 43, 46, 76, 80, 82, 87, 172–76, 186, 189, 195, 197–99, 203,
92–93, 99 209–10
divergent thinking, 7, 55, 196 linguistic knowledge, 201, 206, 208–9
playfulness, 12, 20, 198, 207 teaching thinking, 12, 43, 97, 114,
primary EFL classrooms, 10, 195 116, 202
principles, 8, 10, 46–49, 54, 136–37, 143, teaching thinking skills, 10, 12, 96,
155, 165, 177, 200 174, 202
problem solving, 1, 6, 23, 115, 135, 142, technology, 4, 8, 54, 69, 98, 139,
152, 173, 201 143–44, 148
processing, 136, 147, 153 thinking, logical, 95, 107, 110
thinking dispositions, 9, 42, 45,
reasoning, 3, 100, 172–73, 183, 185–87, 55, 96, 114
189, 203 thinking process, 4, 99, 184, 203
reflections, 58, 60, 62, 68, 70, 99, 102, thinking skills, 1, 8–9, 11, 45, 59, 61–62,
107, 151, 153–56, 160, 162–63, 167–68, 74, 78, 82, 96, 175–76, 181–89, 203,
183, 186 209, 211; higher-order, 1–2, 59, 176,
185, 187; reflective, 98, 173, 175, 181,
Sawyer, 6–7, 196 185–86, 197, 203
scaffolding, 42, 44, 46, 49, 54, 99–100, thinking skills and creativity, 3–4, 8, 11
132, 142, 199 thinking skills pedagogies, 58–61, 63,
scaffolding thinking, 8, 42–45, 55 68–70, 73
socio-cultural contexts, 5, 7–9, 197
Sternberg, 6, 197 Vandergrift, 132–35, 141
Visible thinking, 48
teacher cognition, 10, 195, 197–200, 211 Vygotsky, 6, 44, 46–47, 59, 98, 100, 102,
teacher feedback, 9, 102, 106 155, 187
teacher professional development (TPD),
69, 210 Wegerif, 1, 5, 12, 98, 173, 175, 195,
teachers, creative, 7, 54 197–98
teaching creativity, 198, 208, 210 working memory, 25, 136, 147