Language Learning Strategy Instruction Current Issues and Research
Language Learning Strategy Instruction Current Issues and Research
This chapter begins with definitions and an overview of methods used to identify
learners’ strategies, then summarizes what we have learned from the large number of
descriptive studies of strategies reported by language learners. Research on
language learning strategies has a history of only about thirty years, and much of this
history has been sporadic. The 1980s and early 1990s were a period of substantial
research on language learning strategies, much of it descriptive. This period was
followed by an apparent loss of interest in language learning strategies, judging by
limited reported research and few related conference presentations. Recently,
however, a number of new investigations have reinvigorated the field. The focus of
the chapter is on the evolution of research on language learning strategy intervention
studies, the issues that have emerged from this research, and metacognitive models
that can be useful in the language classroom. The discussion concludes by setting
out directions for future research.
112
learners. However, until relatively recently there have been fewer studies focusing
on the second goal of trying to teach language learning strategies in classroom
settings.
Learning strategies are sensitive to the learning context and to the learner’s
internal processing preferences. If learners perceive, for example, that a task like
vocabulary learning requires correct matching of a new word to its definition within
a specified period of time (as in a test), they will likely decide to use a memorization
strategy. Their choice of which memorization strategy to use will depend on their
understanding of their own learning processes and on which strategies have been
successful in the past (Hsiao, 2004). A different task, such as being able to discuss
the theme of a short story will require strategies different from memorization—such
as making inferences about the author’s intended meaning and applying the learner’s
prior knowledge about the topic. The interpretation of a language learning task is
closely related to the goals advocated within each learner’s cultural context, for a
learning strategy valued in one culture may be deemed inappropriate in another
(Olivares-Cuhat, 2002; Wharton, 2000). A particular learning strategy can help a
learner in a certain context achieve learning goals that the learner deems important,
whereas other learning strategies may not be useful for that learning goal.
Interviews
pausing as necessary, asking the student to describe his or her thoughts at specific
moments during the learning task.
Questionnaires
Written diaries and journals have also been used to identify language
learners’ strategies. In these, learners write personal observations about their own
learning experiences and the ways in which they attempted to solve language
problems (see, for example, Carson & Longhini, 2002). Rubin (2003) suggests using
diaries for instructional purposes to help students develop metacognitive awareness
of their own learning processes and strategies. An interesting variant on the diary
study was recently conducted by Takeuchi (2003), who examined published books
and essays by Japanese good language learners of various languages and analyzed
each author for evidence of learning strategy use included in their descriptions of
their foreign language learning histories.
Think-Aloud Protocols
evaluating (see Chamot & Keatley, 2003; Cohen et al., 1998; O’Malley & Chamot,
1990).
Although self-report may be inaccurate if learners do not report truthfully or
cannot remember their thinking, it is still the only way available to us to develop
some understanding of learners’ mental processing. As Grenfell and Harris have
pointed out: “It is not easy to get inside the ‘black box’ of the human brain and find
out what is going on there. We work with what we can get, which, despite the
limitations, provides food for thought” (1999, p. 54)
Language learning strategies research began in the 1970s with the seminal
work of Joan Rubin, who, like Stern (1975), suggested that a model of “the good
language learner” could be constructed by looking at special strategies used by
successful L2 students (Rubin, 1975). Other researchers followed with descriptions
of learner characteristics and strategic techniques associated with effective second
and foreign language learning (Naiman, Fröhlich, Stern, & Todesco, 1978/1996;
O’Malley, & Chamot, 1990). More recently, Takeuchi (2003) identified the
characteristics of Japanese good language learners through their biographies. Taken
together, these studies identified the good language learner as one who is a mentally
active learner, monitors language comprehension and production, practices
communicating in the language, makes use of prior linguistic and general knowledge,
uses various memorization techniques, and asks questions for clarification.
Later studies comparing more and less effective language students have
revealed a recurring finding that less successful learners do use learning strategies,
sometimes even as frequently as more successful peers, but that their strategies are
used differently (Chamot & El-Dinary, 1999; Khaldieh, 2000; Vandergrift, 1997a,
1997b). A recent study by Vandergrift (2003a) compared the listening
comprehension strategies of more- and less-skilled Canadian seventh-grade students
of French. Students listened to several French texts and were prompted to think
aloud during the process. The more skilled listeners used more metacognitive
strategies, especially comprehension monitoring, than did their less skilled peers. In
addition, more skilled listeners engaged in questioning for clarification, whereas the
less skilled used more translation. Graham (2004) investigated the attitudes toward
learning French of upper secondary English students and found that the less
successful students did not seem to be aware of the potential role of learning
strategies in improving their language performance.
These studies have confirmed that good language learners are skilled at
matching strategies to the task they were working on, whereas less successful
language learners apparently do not have the metacognitive knowledge about task
requirements needed to select appropriate strategies. This trend is apparent with
children in foreign language immersion classrooms, secondary school ESL and
foreign language students, and adult language learners (Chamot & El-Dinary, 1999;
Chamot & Keatley, 2003). In addition, more proficient L2 learners use sequences of
strategies to complete a task effectively (Chamot, Barnhardt, El-Dinary, & Robbins,
1999; Goh, 2002b; Oxford et al., 2004).
three different types of tasks, and their performance was compared to that of students
in a nonstrategies control group (O’Malley & Chamot, 1990). This study was
conducted with 75 high school ESL students randomly assigned to experimental or
control groups. Students were pretested on three types of language tasks—
vocabulary, listening comprehension, and speaking from prepared notes—but not on
their use of learning strategies. The experimental group students were taught various
strategies for the same types of tasks over a two week period. The instruction was
provided by the researchers, all of whom had ESL teaching experience. Students
were posttested on the same types of tasks, but did not report on their use of learning
strategies. The main conclusions of this first language learning strategies
experimental study were as follows:
• Vocabulary learning strategies were effective only for students who had not
already developed alternative effective strategies.
• Listening comprehension improved for students instructed in learning strategies
on texts that were accessible, not on those that were too difficult and/or for
which students lacked relevant prior knowledge.
• Oral reports (presented from written notes) given by strategy-instructed students
were judged to be significantly more comprehensible and organized than those
of control group students.
• Explicit learning strategy instruction embedded within the language syllabus
appeared to be effective.
More recently, Ozeki (2000) followed the example of Ross and Rost (1991)
by first identifying the listening strategies students already used as a basis for
selecting strategies to be taught. In this case, however, the strategies to be taught
were those students had reported that they used least frequently. Although intact
classes of students of English in a Japanese women’s college were used for the
treatment and control groups, randomization was achieved by the assignment of
students to class sections alphabetically by surname. Strategy instruction was
provided in the treatment class during 12 ninety-minute classes focusing on listening
comprehension distributed over a 20-week semester. The sequence of instruction
was as follows: a preparation stage in which students were explicitly taught a new
strategy and earlier strategies were reviewed; and a lesson stage in which students
practiced the strategies with listening comprehension tasks. Pretest and posttest
scores were compared to evaluate the effects of learning strategy instruction.
Improvement in the treatment group was noted in the following dimensions:
development of listening comprehension ability; increased use of learning strategies
(including some not explicitly taught); positive attitudes towards strategy instruction;
transfer of strategies to new tasks; and durability of strategy use after the completion
of strategy instruction.
teachers provided initial strategy instruction in the students’ L1, then asked students
to use the same strategies when reading in English. The remaining eight teachers
attempted to teach the strategies only in English. Data from classroom observations
and from end-of-year individual think-aloud interviews in which students described
(in L1) the strategies they were using to read an unfamiliar text in English showed
the following:
becoming less reliant on the teacher, more selective in their use of the dictionary, and
more careful about their written work.
Some recent studies have used a combination of the native and target
languages for strategy instruction. In an investigation of strategy instruction by
secondary French and German teachers in London, some materials were in English
(especially those used by students for planning and evaluating their own work),
whereas checklists, descriptions of strategies, and strategy activities were written in
the target language, simplified as needed (Grenfell & Harris, 1999). In a study of
As noted earlier, learning strategies are directed toward particular tasks that
can vary in both obvious and subtle ways. Tasks differ depending on whether the
context is a second language or foreign language setting and whether the learner’s
goal is to acquire social or academic language or both (Chamot, 2004; Cohen, 2003;
Cummins, 2000; Oxford et al., 2004). Differences in strategy use also vary
according to proficiency level. Takeuchi’s (2003) multiple case studies of learner
journals found that learners reported shifting their use of strategies as they advanced
to higher proficiency levels. Similarly, a recent reading study found that perceived
difficulty of the task affected use of learning strategies, which were used on more
challenging tasks (Oxford et al., 2004).
The learner’s goals, the context of the learning situation, and the cultural
values of the learner’s society will also influence choice and acceptability of
language learning strategies. For example, in a culture that prizes individual
competition and has organized its educational system around competitive tasks,
successful language learners may prefer strategies that allow them to work alone
rather than social strategies that call for collaboration with others.
The implications for teaching are that language teachers need to find out
what learning strategies students already use for different tasks. An open discussion
of reasons why students use the strategies they identify can help teachers understand
cultural and contextual factors that may be influencing their students. This can lead
to clarification of task demands where there is a mismatch with students’ current
learning strategies. By understanding the task more clearly, students will likely be
more motivated to try new strategies to complete it.
Metacognitive Models
has been completed; Chamot, 1994). Evidence that language learners actually
engage in metacognitive knowledge and processes is reported in most of the research
on language learning strategies, both descriptive and instructional. Even young
children in language immersion classrooms can often describe their thinking
processes, demonstrating metacognitive awareness in their ability to describe their
own thinking (Chamot, 1999).
schemes of learner strategies are needed to describe the information derived from
descriptive studies that seek to chart the subtle permutations and often slippery
definitions of learners’ self-reported strategies. However, these extended and
complex definitions may be less useful in the language classroom where the teacher
is trying to help students become more strategic as they cope with actual learning
tasks rather than the hypothesized learning tasks proposed in the many questionnaires
and interviews designed to identify strategies that language learners claim to use.
REFERENCES
Ozeki, N. (2000). Listening strategy instruction for female EFL college students in
Japan. Tokyo: Macmillan Language House.
Pressley, M. (1995). More about the development of self-regulation: Complex, long-
term, and thoroughly social. Educational Psychologist, 30(4), 207–212.
Pressley, M. (2000). What should comprehension instruction be the instruction of? In
M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of
reading research: Volume III, (pp. 545–561). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ross, S., & Rost, M. (1991). Learner use of strategies in interaction: Typology and
teachability. Language Learning, 41(2), 235–273.
Rubin, J. (1975). What the "good language learner" can teach us. TESOL Quarterly,
9, 41–51.
Rubin, J. (2001). Language learner self-management. Journal of Asian Pacific
Communication, 11(1), 25–37.
Rubin, J. (2003). Diary writing as a process: Simple, useful, powerful. Guidelines,
25(2), 10–14.
Rubin, J., & Thompson, I. (1994). How to be a more successful language learner.
Second edition. Boston: Heinle & Heinle.
Schmitt, N. (2000). Vocabulary in language teaching. Cambridge: Cambridsge
University Press.
Shen, H-J. (2003). The role of explicit instruction in ESL/EFL reading. Foreign
Language Annals, 36(3), 424–433.
Stern, H. H. (1975). What can we learn from the good language learner? Canadian
Modern Language Review, 31, 304–318.
Takeuchi, O. (2003). What can we learn from good language learners: A qualitative
study in the Japanese foreign language context. System, 31, 385–392.
Thompson, I., & Rubin, J. (1996). Can strategy instruction improve listening
comprehension? Foreign Language Annals, 29(3), 331–342.
Vandergrift, L. (1997a). The comprehension strategies of second language (French)
Listeners: A descriptive study. Foreign Language Annals, 30(3), 387–409.
Vandergrift, L. (1997b). The Cinderella of communication strategies: Reception
strategies in interactive listening. Modern Language Journal, 81(4), 494–
505.
Vandergrift, L. (2003a). Orchestrating strategy use: Towards a model of the skilled
L2 listener. Language Learning, 53, 461–494.
Vandergrift, L. (2003b). From prediction to reflection: Guiding students through the
process of L2 listening. Canadian Modern Language Review, 59, 425–440.
Weaver, S. J., & Cohen, A. D. (1997). Strategies-based instruction: A teacher-
training manual. Minneapolis: Center for Advanced Research on Language
Acquisition, University of Minnesota.
Wenden, A. L. (1991). Learner strategies for learner autonomy. London: Prentice-
Hall International.
Wenden, A. L. (2000). Learner development in language learning. Applied
Linguistics, 23(1), 32–55.
Wharton, G. (2000). Language learning strategy use of bilingual foreign language
learners in Singapore. Language Learning, 50(2), 203–243.