1 2000 Ryan Decy
1 2000 Ryan Decy
University of Rochester
and extrinsic types of motivation have been widely studied, and the
Intrinsic
distinction between them has shed important light on both developmental and educa-
tional practices. In this review we revisit the classic definitions of intrinsic and
extrinsic motivation in light of contemporary research and theory. Intrinsic motiva-
tion remains an important construct, reflecting the natural human propensity to learn
and However, extrinsic motivation is argued to vary considerably in its
assimilate.
relativeautonomy and thus can either reflect external control or true self-regulation.
The relations of both classes of motives to basic human needs for autonomy, compe-
tence and relatedness are discussed. 0 2000 Academic Press
Yet, even brief reflection suggests that motivation is hardly a unitary phe-
nomenon. People have not only different amounts, but also different kinds
of motivation. That they vary not only in level of motivation (i.e., how
is,
much motivation), but also in the orientation of that motivation (i.e., what
type of motivation). Orientation of motivation concerns the underlying atti-
tudes and goals that give rise to action—that is, it concerns the why of ac-
tions. As an example, a student can be highly motivated to do homework
out of curiosity and interest because he or she wants to
or, alternatively,
54
0361-476X/00 $35.00
Copyright 0 2000 by Academic Press
All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.
INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONS 55
and creativity, it is especially important to detail the factors and forces that
engender versus undermine it.
However, equally important in the current review is the explication of
the very different types of motivation that fall into the category of extrinsic
motivation. In the classic literature, extrinsic motivation has typically been
characterized as a pale and impoverished (even if powerful) form of motiva-
tion that contrasts with intrinsic motivation (e.g., deCharms, 1968). How-
ever, SDT proposes that there are varied types of extrinsic motivation, some
of which do, indeed, represent impoverished forms of motivation and some
of which represent active, agentic states.
Students can perform extrinsically motivated actions with resentment, re-
sistance, and disinterest or, alternatively, with an attitude of willingness that
reflects an inner acceptance of the value or utility of a task. In the former
case—the classic case of extrinsic motivation—one feels externally pro-
pelled into action; in the later case, the extrinsic goal is self-endorsed and
thus adopted with a sense of volition. Understanding these different types
of extrinsic motivation, and what fosters each of them, is an important issue
for educators who cannot always rely on intrinsic motivation to foster learn-
ing. Frankly speaking, because many of the tasks that educators want their
students to perform are not inherently interesting or enjoyable, knowing how
topromote more active and volitional (versus passive and controlling) forms
of extrinsic motivation becomes an essential strategy for successful teaching.
56 RYAN AND DECI
We detail in this article not only the different types of motivational orienta-
tion that exist within the global extrinsic category, but moreover, their differ-
ential antecedents and consequences.
In sum, our aim in this article is to revisit the classic distinction between
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation and detail the conditions that fosters each.
Second, we model of differing types of extrinsic motivation. Our
describe a
concern here is with how teachers, parents and other socializers can lead
students to internalize the responsibility and sense of value for extrinsic goals
or, alternatively, how they can foster the more typically depicted alienated' ' ' '
INTRINSIC MOTIVATION
Intrinsic motivation is defined as the doing of an activity for its inherent
satisfactions rather than for some separable consequence. When intrinsically
motivated a person is moved to act for the fun or challenge entailed rather
than because of external prods, pressures, or rewards. The phenomenon of
intrinsic motivation was acknowledged within experimental studies of
first
different definitions derive from the fact that the concept of intrinsic motiva-
tionwas proposed as a critical reaction to the two behavioral theories that
were dominant in empirical psychology from the 1940s to the 1960s.
Specifically,because operant theory (Skinner, 1953) maintained that all
behaviors are motivated by rewards (i.e., by separable consequence such as
food or money), intrinsically motivated activities were said to be ones for
which the reward was in the activity itself. Thus, researchers investigated
what task characteristics make an activity interesting. In contrast, because
learning theory (Hull, 1943) asserted that all behaviors are motivated by
physiological drives (and their derivatives), intrinsically motivated activities
were said to be ones that provided satisfaction of innate psychological needs.
Thus, researchers explored what basic needs are satisfied by intrinsically
motivated behaviors.
Our own approach focuses primarily on psychological needs—namely,
the innate needs for competence, autonomy, and relatedness—but of we
course recognize that basic need satisfaction accrues in part from engaging
we do sometimes speak of intrinsically inter-
in interesting activities. Thus,
esting activities, but when we do so we are really only talking about tasks
that, on average, many people find to be intrinsically interesting. There is
Operational Definitions
Intrinsic motivation hasbeen operationally defined in various ways, al-
though there have been two measures that have been most often used. Basic
experimental research (e.g., Deci, 1971) has rested primarily on a behavioral
measure of intrinsic motivation called the "free choice" measure. In experi-
ments using measure participants are exposed to a task under varying
this
conditions (e.g., getting a reward or not). Following this period, the experi-
menter tells participants they will not be asked to work with the target task
any further, and they are then left alone in the experimental room with the
target task as well as various distractor activities. They thus have a period
of "free choice" about whether to return to the activity, and it is assumed
that, if there is no extrinsic reason to do the task (e.g., no reward and no
approval), then the more time they spend with the target task, the more intrin-
sically motivated they are for that task. This measure has been the mainstay
through which the dynamics of intrinsic motivation have been experimen-
tally studied.
The other common approach measurement of intrinsic motivation
to the
is the use of self-reports of interest and enjoyment of the activity per se.
Despite the observable evidence that humans are liberally endowed with
intrinsic motivational tendencies, this propensity appears to be expressed
only under specifiable conditions. Research into intrinsic motivation has thus
placed much emphasis on those conditions that elicit, sustain, and enhance
this special type of motivation versus those that subdue or diminish it. Self-
Determination Theory is specificallyframed in terms of social and environ-
mental factors that facilitate versus undermine intrinsic motivation. This lan-
guage reflects the assumption that intrinsic motivation, being an inherent
organismic propensity, is catalyzed (rather than caused) when individuals
are in conditions that conduce toward its expression.
Cognitive Evaluation Theory (CET) was presented by Deci and Ryan
(1985) to specify the factors in social contexts that produce variability in
intrinsic motivation. CET, which is considered a subtheory of self-determina-
tion theory, argues that interpersonal events and structures (e.g., rewards,
communications, feedback) that conduce toward feelings of competence dur-
ing action can enhance intrinsic motivation for that action because they allow
satisfaction of the basic psychological need for competence. Accordingly,
for example, optimal challenges, effectance promoting feedback, and free-
dom from demeaning evaluations are all predicted to facilitate intrinsic moti-
vation.
CET further specifies that feelings of competence will not enhance intrin-
sic motivation unless they are accompanied by a sense of autonomy or, in
novelty, challenge, or aesthetic value for that individual. For activities that
do not hold such appeal, the principles of CET do not apply. To understand
the motivation for activities that are not experienced as inherently interesting,
we need to look more deeply into the nature and dynamics of extrinsic moti-
vation.
EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION
Although intrinsic motivation is clearly an important type of motivation,
most of the activities people do are not, strictly speaking, intrinsically moti-
vated. This is especially the case after early childhood, as the freedom to be
intrinsically motivated becomes increasingly curtailed by social demands
and roles that require individuals to assume responsibility for nonintrinsically
interesting tasks. In schools, for example, it appears that intrinsic motivation
becomes weaker with each advancing grade.
Extrinsic motivation is a construct that pertains whenever an activity is
Exü-insic motivation
External
regulation Introjection Identification Integration
ASSOCIATED Perceived non- Saliance of Ego involvement Conscious valuing Hierarchical Interest/
PROCESSES contingency extrinsic Focus on of activity synthesis of Enjoyment
Low perceived rewards or approval from Self-endorsement goals Inherent
competence pubishments self or others of goals Congruence satisfaction
Nonrelevance Compliance/
Nonintentionality Reactance
regulation through identification. Here, the person has identified with the
personal importance of a behavior and has thus accepted its regulation as
his or her own. A boy who memorizes spelling lists because he sees it as
relevant to writing, which he values as a life goal, has identified with the
value of this learning activity.
Finally, the most autonomous form of extrinsic motivation is integrated
regulation. Integration occurs when have been fully
identified regulations
assimilated to the self. This occurs through self-examination and bringing
new regulations into congruence with one's other values and needs. The
more one internalizes the reasons for an action and assimilates them to the
self, the more one's extrinsically motivated actions become self-determined.
At the far right hand end of the figure is intrinsic motivation. This place-
ment emphasizes that intrinsic motivation is a prototype of self-determined
activity. Yet, as implied above, this does not mean that as extrinsic regula-
tions become more internalized they are transformed into intrinsic motiva-
tion.
does, on average, tend to become more "internal" over time (e.g., Chan-
dler & Connell, 1987), in accord with the general organismic tendencies
toward autonomy and self-regulation (Ryan, 1995).
Ryan and Connell (1989) tested the formulation that these different types
of motivation do indeed lie along a continuum of relative autonomy. They
investigated achievement behaviors (e.g., doing homework) among elemen-
tary school children, assessing external, introjected, identified, and intrinsic
reasons for engaging in these behaviors. They found that the four types of
regulation were intercorrelated according to a quasi-simplex (ordered corre-
lation) pattern, thus providing evidence for an underlying continuum of au-
tonomy. Differences in attitudes and adjustment were also associated with
the different types of extrinsic motivation. For example, the more students
were externally regulated the less they showed interest, value, or effort, and
the more they indicated a tendency to blame others, such as the teacher, for
negative outcomes. Introjected regulation was positively related to ex-
pending effort, but was also related to more anxiety and to poorer coping
with failures. Identified regulation was associated with greater enjoyment of
school and more positive coping styles. And intrinsic motivation was corre-
lated with interest, enjoyment, felt competence, and positive coping.
Subsequent studies have extended these findings concerning types of ex-
trinsic motivation, showing for example that more autonomous extrinsic mo-
feeling respected and cared for by the teacher is essential for their willingness
to accept the proffered classroom values. In support of this, Ryan, Stiller,
and Lynch (1994) found that relatedness to teachers (and parents) was associ-
ated with greater internalization of school-related behavioral regulations.
A second issue concerns perceived competence. Adopting as one's own
an extrinsic goal requires that one feel efficacious with respect to it. Students
will more likely adopt and internalize a goal if they understand it and have
the relevant skills to succeed at it. Thus, we theorize that supports for compe-
tence (e.g., offering optimal challenges and effectance-relevant feedback)
facilitate internalization.
dren whose parents were more supportive of autonomy and relatedness. Wil-
liams and Deci (1996) used a longitudinal design to show greater internaliza-
tion among medical students whose instructors were more autonomy and
competence supportive. These are a few of the findings in this area that sug-
gesthow supports for relatedness and competence facilitate internalization
and how support for autonomy additionally facilitates the integration of be-
havioral regulations. When competent and
that occurs, people not only feel
related, but also self-determined, as they carry out extrinsically valued activi-
ties.
CONCLUSIONS
We have briefly presented self-determination theory in order to make the
between behaviors that are volitional and accompanied
critical distinction
that support the innate needs to feel connected, effective, and agentic as one
is exposed to new ideas and exercises new skills.
REFERENCES
Amabile, T. M. DeJong,, W. , & Lepper, M. R. (1976). Effects of externally imposed deadlines
on subsequent intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34,
92-98.
Bandura, A. (1986). Socialfoundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Engle-
wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice—Hall.
Benware, C. , & Deci, of learning with an active versus passive motiva-
E. L. (1984). Quality
tional set. American Educational Research Journal, 21, 755—765.
66 RYAN AND DECI
Chandler, C. L., & Connell, P. (1987). Children's intrinsic, extrinsic and internalized motiva-
J.
tion: A developmental study of children's reasons for liked and disliked behaviours. Brit-
ish Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 357—365.
Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1990). Competence, autonomy and relatedness: A motiva-
tional analysis of self-system processes. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe (Eds.), The
Minnesota symposium on child psychology (Vol. 22, (pp. 43—77). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation. New York: Academic Press.
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1998). Extrinsic rewards and intrinsic motivation:
motivation of the rewardee. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 1—10.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human
behavior. New York: Plenum.
Grolnick, W. S., Deci, E. L. , & Ryan, R. M. (1997). Internalization within the family: The
selfdetermination perspective. In J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and
children 's internalization of values: A handbook of contemporary theory (pp. 135—161).
New York: Wiley.
Hayamizu, T. (1997). Between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Examination of reasons for
academic study based on the theory of internalization. Japanese Psychological Research,
39, 98-108.
Reeve, J. , & Deci, E. L. (1996). Elements of the competitive situation that affect intrinsic
motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 24—33.
Ryan, R. M. (1982). Control and information in the intrapersonal sphere: An extension of
cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 450—461.
Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative processes. Journal
of Personality, 63, 397-427.
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and internalization: Examin-
ing reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
57, 749-761.
Ryan, R. M., & Grolnick, W. S. (1986). Origins and pawns in the classroom: Self-report
and projective assessments of individual differences in children's perceptions. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 550—558.
Ryan, R. M. Kuhl, , J., & Deci, E. L. (1997). Nature and autonomy: Organizational view
of social and neurobiological aspects of self-regulation in behavior and development.
Development and Psychopathology, 9, 701—728.
Ryan, R. M. , & Stiller, J. (1991). The social contexts of internalization: Parent and teacher
influences on autonomy, motivation and learning. In P. R. Pintrich & M. L. Maehr (Eds.),
Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 7, pp. 115—149). Greenwich, CT: JAI
Press.