Human Growth and Development - An Introduction For Social Workers (Student Social Work)
Human Growth and Development - An Introduction For Social Workers (Student Social Work)
understand human growth and development. The book locates relevant theory and up-to-date research
at the heart of contemporary lived experiences. This is done through the inclusion of several biographies
and the encouragement to imaginatively apply and see the interplay of knowledge within the context of
relationships.
The book skilfully instructs and interacts with the reader. It presents information in a way that will appeal
to learners at different stages offering both introductory and in-depth levels of knowledge.
These unique features make this book an indispensable text for student social workers in learning and
practice environments.’
Nora Duckett, London Metropolitan University
‘John Sudbery has provided a Human Growth and Development textbook full of interest. Written
specifically for social work, readers will be able to engage with it at various levels – from the personal
impact of narratives, case studies and reflective questions, to the more in depth academic content in
outlines of key theoretical issues. It takes a total life-span approach whilst exploring the complexities of
individual life courses and covers traditional, fundamental theories in a contemporary context drawing
on up to date research. Although designed as an introduction to the topic for social work students,
practitioners will find it a useful reference. It has excellent suggestions for further reading.’
Ruben Martin, Senior Lecturer in Social Work, University of Kent
‘John Sudbery’s book on human growth and development is a very welcome addition to the field for those
of us who teach this subject on qualifying and post qualifying social work courses at undergraduate and
master’s levels.
John’s interest in psychosocial perspectives and his rich experience as practitioner, manager and academic
social worker/researcher ensure the quality and rigor of this text.
John conveys the relevance of a huge array of theoretical approaches. His application to the contemporary
practice of social work is compelling. His voice is clear and present throughout the text. This ensures a
well integrated book which may be taken as a whole to guide a course of study. The book may also be
used according to the need for a particular chapter, as each is dedicated to one specific phase in the life
course. The case examples are moving and familiar to the reader. The additional references and appendices
are generous and absorbing and the format is interesting and illustrative.
John has a sound political perspective that underpins the work. His psychodynamic sensibility and firm
social work identity set this work apart from the more run of the mill texts on life course development.
At a time when social workers are thirsty for sources of nourishment in their complex and conflicted work
roles this book has much to offer.’
Clare Parkinson, Senior Lecturer, University of East London
‘This is a much welcome book providing an engaging, informative and accessible introduction to Human
Growth and Development for Social Workers.
The author’s unique approach facilitates reflection, learning and understanding of human growth and
development knowledge. The writing style is very engaging, enhanced by the use of relevant case
examples to make the link between theory and social work practice. Informed by a stimulating range of
theories and research, and anchored into practice issues and debates, the text cannot fail to encourage
thinking critically about human growth and development.
The book will undoubtedly be invaluable reading for social work students, a learning companion during
social work training and an interesting and enjoyable read for those already in practice.’
Dr Gabriela Misca, Lecturer, Keele University
Human Growth and Development
Social workers work with people at all stages of life, tackling a multitude of personal, social, health, welfare,
legal and educational issues. As a result, all social work students need to understand human growth and
development throughout the lifespan.
This introductory text provides a knowledge base about human development from conception to death. It
is designed to encourage understanding of a wide range of experiences: from the developmental trajectories
of children in care, to adult mental distress and the experiences of people with dementia, to bereavement.
Using engaging narratives to illustrate each topic, the author clearly introduces and analyses different
theoretical approaches, and links them to real-life situations faced by social workers.
Packed with case studies, this student-friendly book includes overviews, summaries, questions and further
reading in each chapter, as well as a ‘Taking it further’ section providing greater depth on key theoretical
issues. A reference section contains a glossary and overviews of the principal theories discussed throughout
the book. It is an essential read for all social work students.
John Sudbery is Senior Research Fellow in Social Work at the University of Salford, UK. He has been teaching
Human Growth and Development for the past ten years and is on the editorial board of The Journal of Social
Work Practice.
Student Social Work
This exciting new textbook series is ideal for all students studying to be qualified social workers, whether
at undergraduate or masters level. Covering key elements of the social work curriculum, the books are
accessible, interactive and thought-provoking.
New titles
Forthcoming titles
Social Work
A reader
Vivienne E. Cree
John Sudbery
First published 2010
by Routledge
2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon, OX14 4RN
Simultaneously published in the USA and Canada
by Routledge
270 Madison Avenue, New York, NY 10016
Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
List of illustrations xi
Acknowledgments xiii
Introduction 1
1 Beginnings 7
2 A secure base 33
ix
Contents
Glossary 331
Bibliography 339
Index 359
x
Illustrations
Figures
1.1 An example lifeline 17
1.2 Generational transmission of genes 20
1.3 The increasingly complex nature of gene-environment correlations 25
2.1 Erikson’s psychosocial stages 44
2.2 Fahlberg’s arousal–relaxation cycle 57
3.1 Scheme used in cognitive behavioural therapy 76
3.2 Risk: the overlap represents a different percentage of the right- and left-hand areas 85
4.1 Hopson’s model of transitions 116
5.1 Microsystems 146
5.2 Bronfenbrenner’s nested ecological model of development 147
6.1 Experience of family events before the age of 25: comparison of different generations 170
7.1 Infant mortality and social class 199
7.2 Characteristics of positive social systems and services for people with learning disability 224
9.1 Dual Process model of response to bereavement 269
E3.1 Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model 302
E7.1 Maslow’s hierarchy of needs 318
E7.2 Rogers’ self-structure 320
Tables
3.1 Percentage of pupils achieving 5 A*–C in GCSE 80
E5.1 Stages of cognitive development, after Piaget 310
E6.1 Stages in Erikson’s psychosocial model 315
E10.1 Kubler-Ross’ model: stages of grief 329
xi
Acknowledgments
I am grateful to the British Association for Adoption and Fostering for permission to reproduce the
diagram on page 57, and to Age Concern for permission to reproduce the information on pages
257–258.
Many people have contributed to the completion of his book. The support and commitment of Grace
McInnes and George Russell at Routledge were essential, together with others involved in the editing
and production processes. My thanks are due to the anonymous academic reviewers for their time and
care.
I owe a particular debt of gratitude to the late Colin Woodmansey, and to Joan Meredith, from
whom I learnt so much of what I have tried to communicate in this book. The text would not have
been possible without the wide-ranging theoretical and practical knowledge which Jeff Edwards, a
colleague at the University of Salford, shared with me. Finally, thanks must go to the parents and
children to whom I provided a service, to the staff I supervised, and students, all of whom over the
years have taught me about human development.
xiii
Introduction
This book is an introduction to human growth and development for social workers. Although designed
for a first-year undergraduate course, it contains sufficient depth to be a resource which remains
useful afterwards.
For the student who has already studied some child development, psychology or sociology, the book
uses this academic knowledge, as well as additional descriptive research material, to help you to see
how varied human life is, to see the world imaginatively through other people’s eyes, and to think about
the impact of ‘welfare’ interventions on people’s lives and development. It will prompt you to think
more deeply about whether you understand the theories and whether they describe life adequately.
Information you need about the theories is summarised in the chapters, and provided at more length
in the technical reference section. Because this is much more of an applied practical study, you may
find that the perspective and emphasis given to theories in this book is different from that you acquired
in previous purely academic study. In this course, you are learning to study life, using different theories
as appropriate, which may be a subtle contrast to academic study which equips you with knowledge
about research findings and theories.
1
Introduction
For the student who has not studied ‘social sciences’ before, the chapters are designed to provide you
with understanding, knowledge and theories about human development. The main chapter text takes
you on an imaginative journey into the experience of life, its variety, complexity and its challenges, and
shows how different theories and research help us to understand the world. To include the detail of
the theories or research would make the chapters too long and complex, and you will need to study
also the ‘Essential background’ sections, which I think you will find as interesting as the main chapters,
so that you are properly informed.
Areas considered in greater depth. Textbooks at this level always give an overview of the basics required
by students. By remaining at this introductory level, however, many texts fail to reflect the different
levels at which even first-year students in this subject must operate. Each student has (and is required
to demonstrate) areas in which they make use of more complete knowledge. These areas are different
for every student – about pregnancy for one, attachment theory for another, learning disability, older
age, and so on. They may arise from placement work, previous experience, special interest, or academic
choices made during a study course. In such a broad field, the options for more detailed study are
endless. In this book, six chapters end with a section which examines an aspect of growth and
development in more depth. They provide examples of how basic knowledge is taken further and can
be written about in more detail.
When you have finished the book, if you return and re-read it, you will probably understand the text
very differently. Casual references which you glossed over on first reading actually hint at a host of
additional understanding which will be evident by the end of the book. This is how it is in real life too
– your experience of someone else’s life (or your own) changes when you have had cause to reflect
and explore some of the complexities of the life cycle. This is also true in this book because you will
know far more about the people in the examples after you have read the book – just like in real life
and in social work practice, an incident at a point in time will have a different significance after you
have had a chance to understand more about the person and their life.
2
Introduction
for whom they are new. For the former it recapitulates the ideas as
a preliminary to showing how they integrate (or not) with each
another and how they illuminate developmental experience; for the
latter, key ideas are introduced without going into technical detail
or specific evidence.
• The aim of the text is to equip the student both to see life more
clearly from the inside, and to become confident in examining it as
a subject of study.
Essential background Concise accounts of commonly used developmental theories. They focus
on ‘understanding theories’ as distinct from ‘understanding life’, but I
hope students will nevertheless find them stimulating and thought pro-
voking. They give a picture of each theory covered, largely from within
that theory’s point of view. They give space to evidence and conceptual
framework. Obviously, they are relevant to imaginatively understanding
life and development, but they are written as reference material. By
clearly presenting them as introductory summaries, it is hoped that they
forestall inappropriate attempts to use elementary presentations as a
basis for critical evaluation. They are written in standard language
accessible to first-year university students.
Taking it further These sections explore a selected topic in greater depth. In general, these
are more demanding intellectually, and I do not expect every student
will read them all. They are written in the appropriate academic style for
the subject – more detached when they are more scientifically oriented,
but more flexible in language when considering emotions and personal
explorations. Some give a flavour of current research knowledge (the
interaction between genes and environment, for example); some explore
a theory in more detail (attachment theory, or Bronfenbrenner’s ecolog-
ical model) and others pick a facet of life to explore (guilt and conscience).
3
Introduction
The module materials, from lectures to discussions and textbooks, therefore provide information, offer
an explanation of theories and models, and nurture the ability to enter imaginatively into someone
else’s life. In relation to the latter, the course equips students to be open to the diversity of life, to avoid
ignorant prejudgements, to refrain from interpreting life through the filter of theory, and yet to be
equipped with the categories, concepts, attitudes and empathy which form the basis of accurate
rapport and critical thinking.
In this book, the chapter text (including ‘Taking it further’) is essentially about exploration – exploring
life, its development, and its complexities. It helps with the student’s task of exploring the value of
different theories and approaches, attempting to integrate their implications and application. Although
the ten technical resources include discussion, I regard their purpose as primarily informational.
First-year social work students need a general overview of human growth and development as well as
knowledge beyond the elementary in some areas (not least because within the year they will be
‘practising’ on real people with complex and urgent needs). In this subject and at this level, students
appropriately will choose some areas which they explore in more depth than others, perhaps linked to
previous work experience, elective seminar presentations, or special topics for course work and assess-
ment. In addition, realistically, some complete their programme with a much more sophisticated grasp
of the subject than others. I am conscious that not all teachers may welcome a book which states from
the outset that not all students will master all the material. Nevertheless, if we are to meet the
legitimate learning needs of the students, we must be aware that some will study material which others
avoid. To include both a basic survey and all the necessary advanced material would require a far larger
book than this and one not well suited to first-year students. On the other hand, keeping all the
material at a basic level results in the typical problems of first-year social work textbooks in this
subject – a ‘middle of the road’ or introductory approach which lacks depth and gives no real guidance
to the student about where their studies should take them. In this textbook I have tried to create a
resource which avoids the dangers of superficiality and blandness. As scholars, practitioners and
educators we find our own solutions to this conundrum in the lecture room, in the seminar room and
in the structures we create for student research and learning. The text is intended to provide a written
resource to match the creativity we put into this task.
In physics, as with plumbing, more basic concepts and procedures are practised first and the more
complex knowledge builds on these foundations. In our subject, students (whether they recognise it
or not) are already operating at an extremely sophisticated level in their own lives, and the basics of
analysis have to be learnt simultaneously with the refinement of already complex skills.
The book does not include any formal introduction to research methods or critical appraisal. However,
it is simply not possible for professional workers in training to acquire all the information they need
from a single textbook and classroom learning, and I have assumed throughout that seminar and
assessed written work require the student to integrate these sources with additional material from
their own investigations.
4
Introduction
Glossary The glossary explains the meaning of terms which may be unfamiliar. Most
are technical terms whose use is explained when the word is first used. The
first time they occur within a chapter, glossary words are printed in bold.
This not done for every occurrence because the use of bold distorts the
rhythm and visual emphasis of a sentence.
Reflective questions These are interactive sections which use open-ended activities to direct the
readers’ attention to themselves and their development. Their purpose is
varied – sometimes to bring home the particular aspect of development
which has been described more academically, sometimes to provide an
alternative learning experience to reading; sometimes as a small con-
tribution to helping students avoid the trap of treating ‘clients’, ‘service
users’, as if they were a different category to themselves; and sometimes
to underscore that in social work, self-understanding is essential.
Questions to the student Sometimes as a variant to continuous prose, questions are asked, to
in the text prompt an active engagement with the academic material. Sometimes
they highlight that a suggested research approach in the text is not the
only one possible; sometimes they check understanding or illustrate how
an apparently simple statement may have complex ramifications.
‘Links’ The digital book hasn’t arrived yet! There are a limited number of marked
‘links’, directing the reader’s attention to a different part of the book
where a related topic is covered.
Further reading At the end of each chapter, indications are given for sources of further
information and discussion. Several of these include further reading
appropriate also for the use of service users (for example, information
designed for children in care, information about mental health issues pro-
vided by the charity Mind).
‘For you to research’ From the numerous further avenues of exploration, a few topics related to
the subject matter of each chapter are singled out as suggestions for inde-
pendent research by the student. These may well be worked on by small
groups of students collaboratively for seminar presentation or portfolio
work. In my experience, students produce excellent work from these inves-
tigations, and remember the material well.
5
Introduction
The chapters follow a roughly chronological sequence. Pauses along the route to consider particular
theories or perspectives do not have an entirely logical place in this scheme – the influence of genes
and environment, for example, or poverty, are relevant to all stages of life. A compromise has to be
made to keep the chapters to a manageable size.
6
In this chapter you will find:
1 Beginnings
• Introductory reading
These words point to the different ways in which a social worker needs to be able to understand human
development.
In the first three months after conception the baby can be said to be ‘taking shape’ – developing the
basic plan of a human body, including a head, arms, legs, hands and feet. In the next three months, the
organs and limbs will be developing in size, complexity and functionality; the mother can feel her baby
moving, and the baby responds to stimuli. Continuing to simplify this finely tuned and intricate process,
in the next three months each interlinked part of the tiny body will continue to grow in size, efficiency
and complexity until birth, and the baby in this period becomes increasingly able to survive outside the
womb. In keeping with this simplified account, if drugs interfere with the process in the first three
months, parts of the baby may be malformed or missing; and malnutrition is more likely to cause small
size if it occurs in the final three months rather than in the earlier phases.
We have no words that can accurately describe the unborn baby’s experience. At some point, the baby
becomes conscious of colour (colour vision is present, albeit still developing, at birth – Atkinson and
Braddick, 1982). We know that it hears sound, as after birth it will respond differently to pieces of music
which have been played repeatedly during pregnancy – presumably most of these sounds are the
internal noises of its mother’s body and the muffled penetration of her voice, talking, singing, shouting.
In a fascinating series of observations, Piontelli (2002) found that at the age of 5, twins were still using
routines for mutual comforting which had been observed by ultrasound when they were in the womb.
And then at some point it will be gripped harder than it is ever likely to be gripped again, so hard that
the bones of its skull fold over each other. Over a period between seven and fourteen hours on average,
it will be propelled in repeated shoves down a narrow tube until it bursts into a noisy, bright, colourful
environment totally different from the world it has experienced previously. This shocking experience
will usually have the effect you might predict – having been massively stimulated the baby will be
awake for an initial period and then fall into a deep exhausted sleep.
You will be able to find many sources of further information about pregnancy, including websites
which you can locate for yourself. Detail appropriate to this level of study can be found in Chapter 3
of Bee and Boyd (2007: 47–83).
We could tell a related story for the mother’s own bodily development during pregnancy, but instead,
let us think about a different perspective: the developmental meaning of the pregnancy in a woman’s
life, its significance and implications, which are different for every mother.
9
Beginnings
Nicola
Perhaps the young woman is Nicola, pregnant with her second child, anticipating that she will leave
paid work for at least the next five or six years. She is 26 years old, and has made a good start to her
career. She and her husband have planned – to the degree that these things can be planned – that his
income and some state benefit will support the family until she goes back to work when both children
are at school. She is not very clothes-conscious, but usually looks smart in her business suit and short
black hair when she goes to work. She’s very busy day-to-day with her first child – let us say a boy –
but her husband, a neighbour with whom she’s close, her mother whom she sees once or twice a week
and some friends who had babies at the same time as her, are all involved in the planning for when
she has her new baby. During her first pregnancy, she had many thoughts and daydreams about how
the life of her child would turn out; she has similar thoughts now, but when asked about the future
she says, ‘Oh, I just hope everything’s going to be OK, I’m really quite stretched this time, what with
work, Mathew, Steve’s job and the pregnancy as well. I just hope the baby will be healthy, have ten
fingers and ten toes, and we’ll get everything sorted in time’. What she says, of course, depends on
whom she is talking to; she has a friend at work with whom she particularly chats about her toddler
and the pregnancy.
Naoko
But every pregnancy, every woman, is different. Maybe the young woman is Naoko and this is her first
baby. She was born and brought up 6,000 miles away in Japan, and is now living in the UK with Paul,
whom she met as a student. She sees her mother only once a year. Her partner’s parents are supportive,
but although they are geographically closer, their routines, expectations, standards of health care,
religious beliefs and daily language are all a second culture for her. English people find her rather quiet
and reserved, and she still occasionally struggles to find the word she wants. She is sometimes
surprised by the behaviour of boys and girls and by the attitudes of women where she lives.
Sharon
And a social worker must be prepared to understand a myriad of different developmental stages. The
mother-to-be may be Sharon, aged 16 and having just left a children’s home. She’s talking on the
phone to Emma, the only person she trusts. Emma’s 17 and also in care. ‘Number 24’ has been Sharon’s
third placement in two years, before which she lived in six foster homes. Her ‘independent living’
keyworker sometimes listens, alarmed, as she jabs her finger into her belly and says, ‘I hate it,’
deliberately emphasising the word ‘it’, and continuing, ‘I hope it’s gone when I wake up.’ She doesn’t
say much at all to this worker, whom she has known for only four months, since she left ‘number 24’,
but she does speak about the time she thinks she got pregnant, which was when she stayed out all
night at a friend’s squat, and felt pressurised, almost without caring what she did, into sex. The
significance of the pregnancy in her life? Her ferocious displays of independence and wilfulness had
always been partly a reaction to her pervasive sense of helplessness before fate; defiant strivings to
10
Beginnings
carve out some control for herself in the midst of major events which usually seemed just to happen
to her. The pregnancy was little different. She is defiantly independent, proclaiming her competence
to do whatever is required; she also has the sneaking hope, daydream, that the baby might be the one
person in the world who will really love her, who will be hers; sometimes she is terrified of the
responsibility and tasks that she hardly dares think about. At the same time, a young woman with a
ferocious temper, a short fuse, intolerance born of frustration, she is bitterly angry towards the latest
interference in her life; and underlying this is the overwhelming sense of helplessness and lack of
control over events. As earlier in her life, she feels that things are done to her, they happen without
her permission, and her attempts at effective influence repeatedly seem to dissolve into a position of
impotence.
Social support
One feature common to each story is that however independent and competent each pregnant woman
is (and all have demonstrated great strength and resourcefulness in their lives to date), each needs
emotional and practical support, and this will have particular significance at the birth and afterwards.
This support may come from many different directions – a husband or other partner; a circle of female
friends; the woman’s mother; a religious grouping; various official, medical or social staff, for example.
As a social worker, you could potentially be involved in any of these situations, and it would be a routine
part of your professional assessment to understand the nature of the woman’s needs, how what you
have to offer fits in with all the other sources of support available (or missing) and the potential
outcome of offering support.
Toddlers to grandparents
There are of course other people involved in this scenario, each at a different stage of life. It is typical
of social work that these all have to be kept in mind. Unlike doctors, psychologists, counsellors or many
other human service professionals, as a social worker dealing with social situations, it is usual for you
to have professional responsibility for several different life stages at the same time.
For Mathew, Nicola’s first child, this pregnancy, and more importantly the birth, may represent a big
milestone. Until now, he has been the sole focus of parental attention, love, annoyance and
preoccupation. In this attention, the adults who keep him safe are concerned about his welfare, ensure
they are there for him, focus all their parental love solely on him. His experience is that they are
captivated by him when he offers a single smile or takes a first few tottering steps. One utterance that
sounds as if it might be a word evokes doting admiration. This is shortly to change forever, a dramatic
change as he is supplanted by a rival for his mother’s love and attention – ‘Can’t we just put her in the
bin?’ as one boy said of his young sister.
It may be, too, that that the husband is facing some of the most stressful periods in his life as he
juggles new responsibilities at work, financial responsibilities at home – and he too may have troubles
11
Beginnings
about the direction of Nicola’s affections, the time she has for him, changes in her sexual impulses –
he turns over choices, perhaps dilemmas, as to how to satisfy his sexual needs; with her he will be
finding his way, managing and relating to an increasingly independent toddler, and later a schoolchild.
Then there are Nicola’s parents, in whose development grandchildren are likely to be extremely sig-
nificant. They were older than many – 64 when Nicola became pregnant again – and they are a major
part of their grandchildren’s lives. Nicola’s first child has experienced much of his daytime care at their
house.
On the other hand, think of the world from the point of view of Naoko’s parents. They see their
daughter and grandchild for only one week in the year. They will perhaps have questions, worries, about
the starkly contrasting gender attitudes and child-rearing practices from those they have believed to
be ‘correct’ and ‘necessary’ in the provincial Japanese village which is their home. The mother of 16-
year-old Sharon – perhaps she hasn’t seen her daughter for twelve years. If she is like many mothers
in such a situation, she will describe the loss of her daughter into care as ‘like a death, only worse’. Birth
parents of adopted children say that long after the event, they still think of their lost child ‘every day,
or two or three times a week’; decades later, they describe themselves as ‘still screaming’. Her sense of
loneliness, hurt and loss may be intensified if she hears by a circuitous route that her daughter has
had a baby. Or perhaps Sharon’s mother is still in touch with her, and alongside her fury with social
workers for what they did in the past, desperately hopes they will be able to help her daughter so her
grandchild is competently cared for.
From time to time in the coming chapters, you will read more of the world through the eyes of Nicola,
Sharon, Naoko and their friends and families. We can’t simply describe their babies’ lives through to
old age and death – if we did, we’d have to start in the early 1920s, at the time when ‘Sharon’ might
be a girl in a workhouse and might have been locked up for the rest of her life in a mental asylum
because she was pregnant; and in a number of occupations Nicola’s employment would have been
ended because she married. We want to start from pregnancies in circumstances you can relate to. But
the book does emphasise the whole sweep of a life, how you have to understand earlier incidents in
order to understand the person before you. So in order to understand the development of Sharon’s
grandfather Bob, who dies before the book is finished, you will read snippets about his infancy and
earlier adulthood, how he got on with Sharon’s mother Bella, and how a social worker became involved
just before his death.
12
Beginnings
Generalising from the introductory snapshots at the beginning of the chapter, you can see the areas
about which you need to become knowledgeable:
• Biological knowledge about the body, its development, and its influences on emotion and
behaviour.
However, there is a further complication to which we will return: you will need to understand the view
that the word ‘knowledge’ in the above information box (we could have used the word ‘understanding’)
has to be viewed with caution. It seems to indicate something absolute, to point to ‘facts’ which exist
independently of the researchers who discover or codify them, independent of the language they use
and where they publish their findings. But social constructionists argue that ‘facts’ presented by social
research are always shaped politically by the cultures of the researchers and the reader. They always
represent a ‘point of view’ and would be different if understood from a different perspective. The
categories used are always social creations, and taking them for granted significantly conceals some
of what is going on in the activity and publication of such research.
Bio-psychosocial knowledge
One of the most straightforward descriptions of social work is that it is a psychosocial activity (Woods
and Hollis, 1968/2000; Howe, 2008), concerned with the ‘person-in-the-situation’: not just the person
on their own, nor just the social arrangements around them, but the two together. In providing a
knowledge base about individual development, this book draws attention to biological factors as well
as the psychological and sociological – a bio-psychosocial perspective.
13
Beginnings
This book provides frameworks and examples to prepare you for seeking such specific
expertise. This chapter concludes with a discussion about the nature of genetic influence
– an introduction to aspects of the nature/nurture debate. Other chapters contain brief
Chapters
2, 4, 6, 8, sections about: the influence of emotional relationships on brain development in infants
EB1
(Chapter 2), physical changes in adolescence (Chapter 4), organic dimensions to mental
health problems (Chapter 6), physical aspects of the stress response (Chapter 6) and the
ageing process (Chapter 8). There are also shorter references such as that about prenatal development
at the beginning of this chapter. ‘Essential background’, section 1 provides a summary of what is meant
by ‘genetics’.
Psychological understanding
The core purpose of this book is to promote your understanding of development in people’s feelings,
behaviour and relationships.
Different researchers and practitioners have created different schemes for making sense of these
subjects. Sometimes, these different perspectives illuminate different aspects of life, but researchers
also sometimes claim that their findings show that another theory is simply mistaken. This book will
discuss many of these disagreements.
Using the examples earlier in the chapter for reference, here is a summary of some of the theories
which will be used in the course of the book:
• Attachment theory, which interprets observational studies as showing that infants are
born with a drive to form attachments and parents are programmed to respond with care
and protection. The ‘attachment style’ of an individual develops throughout life in response
to external relationships and events, but early attachments are very influential in shaping
later attitudes and behaviour. Introduced in Chapter 2 and summarised in ‘Essential background’,
section 2.
14
Beginnings
• Psychodynamic theories, which take for granted the existence of conflicting motivations and
interests, and view some of them as being unconscious. Also introduced in Chapter 2, and
summarised in ‘Essential background’, section 4.
Speculate: what might be some of the conflicting impulses which motivate Nicola’s husband
Steve? What about her son Mathew, or Nicola herself? What influences how they resolve these
conflicts?
• Humanistic models, such as Rogers’, which points out that people from childhood onwards have
a drive to achieve something in life, and also a need for unconditional acceptance; it examines
achievements and difficulties in life in the light of this. Introduced in Chapter 4 and summarised
in ‘Essential background’, section 7.
• Learning theories, including cognitive behavioural and ‘social learning’ perspectives, which
emphasise that behaviour is shaped by rewards and punishments that have been applied in the
past, and also by the perception of what is thought to be advantageous or problematic. Introduced
in Chapter 3 and summarised in ‘Essential background’, section 8.
Think about Mathew at 20 months old. In simplified, cognitive-behavioural terms, what accounts
for how Mathew’s behaviour is developing? How might that perspective account for the
difference between what Naoko’s parents think are good qualities in a woman and what Nicola
thinks?
The answer in each case is that the behaviour shown depends what behaviour has been rewarded
and encouraged in the past, and which behaviours punished or discouraged.
• Models of the experience of ‘spoiled identities’ – growth to positive self-identity in the face of
widespread social attitudes indicating the individual is part of a ‘problem group’. Examples are
black people brought up in a society with racist attitudes, gay and lesbian people brought up in
a society in which their orientation is seen as unnatural or immoral, or disabled people facing
attitudes which confuse speech difficulty with cognitive impairment. Discussed in a number of
chapters, but particularly in relation to adolescence in Chapter 5, and adulthood in Chapter 7.
• Theories of ageing, including the ‘social disengagement’ model, ‘activity theory’, and ‘political
economy theory’. The first of these, for example, builds on research findings to theorise that there
is a mutual advantage for the individual and for society if the older generation gradually withdraws
from social interaction and responsibility. Chapter 8 and ‘Essential background’, section 9.
After the main chapters, the ‘Essential background’ contains a ready reference summary of these and
other theories, as well as an outline of the criticisms which have been directed at them.
15
Beginnings
Sociological thinking
The life course of mother and baby will be profoundly affected by social factors. If the mother were
from a Gypsy family, the Department of Health’s report suggests that the baby is more than twelve
times as likely to die from sudden infant death syndrome, and ten times as likely to die from all causes
before the age of 2; a third will die before reaching the age of 25 and more than seven out of ten will
die before reaching the age of 59 (Parry et al., 2004, explaining they used statistics from Ireland – see
McKittrick, 2007). The life course of Naoko’s mother in rural Japan may have been profoundly different
from that of a woman of comparable age brought up in the UK. A child growing up as part of a group
which is seen as ‘problematic’ or ‘immoral’ by wide sections of the population faces distinctive
developmental issues. Whatever genetic differences are at work, there are differences in the experience
and expectations of males and females throughout the course of life which are massively influenced
by societal attitudes and expectations.
In general, this book will not focus on sociological theory and debate. It will, however, refer frequently
to ways in which social factors and perspectives are areas to explore in relation to individual change
and development. It assumes a ‘cosmopolitan’ view of society – in brief, that people’s inner worlds
(their identity, values, fears, memories, pleasures) may or may not be primarily embedded in the
geographical area in which they live.
One account which sets out systematically the different interpersonal and sociological influences on
development is Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model. This is presented in Chapter 5 and summarised
in ‘Essential background’, section 3.
The ‘life course’ approach to development emphasises that there is no universal ‘natural cycle’ of life.
Lives are always located in specific historical, geographical and cultural contexts, and the search for
‘normal development’ or universal stages may do violence to the diversity of human experience.
16
Beginnings
About yourself
As you follow a course about human growth and development, it is likely that it sets off many thoughts
and feelings about yourself. For reasons that are touched on particularly in Chapter 10, self-understanding
is important in social work. This book contains a series of activities that allow you to look at aspects of
yourself, your attitudes and your development.
The activities can be taken at different levels of sensitivity, but all can touch on painful aspects of life.
Ideally, perhaps, they are activities you would undertake with a person whom you know to be kind,
competent and understanding in taking care of your feelings. There are no right or wrong answers.
For this first activity, draw a lifeline to represent your life to where you are now (see Figure
1.1 below). Mark some important milestones or transitions you have experienced – for
example, being born; birth of siblings; parents’ relationship – separation, divorce, remarriage;
Chapter 10 starting school; getting a job; friendships; marriage, and so on.
There are many different ways this could prompt reflection about your development. For example, the
section of this chapter ‘Making sense of development and change’ stated that you need biological,
psychological and sociological knowledge. Discuss how this applies to one of the periods you have
identified – why were biological, psychological and sociological factors all involved in the development
that was taking place?
Stayed with
nana Birth
Start
school
Moved to
Keen swimmer
England
– won prizes
Rocky patch
Intended
path
Job at Primark
Met Chris Started
university
Figure 1.1
An example lifeline
17
Beginnings
Furthermore, you only understand an individual’s development when you understand the
development of other people which is interlocked with it – the baby’s experience is interlocked
with the mother’s, which in turn is interlocked with (for example) the father’s and the mother’s
mother. As a competent social worker, you have in nearly all situations to understand the
developmental experience of several people (usually of different ages) at once.
If you just wish to scan for intellectual content I have tried to set this out so it is readily
available. The ‘Essential background’ sections should do this efficiently at the basic level, and
the ‘Taking it further’ essays offer more depth. The main chapter text requires a different kind
of attention – a more reflective, personal engagement. The narratives may prompt questions
such as, ‘I wonder how they got to this stage in their life?’ or, ‘How easy is it for me to
understand their experiences?’ In the words of David Howe (2008), it’s important that you
develop as an ‘emotionally intelligent social worker’. You might think about whether you’d be
interested to meet them, and what your feelings would be in their company – would you know
how to set them at ease and so on. In the healthiest possible way, social work is founded on a
curiosity about people and a wish that things should be well for them, and this sometimes
requires a slower pace of thought as you reflect on an imaginary encounter.
18
Beginnings
19
Beginnings
A and
B
Child, Sharon
Figure 1.2
Generational transmission of genes
Figure 1.2 illustrates another feature of genetic reproduction. All the cells in the body contain two sets
of genes, one from each parent, except the male sperm and the female egg which only contain one set
each. When the egg and the sperm combine to form the start of a new person, the fertilised egg now
contains the usual double set.
The (30,000 or so) genes are grouped together into structures called chromosomes. The chromosomes
come in pairs, one containing the instructions from the father and one from the mother. Although the
detail of what they code for may be different (one for blue eyes, one for brown eyes), the genes on
each of a matched pair are for the same function (eye colour in this case). The pair of chromosomes
determining sex are different from this general pattern. They come in two different forms, called
because of their shape X and Y. If the fertilised egg has the pair X-Y, the child will be a boy. If the pair
is X-X, the child will be a girl.
Additional basic resources are given on the website which accompanies this book.
20
Beginnings
?
Lead-up questions
A man has a gene (call it G1) on his Y chromosome. Will that gene ever have been operating in
a female?
Answer: No, because women never have a Y chromosome in their body – they are X-X.
A woman has a gene (call it G2) on her X chromosome. Has this ever been operating in a male?
Answer: Yes, because female bodies have X-X and men have X-Y. A daughter will always have one X
chromosome from her father.
Of all the Y chromosomes in the population (say, a country), how many are in male bodies?
Answer: All of them.
Of all the X chromosomes in a population, how many are in female bodies at any given time?
Answer: Two out of three. Women have X-X and men have X-Y chromosomes.
What else affects your development and who you are other than your genes?
Answer: Well, I hope you found the answer to that fairly obvious! – How you’re brought up, whether you
have always eaten well, what illnesses you have suffered, your choices about which school to go to and
what career to follow. . . . The list is endless. The essay is particularly concerned with the question: What
are some of the factors that affect how genes and environment interact?
21
Beginnings
• pathways: a term used to describe the route by which someone’s personal qualities come into exis-
tence. The pathway to having blue eyes is a genetic makeup which sets off certain chemical and
biological processes. The pathway to becoming a good social worker is . . .?
• correlations: some factors which have an effect on an outcome can be changed separately from
each other. To find what layout of magazine the readers prefer, a publisher can vary the size of
print and the colour of print separately, and find whether one has more effect than the other, and
what colour, size, and combination of colour and size have a good effect. If for some reason the
factors are interlinked, so that changing one changes the other – they are correlated in some way;
if changing the size would for some strange reason change the colour, then it’s a different ques-
tion to disentangle the effects. When the publishers thought they had measured the effect of size,
they might without realising it be measuring the effect of colour. I have chosen this example
because it will seem strange. The essay explains that genes and environment have often been
understood to vary independently, but in fact there may be complicated ways in which they are
linked.
TAKING IT FURTHER
Genes, environment and behaviour: correlations and interactions
Presentations of ‘the nature–nurture debate’ usually conclude by emphasising that both are involved
in human behaviour (for an introductory account, see Eysenck, 2000 – more detail is given in Ridley,
1999; Rutter, 2006). Further analysis explores the particular mechanisms and routes which operate
for specific outcomes. These are usually infinitely complicated. Geneticists (Sudbery and Sudbery,
2009; Bateson, 2001) point out that a very large number of genes are required to cooperate to
produce behaviour, and the same version of a gene can produce different results in a different ‘team’
of genes or in different environments. A multitude of environmental differences are relevant in
different episodes of development. Given this introduction, the elements highlighted here are:
Running through this are various issues about the meaning and operation of ‘cause’ in this context.
The discussion is illustrated by reference to children’s behaviour, gender and mental health issues.
22
Beginnings
23
Beginnings
of the general population or represents a particularly low (or high) value of something that varies
throughout the population.
For example, schizophrenia is listed, with its symptoms, in the authoritative diagnostic manuals
of psychiatry (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) as a classification – the individual ‘has’ it
or not. But the nature of the increasing evidence of its hereditability indicates that it varies continu-
ously through the population – like intelligence, say – and the ‘categorisation’ must be understood
as a convention, an agreed cut-off point. The reference to intelligence highlights how clarity about
this may be important for an accurate understanding. Intelligence varies continuously through-
out the population, with increasingly smaller numbers at either extreme – very high or very low.
This will have one set of environment–gene interactions. But the most common forms of mild
learning difficulties are not part of this continuous variation. They arise from specific conditions
such as Down’s syndrome. This is a category diagnosis (yes/no) and is caused by a specific
genetic condition – the person has an extra chromosome 21. Obviously, people in the lower third
of ability (that is, scoring below two-thirds of the population on measures of intelligence) may
be thus because of the normal variation of multifactorial determinants of intelligence, or
because they come into the category of Down syndrome, having the additional copy of chromosome
21.
Multiple pathways
This illustrates another feature of gene–environment antecedents of behaviour. In many cases, the
same psychosocial behavioural outcome can arise from different gene–environment pathways. For
example (Rutter, 2006: 29), depression in adulthood may be caused by a genetic predisposition
combined with early negative upbringing (including sexual abuse). But it may also be the outcome
for people without the negative upbringing but with particular current social stressors. It is likely
that some genetic component is implicated in a propensity to antisocial aggressive behaviour in
boys or in men. But some boys with this genetic makeup will not commit antisocial acts, and others
without the propensity will do so in particular social circumstances.
Gene–environment correlations
The simplicity of the question as to whether genes or environment are responsible for a particular
trait (such as musicality, sportiness or physical aggressiveness) becomes complicated initially by
recognising that both are always involved. This is shown in line 1 of Figure 1.3 below, which shows
diagrammatically how a person’s qualities in the present are the product of a particular genetic
makeup and experiences in life. This diagram, which is an analysis by Scarr and McCartney (1983),
goes on to show the ways in which genes and environment do not vary independently. As explained
in the following paragraphs, the environment is affected by a person’s genes, so environmental
effects may also be a result of genes.
Line 2 in Figure 1.3 draws attention to correlations in which the child is passive: important features
of the child’s environment are shaped by the actions of parents, but since biological parents are
formed by the same genes as the child, a sporty, musical, or violent environment may itself be part
of a genetic influence. Next (line 3), Scarr and McCartney suggest that differences in environments
may be evoked by the genetic makeup of the children. Active, muscular babies evoke active, playful
24
Beginnings
interactive responses and entertainment choices from those around them; children with a
disposition to musicality may lead their carers into providing musical environments.
Finally (line 4), they point out that children shape environmental characteristics for themselves –
choosing those that are compatible with their genetic predisposition. In this, the child is active in
selecting environments, and they call this active gene influences or niche picking. This links with
Eysenck’s view (1968) that because their nervous system is underaroused, extroverts seek out
stimulation.
So in summary, even studies (say, about aggressiveness) which identify genuine environmental
influences may be also be picking up intertwined genetic influences – the two are constantly inter-
linked.
Child’s genes
1 Child’s
phenotype*
Child’s
environment
Child’s genes
2 Parents’ Child’s
genes phenotype*
Child’s
environment
Child’s genes
3 Parents’ Child’s
genes phenotype*
Child’s
environment
Child’s genes
4 Parents’ Child’s
genes phenotype*
Child’s
environment
*Phenotype: the observed characteristics of an individual resulting from the interaction of genes and environment
Figure 1.3
The increasingly complex nature of gene-environment correlations as analysed by Scarr and McCartney (1983)
25
Beginnings
Gene–environment influences
Conversely, the way the genotype (the information in the genes) is expressed is itself shaped by
the environment – often through what are now called epigenetic effects. Epigenetics examines
the way in which genetic material controls which genes are switched on or off (resulting for
example in the same instruction set producing brain cells in one place and blood cells in another,
or determining the changes in function at puberty or old age). The appalling circumstances of
Dutch pregnant women under German occupation in the famine of 1942 affected genetically
determined factors in their granddaughters’ lives (Ceci and Williams, 1999: 13).The genetic infor-
mation itself was unchanged – there had been no mutation or miscopying of genes – but the eggs
within the Dutchwomen’s unborn daughters contained instructions for expression that took
account of the conditions of scarcity. When these eggs were fertilised decades later in the mature
daughters, they grew into offspring (grandchildren of the starved women) with particular char-
acteristics following the epigenetic instructions laid down at the time of their creation (which took
account of shortage of nutrition). They were genetically more resistant to diabetes and heart attack.
Equivalent effects had been discovered independently for males, in a study of Swedish family
records. In this case, the relevant period in the grandfathers’ lives was just before puberty, pro-
ducing seed that reflected the food conditions obtaining at that time (Kaati et al., 2002). The genes
were unchanged, but genetic vulnerability to particular illnesses was changed by the environments
affecting the grandparents.
This comparatively new branch of study (earlier research concentrated on genetic effects which
are irreversible) has according to Rutter (2006: 147) produced few findings specific to psychiatric
disorder. Nevertheless, it is recognised as a major set of mechanisms for understanding any genet-
ically transmitted effects, and Schore’s work (Schore, 1994, 2003) on the continuing construction
of the infant brain after birth has drawn attention to the way this genetic process is affected by
emotional interaction (see also Corr, 2006: 60). Gerhardt (2004) provides an eminently readable
account suitable for people with responsibilities for parents and children, but relevant to all
workers concerned with troubled emotions and relationships.
26
Beginnings
Problems with genetic origins may be entirely remedied by ‘environmental’ interventions. The
learning disability caused by phenylketonurea (PKU – a genetic condition which leaves the child
unable to absorb a particular amino acid) will simply not occur if the diagnosis is made at birth
and the baby is fed the correct diet. Rutter uses examples of adult depression and antisocial behav-
iour to summarise how genetic makeup, earlier environment and contemporary situation may
interact as risk and protective factors.
Furthermore, the amount of difference that environment may cause in highly hereditable traits
can be much larger than intuitively may be expected. Ceci and Williams (1999: 3–5) provide
both a qualitative explanation and a mathematically worked example. In this example, a group of
adopted children have average IQ measures of 107 in a population with high hereditability. Even
if the average IQ of their mothers is 85, a full 22 points lower, this is still consistent with intelligence
(as measured by the IQ scores) being 70 per cent hereditable.
27
Beginnings
This draws attention to a final important point: for groups who typically are subject to different
environmental variables (say, European Americans and African Americans), differences in average
scores between the two groups will not arise from the same allocation of genetic/environmental
influence as do individual differences. The differences in height among a group of friends may be
highly genetic in origin, but the differences between their average height and that of another group
(say, their parents) may be largely environmental – as a result of changes in nutrition and
healthcare.
Conclusion
Social workers sometimes need to understand references to the relative influence of heredity and
environment in troublesome aspects of human development. This section has been written with
the non-specialist reader in mind, but it builds on rather than explains some of the basics of gene–
environment interactions. The basic account concludes that both genes and environment are
involved in the creation of a person’s psychosocial qualities – her character, the age at which she
will have a baby, her behaviour, intelligence, emotional characteristics, vulnerability to illness, and
so on. This essay has analysed some factors to be borne in mind when considering this subject:
biological features are not necessarily hereditary features; continuously varying features should
be distinguished from ‘categorical’ conditions; the same behavioural outcome can arise from
different gene–environment interactions; genes and environment are not two separate independent
variables (an environmental effect may be the effect of genes, and a genetic effect may be the result
of environmental conditions); the relative contribution of genes and environment to different
population averages may not be the same as that applying to differences between individuals.
Social workers are not specialists and should not claim to have technical expertise in this field;
nevertheless, they will encounter the issue – from antenatal work with people who have had
genetic counselling, to questions from parents about the origins of difficult behaviour or emotional
difficulties. Historically, social workers were involved both for and against the social manage-
ment of individuals within eugenics philosophies (Payne, 2005: 130; Kennedy, 2008), and
with approaches to mental health which underestimated the contribution of genetic factors. It is
important for them as laypeople in this field to be sufficiently well-informed so as to avoid
oversimplification.
28
Beginnings
Summary
This is an introductory chapter, starting with narratives which involved pregnancy. These highlighted
some of the ways in which individual development can be considered.
Biological knowledge, psychological understanding and social perspectives are all relevant to social
work. Often social workers need to understand the overlap between these aspects, and the mutual
influence of body, mind, emotions and society. Social constructionists emphasise that knowledge is
not absolute – it is created in specific language, and in specific social contexts.
To promote understanding of feelings, behaviour and relationships, this book will make reference to
theories which are summarised in the ‘Essential background’ section. These include stage theories
of development such as Erikson’s, psychodynamic theories, attachment theory, humanistic models,
learning theories, and various theories of ageing. A number of the perspectives used (such as
Bronfenbrenner’s and the lifespan perspective) make particular reference to social and historical
features.
The chapter concluded with a section written in a more formal academic style. It requires further
reading of ‘Essential background’, Section 1, and presented six general factors to bear in mind
when studying the relative effects of genes and environment on any specific dimension of behaviour.
These include: ‘biological variation’ is not the same as ‘genetic variation’; there can be different
gene–environment pathways to the same behavioural characteristic; genes and environment are
not independent of each other; and the gene–environment contribution causing population
differences (averages) may be different from that at work in individual variations.
Further reading
Further resources are provided on the website which accompanies this book. They include pictures and
diagrams, health guidance, a glossary of terms and a summary of research about sensory experience before
birth.
About the classic psychosocial formulation of social work:
Cooper, A. (2000) ‘Psychosocial perspectives’. One-page summary in Davies, M., Blackwell Encyclopedia of
Social Work.
About the experience of pregnancy:
Rayner et al. (2005) Human Development: An Introduction to the Psychodynamics of Growth, Maturity and
Ageing. London and New York: Routledge. Chapter 2, ‘Being pregnant’, written by Angela Joyce, is an excellent
amplification of the early parts of this chapter, about the experience of pregnancy.
Raphael-Leff, J. (2005) Psychological Processes of Childbearing. London: Anna Freud Centre. Although a
psychology (psychodynamic) account, this is written for people who work with parents and parents-to-be, and
makes excellent further reading.
29
Beginnings
Prenatal development:
Boyd, D. and Bee, H. (2006) Lifespan Development. Boston: Pearson/Allyn and Bacon, pp. 53–78. A readable
and engaging textbook set out to take the general student systematically through the subjects.
Reading about each of the theoretical approaches mentioned in the chapter will be given as the topics
are covered in more detail. Typical textbooks giving overviews or easy summaries:
Boyd, D. and Bee, H. (2006) Lifespan Development. Boston: Pearson/Allyn and Bacon; for example pp. 3–45.
Eysenck, M. (2000) Psychology: A Student’s Handbook. New York and London: Psychology Press. Chapters 14
to 18 in particular present findings from developmental psychology.
Giddens, A. (2009) Sociology, 6th edn. Cambridge: Polity Press. Various chapters present the sociologist’s view
of the impact of society on the individual.
An overview of the life course from a sociological point of view (both available online at www.esrc.ac.uk –
use the search facility to find either document):
Stewart, S. and Vaitilingam, R. (eds) (2004) Seven Ages of Man and Woman. London: ESRC.
Iacovou, M. (2004a) Life Chances: The Impact of Family Origins and Early Childhood Experiences on Adult
Outcomes. Includes two-page summary, with references.
Genetics:
Further introductory material is available through the website which accompanies this book.
?
Questions
1 Choose a situation in which social work/social care have some responsibilities to illustrate how
effective practice may require knowledge of biology, psychology and social factors.
2 What theory of human development considers that human infants are born not only with innate
instincts to feed, etc., but also with a drive to form attachments?
3 How many Y-chromosomes does a man normally have? What do you think the genetic conditions ‘XYY
syndrome’ or ‘triple X syndrome’ may be? Use a reference source to check your answer.
30
Beginnings
31
In this chapter you will find:
2 A secure base
• Development in early childhood
Sharon was right about one thing – Nicola’s husband had driven her to the hospital (and,
yes, she had attended all the classes). He was with her when their second son, Jamie,
was born.
Immediately after birth, the baby’s experience is presumably undifferentiated – a busy, technicolour,
noisy set of stimuli which makes none of the sense it makes to adults. There are no objects which exist
beyond this, let alone other people with feelings. There are sights, colours, warmth, cold, wet, light,
dark, hunger and satiation.
As Joyce (2005: 22) puts it, the dramatic discontinuity of birth for the infant must bring about a
fundamental psychological reorganisation. Our adult words gesture towards pre-verbal experience but
cannot capture it – our words are devised to describe or express a world which is very different to the
infant’s. However, we should not think of the baby as a ‘blank slate’ upon which nurture and envi-
ronment write their message, a body initially equipped only with ‘reflexes’. As a standard textbook in
infant development describes, ‘autonomy and identity form the kernel of an infant’s activities at and
even before birth’ (Smitsman, 2001: 72). The sensory experiences may be startlingly and overwhelm-
ingly unfamiliar, but the infant is using aware and constantly active systems day and night (see also
Chamberlain 1992, a scholarly but not impartial account from someone who believes passionately that
there is a neglect of prebirth and infant experience).
35
A secure base
In this chapter, we will look at some aspects of behaviour in the first two or three years of life, taking
your thoughts and feelings through the experience of the early years. At the same time, since a child’s
life is inextricably linked to that of the adults around it, we will look at the experience of adults,
particularly parents, in interacting with the young child – ‘there is no such thing as a baby,’ wrote
Donald Winnicott (1952/2007), ‘always a baby-and-someone’; and as the saying has it, ‘when a child
is born, a mother is born’.
There are a number of theories which link later adult development to early experiences. This chapter
will refer to three: Erikson’s stage model of development; Melanie Klein’s idea that later ‘states of mind’
emerge out of earlier ones; and attachment theory. So although the first part of the chapter is about
babies and young children, the later sections build on this to examine adult experience.
Observational research shows the many ways in which a baby is finely tuned to social factors from the
earliest days. For a few moments, think of the world through baby Jamie’s body: immediately after birth,
he cries less if placed on his mother’s belly than if tucked up elsewhere (Klaus and Klaus, 1998, reported
in Joyce, 2005). A first-born baby at four months is more likely to relate equally to both parents if the
mother has integrated during pregnancy the transition from being a couple to being a triad (Von Klitzing
et al., 1999 – the research relates to heterosexual couples). Babies mimic bodily movements of parents,
specifically facial expressions, soon after birth (interestingly, they later lose this capacity for a while).
The schematic pattern of a face (two dots and a line appropriately placed inside a circle) are among the
earliest visual stimuli to which babies show preferential recognition. After a few weeks, babies stopped
crying at the sight of their mothers’ faces, but not faces of others (Joyce, 2005: 34). As we shall discuss
shortly, babies and their parents (particularly mothers) regulate each others’ hormones and behaviour.
As the first year progresses, we as adults would describe the child as behaving in a more purposeful,
organised way. The baby develops expectations of what happens externally when he or she has certain
experiences or responds in certain ways. The presence of different people evokes different patterns of
behaviour.
If all goes well, the child comes to explore the world in a more coherent, purposeful way, using
established relationships with adults as a secure base. Become Jamie again, aged 18 months in a
strange house with his mother. At one point he crawls out of the front room door into the hallway.
Having had a look round, he scampers back to hook an arm round his mother’s leg, hauls himself up
to rest his head on her knee and puts his thumb in his mouth. Her presence, her relationship with him,
provides the secure base from which he can explore the world.
?
Reflective thinking
To understand this best, you should pause and think yourself into the body of Jamie – if you are
on all fours in this way, what do you see, experience physically and feel emotionally?
36
A secure base
Sometimes, the behaviour of the young child will challenge the parents. Babies and toddlers evoke fury
and all-consuming anger, as well as the fiercest of protective and love instincts, in parents. Faced with
a toddler’s obstinacy when they need to get out of the house or meet a deadline, parents will feel angry
and thwarted. If they want to comfort the child but find it inconsolable, they may feel worn out.
Finding that the child erupts into a paroxysm of contorted screaming when crossed, they may feel
furious and wonder whether they should be firm or accommodating. Their dilemma may be worsened
by advice or by the expectations of others.
Researchers are at the very early stages of penetrating the intricacies of links between brain
development and human experience and behaviour. One leading authority is Allan Schore, who
provided a short, accessible summary of his views in a foreword to a classic work by John Bowlby
(Schore, 1999). Sue Gerhardt (2004: Chapter 2) provides a readable background and explanation to the
subject, which she entitles ‘building a brain’. (You should be able to find a scholarly article on the
internet in which Schore (2001a) gives more detail.)
Schore (and Gerhardt) examine the way in which a baby ‘is an interactive project, not a self-propelled
one’. The interaction occurs constantly – in the ‘milk, poo, and dribble’, in the numerous occasions of
mutual physical coordination, and also in the quieter times when the baby’s mother sits daydreaming
with it in her arms. It occurs in tears, anger and laughter, and in talking and singing. What happens in
this ‘interactive project’? Within your body, hormones regulate affect (expression of feelings) and
37
A secure base
behaviour. The converse is also true – as another leading authority, Damasio (1997: sec. 1.1), puts it,
‘emotions are the highest order bioregulation in complex organisms’. The baby’s affect is initially
unregulated, but in its interaction with its significant carer patterns emerge. Schore writes about the
finding that the minute-by-minute levels of cortisol (a stress/arousal hormone) and noradrenalin vary
in tandem between mother and infant, each influencing the other. He calls this ‘co-regulation’
between infant and caregiver. The ‘relational context’, Schore (2001a) explains, affects the development
of the emotions, hormones and brain of both mother and baby. Breastfeeding (as well as childbirth,
physical stroking and sexual stimulation) causes major increases in the hormone oxtytocin in the
mother’s body, a hormone which results in a certain detachment from external stressors, an ‘inward’
orientation and a direction towards relaxation. It promotes daydreaming, a feeling of contentment,
and bonding. The hormone is also produced in the baby during breastfeeding. It not only produces an
emotional effect at the time and influences the developing structure of the brain, it also provides the
developing brain with chemical receptors which enable these same hormones to be better
used later in life (Pinker, 2008; Corr, 2006: 180; Gerhardt, 2004). As will be discussed in
Chapter 6, this period is immensely developmental for mother as well as child. Schore
quotes the research showing the reshaping (and ultimate improvement) of the mother’s
Chapter 6
brain in the period following birth.
Schore (2001a) goes on to discuss how in ‘good enough’ parenting, the inevitable episodes of
misalignment between the child and the caregiver are remedied in a timely fashion. He then outlines
in a second article in this series (2001b) how attachment failures which leave the infant emotionally
overwhelmed and unregulated prevent the development of resilience. They leave the regulatory
systems of the brain inadequate to the emotional challenges which will be faced later in life. As David
Howe (2005: 11–26) discusses in more detail, the process of developmental co-regulation of feelings
has a direct link with sensitive and insensitive caregiving, and therefore with social work responsibilities
for children.
Being a parent is discussed in Chapter 6; brain development and attachment are referred to again in the
concluding section of this chapter; neglect and abuse are mentioned in the next section of this chapter,
and in Chapters 3 and 7.
38
A secure base
effect on the structure of the brain which is being created during the first two years after birth. Turn now
to the second sort of knowledge referred to in Chapter 1, and consider the growth of the child’s ‘mind’.
David Howe (1995, 2005) emphasises that minds grow and develop in the context of other minds. The
baby initially has no sense of what a ‘mind’ is – their own or others. This ‘theory of mind’ will develop
by engagement with other minds. Parents who display what we may call ‘mind-mindedness’ are aware
of the child’s mind and respond accordingly. They respond to the child not as an object but as a subject
with wishes, feelings, capacity for pleasure, pain, affection and hatred. They respond to the child in
terms of its current capacity for understanding and speech and its future potential. They recognise its
need (and drive) to understand, its inner response as well as its behavioural reactions to frustration,
fear, pleasure and abandonment. Sometimes these responses involve the adult simply acting on their
natural impulses, and sometimes they involve the adult holding their own frustration (caused by the
child) in favour of expressing their deeper drive to do what is right and respond to their child’s expe-
rience. Meins (2005) followed more than 200 mothers and babies over a period of a year and a half.
She found that of all the potential predictors of development (family income, parents’ education,
maternal depression, family support and so on), the best predictor of talking, playing and other cog-
nitive development was mother’s ‘mind-mindedness’ – her ability to ‘read’ the child’s mind and respond
accordingly. Parental ‘mind-mindedness’ was found similarly to relate to the child’s development of a
‘theory of mind’, to which we turn next.
The growing child forms its idea of what a person is, and what a mind is, by its experience of this adult
mind and by what through the operation of the adult mind it discovers its own self to be. If all goes
well, the baby discovers that there is at least one significant ‘mind’ which cherishes it, thinks about it
when it is not present, is concerned about its feelings and well-being, is reliable and predictable. Its
own ‘mind’ is distinct from but in a predictable relationship to that mind – loveable, able to understand
and be understood. Experiences which we as adults would call fury or anxiety (as well as contentment)
may be all-consuming and seemingly eternal, but develop in such a way that the child finds it survives
and is kept safe.
If you take this view, it is profoundly disturbing to explore the experience of a baby for whom things
are significantly wrong. Suppose that after birth, Sharon were to come to hate the baby. When she
looks at it, she hates it for the problems it has caused her, the interference it represents in her life; the
dirt, restriction and tiredness it causes. The destructive, painful feelings provoked when the baby won’t
behave conveniently – when it cries through the night or, as a toddler, will not do as it is told – are not
moderated by feelings of love, care and responsibility. When the child relates to this parent and begins
to form a concept of what a ‘mind’ is, what does it discover? It discovers hatred towards itself, a wish
that it did not exist, a desire to harm and distress. In this environment, what does it discover its own
mind to be comprised of? Probably some satisfactions when it is on its own, but hatred, resentment,
fear and alarm when relating to a ‘significant other’.
What if one of the significant carers is violent and inflicts physical abuse? The concept of ‘mind’
discovered will depend on the individual situation, but may be such that expressing opposition causes
39
A secure base
physical retaliation and physical harm; perhaps ‘minds’ are dangerously unpredictable – friendly and
unexceptional one minute and seriously dangerous the next.
In the situation of neglect rather than abuse or rejection, the child who has the capacity to develop a
sense of mind looks to the adult to respond to its (unformulated) feelings and impulses and finds –
nothing. Where the child in need of food, comforting or stimulation should meet a responsive parental
‘self’ which allows the child to form a sense of ‘mind’, instead it meets an absence. The child needs a
context of ‘minds’ to form its own mind, but with neglectful parents, its mind has to grow in a void
(Howe, 2005: 115).
Situations of sexual abuse of young children by carers vary enormously – they may or may not be
accompanied by otherwise attentive affection and parenting, so there are many different effects on
the child’s sense of self and ‘mind’. The growing child perhaps discovers that the carer’s mind from
whom they learn will use the child for their own gratification, and in due course, create confusing
conundrums, uncertainties and paradoxes instead of providing a secure, reliable emotional base.
?
Reflective thinking
People sometimes view the job of the child protection agencies as ‘rescuing’ children from abuse,
particularly family situations of abuse. How do the previous paragraphs help to explain why even
when it is an appropriate course of action, this ‘rescuing’ and placing with carers may be only the
beginning of a very long task?
Possible answer: The emotional experiences of abuse or neglect (and their consequences) do not
disappear because the child is no longer living with abuse. Early experiences of abuse may shape the child’s
expectations and experience of the world, its emotional makeup. A child’s distrust or hostility towards
adults may make it difficult for even well-intentioned carers to care for it, so the child with special need
of stability may be faced with constant changes in placement.
Neglect and abuse are mentioned again at various points throughout the book – particularly in Chapters
3 and 7.
40
A secure base
Being a toddler
It was an early spring afternoon, but the sun beat down forcefully on the unshaded
grass, creating the atmosphere of high summer. Jamie, now 22 months, had found a
window wiper, a rubber blade on a short handle, and was swishing it vigorously over the
miniature daffodils bordering the lawn. ‘Oy, you’re daft, you!’ called his grandfather,
forcefully but affectionately, ‘That’s for windows . . . win-dows,’ the last words enun-
ciated slowly as if to mimic speech rhythms from which Jamie had learnt before. Jamie
stopped, and looked up for several seconds as his body straightened. His face was
turned towards granddad – blank, you might have thought, his eyes wide and unblinking,
apparently unfocused, his mouth open. Something was undoubtedly processed, however.
He straightened completely, turned, and walked unsteadily but purposefully, negotiating
the grass, the verge and a narrow path. His target was evidently the window of the patio
door, for, having reached it, he intently rubbed the blade of the bone-dry wiper up and
down its glass. There was a general cheer around the garden as Jamie laughed, cheerfully
and manfully continuing his work on the patio door.
A little while later, Jamie had spotted the narrow gap between the garden shed and
the retaining wall. ‘Oy, not there!’ called his grandfather, ‘You’ll get black, you’ll be filthy.
Yer mum’ll be cross!’ Jamie stopped, looked across again, this time his eyes clearly
focused on grandfather, and then he turned back to the enticing dark crack which invited
exploration. ‘Mummy poss!’ he said clearly, and then walked rapidly to the site for
exploration and clambered into the pile of mucky pots, spiders’ webs and abandoned
bamboo canes.
Had Jamie understood the meaning of the words used? Clearly he had – everyone was tickled that he
had understood what ‘windows’ meant and made a link to the function of the wiping blade. They
laughed because he had almost fooled them, choosing the very objects – patio doors – which have an
ambiguous description in adult language. And there seemed little doubt that in saying ‘mummy poss’,
he was reflecting back to granddad exactly what had meant to be conveyed. Did he want to please
adults, to obey direction? Well, in the first instance, yes, even though it disrupted his previous purposes.
But in the second, the implied direction from granddad and the prospect of mummy being cross were
no deterrent.
Not all toddlers experience exploration and learning as Jamie does. When he was that age, Sharon’s
grandfather Bob would sometimes be left entirely to his own devices, to discover which items in the
world taste good and which are bad, or which materials cut you and which are flexible. Sometimes
his enquiries could be followed to their conclusion, and at other times they would be abruptly inter-
rupted by a wallop to his head as adults stopped him doing something which they realised was
dangerous.
41
A secure base
Erikson’s view (1950/1995) was that at the stage of life just described, Jamie is gaining conscious
control of his body, becoming aware of his independent identity. All this is illustrated in Jamie’s slightly
studied and careful standing up (he’s controlling his own body, like a recently qualified driver with
a car), the beauty of his maintaining balance as he arises from a squatting position (try it with
a doll with human proportions – it’s very hard!), his reciprocating non-verbal ‘conversation’ by
performing the realistically irrelevant act of swishing the window wiper on the glass, his apparently
conscious decision a minute later to go against the adult injunction about playing behind the shed.
All contrast with earlier and later responses, but they arise from the former and will influence the
latter.
Control over walking, running and jumping – greater control over limbs and movement – is one
obvious example of increasing personal autonomy. Another which is relevant to this stage of life is
control over bowels and bladder, which removes dependence on adults for the management of these
bodily functions, another area in which the previous control of others over one’s body is left behind.
Erikson identified the experience of intentionally ‘holding on’ or ‘letting go’ as important aspects of
social interaction.
You can see that Erikson’s ideas about this phase of life knit together biological,
psychological and social factors, areas identified for study in Chapter 1. Biologically, before
the brain, nervous system, muscles and bone have reached the right level of function,
Chapter 1
there can be no ability to walk and run, to control bowels and bladder. Erikson’s con-
clusions about the component of identity which is built on the early experience of autonomy are about
psychology. And he emphasised that the actual outcomes for an individual child are dependent on
social factors – the behaviour and attitudes of adults and children around the child. Jamie’s experience,
for example, is so different from a child in an East European orphanage who lived his first two years
almost entirely in a cot with high iron sides (see Rutter et al., 2000). So too, seventy years ago, was the
experience of Sharon’s grandfather Bob, who was sat on a potty every afternoon at 2.00 pm until he
performed, after which he was put down to rest on a camp bed, like the other twenty-five children in
his day nursery. These ways of behaving towards children are in turn heavily influenced by the ideas
of prevailing ‘culture’.
42
A secure base
The processes discussed in this chapter each have implications for understanding what can cause
difficulties in early childhood development. Thinking in terms of co-regulation, or mind-mindedness
(or attachment theory, discussed shortly) gives you ideas about the hurdles some children face, as well
as the processes of productive development. For this stage of ‘toddlerhood’, Erikson summed up the
characteristic positive developmental outcome as autonomy; the alternative, the negative outcome for
the child who is made to feel dirty because he cannot control his bodily functions, or who is controlled
by adults who want to rule his life instead of letting him discover his own freedom, is shame and doubt.
For each child, the outcome of this stage will be some mixture of these characteristic qualities,
depending on how positive or negative are the social demands.
This growth of autonomy as a component of identity, and the admixture of shame and doubt depend-
ing on circumstances, will build on the outcome of earlier stages. In turn, it will provide a more or less
secure foundation for other components of identity which are characteristically embedded in later
stages. The next section of the chapter broadens out from the example of the toddler to give an
overview of Erikson’s model of all the life stages.
The previous section referred to his second stage, in which the child is developing autonomy; as
discussed, the typical problems in development at this stage are associated with shame and doubt.
Erikson considered that there are eight stages discernable in life, as outlined in Figure 2.1.
For each stage, there is a characteristic positive outcome when things go well (autonomy in the second
stage), and a characteristic impaired outcome when there are problems (shame or self-doubt in the
second stage). The drive to achieve the characteristic positive, with the danger of the damaged
outcome, is described by Erikson as a developmental crisis. For a number of thinkers, this is an
important concept – it emphasises there are psychological ‘crises’ which are entirely normal and
unavoidable, and are part of development. The first stage is early infancy. If the child has parents who
attend to its needs, the positive outcome is the achievement of a sense of basic trust in the world. The
opposite outcome, in a developmentally unsatisfactory environment, is the failure to develop a basic
sense that the world is safe – basic mistrust.
43
A secure base
Infancy Consistent care lets the child find the world safe, nurturing and reliable
Early childhood The child discovers control over its life and body, requiring parents who assist the
child to manage itself
Play age The child explores the world in its own way
School age Acquisition of systematic knowledge and a sense of valued achievement
Adolescence The stage of forming an independent adult identity
Young adulthood A search for intimate and lasting relationships
Adulthood A time of productivity and creativity, in family and work
Old age The period when the whole of life can be reviewed and integrated
Figure 2.1
Erikson’s psychosocial stages
The model emphasises that developmental outcomes are social as well as emotional in nature, and
depend on the social and cultural context as well as inbuilt patterns of development. Erikson therefore
described his model as a psychosocial model.
Refer to the technical summary in ‘Essential background’, section 6 for more detail.
Erikson’s stages will be referred to in appropriate chapters throughout the book. Two
Chapter
further examples are his description of the fifth stage of ‘adolescence’ and the eighth
EB6 stage of ‘old age’. He sees the positive outcome of adolescence as a coherent adult identity,
more independent than previously from the influence of parents and teachers, and the
negative outcome is role confusion. In old age he sees the positive outcome as a sense of completeness
and integration, an ability to look back over the whole of life, and the potential negative outcome as
despair – a giving up on life as meaningless and without value.
Erikson’s psychosocial stages are referred to in Chapters 3, 4, 6 and 8, as well as in ‘Essential background’,
section 6.
‘States of mind’
The next sections introduce psychoanalytic ideas about development, beginning by emphasising the
insights that come from careful attention to ‘states of mind’ from the first days of infancy through to
the end of life.
Some aspects of development, such as changing height, increasing muscular strength and even
intellectual performance, can be specified in quite measurable terms. On the other hand, your changing
44
A secure base
experience of life, the emotions you feel and how they develop over time, the interplay of inner
maturation and external influence, is harder to quantify. ‘States of mind’ is a term used to draw
attention to emotional and relationship aspects of developing experience.
Margot Waddell’s book Inside Lives (Waddell, 2002) is referred to several times in the coming pages.
She gives an account of our changing and developing ‘states of mind’ from infancy through to the last
stages of life – ‘an attempt to trace the unfolding of the inside story, of the inner life of a person, the
ways in which he or she may become more or become less capable of having their own experience’.
She draws on her experience as a psychoanalytic therapist and her thought is influenced especially by
the ideas of Melanie Klein and Wilfred Bion.
Present-day states of mind emerge and grow out of earlier states of mind
Each state of mind emerges seamlessly from those before it. A psychoanalytic understanding empha-
sises how, in some way which is not easy to specify, present-day states of mind still contain echoes of
earlier states of mind. Waddell quotes the poet T. S. Eliot: ‘Time present and time past are both perhaps
in time future’ (‘East Coker’ from Four Quartets). To a degree – just how far you will make your own
mind up about – your experiences in the present are made from the same fabric as earlier states of
mind, and what you experience now is part of what will shape your experience in the future.
Think of two brief examples of Jamie’s behaviour given earlier in the chapter – at 11 months he crawled
out of a strange living room and then scuttled back to the safety of his mother’s knee; at 22 months
in his grandfather’s garden he first conforms to adult guidance as it leads him to understand the
external world (and receives recognition and enjoyment from the adults), and then goes against adult
guidance in order to continue explorations further (and is met with good-humoured management by
the adults). Both of these are part of a consistent pattern of experiences, and in psychodynamic terms
they lay the basis for later states of mind in which competent protective figures can be relied on,
exploration and learning can be enjoyable, and responsible adult figures protect without being
aggressive. In psychoanalytic terms, a father responding to his child’s needs will be drawing on ‘states
of mind’ that have origins in his own childhood; how he was dealt with when he disobeyed his father’s
requests, and so on.
Sharon’s grandfather Bob, on the other hand, was brought up in the 1930s in an impoverished and
very strict environment where the child’s place, his duty, was to obey adults. In areas where they could
exert control, adult caregivers regulated much of the child’s life, even including his bowel movements.
On his own, he had perhaps more freedom than many children today, and less interaction with his
parents. But when with adults, behaviour which was not sanctioned by them, particularly
‘disobedience’ or expressions of resentment, was met with punishment which hurt (and
of course provoked the urge to complain further). As he explains in his own way (see
Chapter Chapter 9), these attitudes were influencing him as he tried to deal with his ‘rebellious’
9
daughter Bella (Sharon’s mother). Difficult childhood experiences do not predict the
45
A secure base
future – Bob himself could have rebelled against these attitudes and determined never to inflict them
on his own children. It would be no surprise to a psychoanalytically minded listener, however, to hear
Bob describe how in dealing with his daughter he couldn’t get away from the underlying feeling that
it was dangerous to allow a child, particularly a girl, to express rebellious feelings and to be disobedient
with impunity. It is often at times of stress that the least resolved aspects of earlier feelings are felt –
as abandonment, rebellion, unconstrained violence or desolation. It is important that social workers
understand the power of these primitive feelings.
Social workers are human, and when they deal with the very emotive human problems which are the
day-to-day material of their work – mothers overwhelmed by their children, physical or sexual abuse
of vulnerable people, dangerous and uncontrollable adolescents, mental health problems and intense
family conflict, as well as friction within their work environment – their ‘states of mind’ in the present
may have awakened highly charged elements which date from ‘states of mind’ much earlier in their
lives.
These unconscious elements may not follow adult logic. You may recall that in Chapter 1,
a component of Sharon’s expectations of her baby may have been that it will be the one
person who will unconditionally love her, who will be hers – a baby content in its mother’s
Chapter 1
arms or playing happily with her is the very symbol of love. But in reality the baby can’t
meet these needs. The baby will be totally self-centred: will make demands in the middle of the night,
will sometimes scream and not be pacified, will reject Sharon when she tries to be close to it. Perhaps
Sharon’s state of mind in this situation will contain those components which derive from all the other
times when she should have been loved unconditionally and was met with rejection, complaint, self-
centredness and hostility. If you think about this, you will realise that, paradoxically, Sharon as a parent
is looking for a baby to meet the needs in her which should have been met by her own parents. This,
of course, is a flashpoint for temper tantrums vented on the baby – child abuse.
As it happens, this also provides the workers with the clearest indication of what they need to provide
in order to prevent child abuse – it is to meet Sharon’s own needs for care and affection. A residential
worker from her last accommodation may well say this without any theorising: ‘She’s still a child – she
needs looking after and loving . . . my worry is when she’s frustrated, she goes into a fury, just like a
46
A secure base
little child.’ One of the early workers in modern child protection put it like this: ‘Most of the mothers
in our study talked quite openly about my giving them the mothering they had never had before’
(Davoren, 1974: 145; see also Chapman and Woodmansey, 1985: 3). This ‘looking after’, this ‘mothering’
in Davoren’s words, does not mean intruding. In fact, as will be mentioned later in the chapter, it is
quite likely that one of Sharon’s ‘resilience’ factors is her independence. The affection and uncon-
ditional care she is entitled to should value, respect and enjoy this quality in her. The mind-mindedness
she needs requires someone to hold her in mind even when she is not physically present
– someone who does not abandon her, who thinks about her and lets her know they are
thinking about her, someone who is there when she does need someone to confide in. This
Chapters is discussed again in Chapters 6 and 7, which refer to first-hand accounts given by young
6 and 7
mothers in difficulty.
Waddell emphasises that to acknowledge the power in the present of earlier states of mind is not
determinist – everyone has a drive for development, and although unresolved emotional responses to
adverse circumstances ‘may imprison him within a regressive or self-protective mode’, they may
alternatively be ‘part of a holding operation, relaxed in the light of later more positive experiences’. She
goes on ‘Development . . . runs unevenly’ (2002: 4).
Returning to the lack of intellectual logic in the components of a ‘state of mind’, one might well say
that the different components follow the ‘logic of emotions’, but even this would seem suspiciously
organised to some psychoanalysts. They would emphasise that in the unconscious mind you can hate
and harm a person one moment and then expect them to be totally loving towards you the next; you
are entitled to lie to and deceive someone as much as you like but they are expected always to be
honest to you; you can bite and cut people, both out of love and hate.
And we can note in passing that these ideas can be applied to each social worker’s development, to
their states of mind in their work. The fact that they are being professional and responsible in their
work does not mean that they too do not have other active parts to their state of mind. Their sense
of wanting to be helpful may be linked with a wish to gain approval from the person they are
working with – not a motivation they are acting out, but nevertheless a real component of their state
of mind which may exert a pressure to do or say certain things. And many young social workers in
their first appointments at a headteacher’s office to discuss a child may feel school experiences come
flooding back!
This use of ‘states of mind’ recurs throughout the book – for example at the end of Chapter 3, discussing
unrealistic guilt and self-attack; in Chapter 4 about adolescence; in Chapter 6 about sex; and in Chapter
9 about death and bereavement.
47
A secure base
48
A secure base
destroy the very object of its total affections. These states of mind are found later in life, as people like
to have heroes who are all good and villains who are all bad. Many analytically inclined people would
see echoes of infantile ‘splitting’ in the enjoyment of the clearly presented ‘goodies’ and ‘baddies’ in
fairy tales and films. Mothers will sometimes joke, ‘Oh, I’m the wicked witch today, Cruella de Ville –
can’t do anything right.’ The child is convinced the mother is totally against it, is deliberately out to
refuse it what it wants at every turn. It is much harder to tolerate and sort out how to proceed in a
relationship in which the other person is sometimes good and sometimes bad – adults and adolescents
in difficult relationships feel consciously and acutely the dilemma first felt by the infant.
From this point of view, the ability to tolerate loving and hating the same person, to be depressed about
that situation, to feel guilt and to want to make reparation are important processes. On the other hand,
to have to avoid these processes at all costs, to defend against having to deal with them (for example,
by idealising loved people and vilifying problem people), is an emotional restriction to living a full life.
From this psychological perspective too, in which adult logic has not yet made itself felt, the impulses
to harm and destroy are not clearly distinguished from the actions of harming and destroying. We see
this confusion still at work in adult states of mind, when people think they are bad for feeling
ungrateful, or angry with their children.
In this tradition of developmental understanding, other important ideas include those about ‘being held’
and ‘being held in mind’. The fragmentation referred to in the last paragraph – of feelings of love and
hate, of people being in parts, needs somehow to be counteracted. Melanie Klein’s view is that
the infant makes no distinction (as we can do as adults) between the ‘bodily’ facts and ‘psychological’
facts. Much of what we as adults would describe as psychological features are experienced by the child
simply as experiences of ‘themselves’ – body or mind or whatever (we are back again to the lack of words
to use for early experience). Physical holding may play an important part in enabling the ‘person’ to be
held (and later on, the emotionally responsive parent or social worker may ‘hold’ the person psycho-
logically without physically holding them). The sensitive parent ‘holds the child in mind’ even when
they’re not physically together (if at work or in college you ask mothers what their children are doing,
many will tell you straight away, and will say what the plans for the child are for the next few hours).
It is this holding in mind which enables the child progressively to develop a life away from parents.
Many people who have not had an adequate experience of parents holding them in mind need to have
this experience later in life in order to feel integrated and safe and competent in the world. One of the
features of good social work is that workers often demonstrate by their responses to, and actions on
behalf of, service users that they are doing this. Unfortunately, institutional services often fail to
provide this holding in mind, or may neglect the implications for staff. It is a process rarely recognised
or expected in other occupations, and it will not show up in measurable targets or job descriptions.
Wilfred Bion’s view is that the sensitive parent (or staff caring for disturbed or overwhelmed people)
acts as a ‘container’ for the emotions which are beyond the coping abilities of the person they care for.
Staff or carers who are appropriately providing this function are likely to feel preoccupied by the
welfare of those for whom they are responsible and are entitled to support that will enable them to
leave their work worries behind.
49
A secure base
The importance of staff support is referred to again in Chapter 4 (references to work with adolescence);
Chapter 6 (about sex); Chapter 9 (about death and dying); and Chapter 10.
In general, the ideas discussed so far relate to the very early experiences which persist as states of
mind. Many would be seen as explorations of the first stage of psychological development as Sigmund
Freud (1905/1991) understood it. He considered that the states of mind in this first stage relate to
gratification, pleasure and frustration which come through the mouth (his ‘oral stage’). The echoes of
these states of mind in adulthood are understood to be features of life such as comfort eating, sexual
kissing, and a whole range of impulses including those expressed in the mother’s words to her baby,
‘I could eat you all up!’ The next stage of childhood as he understood it was associated with pleasure
or control exerted around defecation and bowel movements. The later states of mind associated with
this stage are the pleasure in being messy, or the inability to keep things from becoming messy, or a
need to control everything and a fear of ‘letting go’. Apparently it is common in cultures throughout
the world for the terms associated with holding on to things, being mean, to be like our slang ‘tight-
arsed’ or the more polite term derived from Freud’s ideas, ‘anal retentive’.
Freud’s view was that boys were faced with a struggle as to how to detach from their identification
with the mother as love object and instead identify with the father. He linked this to the boy realising
that he could not have the mother for himself because he had a rival, his father; finding ultimately
that he could never overcome his bigger and more powerful rival, the boy resolves the situation not
by getting rid of his father but by identifying with him, a defensive process described as ‘identifying
with the aggressor’. He saw this as a necessary stage in development, after which the boy could allow
his developing sexual energies to be associated with his penis (but not as in an adult sexual relationship
– simply as the focus of his sexual energy). After this, internal struggles could lie dormant (‘latency
period’) until the emergence of full genital sexuality in which sexual energies are invested in other
people.
To make more sense of these ideas of Freud, that psychosexual stages are at the centre of psychological
development, it is worth understanding two concepts in rather more depth. Freud, and the later
psychoanalytic tradition based on his ideas, regards the driver for development to be a psychological
force which is present at the very beginning in the baby’s urge to feed and develop, and later in the
drive to procreate and reproduce. His view of ‘sexuality’ is much broader than the simple idea of genital
sexuality. ‘He sees it not just as an animal instinct but as specific to human culture and the form of
conscious and unconscious life we lead within it . . . it is not one drive but a compound of many
“component instincts”’ (Minsky, 1996). This force he saw as developing over time, changing its source,
its aim and its object, finally arriving at the form of adult sexuality. This cluster of forces, drives and
instincts he called ‘libido’. Second, it is important to recognise his conclusion that much of what goes
on in psychological life is unconscious, possibly such that it can never actually be brought into
consciousness, only ever inferred.
50
A secure base
The basic ideas of psychoanalytic theory are summarised in ‘Essential background’, section 4.
Key points
• Present states of mind contain elements deriving from the past.
• Kleinian psychoanalytic approaches regard the forces which shape emotional development
to derive from the conflicts around destructive and loving feelings. They place emphasis
on guilt and self-destructive impulses.
• Freud’s psychoanalytic work focused on libido and sexuality as the forces which have to
be managed by the growing person and society.
• Impulses and drives are as likely to be unconscious as conscious.
Both these psychoanalytic thinkers emphasise the centrality of basic bodily experience in our ‘psychic’
life. Both, too, and particularly Melanie Klein, are important for insisting that adult states of mind
which embody, reflect, or echo early infantile experience contain non-verbal and non-verbalisable
experiences. As a social worker, it is often important to recognise this – if you expect someone to put
their feelings accurately into words you may be expecting the impossible. To ask someone, ‘What are
you feeling?’ or, ‘What did you feel about that?’ may be a sign that you are not in touch
with their state of mind. It is quite possible for you to be in tune with their experiences
(as a mother can with her baby) and to demonstrate this attunement, but the
communication will not necessarily be through words. This is evident in examples
Chapters 4
and 9 throughout the book, such as those in Chapters 4 and 9.
About yourself
51
A secure base
When you have finished the picture, it can help to write quickly on the back any words which come up
for you in connection with it.
Suggested questions:
• Every picture tells a story – I wonder if there is a story to tell about this one?
• I wonder what s/he likes to do especially? Is there anything in particular s/he’s proud of?
• The child who’s drawn this, or the person who’s drawn, like everyone, has probably had tough times.
What might worry him/her?
• Does this prompt any thoughts about Melanie Klein’s concept of states of mind and their per-
sistence/evolution through life?
Attachment – an introduction
In developing her ideas, Melanie Klein focused very much on the inner world of the child. Waddell
(2002) describes this as recognising that the mind is ‘a kind of internal theatre, a theatre for generating
the meaning of external experiences’. Winnicott (1979: 177) believed she became capable of exploring
this internal drama of the pre-verbal infant only by being ‘temperamentally incapable’ of allowing the
importance of the real external environment. John Bowlby took psychoanalytic thinking in a very
different direction from Klein’s speculations about the unknowable unconscious mental life of the
infants.
Bowlby’s work started from the experiences of children in institutions, recognising that however well
fed and clothed they were, what they were often missing was ‘mother love’ from whoever was to
provide it – whether the birth mother, a nanny, a foster carer or a widowed father. His work sixty-five
years ago set off a tradition of research that tries to combine statistical and observational studies with
standardised testing to explore the needs of children for attachment. This perspective started within
psychoanalysis and is still combined with psychoanalytic approaches by many professionals, but is also
an independent tradition in psychology, not necessarily linked with psychoanalysis. There is a very large
body of recent research about attachment theory in its various forms.
In current attachment theory, attachment behaviour is seen as a biological instinct. An attachment
relationship exists when one person responds in such a way that these attachment behaviours achieve
their purpose. In childhood, ‘proximity to an attachment figure is sought when the child senses or
perceives threat or discomfort’ (Prior and Glaser 2006: 17). This proximity is expected to relieve the
52
A secure base
threat or discomfort. When there is no threat or discomfort, the attachment figure provides a secure
base from which the world can be explored. The attachment system remains active throughout life: in
adulthood, people may be mutual and reciprocal attachment figures.
Many attachment theorists regard various related features as comprising an ‘attachment system’. There
is also an ‘exploratory system’ which drives exploration and enquiry physically, intellectually and
socially (that is, in relationships). In the examples given earlier in the chapter, Jamie’s exploratory
systems are at work when he crawls out of the living room to see what’s outside in the hallway and
beyond, and also when he’s attracted to explore the dark corner behind the shed; by contrast his
attachment system came into play in the earlier example when, realising he’s not sure what might be
ahead, he scurries back to hold on to his mother’s knee, his secure base. The attachment system is
constantly at work in the background, using the secure base effect and scanning the environment in
case attachment behaviours are needed. In this view, attachment behaviours are incompatible with the
activities of the exploratory system. So, for example, if attachment needs are unmet (if a child is newly
placed with foster parents, for example, and has not yet become confident in them as attachment
figures), the child may not settle to school work which requires the active application of exploratory
systems. The child’s systems will be searching for the secure base and will be activated (realistically or
not) to get out of the current situation.
The quality of attachment is said to be represented in the child by an internal working model. This,
contains behavioural (not necessarily verbal) answers to questions such as: is the attachment figure
usually available when needed?; is the child likeable and valued so that the attachment figure normally
wants to protect and reassure?; and is the relationship reliable? Attachment theorists believe that this
‘working model’ the child develops is influenced particularly by the degree of attunement which the
attachment figure shows towards the child – the degree to which the adult is in tune with the child’s
state from moment to moment. You will see that this has obvious links to the ideas of ‘co-regulation’
and ‘mind-mindedness’ discussed earlier in the chapter.
The internal working model develops over time depending on actual experiences – this is one of the
research topics in attachment theory – but is heavily influenced by early attachment relationships (or
their absence), whose impact may be hard to change.
Before reading the essay below, you should review ‘Essential background’, section 2 and
be sure that you understand it.
Chapters
EB2
53
A secure base
TAKING IT FURTHER
Understanding attachment
Background
Attachment theory emerged from psychoanalysis out of a resolve to ground developmental theory
on a more ‘scientific’ basis (Bowlby 1979).
This section offers an overview of attachment: it summarises attachment as viewed as a form of
animal behaviour; it outlines the biological processes which have been linked with attachment; it
expands on the inner psychological experiences of attachment; it discusses attachment through the
life course; and it discusses attachment as a social construct. It concludes with the answers given
within attachment theory to four commonly asked questions: Is there a critical period during which
a child must form attachments? Does attachment research indicate that those who don’t form
attachments are doomed to be antisocial or does deprivation of childhood attachment cause mental
illness – in other words, is attachment theory deterministic? Does attachment research and attach-
ment theory hold that there is a single essential attachment (to the mother) and that the constant
presence of the mother in childhood is essential?
54
A secure base
Schore lists in detail the relevant areas of brain development that are at their peak in the first two
years after birth. Amongst many others, these include the organisation of the cortical limbic areas
(Schore, 2001b: 217) and the right hemisphere of the brain (pp. 231–236). He describes what is
known about the effects of relational stress on the biological development of these brain structures,
and goes on to describe the effects of physiological impairment on personal functioning. Damage
to the orbital prefrontal limbic system, for example, ‘disrupts the behaviours of social bonding, and
causes failure to acquire complex social knowledge’, and efficient functioning of the right hemi-
sphere ‘plays a predominant role in the physiological and cognitive component of social processing’.
He emphasises that attachment trauma is not an isolated incident, identifying the evidence that it
is a continuing failure of the conditions for adequate brain development. Bringing the different
studies together, he argues strongly that developmental neuroscience now offers explanatory path-
ways linking early failures in attachment functioning (and abuse) with impaired brain development
associated with later problems in such areas as regulating emotion, cause–effect thinking, and
recognising emotions in others. As life proceeds, the individual is likely to be less able to articulate
their own emotions, may have a less coherent self and autobiographical history, and may appear
to have a lack of conscience.
Resilience
However, although some children undoubtedly suffer long-term effects of abuse and neglect, others
develop relatively unscathed. An important area of research has been the identification of factors
which lead a child to thrive after abuse. These are generally categorised (see Howe, 2005) as:
• individual characteristics including intelligence, humour, and creativity;
• family and social support such as external positive support care;
• community well-being and stability.
55
A secure base
This ‘resilience’ research, and its translation into practice, is important because many childcare
reports and studies have focused on ‘risk’ rather than resilience, and this can easily lead to unbal-
anced childcare practice. Newman (2002) reviews the research literature on resilience, and points
out that three forms are generally identified: qualities which cause children not to be affected even
though they are otherwise in a ‘risk’ group; coping strategies created by children who cope satis-
factorily with chronic stress; and features which enable children to develop positively even though
they have experienced trauma which leads to damaging outcomes for other children. Good
attachment relationships in general create resilience against many later adversities (Cicchetti and
Rogosch, 1997; specifically in relation to later mental health difficulties, see Svanberg, 1998).
However, where these are missing, for example when children have been abused or have been
brought up in care with multiple and impermanent caregivers, some children display resilience
against the adverse effects. Howe (2005: 219) points out that the resilience factors for these chil-
dren seem to cluster more around the individual characteristics such as independence rather than
the external social factors which foster resilience in the non-maltreated children. This means, for
example, that in developing work with someone such as Sharon (the example used in this book)
much effort should be put into supporting and valuing her evident qualities of independence of
spirit and self-sufficiency, as well as supporting her use of potential attachment figures and allow-
ing the creation of supportive social networks.
56
A secure base
Physical or
Relaxation of psychological need
Tension (hunger, cold, alarm,
etc.)
Security
Trust
Attachment
State of high
Satisfaction arousal
of need (rage, etc.)
Figure 2.2
Fahlberg’s arousal–relaxation cycle
attachment figure, and does not expect to use the attachment figure to resolve distress.
‘Disorganised attachment’ relates to behaviour and an internal working model which has no
consistency.
One of the early observations made by attachment researchers was that separation from the
attachment figure, if not properly attended to, resulted in a characteristic sequence of responses:
• protest
• resignation
• detachment.
In the first phase, protest is designed to bring the attachment figure back. In the phase of resig-
nation, the attachment relationship is considered still to be psychologically present, but the child
has given up on the possibility of bringing the attachment figure to them. In the third phase,
‘detachment’, the child is considered to have shut down its attachment systems and to have
separated psychologically from the attachment figure. These phases were originally recognised in
institutional care, and the remedial focus of the work (Bowlby and Robertson, 1953) was first to
draw people’s attention to the existence of these phases and to demonstrate that the sequence was
not inevitable if the child’s attachment needs were attended to. At the time of this early work on
attachment, ‘detachment’ in institutions such as hospitals or residential schools was commonly
identified as ‘settling down’, because the protests stopped; and the subsequent lack of consideration
for other people, the affectionless response, was attributed to various personal failings of the child.
57
A secure base
This links to a more general feature of attachment theory as a social construction. Necessarily, the
terms the theory uses have their meaning in relation to other terms in a particular culture at a
particular time. Attachment is centrally concerned with relations between children and their
mothers, fathers, grandparents and other carers. Burman (2000: 77) finds it indisputable that
children need ‘warm continuous and stable relationships’, but argues that the interpretations of
this in attachment theory form part of a patriarchal system of organisation designed to define
58
A secure base
women by their child-bearing, ensure their economic subjugation and keep them away from paid
employment. In a similar vein, others have argued that the theory is based on idealising a particular
‘atomised’ version of family arrangement – mother and father living in a home independently of
other people. This form of family is associated by critics of the theory with white English-speaking
patterns. By setting this up as the ‘standard’ form it is argued that the theory ensures that other
childcare arrangements – such as those in which an African ‘village’ as a whole is said to rear its
children – are defined as inadequate. In summary, the theory is said by these critics to incorporate
terms and meanings that arise essentially in a sexist or racist culture.
Attachment theory is a hypothesis that links various observations. The previous three paragraphs
have referred to its possible links with social practices beyond the factual study of human devel-
opment. Within the formal and rigorous study of development, it is well established as a model,
but there are disputes. One way of summarising these is that various different aspects of social
influence are grouped together into one entity (‘attachment’). In this view, social influence on brain
development, responses to separation from loved people, the need for continuing relationships, the
variety of problems in institutional upbringing, possible continuities of attitude between childhood
experience and parenting, parents’ feelings towards their children, and so on, are amalgamated
into a single feature. Meins and colleagues (2003) find that ‘attachment’ as a scientific construct
lacks discriminative ability: it has too many features rolled into one, and current research is better
separating them out and examining them independently (this is illustrated by her own research
(2005) which specifies the behaviour that displays parental mind-mindedness and examines its
effects). Some, for different reasons from those who regard the theory as privileging Eurocentric
or patriarchal society, would nevertheless argue that constructing ‘attachment’ in this way leads to
a neglect of other approaches to child-rearing and other significant influences. Harris (2009), for
example, argues that there is an overemphasis on the relationship with parents and insufficient
attention to the influence of peers. Thompson (2005) edited a collection of papers in which
researchers presented both sides of this debate. His introduction (2005: 102) is a summary of the
issues involved.
Attachment is a widely researched area. Although the critics may present their ideas as undermining
its foundations, many of the issues on which they focus are regarded by others as areas for attach-
ment theory to explore. Indeed, the research interests of the critics are often ‘congenial to the
concerns of attachment theory’ (Thompson, 2005: 102). Cassidy and Shaver (1999) present authori-
tative summaries of research about areas such as gender differences in attachment, differences in
attachment to fathers and mothers, the nature of ties to day carers, cross-national comparisons of
attachment patterns, attachment patterns in group child-rearing, attachment to people who are not
physically present (such as grandparents who live in the parents’ country of origin) and so on.
Questions
Here are some commonly asked questions about attachment theory and the answers as given by
attachment theorists (in drawing up this summary, I have made particular use of Sroufe, 1988:
22–27):
59
A secure base
in relating to others which underlie both mental illness and socially problematic behaviour were
seen as a consequence arising from the need for attachment not being satisfied. This need was
presented as like any other essential for healthy development (such as vitamins in the right
quantities). The children were seen as needing the experience of ‘mother love’, whoever was to
provide it; whether the birth mother, a nanny, a foster carer or a widowed father. The attachment
relationship was not seen as something provided only by biological mothers, but as something that
mothers do normally provide. The absence of this experience (of devoted caring) was described as
‘maternal deprivation’. Increased clarity came with the analysis that children in barren institutional
settings were deprived both of a continuing devoted affectional relationship and of a host of other
inputs – play materials, appropriate verbal stimulation, encouragement and so on. They also suffered
a range of destructive inputs – punishment, harsh and depersonalising regimes, poor food. Further
analysis emphasised the distinction between the loss or serious interruption to an existing
relationship (as when a child is in hospital or residential school) and the experience of never having
a particular affectional relationship (both were originally termed ‘maternal deprivation’).
Is there a critical phase when attachments must develop or the baby is harmed?
Attachment is not a ‘critical period’ theory. Children who have had poor early attachment
experiences are quite capable of developing secure attachments later. There is, however, a phase
when the child’s systems are at their most ready for developing attachment, and research shows that
there tends to be continuity over time in the attachment patterns laid down (Prior and Glaser, 2006).
Later attachments are more difficult if early experiences are adverse. An example of the flexibility
of attachment systems is shown in Steele and colleagues’ findings (2003): that in a study of sixty-
three ‘late-adopted’ children there was a strong tendency for the attachment style of the child to
change to become like that of their adopted parents, and less like those of their birth parents.
Is attachment theory deterministic – if attachments are not formed does this cause mental health
problems later?
Disrupted attachments and destructive behaviour within attachment relationships are significant
risk factors for certain later problems. These include antisocial behaviour, difficulties in bringing
up children and mental health problems. But there is no simple cause and effect relationship.
Where attachment difficulties are regularly associated with these problems, so also are problems
such as childhood poverty, lack of consistent intellectual attention, disruption to living arrange-
ments, and adverse adult and peer cultures. Severely institutionalised children such as those in
East European orphanages have suffered numerous other adversities – neglect, abuse, malnutrition
and lack of stimulation – alongside the deprivation of an attachment relationship.
The children adopted in England from these institutions have been systematically studied by the
English and Romanian Adoptees Study Team, headed by Michael Rutter (2001; Rutter et al, 2000).
Among their findings is evidence that better outcomes are associated with shorter initial exposure
to institutional regime, and earlier adoption.
60
A secure base
Conclusion
‘Attachment’ has a natural appeal to workers in many areas of social work. It highlights an area of
need which can otherwise be neglected in favour of more concrete and observable services and it
helps social workers understand the development and responses of people who have had disruptive
attachment experiences. It provides a particular perspective on the relationship provided by the
social worker. However, whilst it is invariably helpful in providing a framework for positive inter-
ventions, its use has been criticised as having hidden ideological roots and as being scientifically
debatable. Great caution should therefore be exercised before using it in processes which are
negatively intrusive on someone’s life, particularly if its use involves claims to ‘expert’ evaluation
not otherwise available (such as using a supposed expert evaluation of the quality of attachment
to justify the removal of children).
Summary
This chapter initially focused attention on infancy and early childhood.
It referred to brain growth, which continues at least until the third year, and emphasised the
research which shows that emotional interaction is a key component in shaping brain development.
These ideas were presented using concepts from attachment theory – asking you to think about the
need for a ‘secure base’ and about the child’s working model of ‘minds’ and of relationships.
The example of a toddler drew attention to the increased sense of control over the body which is
experienced after early infancy, and referred to Erikson’s naming of this sense of autonomy as a
characteristic of this stage of life.
This led to an account of the framework, using the growth of ‘identity’, which Erikson used to make
sense of human development through the course of life. He identified eight key stages, in each of
which there is a characteristic challenge or ‘crisis’.
Next, the chapter considered another set of ideas which highlight how early experiences influence
later development – Melanie Klein’s psychoanalytic concept of ‘states of mind’.
The concluding section took the basic account of attachment in Essential background,
section 2 further by setting it in its contexts. These contexts are: animal behaviour;
Chapter
biology; psychological experience; attachment through the life course; and its inter-
EB2 action with society.
61
A secure base
Further reading
There are many suitable textbooks giving more detail about development in early childhood. These texts
also include more detail from a generalist (not a social work) point of view about psychoanalytic theory,
Erikson’s model and attachment theory. The following, also referred to at the end of Chapter 1, are written
in an accessible and engaging style:
Bee, H. and Boyd, D. (2007) The Developing Child, 11th ed. Boston, New York, London: Pearson, chapter 3, pp. 63–91
Boyd, D. and Bee, H. (2006) Lifespan Development. Boston: Pearson/Allyn and Bacon.
Eysenck, M. W. (2000) Psychology: A Student’s Handbook. Hove, UK: Psychology Press, pp. 380–407.
The following stimulating and thought-provoking books, written for readers with a more specific interest
and experience in the helping professions, are accounts of development using the models discussed in this
chapter:
Howe, D. (2005) Child Abuse and Neglect: Attachment, Development and Intervention, 1st edn. Basingstoke:
Palgrave Macmillan.
Gerhardt, S. (2004) Why Love Matters: How Affection Shapes a Baby’s Brain. Hove: Brunner-Routledge.
Rayner, E., Joyce, A., Rose, J., Twyman, M. and Clulow, C. (2005) Human Development: An Introduction to the
Psychodynamics of Growth, Maturity and Ageing, 4th edn. London and New York: Routledge.
Waddell, M. (2002) Inside Lives. London: Duckworth (Tavistock Clinic series).
On the internet:
www.netmums.com.
62
A secure base
?
Questions
Read the following, which is the summary of a real situation, and answer the questions that
follow.
In a fit of temper, Mandy pushed her first child Freida (in a buggy) into the road, in the path of a
speeding car. Fortunately, the subsequent episode at the hospital revealed only minor injuries, but this
was the culminating incident in a series of problems and Freida was taken into care. Mandy had
consistently refused to talk constructively to any of the social services; she was exploited by men,
erratic in her lifestyle and had an explosive temper. She first came to the voluntary organisation for
which you work, providing assistance for families, a fortnight after Frieda was removed from her care.
Mandy was angry, suspicious and non-communicative. Freida, her first child, was later adopted,
against Mandy’s wishes.
Over the subsequent year, Mandy revealed a little more about her life, and it became clear that she
had been abused and rejected from an early age. Three years later she became pregnant with her
second child. There were many worries and dilemmas for the social workers and health service staff
who were involved, but she kept the child and continued to use social work help. She was by then
more able both to express her anger with staff and to seek help from them about the practical,
relationship and child-rearing problems she faced.
1 What are the impulses of the mother (Mandy) to which attachment theory draws attention?
2 What are her needs?
3 What may be the role of the social worker in relation to her impulses and needs?
4 Can the social worker meet attachment needs without emotional involvement?
5 Discuss the attachment needs of Freida, Mandy’s daughter.
continued
63
A secure base
3 In relation to Mandy, a social worker offering help must understand that independently of the
needs of her daughter, Mandy is in a very threatening situation. She needs to be allowed to find
(or be offered) a reliable relationship with someone who will provide her with what an attachment
relationship should have offered much earlier in her life: genuine care about her emotional state, a
concern to protect her, and a secure base when she is confronted with emotional challenges.
4 Competent help in the situation is almost bound to involve the feelings of the social worker – of
concern about the child, and possibly anger towards the mother because of what she has done; of
concern about the mother's needs; possibly of anger in response to unreasonable behaviour from the
mother towards the social worker. If progress is to be made with the mother, the situation will elicit
attachment and care about her. This will necessarily involve the social worker's emotions. It will be
difficult because of the contradictory emotional demands in the situation. The required response will
involve (a) feelings (b) thinking (c) actions - and none of the three elements can be bypassed.
5 The needs of Freida, Mandy's daughter, are for care and effective protection through which her
physical, social and emotional needs can be met. This includes attention to her attachment needs, but
is likely to present difficulties for carers because of her earliest experiences. A student with some
knowledge of the childcare system may refer to the challenges which lie ahead - of the multiple
changes of carer likely once the state takes responsibility; of the destructive delays and compromises
which will occur; perhaps they will refer to 'concurrent planning', the system of childcare in which
temporary foster carers can become adoptive parents if the child is unable to return home.
64
In this chapter you will find:
3 The developing
child
• Middle childhood: ‘the play age’
As the child moves out of the toddler stage, she consciously creates her own world out of the reality
of the environment. In Erikson’s terms (1950/1995), she is in a third stage, the ‘play age’ in which she
has the experience of acting effectively on the outside world. Observing this with a slightly different
perspective, attachment researchers note that her attachment relationships are the result of a two-
way influence, in which her use of the attachment figure is consciously proactive, using and attempting
to change the individual personal qualities of different attachment figures according to whether they
are assets or problems for her. She modifies the attachment figure’s behaviour, as they
modify hers. As Bronfenbrenner points out (1979/2006 – see ‘Essential background’,
section 3) the individual is part of a system in which each part influences others, thereby
Chapter creating ‘feedback loops’. That is, to a degree, the mother who interacts with the child is
EB3
the mother the child’s interactions have created.
67
The developing child
From the many topics relevant to childhood, this chapter introduces the following: the development
of thinking processes (‘cognitive development’); behavioural learning; schooling/education; abuse and
its effects. The chapter concludes with a more detailed look at the development of guilt, conscience
and morality.
Cognitive development
How does thinking develop?
When Sharon’s mother, Bella, was a newborn baby, the barking of a dog meant nothing to her beyond
a sense impression. When she was 2, her mother was amused that when she was taken to the zoo, she
called yaks and camels ‘moo cow’. Naoko’s mother at that age called any four-legged animal ‘inu’, the
Japanese word for dog. By 15, on opposite sides of the world, they were enthusiastically discussing the
different merits of Elvis Presley and his rivals. As their children grew up, they discussed with their
friends what behaviour was acceptable and what was not. They had very different ideas about the
proper balance between individuality and conformity, and how to deal with difficult behaviour.
So, as people grow there is a change in the power of their thinking processes. One area of developmental
study is to describe and research this change. What is the best way to describe the changes? Do they
occur at the same age for everyone? If not at the same age, do they occur in the same order, perhaps at
different speeds for different people? How much does the development depend on teaching or the edu-
cative behaviour of parents? You must think about the possible answers to these questions. As an active
student of human growth and development, you should then find out if there are people who have
systematically carried out observations and experiments which lend weight to one answer or another.
One of the most widely researched approaches puts the emotional and social influences on one side
and studies ‘cognitive’ development in itself. This field of research, associated particularly with the work
of Jean Piaget (1896–1980), concludes that the development of thinking can be described in ways that
occur in all children in the same order. To make more complete sense of this in the real world, it will
be necessary afterwards to put back the influences of biology, the emotions and social context. Aspects
of biology relevant to understanding cognitive development include normal or abnormal brain
development, the impact of alcohol, or the effects of different types of brain damage; relevant areas
of emotional and social influence include the effects of emotional abuse, neglect, disrupted attach-
ments, different educational systems and cultures.
68
The developing child
summarising how Piaget made sense of his enormous research material (he published more than fifty
books and 500 papers as well as editing thirty-seven journal volumes), and then commenting on some
of the ways in which other research has confirmed, modified or developed his findings.
For thinking to take place, he theorised that experiences have to be represented, organised and inter-
preted. These three processes link to the mind’s increasing ability to structure and analyse its experience
of the world (Piaget, 1950/1997; Piaget et al., 1995). The box below summarises the stages in this
development as identified by Piaget.
• In the first stage, structure occurs simply in body experience and behaviour
– typical for example, of a baby and young child. The level of the child’s
intellectual ‘processing’ is simply the organised pattern of behaviour in
Chapter
EB5 response to particular objects and experiences. Examples and substages are
given in ‘Essential background’, section 5.
• At the second stage, from about the ages of 2 to 7 years, actions, objects and experiences
can be present in the conscious mind because the child develops the ability to symbolise
external events mentally.
• In the third stage of cognitive competence, the mind has processes which allow these
representations in the mind to be manipulated mentally – ‘If Helen cut the cake in two, she
could eat half of it now and still have some for another time.’
Piaget called this the concrete operations stage*.
• Fourth level: at first, the manipulation just described is applied to concrete objects, (as in
the example) but in the mature stage of development, after about 11 or 12 years of age,
general concepts can be abstracted from the individual examples and manipulated in their
own right, as happens in mathematics or moral discussions. Problems can be solved by
abstracting principles from examples, logically manipulating the principles, and then re-
applying them back to the individual instance.
Piaget called this the stage of formal operations.
*The second level has this apparently unhelpful label because the final stages involve the
ability to perform mental operations with concepts – so the second level is ‘pre-operational’. At
the third level, mental operations are performed, but only on concrete ideas, not abstract
concepts.
69
The developing child
He found that children tend to make the same mistakes in thinking at similar ages. These typical
mistakes gave him essential clues about the nature of children’s thinking. Here are three examples
which are characteristic of Piaget’s work, relating to the different stages of development he identified.
In the very earliest stages, one lack of competence is that the baby doesn’t know about objects which
persist – it doesn’t know that if two toys go behind a screen, there is something wrong if there is only
one toy when the screen is removed. A little later, a common ‘mistake’ is that viewpoints are always
related to the child themselves, not to the viewpoint of the other person – a child may know that she
has a sister, but deny that her sister has a sister. Until a certain age, if children see a quantity of
coloured water poured from a shallow dish into a tall narrow jug, they say there is more liquid in the
tall jug.
Many other researchers performed related observations and experiments, so current ideas are
not identical to those of Piaget. For example, modern techniques for studying babies show that some
idea of how objects can be expected to behave are present much earlier than Piaget thought – if a
‘trick’ is performed so that two objects are taken behind a screen and then another two, and when the
screen is lifted there are only three objects visible, a baby pays much longer attention to the ‘peculiar’
situation than to the ‘predictable’ one (see, for example, Baillargeon, 1987 – at the time of writing there
are papers by her and about her work on the website of her Infant Cognition Lab, Baillargeon, 2008).
At some level, the baby has recognised there is something odd – there is evidently some awareness
of objects and their properties, not just the experience of bodily sensations that Piaget thought was
occurring.
Errors in the child’s thinking can cause it to misinterpret the world in ways that cause anxiety. Especially
if they go unrecognised by adults, the anxiety may persist into later life. For example, a child who
disobeyed its parents on the day it later heard of the death of a grandparent may come to believe it
caused the death by disobedience.
Looking closer
‘Essential background’, section 5 summarises the four main stages in logical thinking that
Piaget identifies and picks out some of the characteristics of substages within each.
Two examples are given of mistakes in thinking which occur during stage 2, the stage of ‘pre-
operational thinking’. What are they?
70
The developing child
71
The developing child
Vygotsky
Piaget presents a universal model – the development of thinking operates similarly across cultures,
and is driven by the internal development of the child (in fact, in Piaget’s view, its biological devel-
opment). Vygotsky (Vygotskij and Cole, 1978) understood that cognitive growth has to be understood
in the context of what the child’s culture values – complex forms of thinking have their origins in social
interactions. More knowledgeable members of the culture, whether parents, siblings or teachers, struc-
ture the child’s learning in a process Vygotsky called scaffolding. He refers to the area of development
which is beyond the child’s current ability to do alone, but within their capacity to do with assistance,
as the ‘zone of proximal development’. Good education provides scaffolding for development in
this zone.
Stage theories such as Piaget’s should identify stages which apply across all domains of development
(his theory does not say that children will be at one level of development when they study science, but
another when they think about relationships). The universalist nature of his claims mean that there
should either be definite ages at which the stages occur, or there should be a consistent order in which
72
The developing child
they take place. In fact, researchers have found that young people (and adults) may function at the
level of ‘formal operations’ in some areas of their life but not in others. Hunter-gatherers show
sophisticated formal reasoning in their hunting, but failed the ‘formal operations’ problems devised by
Piaget (Flavell et al., 2002). So achieving this stage in the way described by Piaget may in fact be
‘gradual, haphazard and often limited to particular domains’ (Smith et al., 2005). Critics (see Eysenck,
2000: 417) argue that he confused ‘performance’ with ‘competence’ – performance is what is done
on a particular (experimental) occasion, whilst ‘competence’ is the underlying ability, which may
be demonstrated in familiar, readily understood situations. It is argued that the stage theory is
arbitrary – he underestimated the differences within stages at the same time as he overestimated the
differences between stages.
The puzzle of how to describe human development, which is so ‘plastic’ (changeable according to
culture and environment) and also regular and stage-related, is a theme which can be found in the
work of many developmental thinkers and researchers. As you examine different theories of develop-
ment, you will find that some (such as Erikson and Piaget) are criticised for being too committed to
identifying stages, ignoring the diversity which is found in psychosocial experience and behaviour –
while others are criticised for failing to take account of the sequence in which changes occur, not being
clear enough about which changes become possible at which stage, and which changes are ruled out
until a certain level of maturity is reached. In its outlines, the next area to be considered – behavioural
learning – contrasts with Piaget’s work in being essentially unconcerned with universal developmental
stages. Its core concern is with the way in which behaviour is shaped by environment.
Beyond this simple form of learning, there are three main models of learning which describe scien-
tifically how the environment shapes behaviour. Boyd and Bee (2006) are reluctant to describe them
as developmental theories because they do not place their main attention on age-specific changes. In
this, they contrast with theories of cognitive development such as those based on Piaget’s work.
However, their work undoubtedly provides one accurate way of describing why people display the
behaviour they do, and what changes that behaviour (their strongest proponents may go further and
say theirs is the only worthwhile account).
These three models, each of which is well established by experiment and observation, are termed clas-
sical conditioning, operant conditioning, and social learning. There is a large body of knowledge
about this science of behaviour in general, and specifically its application to planned behavioural
73
The developing child
change. In this section I will introduce the theories and indicate their applications,
including the most common present-day form – that of cognitive behavioural therapy.
Basic technical details are summarised in ‘Essential background’, section 8.
Chapter
EB8
And an example in social care: Bob used to respond with fear and anger when his father came
home and shouted at him. By the age of 4, the sound of his father entering the house and
slamming the front door was enough to make his body tense.
The principle of classical conditioning is that a naturally occurring response (‘reflex’) gets
triggered by an ‘artificial’ signal (stimulus) which has become constantly associated with the
naturally occurring one.
Operant conditioning
Nicola’s son Jamie is clean and dry, day and night, by 2 and a half. In the previous six months,
Nicola has focused a lot on this; she gave Jamie lots of praise and attention every time he used
the potty or gave signals that he wanted to go, or was dry through the night. Sharon’s
daughter, on the other hand, still uses disposable nappies at 3 and a half. The disposables mean
she is unaware when she wets herself, and there is no different response from those around
her whether she is wet or dry.
Think about classical conditioning as being about what happens before the behaviour in
question, and operant conditioning being about what happens afterwards – behaviour which
is reinforced tends to persist. This definition is not precise – classical conditioning is about the
association, not the timing – but it conveys the idea of a ‘signal’ for the behaviour.
74
The developing child
Going beyond classical and operant conditioning, Albert Bandura’s social learning theory
(1977) pointed out the mechanisms at work in learning when there is no external reinforce-
ment. In this view, all behaviour is acquired, but it is learned by observing other people and
modelling behaviour on theirs. Behaviour is regulated to a large extent by anticipated con-
sequences. Cognitive abilities are emphasised more than in the other two models – cognition
allows humans to have insight about likely consequences based on observation of others.
When the behaviour of prestigious individuals achieves results desired by others, that
behaviour is likely to be recreated.
Learning theory is sometimes applied directly using the ideas of operant and classical conditioning.
Social workers, however, are probably more likely to encounter it in the form of cognitive behavioural
therapy (CBT).
The technical descriptions of classical and operant conditioning deliberately make no reference to
thoughts or emotions. They refer to external (environmental) stimuli which happen before behaviour,
and the external reinforcements which follow behaviour. CBT, in contrast, incorporates cognitions
(thoughts) and emotions into the stimulus–behaviour–reinforcement schemes. It makes use of the
insights from cognitive therapy (developed particularly from the 1960s – see Beck and Weishaar, 2005),
which explored the way in which changing negative thinking can alter behaviour or mood. For some
problems, emotions are inserted into the scheme for changing behaviour – for example, teaching the
individual to recognise that the behaviour which produces negative consequences is preceded by
particular feelings, and creating a learning schedule for the person to act on when they experience
these emotions. In other problems which appear to be primarily about feelings (such as panics or
depression), the therapy installs behaviours which are such that the problematic emotions do not arise.
In other situations, work is put in to reframe the cognition, and this avoids the negative feelings which
may be unwanted or may lead to unwanted behaviour. Like other behavioural models, the basic
processes are didactic – the expert teaches techniques for the management of a problem – but the
manner in which it is implemented may involve attention to the relationship and mutual exploration
of the meaning of the issues to the person concerned. A simplified scheme used to describe the
framework leading to CBT is shown in Figure 3.1.
75
The developing child
Situation
Actions
Thoughts
Feelings
Figure 3.1
Scheme used in cognitive behavioural therapy
As an example, cognitive behavioural therapy might be offered to a student who becomes immobilised
at the prospect of academic assessments. She suffers enormous stress because of this and in tests
achieves below her ability as demonstrated in other settings. Cognitive behavioural therapy might
proceed by giving her weekly ‘homework’ in which she practices making statements which reflect her
true ability, not her perceived incompetence. This results in the confidence to continue realistic
preparations for her assessment and allow her to experience the assessment itself as something she
expects to complete well, rather than a mountain that she can never climb properly.
Cognitive therapies explored the way in which changing thinking can alter behaviour and emotion
(who will learn maths better – someone who believes they are no good at maths, or someone who
expects that if they are shown, they will learn?). In summary, cognitive behavioural therapy combines
behavioural therapy with cognitive therapy.
76
The developing child
There are extensive empirical studies of behavioural learning, and the outcomes can usually be trans-
lated into readily understandable everyday logic. However, there is also significant scope for error in
translating everyday experiences into the technical language of learning theory. A commonly quoted
example is that of the child in the supermarket who pesters her mother incessantly until she is given
sweets, at which point she becomes quiet. The process at work here is that the child is shaping the
mother’s behaviour by negative reinforcements – the reinforcement for the mother’s behaviour is that
the nasty feeling is avoided if the sweets are bought and given. So ‘negative reinforcement’ shapes
(produces) a behaviour, different from ‘punishment’ which extinguishes a behaviour. Researchers study
which type of reinforcements are most effective and which schedules of reinforcement are most
effective for which types of behaviours (Eysenck, 2000: 233).
Behaviour which is reinforced tends to persist and behaviour which is punished or not reinforced tends
to weaken or be extinguished. The problem with punishments in social life is that they can have varied
and not necessarily advantageous effects. The behaviour which is extinguished may be not be
‘behaviour X’ (hurting little sister) but ‘behaviour X when someone is watching’; punishments have
effects other than extinguishing the ‘target’ behaviour. For example, they are likely to instil fear, secrecy,
resentment or rebellion into a relationship which an adult wishes to be caring, trusting and reliable;
the punishing parent has trained the child not to reveal in adolescence the very matters the parent
may wish the child to talk about, and punishment may be a positive reinforcement of a valued ‘anti-
authority’ identity.
Bee and Boyd (2007: 18) concisely summarise the ways in which classical conditioning offers expla-
nations for the development of emotions and emotional bonds, as the parents or other carers become
constantly associated as triggers for pleasurable feelings.
?
Reflective thinking
‘Behavioural learning’ principles are sometimes used to shape behaviour in education, child-
rearing, or social welfare. Can you give examples? Are there examples of your own behaviour that
have been shaped by rewards and punishment?
In order to be effective, does behavioural shaping require the consent of the person whose
behaviour is targeted? Does this raise any ethical questions?
77
The developing child
Schooling and its system of measuring interacts with the child’s age-related cognitive abilities
discussed earlier. Children who, because of the administrative system’s cut-off age, entered school a
year ahead of children only a week or so younger were found to be using more advanced cognitive
processing and memory approaches. But bear in mind also that many parents and teachers will be able
to tell you of the lower self-image and related academic performance of these youngest children in a
class, who may be developmentally a year behind the oldest children, and suffer as a result. Crawford
and colleagues (2007) found that younger children in a year class consistently scored lower in academic
measures than the older children. Considering various implications in detail, they suggest that
children’s scores should be age-adjusted, so that young children in a class who obtain the same raw
score as older children would be awarded a higher exam mark.
Social workers will be particularly concerned in the problematic areas of school life – bullying;
educational achievement of abused children and children in care; drug-taking and drinking; and
disruptive or violent behaviour in school. Bronfenbrenner’s model is sometimes used as a way of setting
out the factors involved in these problems and the points at which intervention is necessary. For
example, the Assessment Framework for Children in Need (Department of Health, 2000) uses an
ecological model in assessing the needs of the child (individual, family and environmental); Morrison
78
The developing child
and L’Heureux (2001) similarly examine suicide risk and necessary interventions among gay and lesbian
teenagers, and the work by Moore and Glei (1995) uses Bronfenbrenner’s approach to create a model
which examines positive indicators and resilience as well as risk factors. The ‘Sure Start’ programme in
the UK (Belsky et al. 2007) was directed at improving children’s prospects by intervening simultaneously
in the different interlinked areas which contribute to outcomes.
Jencks and Phillips (1998) describe the analysis of social statistics in education as ‘a statistician’s
nightmare’. A number of variables, such as class, ethnicity, gender and family characteristics are clearly
relevant, but educational outcomes often depend not on the variable themselves, but on their
interrelationships (both Bangladeshi and white girls achieve better than boys, but whereas Bangladeshi
girls achieve better than white girls, the reverse is true for boys – see Table 3.1). The educational rela-
tionship with the variable has changed over time (boys used to do better than girls, but now the reverse
is true), the interrelationships have varied over time, and unfortunately it is not easy to establish how
the educational outcome measures (such as ‘A-level passes’ in the UK) are stable or are changing over
time. Any theory of what ‘causes’ the differences has to account for all these features and many more.
As with all inter-group population statistics, perhaps the first caution is to be clear what difference in
‘variation’ is being referred to alongside a difference in ‘average’. Two populations may have different
average scores, and yet there may be more variation within each population than between them. You
may recall that the ‘Taking it further’ section in Chapter 1 explained that the balance of factors that
account for individual differences may be different from that for group differences.
The social factors that have been examined in relation to educational outcomes include social class,
gender, family income/poverty, ethnicity and various features of family structure and functioning.
Other significant variables examined include pedagogical (teaching) methods, school structure and
organisation, educational system (for example, selective, non-selective, single-sex or mixed, public or
private). These are in addition to ‘individual’ variation such as disability, specific learning difficulties
(such as dyslexia), experience of abuse and the relation to subsequent economic and social achieve-
ment. Outcomes have been studied in relation to children who are in public care and in different sorts
of care (fostering, residential care or adoption, for example).
In the 1970s, the sociological concern by feminists was with the low achievement of girls in the
educational system compared with boys (see for example, Weiner, 1994), which they usually saw as
‘inequality’, attributing the difference to sexism in education. By 2007, the statistical picture was
79
The developing child
reversed, with girls on average being likely to achieve more highly on measures such as GCSE grades
A*–C or A-level passes or entrance to university. This time round, concern about boys’ low achievement
was criticised by some feminist writers; Abbott and colleagues (2005: 91), for example, who regarded it
as a ‘moral panic’. The difference between boys and girls is different for different ethnic groups. Grouping
women from non-white ‘ethnic minorities’ together, they had a higher participation rate (58%) in higher
education (university) than – in order of participation – men from non-white ethnic minorities (55%),
white women (41%) and white men (34%). However, Pakistani and Bangladeshi women are much less
likely to participate than men (Botcherby and Hurrell, 2004). White men are the largest priority group
to improve access and increase participation rates in higher education. The situation is different for
achievement at GCSE level, where likelihood of success follows that shown in Table 3.1.
These percentages refer to categories of very different sizes, using ethnic categories which are
artificially constructed and are ambiguous, representing varying degrees of social reality; ‘ethnic
identities are not fixed’ (Heath and McMahon, 2001), but are in constant flux. Given these differences
in group size, the value for the ‘white’ group is close to the average for the whole population of Great
Britain, so the table does not show, for example, which self-identified groups within the ‘white’
category (52 million adults), of equivalent size to the Chinese group (1⁄4 million adults) would achieve
higher than 79 per cent, nor which groups of white girls of equivalent size to the Black Caribbean boys
(1⁄4 million adults) achieve less than 25 per cent. Nor does it show the substantial differences within
these smaller groupings – so, for example, two men in the same city who came to the UK at the same
period from the same rural village in Pakistan may have grandchildren in two very different educational
cultures. One may be in a family (and locality) which is aspirational with educationally high-achieving
young people, whilst the other may be in an area 2 miles away where family patterns are still very
similar to first-generation life – new mothers coming from the ‘home’ village each generation and
speaking little English, cultural patterns which directly express life in rural Pakistan, and girls having
little expectation of independence or educational achievement.
Table 3.1
Percentage of pupils achieving 5 A*–C in GCSE
Source: Department for Children, Schools and Families (DCSF 2007a, Table 28)
80
The developing child
These statistics are available online, and more detailed tables than those in Table 3.1, with some
introductory comments on a range of comparisons, are presented by the Department for Children,
Schools and Families (DCSF, 2007b).
In relation to future life course, the analysis by Heath and McMahon (2001) concludes that educated
people from a minority ethnic background (particularly Indian and Chinese) received the same benefits
as those who categorised themselves as ‘white’, but those who competed in the manual labour market
were disadvantaged in outcome. That is, at the upper end, educational achievement works to assure
entry into higher status jobs in the same way as it does for whites, but at the lower end, it is less
effective in preventing social exclusion.
In addition, gender and ethnicity then interact in a complex way with social class. Educational achieve-
ment has always been systematically linked with social background – as researchers at the Joseph
Rowntree Foundation summarise the relevant research, ‘low income is a strong predictor of low
educational performance. White children in poverty have on average lower educational achievement
and are more likely to continue to under-achieve’ (Hirst, 2007).
One cultural view of education has been that it is the way to improve society (latterly there is also a
prominent view which conversely holds failings in education responsible for social ills). But increased
meritocracy, widening access to higher education, and improvements in measured educational
attainment of working-class children have consistently failed to affect relative achievement in respect
of social class. Policy changes designed to reduce inequality in educational outcomes consistently leave
class differentials untouched, and may even widen them. Halsey and colleagues (1980) found that
improvements in the mid-twentieth century had done ‘little to iron out class differentials’ (see Bilton,
2002: 271–275). Both in the UK and elsewhere, changes in education consistently benefit middle-class
compared with lower-class children, even when the aims of changes are explicitly to affect social
structure by increasing mobility.
Ethnicity, gender and class are understood by sociologists to be ways in which our society structures
itself. There are many other factors which influence the lived experience of a child at school, the
development fostered or hindered, and the measurable educational outcomes achieved. Sometimes
the reasons for this and the way the factor operates is obvious; at other times a statistical correlation
is known, but there is no convincing cause-and-effect explanation which accounts for both the
correlation and the different achievements of those in similar situations. Parental expectations and
support are influential, as is the level of parental (particularly the mother’s) education. Peer group
norms towards learning are significant, as are attitudes of teachers, both towards individual children
and about groups of children. Racism at school reduces performance. In each income group, children
of parents who are married do on average better than children whose parents are not married, and
children with two parents living together in the family do better than those with one parent. On
average children’s educational performance drops after parental separation or divorce – there is some
evidence that it rises again after a few years (Smallwood and Wilson, 2007).
81
The developing child
?
Reflective thinking
In your view, what are some of the factors that affect success and happiness in school?
82
The developing child
called ‘child abuse’ in an objectively defined way. This is not in fact so – there is no objective way to
define what is commonly called ‘child abuse’.
From what we know, few human societies have not had the expectation that parents will love, cherish
and prize their children; most have regarded it as a praiseworthy responsibility for adults in general to
protect children from harm. But this leaves enormous scope for differing attitudes towards children
across cultures; even for the celebration (as desirable) of practices which are grossly harmful to
children. The Spartan Greeks left baby boys on a mountain so that only the strong would survive (the
others presumably crying themselves to coma and death), and then brought them up to value fighting
above all else; mothers and grandmothers in some cultures think it desirable to genitally mutilate their
daughters; in English public schools teachers have viewed regular corporal punishment as beneficial.
The Victorians thought children should be protected and nurtured, but, as described in the work of
novelists like Charles Kingsley (The Water Babies) and Charles Dickens (Oliver Twist), they tolerated child
labour, child exploitation and child prostitution. What is defined as unacceptable ‘child abuse’ varies
from society to society and from decade to decade. Judging by history over the last century, there is
every expectation that practices which are tolerated with little comment today will be condemned as
obviously neglectful or abusive in a decade’s time.
There are references to child abuse in Chapter 2 – ‘states of mind’ and ‘attachment’; in Chapter 4 – an
example involving teenagers, one of whom ran from genital mutilation, and one of whom is a teenager
in care following abuse; in Chapter 5 – a young man with learning disabilities; in Chapter 6 – intimate
relationships in adulthood are coloured by childhood sexual experiences; and Chapter 8 in relation to
mental health.
Current UK law and social policy refers to duties of ‘child protection’. The Children Act 1989 places a
general duty on all public bodies to safeguard children and to promote their welfare. So the first step in
preventing child abuse is to provide support for children in need and their families. ‘Safeguarding’
children from harm includes a duty to investigate any instance where there is reason to believe a child
is suffering significant harm. The local authority must keep a register of children who are in need
of protection and implement a protection plan. Court orders such as a care order are issued if a
court is satisfied that the child is suffering significant harm, and that this will continue unless the order
is made.
The philosophy of current regulation and policy is that children will only be protected from abuse if all
the community’s resources are involved – if schools, health services, social workers, and a variety of
parental support services are working together. Partly as a result of this, the work of child protection
is heavily regulated and directed by procedure. Studies show that in comparison with other European
systems, UK social workers are much more driven by the law, procedures, coordination and regulation.
In a cross-national comparison (Hetherington et al., 1997), these issues consumed much time and
83
The developing child
emotional energy for English social workers. Workers from countries on mainland Europe, by contrast,
spent more energy discussing what was happening to the different people in the families they worked
with, and what might help to produce change – although the researchers concluded overall that there
is a clear commonality about the way in which social workers from the countries surveyed approach
the dilemmas of child protection. As we shall discuss shortly, remedying the problems of child abuse
is a long-term task, one that requires a perspective of ten or twenty years. Unfortunately one major
current problem in the UK is that the teams dealing with these situations are often staffed with the
least experienced social workers, who may not stay in their posts long – childcare is the area of social
care with the greatest problems both in recruitment and in retention (Local Authority Workforce
Intelligence Group, 2006). So although it is an area with a particular need for very experienced staff
who have long-term knowledge of the families and children concerned, the severity of the work and
the current context and practice of child protection makes it hard to achieve this.
Current approaches identify four types of child abuse: physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional abuse
and neglect. These are overlapping categories – for example, by definition someone who is sexually
abused is also being emotionally abused. We have already seen that society’s definitions of what is
abusive in these categories vary over time.
When the state takes care of children who are abused, it has to take the responsibility
to halt the damage in the present; but also to provide something better in the future,
and to help undo the damage which has been caused. This tends to be very difficult. An
Chapter
EB2 abused child has often been brought up to realise that those in the world who seem best
suited to care (parents) turn out to be destructive. This ‘internal working model’ of
relationships (‘Essential background’, section 2) makes it unsurprising that adults who offer care
might not be trusted. This may test subsequent carers (and their families) to the limits, as foster parents
or residential staff initially offer genuine concern, but find it met with deceit, mistrust, or exploitation.
In addition, the difficulties presented by the child, or the problems of organising personal life through
a public system – with its regulations, varied policies and changing staff – or unexpected events in
foster carers’ own family lives, frequently result in these children lacking a consistent caregiver. The
initial difficulties experienced by children who have been abused are compounded by their experience
that when they expose themselves by trusting someone to care for them, the relationship does not
last. Although there may be pressure in formal planning meetings to focus on risk to physical
well-being, measurable educational outcomes or difficulties in placements, one central priority for
social workers responsible for a child’s well-being is to understand the impact of life events on the
child’s internal world. Responding in a restorative way will not necessarily be best served by referring
to another professional for ‘treatment’ – this is one area of many where a competent social worker
needs to have therapeutic skill and the ability to support others in work which has a therapeutic
outcome.
There is no single pattern to the after-effects of abuse. The most severe experiences of physical
abuse lead to death or permanent injury. For some children facing problems at home, school may
84
The developing child
be a welcome refuge; but for others, constant emotional pressure inside the family leads to a lack
of resources to cope in a polite, well-mannered way in school – frustration resulting in outbursts
and aggression. Sexual abuse may lead to an inappropriate precocious sexualisation or an aversion
to sexual contact. It may lead to lack of trust and difficulties in later intimate relationships.
All forms of abuse may well lead to an inner lack of confidence that is covered up in later life,
but continues simmering under the surface. In many of the adult mental health situations in which
social workers intervene, this vulnerability will be a factor. Faced with difficulties with their own
children, parents who were abused may be beset by doubts about their ability to be good parents.
Hearing the frequency with which sexual offenders describe having been abused themselves, they may
worry that their own experience of abuse will lead them, despite themselves, to abusing their own
children.
Figure 3.2
Men who were Risk: the overlap represents a different percentage of the
Men who
hoo
right- and left-hand areas
abuse abused as boys
children
continued
85
The developing child
Supporting the contention, see Glasser et al. (2001); opposing it, see Boyd and Bromfield
(2006); reviewing the evidence as inconclusive, see Rezmovic et al. (1996). The issues are
analysed by Gelles (1998: 18), who insists that the question to be addressed is not, ‘Do abused
children become abusive parents?’ but ‘Under what conditions is the transmission of abuse
likely to occur?’
In the last two decades, childcare research has taken particular interest in the protective factors
causing ‘resilience’ – the ability to adapt successfully and to overcome adversity. These protective
factors include intelligence and the availability of a protective, concerned adult who is not part of the
abusive situation. Howe (2005: 219) summarises further aspects of this research by suggesting that it
may be that different factors build resilience for maltreated and non-maltreated young people. For
children who have been abused, self-reliance, self-confidence and autonomy seem to be of high
importance – ‘independence’ personality factors – rather than the relationship factors prominent for
children in general.
Good parenting provides the child with a confidence in their own worth; abusive parenting can instil
the message that the individual has no basic worth and that behaviour which is self-destructive or
destructive of others is only to be expected. The challenge for the after-care of children who have been
harmed is to provide an experience sufficiently enduring and loving that it makes real inroads into the
problems created by destructive parents. Because it is often unrealistic for this restorative task to be
completed during childhood, and because ‘parenting’ in the best of situations continues into
adulthood, the state’s duty of care for children harmed by their parents must have a perspective that
continues in a supportive, therapeutic and non-stigmatising way into adulthood.
?
Reflective thinking
• What elements in how and when you learnt can be understood in terms of Piaget’s stages of
cognitive development?
• What elements can be discussed in terms of operant conditioning? (Behaviour which is
reinforced tends to persist – see box on page 74.)
• No doubt you remember your school achievements partly as the result of your own efforts
(or lack of effort!). But what were the social influences that also determined your
educational outcomes?
86
The developing child
The final section of this chapter is a more formal review of ideas about guilt, conscience and morality.
In the course of the preceding sections there have been several references to people inappropriately
blaming themselves – in the examples used about Piaget’s work and in relation to the lack of self-
confidence that can be caused by abuse. We could have added that many people subjected to abuse
blame themselves for the incident itself – ‘I know he went too far, but I have to say I did deserve some
of it’ – so the following section is concerned both with the development of morality and with the
nature of guilt.
TAKING IT FURTHER
Introduction
A person may feel guilty or blame themselves in circumstances where they have done nothing
wrong. Another person’s behaviour, by contrast, may be self-centred, showing no sense of respect
for the rights or property of others. A social worker may have to deal with both issues – of
unrealistic guilt and of a lack of ‘moral’ standards – in the same person. To give a rather dramatic
quotation, psychological researchers in a youth offending institution found that in response to
stress, a ‘characteristic inmate reaction pattern could be described as anxious, acquiescent, self-
demeaning, and depressive-like’ (Hokanson et al., 1976).
This section of the chapter outlines some models that have been put forward about the devel-
opment of thoughts and emotions related to morality. The areas touched on are only a small
component of the study of antisocial behaviour and criminality, and the essay should not be seen
as an introduction to that subject. The first part is concerned principally with cognition, and the
second with one approach to the emotions of guilt and self-attack.
Overview
This subject touches on a range of philosophical issues about morality. An absolutist view of
morality holds that there are standards which are above personal wishes and cultural conventions.
This view has the advantages of matching the everyday meaning of ‘morality’. It allows one’s
moral values to be applied to another situation and culture (so that one can view a family’s
‘honour killing’ of their daughter because she has been raped as barbarous and immoral) –
maintaining that there are values which transcend local practices. These absolute moral values
may be understood as coming from a religious authority or from the ‘natural law’. A relativist
view points out that in reality, these authorities prescribe different codes as moral – so how can
they be right? Who has the authority to decide between contradictory moral codes? Such a view
points to the conclusion that ‘moral’ standards are always constructed by society, and vary
according to the local culture. The literature on ethics, the philosophy of morality, analyses these
views and others. Each generation of philosophers adds its own contribution to these issues; some
87
The developing child
building on earlier insights, some proposing new formulations and some adopting one stance, some
another.
It is not the purpose of this essay to explore these philosophical questions, but they often underpin
and complicate the discussions which follow. They point to a further range of questions which
could be asked. Sometimes a thoughtful practitioner may feel that the pragmatics of the situation
make such discussions entirely hypothetical. At other times, the same person may acknowledge
that it is important to consider these angles.
The first part of what follows presents the conclusions of Piaget, Kohlberg and Gilligan about the
stages children go through in coming to an adult understanding of ‘right and wrong’. Originally,
learning theories of behaviour were not primarily about cognitions, but about the emergence and
maintenance of behaviour through reward and punishment. However, Bandura’s ‘social learning’
theory, and the development of cognitive behavioural approaches are nowadays central to under-
standing how ‘prosocial’ or ‘antisocial’ attitudes are developed, and the essay considers the place
of these approaches. The second part of the essay considers psychoanalytic views which explore
the continuing emotional dynamics of the development of guilt and shame.
1 A little boy called John is in his room. He is called to dinner. He goes into the dining room.
But behind the door there is a chair and on the chair is a tray with fifteen cups on it. John
does not know that all this is behind the door. He goes in, and the door knocks against the
tray. Bang go the cups! And they all get broken.
2 There was a little boy called Henry. One day when his mother was out he tried to get some
jam out of the cupboard. He climbed up on to a chair and stretched out his arm. But the
jam was too high up and he could not reach it. While he was trying to get it, he knocked
over a cup. The cup fell down and broke.
88
The developing child
Piaget first checked that the children had understood the stories and then asked them which child
they considered the naughtiest and why. In the stage of moral realism as described above, children
tended to say that the child who broke the most cups was the naughtiest. The sheer amount of
damage is the measure, not whether the act was intentional. By contrast, older children tend to
take motivation into account, deciding that whether someone is morally culpable or not depends
on their intentions.
In a further series of observations and theorising, Piaget examined children’s ideas about punish-
ment. For example, describing a child who had damaged the toy of another child, he asked which
would be the fairer punishment – to replace the toy with one of their own, to pay for it to be
repaired, or to be forbidden to play. Younger children tend to think that the latter punishment
should be made, because it makes the person suffer – the greater the misdeed, the greater the
suffering which should be inflicted. Older children tended to consider that ‘the punishment should
fit the crime’; they took motivation into account, and thought the purpose of the punishment would
be to ensure the rule was observed next time. Piaget found that younger children tend to believe
in ‘immanent justice’ – breaking the rule will always result in punishment, and justice is the natural
order of things. Older children have learnt that rule violation will often go unpunished, and tend
not to believe in immanent justice.
Kohlberg was a Jewish psychologist. In the decades following the Second World War he worked in
America and led a free-thinking series of seminars. He presented children of different ages with a
story in which there is a moral dilemma. Perhaps the most famous involves a fictional character
Heinz who has a sick wife; the chemist stocks the necessary drug, but it is too expensive for Heinz.
The question presented by Kohlberg as a moral dilemma is: ‘Should Heinz steal the drug?’
His research was not interested in the judgement made by in the respondents, but in the reasoning
they gave.
He identified the following stages of moral reasoning:
Level 1 – Preconventional Morality: This is similar to Piaget’s stage of moral realism. Kohlberg thinks
of this level as applying to most children under 9 years of age and to some adolescents, and to
adolescent and adult offenders. Morality at this level is external. The child conforms to rules in
order to avoid punishment or to obtain personal rewards.
Within this level are two stages: one when obedience is valued for its own sake, and the criteria
for what is right and wrong are determined by what is punished; and the second where rules are
followed if they are in the immediate interests of the individual – ‘It’s not good to steal from a shop.
It’s against the law. Someone could see you and call the police.’
Level 2 – Conventional Morality: Morality is considered to consist in obeying the laws of society and
religion. The purpose of morality is to uphold the rules, to avoid censure from legitimate author-
ities, and to gain their praise and respect. Laws are regarded as necessary to ensure the good order
of society, and are in the best interests of everyone. Kohlberg differentiated two substages to this
level of moral thinking.
Level 3 – Post-Conventional Morality: In this level, the individual recognises that conventional morality
may not always be right, that social rules may not always be moral. There have to be general moral
rules underlying specific injunctions. If an individual’s principles are in conflict with society’s rule,
then the individual’s moral principles should take precedence. Here, too, Kohlberg identified two
substages.
89
The developing child
Kohlberg considered that the identification of how people develop their moral thinking had
implications for education. He linked his ideas about moral development to the stages of cognitive
understanding.
90
The developing child
spouse or a parent. There are many other problems in taking ‘moral reasoning’ as understood by
Piaget and Kohlberg to be a valid indicator of ‘moral development’. It may be, for example, that
the primary purpose of statements about what ought to be punished is not to express dispas-
sionately a person’s moral principles. Instead, the statements may be a way of criticising the actions
of others (the rule-breakers themselves or the judiciary). Accurately describing one’s own moral
principles is a different task.
Nevertheless, cross-cultural and cross-gender studies have in general supported the findings of Piaget
and Kohlberg: that reasoning about punishments and rule-breaking develop in stages, and that these
are broadly comparable across culture and gender. Further, there are some messages for social work
and education from their research. Continuing studies have found that adolescents with the highest
level of moral reasoning are also those who show the most prosocial behaviour, and that lower levels
are associated with antisocial behaviour (Schonert-Reichl, 1999). This supports the ideas of Kohlberg
that there are significant implications for education, including the educative processes which take
place in interventions linked with social exclusion or youth justice. The lessening of egocentrism
which Piaget associated with greater cognitive development and with greater empathy has also in
Kohlberg’s view to be enhanced by opportunities with peers and adults for ‘meaningful dialogue
about moral issues’ (Boyd and Bee, 2006: 340). Koenig and colleagues (2004) found that at the age
of 5, abused children had less understanding of situations such as cheating, stealing and other rule-
breaking which other children recognised as producing guilt and shame.
As so often in matters affecting social work, developmental understanding involves an appreciation
of the interaction of cognition, emotion and social experience.
91
The developing child
Bandura’s social learning model incorporates the dimension, always important for social work, that
the individual learns by observing significant others. The growing child’s behaviour reflects not just
what adults are ‘training’ in him, but even more fundamentally the principles underlying the adult’s
behaviour. Whatever ‘good’ behaviour a parent attempts to instil by beating a child, that child also
learns about authority figures’ apparent ‘right’ to inflict violence. This observational learning from
respected adults is clearly a major influence on the development of moral attitudes in children; it
is potentially a major dynamic in effective social work practice with people who harm others (the
parent who hurts their child learns more from how the social worker treats their own ‘delinquency’
than from the social worker’s child-rearing instructions, which they have usually heard many times
before), and observational learning from respected colleagues and supervisors is a major factor in
the development of mature professional ethics.
92
The developing child
where it defers gratification of its own immediate wishes out of regard for other people – just as,
in fact, the parent did for him.
However, through a process that Woodmansey describes as ‘the internalisation of external conflict’,
another part of the self may develop which is hostile towards the child’s own basic impulses. It is
this part, called by Woodmansey the ‘hostile’ or ‘punitive’ superego, which is at the root of self-
attack, self-punishment, guilt, and unrealistic self-criticism. Woodmansey suggests that the
interaction to understand is that in which a parent, determined to stop a child in some strongly
motivated behaviour, uses punishment and deterrence until the child obeys. Since a healthy child
will at first resist such attack, ‘punishment and retaliation will provoke each other in turn until the
child can stand no more . . . yet, he will often be unable to escape’; his own impulses are each time
to complain further, which attracts a further dose of punishment from the adult. Woodmansey
suggests that the child fortuitously finds a way out of this cycle. The child turns its anger on itself,
regarding itself as the cause of the problem, and since the fight can’t be continued on two fronts
at once, self-directed aggression has the by-product of removing the child from conflict with
parents. The cost, however, is that the child’s ego is split – one part, the basic self, owns the natural
impulses, while the other, the punitive or controlling ‘superego’, becomes antagonistic and
controlling towards aspects of itself.
The aspects of self (actually unproblematic, but experienced as dangerous) which tend to attract
this self-punishment and self-criticism are those which have provoked rejection or punishment from
the parent. These are commonly: aggressiveness – the child’s tendency to be angry when thwarted,
for example; the need for comfort (if this is met with rejection); and untidiness or sexual impulses.
The punitive/controlling state of mind – an ‘ego’ in its own right, unsympathetic to the basic needs
of the self – is likely to be recreated in other, similar situations later, causing self-criticism,
repression and self-punishment in relation to natural impulses.
The core idea is that these two ‘states of mind’ have contradictory qualities and are mutually
antagonistic. One state of mind is of self-attack, self-criticism; the other is the state of mind of
being attacked. In a helping relationship, the two states of mind require different responses. One
requires sympathy and support because they are constantly blamed for matters which are not
within their control; but the other thinks the basic self is bad and deserves criticism or punishment.
In the controlling state of mind, a woman vigorously and constantly cleans her house, reproaching
herself that is not clean (even though to an outsider it is perfectly presentable). In the other state
of mind, she may feel depressed and defeated that however hard she tries, she always seems to
fall short.
Although these states sound similar, as Woodmansey pointed out, in practice there is a difference
in the way they feel. One is like the critical parent controlling and wanting to punish the child,
keeping up standards by driving ever harder; and the other is like the hurt child being constantly
criticised, feeling a failure. The latter wants warmth, care and reassurance, whereas the former
despises such things. The ‘punitive superego’ wants – and offers – no sympathy for the weak,
undisciplined self with whom it shares a life (and a body). The two states of mind do not exist
simultaneously, and it is fruitless to respond to one when the other is speaking.
So when discussing difficult situations, there may be three elements to the social worker’s
conversation. One is the practical discussion of the outside situation, and the offer of assistance with
appropriate plans or actions. The second is respond to the punitive state of mind, and the third is to
respond to the self-criticism and feelings of failure.
93
The developing child
An example from social work in a children’s hospital is given by Sudbery and Blenkinship (2005).
A child had been severely burnt in a kitchen accident. Some exchanges in a conversation with the
parents were about making financial applications (which the parents had never done before), and
reassuring them that the circumstances of the accident were such that no one would blame them.
In the same conversation, other exchanges involved acknowledging that ‘it doesn’t matter what
anyone else thinks, you think you are to blame for what happened’ (acknowledging, but not
agreeing with, the views of the punitive superego). In yet other parts of the conversation, the social
worker was essentially sympathising with the basic feelings of the parents, staying with them in
their pain, and pointing out that it is unjust for them to be blamed,on top of that pain, when there
was nothing they could realistically have done.
In summary, besides external relationships (which are often the obvious concern of social workers),
people also have relationships with themselves, which can be amicable (accepting) or hostile
(punitive). Mixed in with the practical problems presented to social workers, there are often
difficulties in the relationship with the self. For effective social work, these are as important as the
other problems. In day-to-day social work, the sensitive social worker may address the two sides
of a person’s self-attack without consciously thinking about psychological structures. Sometimes,
particularly when practical help or sympathetic comments are missing the mark, it can be helpful
to realise that in self-criticism there are two different states of mind being expressed – one punitive
and one punished (in Woodmansey’s terms, an ego and a split-off hostile superego).
Roberts (2005) suggests that staff in social work and other helping professions are particularly
prone to self-questioning, and certainly their work will regularly provoke self-doubt. They are
therefore entitled to supportive supervision which lessens their tendency to self-criticism, and this
experience itself will directly increase their sensitivity and competence about the issues faced by
the users of their service.
Cooper and Lousada (2005), using ideas from Melanie Klein, suggest there is another aspect to the
experience of guilt in welfare work. They regard the ability to experience and withstand guilt as
essential because otherwise policy makers and practitioners will go to unrealistic and potentially
damaging lengths to avoid it.
History shows that welfare work and welfare policy sometimes fail people in severe ways. There
are regularly reports of children who have been left at home only to be later harmed by the parents
(or the removal of children from parents who in fact have not harmed them). To avoid ‘guilt’,
Cooper and Lousada suggest, policy makers may engage in grandiose schemes after tragedies such
as these. Faced with the enormity of the damage, policy makers may implement unrealistic and
ultimately unhelpful reorganisations and procedural provisions ‘to ensure this will never happen
again’. This may well be impossible for anyone to guarantee, and the proliferation of reorganisation
and ever-increasing regulations may hinder services rather than improve them.
Practitioners need to be able to cope with guilt because the experience of history is unfortunately
that, even with good intentions, conscientious work and approved practice, they are likely to
be avoidably harming the users of their service. In retrospect, during every decade ‘good’ social
work and welfare practice has been harming people (even though it is only with hindsight that
this becomes clear), so it is unlikely that it will suddenly stop. In the post-war years children
from English inner cities were sent out ‘to new lives’ in the commonwealth countries of Australia
and Canada; unfortunately, the effects of the consequent exploitation, dislocation and lack of
care still drives many of these lives sixty years later. In the 1950s, young children were still being
94
The developing child
placed in ‘residential nurseries’ entirely ill-suited to care for the emotional needs of young children;
in the 1960s, social workers were still giving childless couples the joy of an adopted baby,
and the child a chance of a good family life, but at the expense of young women who had given
birth outside marriage, and who have continued to suffer through the decades since. None of
this detracts from the essential assistance that has also been given over the years by social workers,
but they have to be able to accommodate appropriate guilt for actions they take in good
faith, which later turn out to have been against the best interests of the citizens they tried
to assist.
Guilt, Melanie Klein’s analysis suggests, is an unavoidable component of early childhood experience
(coming originally from the realisation that the child has wished to harm and destroy the very
person they love). Both then and later in life, it can be avoided only by using psychological defences
that are ultimately unhelpful. If social workers themselves are at ease with this component of guilt,
they will be well equipped to alleviate the unrealistic, punitive superego. In fact they will realise
that if they perform this service properly for their clients (whatever harm they have done to others),
they will, by however small or large a degree, make it more likely that the person will experience
true reparative guilt. The victim and those concerned about her require the perpetrator to feel guilt
based on the harm done, not on unrealistic self-criticism.
Summary
Thinking ability develops from unreflective engagement with the material world through to the
ability to manipulate abstract concepts. Piaget envisaged the individual as like a scientist gradually
constructing a model of the world. He concluded from his research that thinking develops in
universal stages from infancy to adulthood. At each stage, there are characteristic mistakes in
thinking processes (cognitions). Vygotsky placed more emphasis on the culture in which knowledge
can develop and the influence of significant adults like teachers. He understood children to have a
‘zone of proximal development’, an area of learning they are ready for; good education should
provide ‘scaffolding’ to enable this potential learning to take place. The tradition of research set out
by Piaget has been fruitful, but many researchers have found that the description of stages needs
modification, and that the precise nature of cognitive ability depends on tasks which are commonly
performed and varies across different intellectual domains. Information-processing approaches
place more emphasis on the development of functional components of thinking ability (such as
short-term memory and working memory).
The chapter summarised three main theories of learning and behaviour. In classical conditioning, an
artificial (conditioned) stimulus becomes associated with an unconditioned response – the dog
begins to salivate at the sound of a bell. In operant conditioning, behaviour is shaped by the
reinforcements which follow. In social learning, behaviour which is rewarded when others display
it is learnt by observation. All are relevant to work in social care. Cognitive behavioural approaches
also emphasise the cognitions which affect behaviour and emotions.
95
The developing child
Education is a major location of social, emotional and intellectual experience for children. Statistics
show clearly how educational outcomes are related to the way society is structured, through class,
gender and ‘race’ for example. The pattern of these statistics seems to rule out any simple expla-
nation of the causal mechanism through which educational outcomes are linked to social structures.
Abuse damages children, sometimes fatally, and tends to have lasting effects. Social workers need
to be alert to these effects long after the abuse has ended. Current policy distinguishes physical
abuse, emotional abuse, sexual abuse and neglect, but these are overlapping categories. It is
important for social workers to understand ‘resilience’ as well as ‘abuse’.
The formal essay discussed the development of cognition about morality and the emotions asso-
ciated with guilt and conscience. Influential ‘stage’ models of moral cognitions have been put
forward by Piaget and Kohlberg. Gilligan believes these are too focused on concepts of ‘justice’ and
punishment, and highlighted the importance of empathy and relationships. Cognitive behavioural
work operates to change inappropriate thinking which is involved in antisocial behaviour. In relation
to punitive guilt and self-reproach, social workers must understand that people have a relationship
with themselves as well as with others, and this relationship with self can have any degree of self-
directed hostility. They have to relate both to the punishing impulses of the individual and to the
effects of being punished.
Further reading
There are many readable accounts of cognitive development which build on the enormous quantity of
research material. About cognitive development (the work of Piaget and later researchers):
Boyd, D. R. and Bee, H. L. (2006) Lifespan Development. Boston: Pearson/Allyn and Bacon. This includes illus-
trations, evaluation and discussion of alternative models: a summary and evaluation, pp. 33–37; in infancy,
including the substages of the sensori-motor stage, pp. 114–121; in early childhood, pp. 173–181; in middle
childhood, pp. 235–240; in adolescence, pp. 304–308. The authors also present the material in a single chapter
in their textbook devoted solely to childhood: Bee, H. and Boyd, D. (2007), The Developing Child, 11th edn.
Boston, New York, London: Pearson, pp. 140–181.
Piaget, J. (1973) The Child’s Conception of the World, translated by Joan and Andrew Tomlinson. London: Paladin.
96
The developing child
The NHS library website has a wide range of downloadable leaflets about child health and children’s
medical conditions:
http://cks.library.nhs.uk
?
Questions
Based on the text:
1 The introduction to this chapter refers to features of early childhood identified by: (a) Erikson’s
psychosocial model; and (b) attachment theory. What are they? (The features are subtly different from
experience in infancy.) Give a brief example to illustrate your answer.
2 Explain how the following topics, discussed on a television programme, relate to different models of
behavioural learning:
• ‘In the old days’, perhaps children’s behaviour was better because policemen could administer
a quick physical slap to young people who misbehaved;
• Footballers should be good role models for young men; and ultra-thin fashion models influence
girls in the wrong way.
3 ‘People have a relationship with themselves as well as with outside people. This internal relationship,
too, can be friendly and accepting or hostile and punitive.’
Use this as the title of an essay. In the essay:
• Describe some of the ways a relationship with self can be critical or punitive.
• Give examples relating to the experience of social work students, practising social workers or
their clients.
• Discuss some of the possible origins of this harsh attitude towards the self.
• Using a social work example (which may be from the literature or an anonymous account from
your own experience), describe how services may either worsen the problem or help to remedy
it; in your answer focus particularly on the contribution of the individual social worker.
97
The developing child
98
In this chapter you will find:
4 Transitions and
adolescence
• Adolescence – biological changes; theories of
adolescence; sexuality; Erikson and identity
• Care leavers
Steve
‘Teenajoz,’ said 10-year-old Kenny to his brother, a year older. His voice was an urgent,
hoarse, conspiratorial signal about the supposed dangers on the footbridge ahead,
lengthening the first syllable and deliberately broadening the local pronunciation of the
final syllable, which rhymed with ‘Oz’. It was the same tone of voice used in by cowboys
in 1950s films when they spotted bandits. ‘What’s up with you two?’ laughed their
mother, Kath. ‘Teenajoz,’ repeated Kenny, ‘They’ll batter us!’ Kath smiled as she told the
tale to her friend; ‘It was Brian and Mike. They wouldn’t hurt anyone.’ She laughed as she
again imitated Kenny’s dramatised warning, ‘teenajoz’.
Vivian
Vivian had been 15. That was ten years ago now, a world away from her uncertain life in
England as she awaits the decision on her asylum application. Back then, the struggle
for power in Somalia had intensified over three weeks as the president clung to power.
One by one, the small towns where Vivian’s friends lived had changed allegiance, turning
to support the rebels who promised to install a new, democratic government. Vivian’s
father had no real interest in politics, and Vivian understood even less. But her father
had been faced with a choice between the burning of his house by the rebels or changing
his allegiance away from the governor; and he saw the writing on the wall.
Vivian came home from school at two in the afternoon, hungry as usual; for in these
troubled times food was short. She saw the men in uniforms at several of the houses,
101
Transitions and adolescence
including her own. As she walked past the soldier he looked her up and down but said
nothing. Inside, her mother’s eyes met hers, and then closed for the last time. Vivian had
walked back to a scene of indescribable horror. Faced with the results of the men’s
butchery, she was just in time to see her father dragged away, still living. She never saw
him again.
Adolescence
This chapter will outline various aspects of adolescent life. In presenting these ideas, I have as usual
thought particularly about the people you will be working with as a social worker. Children who come
into care will be teenagers before very long, and I refer briefly to some of the patterns of experience
of adolescents in care. Young people may be both adolescents and parents, and the adults you work
with will have had many and varied experiences as adolescents. Someone you see as a 90-year-old
man has spent his teenage years in circumstances that are both highly individual and shaped by
historical and cultural patterns. Similarly, if you work directly with adolescents you are likely to feel
that there are features of their life that are typical of their generation, and others which are specific
to them and their individual difficulties.
Adolescence is defined by its stage as a transition between childhood and adulthood, but social workers
are constantly dealing with transitions at all stages of life. This chapter outlines some theoretical
approaches to understanding transitions. It concludes with a more academic discussion about the
degree to which theories offering a universal view of human nature can capture the varied experience
of people in different cultures and in different social positions.
102
Transitions and adolescence
(the rate of growth is higher than in the years preceding or following). Neurologically, it appears that
the brain reorganises itself in this period, which is followed by a further spurt in growth from the age
of 17 into the early twenties. This latter growth is in the areas which have particular importance in
logic and planning. The networks in the brain used by mature adults for decision-making may well be
different from those used in adolescence (Blakemore et al., 2007). Once again, as described
in the brief account of infant brain growth in Chapter 2, the process is one of massive
growth of brain material which is then constantly pruned, with the surviving material and
Chapter 2 connections being made more permanent and enduring. (Sowell et al., 1999; Paus et al.,
1999). As with all accounts of brain functioning, these ideas have to come with the warn-
ing that we are still only just beginning to understand the brain and its links to behaviour, cognition
and feelings; simplified accounts (for example, about different areas of the brain being used for
different purposes) do not represent the immensely complex reality.
Body fat and muscle development. Muscles also develop rapidly in adolescence, the increase being more
pronounced in males than females. Boyd and Bee (2006: 274) quotes the research to show that
between the ages of 13 and 17 the percentage of the body made up of fat rises from 21 per cent to
24 per cent among girls but drops from 16 per cent to 14 per cent in boys.
Survival. The bottom biological line is survival. In 2007, just under six children in a thousand died before
they reached the age of 20, 50 per cent more boys than girls (ONS, 2008d – figures for England and
Wales). In the adolescent years, 52 per cent of deaths are result of external causes, including accidental
death, homicide and suicide (Sidebotham et al., 2007), a contrast from the years immediately before,
when natural causes predominate.
Sexual development. Human sexuality, as discussed shortly, is a complex mix of behaviour, emotions,
personal relationships, societal attitudes and, of course, biology. Changes in the body’s reproductive
system are significant in adolescence. The pituitary gland produces hormones which trigger both
the growth spurt and, in the first place, the growth of pubic hair. In girls, this and the early stages of
breast development are followed about two years later by the first period (menarche – pronounced
‘men-ar-che’). Irregular menstrual cycles are normal at first, and still into the third year of
menstruation no eggs (ova) may be produced in about half of the monthly cycles (Boyd and Bee, 2006:
292). Bee states that there is still uncertainty about the age at which boys’ sperm becomes capable of
fertilising an egg, but that it is usually between the ages of 12 and 14. In boys, the stages of genital
development begin with the enlargement of the testes, scrotum and penis. The scrotum skin loosens,
reddens and darkens. The development of a beard and the lowering of the voice occur during the later
stages.
The ovaries, uterus and vagina, or the penis and testes, are described as primary sex characteristics;
features such as the breasts, body shape, voice pitch and (in boys) beard are secondary sex
characteristics.
The age at which puberty occurs is variable. It varies between boys and girls, between individuals, across
subcultures within a society and between different societies. Some of the factors affecting this
103
Transitions and adolescence
variation can be clearly isolated, whereas others are part of a complex and subtle interaction of biology,
personality and society, and are the subject of much debate. The female body needs to have about 17
per cent body fat before puberty will begin, and about 22 per cent body fat for periods to continue.
Young female athletes such as dancers, gymnasts and swimmers do not have these proportions of fat,
and so menarche typically occurs for them around the age of 17. This is normal for their reference
group, and indeed earlier puberty might be bitterly disappointing for them – athletes after their first
period are taller, have significantly more body fat, and practise less (Bee and Boyd, 2007: 293; Klentrou
and Plyley, 2003). In most industrial countries, a random sample of 12- to 13-year-old girls will
find some who are already completing mature puberty and others who are just beginning. Ninety-five
per cent of girls have their first period between the ages of 11 and 15 (Bee and Boyd, 2007: 290, quoting
Malina, 1990). However, in 1850, the average age of menarche in industrial countries appears to
have been about seventeen. Better nutrition is most often quoted as the reason for earlier menarche
in global patterns, but Whitten (1992) argues that this is insufficient to account for the change,
and believes that the worldwide spread of industrial chemicals which mimic hormones must also
be involved (a classic statement of the evidence is found in Colborn and Clement, 1992). Stress in
early childhood is one of a range of other factors which can bring on female puberty earlier (Chisolm
et al., 2005).
In affluent Western societies, youngsters are dependent on their parents for many years, and this
extends the period of adolescence. In 1910 in the UK, the majority of young people had started work
by the age of 14, but their wages were usually low and contributed to the family income (often given
to mother). Girls remained in the family until they married, when they became part of their husband’s
household. The extended period of transition is a comparatively new phenomenon. The arrival of
‘teenagers’ with significant money of their own, creating a target for advertisers and producers of
goods (particularly music related), was a new feature of the 1950s, following the Second World War.
The situation today, with adolescents forming a major and lucrative market for clothes, entertainment
104
Transitions and adolescence
and lifestyle goods, is distinctive to our culture and period of history. This drawn-out ‘adolescence’
contains various markers of the arrival of ‘adulthood’. The age of criminal responsibility in Scotland is
8, and in England and Wales it is 10 (the lowest ages in Europe). The age of majority is the age at which
a person has the majority of the – not the total – legal rights to manage their own body and property,
rather than it being vested in their parents, and in the UK it is 18. Sex is illegal when it involves a girl
under the age of 16 in the UK (in a study to which reference is made later in the chapter, Singh and
colleagues found that surveys find 20 per cent of girls and 30 per cent of boys report first intercourse
before this age), but an offence is committed if sexually provocative images of her are made or owned
before she is eighteen. Conviction of these offences bars the person from working in many occu-
pations. A driving licence can be obtained from age 17, the armed forces can be entered from 16.5 (the
lowest age in Europe), and the right to vote arrives at 18. Until the age of 25, income support is lower
for single people because they are assumed to have parental support (among other things, this has
serious implications for children in care whose parents have been deemed unable to carry out their
parental responsibilities, as until recently they were not entitled to any further state ‘parental’ care
after the age of 18). Young people generally leave home later than in previous decades, and because
of delayed entry into employment, increasing numbers return home before finally leaving for good
(Morrow and Richards, 1996). Some of those with domestic problems risk becoming homeless.
Modern British society is varied, made up of many micro-cultures with expectations differing between
groups. For some, adolescence is a heavily sexualised period. In one subculture the dominant values
may prize academic, athletic or musical achievement, whilst in another these may be significantly less
important than social activities with peers, largely outside the supervision of parents and other adults.
In one subculture, prestige or fear may lead to physical violence and the use of guns, whilst in another
the routines set by religion or other established adult systems may provide a framework for the
transition to independence. ‘Young people who feel that they have been let down by authority earlier
need to have these feelings addressed before they can re-engage with the system’ (Frankham et al.,
2007). Evidence from the education and poverty programme of the Joseph Rowntree Foundation (for
example, Wikeley et al., 2007; Hirsch, 2007) suggests that learning that takes place in activities outside
school is crucial to positive social development; on the other hand, Burton and Marshall (2005) found
in a survey of 169 students in Glasgow that there was an opposite correlation for sport – for the boys
in their sample, involvement in sport was associated with a greater likelihood of being involved in
delinquent behaviour. Wikeley found that young people from families in poverty participate in fewer
organised out-of-school activities than their more affluent peers.
In respect of adolescence, culture, then, both shapes and is shaped by the experience and behaviour
of young people. Common cultural ideas about adolescence include that it is a time of fluctuating
moods and conflict with or defiance of authority; a time when attempts to assert an individual
identity may well create stormy situations – leading one writer to assert rather provocatively that
‘youth on the other hand, is contemporaneously expected to be an age of deviance, disruption and
wickedness` (Brown, 2005: 3). The next section begins by looking at the research evidence about
these views.
105
Transitions and adolescence
Generational conflict?
Although the peak age for offending seems to be about 17 – and in 2005, 25 per cent of people
between the ages of 10 and 25 reported committing an offence in the previous twelve months
(Wilson et al., 2006) – research on the whole does not support the idea that adolescence is a time of
particular stress or intergenerational conflict, except for a specific minority of people. On the other
hand, reviews of findings about adolescent–parent conflict have interpreted it both as functional
for the continuing development of the family and of society, and as crossing cultural boundaries;
it is neither specific to ‘individualistic’ societies (like American British) nor absent from ‘collectivist’
cultures (like China, where conformity is prized). There appear to be complex interactions between
parental and child temperaments which vary with gender – for example, Kawaguchi and colleagues
(1998) found that low adaptability in fathers and daughters was related to greater adolescent–
parent conflict, but this was not the case for fathers and sons both low on adaptability. Some of their
research about mothers echoes those of Silverberg and Steinberg (1987), who found that it was
mothers’ temperament and emotional well-being that related to the intensity of adolescent conflict
rather than fathers’. However, this is in the context of their finding that only a minority of American
families (perhaps 10 per cent) experience a deterioration in parent–child relationships in early
adolescence, and this is often related to (it builds on) difficulties earlier in childhood. As you may
surmise, this is an area in which there has been extensive research and numerous different inter-
pretations of the data.
The overarching messages from much of the research is that: teens generally retain strong attach-
ments to their parents; it is parents who worry more about conflict than their children; good early
attachments to parents correlate strongly with adolescent happiness and well-being, as well as with
educational achievement and (for girls) reduced risk of early pregnancy. These correlations hold
across cultures, and when asked to identify ‘someone you can talk to about problems/someone who
makes you feel good’, teenagers overwhelmingly put parents top of their list, above their peers (for
information about various research studies, see Boyd and Bee, 2006: 329).
This section, however, points you towards the way other features of the internal world can be under-
stood during adolescence. Adolescence reawakens ‘states of mind’ from the earliest days up to the
present. In adolescence, the states of mind of an autonomous adult individual may alternate quite
106
Transitions and adolescence
quickly with those of a child who needs comfort and protection, the state of mind which requires
adults to take care of it. The person whose destructive and violent impulses in childhood were not met
with containment and responsive care experiences frustration and contradiction as a threat to the self
which will not be resolved. The natural response is fear, aggression and attack. The person for whom
early experiences were genuinely life-threatening, and who as a child had to cope with the outcomes
as best they could, may experience passing setbacks in adolescence as if they are a threat to the
existence of the self. The young person whose emotions were never regulated in a caring, attentive
and resilient relationship may express the fury of a child’s tantrums but with the power and aggressive
force of a mature body. A number of researchers and practitioners emphasise that in adolescence,
dilemmas and challenges resolved in one way during infancy and childhood can come up afresh for
new resolution as the person enters adulthood.
As described in Chapter 2 and ‘Essential background’, section 4, attachment needs are
understood to persist throughout life. The attachment relationship has the function of
providing comfort in times of distress, protection from danger and a secure base from
Chapters
2 and EB4 which to explore. As these years progress, the attachment bond increasingly does not need
physical proximity to be effective – for the securely attached person (unlike for the 3-
year-old), the parents may be taken for granted as a protective and caring force even when the young
person is temporarily living away from them. On the other hand, the consequences of negative early
attachment experiences may show themselves dramatically. Attachment theorists consider that one
of the functions of attachment is to allow a coherent ‘narrative’ of the self to develop, and allow
cognitive functioning to develop correctly (Holmes, 2000: 49–51). And conversely, a disrupted and
destructive attachment-type relationship can hinder the development of a coherent sense of self and
interfere with accurate thinking. The young person who does not know where he has lived at different
times in his childhood, or who he lived with, who has been told lies about himself, told that he does
not have the feelings he actually experiences (‘Come on, stop being silly, you’re not frightened at all
– stop being stupid!’), or that those who care for him do not have the feelings they actually have –
(‘I love you so much, you’re the apple of my eye’, said by a parent who is actually self-obsessed and
unable to attend to the feelings of anyone else) may end up having significant difficulties in logical
information processing, particularly under stress. The experience of duplicity, confusion and a lack of
certainty about events as an endemic experience in early years can later be expressed as ‘lying’ and a
lack of accuracy about facts in the present. Youngsters in trouble may come to believe their own
falsehoods about what has happened on a particular occasion because ‘facts’ about personal life have
always been arbitrary.
Unfortunately, it may be those who are most in need of a secure base (because of a lack of love, security
or care in their childhood) who have most firmly decided that they no longer need or expect one, and
therefore are least likely to find it.
Adults who are responsible for adolescents in trouble need themselves to receive ongoing support so
that they can remain attached to the (often difficult) young person – caring and non-retaliatory even
when faced with severe provocation. Persisting in this attachment is important for the happiness and
107
Transitions and adolescence
security of the young person in the next phase of their life. Here are the words of ‘Emma’, who spoke
to me when she was 21, some years after she had left the children’s home she refers to (she had friends
similar to the fictional ‘Sharon’):
When I first went to X (home), I thought, ‘Another home’ – you know what I mean; it’s going to
be one of them again, where I would just stay so long then I would get moved. So when I was at
X, everyone was my enemy. . . . Me and Ann-Marie [staff member] never used to get on; I used to
hate Ann-Marie. I have threatened Ann-Marie with a knife – I’ve bitten her and everything because
I didn’t like her.
So for me, for X (home) to put up with the way I was, I am quite surprised. For a child that can’t
think straight, and not being able to trust one person in their life because they have been moved
about . . .
And it’s weird how Ann-Marie and me now get on dead well . . . I was fostered with her after a
while, for a year. It was hard when I had to move on, but she was like the mum I always wanted,
even though I know she’s not.
Singh and colleagues (2000) reviewed the most recent nationally representative surveys in fourteen
countries (a total of over 25,000 young people), and found that in most countries, roughly one-third
or more of teenage women and between half and three-quarters of men have had intercourse; in four
countries (Ghana, Mali, Jamaica and Great Britain), about three in five are heterosexually experienced.
In Great Britain and America, the relevant surveys referred only to heterosexual activity, so there is
likely to be a smaller additional group who have been sexually active with someone of the same sex.
In most countries, sexual intercourse during the teenage years occurs predominantly outside marriage
among men but largely within marriage among women. According to the authors, who describe the
108
Transitions and adolescence
UK data as among the most reliable in their study, about 20 per cent of women and about 30 per cent
of men have sexual intercourse before 16, the legal age of consent. These figures are broadly in line
with Wellings’ 2001 study of 11,161 men and women. This figure relates to any individual occurrence,
not necessarily to a continuing pattern of behaviour – the study found that ‘never-married young
people are considerably less likely to be currently sexually active than to be sexually experienced’.
This summarises statistics which come out of the answers to questionnaires in a very delicate area of
life. To create a picture of what actually happens in any meaningful way requires a lot of other
information about whether the sex is genuinely consensual, whether the contact is sporadic and with
different partners or constant within a consistent relationship, and so on. In a longitudinal study of
health-related matters in New Zealand, 54 per cent (211/388) of women reported that they should have
waited longer before having sex, and this rose to 70 per cent (90/129) for women reporting first
intercourse before the age of 16. It appears to be the case that for most women who have sex as
adolescents, their first heterosexual contact is remembered as a negative experience. Based on her
interviews with 400 teenage girls, Thompson’s study (1990) described the most prevalent stories
including negative themes such as boredom, physical and romantic disappointment, alienation
from the body or coercion. No doubt this is partly a matter of their own bodily exploration and partly
to do with the behaviour of their partners. The process of learning and maturation takes place
in different ways for different people. Equally, young men who engage in early sexual activity may
be unaware of the range of the emotional and sensual needs, of themselves and of their partners,
which are potentially met through sexuality. Once again, the subject highlights the complex interplay
of biology, bodily experience, emotion, interpersonal relationships and social attitudes which are
involved in sex.
Most people, male and female, will find that some core aspects of their sexual experience of themselves
are ignored, disapproved of or condemned by the majority culture in which they live; but this is
particularly acute for those whose objects of desire are of the same sex – they may be deprived even
of the acceptance (albeit often unspoken) of sexual reality which may be found in peer relationships.
Gay and lesbian young people, and to a lesser extent anyone else who experiences same sex attrac-
tion, have to affirm a large part of their sexual persona for themselves, without the support of social
structures which validate their orientation, and in the face of ‘cultural denial, distorted stereotypes,
rejection, neglect, harassment and sometimes outright victimisation and abuse’ (Morrow and
Messinger, 2006: 179). ‘I thought I was the devil’s disciple,’ said a young Muslim man (BBC 2007a),
‘I knew from about the age of 10 where my sexual attraction lay, but I was told that this was the work
of the devil and I would go to hell.’ Gerard Mallon (Mallon, 1998: 98) reports that in his interviews with
young American people who were in the care system and aware that they were gay, a common
description was that they experienced themselves as a ‘throw-away child’, part of the garbage, and
those who did not experience this directly had kept the sexual part of their identity secret for fear of
being treated in this way. In a study of young gay and lesbian people, the average age of awareness of
their sexual orientation was 10, the average age of labelling themselves as gay or lesbian was 14, the
average age of first disclosure to a friend was 16 and the average age of disclosure to family was 17.
109
Transitions and adolescence
So these young people are forming a socially stigmatised identity at exactly the time when peer
pressure to fit in is particularly strong.
Lillian, now 25, was talking about her teenage years: ‘In my first years at secondary
school I was very shy. Then I was outed without my consent. My girlfriend gave me a kiss
right in the middle of the playground. I found it very hard to cope and . . . just put a
cocoon around me. I became really, really butch. I wasn’t going to let anyone . . .’ Her
father was in the room during this conversation, and said: ‘But then it was just so
difficult. Everything was “because I’m gay”.’ Lilian argued back, ‘Well, you said I’d just grow
out of it, I’d get married, you wanted grandchildren.’ Father: ‘Well, yes, but, . . . and every
parent wants grandchildren.’
McCarthy (1999: 13) in her interviews with women with learning disabilities similarly found that ‘only
a small minority’ felt positive about their sex lives. In her concluding recommendations for policy and
practice, she highlights the imperative need to protect these women from abuse and unwanted sex,
and also the requirement that sex education should take the form of what her co-writer, David
Thompson, calls ‘erotic education’ – none of her respondents, for example, knew either the name for
or the existence of (let alone the potential) of their clitoris. These unsatisfactory experiences were with
men, and she highlights the specific need for education and support for men with learning disabilities
to reveal to them the satisfactions of giving pleasure and how that may be done.
110
Transitions and adolescence
Identity involves gender and sex, being a man or a woman in a particular society. Gilligan, Bingham
and Stryker would all identify the developments just described as occurring in a context in which there
may be a tendency to socialise girls to be more compliant and dependent. Lavinia Greenlaw, on whose
words the first example at the beginning of this chapter is based, talks of writing her book: ‘I set out
to write a book about music and found I was writing about becoming myself. I had to write about my
struggles with wanting to be and then not wanting to be, a girl. This was a time when I was trying not
to notice I was a girl’ (BBC Radio 4, 21 August 2007).
These ideas about adolescent development are placed by the authors in the context of the
work of Erik Erikson, and to varying degrees in opposition to them. He saw this fifth life stage
(see Chapter 2 and ‘Essential background’, section 6) as one in which a distinctly new identity
Chapters
2 and EB6 has to be forged, one more separate from the adults whose values and characteristics had
formed the child’s earlier identity. In his view, adolescence is the intermediate phase in which
the child’s identity and social roles are relinquished and an adult identity created. This is in preparation
for maturity – living independently, being financially autonomous, playing adult roles in sex, work and
family. The identification with the peer group so evident in adolescence provides a base, while the
dependence on parents’ definitions and evaluations of self are gradually left behind.
Erikson regarded this process as creating an identity crisis. It is no longer possible to rely on other
people’s perceptions of one’s own identity, but security in self-definition, in a sense of one’s unique
characteristics and value, is not yet established. You may recall that he regarded crises as inherent in
the process of development, and that each crisis can have a positive or a negative outcome. The positive
outcome of this crisis he saw as a mature sense of self – and the negative outcome as role confusion.
In discussing ‘identity’ in this way, developmental theory refers both to what is created by the individual
(a unique personal sense of security and confidence in who one is, independent of other people’s
perceptions), and to the influence of outside expectations, which are often based on ideas about group
characteristics. Different writers on matters affecting social work emphasise different sides of this
balance. Someone who has ‘a strong sense of their own identity’ is someone whose idea of themself,
their qualities, and how they should behave socially, is not thrown off course by other people’s
expectations; but people also talk of their identity ‘as a Muslim’ or ‘as a black person’, a man, or a
woman – categories making heavy use of mutual social expectations.
Children in care
For children in care1 the path to adulthood is often more difficult than for their peers. Of children who
are looked after by the state, 80 per cent are in care because their parents’ care was deemed to cause
them ‘significant harm’ (Children Act, 1989: secs 31, 47). But their developmental needs will not
necessarily be met just because the state has taken responsibility. At the time of writing, 42 per cent
of young people in Britain go to university. Of children in care, however, the figure is just 1 per cent
111
Transitions and adolescence
(Jackson et al., 2005). The survey by Meltzer (Meltzer, 2003) for the Office of National Statistics came
up with similar figures to those of Dimigen and colleagues (1999), indicating that 40 per cent of
adolescents in care suffer emotional distress of a level which is of psychiatric significance. Very high
levels of depression were found to be common. Statistically, in all kinds of ways, the risks of negative
outcomes in adolescence are much greater for children in care. Of people in prison under the age of
20, 50 per cent were brought up in care. Payne and Butler (1998) argue that the health needs of
children in care are not adequately met. Hobbs and colleagues (1999) put forward the view that the
very fact of being in care should be regarded as a risk factor for abuse, on the basis that the rate of
abuse is 3.9 per 1,000 in birth families, 39.5 per 1,000 in foster care, and 23 per 1,000 in residential
care. Note that although these figures probably give an accurate figure of the risk of abuse experienced
by young people, they require significantly more interpretation in order to be understood as giving
information about carers’ behaviour (see Sudbery et al., 2005: 73–90). Similarly, the other risk factors
described in this paragraph are not necessarily caused by being cared for by the local authority –
educationally, for example, children in care are perhaps ‘low achievers’ rather than ‘under-achievers’.
Nevertheless, these statistics show that special attention must be paid to the issues faced by children
in care.
Earlier sections of this chapter referred to the process in adolescence of moving physically away from
an attachment figure whilst keeping the attachment bond intact, allowing it to mature into a different
form. What is the nature of the attachment which is being modified for the child in care, and what is
the meaning of physical separation – living away – for such a young person?
Adolescents in general are reticent about sexual and emotional matters when talking with adults.
Often, therefore, the carers of a young person in care who is subjected to violence, or involved in
prostitution, or is suicidal, are faced with a challenging task. They must be able to allow the young
person their burgeoning (if at times unrealistic) autonomy, and yet find a way of being available to
discuss matters which may be of grave significance in the young person’s life.
Teenagers examine their peers for any difference, and difference can single a vulnerable child out for
taunts or insults. Equally, the individual does not want to be ‘different’ in any way which attracts low
status. How then do children in care construct their social identity? Matters such as discussing where
they live or inviting other people back to their house are everyday matters of social concealment or
lying. The fabric of teenage life that builds identity, such as personal privacy or ‘sleepovers’, are not
matters of personal negotiation but matters of state regulation and bureaucracy.
Sue lay in bed, unable to sleep, fuming. She hated moving to a new place and here she
was with new foster parents. It meant all that stuff at school all over again. They ask
you where you live. What are you supposed to say? And you can just see the ones who
are going to dig and dig and dig until they get it out of you. ‘Bastards. Do they see it
straight away? Is it like I’ve got “Child in Care” tattooed across my forehead?’ she
thought. This always made her think of something else. Her mother used to feel she had
been through World War Three when she eventually got to her bar job in the evenings. Sue
112
Transitions and adolescence
did everything she could think of to stop her going out. Her mum came to hate her for
it. But after her mum had gone, Sue would go straight to her room, couldn’t
concentrate. And then every bloody time she’d hear those steps coming up the stairs,
one by fucking one – she could hear every footfall of her stepfather.
She had very little doubt that everyone who looked at her could see she was sexually
abused, knew what had gone on. Just looking at her they could see everything.
She didn’t sleep at all that night, was up before anyone else but didn’t show her face
outside the room. Made an effort to get to school. And then there’s all that stuff –
‘Susan Bates, why aren’t you concentrating?’ She knew what she wanted to say – ‘Oh,
just fuck off.’
The risk of poor outcomes for children in care varies considerably, even between neighbouring
European countries, but it is easier to see problems than to be certain of the remedies. One approach
has been to set governmental targets – for improving educational performance, for example, or
reducing the number of moves experienced by children in care – with the threat of penalties if
authorities fail. This, however, can have the paradoxical effect of care authorities being preoccupied
with meeting government targets rather than with the individuals in their care. Children can become
(in the words of a report into the management of child abuse – Butler Sloss, 1988) ‘objects of concern’
rather than individuals. Gaskell in her study of young people in care found that what they most wanted
was for someone to care about them and to demonstrate this care – for example, by listening to them,
by doing the things which reasonable parents do for their children. ‘If the social worker had called, it
would have shown they cared’, as one young person said to her.
The particular qualities of resilience that are found in children who have faced exceptional
adversity tend towards independence and self-reliance (Howe, 2005; see Chapters 2
and 3 above). The task for those responsible for ‘looked after’ children is both to support
Chapters
2 and 3
qualities such as these at the same time as recognising that there are urgent unmet
attachment needs. This delicate balancing act requires particular maturity. McMurray and
colleagues (2008) found in their study that social workers tended to have a simplified view of the
resilience of children in care that could lead them to underestimate the psychological challenges faced
by those children.
113
Transitions and adolescence
be as interested as anyone else in the changes that occur in their bodies and emotions, but some,
because of the very limited time ever spent away from adult surveillance, will miss out on these conver-
sations. For teenagers with spina bifida, Blum and colleagues (1991) found ‘the majority of information
comes from parents and teachers’. These adults may be unwilling to fill this particular gap, and the result
may be that the teenagers experience loss, doubts and an unnecessary sense of difference.
Adolescents in general are expected to take increasing responsibility for their life decisions, leisure
activities, health care, accommodation and work. They are expected to gain increasing control and
authority over their bodies and their lives.
Nowadays, these expectations are no different for young people with learning or physical disabilities,
to the extent that is possible given their impairment. In adolescence, however, disabled teenagers are
likely to become aware of the obstacles they will experience in achieving their adult roles. They learn
the challenging mechanisms involved in tackling these barriers. The law, education and employment
will take the official view that all individuals are entitled to equality and access. There may be a
dramatic difference, however, between the way things are organised for children and the way they are
organised for adults. These discontinuities and disparities may make transition particularly difficult for
people with disabilities, especially as changes in organisation and philosophy are determined according
to chronological age, not the developmental stage and experience of the individual.
Many parents find that it is a constant battle to get their disabled children the benefits and services
they are entitled to (Emerson and Hatton, 2005b; Quinn, 1998; Sloper and Beresford, 2006). In general,
however, services are intended to be person-centred and child-focused. Adult services may be
organised on a basis that gives much less priority, even in principle, to continuity and the emotional
components of care. An individual may need to make persistent efforts in order to be offered
‘reasonable adjustments’ for their capacity, such as low intellectual ability, in an educational or
employment setting. Young disabled people are likely to be creating their identities and working out
how to tackle barriers when they have restricted personal spending and have incomes (Hirst and
Baldwin, 1994) below those of other young people.
There are different challenges according to the different ways a person’s body or brain may differ from
the average. Most people learn about sport and other social activities by watching others at play. An
adolescent with severe visual impairment, however, may need to rely on someone else realising that
they need explicit information and encouragement. Adolescents with Down’s syndrome may gradually
become aware of differences, and of sexual issues, but should not be expected to understand and make
sense of these (because of their limited capacity to learn from incidental social cues) without explicit
explanation. Adults thinking about the sexual education of severely visually impaired girls and boys
have to realise and be comfortable with the reality that they are much more dependent on learning
through feeling and touch than though visual explanations. Teenagers who lack sensation below the
waist are entitled to conversations about sexual needs, which apply to them just as they do to others
– for example, the role of sexual intimacy in providing personal contact, reassurance, relief from anxiety
and alleviation of stress, and the different forms this intimacy may take. For many young people with
114
Transitions and adolescence
extensive physiological difference from others, the constant public gaze in therapeutic and medical
examination may not have prepared them to construct appropriate personal bodily boundaries. A child
with cerebral palsy may experience pubertal changes earlier than her peers, and this for a girl may
increase her sense of being apart and different (Quinn, 1998: 123).
Measures of self-esteem comparing disabled adolescents with the general population present contra-
dictory pictures. Hirst and Baldwin (1994) found that disabled adolescents were more likely than their
non-disabled peers to report feelings reflecting a poor sense of worth. King and colleagues (1993), on
the other hand, report roughly equivalent self-esteem measures for both disabled and non-disabled
adolescents, indicating that when there are differences, they involve psychosocial characteristics of
families, parental values and expectations.
Looking closer
In Erikson’s view, at adolescence the individual identity, previously shaped by the definitions of
others, becomes an adult identity based on understanding one’s own unique characteristics.
Gilligan’s work is based on the perception that this tends to set up a ‘masculine’ idea of rugged
individualism as normal development. For many women, in her view, mature identity is based on
‘relatedness to others’.
• Do these ideas fit with your experience?
• Are Erikson’s ideas of identity formation in adolescence more applicable to that stage than
earlier or later transitions?
• How far is individual identity separate from others’ definitions and social expectations?
Transitions
Adolescence can be described as a ‘transition’ from childhood to adulthood. This chapter has referred
to many different transitions that may take place – the transition from experiencing life in a child’s
body to experiencing life in a sexually mature adult body; for the disabled child, the transition from
dealing with services organised for children to those organised for adults; the transition to the
expectation of greater personal control over finance, or the use of time.
All social work is about transitions in people’s lives. Successful family support work is concerned with
helping parents and children to find a happier, more satisfying, less frightening life – less stressful and
less unhappy. With a child coming into care (and all too frequently with children who are in care), you
will be concerned with the move from one accommodation to another; with people with mental health
115
Transitions and adolescence
Higher
Mood
Lower
Stage 1 Stage 2
Immobilisation 2a) Elation or despair Stage 3 Time
2b) Minimisation Self-doubt Stage 4 Stage 5 Stage 6
Stage 7
Accepting reality Testing Searching for
and letting go meaning Integration
Figure 4.1
Hopson’s model of transitions
116
Transitions and adolescence
Hopson’s studies suggest that transitions are more stressful if they are unpredictable, involuntary,
unfamiliar, of high magnitude, or frequent. He believes that every transition, however undesirable,
offers the opportunity for personal growth and development.
1 Immobilisation: A sense of being overwhelmed: ‘It can’t be happening to me’; ‘This isn’t true’.
2 Reaction:
i) Elation or despair: a sharp swing of mood in the direction determined by the nature of the
transition.
ii) Minimisation: playing down the significance of the change, even to the point of denial. This
may take the form of thoughts like, ‘Well, it’s all very well getting this great job, but now I’ve
got to do it!’ or, ‘Well, it’s not so bad really – life goes on.’ Denial can have a positive function,
enabling a person to cope with a situation that would be too overwhelming to face head on.
3 Self-doubt: A dip in feelings as the person becomes particularly conscious of the implications and
challenges ahead. May involve feelings of depression, anger or apathy.
4 Letting go: The earlier phases involve much looking back. In this phase, the past is put behind the
individual, who can now face up to the future. This is regarded as crucial in managing transitions.
Levinson describes it as a step into the unknown, as the person ‘is cut adrift from the past, but
cannot yet see the land of the future’.
5 Testing: Engaging with the new reality, the person tries out new strategies and approaches. New
lifestyles and identities are sampled and discarded.
6 The search for meaning: a period of reflective thinking as the significance and personal meaning
of the transition is explored.
7 Integration/internalisation: The final phase, involving the internalisation of the new meanings that
have been discovered; the incorporation of these into behaviour, roles and outlook.
As with all the topics in this book, there is extensive literature exploring the subject further. For
example, research has analysed the different factors involved in coping, identifying: the situation; the
individual’s characteristics; the nature of support available; the coping strategies adopted (Sugarman,
2001). Other studies explore which transitions tend to be most stressful (Holmes and Rahe, 1967), how
transitions can be negotiated more or less successfully, and the vulnerability created by poorly
negotiated transitions.
Looking closer
Hopson analysed how the journey through the phases is affected by characteristics of the
transition – whether it is positive, negative, sudden, unpredictable, frequent, of high intensity.
Read more in Sugarman (2001).
117
Transitions and adolescence
?
Reflective thinking
List three transitions – including at least one that took place in your own life (for example,
starting a university course) and one that would involve a social worker.
How far do you think the model proposed by Hayes and Hopson fits the examples you have chosen?
The final section of this chapter is about the difficulty of describing ‘universal’ characteristics
of human development. Thinking about the earlier sections of this chapter, what are some of
the problems in describing ‘universal’ truths about human development? Write some general
statements about adolescence, and then explain why they are not always true.
TAKING IT FURTHER
Are we all the same? Humanistic models and their critics
The humanistic models created by Abraham Maslow and Carl Rogers have natural appeal in
emphasising the common humanity of all people, the universal need for unconditional positive
regard, and the desire in all people to achieve something worthwhile with their lives. For many,
these ideas form a natural basis for social work. But they were originally developed in mid-
twentieth-century America and were expressed in phrases littered with statements about ‘man’ and
phrased with the gendered pronoun ‘he’. Do such universalised formulations inherently (but
sometimes covertly) privilege the cultural position of a particular commentator? Some have argued
that they are based on a highly individualistic patriarchal culture, and result in superficially ‘similar
treatment for everyone’ (thereby doing violence to the different experience of men and women,
and the specific needs of different cultural and religious groups). This is an issue for all
universalising theories of human need and development – here, the question is examined in
relation to the humanistic models.
118
Transitions and adolescence
• The drive for growth comes from within the individual (so, for example, the counsellor or other
helper is more like a nurturing gardener than a physician dispensing medicine).
• Each individual is inherently ‘self-actualising’.
• For Maslow, fundamental survival needs (such as physiological needs and the need for physical
safety) are the foundation on which self-actualising builds. Once survival needs are met, they
are no longer motivating, for they are experienced only when they are deficits. The growth
and self-actualising motivation, however, continues.
• For Rogers, all organisms, including ‘man’, fundamentally know what is good for them, and
value whatever helps them achieve their full potential. Humans have a need for both positive
regard (from others) and positive self-regard.
• During development, however, positive regard from others may come only ‘conditionally’. The
conditions attached may be out of accord with authentic organismic valuation – that is, ideas
about who the child ‘ought’ to be, its ‘ideal self’, are out of tune with the real self. The bigger
the gap between the socially induced ‘ideal self’, and the authentic individual, the ‘real self’,
the greater the suffering. To survive, the individual has to use distortion and denial.
According to the research associated with this model, the key features of helping relationships are
not the techniques used by the helper, but the qualities of the helper – that they give unconditional
positive regard, show empathy and offer non-possessive warmth (amended to ‘congruence’ in later
models).
119
Transitions and adolescence
In summary, then, the humanistic model of development is that everyone needs both positive
regard and positive self-regard. If they are cared for and grow up in an atmosphere of acceptance,
their own natural tendencies will cause the most valuable of human qualities to develop. If they
grow up with conditions attached to positive regard (to be a good girl, a female must be clean, neat
and submissive; to be a man, a boy must be aggressive, loud and never frightened), then they will
develop a ‘false self’ as well as a natural self. This causes pain and social difficulty – there will be
a split between what their natures want them to be and how they think they ought to be. An
essential foundation for developing valued ‘human’ qualities is having basic needs met –
physiological needs, safety needs, needs for care and affection.
120
Transitions and adolescence
has been done in relation to many models – attachment theory, cognitive development and
bereavement, for example. In this way it can be established whether it holds for both men and
women, Western and non-Western cultures, gay and straight people, and so on; and if so, what
differences there may be. Like the generic criticism outlined in the previous paragraph, however,
this will be beside the point in relation to humanistic theory – it could hardly be that men need
unconditional positive regard, but that it does not matter to women!
It is a different kind of criticism that is being made. The critics argue that the apparent universality
of the theory conceals its partisan nature. Because it fails to mention the specifically gendered
obstacles to development faced by women, humanistic psychology may be seen as one of the
apparently gender-neutral theories which in fact collude with the oppression of women – ‘in all
societies which divide the sexes in differing cultural or economic spheres, women are less valued
than men’ (Humm, 1992, quoted in Kagan and Tindall, 2003). It is argued that to be gender-neutral
is to be gender-blind, and that any theory which does not mention gender is colluding with gender
oppression.
Carol Hanisch wrote an article in 1969 (republished in 2006 with a commentary by her) with the
title The Personal Is Political. In other words, personal matters embody the structures of society.
For her, as for a number of feminist writers who have used the phrase since, the ‘political’ includes
the systematic oppression of women by men. This is seen as inherent to our society and is described
as ‘patriarchy’ by these authors. Hanisch wrote, ‘Women are messed over, not messed up! We need
to change the objective conditions, not adjust to them. Therapy is adjusting to your bad personal
alternative . . . personal problems are political problems’. As Morrow and Messinger (1998, quoted
in Kagan and Tindall, 2003) put it, ‘The individual experiences of dis-ease experienced by women
have their roots in the powerlessness of women as a class’. For these feminists, a central feature of
female development is the creation of a conscious awareness of this ‘patriarchal’ status quo. In this
view, male gender power is seen as central to personal development. However highly
girls achieve (Chapter 3 of this volume), according to Abbott the educational system
always disadvantages them (Abbott et al., 2005: 89). Societal power dynamics are
Chapter 3 understood to be inescapably bound up with personal relationships – for example, ‘the
question of whether heterosexuality is a tenable practice for feminists remains a source
of contention’ (Abbott et al., 2005: 228). From this point of view, a philosophy which centres on
personal relationships and experience but makes no reference to the dynamics of privilege
inherent in them is seen as inadequate.
Related critiques are made by those who regard society as inherently racist. If language and social
structures (particularly in post-imperial and post-slavery societies and culture) privilege those seen
as ‘white,’ then components of personal development for others involve the challenge of this. The
‘personal’ conditions for this to take place, as specified by humanistic theory, should also specify
that this political dimension is built into the personal.
Taken to its logical conclusion, similar criticism could be advanced in turn by those who regard
present-day social relations as inherently ‘disablist’ or class-based or Islamophobic.
A number of such commentators quote Foucault as a thinker who emphasised that all concepts in
a culture are interlinked, depending on others for their meaning. If apparently gender-neutral
statements in fact refer to male hegemony in the culture, then close analysis will always show that
gender-neutral statements of theory (such as the ‘humanists’’) in fact privilege male experience and
rights at the expense of females.
121
Transitions and adolescence
In detail, critics argue that the model presents the ‘self’ (understood to be ‘self-actualising’) as a
kind of heroic male. For women, it is argued, the self is always experienced in relationship, not as
an isolated figure who prizes above all else her ability to form ideas independently of others (Wine,
1989/2007). The process of development is inseparable from relationship. Black commentators
have argued similarly that the African way of being does not privilege the ‘self-actualising’
individual (which is seen as a white Anglo-American concept) but places priority and value on the
group, the common good, so the humanistic model prioritises the isolated individual in a way that
is alien to African tradition. Finally it is pointed out that the attributes identified by Rogers (2004)
and Maslow (1954/2003) sound suspiciously like the self-description of privileged, white, middle-
class, mid-twentieth-century America – with no sense of the power dynamic and oppression that
maintain this privilege. The qualities are:
(Remember that the accounts were developed in the years following Nazism in Europe, in which
whole nations had shown how minds can be taken over by the power of a charismatic but
murderous leader.)
The argument is not with the words of the formulation, but that it focuses on personal interaction
in a way that ignores, and indeed makes invisible, the social positions of the individuals involved.
Those who adopt this stance see it not as the absence of something that can be added in, but as
a blindspot of the definition. For these authors, the personal is intrinsically also the social,
particularly in relation to the position of women and of people socially defined as ‘black’: a
developmental model concerned with the personal which ignores positioning in society is unfit for
its purpose.
The humanistic model values the ideal of a relationship-based self which seeks interpersonal
integrity and satisfaction. A final critique comes from those who are sceptical of the existence of a
core, integral developing self – as Bilton (2002: 331) summarises, ‘Many postmodern social
theorists have engaged in a sustained assault on the humanistic idea of a willed, creative, choosing
actor . . . As Foucault stressed, the very notion of the originating free-willed actor or subject is a
historically specific construction.’
There are many counter-arguments. Feminists adopt a wide range of different viewpoints, and
many regard the humanistic model – with its emphasis on equality, common humanity, empathy
and acceptance, the centrality of ‘human’ contact regardless of measurable outcome or efficiency
justification – as an excellent starting point. The idea of ‘patriarchy’ as a pervasive universal force
to be blamed and fought against can be seen as simplistic. Human societies are created by both
men and women, and different groups have different areas of privilege in different stages and
segments of life, from the power and influence that women typically exercise over young children
to the power of street gangs, policy makers, judges and lawmakers (Pollert, 1996).
122
Transitions and adolescence
Some have regarded the criticisms as misguided because they suggest that women are ‘essentially’
all the same, and essentially possessing different needs and characteristics from men, rather than
acknowledging the diversity of, and the impact of culture on, the way in which femininity is
expressed and experienced. A similar criticism had been made by those concerned with cultural
diversity – namely that the critique outlined above assumes the existence of an African ‘essence’,
which is factually wrong and inappropriately based on stereotypes (Appiah, 1997). The criticisms,
it is said, group ‘women’ or ‘black people’ together even more unhelpfully than the original
reference to common humanity. If this criticism of humanistic theory stands, then the same
arguments should be raised for disabled children, gays and lesbians, people excluded because
of income and family background, people with learning difficulties, and every combination of
these – there is no end to the divisions in society that every child has to encounter and rethink for
themselves. Faced with this, it is argued, there is indeed value in underpinning such diversity with
statements of common human need.
Whilst the ‘postmodern’ arguments emphasise the way our sense of self is constructed within par-
ticular social arrangements, social workers find that the people they assist need to be treated as
authentic individuals with consistent identities and important personal histories. They experience
themselves, in their work as well as in other aspects of their lives, setting out to achieve something
of value, and to use their time and personal qualities in a worthwhile way – just as the humanistic
models describe.
All social workers must grapple with some of the ideas presented in this essay. They will sometimes
wonder what is the value of their work, and wonder what of its satisfactions are illusions created
for self-regarding motives. Questions related to the issues discussed will arise in practice – as, for
example, when there is a choice between recommending an adoption placement for an African
child with white parents who appear fully capable of offering ‘the core conditions’, or waiting for
an indefinite length of time to find a more ‘culturally appropriate’ placement.
Summary
In adolescence, there is a spurt in the growth of bones and muscles. The proportion of fat to muscle
changes – differently in males and females – and there are notable changes in the structure and
size of the brain. These changes are triggered by the hormone system, and changes in the sex
hormones are particularly distinctive of this period, as both male and female become adults capable
of reproduction.
The age of adolescence varies in different cultures, as does its meaning and characteristics. How
adolescence is typically viewed in developed industrial countries at the moment – including
characteristics of ‘teenagers’ and older adolescents, duration of adolescence, the age at which it
occurs – are not universal, but are distinctive of these particular cultures at this point in history.
Attachment needs persist through adolescence, and states of mind otherwise typical of a child and
of an adult may alternate.
123
Transitions and adolescence
Adolescence is a time a when the individual explores and begins to form an adult sexual identity.
Erikson’s model identifies adolescence as the time when the child’s identity, shaped by adults, is
being left behind in favour of one formulated by the individual themselves (with peer influence).
The chapter referred to the work of Gilligan, and Bingham and Stryker, who emphasised some of the
ways in which females are fashioning new ways of relating to others, and exploring the ways they
value themselves; these ideas are, to varying degrees, in contrast with Erikson’s ideas that the
adolescent’s core development is in autonomy and independence.
The chapter referred to the particular challenges faced by young people in care and by disabled
adolescents.
Adolescence is a transition, and social work often involves assisting people in transitions (such as
bereavement). The chapter outlined the seven stages identified in Hopson’s model of transitions. The
student is directed to sources of further detail about this – how passages through transition are
affected by the characteristics of the individual, the situation and the support available.
Finally, the chapter outlined some of the objections to one theory that attempts to provide a ‘uni-
versal’ statement of human development. The humanistic model emphasises people’s drive to make
something of their lives, the way they strive for higher qualities beyond basic survival needs, and
the individual’s need for unconditional acceptance in order to establish a resilient and rounded
personality. An objection expressed by some feminists and some anti-racist commentators is that
this ignores the social position of the individual, the distinctive features of women’s (or, for example,
African) experience and takes no account of the way ‘the personal is the political’.
Note
1 The legal term for children referred to in this section is ‘children looked after by the local authority’, or ‘looked
after children’. The grammatical oddness of the briefer version can cause confusion in written text, and I have
often used the less precise but commonly understood term ‘children in care’.
Further reading
About physical, cognitive and social development in adolescence:
Boyd, D. R. and Bee, H. L. (2006) Lifespan Development. Boston: Pearson/Allyn and Bacon, pp. 288–348.
Social work or other professional practice with adolescents:
Briggs, S. (2002) Working with Adolescents. Basingstoke: Palgrave.
An excellent, down-to-earth and practical overview of the responsibilities of services in relation to
adolescents with disability:
Quinn, P. (1998) Understanding Disability: A Lifespan Approach. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
124
Transitions and adolescence
More detail and subtlety about Hopson’s model of transitions – a good introduction to the literature on
transitions is found in:
Sugarman, L. (2001) Lifespan Development: Frameworks, Accounts and Strategies. Hove: Psychology Press.
More detail about the application of transition theory:
Schlossberg, N. K., Waters, E.B. and Goodman, J. (1995) Counselling Adults in Transition: Linking Practice with
Theory. New York: Springer.
125
Transitions and adolescence
?
Questions
1 What are some of the issues facing parents of an adolescent with Down’s syndrome?
2 How would you summarise the changes ahead of the child who is about to make the journey through
adolescence to adulthood?
3 In Rogers’ Humanistic model (‘Essential background’, section 7) people have a basic
tendency to make something good with their lives. For this to be achieved, it is important
they experience ‘unconditional positive regard’. Those who are helping others need to
Chapter offer empathy and unconditional acceptance. Illustrate how this model might apply in
EB7 a social work situation. What might be described as the weaknesses of this model?
126
In this chapter you will find:
5 Living
independently
• Leaving home – young adults living independently
• Prison
Bella, aged 19
Bella was quite clear in her mind. Her father had thrown her out. Initially she lived with
George, and for a few months lived the life of a teenager that had been denied her whilst
she was at home. After she become pregnant with Sharon, she often felt unwell, and felt
increasingly lonely and isolated. She knew in her heart that, for all his macho posturing,
George was incapable of caring for her and a baby. Things got worse after the birth.
George often stayed out; there were constant rows and many weekends when she did
not know where the food would come from. Well, she’d moved out before, and she’d do it
again. Offering to share the rent, she moved in with a friend. The one-bedroom flat,
15 miles from her home town was dark, damp and much too small for the two young
women and their children. She was relieved when a year later she took tenancy of a
council flat, even if it was on a large estate in an unpopular area. She had a deep love for
her daughter, but longed for some respite from the constant demands. Sharon was an
attractive child with a ferociously independent spirit, and mother and daughter clashed
constantly. Bella would fly into a rage at her daughter’s defiance, but her threats would
not bring the young girl to heel. Occasionally she thought of her mother. Her last twelve
months, when Bella was 15, and the cancer developed so rapidly, seemed to have been
the beginning of the nightmare. She felt very alone.
129
Living independently
Vivian, aged 25
The ball of Vivian’s hand, her oiled palms and fingertips, moved slowly down Ino’s back.
She concentrated on what she was doing, felt each bone from shoulder blade to base of
the 18-year-old’s spine, under the muscles and the dark brown skin. As well as giving
comfort, she herself felt briefly reassured by the bodily contact. The project was based
in the Red Cross Centre in Birmingham, and twenty minutes later she was speaking to
the BBC reporter in the front office. ‘I was told they hit my father with a sledgehammer.
Sometimes it makes me sad when I give the massage.’ The reporter said nothing; what
could she say? Ino had come in: ‘Everyone here has been running. Always running. At
home, it was a month before they wanted me to be married. Two aunties were holding
my legs and another was holding my hands. The older woman was between my legs and
had started cutting, but I was fighting. Then when they saw all the blood they stopped.
I have seen friends die from it. And so that night I ran. I came through many countries
in Africa and Europe, and now I am here, and we wait for your government to decide. They
send us there, and they send us here. We do not know where they send me next week.
Maybe they send me back. I live in a constant state of terrible anxiety, which is very hard
to describe. It is the best thing that the Red Cross has organised this scheme that
Vivian gives the massage. No one can solve the problem, especially until the government
decide my application. But only someone who is in the same position, like Vivian, who is
also a refugee waiting to hear whether she is to be sent back, only someone who knows
can give you a little comfort. For that small time you are not on your own.’ Vivian spoke
quietly – ‘When you’re an asylum seeker, when you are abused, you’re like a piece of dirt
floating on the wave. You’re running but you have no power. When I look after Ino or the
others, for that moment I know I can do something – and for that moment, they know
they are important.’
(For information about related Red Cross services, see British Red Cross, 2009.)
Introduction
This chapter is about ‘living independently’. It takes its theme from the phase of life which often follows
adolescence, but draws attention to the many other situations in which adults find themselves setting
out on a new life away from their earlier social contacts and surroundings. For many people assisted by
social workers, the subject has dark overtones. Problematic transitions disrupt social bonds and cut
people off from the social networks which provided meaning, comfort and security in their lives. We
look at loneliness, one aspect of this which can also be a feature of many other social work situations.
It is the first of four chapters concerned with features of adult life. There are many different paths
through adulthood. A choice, a chance event or illness combines with social circumstances to create a
130
Living independently
different future. History, politics, family traditions, gender, education and class all set the developmental
scene within which an individual makes decisions. As with ‘loneliness’, some of the themes picked out
in these chapters do not just apply to adults.
The narratives
The narratives at the beginning of this chapter are fictionalised. They are self-
contained, for readers who are consulting this chapter in isolation, but they draw
you into the life course of individuals. Sharon, whom you first met in Chapter 1,
Chapters
1, 2, 3, is given some background: the circumstances in which she grew up as her mother
8, 9 Bella set up home on her own, and how this was closely tied up with Bella’s rela-
tionship with her mother, and her father Bob, whom you met in Chapters 2 and 3
and will read more of in Chapters 8 and 9. Everyone’s life is different, but nothing in these
accounts would seem unusual to a social worker.
Vivian was introduced to you as a teenager in Chapter 4. Since then, ten years have
passed – years in which she has shown resourcefulness, strength and vivacity.
Regrettably, neither her story nor that of Ino is unusual in terms of female
Chapter 4
development. Up to 500,000 women in the EU alone are subjected to or threatened
by genital mutilation. Worldwide, Amnesty International (2008; see also WHO, 2006) estimates
that 135 million women have been subjected to genital mutilation, and approximately 6,000
girls undergo the operation each day of each year. The United Nations Refugee Council
estimates the number of people affected by conflict-induced forced displacement to be
26 million (UNHCR, 2007).
More information about asylum seekers and refugees in the UK may be found in the report of the
Independent Asylum Commission Deserving Dignity (Hobson et al., 2008). In summary, at the time
of writing, the chair of the commission (a former senior judge) described the UK as falling
‘seriously below the standards of a civilised society’ and condemned its treatment of asylum
seekers as ‘shameful for the UK’. The report and video recording of some of the witness testimonies
are available from the commission’s website (www.independentasylumcommission.org.uk).
Waris Dirie (Dirie et al., 2005; Dirie and Miller, 2006) gives a first-hand account of the life of
an African woman who has been subjected to mutilation as a girl.
131
Living independently
132
Living independently
Previous studies had found that women tend to maintain closer relationships with their
parents during the transition to adulthood than men (who tend to view separation from their
parents during late adolescence as promoting increased independence). These studies also
found that women tend to be more strongly affected by their relationships with their parents
than men are. The findings of Sneed and colleagues were in keeping with this – they found
that family contact tended to decrease more quickly for young men than for young women.
In general, they found higher levels of instrumentality amongst men in matters of finance.
As family contact decreased, ‘instrumentality’ increased over the ages from 17 to 27 for both
men and women. Statistically, in general there was a negative relationship between family
contact and ‘instrumentality’ – the more family contact a young person had, the less they
showed independence in finance and relationship matters. There was a change in this
relationship over time. For both males and females, as they grew older, the link between family
contact and lack of ‘instrumentality’ weakened, so in the older groups, increased family contact
became less likely to reduce instrumentality. In fact for men, at 27, the relationship between
the variables changed round, so that increased family contact had a small positive effect on
instrumentality. In discussing this, the researchers suggest that this concept of ‘instrumentality’
needs to be examined more closely. The existing psychological understanding of instru-
mentality is a kind of ‘rugged individualism’ prized by male culture, but they suggest there is
another sort which is based on responsibility in attending to relationships, which is more likely
to be typical of women, and is fostered in those mature men who have more contact with their
family.
The study was based on single interviews with 200 participants, so the data are vulnerable to
the effects of memory distortion, although the researchers took steps to minimise this.
A report by the Joseph Rowntree Foundation (Morrow and Richards, 1996) found in 1996 that while
young people expected autonomy and independence earlier than in the past, economic factors and
social policy had made them dependent on family support for longer, right into their twenties. They
entered the world of employment later than previous generations, and policies to do with income and
student support made them dependent on parents for longer. The report expressed particular concern
that ‘for children who are effectively “without kin” the “dependency assumption” is especially
problematic: research has shown that young people leaving local authority care who have no contact
with their families face a range of difficulties financially, socially and psychologically’ (page 3). Another
study compared the experiences of two large samples of young adults born over twenty years apart
(Bynner, 2002). It compared data collected on all the 10,000 people who had been born in a single week
in 1958 with that of nearly 16,000 people born in a single week in 19701. It described how a larger gap
had opened up between those who achieve a high level of educational qualification and those who do
133
Living independently
not, and noted that the ‘poverty penalty’ underlying this (people from poorer backgrounds were less
likely to gain qualifications) seemed to have increased.
Two of the risk factors highlighted in the previous paragraph – the lack of kin and lack of higher-level
educational achievements – bear heavily on children brought up in care. The next section summarises
some features of the experience of independent living for young people whose upbringing has been
the responsibility of the state.
Most young people setting out in their independent lives have the emotional (and often practical)
resource of their parents to keep in the background. Even when they rebel against the ideas of their
parents, consistent relationships have existed as a foundation on which they can build their own. Those
who because of earlier experiences are least equipped for living effective independent lives and coping
with the emotional and practical complexities of life are unfortunately often sent out to manage on
their own. The UK government prepared a leaflet for young people about their proposals, ‘Care Matters’
(DCSF/DfES, 2007). In it, they wrote:
Most young people do not leave home until they are ready. But young people in care often feel
they have to leave care, even if they are not ready to cope on their own. This is not a good or safe
way to leave home.
About a third of children in care move to independent living at just 16 or 17 (Morgan and Lindsay,
2006). One of the most important tasks for ‘corporate parents’ is to provide continuing resources for
the vulnerable people for whom they have assumed responsibility as children. All too often in the past,
official policy has determined a ‘cut-off’ point for this support, and it has been left to individual staff
(often without supervision and sometimes in opposition to official policy) to remain in touch with the
children they have cared for. The Care Leavers Act (2003) and other developments in process at the
time of writing make better official provision. They require the local authority to keep in touch with
care leavers until they are 21 and to provide a number of personal and financial services. It remains to
be seen whether the state activates this aspect of its responsibilities for children more effectively in
the future.
134
Living independently
Chapter 4 referred to some statistical outcomes for young adults who have been brought
up in care. In relation to education, for example, 1 per cent of care leavers, compared with
42 per cent of the general population, go to university. Regarding prison, 40 per cent of
Chapter 4
the young adult prison population have been brought up in care. Biehal and colleagues
(1992, 1995), Stein (2004) and Wade and Dixon (2006) are among those who report on a variety of
other outcomes, including rates of homelessness. Wade and Dixon, in their year-long study of 107
young people leaving care, found that a third experienced periods of homelessness within that period,
and the charity Rainer (Rainer, 2007) quotes research that one-third of rough sleepers experienced
periods of being in care as a child. Almost half of young women leaving care are mothers within 18–24
months of leaving care. Although many people brought up in care are grateful to the foster carers and
others who looked after them, these statistics point to the gap between what any parent would wish
for their own children leaving home and what care leavers face as they set out on their own in what
will often be a lonely and intimidating adult world.
Looking closer
Corporate parent
This is a term used when the responsibilities of parenting are carried by an organisation, not a
person – often used when the local authority has parental responsibilities. Even though this
arrangement is usually made with the best of intentions (to protect a child who has been
harmed by its own parents, for example), an organisation has salaried staff who take holidays,
change jobs and work 35-hour weeks; they have to obey written procedures and job
descriptions. When an organisation is responsible, it is very hard to provide the kind of
spontaneous, loving and flexible care an individual parent supplies.
The UK government’s current proposals set out responsibilities for children in care:
‘The state has a unique responsibility for children in care. It has taken on the task of
parenting’ and ‘it must offer a nurturing home and a happy childhood, must be ambitious
for children’s future and must be demanding of schools and services to get the best for
these children.’
(DCSF/DfES, 2007)
It contains a specific proposal to improve services for people after they have left care, including
support for education, employment, personal contact and housing.
continued
135
Living independently
If you were the parent of a child leaving home, what are some of the things you would wish
for your son or daughter, and what responsibilities might be involved?
The state (in your name as a citizen) has taken parental responsibilities for children who have
been more hurt than most. In the light of your previous answer, what responsibilities do you
think might be involved after such a child has ‘left home’ at 16 or 17?
136
Living independently
here, but when she meets with others at a Japanese event, she knows there will be many who regard
Britain as simply a place to work or study for the moment – next year it could be Germany or the
USA or back to Japan. The money transfer of migrant workers has been described as the biggest force
for wealth redistribution in the world at the present time. The concept emphasises that many people
have a sense of identity and culture which is international and goes beyond where they currently
live.
Loneliness is not the same as alone-ness. A person can feel lonely in the company of many others, or
not feel lonely even though they keep their own company. Specific research findings related to this are
set out, for example by Qualter and Munn (2002). Think about the different contributions of social and
emotional loneliness in the situations discussed below. What should the social worker’s contribution
be to addressing those feelings? They should certainly be a component of the professional assessment
you make as a social worker (independently of your employment procedures), but would they show up
in the formal procedural assessment and prioritising you may be asked to complete?
Prison
Social workers often need to empathise with people for whom others feel little sympathy. There are
many aspects of life for people who have been imprisoned in which they set out on a journey alone.
This alone-ness may be from the presence of someone who is felt essential for their emotional life,
as we can see in the statistics that 66 per cent of women in prison have dependent children under 18,
and each year more than 17,700 children are separated from their mother by imprisonment. On
average women are held 58 miles from their home, and 60 per cent of women are in prisons outside
their home region. For 85 per cent of these mothers, it is the first time they have been separated
overnight from their children, and they often have no control over who looks after children in their
137
Living independently
absence (once a mother has been sentenced, only 5 per cent of children remain in their own home
afterwards). Of men in prison, 49 per cent have children, and 45 per cent of men in prison lose contact
with their families (Hansard 2007; NACRO 2000, quoted by the Prison Reform Trust, 2007). The alone-
ness is also particularly likely to be felt on release from detention.
?
Reflective thinking
Imagine you are a 21-year-old woman with a 2-year-old daughter (if you have followed the
narrative, think of Bella, the mother of Sharon). You have been sent to prison and don’t know
who will care for your 2-year-old daughter – you have never been separated from her even for
one night. You don’t really sleep for the first two nights. Write an imaginative account to
represent thoughts and feelings that go through your head, particularly those about your
daughter, as the lights are switched off on the third night.
Some refugees may have experienced torture, or have witnessed the torture of those close to them,
and all will have fled from deeply traumatising situations. The Medical Foundation for the care of
victims of torture is a British charity with four centres in major cities. It receives around 2,000 referrals
each year, the majority in recent years being refugees. It quotes a figure of 3,000 people who receive
medical help in Britain each year following torture (Mughal, 2008). These experiences are so traumatic
victims are unable to process them – in the psychoanalytic words of Truckle (2000: 174), ‘things done
to the ego which totally overwhelmed it’.
But it is not only in specific work with refugees that social workers meet people whose development
has involved either forced migration or torture. Mental health workers will be involved with the
138
Living independently
psychiatric after-effects. Childcare workers are responsible for the care of accompanied and
unaccompanied asylum-seeking children, who might have arrived alone from countries torn apart by
the savagery of civil war and life as a brutalised child soldier, and are informed that after living here
for perhaps four or five years, they will be returned to their country of origin when they reach the age
of 16. Workers with older people will encounter English survivors of Japanese prisoner-of-war camps
and also the children of parents whose lives ended in torture, such as the 10,000 children (now in their
seventies and eighties), who came to Britain in 1938 as ‘kindertransport’, most leaving parents who
were subsequently worked to death or gassed in concentration camps.
In a different context, a social worker might encounter some of the 150,000 children who were moved
from Britain, aged around 8, without their parents, to Canada, Rhodesia, New Zealand and Australia
between 1922 and 1967 (Parliament of Australia, 2001). Agencies such as the Child Migrant Trust are
now concerned with the emotional and relationship consequences of the child welfare policies of that
period, both on the children and their parents, which in the words of an Australian Official enquiry,
‘deeply scarred them and had an immeasurable impact on the rest of their lives. . . . We heard stories
of children feeling worthless, vulnerable, stigmatised, unloved and being denied opportunities, and
adult lives filled with poor personal relationships, broken marriages, suicide attempts, uncertainty and
insecurity . . . a number of witnesses to the Inquiry have described severe and prolonged trauma’
(Forde, 1999).
Refugees are frequently fleeing a situation in which emotional and physical abuse perpetrated by
gangs of powerful people has forced them to escape into an uncertain future. However, many survivors
of physical and sexual abuse (as adults or children) in this country will have been traumatised in similar
ways. For many, with the return of these experiences as memories they feel alone with impossible
problems, and surrounded by people who do not understand.
Looking closer
Work with older people who arrived in Britain as kindertransport illustrates how social workers
need to understand development across the life cycle, not just the life stage defined in terms
of their ‘client group’ (understanding the experience of childhood trauma may be relevant to
sensitive work with older people).
139
Living independently
of the social and emotional world. We have seen how the mental rhythm of mothers in particular
(whether by biological fact or social arrangement) is closely mingled with that of their baby. When a
mother sits on the sofa watching television, with her child in her arms, her brain is closely aligned with
his, as it is when his cries alert her and cause distress or frustration. For most mothers and fathers, the
well-being of their children, the tasks and responsibilities associated with them, form a major part
of their mental life. Later on, for some parents, the period after children have left home comes the
challenge of creating a satisfactory social existence. Specifically, however, the death of a child leaves
a gap in their world, a gap that may not always be noticed in the future, but one that is
always there. Bereaved parents are living without someone who seems an integral part of
their emotional survival – ‘it is why we feel so isolated from society, from family and
Chapters friends’ (White, undated). Other aspects of the death of a child are discussed in Chapters
7 and 9
7 and 9.
Homelessness
Some young people are forced to leave home. Centrepoint (1999, 2008) identified risk factors, for
young people becoming homeless, as listed in the box opposite. The main trigger for homelessness
among young people is the breakdown of relationships with parents. A significant minority have
experienced violence at home, and one in four of the young people whom Centrepoint assist are
refugees. Centrepoint stresses that few young people ‘choose’ to be homeless. They propose that the
correct strategy for preventing youth homelessness is to identify the young people in these risk
categories, and provide support to these young people and their families to tackle problems at an early
stage. The organisation surveyed 100 young people about debt and found that it was a considerable
problem for 82 per cent of their respondents, and that the average level of debt was £1,000.
Homelessness is associated with poor physical health, and can precipitate a range of drug-related and
mental health problems, as well as being caused by them. Quilgars and colleagues (2008) comment
that it is difficult to establish the rate of youth homelessness in the UK, but estimate that those in
touch with services range from five to fifteen young people per thousand in the different regions of
the UK. This may be a figure of about 75,000 young people in 2006–7. The UK government has made
a priority of tackling youth homelessness in recent years, and this appears to have resulted in many
successful initiatives. Homelessness is not the same as rough sleeping, and Quilgars and colleagues
found that the number of young people sleeping rough on any given night is low (the number over a
year is thought to be considerably higher).
140
Living independently
• Family poverty.
• Being in care (one in six (17%) of young people supported by Centrepoint have been in
local authority care).
• Not getting on at school (being bullied, excluded or playing truant).
• Physical, sexual or emotional abuse (45 per cent of young homeless people have expe-
rienced violence in the family home on more than one occasion) (Centrepoint, 1999, 2008).
Homeless adults without children are not a priority group for statutory services, and there is no reliable
source of statistics on single homeless people. The risk factors for homelessness that have been
identified by research (Fitzpatrick et al., 2000) are housing trends, labour market forces, poverty and
the break-up of family relationships. There is little longitudinal research which points reliably to
patterns and outcomes, but homelessness is associated with poor physical and mental health.
Fitzpatrick et al. quote arguments that poorer physical health should be attributed to inadequate
health provision rather than to homelessness in itself. The involvement of alcohol in a proportion of
homelessness is widely recognised. For younger people, Quilgars et al. (2008) found that homelessness
is associated with the break-up of existing social networks, but also with the formation of supportive
relationships in new settings. Fitzpatrick and colleagues highlight that there is little reliable research
about the friendship pattern of adult homeless people. Asylum seekers and people released from prison
are particularly at risk of being homeless (Shelter, 2008).
141
Living independently
Practical responses
On some occasions, a social worker may help by introducing the person to a community resource,
day centre or club which turns out to reduce the isolation, or by facilitating the provision of aid for
mobility. Implementing the person’s wish for more suitable accommodation may similarly result in
tackling a problem of social isolation. In certain fields, introducing a person to self-help or specialist
interest groups may turn out to be valuable, or, recognising a gap, the social worker may be instru-
mental in setting up a support group. There are, in short, various practical ways in which social
work actions affect a person’s life experience in this regard. Equally, it will turn out that there are many
occasions when such interventions are inappropriate, irrelevant, undesirable, or impractical.
The social worker is unlikely to be the person whose physical presence reduces isolation (that is usually
the role of companions, friends and acquaintances), but there are a number of settings in which
arranging volunteer contacts and befrienders turns out to be useful. For example, in relation to
postnatal depression (and other difficulties facing parents), Crispin and colleagues (2005), and the
Home Start website, describe the kind of support which can be offered by volunteers. Dean and
Goodlad (1998) examine the role and impact of befriending schemes in a variety of communities and
for a variety of purposes. Some are based on a specific routine while others involve varied activities.
Their study examined the ways in which these schemes enabled people to participate in community
activities. The organisers, volunteers and users of the service all emphasised the importance of the
relationship between the user of the service and the volunteer.
Relationship responses
Although there are many situations where the last thing people want a social worker to be is a ‘friend’,
there are others in which the value of the social worker’s ‘befriending’ should not be underestimated.
The young care leavers quoted by Morgan and Lindsay (2006: 20) wanted practical support in setting
up independent living, but they also wanted the social worker to ‘like, be there for us’. At some time or
other, most committed social workers have heard people who clearly appreciate the service offered say
something like, ‘I think of X more like a friend than a social worker’. There is much to understand in
this, but if the service is offered correctly, the relationship is certainly not like other social friendships.
It is not a symmetrical relationship, since the social worker has obligations which are not reciprocated
by the person they see. Social friendships have elements of mutual support which are not in the
professional relationship, and so on. Nevertheless, it is the word people choose, and it is not difficult
to understand why.
It is a basic service to offer ‘the hand of friendship’ when someone has fled from persecution to what
they hope will be a place of refuge after months of days and nights of worry, flight and fear. This is
a first psychosocial gesture which underlies others. The social worker has to understand a wide range
of possible ‘pasts’ and must do whatever will make a positive psychosocial ‘future’ more likely for the
person.
142
Living independently
When people feel alone with a problem that seems too much to cope with, some of the anxiety may
be alleviated by contact with someone who is regularly emotionally available to them. Writing about
the response to loneliness, Hobson (1974: 73) analyses what the professional offers as ‘friendship of
a curious kind’. He quotes the words of Bertrand Russell: ‘In human relations one should penetrate to
the core of loneliness in each person and speak to that’. This often rings true in social work situations.
Hobson points out that this can only be done if the response is one of ‘real true feelings’ rather than
the response of someone ‘only playing a part’.
The detail of what happens within the relationship will be as varied as people are. The response will
be conveyed by unforced body language – eye contact, a reassuring touch, the change in a tone of
voice. In social work, perhaps unlike some disciplines with more circumscribed responsibilities, the
communication may come through preparedness ‘to go the extra mile’, to do something which might
not be directly prescribed by the role. The director of Children’s Rights found in his investigation
(Morgan and Lindsay, 2006: 20) that care leavers were particularly appreciative of this. For one person,
a conversation may allow the worst terrors and fantasies of the sleepless night to be seen for what
they are – unrealistic and unlikely outcomes. For another, the contact may leave them appropriately
reassured that it is they who are ‘OK’ and it is the people who cause their problem who are unreason-
able, destructive and harmful. The right words in a single conversation can remove the sense of
isolation – a young Muslim described in a radio programme the reassuring effects of a conversation
with someone at the end of a telephone (BBC, 2007a): ‘For a while I was just very upset because
I thought, well if I am gay and gays go to hell, then . . . then I am going to go to hell. . . . I was really
worried that I thought maybe I was – umm – maybe I am one of the devil’s own disciples because I had
such a conviction that there was something wrong being gay, and I must be evil and then I am going
to go to hell.’ He describes the reassurance he gained from a helpful conversation with a professional
person: ‘And they [the person with whom he spoke] said, “God made love so how can be a sin to love
someone else?” and I thought, yes, it’s not just sex, it’s love as well, and then I went back to the Koran.’
‘I’m going to Torquay, but it doesn’t matter, because you say things in
my head, so I’m not lonely’
Truckle (2000: 178–182) describes her work with a 22-year-old man who had learning disabilities. She
reports the above, which he said to her over a holiday period. ‘Donald’ had been sexually assaulted in
a residential setting, and had subsequently severely sexually assaulted his 3-year-old niece. A further
incident of concern had involved a young woman of his own age. Here are some extracts from Truckle’s
description of her relationship with him:
Donald was a normal adolescent, full of raging hormones and sexual feelings, heavily into pop
music. Yet I discovered he did not have the most rudimentary sexual knowledge. No one had ever
used the words ‘erection’, ‘masturbation’ or ‘contraception’ with him. . . . I tried to explain to
Donald that the female student’s parents might be afraid that she would get pregnant. ‘How?’ he
143
Living independently
asked, looking bright eyed and alert. I explained to him in much the same way that I would to a
five year old, but Donald’s reaction was totally different from a child’s. It was adult. ‘Wow,’ he said,
with his eyes glistening with what I thought might be tears. ‘I could make something as perfect
as a baby.’
I was also aware that Donald might be gay, so I decided to tackle both the female and male student
in one fell swoop. I said to Donald that he had the same problem that any young man had: he
wanted to be held and touched and made love to. He nodded. I asked him if he knew the word
‘tactful’. Donald shook his head. I explained it and that he had to manage to get someone to have
sex with him in a tactful way that didn’t scare them, and allow them to say ‘no’. We had by this
time talked extensively about Henry, and how frightened and confused he must have been when
he leaped on Donald’s back and got shouted at [Henry was the boy who had three times sexually
assaulted Donald in the residential college] . . . I reminded Donald of how confused and frightened
he had been and said that this is what is meant by abuse, and if you really force yourself on
someone, that is rape . . . Donald looked at me very sadly and said ‘Like what I did with Geraldine?’
[his niece]. We could then think about the abuse he had perpetrated on Geraldine . . . and at the
same time he could identify with Geraldine in her pain, terror and horror at the way he had
betrayed her trust.
Alleviating his ‘loneliness’ involved a genuine and fully human relationship. In the chapter we have
quoted, Truckle discusses the appropriateness of expressing any feelings a worker has, but she leaves
the reader in no doubt that if a genuine relationship is offered, it may evoke strong feelings in the
worker – ‘I said goodbye to him for the last time . . . I must admit I went away and had a private cry
at the thought of him trying to reach maturity in a world that offered him so little help, recognition
or respect.’ If effective social work is offered, there will always be times when it will evoke such feelings
– sometimes of care and protectiveness, sometimes of sadness, sometimes of anger, some-
times of self-doubt, and sometimes of horror. As the final section of Chapter 10 discusses,
this is not a sign of weakness or unsuitability, and it is one reason why the worker is
Chapter 10
entitled to ongoing confidential support.
There may be times when there is no practical arrangement which can alleviate the sense of being
alone, and no emotional remedy that another person can offer for the harm or trauma that is being
addressed. Nevertheless, inadequate as you may feel it at the time, the person you are working with
may tell you that they value your relationship ‘because you understand what’s going on’. It is the quality
of understanding which has left them feeling less alone. This is not always easy to get right, which
is one reason workers should have received help for their own problems, in the process exploring
what facilitates their own development and what they value in a helping relationship (this is touched
on again at the end of Chapter 10). Paradoxically, the very sense of not understanding
may be an important communication of empathy – after all, the person you are helping
may not understand their own feelings and behaviour, let alone those of others, so why
Chapter 10
should you?
144
Living independently
However, any worker wants to feel competent, and appear helpful; it may take a lot of confidence to
provide authority, security and empathy at the same time as admitting that you don’t understand.
Patrick Casement (originally a social worker but now a widely respected authority in psychoanalysis)
goes further; as he explores with great sensitivity in his book, On Learning From Our Mistakes (2008),
it is by making mistakes with those we are helping that we develop greater empathy.
The biggest challenge is presented by a sense of aloneness in the wake of torture or abuse that cannot
be alleviated at all. Hollander (2000) points out that when pain is inflicted from sadistic motives, its
purpose is to dehumanise, and in many cases it succeeds. The victim is unable to share the experience
and is cut off from subsequent human comfort. Social workers encounter this in relation to some forms
of child abuse and domestic violence, or when working with refugees or former Japanese prisoners of
war. Writers such as Blackwell (1997) and Truckle (2000) write persuasively that in this case, there is
an ethical aspect to simply ‘bearing witness’ to the social atrocity that has been committed. This is one
element of the social work with child migrants referred to earlier.
There are many different kinds of social processes – the interaction between a boy and his sister is one
example, but at the other extreme, so is civil war and political violence. The word ‘ecology’ describes
how living things fit in with their environment, and Uri Bronfenbrenner presented an ‘ecological’ model
of human development. This model provides a scheme for understanding how different social
processes influence development. Its main outlines are easy to understand, and fit quite naturally with
the way social workers view their responsibilities.
A straightforward summary of the ecological model is provided as ‘Essential background’,
section 3, and you may find it helpful to read this before the essay which concludes this
chapter. The activity ‘About yourself’ also illustrates the use of Bronfenbrenner’s model.
Chapter The essay sets the earlier part of the chapter (very much about individual experience) into
EB3
a social context by discussing the use of Bronfenbrenner’s model in social work.
145
Living independently
About yourself
Applying the ecological model
The ecological model shows the various levels of the social environment, each of which affects our
development. In written form, the model can seem intimidating to students, not least because of the
unfamiliar technical terms from systems theory. In practice it is easy to understand and presents an
approach very compatible with social work. The following activity should make its application clear.
1 Think of a period in your life (in view of the theme of this chapter, you may possibly choose a period
when you set out to live independently, or when you were isolated or lonely). Draw some of the
relationships which turned out to be influential on your development, or whose disruption had caused
the loneliness. For example:
Jane
Work Family
Ben
Figure 5.1
Microsystems
2 Next, consider how the relationships between the microsystems had an effect on you – in the exam-
ple, perhaps it was a positive factor that the ‘family’ microsystem had knowledge of what was going
on in the microsystem with Ben, or perhaps it was difficult to maintain the link with Jane at the same
time as the link with Ben. The relationship between the microsystems is called the mesosystem.
3 Draw a surrounding circle, and in it name some of the systems of interaction which have an effect
on your development but which do not directly involve you. For example, for a child, a financial crisis
in the father’s workplace may have an effect. These are described as exosystems. Finally, the large-
scale systems which affect a person – perhaps attitudes towards marriage, expectations of women,
availability of jobs, are described as the macrosystem. They influence what happens in all the other
systems:
146
Living independently
Mesosystems
Jane
Work Family
Large-scale systems of
society as a whole Macrosystem
Figure 5.2
Bronfenbrenner’s nested ecological model of development
The model is competed by taking account of the chronosystem (from the Greek chronos for ‘time’), the
way in which the systems change over time – perhaps in the ‘mesosystem’, the relationship with Jane
became disrupted because of the link with Ben.
147
Living independently
TAKING IT FURTHER
Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model
Ecological systems
A bee obtains its nectar for nutrition from flowers, and as it does so, it pollinates the flower,
enabling it to reproduce. Bee and flower form a system, in which each component influences
the other. The bee’s individual behaviour with the flower is dependent on the organised
behaviour of the swarm, so if this is disrupted, the flower is affected. The flower is also depen-
dent on water, and this arrives through a system external to both – it falls as rain, is transported
though a river system which irrigates the land as it passes through, flows into the sea where
the sun’s heat evaporates it into clouds, and atmospheric currents move the clouds over land
where they start the cycle again. If this system gets disrupted, deserts form – the bee–flower
system is affected by the external water system.
Uri Bronfenbrenner sought to explain clearly how human development was not driven just by
internal characteristics of the individual but by a whole range of interacting systems. The bee and
flower form an ecological system, and Bronfenbrenner used many ideas from ecology. For example,
ecological theory holds that the behaviour of a whole system can never be understood just by listing
the properties of each element in the system (in the way that the social behaviour of a person at
work could not be predicted just by understanding the biology of each cell in his body); each
element in a system is affected by how the others behave. Changes in the system behaviour
resulting from the changing behaviour of one element may then go on to change the behaviour of
other systems which are interlinked with the first. The model explains how the development of the
individual cannot be predicted just from personal characteristics – it is shaped by wideranging
forces (‘systems’) in which the individual is not directly involved, as well as patterns of interactions
with people in the immediate environment.
Bronfenbrenner’s exposition of his model (1979/2006) clearly sets out to be a major new com-
prehensive model of development. In the course of the work, he defines a set of specially devised
technical terms, and identifies fifty hypotheses using these terms. Subtitling this work ‘experiments
by nature and design’, he examines numerous research reports to support or refine his propositions.
This essay, however, is not so concerned with the use of his ideas as a detailed positivist science
that will predict development outcomes, and produce rigorous statements about relationships
between variables. Rather, it uses his research as a framework for analysing the situations encoun-
tered by social workers. He himself saw his work as a challenge to existing theories, including
attachment theory. Many researchers, however, were already sympathetic to the importance of
social influences, and felt him to have set out a framework which suited their model. Understood
in this way, it sheds light on the experience and problems faced by the people who use social work
services.
The purpose of this essay is to illustrate how Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model of development
can be applied to social work situations. An outline is given of the model’s application to a relatively
straightforward life transition (a young adult leaving home) as a baseline for examining the more
conflicted situations of service users who find themselves in a changed environment.
148
Living independently
Mesosystems
There may equally, of course, be tensions, arguments and disagreements which these young people
must now manage much more on their own. A young person at home may not have wished parents
to know about, let alone get involved in, any problems they have with their friends. But when living
at home, family relationships provide constant alternative microsystems. When they live on their
own, these family microsystems may be a long way away, so the friendship microsystem may be
the main social context. Disintegration or destructive interaction in that system may leave the
young adult isolated in a way that was not the case previously.
This highlights changes in what Bronfenbrenner terms the mesosystem. This is defined as the
?
system of relationships between microsystems. In the earlier ‘About yourself’ section, a fictional
suggestion was made that ‘perhaps it was difficult to maintain the link with Jane at the same time
as the link with Ben’. This is an illustration of the way that the interaction between microsystems
has an effect on the individual. For a child, the way the school microsystem relates to the family
microsystem (harmoniously, with shared values, or the opposite – with hostility and suspicion)
affects individual development – not the home in itself, not the school in itself, but the
relation between the two. Making links with the earlier work of Piaget (1950/1997; and
see Chapter 3 of this volume) and Lewin (1935), Bronfenbrenner emphasises that the
Chapter 3
‘microsystem’ is not an absolute ‘objective’ reality, it is what is perceived or constructed
as the immediate environment (Bronfenbrenner, 1979/2006: 9). In the case of a young
adult from a supportive family making a relatively smooth transition to independent living, the
newly developing (and, we may hope, often exciting and challenging) microsystems are supported
by the stability of older microsystems which remain in the background. These hold varying
relationships to experience in the new settings. Being valued, loved, held in good regard and
high esteem in the established microsystem is a protective factor for coping with challenges and
setbacks in the new one. Security provided in the established systems can make threats to new
systems less anxiety-provoking and disturbing. In a similar way, a supportive married relationship
can provide the resource for an individual to deal with difficulties and challenges at work.
Bronfenbrenner (1979/2006: 25, 209–237) identifies the forms taken by interconnections between
149
Living independently
microsystems: some of the same people may be active in both; there may be knowledge or ignorance
existing in one setting about the other and there may be positive or hostile attitudes from one about
the other.
Exosystems
Bronfenbrenner’s exosystem refers to systems of which the individual is not a part, but in which
changes directly affect the person. In the example on page 132, the parental diad is an exosystem
for the individual concerned (a university student) but changes in that exosystem (the parental
relationship) unsettled the student to the extent that they abandoned their university course.
Other relevant events in exosystems could be a financial crisis for their new landlord – he may stop
properly maintaining their property; or processes in the functioning of the university or business
employer (these shape the nature of their studies or employment, or affect their potential for
advancement). It may well be the characteristics of ‘exosystems’ such as neighbourhood gangs or
policing policy which determine the physical safety of a young woman or man living away from
home.
Macrosystems
The macrosystem is the term used by Bronfenbrenner to refer to national and cultural systems
– attitudes, social policy and economic climate, for example. Many features of the macrosystem
affect the developing experience of the young adult leaving home. Social security policy may treat
the individual as more or less an independent adult. Bynner (2002), for example, concludes that
over the last four decades, young adults are better off in absolute terms but less well off relatively
compared with older adults; they remain in education longer than previously, but people from
poorer families suffer a ‘poverty penalty’ related to educational achievement which has got worse
over time. The changing economic situation will affect their earning capacity and their ability to
own their own home.
Applying Bronfenbrenner’s analysis: young people leaving care, and asylum seekers
Bronfenbrenner’s model of the different systems which affect development can be applied to care-
leavers and to asylum seekers.
Like any young adults, care leavers value the greater freedom they have to manage their own
‘microsystems’; they enjoy the greater space and privacy they find in their own accommodation,
and greater independence (Morgan and Lindsay, 2006: 5). ‘You don’t have people interfering
[social workers]’, and one of the ten best things about leaving care was ‘not having to ask
permission to go places’.
But there are also particular challenges. ‘One in three care leavers did not feel safe where they were
living’ (A National Voice, 2009 – the organisation run by and for young people who have been in
care). In the twelve months of Wade and Dixon’s sample (2006), a third experienced homelessness.
Rainer (2007), an organisation whose work includes supporting care leavers, quotes research that
found a third of rough sleepers have spent some time in care as a child. In their own research with
1,244 care leavers, they report that ‘in the worst cases, vulnerable young people were placed in
housing that was physically unsecured and where they were subjected to harassment and discrim-
ination by other tenants and staff. They could find themselves miles away from work or training
150
Living independently
and effectively cut off from friends and other support.’ The fear of loneliness has already been
mentioned, but also in the top ten fears of care leavers is that of being homeless, having nowhere
to live. Of the young adult population in prison under 21, 40 percent has been in care. The
microsystem, as Bronfenbrenner defines it (1979/2006: 27), includes the ‘physical and material
characteristics’ of the immediate environment as well as the ‘pattern of activities, roles and
interpersonal relations’ experienced in that setting. Compared with the general population there
is clearly a greatly increased risk for young adults who have been in care that the ‘microsystem’ is
not positive.
Bronfenbrenner uses what he himself describes as an ‘unorthodox’ definition of development.
He defines it as the person’s growing capacity to ‘discover, sustain or alter’ the properties of the
environment, in the light of their ‘evolving conception of it and their relation to it’ (1979/2006:
9). Like many others features of his model, this seems in general to make more sense when applied
to child development, but in relation to asylum seekers or to people in prison, it underlines that
there may be barriers or limitations to personal development precisely because central features of
their environment are determined for them by other people. Until their refugee status is confirmed,
an asylum seeker who has been subject to torture in their own country may have little control over
where they live and how they support themselves in their new country, and the decision about
whether or not to return to the place of persecution is outside their control.
At the current time, many asylum seekers live with untruths in the relationships within their
microsystem. To evade asylum regulations, protect family from vengeance in their country of
origin, or hide from pursuers, they keep truths about their past lives hidden, particularly from
officials. In his study of social work with thirty-four asylum-seeking children, Kohli (2007: 102)
found that their stories reflected those found by Ayotte (2000): about two-thirds, the largest
subgroup, were those who had clearly fled from persecution. The next largest groups were those
whose stories were similar, but were not believed by investigators. The third group were those
thought to be trafficked. Social workers, however, felt that none talked openly about their past
lives. Some had been threatened to keep quiet by people in their country of origin; some were too
shocked by their experiences to talk; some lied to evade asylum regulations; some wanted to forget
their previous lives and put it all behind them. In general, as summarised by a social worker, their
concern is with ‘the present first, the future next, the past last’. In terms of the microsystem, well-
intentioned people intent on their welfare had to relate to them knowing that they were not being
told the truth. Kohli points out that there is no clear distinction between the different categori-
sations – for example, all in a sense were economic migrants because they had come to the UK in
order to seek a better life. The Independent Enquiry into Asylum commented that they found the
asylum seekers in their study (whether or not their asylum application was accepted) ‘were not
scroungers and ne’er-do wells, but decent people trying to maintain their dignity in difficult
circumstances’ (Hobson et al., 2008).
Bronfenbrenner analyses the nature of the links between microsystems. Describing the structure
of these links – the mesosystem – again highlights the vulnerability of many care leavers compared
with other young adults. ‘Shared knowledge between settings’ is highlighted by Bronfenbrener
(1979/2006: 210) as one positive linkage – but although a care leaver may share some of their
recent life experience with other rough sleepers, they may feel that in a conventional work setting,
or in other friendship circles, it is a private matter they are reluctant to share. For the general
population, new friends may have a degree of interest in each other’s family background. Care
leavers find the situation easier than when they lived in a children’s home, but they may feel that,
151
Living independently
even if they did reveal it, most people would have little comprehension of the nature of their family
life and their subsequent life in care. They face a much more loaded choice than many other young
adults in choosing what to keep private and what to share. As already remarked, government
initiatives in the UK now require much more involvement from the authorities which had
previously been responsible for care, and it quotes the apparently promising statistic that in 2007
the number of 19-year-old care leavers who were not in contact with their previous care authority
had reduced from 20 per cent to 8 per cent over the previous five years.
Similar discontinuities between different microsystems may exist for many asylum seekers. At the
time of writing, the term ‘asylum seeker’ has negative connotations for many of the general public,
being closely linked with ‘illegal immigrant’ or ‘street beggar’ in widespread public discourse. This
is just one aspect to the ignorance they encounter. Most of the people they meet will have little
idea of life in their country of origin. A doctor or company director seeking asylum may find
themselves treated as ill-educated and undeserving of respect.
Bronfenbrenner considers that, in general, growth is enhanced when there are intermediate struc-
tures and people who operate in more than one microsystem of an individual’s experience, with a
degree of shared knowledge (he acknowledges the value of diverse and cross-cultural experience,
but sets this value in the context of well-managed change). He identifies that mesosystems are
positive when there is smooth transition between microsystems – positive reasons for the move,
careful management, and multiple links between the systems. In these terms, young adults who
have left care and refugees and asylum seekers are likely to have experienced poor ‘setting
transitions’ (Bronfenbrenner, 1979/2006: 25, 210).
Bronfenbrenner uses a rarefied, abstract term, but it can accurately be said that in many situations
in which social workers intervene, many developmental challenges reside in the ‘mesosystem’.
There is often discontinuity, conflict, unhelpfulness or misunderstanding between different
microsystems. Social workers should be aware of these as part of the problem, their assessments
should identify them, and since they are social workers – with a routine professional responsibility
to get to the bottom of problems and not just apply a palliative sticking-plaster – they have a
responsibility to tackle these problems as well as the immediate, individual issues of housing,
health or child welfare which may be the tasks identified by their statutory employment. Loneliness
does not only arise because of features of the microsystem – it can be generated by the inadequacy
of the mesosystem.
It is Bronfenbrenner’s thesis that the behaviour of micro- and mesosystems is always affected by
exosystems in which the individual plays no direct role. Many such systems are relevant to the
development of people whom social workers should be assisting. For example, statistics quoted
above indicate how a change in local authority policy caused a fourfold reduction in the number
of care leavers who were left without contact with their previous carers. The activity of voluntary
organisations directly impact on the experience of those who are sleeping rough. Asylum seekers
are directly affected by policies about their dispersal around the country and separation from other
people from their region or culture.
152
Living independently
terms – ‘British’, ‘Somali’, ‘over 85’, ‘Muslim’, ‘black British’, ‘urban’ – for example, does not convey
the characteristics of the macrosystem. He insists (1979/2006: 260) that research attempting to
understand developmental influences, processes and outcomes need to be based on interview, con-
versation and observation. The same is true of the social work efforts to understand the influence
of macrosystems on any individual. Attitudes to rough sleepers, to children in care, to asylum
seekers, to Muslims, to gay and lesbian relationships, to African people, to men and women, are
important factors in shaping the development of adults or children. It is important that social
workers do not ignore the influence of these ‘macro-’ (cultural) factors. But neither can they think
they understand these factors by generalising – they must listen and observe. To simplify is to
misunderstand and to stereotype. Within Bronfenbrenner’s ‘macrosystem’ we must now add, to a
degree that he perhaps did not, the influence of global structures and cosmopolitan identities. The
lives of many people whom social workers assist are caught up in politics and economics that are
international in scope; and their identities, sense of belonging or sense of isolation may be linked
with cultures, identities and family events thousands of miles from where the social worker
meets them.
Finally, in a later addition to his initial presentation, Bronfenbrenner identifies the ‘chronosys-
tem’ as the system of elements that involve time – external events such as the political changes
which result in a person’s exile from their own country, or internal events such as maturational
processes affecting their ability to understand and cope with stressors. Clearly, the social worker
cannot understand the social problems confronting people in the examples we have cited without
taking this time dimension into account. In the case of people with traumatised pasts, the self-
narrative which is the ‘internalised’ chronosystem may be fragmented and contradictory.
?
tasks with the individual, that they may be mis-reading the situation if they do not also take a
broader view. Systems theory not only locates the individual at the centre of ever-widening spheres
of influence (which is self-evidently accurate), but also draws attention to the importance of links
between elements in the wider systems. In acknowledging the mutability of human characteristics
in different developmental domains and in different environments and cultures, it offers a more
comprehensive view than undifferentiated stage theories. In childhood cognitive
development, Bronfenbrenner recognises the work of Piaget in emphasising the child’s
perception of the external world, but draws attention to his lack of reference to social
context (1979/2006: 125). He clearly values the work of Vygotsky and later
Chapter 3
psychologists (see Chapter 3 of this volume).
Bronfenbrenner’s work had practical applications in work with children (US Department of
Health and Social Services 1981, 2007) and with young people in trouble. It expresses the
passionate awareness he demonstrated in his own interventions that the environment of the child,
the community setting of the mother or childcare institutions and the political structures within
which they are living are all relevant when considering programmes to protect and improve
children’s development. Most prominently, in UK public policy, the ‘ecological’ model was
153
Living independently
described as the underpinning of the Framework for the Assessment of Children in Need (Department
of Health, 2000). Bronfenbrenner has been a prominent figure who has used his influence to
argue that while society has accepted the change to post-industrial conditions in work and
finance, it has failed to safeguard the social environment, producing hazardous conditions for the
rearing of children, and expecting that women should enter the paid workforce without proper
respect for the resulting ‘social ecology’ of their lives and those of their children (Bronfenbrenner,
1990).
However, Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model was set out as a specific theoretical research
programme. This was intended to determine how different mesosystems produce different
microsystems, how the features of macrosystems have a consistent relationship with the
mesosystems and the microsystems they then produce, and so on. Although his work has been a
vigorous prompt to developmental researchers to take the various systems into account, it is hard
to see that there is a body of research creating his ‘grand unifying theory’. For the last forty years
the UK (as well as the United States and other countries) has been running large-scale cohort
studies which attempt to map the development through life of people born on a particular day, and
these studies are particularly interested in the effects of various environmental variables. But they
have not arisen because of his work, and have not been used been used to validate or refine the
fifty hypotheses he sets out in The Ecology of Human Development (1979/2006). His perspective is
broad, flexible and comprehensive, but as a modernist theory setting out relations between
variables (for example, ‘Development is enhanced as a direct function of the number of structurally
different settings in which the developing person participates in a variety of primary activities and
with others, particularly when those others are more mature or experienced’), it has not been
especially important. Many of the examples in his work concern children or young people and their
families, and are most plausible in relation to them. Although he makes some reference to adult
development, the simple relationships proposed in his hypotheses can become impossibly complex
(or full of exceptions and counter-qualifications) if applied to the social reality of adult lives
such as that of asylum seekers. Perhaps by analogy with the improved mathematical analysis of
the chaos and complexity (Elliott and Kiel, 2001) in everyday physical systems, we are now less
likely to look for the simple relationship between variables he suggests in human interaction and
development.
Summary
This chapter began with information about young people leaving home. It summarised research
about gender differences, and illustrated how patterns of ‘leaving home’ change in different times
and cultures by commenting on changes with in the UK in the last forty years.
Young people leaving care are particularly vulnerable to bad experiences as they begin to live inde-
pendently. The chapter looked at statistics, differences in outcomes for young people from different
ethnic groups, and drew attention to current policy initiatives to improve the situation.
The chapter then moved on to consider some other life events where social workers are responsible
for people who will feel alone with problems. It emphasised that in all fields of work, social workers
154
Living independently
are likely to deal with people who are or have been refugees and torture victims. It referred to the
experiences of parents whose child has died and people who are homeless, and described the expe-
riences of a young man with learning difficulties. For social workers, it will sometimes be possible
to tackle the problem of isolation or ‘aloneness’ by opening up opportunities for social contact, but
at other times, the response depends on their own emotional availability and consistency. This is
particularly important where they have a statutory duty of care.
Bronfenbrenner’s model attempts to classify the various influences on development. After an activity
to help understand the application of his ‘nested ecological model’, the chapter concluded with an
essay illustrating its use.
Note
1 All the people born in a single week in 1958 have been followed up in the course of their lives by The National
Child Development Study; a second group, The British Cohort Study, follows all the people born in a particular
week in 1970 – these are extremely large studies. The cohorts were followed up during their childhood and
then at approximately ten-year intervals. The Birth Cohort Study was augmented by including immigrants
born in the relevant week for the first three follow-ups samples. The website of the Centre for Longitudinal
Studies (CLS, 2008) is an invaluable resource about life, development and change. For examples of findings
about the comparative development of three generations of people in Britain, see Ferri et al., 2003.
Further reading
About physical, cognitive and social development in early adulthood:
Boyd, D. R. and Bee, H. L. (2006) Lifespan Development. Boston: Pearson/Allyn and Bacon, pp. 348–403.
About care-leavers:
DCSF/DfES (2007) Care Matters: Time for Change. Norwich: TSO, p. 11 (para 13) and Chapter 6, pp. 107–123 (also
available online at www.dcsf.gov.uk/publications/timeforchange/docs/timeforchange.pdf).
There is also a version of this document prepared as a pamphlet for young people and a twenty-page summary
version, both available online.
About Homelessness:
www.homelesspages.org.uk/ – includes a specific section abut care-leavers.
155
Living independently
?
Questions
Based on the text
1 Is ‘leaving home’ a characteristic life phase associated with young adulthood in all cultures? What
characteristic differences between males and females leaving home did Sneed and colleagues
(including Gilligan) refer to?
2 Using an example from social work or from fiction (a novel, film or soap opera), use Bronfenbrenner’s
ecological model to describe relevant features which have influenced someone’s development.
?
156
In this chapter you will find:
• Gender perspectives
• Being a parent
159
Sex, love, work and children
For those who are compatible and relaxed, it may seem like the most natural and unforced activity, but
sexual confidence is readily disturbed by early negative experiences, by anxiety, by cultural and religious
attitudes – by almost any perturbation in psychological well-being.
One stereotype is of adolescents awakened to the power of sex. For many teenage girls, the subject
may be linked with romantic visions, fully persuasive and yet only half-believed, of finding a perfect
soulmate; a person who will be reliable and protective in times of stress, but leave them their
independence and individuality - 7 million Mills and Boons romantic novels for women are sold each
year in the UK (Michaels, 2008). For a girl deprived of sufficient caring in her childhood, sexual attention
may appear to be a welcome offer of valuing and affection, but may turn out to be exploitation by a
predatory male. Some (perhaps many) will consciously focus on being sexy to please boys rather than
being sexual for themselves; further exploration, experience and confidence will be needed before they
understand their own sexual potential.
A young man may have wishes, desires and hunger. He too will have needs for adult comforting, care,
reassurance, and affection, but some young men possess as yet only the beginnings of the social skills
or experience to create in reality the relationships in which the needs may be met. Some will continue
to focus on gratification, and may remain deprived of the satisfactions and pleasures of giving care.
Unfortunately, the absence of this satisfaction is likely in turn to be part of a negative cycle – for in
men, the experience of giving sensuous care is integral to the overall power of sexual activity to
modulate troubled feelings, provide confidence and alleviate distress.
What do people desire in and through sex? There are many aspects, and desire takes many forms. Some
?
are associated narrowly or in a straightforward way with sexual arousal and subsequent satisfaction,
and some are not. For most people, over the decades of adulthood there is likely to be a journey of
discovery they could not have anticipated.
Each generation, of course, discovers good sex for the first time in human history, and even students
of human development may need to be reminded to be realistic about the lives of their elders. Some
autobiographical notes by the author and playwright Alan Bennett tactfully bring this point home. He
recounts a conversation with his mother as she recovered from an illness:
Dazed by her own illness, and stunned by his, she lay in bed talking about Dad. . . . Out of the blue
she suddenly said ‘He does very well, you know, your Dad.’
160
Sex, love, work and children
Intimacy – specifically romantic intimacy – has many dimensions. For one couple, physical comfort,
sensuous attention, the gift of giving physically may be at the core of their relationship, whilst for
another, erotic engagement may be absent. Affection, attachment, loyalty and commitment can be
expressed with varying degrees of physicality.
‘When you fall in love, it is a temporary madness,’ a father tells his daughter in the novel Captain
Corelli’s Mandolin by Louis de Bernières (1994). ‘Love is not lying awake at night imagining that he is
kissing every part of your body. No, don’t blush . . . that is just being in love. Love itself is what is left
over, when being in love has burned away.’ Some couples will remain deeply in love over the course of
their relationship for many years. Others will not. For most, the relationship will contain many elements
– of children, finance, surmounting difficulties more or less together, responsibilities for their own
parents, enjoyments, pains, disagreements and more or less shared activities and enthusiasms. Sexual
relations are dealt with at some length in this section of the chapter, not because they are necessarily
experienced as the most important aspect of an intimate relationship, but because they may be one of
the least well understood.
Intimate adult relationships (and their absence) are a core aspect of social experience and social
distress. Having responsibility for social well-being, competent social workers should be as skilled in
supporting people through difficulties in relationships as they are in childcare, health, finance,
interpersonal violence or housing. If you glance again at this list of ‘practical’ difficulties, you will see
that they are often closely bound up with the quality of adult relationships.
Social workers are human, and they are no more immune than anyone else from difficulties in relation-
ships – rejection, violence, betrayal, uncertainty, problems of mental health or drug use. For themselves,
?
they may wonder if satisfactory intimate relationships are ever really possible, or may have made a
decision not to share their lives. This is part of what they bring to their work, and it does not in itself
impede their ability to help others with relationships. They, too, are entitled to support in their diffi-
culties (and it is probably true that this is the surest way of knowing that there is nothing
demeaning in receiving help for personal matters), but they are not diminished by their
difficulties. They have to ‘hold’ their personal experience as a valuable resource, but there
are times when they may need to work hard to ensure it is not a filter which distorts their
Chapter 10
understanding of the experience of others.
161
Sex, love, work and children
162
Sex, love, work and children
attachment in both childhood and adulthood, and disruption of the attachment through separation and
loss cause similar patterns of behavioural/emotional consequences. The most obvious difference in the
functioning of the attachment system in adulthood compared with childhood is that it acts through a
mutual, not asymmetrical, relationship – in adulthood, each party provides for the attachment needs of
the other. Hazan and Zeifman regard the ‘pair bond’ as the integration of the sexual mating system, the
caregiving (parenting) system and the attachment system. Sex, they assert (1999: 340) plays a key role
in the transfer of the attachment system from its childhood targets to its adult form.
You may feel that this account says little about the development of care, affection and intimacy in
bonds not mediated by sex – such as gay and straight partnerships that are not physically sexual.
Furthermore, to propose this (comparatively) simple model of the function of sex in development is a
different matter from understanding the dynamics of sexual experience and relationships.
163
Sex, love, work and children
young man without the support for the feelings of rejection that he experiences in the search for
intimacy.
In the early stages of your career, there will be occasions when you should ideally be asked to
understand this aspect to development for one person at a time, with others (other workers, a
supervisor, various planning structures) taking responsibility for the other people in the situation. There
will be a number of occasions when this is not appropriate, and, unfortunately, agency practice may
militate against it even when it is desirable. But ultimately, as a social worker you often need to
understand simultaneously the developmental experience of several people in a situation – for
example, the nature and effects of sexual abuse and the sexual motivation and needs of the abuser, or
the person in an intimate relationship who feels sexually deprived and their partner who feels put
under sexual pressure.
There are specific areas of sexual life in which social workers may have responsibility. These require
them to build on a more general understanding, and include:
• promoting sexual health;
• ?
imparting sexual knowledge;
• sexual satisfaction and enjoyment in relationships – for example, in relation to learning or physical
disabilities, or the effects of childbirth, illness or surgery on sexuality.
These areas, of course, overlap. For example, Kristensen Whitaker et al. (2006) found that
boys who were sexually violent or abusive had lower levels of sexual knowledge. Like David
Thompson’s emphasis on ‘erotic education’ for men and women with learning difficulties
Chapter 4
?
(Chapter 4), Jenny Higgins and Jennifer Hirsch (2007) emphasise the need in all these sub-
jects, especially ‘sex education’, for constant attention to the specifics and variety of sexual pleasures
(and desires) of all the individuals concerned. The aim of sexual support and education is, as these
authors put it, ‘to maximise sexual enjoyment and minimise sexual harm’.
A social worker will sometimes be unsure about whether a client conversation should have
been more explicit about sex or less intrusive. Sooner or later, if this subject is raised, the
social worker is likely to experience either sexual attraction and fantasy, or alternatively
Chapters
2, 10, EB4 distaste, in relation to their clients (in psychoanalytic terms – Chapter 2 and ‘Essential
background’, section 4 – these are aspects of counter transference). The hazards are to
deny these feelings on the one hand, or on the other to behave unprofessionally. One measure of social
worker’s competence is their own self-awareness about sexuality – and social workers are as likely as
anyone else to have had difficult sexual experiences and relationships and will have to face questions
164
Sex, love, work and children
which they find difficult to answer. Adequate supervision and support should allow these matters to
be discussed.
Social workers must have an educated awareness of the basic processes in sexual relations and in
reproduction. But they also need to be comfortable (if sometimes curious) about the fact that
there are many things about sex and sexuality that they, like everyone else, do not understand. If all
goes well, the social worker is likely to discover that there are people whose attitudes, knowledge and
experience can assist their understanding. There is no one, however, who ‘has all the answers’. There
are many questions about sex, about what is common and what is unusual, to which no one has the
answers.
Clearly, sex is part of a biological imperative to reproduce, but this should not be interpreted
simplistically – as in other animals, and sometimes to a greater degree, sexual activity may be
homosexual or masturbatory or occur in other contexts in which it is not directly linked to repro-
duction. And a preference for a sexualised link (for example between same sex partners) which is
not reproductive does not mean there is no drive to reproduce – a woman in a lesbian relationship
may feel the drive to have children as strongly as anyone else. This should hardly need empirical
justification, but if sought, it can be found in Morrow and Messinger (2006; see also Siegenthaler and
Bigner, 2000).
No one is born with knowledge about the biology of intercourse and reproduction. Curiosity, peer
conversations and information from books, television and the internet dispel that ignorance for many,
but social workers are particularly likely to be dealing with a proportion of the population that has
gaps in essential knowledge, either while they are young or later on. Van den Akker and her colleagues
(van den Akker et al., 1999) found in their survey of 212 UK teenagers that boys were less informed
than girls. Beyond the most unsophisticated awareness of their own physical responses, males in
165
Sex, love, work and children
general are unlikely to understand the physiology of their arousal and reproductivity until there is a
physical problem (such as erectile dysfunction, infertility or prostate trouble) which requires them to
do so. One of the more obvious differences between heterosexuality and homosexuality is that opposite
sex partners are likely to interact with less realistic fantasies about each other’s biological nature and
experience.
The psychological experience of desire and attachment takes us into a very different domain from the
biological. Lust, disgust, love and hatred are familiar to everyone. They may drive the most satisfying
?
and important relationships in life, or result in the most destructive of behaviour. Classical Freudian
theory regards the unfolding of psychological life as essentially the history of the individual’s struggle
with an underlying drive (life force) related to all these emotions. According to this view, the fantasies
and behaviours associated with adult sexuality – of union, of possession and devouring, of pain as well
as pleasure, of giving and receiving, of ‘wickedness’, transgression and punishment, of
feeding and contentment – are linked with fantasies of the body which predate adult
knowledge by many years. To use the terms introduced in Chapter 2, the relevant ‘states
Chapter 2
of mind’ in adult sexual encounters have evolved out of these earlier states of mind.
166
Sex, love, work and children
practical concerns. Women are likely to be interested in and concerned about their sexual response and
that of their male or female partner, but do not necessarily feel distress when they lose interest in
sex. This creates ambiguity because for some women the loss of sexual desire may be wrongly
understood to be a sign of ‘sexual dysfunction’, while for others, lessened sexual appetite may be
incorrectly accepted as unproblematic.
Lisa Diamond (2008) interviewed 100 women over a period of ten years, talking regularly with each of
them over this period about their sexual desires and sexual relationships. She describes about three-
quarters as ‘sexual minority’ women, but her findings and their relation to existing literature lead her
to the view that her constructs are likely to be relevant to women in general. She concludes that it is
important to distinguish sexual orientation (which, although open to different definitions, she found
to be relatively stable and located on a scale of entirely lesbian to entirely heterosexual) from three
other factors – sexual behaviour, sexual desire and loving/romantic attachment. Each can vary
independently of the others. The last she found to be aligned with personal, relationship and situational
factors rather than gender. She describes this as sexual fluidity, and concludes it is characteristic of
women in a way that it is not of men.
Like Basson, she quotes studies showing that whereas men’s bodies respond physiologically according
to their orientation to the sexual attractiveness of males or females, ‘women do not appear to be
sensitive to these categories’. Although they may express same- or opposite-sex attraction, and ‘may
subjectively prefer one sex over the other, their bodies respond to both’. (Chivers et al. (2007), whilst
confirming this general statement, also report differences between lesbians and other women.) Men’s
physiological responses, sexual behaviours and emotional attachments tend to follow their sexual
orientation far more predictably. For example, 30 per cent of lesbian women had full-blown romantic
relationships with men in the course of Diamond’s study, but did not necessarily feel that this had
changed their sexual orientation, although it caused some ideological friction with friends. Two-thirds
of her sample of sexual minority women changed their ‘identification’ over the period – not, she
believes, because their sexuality was changed or misjudged, but because the categorisations did not
fit the nature of female sexual fluidity. To summarise a complex work, she sees women’s romantic
behaviour as highly specific to the partner (independent of gender and sexual orientation), and located
in the situation and social influences in a way that men’s is not, their physical sexual behaviour being
much more tied to their sexual orientation.
About 10 per cent of men describe some level of same-sex attraction. Estimates for women are varied,
and depend significantly on the criteria used – for example, whether the attraction was an isolated
incident; the frequency or intensity of attraction; or whether the woman’s behaviour was affected
by it.
Sexual experience within enduring relationships is likely to change over time. Laumann (Diamond,
2008: 140) found in a large-scale study of American women that 30 per cent reported low or non-
existent sexual desires, and Diamond believes this too should be linked with the fluidity of female
sexuality to biology, environment, situation and relationship, quoting some clinicians’ suggestion that
167
Sex, love, work and children
‘low sexual desire’ should be reframed as ‘desire discrepancy’ between partners. Klusmann, studying
2000 people in Germany, is one of a number of researchers who found that when settled in a stable
relationship, a much greater proportion of women than men no longer expressed the sexual hunger
they expressed when seeking a partner or when their current partner was absent from home
(Klusmann, 2002, 2006; Wellings et al., 1994). This is in keeping with the model of Basson and the
ARH (2005) mentioned above that, particularly when in a relationship, women’s erogenous sexuality
tends to contrast with men’s in having a responsive motivation. This did not mean that sexual contact
?
necessarily discontinued. But women described their motivation as different – it might be out of
affection for their partner, out of the pleasure that arose, out of a responsiveness to sexual interest
shown by their partner or out of negative motivations like fear, threats, or money. This should not be
described as a ‘lessening’ of sexuality, although it would appear to indicate that its expression has
changed. Nor does it mean that some men (the numbers are unclear) do not lose sexual
drive, sometimes to the distress of their female partner. As discussed in Chapter 8, a
significant proportion of married couples report that sex continues in to their seventies,
Chapter 8 discontinuing when interrupted by events such as the hospitalisation of one partner.
Bengtson (1996) describes marriages in later life as being less based on romance and more on loyalty,
cohesion and solidarity with the wider family. His analysis of bonds within the family draws
on research which shows older partners experiencing less friction and resolving conflicts with less
negativity.
?
Reflective thinking
• What do you understand by ‘sexuality’? (It is unlikely that you will find a simple answer!)
The expression of sexuality is very diverse. Researchers differ in the degree to which they
regard human sexuality as socially constructed, and where they place the limits of variability.
Having said that:
• What aspects of their own sexuality might people find troublesome? . . . How does this
apply to men? . . . And to women?
• What aspects of male sexuality may women find troublesome?
• What aspects of female sexuality may men find troublesome?
168
Sex, love, work and children
?
6 per cent who live in stepfamilies), 18 per cent live with lone mothers and 2 per cent with lone fathers
(Smallwood and Wilson, 2007). In a classic work, Holmes and Rahe (1967) asked participants to rate
the stressfulness of various life events. Top of their scale came death of a spouse, followed by divorce
and then marital separation (fourth was being sent to jail). In this textbook, I have referred to various
models that chart responses to transitions – Chapter 4 introduced Hopson’s model of
transitions, and Chapter 9 will refer to models that have been put forward in relation
to dying and bereavement. Before either of these were formulated, the anthropologist
Chapters Bohannan (1970) analysed what he called six ‘stations’ in the route of relationship
4 and 9 separation.
Bohannan’s work referred to this specifically as divorce, but aspects of it apply equally to separation
of other intimate relationships. This separation is complex, he says, partly because the following
different aspects of separation occur simultaneously. The first phase of ‘emotional divorce’ occurs as
the couple grow apart, the relationship deteriorates and there is increasing tension. The ‘legal divorce’
requires the transformation of (only partly understood) subtle and intangible reasons into concrete
legal terms. Extensions of the ‘legal divorce’ are the third and fourth ‘stations’ – ‘economic’ and
‘coparenting’ divorce. In these, property and care of children are settled, and bad feeling in various
other aspects may contaminate attempts to resolve these issues, both at the time of the separation
and later. The ‘community’ divorce involves changes in friends and community for the two people, and
tackling the resulting loneliness. Emotional ties to the other person may persist (even in hostile
relationships) long after the practical separation, and Bohannan regards the final ‘psychic divorce’ as
the most difficult of all.
?
Hazan and Zeifman (1999: 343), like Parkes and colleagues (1991), understand the distress and other
reactions which follow the loss of a partner as expressions of separation from an attachment figure.
They note that these reactions can end either in emotional detachment from that person or in
emotional reorganisation in which the attachment figure is ‘relocated’. This is discussed again in
Chapter 9, which considers the subject of loss and bereavement. They describe the
patterns, which in grieving include anxiety, searching, and depression, as ‘the norm among
adults separated from their long-term partner’, and yet not the normal reaction to the
Chapter 9
breaking of other social ties.
169
Sex, love, work and children
? ?
Reflective thinking
Think back to Chapter 5, about living alone.
Weiss, who was quoted there, found that loneliness takes two different forms – one
associated with absence of an intimate companion, and another caused by lack of
Chapter 5
friends. Hazan and Shaver refer to this work, and point out that further research
has confirmed that the two forms of loneliness have different origins and different outcomes.
Social support in the form of friendship does not alleviate the loneliness arising from the loss of
an intimate companion. However, the renewal of relationships with parental attachment figures
was found to be helpful.
Thinking about social work situations, are there implications about the way social workers should
understand what they need to offer or arrange?
Percentage of
women who 80
experienced event
before they were 25 Marriage
60
Birth of child
40
20
Marriage breakdown
Current age
Cohabitation of women
0
25–29 30–34 35–39 40–44 45–49 50–54 55–59
Figure 6.1
Experience of family events before the age of 25: comparison of different generations
Source: ONS/Smallwood and Wilson 2007, figure 1.13)
170
Sex, love, work and children
This data from the Office for National Statistics shows, for example, that for women who are in the
25–29 age group now, 21 per cent lived with a partner (‘cohabited’) before they were 25, but of women
who are 55–59 now, only one twentieth of that figure – 1 per cent – had done so (follow the
downwards path of the dotted line marked ‘cohabitation’ in the graph). Around a quarter of women
aged 25–29 now had married before they were 25. But about three-quarters of women now aged
55–59 had married before they were 25 (follow the unbroken line marked ‘marriage’ upwards
from just above 20 to nearly 80). Similarly, less than a third of the younger women today had given
birth to a child before they were 25, compared with over a half for women who are now in their late
fifties.
In 1955, many women stayed at home until they married, at which point they set up a joint home with
their husband. Today, a much larger proportion of women live independently of their parents and
create their own home or one shared with friends before setting up a shared family household with a
partner. Similarly, in 1955, men would generally need to secure income for a whole household before
marrying, and this part of their culturally created role has been a valued if burdensome part of personal
identity for many men. Even now, some men might find the prospect of forming a partnership without
the ability to financially support a household through paid employment undesirable, as though it
implies a loss of place in the world. Changing patterns of adult intimate relationships imply emotional
and social changes in other areas – for example, fathers face an increasing risk of losing touch with
their children, with severe emotional and social consequences for both.
Until 1967 (1980 in Scotland), male homosexual activity was illegal in England, so a gay lifestyle was
inherently countercultural. Hicks (2000) analyses lesbians’ discourse about the relation between their
chosen partnerships and traditional family relationships (in the context of their applications to become
foster carers or adoptive parents). He distinguishes two contrasting ways in which the ‘lesbian family’
is presented. On the one hand, lesbians are at pains to emphasise that loving and consistent care for
children meets the essential needs of childhood as they have always been understood – the gender of
the parent is not relevant to the child’s well-being. And on the other, he analyses the way in which
they present gay and lesbian relationships as breaking new ground, challenging repressive, taken for
granted aspects of ‘traditional’ marital and family structures.
So social and cultural factors impact as strongly as biological and psychological factors on behaviour
within partnerships, and there are changes in socio-cultural influences over time. Sexual behaviour and
thoughts seem to be a specific target for those who tell others how they ought to behave – from
traditional Judaic prescriptions about when to have sex, through Christian or Islamic prohibitions
of masturbation or homosexual acts (and thoughts), through to some feminist polemicists. This
does not mean that intimate behaviour will conform to the dictats of respected figures in a culture
– the published speakers of a culture may be strident in proclaiming what is ‘approved’ precisely
because behaviour is known to be different. At the time of writing, official Catholic orthodoxy states
that contraception other than ‘natural’ methods is immoral, as are homosexual and lesbian relations.
But the majority of Catholic couples in the UK use conventional contraception, and gay and lesbian
171
Sex, love, work and children
Catholics find no contradiction between their life and their religion (Sullivan, 2002; Potts, 2006).
Homosexual acts may have been illegal until 1967 but diaries of ordinary people such as ‘Barry Charles’,
in Garfield’s (2005) collection Our Hiddden Lives, indicate how individual private lives varied from
‘official’ culture. Public attitudes made some partnerships (permanent publicly recognised households)
difficult, but enabled the development of counter cultural attitudes. Similarly, the stigma relating to
sex before marriage, or sex with multiple partners, created both external problems (prejudice and
disapproval) as well as internal pain (guilt and confusion) for some women – but this didn’t stop it
happening. Social factors influence private behaviour, but do not necessarily produce conformity.
In general, sexual desire and expression are targets for social and religious condemnation, resulting in
shame or guilt.
In this context, there are three mechanisms through which development may be negatively affected,
?
and the social worker has to adopt different strategies in relation to each. First, non-conformity may
realistically result in penalties. As in the case of a woman ostracised by her family (and facing violence)
for leaving an arranged marriage, these may be severe. Second, the social attitudes become internalized
so that external threats are no longer needed. Ino’s mother and aunties (chapter 5 above) may well have
believed that it is necessary to cut out her clitoris or she would be unable to be sexually
faithful; no one spoke to Donald (chapter 4) about erections, masturbation, and sexual
relations because they had internal barriers to talking about such things; many boys have
Chapters
4 and 5 felt guilty or ashamed about masturbation; girls feel they ought to look pretty or form a
self-valuation based on a peer-measured idea of attractiveness to boys; the young Muslim
quoted in chapter 4 knew the nature of his sexual attraction, but thought this was the work of the devil
and that he would go to hell. Rogers’ ideas of the ‘false self’ and Woodmansey’s of the ‘punitive superego’
(pages 120, 307) are attempts to describe this ‘internalisation’. And thirdly, we can only use the words of
a language to communicate, and these derive their meaning from host of related concepts and their
social connotations. So ‘promiscuous’ is typically used of females, not males, and carries a host of value
judgments with it. ‘Childless’, ‘impotent’, ‘barren’, ‘queer’, ‘wanker’ are words which each connect with
whole range of social ideas. Although the three processes – external consequences, internal guilt, and
cultural construction – require the worker to be alert to different remedies required, they are often
interlinked in any given problem.
In relation to cultures other than your own, it is important to have two complementary views. On the
one hand, to recognise that another culture is more or less strange, remote and different, and that
ideas (such as child abuse, romantic love, marriage) may have genuinely different meanings which are
embedded in a whole network of other experiences you will not understand. But also to recognise the
likely common understanding which arises from your shared humanity. Many (perhaps all) cultures
have aspects which are oppressive or restrictive in relation to intimate relationships. However, with all
its irrationalities and injustices, culture always plays a crucial role in supporting sexual development
and social relationships. Individual experience can never be predicted from summaries of cultural
attitudes and practices, although the elements of an individual’s life always take meaning within a
specific culture. This can be particularly paradoxical in relation to those beliefs which you will want to
172
Sex, love, work and children
say are absolute (for example, that genital mutilation is wrong) – these beliefs will nevertheless always
be expressed within the terms of a particular culture.
Marriage is a remarkably widespread institution across human societies, but, notwithstanding this, the
forms and expectations of marriage vary considerably across cultures. Buss and colleagues (1990)
examined the views of married people around the world to examine how different marriage arrange-
ments and cultures affected what qualities are valued in partner selection. In their sample of nearly
10,000 people, they found agreement across cultures, across marriage procedures, and between men
and women that the most important factors are love and mutual attraction, with very little variation
from the recognition of dependability, emotional stability, kindness, and understanding as key qualities
being sought. The research by Hazan and Zeifman (1999) into attachment and pair bonding also makes
reference to this finding. Many Westerners, particularly modern feminists with an individualist outlook,
regard the possibility of satisfaction in arranged marriages with elements of curiosity, questioning and
disbelief. Factual cross-cultural findings are ambiguous. Myers and colleagues (2005) quote Shachar
(1991) as finding minimal differences in marital satisfaction in an Israeli sample of 206 married couples
between arranged and autonomous marriages. Yelsma and Athappilly (1988) in a study of eighty-four
couples, found higher marital satisfaction scores for those in arranged marriages; whilst Xu and Whyte
(1990) found among 586 married women in China, higher satisfaction for free choice marriages.
In their own study, Myers and her colleagues reported that they found no difference in reported
satisfaction with marriage between arranged and free-choice marriages.
Gender perspectives
‘Sex’ and ‘gender’
When do you use the word ‘sex’, and when the word ‘gender’?
When she’s pregnant, Nicola may be asked, ‘Do you know what sex the baby is?’ But the application
form Sharon fills in might have spaces for name, address, date of birth and boxes to select for ‘gender:
m ❑ . . . f ❑’. As you will have found with many of the terms in this book, there is no single definition,
but a range of overlapping ways in which the words may be used, overlapping concepts to which they
may refer:
• ‘Sex’ can refer to the biological characteristics of the body, and used in this way, the great majority
of people (but not all) can be allocated into one of two categories, ‘male’ or ‘female’.
• ‘Sex’ can also refer to sexual activity, and with its adjective ‘sexual’ this can be associated with other
matters – ‘sexual instruction’, ‘sexual initiation’ and so on.
• ‘Sex’ and its adjective ‘sexy’ can have even more specific reference to subjectively erotic subjects.
Using the same word can lead to the second and third uses being seen incorrectly as the defining
feature of the first (that is, that what makes a woman female is her ‘sexual function’ whether
173
Sex, love, work and children
?
romantically or in relation to childbearing). To avoid double meanings, and probably also sometimes
out of an English-speaking squeamishness about using the word ‘sex’ in certain conversations, people
sometimes use the word ‘gender’ instead of ‘sex’ in the first meaning:
• ‘Gender’ refers principally to masculine and feminine (not male and female). ‘Gender’
is used particularly to indicate the social constructions and consequences associated
with being male or female. The finding (see Chapter 8) that more women than men
Chapters
3 and 8
over 65 live in poverty, and the patterns of educational achievement described in
Chapter 3, are examples of gendered features of society.
• More specifically, ‘gender expression’ refers to socially constructed ways of behaving that have a
link with sex – for example, wearing skirts in modern European dress.
• Finally, as explained below, there is the complex argument that ‘gender’ is the essential fact, and
the categories ‘woman’ or ‘man’ are socially created within this context.
In relation to the second of these, the term has significance because some cultural constructions
wrongly emphasised an intrinsic link with biological sex (that men were more suited to intellectual
work and women to domestic work, for example; or that a man dressing in skirts or a woman in
trousers is ‘unnatural’). Once these expressions of gender are understood to be created by society, they
are less bound to the idea of the biological sex of a particular individual. There can be a wide range of
gender expression which has no deterministic link to the two sexes. At one stage in her life, a girl may
like rough physical activities and taking physical risks, mainly in the company of boys, and describe
herself as disliking pink and ‘girlie’ things, but at another may enjoy courtship and romance that has
many traditional heterosexual features. A man may sense that his own ‘gender expression’ challenges
oppressive traditional norms. Because conventional gender expression has been so bound up with
sexual orientation, there are many respects in which gay men and lesbians are conscious of creating
their own gender expression. Later in the chapter you will see the words of Helen Hill who was born
intersex, brought up as a boy but feels more comfortable and integrated now that she is a woman. She
describes her experience of ‘gender expression’, and her website (Hill, 2008) contains resource articles
about this aspect of gender.
So you can often think of ‘sex’ as referring to physical characteristics and ‘gender’ to social features.
But the previous paragraphs remind you that this is not the only way the terms are used, and the
third bullet point above indicates one final analytical approach which specifically questions it.
Recognising this physical/social dichotomy between the two terms, Butler argued that we need to
revise them: it is not that physical sex is the basic fact, and gender is built socially on it. Sex is not ‘a
bodily given on which the construct of gender is artificially imposed, but . . . a cultural norm which
governs the materialisation of bodies’ (Butler, 1993). Any reference to bodies, she argues, will always
be expressed in cultural terms, which have a strongly gendered basis. As Jackson and Scott put it,
‘women’ as a social category needs explanation independently of the biological: ‘If sex, as well as
gender, is a construct, it follows that the body does not have a pre-given essential sex’ (2002: 19,
commenting on Butler).
174
Sex, love, work and children
Gender expression ?
For many people, ways of expressing that dimension of their identity we call ‘gender’ is largely
hidden, because it is so much taken for granted as part of their everyday interaction. There is a long
sociological tradition (Goffman, 1959/2008; Butler, 1990) which sees social roles as
‘performance’ rather than innate identity, and examines the stigma which results when
this ‘performance’ is unacceptable to others (see Chapter 7). People whose ‘performance’
Chapter 7 is not intuitively or comfortably within the approved range have cause to become self-
consciously aware of the processes involved. The details/specifics of gender expression
vary from micro-culture to micro-culture, from relationship to relationship and from setting to setting.
A woman may enjoy a pole dancing class as part of her ‘hen night’ as an expression of something
quite different from that of her usual behaviour in mixed-sex evenings out. ‘Gender expression’ being
largely hidden (because taken for granted), it can take an unusual experience to highlight what is
involved. Here are the words of Helen Hill and the response from ‘Gerda’, another correspondent, in
an online discussion sponsored by the BBC (see also Hill, 2008). Helen is a therapist who was born
hermaphrodite/intersex, brought up unhappily as a boy, and then settled as a woman when she became
adult. Helen and Gerda are telling personal accounts which highlight ‘gender’ as a learned feature of
social presentation:
I find this topic fascinating, as I have had to learn a lot really, really fast so that I can stay safe,
and present my body as it was meant to be without being ashamed of it. . . .
It was really hard to learn in a few quick years (5 now) what other women have learned ‘from the
ground up.’ It was also really hard to learn to be safe. Not that I didn’t know I was now in a
vulnerable position. Knew that one really well. But I didn’t realise how QUICKLY I would be put in
compromising, vulnerable and threatening positions!. . .
175
Sex, love, work and children
So my education as Helen has been quite an eye-opener. I never fit in as a guy anyway, being
hermaphrodite/intersex. But the immersion into the world of women was baptism by fire. And the
matter of sexualisation seems to be a catch-22.
[. . .]
‘Gerda’ replies: Well put, Helen! I had a harrowing few years when I lost a lot of weight. Previously,
as a ‘fat lass’, I had not been preyed on socially as much as other girls, and as a punk/biker I felt
safe walking at night because I was wearing big boots and a leather jacket (and nice sharp keys
in my pocket) – but suddenly I was ‘in scope’ and I had not developed those protective tactics
either.
(BBC, 2007b)
Both go on to comment on how the change in what we can call ‘gender expression’ led to violence
and unwanted approaches from men (the double bind, of course, is that for a woman to present in a
way that is not conventionally ‘feminine’ may also provoke male complaints and disparagement).
Helen’s experience gives an adult’s self-aware view of what girls learn by trial and error without the
conceptual sophistication possessed by adults, although they may be intuitively aware. Norah Vincent
(2006), a lesbian woman who, for an experiment, lived for a year as a man, was often surprised by the
gendered behaviour of women towards her as a man. She found women less tolerant of varied gender
behaviour than men, and in romantic encounters (dates and approaches for dates), more rejecting and
hostile towards their partners. She describes men’s gendered behaviour in romantic relationships as
forged in the crucible of the risk and reality of rejection from women, a rejection that she was shocked
to experience, and felt that her life as a lesbian had not prepared her for it. Social workers often deal
with men who are least equipped to manage this challenge, and also with women who have been most
exposed to the destructive effects of sexual intrusion.
‘Gender expression’ is a mediating factor in the power of women’s sexuality over men (and vice versa);
it is bound up with sexual orientation, and is subject to wide variations in different cultures. It is
inextricably bound up with sexuality in all its aspects, but more importantly, serves as a marker in many
areas in which society organized itself through gender.
Health
?
The experience of health is strongly linked with sex and gender. Physical wellness is experienced in a
sexed body, patterns of health and its maintenance are gendered, and working in health provision is
gendered. Other matters related to health, such as disability and ageing, have strongly gendered aspects.
A man’s life, on average, is likely to be shorter than a woman’s, and men are more likely
to suffer from a chronic illness (Smith et al., 2008). Some 72 per cent of (female) breast
cancers are successfully treated but only about 53 per cent of (male) prostrate cancer
Chapter 7
(ONS, 2006); as discussed in Chapter 7, women are more likely to be diagnosed with
176
Sex, love, work and children
depression, and men with schizophrenia. In general, women make more use of the health service. In
many of these areas, there are different explanations: at the biological level, at the level of individual
behaviour and experience, at the level of organisation and policy, and at the level of society and social
organisation. Particularly at the level of social theory, the explanations may be contrasting and
disputed.
In the UK, health provision is clearly gendered. Health practitioners – in the National Health Service or
in self-employed complementary health activity – are predominantly female, and health management
is also predominantly female, although in both, the proportion of men increases as the jobs become
more highly paid.
Family care – of children or dependent parents – is disproportionately undertaken by women, and this
means that they are also far more involved in negotiating with the health service and health prac-
?
titioners on behalf of others.
Nicola had changed career and was on a placement, training to be a youth worker. The
noise in the large hall told her that something was going on. The room was in uproar,
and she saw Melissa on the ground, being dragged the whole length of the floor by Josh
– 20 or 30 metres of violent pulling. One hand pulled her by the hair, and the other
yanked her by her coat, pulling her this way and that, Josh swinging her violently and
triumphally as he covered the ground. ‘Never mind the damage to the coat,’ Nicola said
later, ‘I thought Melissa’s hair would come away in handfuls. It must have been
excrutiating. It was straight caveman stuff.’ The club was officially called the Weirfield,
but was known locally as the ‘warfield’ because of the problems displayed by the
youngsters who attended. Apparently, the incident had started when Melissa had
snatched Josh’s leather cap from his head and thrown it away. Everyone knew Josh was
inseparable from his leather cap; it was his pride and joy, and he was never seen without
it. When the incident had sorted itself out, the youngsters stood in a group of five,
177
Sex, love, work and children
Nicola with them. Suddenly, Melissa looked Josh straight in the eye, snatched his cap
and threw it away. The whole scene started up again.
Nicola said that she knew it was ‘just teenagers’, but it worried her – ‘it seems to be the
only way the boys and girls communicate,’ she said. ‘The girls just keep going back for
more, and it’s how the lads relate to them.’
?
Reflective thinking
• Would you share Nicola’s concerns? How might Nicola have an influence on the girls’
attitudes? What about those of the boys?
• What are your thoughts about the biological, psychological and social influences on violence?
A standard textbook (Boyd and Bee, 2006: 271) asserts that in all societies that have been studied, and
in all primate species, males display more violent behaviour than females. The rates of physical child
abuse are not really an exception, as they probably relate to specific circumstances – women spend
more time in stressful situations with children.
There is some evidence from American studies that ‘relational aggression’ – intended to hurt the
person’s feelings, damage their self-esteem, or isolate them from their friends – is shown by females
in about the same frequency as physical aggression is in males (Boyd and Bee, 2006: 271, citing
research by Crick, Grotpeter and others).
Figures for the extent of violence in intimate relationships vary, partly because of definitions and
research methods used. The British crime survey for 2001–2002 estimated that there were 635,000
incidents of domestic violence (514,000 against women and 122,00 against men). Other work
undertaken by the British crime survey estimated that actual rates are likely to be three times higher
than their survey figures (Home Office, 2008). Walby and Allen (2004) reviewed the British crime survey
figures for the Home Office directorate of research, and suggest that 4 per cent of women and 2 per
cent of men had experienced domestic violence in the last year, with one in five women and one in ten
men between the age of 16 and 59 having ever experienced at least one incident of non-sexual
domestic threat or force. If sexual force and stalking are included, the figures are higher. The vast
majority of serious and recurring violence is perpetrated by men towards women. Renzetti (1998: 199),
like Rogers and Pilgrim (2003: 145), reports a number of studies showing equal or higher rates of
intimate violence in same-sex relationships, but urges caution in making statements about the
frequency of violence and abuse in gay and lesbian relationships.
178
Sex, love, work and children
These references to gender and violence in intimate relationships prompt a return to the theme with
which this chapter began – the development of sexual relationships. One’s understanding of what is
involved develops through experience. For most, this picture is influenced by the experience of man-
aging conflict and difference. For some men and women – and social workers may often be in contact
with them – violence, inflicted or received, will be a significant part of their developing understanding
about what is involved in ‘intimacy’.
Once again, biology, psychology and culture interact with specific situations to produce the problems
of violence with which social workers must contend.
This incident of Josh and the leather cap is closely based on a real episode. It concerns
a student youth worker, a 40-year-old woman with teenage sons of her own, who had been
brought up not far from the area in which she worked. While accepting that the trouble
at the youth club was ‘just teenagers’, she was concerned that it showed an emerging
pattern in which provocation and aggression were intrinsic to relationships, to the
detriment of men, women and children. She stored the incident away, knowing that sooner
or later there would be an opening to discuss the issues that had been graphically
demonstrated. In particular, she knew that both boys and girls enjoyed cookery sessions
with her, and would take the opportunity to have serious conversations with her while their
hands were busy. When the time was right, she would raise it with the girls. She was less
able to predict when the opportunity would come with Josh, but in addition to
demonstrating ‘alpha male’ behaviour, he would confide in her about the violence and
rejection he experienced at home. Whether or not this could be raised with the whole group,
there would come a time when it would be natural to discuss it with him.
?
Reflective thinking
Are there occasions in your work or placement when your professional responsibility is to
influence human growth and development?
?
Being a parent
The subject of this book is growth and development. Being a parent – living life as a mother
or father – involves personal development and change on an unprecedented scale. For
reasons of space, Chapter 1 made only the briefest reference to the dramatic physical
Chapter 1
changes experienced by Nicola, Naoko or Sharon during their nine months of pregnancy.
179
Sex, love, work and children
Many women after birth feel their bodies are never the same again, though that change is different for
women of different ages – the effects on a 16-year-old are very different from those on a 42-year-old.
The chapter referred to the complex social lives in which these changes were set. I quoted Donald
Winnicott’s words: ‘There is no such thing as a baby, only a mother-and-baby’ – and certainly, for most,
the experience of constantly having to care not just for oneself but also for another, totally vulnerable
person is a dramatically different life experience. This is the beginning of many such revelations over
the coming years. With the arrival of the first baby, a woman may well feel her social position changes
– as one writer put it, she has ‘joined the matriarchy’. This of course may partly be reflected in the actual
composition of her immediate social circle and topics of conversation, as well as the marketing group
in which her shopping habits and interests now place her.
Commonly, after nine months inside her, the newborn baby preoccupies the mind of the mother.
Although this will lessen as the years go by, many mothers will be generally aware of what their
children are doing at any stage of the day, and planning the hours ahead even when they are not phys-
ically with them. As ever, every woman’s experience is different – for many, birth brings the experience
of falling in love, of closeness and protectiveness – and Nancy Chodorow (1999) claimed that women
are more likely than men to have ‘powerful experiences of connectedness’. But equally, there may be
?
feelings of being overwhelmed at the responsibility for another person, or of disappointment at
the lack of a bond, or, indeed, birth may bring with it protracted desolate feelings, of recognised or
unrecognised depression.
Looking closer
In Holland, the Kraamzorg (approximately, ‘midwife’) may live with the parents for the first
week after the birth to give practical help with many domestic matters and to assist the parents
with their care of the new baby. In a radio discussion (BBC Radio 4, Woman’s Hour, 17 June
2008), a Kraamzorg explained the satisfaction of her work – ‘You’re there at the birth of a
new baby, but also at the birth of a mother.’ A father commented on what he learnt from her –
‘I learnt from her how to give support to a mother, what she needed me to do.’
Basing her work on interviews with over 200 parents, Ellen Galinksy (1982) described how adults develop
through interaction with their children. She worked within Erikson’s framework and identified six stages
of development, starting with a ‘parental image’ stage, in which the man and woman start to form an
image of themselves as parents, and progressing through constant changes to a ‘departure stage’.
For the moment, continue to think about the mother–child dyad, one of the different evolving
‘microsystems’ (in Bronfenbrenner’s terms – ‘Essential background’, section 3). The constant awareness
of someone else and their state of mind will in time be fitted in alongside other external concerns,
180
?
Sex, love, work and children
whether domestic (washing, cleaning, cooking, shopping and social contact) or work- and
career-related. Later, a dramatic change in this physical awareness is no doubt one reason
why many women feel a great loss and a major transition (‘empty nest’ syndrome) as
Chapter
EB3 children leave home.
The experience of motherhood, its place in life, varies considerably between different cultures and
historical periods. It is different if it begins during or immediately after adolescence, compared with
its beginning when a woman is in her late thirties or early forties. The likelihood of its happening at
one age rather than another reflects social and historical trends as well as personal choices, as
illustrated in Figure 6.1. The Office for National Statistics (Summerfield et al., 2003) gave mothers’
average age of first birth in 2001 as 27 compared with the age of 21 thirty years earlier. Compared
with the past, women are more likely to have an established career before motherhood becomes part
of their experience. Later childbearing is associated with significantly more difficulty in getting
pregnant, and women are not always aware of this, or of the stress and invasiveness of IVF treatment.
Some are acutely aware, though, and struggle with competing demands - the urge to have a child,
the lifestyle associated with work and career, and pressures about partnership. Compared with thirty
years ago (but not seventy years ago, when the massive killings of the First World War were still felt
and there were 1.7 million women surplus over men – GBH GIS, 2008), women are more likely to be
child-free. Women who are single now have greater social freedom and more practical options about
becoming mothers, and more women are choosing to be child-free. The ‘pathways’ to being child-free
appear to differ between men and women. In a study of 6,000 men and women, Keizera and colleagues
(2007) found that both educational attainment and a stable career increase the likelihood of remaining
childless among women, but, on the contrary increase the likelihood of becoming a father for men. For
men more than women, spending years without a partner, or having multiple partnerships, are
associated with childlessness.
Many fathers are in full-time work. For them, the transition to parenthood will be different from that
experienced by a mother. For a proportion of women, the length of time they stay away from work is
affected by public policy on maternity leave. Many would like to stay out of paid work longer, and
current changes to regulations will allow that to happen for a year rather than the present nine months
after giving birth. Similarly, depending on the extent of take-up, increases in the parental leave
entitlement for fathers will change the situation for them. Michael Lamb’s research (2004) has been
influential in examining the role of fathers in child development, and in (heterosexual) families, he
found that the relationship between mother and father was a significant variable in affecting the
attachment and interaction between father and child – research findings are fairly reliable that the
warmer and richer the relationship between the mother and father, the better the relationship between
father and child. This leads him to emphasise that child development is influenced by ‘family climate’
as well as by individual relationships with caregivers. He found that in the early stages of development,
the father’s attachment has similar functions to the mother’s: a secure bond helping the child to be
confident in social relationships and exploration. Subsequently, in a number of countries (but not all),
fathers show a more active physical interaction with their children than mothers (see also Boyd and
181
Sex, love, work and children
Bee, 2006: 142). He suggests that this may have led some researchers using mother-centred measures
to underestimate the attachment of children to fathers. More notably, he reliably found that there is
a long-range effect as the child grows older: in two-parent families, a strong relationship between
father and child at the ages of 7 and 11 was associated with more mature romantic relationships in
adolescence, better examination results at 16, and no criminal record at age 21. His main explanation
for this is that the earlier strong relationship probably continues through the years of adolescence.
Susan Golombok’s studies of same-sex parents (particularly lesbian parents) ‘tend to show that
children . . . are no [more] disadvantaged in terms of their emotional well-being or other aspects of
development than children in comparable heterosexual families, which leads to the conclusion that it
is really the second parent that is most important . . . the relationship is more important than the
gender of that parent’ (UK Parliament, 2007).
It is not only that the child as an outside object of concern preoccupies the mind of the mother to a
greater or lesser degree. ‘Mind-mindedness’ in a responsive parent involves the flexible ability to attune
to the mind of the child. This requires the adult adapting to cognitions, emotional needs and responses
which are very different from their own. Sometimes very fast-moving, sometimes unreasonable by
adult expectations, sometimes fantasy-laden or rooted in cognitive errors, the child’s mind, as
discussed in the earlier chapters, requires active adaptation as well as stimulation from the mind of
a caring adult. This may be experienced as fascinating, extremely tiring, frustrating, infuriating or
mind-numbingly boring. Along with the physical work which childcare entails, this effort is undervalued
in much contemporary society. The child’s mind, too, is constantly exercised as it engages with all the
different people in its life. The two parents have different personalities and approaches to giving
comfort or behaviour management. In sharing the tasks of parenting, more or less harmoniously, each
parent’s developing attitudes, expectations and behaviours will be influenced by the other’s. When
grown up, the children will no doubt find that they have taken different qualities, beliefs and skills from
each parent.
There is give and take in parenting. Sometimes the wishes and demands in the mind of the child can
be met, and sometimes they cannot. But, in general, enjoying parenthood involves sacrificing some
self-regarding wishes. This is a real demand, a real loss, but many parents in the long term achieve a
special satisfaction because of the exchange they have been prepared to make. Both mother and father
are likely to grow in maturity as they have the new experience of dealing with a potential battle of
wills – an experience which they resolve not by imposing their will, nor by retaliating, nor by allowing
the other will to win because they ‘ought to’ submit, but because it makes sense for them out of a
higher drive (the child’s well-being) to put their own needs on hold. The hazards in this are, on the one
hand, that a parent (typically a mother) submerges herself in the task to the extent that she experiences
a loss of ‘self’, and feels that her identity is entirely bound up with that of her child; or, on the other
hand, that parents are unable to put their own feelings and impulses on hold in order to attend to
children’s needs. At each stage, from infancy to adolescence, the child’s emotions are likely to evoke
comparable feelings in the caregiver – whether of calmness and satisfaction, of murderous frustration
or rebellion against external control.
182
Sex, love, work and children
All parents need support and recognition in their experience of these tasks and challenges, and directly
or indirectly this is often what a second parent provides; for parents – men or women – who are limited
in their ability to cope with these feelings, social workers or other social care staff have an important
role in becoming part of the developmental experience of the parent. The experiences of a child evoke
age-appropriate related feelings in a parent. It is much easier when they have someone who cares;
someone who comforts and supports them, lessens stress, takes care of some of the practical matters,
and supports self-esteem and mutual learning about behaviour management. For many, but not all,
this comes from intimate or ‘extended’ family relationships, as well as naturally evolving social net-
works. But in modern society, social workers, school supports and organised family services are part
of the networks created to meet this need.
One component of this family support relates back to the parental relationship, the subject of the
earlier part of this chapter. Problems with children can pull this relationship apart, and strain between
parents certainly has an impact on children. Social workers have an important function when they
enable parents to maintain their relationship, to recover respect and care for each other. Sometimes
separation seems unavoidable, and the parents collaborate effectively in the continuing lives of the
children. On other occasions, though, in the words of a UK government report, ‘parental separation
often has a traumatic effect on children’ (DCSF, 2004). Data from the Office of National Statistics shows
that on average, the effect of parental separation is negative on each aspect of a child’s life – educa-
tional attainment, emotional and social well-being (Smallwood and Wilson, 2007). The chances of
adverse outcomes in these areas are doubled after separation or divorce (the evidence also shows that
children’s emotional difficulties precipitated by divorce are counteracted after a period of years).
Obviously, separation does not necessarily reduce the parental conflict which children report as
harmful to their happiness, or propel parents into cooperating in the care of their children. In addition,
the effect on at least one of the parents is often destructive. Social workers also have a role in
understanding the child’s experience of loss and in enabling – or arranging for – mediation with the
aim of collaboration and shared care. Principles and practical guidance for parents and others involved
are given in a resource from the Department for Children, Schools and Families (DCSF/Children and
Family Court Advisory and Support, 2006).
183
Sex, love, work and children
it differently, but most will tell you that it is not something that can be resolved. The death of a child
has its own way of marking a parent thereafter. So too does the experience of a loss which seems just
as final (through adoption, for example) but which has to exist irreconcilably with the knowledge that
somewhere the child is growing, developing, being happy or unhappy. As the mothers in Charlton’s
study describe the adoption of their children, ‘It’s like a death’, and decades later described themselves
as ‘still screaming’, thinking about their children ‘most days’ or ‘most weeks’ (Charlton et al., 1998; see
also Howe et al., 1992).
What is harder is having kids, knowing they live quite nearby but now being separated. They are
withheld from you by the mother – that is a kind of torture, especially when you are powerless to
do anything about it.
(contribution in an online discussion on BBC Radio 4, 2 June 2008)
Although development after this parent–child separation is a central social work concern, and an
important dimension to be aware of in any assessment, you will often find that it is not drawn to your
attention in procedural guidance or assessment forms. In care proceedings it is the child who is the focus
of official work, not the future needs of the mother. After its medical function has ended because a child
dies, the Health Trust management may not understand that the social worker involved still has a
professional task in relation to someone they have been helping daily, but who has never been the
patient. The court service, in preparing reports about child custody, does not allocate any ‘aftercare’
responsibility. In family support services, the emotional needs of a father who is problematic in the
immediate situation may not be appropriately understood – his childcare incompetence in the present
family, aggressive resentment of anyone threatening to take ‘his’ children, might need to be seen in the
?
context of his earlier experiences involving loss of children. In all of these situations, however, it is the
social worker’s responsibility to make an assessment of the needs of each person in the situation and
to appraise realistically and holistically how the function of their agency relates to those identified needs.
Constant development
In summary, changes in the parental experience occur all the time as children grow and new babies are
born into the family. A child reaching adolescence is a particular point of change as the child expects
new levels of responsibility and autonomy, as are the transitions when a child leaves to live on their
own. Some 60 per cent of marriages around the world are arranged, so at this stage in their children’s
lives, the parents are actively engaged in negotiating with their children and seeking spouses for them.
Moving on again, the parents have new experiences as the ‘children’, now adults, take responsibility
for them in everyday ways (such as paying for meals out, which may seem like a reversal of roles!).
184
? Sex, love, work and children
Children thrust new learning upon their parents as they marry or settle in partnerships, bestowing the
new role of ‘in-laws’. Later, a further phase for the parent is created if the child cares for them, possibly
with the parent being behaviourally difficult or resentful and intellectually deluded about
the relationship (as in dementia). This is discussed further in Chapter 8, about old age. In
this phase, the parent is adjusting to the psychological dynamic of placing burdens on
Chapter 8 their children (not removing them). The ‘child’ is highly likely to be a woman in middle-
age, with children of her own and a job – as Brody and Saperstein (2006) say, a ‘woman
in the middle’. In the introduction to their book, the family care of older people by women is described
as a ‘normative phase’ in family life. As a stage in life, it has not received the ‘normative’ attention (and
support and recognition) it deserves. It has, perhaps, been understood non-developmentally by ideo-
logical perspectives on women’s experience, and sidelined in policy formation about public provision.
?
Reflective thinking
Discuss the development that takes place in parents, as parents, after their children’s school days
are over.
Work
Adults who are in full-time paid employment spend a large proportion of their waking hours at work.
It is the setting of much of their physical, intellectual and emotional activity. At its best, it is the setting
for intellectual stimulation and learning. For social workers, for example, it is a place of learning about
the world of organisations and social policy, but also of learning about emotions and people, about
human need, and the management of conflict; it forces continuing reflection about what, in the long
term, is of value in human life. The majority of social contact may take place through work – when
people retire from full-time work, their level of social contact may drop dramatically. At its worse, work
is a cause of injury, early death or a source of constant stress (Walsh et al., 2005).
Jahoda (1982) described obtaining income as the ‘manifest’ (openly visible) function of work, and
identified a further six ‘latent’ (concealed, or dormant) functions. This scheme is summarised in the box
below.
185
Sex, love, work and children
• a source of income;
• a form of activity;
Warr and Warr (2002) built on this work to develop a ten-factor model of work. This can be used not
only to analyse different work experiences but also to understand the effects of unemployment. For
example, one of their factors is ‘opportunity for control’. Workplaces differ in the degree to which they
give the worker autonomy and responsibility to control their own work and to participate in decisions
which affect it; greater control is associated with greater satisfaction at work and better mental health.
Unemployment and dependency on welfare tends in general to restrict environmental options and
decrease opportunities for making life decisions. Work provides finance, one of the most obvious of
Warr and Warr’s (2002) categories, and they write of unemployment: ‘Studies of unemployed people
consistently indicate that shortage of money is viewed as the greatest source of personal and family
problems. Poverty bears down not only upon basic needs for food and physical protection, but also
prevents activity and reduces one’s sense of personal control.’ Rogers and Pilgrim (2003: 117) point out
that unemployment correlates with higher psychiatric diagnosis, and re-employment is consistently
shown to reduce diagnosis of psychiatric disorder. They discuss the factors involved in this statistical
?
link. These include the question of whether unemployment causes the mental health problem or the
reverse, the different effects of voluntarily or involuntarily leaving the workforce, the difference
between secure and insecure employment status, and the level of financial pressure caused by lack
of work.
Poverty has a major influence on development throughout life, and is one of the subjects
in Chapter 7. It is a prime example of a problem which social workers are constantly
dealing with, and in which they cannot professionally just deal with the personal – to act
Chapter 7
with integrity, they must also be concerned with societal causes.
The social psychologist Bales introduced the terms ‘expressive’ and ‘instrumental’ (originally about
social roles) to distinguish between activities whose value lies mainly in personal and interpersonal
expression, and activities whose value lies strongly in practical outcome (you have an expressive
relationship with a love partner, but a purely instrumental relationship with the people who keep the
186
Sex, love, work and children
electricity supply working). For many, work is closely bound up with social identity and has an
expressive function. While this may be true to a degree for many mothers with a career, they are equally
likely to be in the category of people whose work has a purely instrumental value – it needs to be
trouble-free, leave them energy for their children, and pay them reliably – although social relations
within work, as distinct from the formal work itself, may have an important expressive function in
their lives.
Emotions in work
Some work obviously involves hard physical labour and requires physical strength. Arlie Hochschild
devised the term ‘emotional labour’ to describe work which involves working with emotions. The value
to the employer is the effect the employee has on the customer’s feelings. Her classic study was of
flight attendants, who at the time were usually female (previously called ‘air hostesses’ – ‘from trolley
dolly to emotions manager’ as one reviewer put it). Some parts of their job involved physical labour –
managing the food and physical space in the aircraft. In addition, however, they also were expected to
keep the atmosphere calm; soothe passengers’ emotions, leave them feeling contented, relaxed,
attended to, and even pampered. Many features of the job – appearance, uniform, demeanour, constant
?
and even-tempered presentation – were related to this. Hochschild saw this emotional labour as an
economic exploitation. It violated the integrity of workers by getting them to display pseudo-emotion,
not as deliberate acting (as in the theatre), but as if it was truly felt. It devalued genuine human contact
by mimicking it for profit motives.
To be practised effectively, social work also involves demanding emotional work (Howe,
2008; Sudbery and Bradley, 1996). This is skilled, it is sometimes wearing, it can be
stressful, and it is the source of much of the satisfaction in effective social work. To be
Chapters 9
and 10 helpful when dealing over a period of time with a volatile (or dangerous) teenager who
clashes with his parents and others in authority, requires considerable emotional invest-
ment. Intense feelings are involved in working out the best course of action when dealing with a baby
who has been severely injured by his parents, or, as in the example in Chapter 9, helping a dying person
when the practicalities of what you can offer turn out to be inadequate. In all these cases, good
working practice requires that the worker finds and uses someone helpful and reliable to support their
heightened feelings. It is a sign of maturity, not inadequacy, to be able to use a senior or more
experienced person for emotional support.
At the moment, men’s earning power reaches a peak about the age of 45. The work prospects (including
pay) of childless women are similar to those of single men (Waldfogel, 1998), but in many other
respects, women’s lifetime experience of work is often very different from men’s. They are more likely
to find that being a parent, a partner and an employee creates conflicting demands and priorities –
women are actively mothers (and ‘wives’) while they are at work, but men are more likely to experience
their roles as sequential – ‘workers’ at work and ‘fathers/partners’ in separate periods during the
day (Boyd and Bee, 2006: 402). Women tend to judge themselves about how well they manage
187
Sex, love, work and children
relationships (as mother and partner) as much as their performance as an employee. At the moment,
the degree to which the boundaries around work time can be penetrated by other responsibilities
seems to be different for men and women.
?
Reflective thinking
Write down some of the ways in which, from your observation or experience, emotions at work
are an important part of social work.
(Think of the team and organisational setting; the demands of the task; the sort of understanding
required of social workers; the impact of the work; the effect of non-work life on work
performance.)
Most women move in and out of the world of external work at least once, and often many times. In
2004, 70 per cent of married mothers worked outside the home, again marking a social change as
in 1974 the figure was 47 per cent (Iacovou, 2004b). Women who are in continuous paid work earn
more than those whose work pattern is not continuous (Boyd and Bee, 2006, quoting Drobnic
et al., 1999). Hersch and Stratton (2002) found that time spent in housework (not necessarily including
childcare, and for both men and women) explained a significant amount of the income differentials in
paid employment – the effect is strongest for housework that occurs daily and this tends to be
undertaken by women. On average, women earn less than men. This has complex causes (Women and
Work Commission, 2006). In the UK, this does not seem to be a result of different pay rates for the
same job. It largely disappears as women have fewer discontinuities in their employment careers, and
it relates in large measure to child-free women versus mothers. This pay gap has proved difficult for
policy makers to eliminate. Current initiatives include the creation of better quality part-time jobs,
improving vocational training, and encouraging women into higher paid employment sectors –
ensuring that ‘horizontal’ segregation of work between men and women does not cause gendered
differences in income. Some interpret the pay gap as an inequality to be eradicated in particular by
better provision for childcare, while others see it as a descriptive difference caused by choices and work
careers which are not obviously the realm of social policy (O’Neill and O’Neill, 2005).
188
Sex, love, work and children
These have the merits of continuing developmental study into adulthood, but there are many
?
difficulties with such attempts.
In describing adulthood, many authors make outline use of three stages – early adulthood, described
as the period from about 20 to 40; middle adulthood from 40 to 60; and older age from 60 onwards.
Older age in turn is often described using the terms ‘young older age’ from about 60 to 75; ‘middle
period’, from about 75 to 90; and the ‘oldest old’, who are over 90. Erikson’s scheme
(Chapter 2 and ‘Essential background’, section 6) places emphasis in early adulthood on
the formation of close personal arrangements – the characteristic ‘crisis’ he identifies is
Chapters intimacy versus isolation. As with all his stages, he understands the positive development
2 and EB6
of identity in the previous stage as a springboard for relinquishing it in the next. In the
case of the transition between adolescence and early adulthood, he suggests that the person who
has successfully avoided identity confusion or foreclosure in the earlier stage will have sufficient
confidence in a secure adult identity to relinquish some of it without damage in order to form a close
intimate relationship. In the next stage – middle adulthood, aged 40 or so to 60 – he places issues to
do with ‘generativity’; doing something of value for the world, leaving something creative behind. It
is in this stage that he places parenthood. Although this might seem rather late, and is an aspect which
has sometimes been criticised over the decades since he wrote Childhood and Society, perhaps it is
placed appropriately for many people today, as more women have their children later. Boyd and Bee
(2006: 433) summarise later empirical evidence which backs up or critiques this analysis by Erikson.
Daniel Levinson devised a stage model of adult life. He regarded the totality of a person’s roles, rela-
tionships, responsibilities, conflicts and so on as forming a ‘life structure’. Basing his research on forty
men (1978) and forty-five women (1986), he examined a wide range of factors in people’s lives –
biological, economic, work-based, and psychosocial. He concluded that life structures are formed and
dissolve during adulthood in fairly predictable ways. So there are periods of structure-building
(entering early adulthood, entering middle adulthood, and entering late-life) and periods of structure-
changing (the early adulthood transition, mid-life transition and late adult transition). Like Erikson, he
regarded these changes as presenting the person with a life-crisis as they adjust and transform (as
Erikson is responsible for ‘identity crisis’ entering common language, Levinson is responsible for the
?
phrase ‘mid-life crisis’). Once again, the concept of crisis here does not mean something which is
avoidable and unwanted, but is something inherent in human development.
Lifespan theory (the framework behind Sugarman’s textbook (2001) and Boyd and Bee’s (2006; Bee
and Boyd, 2007) is less focused on finding universal ‘stages’ in life development. Those working within
this framework regard human development as always the outcome of a specific culture
and a particular historical period, ‘so it becomes less relevant to search for predictive
theories which hold across cultures and across generations’ (Sugarman, 2001: 3). This
Chapter 10 approach is used as a summary framework in Chapter 10.
189
Sex, love, work and children
Summary
Sexual behaviour is shaped by biological, psychological and social influences. Social workers have
to build on a general understanding in order to deal appropriately with specific areas of respon-
sibility such as promoting sexual health and preventing or dealing with the aftermath of sexual
abuse or sexual assault.
As a category word, ‘sex’ is generally used to refer to bodily characteristics, while ‘gender’ refers to
expression, behaviour, expectations and social outcomes which have some link with sex.
Biological influences on sex include the drive to reproduce and immediate factors related to
hormones. It appears that sexual orientation, sexual behaviour and romantic attraction can vary
independently of each other in different ways for men and women. Circumstances and personal
psychological history affect sexual relationships, as do cultural and historical factors.
Parenthood is a major influence on development for many adults. Galinsky described a six-stage
development, from forming an image of being a parent through to ‘departure’. The experience of
parenthood has both universal elements and elements strongly affected by culture (such as average
age for becoming a parent). Many people describe their relationship with their children as the most
important in their lives, and social workers are particularly likely to be working with parents
separated from their children.
Work has both an explicit function and a number of other latent functions, including that it can
provide a source of identity and a sense of purpose. Some work, such as social work, involves genuine
emotional investment. The great majority of women move in and out of the workforce at least once
and often many times; being a parent, a partner, and an employee is likely to inflict more incom-
patible demands and clash of priorities for women. Patterns of men’s and women’s work change
over time and culture. Social workers are likely to deal both with the practical financial effects of
unemployment and the reduced well-being (including the increased rate of psychiatric disorder)
which it brings.
Further reading
Adult relationships:
Boyd, D. and Bee, H. (2006) Lifespan Development. Chapter 16, ‘Social and personality development in middle
adulthood’, pp. 431–455.
Stewart, I. and Vaitilingam, R. (2004) Seven Ages of Man and Woman. London: ESRC. Chapter 3, pp. 16–19 (maps
relationships in modern Britain).
190
Sex, love, work and children
There are many websites offering relevant information and advice, such as that of Relate:
www.relateforparents.org.uk/divorse.shtml#3 [sic.].
?
Questions
1 ‘Mother–father–child (one year old).’
This triad, which may exist in varied living arrangements, contains both adult–adult and adult–child
relationships. What are some of the influences (on the experience and development of the three
people) highlighted by attachment theory and by Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model?
In your answer, you may refer either to a single illustrative example or to a variety of possible family
arrangements; you may wish to vary the question title to include same-sex parents.
2 Choosing one of the topics introduced in this chapter (adult sexual relationships, gender, parenting,
work), discuss the application of Bandura’s model of social learning.
191
?
Sex, love, work and children
192
In this chapter you will find:
7 Maturity
and some of
its hazards
• Poverty
• Mental health
• Stigma
?
When the Child Poverty Action Group presented their book, Poverty First Hand: Poor People Speak
for Themselves, they noted that ‘poverty . . . affects young and old, respectable old and disrespectful
young; people conventionally included in the middle class as well as working class’ (Beresford and
CPAG, 1999: 47).
As a social worker, you have to make a ‘social assessment’ of a situation, either as formal
procedure or as a non-formalised basis for deciding how to act. At the core of the
assessment must be your understanding of people’s day-to-day experience and (in
Chapter 5
Bronfenbrenner’s terms, see Chapter 5) the different microsystems which frame their
reality. A large proportion of people who use your service will be in poverty. Users of the service
frequently criticise social workers for not taking sufficient account of this and not realising that their
main problem is to do with finance.
The most basic experience of poverty is going to bed hungry – mothers going without food for
themselves because they have spent on their children, or feeling too worried to eat properly.
Particularly for an older person, it may mean choosing between eating or keeping warm.
For many people, being poor creates a sense of being locked in: ‘You’ve got no money and you can’t
see any way out of it’. It is ‘a lack of choice . . . I enrolled on a college course which had the fees paid
195
Maturity and some of its hazards
for me. I could have continued with the course but I looked at my piece of paper and worked out my
expenses and I couldn’t afford the bus fare in and out, and I couldn’t have afforded food, so I could
not go on that course . . . maybe in the future I could think of going back to college, but not at the
moment’ (Beresford and CPAG, 1999: 61).
Money does not create happiness, but poverty can create unhappiness: ‘It creates disharmony in the
family – because if you’ve got a teenager who can’t get a job and they’ve got no income coming in,
you’ve got to keep them here . . . 16- or 17-year-olds – they can’t even buy clothes. They’ll not be able
to go out anywhere for a night out to the pictures – any money to go with their friends. I think it could
put them in a depression. It can lead to them feeling useless and hopeless’ (Beresford and CPAG, 1999:
81). About half of the participants in the CPAG discussion groups thought that poverty causes
depression. It makes people anxious, frightened and miserable; ‘it causes fear, humiliation, rejection,
stress . . . constantly worrying, twenty-four hours a day, about money and having to manage for the
rest of the week, month, year, whatever.’ Poverty does not ‘cause’ bad treatment of children, but money
makes it much easier to keep level-headed when a child comes home having torn his new pullover or
kicked the toes through a new pair of trainers.
‘Poverty’, says one of the participants in the CPAG work ‘strips your dignity’. How do you go to the
supermarket, she asks, and then go home and tell your children you haven’t bought enough food?
There’s nowt as cruel as kids, is there? I mean, they that get free clothing – you can tell. So you
have to go out and buy a uniform and while they’re wearing that one you have to save up and get
another one because it’s changing.
On benefits I don’t think any allowance is made for brushing your teeth, or buying loo rolls or soap
or the odd bubble bath . . . luxuries for people in benefits are, ‘Do I buy toothpaste this week or
soap? . . . Buy toilet rolls, toothpaste, scouring powder, washing powder, washing up liquid, and
you can double just what you pay for your food bill.
(Quoted in Beresford and CPAG, 1999: 51, 55)
Research shows that adolescents who are poor are more likely than others to be ashamed of them-
selves, or to think they are no good (Iacovou, 2004). Roker’s account (1998) of the experiences of sixty
young people in low incomes identified that one common theme was having to take early and
significant family responsibilities.
196
Maturity and some of its hazards
• There are 2.8 million children in poverty (3.8 million if comparisons are made after housing costs
are deducted). Half of these children live in families where at least one parent is in paid work.
• Among those aged 25 to retirement age in low income, more than half have someone in their
family in paid work.
• A half of all lone parents are in low income, two-and-a-half times the rate for couples with
children.
• But in absolute numbers, the vast majority of people with very low incomes are either working-
age adults without children or couples with children. Relatively few were either lone parents or
pensioners.
• Some 30 per cent of disabled adults live in low-income households – twice the proportion of non-
disabled adults. The New Policy Institute notes that a graduate with a work-limiting disability is
more likely to be lacking but wanting work than an unqualified person with no disability.
There is much academic analysis about different ways of specifying and understanding poverty. Many
people on low incomes say that they cannot afford selected essential items or activities – but so do
quite a lot of people on average incomes. A classic definition of poverty was given by Peter Townsend,
a major campaigner and researcher: people are in poverty when they ‘lack the resources to obtain the
type of diet, participate in the activities and have the living conditions which are customary in the
societies to which they belong’ (Beresford and CPAG, 1999, quoting Townsend 1979: 31). Note two
particular points about this. It is a relative definition of poverty, like the government’s own measure –
it relates to the income and living standards of the society in which people live. This means, for
example, that in periods of prosperity when average incomes rise, poverty is deemed to increase
(because the level at which an income counts as in poverty rises). Second, the definition does not refer
?
just to income and wealth. Living in a damp, unpleasant and dangerous house without proper services
is poverty, as well as income poverty; a child is impoverished by a lack of access to education and a
healthy, safe environment with opportunities to play and explore.
Looking closer
The first sentence of this section states that ‘analysing poverty is inescapably a political issue’.
In the light of the text above, or other arguments, in what sense is this true?
Two relevant areas:
(1) Some politicians, concerned about inequality and how people experience their lives in
relation to others, will accept the relative definitions of poverty, even though this means that
continued
197
Maturity and some of its hazards
in periods of prosperity, measured levels of poverty may rise. Other politicians may be scornful,
arguing that poverty must be about not having food, clothing, or heating.
(2) Political debates will be concerned about causes of poverty, and take different views about
the balance of responsibility of the individual and of social factors (the economy,
the level of benefits) in causing poverty. These different views about cause imply different
political remedies for the problems of poverty and inequality.
Pensioner poverty
In 2006, about 17 per cent of pensioners lived in poverty, a marked decline since 1997. The risk of living
in poverty increases with age, and is greater for people over 75 than for younger pensioners. Women
on their own over pensionable age are more likely to live in poverty than couples. For any specific single
pensioner, the risk of low income does not rise with age, but the proportion of single pensioners rises
with age, and in general single pensioners are more likely to be poor. For a pensioner couple the risk
of low income does rise with age. The lower wealth of older pensioners is partly a ‘cohort’ effect rather
an a pure ‘ageing’ effect – as time has passed, a greater proportion of the population has owned
their own house, so fewer older pensioners than younger pensioners have this investment (Palmer
et al., 2007: 310).
As is the case with so many factors considered in this book, the experience of life is not determined by
poverty or other social circumstances – a full understanding has to include the influence of culture,
individual makeup and individual ‘agency’. But to think that these override the importance of social
factors is to misunderstand human experience.
Effects of poverty
Poverty has both immediate and long-term effects. Childhood poverty is associated with lower educa-
tional achievement and leaving home early. Young women are more likely to have children early. When
they grow up, children from poorer families are more likely to be out of work and to have children with
similar disadvantages to those which they suffered (Stewart and Vaitilingam, 2004: 15; Smallwood and
Wilson, 2007). Pensioner income ‘is highly reflective of financial circumstances earlier in life’.
Poor people die younger, enjoy poorer health and make less use of health services than richer people.
Figure 7.1, for example, shows the rates of infant deaths in different social classes (broadly, income
declines from social class 1, ‘managers and employers in large firms’ to class 8, ‘never worked and long-
term unemployed’). In social classes 4–8 the rates of infant mortality are 5.4 per thousand births. This
is 50 per cent higher than that in social classes 1–3, for whom the rate is 3.8 (Palmer et al., 2007: 111).
198
Maturity and some of its hazards
Although falling, infant deaths are 50% more common among those from
manual backgrounds than among those from non-manual backgrounds
8
Social classes 1–4 Social classes 5–8
7
Infant deaths per 1,000 live births
0
1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 ?
2003 2004 2005
Figure 7.1
Infant mortality and social class
Looking closer
Go to page 113 of the printed report Monitoring Poverty and Social Exclusion 2007 (Palmer
et al., 2007) or find section 42 online (www.jrf.org.uk/sites/files/jrf/2152-poverty-social-
exclusion.pdf). Who is most at risk of premature death?
Go to The Poverty Site (www.poverty.org.uk/summary/uk.htm). Different health indicators
linked to poverty are very clearly set out by age. Examine one indicator from each age group.
Violence
Violence is a significant feature in the experience of many people who use social work services. This
may be because they have had to live with violence or because they themselves are violent and
dangerous. It may be because they have been involved in war or communal violence, in which one
section of a population seeks to harm or exterminate another.
199
Maturity and some of its hazards
Hostility, harshness and violence from a mother, father or stepfather to a child is violence in an intimate
relationship. It is a form of ‘domestic violence’, a term often used (as it is in UK national statistics) to
mean violence between adult partners. The many forms of destructiveness in close relationships are
interlinked and while different terms have their merits – ‘domestic violence’, ‘violence against women’,
‘emotional abuse’, ‘child abuse’, ‘sexual or physical abuse’ – they can sometimes be misleading by
identifying a situation by one component only, or by focusing the hearer’s attention in a selective way.
Words used to characterise the people concerned can create similar problems – ‘perpetrator’, ‘victim’,
?
‘abused child’ can partialise a person’s qualities and experience and homogenise them with other people
who are very different. Terms such as ‘marital disharmony’ or ‘over-punishment’ can carry as many
unhelpful as helpful implications. Chapter 6 referred to statistics about the extent of domestic violence.
Looking closer
Definitions of ‘abuse’ are used for different purposes:
• for research;
• for criminal investigations and convictions;
200
Maturity and some of its hazards
The different purposes can produce different statistics about the prevalence of abuse. Why
might definitions used for research and for criminal investigations be different, and why would
they produce different indications of the prevalence of abuse?
Response: Criminal investigations are based on the exact wording and purpose of the law.
Statistics require victims to have come forward to the police, but the threats of further abuse
and dependence of the victim on the abuser may make this impossible. Research, on the other
hand will use the definitions appropriate for its study – it may be seeking to establish if there
is abuse which is not defined as a crime by the law; it may be using anonymous self-reports by
victims or abusers to investigate the extent of abuse which is not reported. For these and other
reasons it will produce different results from crime figures.
Violence committed by a man towards his partner’s children is likely to be intrinsically damaging to
her. His violence hurts her emotionally, and has the effect of putting her in an impossible psychological
position, in which her self-respect as a mother and protector of her children is compromised.
?
Reflective thinking
• What might be the impact on a woman whose partner beats her child?
• Why might she stay with him?
• What is the impact on a child when a mother is abusive to the father?
From a child’s point of view, violence towards their mother is intrinsically hurtful. As child protection
guidance (DCFS, 2006), points out, it should also alert workers to the possibility that there may be direct
violence towards the child. Attempts by a child to intervene to protect the mother increase its risk of
injury (Rogers and Pilgrim, 2003: 146). Whether or not they live together, a woman’s relational violence
(see page 178) towards the father of her child will also be a painful experience. It either presents
conflicts the child will struggle with, or it destroys the relationship with his father that both are entitled
to have.
As mentioned in Chapter 6, Renzetti (1998: 199), like Rogers and Pilgrim (2003: 145), reports a number
of studies showing equal or higher rates of intimate violence in same-sex relationships, but emphasises
201
?
Maturity and some of its hazards
the difficulties in establishing how representative are the samples used. Morrow and
Messinger (2006) highlight some of the particular pressures involved in intimate gay and
lesbian violence. Social and family support may be more restricted, the abuse may involve
Chapter 6
?
threats of ‘outing’ the person to family or work, and fear of misunderstanding may make
it harder to approach social agencies or the police. In these circumstances, as in those involving
violence by women towards men, social attitudes may be felt by the victim to add to their difficulties
in seeking help
Older people
The issue of violence towards older people by their caregivers has received more attention in the last
couple of decades. In family care, the most common location of the problem, five types of risk factors
have been examined by research (Pillemer and Suitor, 1998, who cite a variety of further studies).
Caregiving demands were hypothesised to increase resentment as a relative becomes increasingly
dependent and increasingly disruptive of the caregiver’s life. It was thought that this resentment might
escalate to hostility and violence. The evidence on the whole has not supported this view – for example,
there seems to be little difference in the degree of dependency and impairment between abused and
non-abused older people. Situational stressors are problems in immediate family interaction such as
aggressive behaviour by the person being cared for. Certain caregiver characteristics – such as low self-
esteem, alcohol problems and younger age – have been associated with other forms of intimate
202
Maturity and some of its hazards
violence. Family dynamics are relevant, as it has been suggested that aggression in the later stages of
life may simply be a continuation or development of earlier relational patterns between spouses or
between parents and children. In terms of caregiving context, social isolation and spousal relationship
?
(rather than child–parent) are more commonly associated with abuse than other contexts.
In any population, the ‘cohort effect’ of the experience of war is strong. Some 870,000 men from Britain
died in the First World War, and in the 1921 census there was a surplus of 1.7 million women over men.
In 1945, as the result of two bombs alone which brought the war to an end, Pilgrim and Rogers quote
that over a third of a million people died in Japan, some of which were lingering deaths. In Northern
Ireland’s sectarian conflicts, or in Uganda’s ethnic cleansing, generations of children have grown up
with violent death, barbaric retribution, and hatred between communities as part of the public
landscape. This picture seems remarkably widespread in human society. The more far-reaching effects
of public violence last long after the immediate physical damage.
When traumatising violence ends, one possible self-preserving human response is to lock the expe-
rience away, and avoid recollections which bring back the unmanageable memories. This can apply
both to children who were abused and to soldiers in battle situations. Years later, in order to feel
understood the person may wish to raise this part of their life without going into detail, or may need
to talk much more specifically, sometimes graphically. This can arise particularly when these
experiences are reawakened by later, deeply emotional events (such as the illness of a child or the death
of a partner).
In these circumstances, social workers need the skill to be able to listen without prying, and to respond
therapeutically at the level needed by the individual. This response will sometimes be a dispassionate
but concerned and attentive listening. At other times it may involve an overtly emotional interaction.
The response may be appropriately contained within a single meeting, or may require further contact
in order to be handled in a developmentally helpful way. It may combine practical help, social inter-
vention and emotional support. It is similar to other activities in which the social worker explicitly
offers a relationship to attend to the after-effects of earlier trauma (Howe, 2008; Sudbery, 2002). This
is sometimes described as using ‘counselling skills’ but is more appropriately understood as ‘therapeutic
social work’, a psychosocial intervention with a long tradition.
203
Maturity and some of its hazards
?
Reflective thinking
This section has focused on the destructive effect of violence. A female university graduate, now
a professional ‘cage fighter’ said reflectively in a radio interview that she thinks there is an intrinsic
human pleasure in inflicting violence. This may be a very frightening, disturbing thought for you.
Do you agree? What are some of the sources of human violence and destructiveness as you
understand them?
Mental health
To understand development is to understand the unfolding sequence of a person’s life – of biological
expression, of cognitions, emotions and other components of states of mind and of external social
experience. Problems in adult mental health require a range of factors to be taken into account.
• Social construction: societies and groups within society interpret the issues in different ways.
• Genetics: there is good evidence that one factor in a number of mental health problems is the
unfolding of genetic makeup.
• Other biological factors may be at work which are not known to have a specific genetic origin. For
example, Alzheimer’s disease is a form of dementia found mainly in older people and is caused by
problems in ‘nerve’ (‘neural’) pathways in the brain, resulting in the death of brain cells. Epilepsy
similarly has a biological cause, and illustrates how conditions may be described at different times
as the province of psychiatry or of neurology, both disciplines which are concerned with the
functioning of the brain (see Corr, 2006: chapters 14–16).
• Often, as is thought to be the case with schizophrenia, a problem in mental health arises when a
raised genetic risk combines with predisposing environmental experience in earlier life (adversity
such as poverty or abuse) and a present-day precipitating factor (such as domestic violence or
other social stress). ?
• Childhood suffering and the experience of neglect, abuse or other adversity increase the likelihood
of some kinds of mental distress in adulthood.
• Other aspects of early experience: behaviourists (Chapter 3 and ‘Essential background’,
section 8) account for some forms of disorder by early conditioning – the forms
of behaviour and thought patterns that have been reinforced; attachment theorists Chapters
refer aspects of adult experience back to aspects of early attachment relationships or 3, EB8, 2,
EB2, EB4
their absence (Chapter 2 and ‘Essential background’, section 2); and psychoanalytic
204
Maturity and some of its hazards
Psychiatry is the medical discipline concerned with the origins and treatment of emotional disorder
and ‘abnormal states of mind’. A distinction you will come across, though it is somewhat dated, is that
between neuroses and psychoses.
The distinction is not clear-cut and professionals from different disciplines (psychiatry, sociology,
psychology, social workers and philosophers) have very different understanding about what is
being described when psychiatrists make a diagnosis. However, in a rough and ready way neuroses
may be thought of as exaggerated versions of the universal emotions described above – anxiety
which persistently interferes with normal day-to-day life; depression that doesn’t lift (interfering
with sleep, appetite and sex drive); or the excessive, repetitive need to perform rituals and check,
clean, or wash in order to keep anxiety at bay (Horrobin, 2002: 157; Corr, 2006: 405; Rogers and
Pilgrim, 2003). We can all understand these as an extreme versions of familiar feelings. Psychoses,
on the other hand, using the same broad definition, involve experiences that do not seem continuous
with everyday worries. Schizophrenia is an example of psychosis. Its active phase may include
delusions, hallucinations, speech and thought disorder, and feelings of persecution (American
Psychiatric Association, 1994).
This distinction does not minimise the suffering experienced in neurosis, which may disable people in
their day-to-day life. ‘The onset of my neurosis was marked by levels of physical anxiety that I would
not have thought possible,’ wrote a professor of psychology. ‘If one is involved in a road accident, there
is a delay of a second or two and then the pit of the stomach seems to fall out and one’s legs go
like jelly. It was this feeling multiplied a hundredfold that seized me all hours of the day and night’
(Sutherland, 1998: 2, quoted in Corr, 2006: 413).
205
Maturity and some of its hazards
The Schizophrenia Bulletin is a research journal that sometimes carries personal accounts. Here David
Zelt, a regular scientific contributor to the journal, gives his own account of a psychotic episode,
written in the third person to convey a sense of psychological distance:
A drama that profoundly transformed David Zelt began at a conference on human psychology.
David respected the speakers as scholars and wanted their approval of a paper he had written
about telepathy . . . David knew that the paper, in reflecting engagement with an esoteric subject,
was a signpost of his growing retreat from mundane reality.
David’s paper was viewed as a monumental contribution to the conference and potentially to
psychology in general. . . . his concept of telepathy might have as much influence as the basic
ideas of Darwin and Freud.
Each speaker focused on David. By using allusions and non-verbal communications that included
pointing and glancing, each illuminated different aspects of David’s contribution. Although his
name was never mentioned, the speakers enticed David into feeling that he had accomplished
something supernatural with his paper. . . .
David was described as having a halo around his head, and the second coming was announced as
forthcoming. Messianic feelings took hold of him. His mission would be to aid the poor and needy,
especially in underdeveloped countries. . . .
Several hundred people at the conference were talking about David. He was the subject of enor-
mous mystery, profound in his silence . . . [Over the next few weeks] it dawned on David that the
CIA was listening to most of his thoughts wherever he went, even sometimes during sleep
. . . Because his thoughts were broadcast around him, David often felt that his consciousness was
controlled from outside himself.
(1981, quoted in Horrobin, 2002: 138)
Sometimes, mental health problems involve one aspect which is diagnosed by psychiatrists as a
medical condition, and another which is not. Some people who are diagnosed with neurotic or
psychotic mental illness, for example, are also misusing alcohol or drugs. Studies quoted by Rogers and
Pilgrim (2003: 151–155) show that these people are more likely than others to be involved in violence
after discharge from mental health services. These situations are commonly referred to as ‘dual
diagnosis’. Mental health services may also be responsible for services to people who misuse drugs but
have no mental illness. The services are also concerned (particularly in relation to control rather than
treatment) with people who are deemed to have mental disorders such as antisocial personality
disorder (psychopathic personality) which are not regarded by psychiatrists as treatable illnesses.
The term ‘dual diagnosis’ is also used in relation to people with learning disabilities who have mental
health problems. A learning disability does not, in itself cause mental distress or personality disorder,
Nor does it ‘get better’; it is not an illness. People with learning disabilities have limited ability to think,
206
Maturity and some of its hazards
and take longer to learn than other people. As discussed later in the chapter, the term ‘learning
difficulty’ is currently the term preferred by the people concerned. ‘Learning difficulty’ is also, however,
the term used by people who have no intellectual impairment but have specific difficulties in learning
(such as dyslexia). The ‘Taking it further’ section of this chapter analyses how many of the difficulties
experienced by people with learning disabilities are the product of social arrangements. The
organisation Mind (undated) quotes statistics that between a quarter and a half of people with learning
disabilities have mental health needs.
There is a paradox about the sociology of mental illness compared with the sociology of physical illness.
There has been extensive examination of the question whether mental health problems reflect social
processes or medical conditions; nevertheless, the understanding of how social class and social causes
relate to physical illness is in fact much more advanced (Rogers and Pilgrim, 2003: 24). Research does
not fully resolve the resulting debates in relation to mental health, but has led to an ever-increasing
body of findings and analysis allowing for complex and competing interpretations. At its simplest, the
statistical association of mental illness with social adversity – with poverty and social exclusion, for
example – can be described as having three explanations.
The first explanation is that mental ill-health may be caused by poverty, stress, poor environment and
housing. One important implication, if this is correct, is that ameliorating the stressful conditions, or
preventing the occurrence of stress in childhood, will lower the incidence of mental health problems
in the population.
The second, almost opposite, explanation is that people with mental health problems are more likely
to be found in poor and stressful environments because that is where their difficulties cause them to
live. If this is so, there may be other reasons to improve such conditions, but it will not necessarily
reduce the occurrence of mental illness.
And third, there may be an independent factor that is associated both with areas of poverty and
deprivation and with mental health. Contentiously, genes have been suggested, or cultural behaviours
alleged to be associated with communities in poverty (such as poor antenatal care, or rigid emotional
behaviour and outlook).
A moment’s thought will tell you that these explanations can be associated with particular political
ideas – the first is emphasised by those concerned about the adverse effects of exclusion and depri-
vation, and by people who emphasise how life is shaped by social forces beyond the control of the
207
Maturity and some of its hazards
individual. Those who emphasise that people’s position in society is related to their individual qualities
and behaviours will be drawn to the second and third, as will those right-wing political philosophies
which believe that some people and groups are superior to others.
Another aspect of social debate concerns the rates of ‘service’ among different groups of people.
Higher rates of diagnosis and treatment are not always interpreted as signs of better health provision
and social inclusion. This is because, unlike in most areas of physical medicine, there is what Rogers
and Pilgrim describe as a ‘treatment-coercion gradient’ in mental health. High rates of attention to
postnatal depression in prosperous suburbs may imply good attention to the well-being of new
mothers, but high rates of schizophrenia diagnoses in an inner-city black population may imply greater
levels of state control and coercion, because schizophrenia is associated with compulsory removal of
freedom and enforced medication. One concern of those who contest the medical model of psychiatry
is that different rates of diagnosis between different social groups (men and women, or African
Caribbean and white European men) reflect differences in how social behaviour is interpreted rather
than differences in mental disturbance.
?
Many people diagnosed with anxiety or depression find that these problems spontaneously lessen
(Andrews, 2001). In a medical model, this does not necessarily mean that the problem has been
‘cured’ – Andrews argues that it should potentially be assessed as ‘in remission’. Neither does it mean
that there is no need for assistance during the episode.
The largest proportion of people diagnosed with a mental disorder will receive service
from ‘primary health care’ such as their family doctor and the related provision (Rippon,
2004). They may receive a prescribed course of drugs or services such as counselling or
Chapter 3
CBT – cognitive behavioural therapy (see Chapter 3). They will continue living at home,
and experience only small disruption to their lifestyle.
Those seen by the specialist mental health services (initially by a psychiatrist) are more likely to
experience marked changes in their social and life circumstances. A significant number will take part
in self-help groups or use activities and services provided by voluntary organisations (such as MIND
or facilities for specific ethnic groups) – sometimes, since these are user-led, becoming a service
provider as well as a service user. Staff from the mainstream services will value and support these
opportunities, but voluntary organisations are frequently critical of the services provided by national
and local government. Bamber (2004) discusses the range of service-user organisations, their
relationship with state psychiatric services and their role in raising debates about the nature and
treatment of mental health problems.
After being diagnosed by a psychiatrist, people should find that the facilities to be made available
to them are organised under the Care Programme Approach (CPA – see Department of Health,
2008, especially section 4; Agnew, 2004). This sets out where they are likely to be seen and by
208
Maturity and some of its hazards
whom; which hospital inpatient or outpatient or community-based services are offered and who will
be involved.
For people with severe conditions, an episode in hospital may at first be frightening. Sometimes it
becomes a place they can trust, or they may continue to be alarmed at the variety of people they meet,
the lack of control, the unpredictability of other people’s behaviour and the difficulties of sharing
accommodation with other patients. Bamber (2004: 184) points out that many people in the service-
user movement prefer the term ‘survivor’. This is not only because they regard themselves as survivors
of acute mental distress, but also ‘because it acknowledges the ordeal they have experienced with
services’.
For many, maintaining a positive life in the community (that is, not in hospital) depends on taking
regular medication, and having this checked at regular intervals. Patients are likely to have mixed
feelings about their medication. Although medication enables them to ‘get by’, they often dislike its
side effects or the sense that their feelings, their ‘self’, is controlled by drugs. Some, particularly in the
service-user movements, may regard it as part of the inappropriate pathologising and medicalisation
of life problems.
For some, their life may feel (and be regarded by others) like a deterioration, a part of destruction and
decline. But many others will feel that, although unwished for, their difficulties have enriched their
understanding of life and of themselves. They can take pride in overcoming something that many other
people do not have to:
my perception of myself and the world around me has almost completely reversed. I have
abilities now that as far as I was concerned those years ago did not exist. I have uncovered
creative thinking and abilities that I did not have or were buried . . . I can say, and do, that Manic
Depression is not an illness, on the contrary it was for me a fundamental part of my growth
process.
(Myerscough 1981: 134, as quoted in Allott, 2004)
At the opposite end of the spectrum to these comments about the enriching effects of mental illness
on life, for 5,500 people aged 15 and over in the UK (seventeen men and five women in every 100,000
of each sex; between the ages of 20 and 24 the ratio is even wider, at more than 4 to1), the mental
distress ends in suicide. The highest cause of death for young men is suicide. This is a changing picture,
as in the early 1990s the biggest risk of suicide was for men aged 75 and over. The figures quoted are
for 2006 and are part of a consistently falling statistic – they represent a drop of over 7 per cent since
1995. The government’s target is to reduce suicides by 20 per cent between 2002 and 2010. The risk
for young Asian men appears to be less than that for white males, but the suicide rates among young
Asian women is a major cause for concern, being twice the national average, and possibly three times
that of equivalent white women. The majority of people with diagnosed mental health problems who
commit suicide have recently contacted a professional person, and a negative response from that
209
Maturity and some of its hazards
person seems to be a precipitating factor (statistics and policy information from sources: Hatloy and
Stewart, 2007; ONS, 2009).
In this area, as in all others, social workers deal not just with the individual experience of mental
distress, but with its causes and effects in the social network. These require a perspective of what has
happened before the social worker’s involvement and sensitivity to the need for continuing tactful
attention afterwards. In an appeal leaflet, the Mental Health Foundation quotes the words of Jamie,
aged 12 (undated):
I was nine when it happened. I’d been at a sleepover and I still remember the terrible atmosphere
in the house when I walked in. My mum told me dad had taken a lot of tablets in the night to try
and kill himself.
Some mental health problems are caused by brain deterioration and at the moment are irreversible.
Alzheimer’s disease and other dementia may involve a progressive loss of self in older people – as
painful for those around them as for the person themselves. This is a fictionalised version of the words
of a woman who spoke to me about an incident involving her husband:
We had our ups and downs like any couple, but I had shared decades of my life with him, and
we loved each other. As his dementia developed, the man I had loved was taken away from me
in a confusing and disturbing way. When it became too much, I gave up my work as a social
worker to care for him. After he went into [a home], I put myself out to make sure I visited him
constantly, with all that entailed. Then one time the staff told me (I think they thought it was
amusing) that he had taken a fancy to another resident, Clare. I had given up my life for him and
I saw him fawning over her, being sweet and charming and flirtatious, and he didn’t even
?
acknowledge me. I felt murderous hatred, that he should take me so for granted, show me no
feeling, and offer his affections to another woman in front of me after all I did for him and all we
had together.
Equally, however, many problems in cognitive functioning and mood in later life (such as
depression, which affects one in five older people (Mental Health Foundation, 2007) are
responsive either to drug treatment or nutrition, to psychological techniques such as
Chapter 8
memory training, or to attention to social problems. This is discussed more in Chapter 8.
In the survey by the Office of National Statistics, one in six adults were found to suffer from a diag-
nosable mental disorder, and a lifetime prevalence is commonly quoted of one-quarter to one-third of
people being diagnosed with a psychiatric disorder (Persaud and Royal College of Psychiatrists 2007;
Singleton et al., 2001). Mental health statistics contain many ambiguities as they attempt to reduce
problems in social life to numbers, but you are certain to encounter the issues for individuals and
families in your work. There are constant developments in the field, and areas about which you will
discover more detail in your specialist studies and continuing practice are:
210
Maturity and some of its hazards
• Psychiatric perspectives, the medical approach to diagnosis and treatment (in more detail
appropriate to your work or level of study).
• More details about the debates relating to mental health – the opposing accounts of what mental
illness is, differing views on their causation, including the social origins of mental distress, and the
place of social inequality (Wilson et al., 2008: 576–586).
• The role of the social worker in providing an emotionally therapeutic and developmental experience
(Firth et al., 2004; Howe, 2008; Sudbery, 2002; K. Wilson et al., 2008: 318–322, 575–577) .
• Policy development as it affects social workers.
• Assessment and other practice procedures in mental health – attending to needs, rights and
risks.
When someone reveals that they are receiving psychiatric treatment or ‘care in the community’, they
have to deal with many prejudgements and false expectations. In some contexts it will raise questions
about whether they are ‘normal’, whether they are safe to be near children, or whether they are violent.
One way of describing this is to say their social identity has been ‘spoilt’. To have a ‘spoilt identity’ has
many consequences. It exposes the individual to prejudice (essentially the same word as ‘prejudge-
ment’) and it may mean the individual has to deal with fear, discrimination in practical and financial
matters, and physical attack. The next section of this chapter is concerned with ‘spoilt identity’.
211
Maturity and some of its hazards
When a newly qualified person says, ‘I’m still coming to terms with my identity as a social worker’, both
aspects are in play. The two aspects interact; people can place themselves deliberately into a particular
social identity, as when a gay man decides to be out to his family, friends and colleagues, or a social
worker from an Asian family can decide before going to court whether to wear a business suit and
accessories which will blend in, ‘pass’ with ‘white’ colleagues, or to wear a sari, hairstyle and make-up
which will identify her socially as ‘Asian’. Equally, the ‘psychological’ aspect of identity is heavily
influenced by the social attributions from others. Cooley (1902) coined the phrase ‘looking-glass self’
to describe the way in which the sense of self is created out of the ‘reflections’ received from others.
Bilton quotes G. H. Mead’s concept of the ‘I’ and the ‘me’ to represent respectively the impulse to act
socially and the social identity which is formed through social action. Individuals differ in the degree
to which they are influenced by others’ attributions, and there are profound disagreements between
theorists about the relationship between the social and the individual (see Bilton, 2002: 501–6,
528–31).
Among those who emphasise the social creation of the self are ‘role theorists’ (Biddle, 1979; Goffman,
1959/2008) who point out that a person’s identity is always structured by their relations to those
around them – parents, siblings, partners, employers, social ‘location’. A ‘role’ is defined as a ‘set of
mutual expectations’, and ‘role conflict’ occurs when the expectations of different roles are incom-
patible. For example, it was suggested in the last chapter that the roles of parent, partner and employee
are likely to contain more contradictory expectations for women than for men. The usefulness of role
theory is illustrated by the way it describes the effect on men of becoming unemployed (they lose a
major role in their life) or of a woman becoming a mother (she gains a new role and a new identity).
The criticisms come from those (such as Raffel, 1999) who argue that some of the major role theorists
leave no room for the ‘self’ at all; it as if they see the person as solely created by the social roles they
enact. ?
‘Postmodernists’ also emphasise the social creation of the ‘self’. They regard ‘personal identity’ as much
more flexible and fluid than previous theorists acknowledged: ‘There is no self behind the mask’; the
individual is the sum of their performances. Earlier generations might have looked upon
a stable and consistent sense of self as a sign of maturity, with qualities similar to those
listed by Maslow as the ‘fully achieving person’ (see Chapter 5). A person with a changing
Chapter 5 identity might perhaps have been seen as ‘still trying to find himself’, but this strand of
‘postmodern’ thinking sees personal and social identity as always open to transformation
and personal redefinition – there is no such thing as a core ‘self’ and a consistent sense of identity is
largely an illusion. One positive effect of this view is to undermine the rigidity of certain identities that
are couched in terms of binary opposites – hetero-/homosexual, sane/mad, indigenous/foreign – which
have always been troublesome for those who do not fit the social categories.
212
Maturity and some of its hazards
Prejudice – examples
Some people have a harder task than others dealing with social expectations directed at them. There
are numerous ‘out-groups’ who are subject to the threat of a negative social identity. The discrim-
ination they face may be subtle or hidden in some areas and open, even directly dangerous, in others.
The following paragraphs refer to some of the issues in relation to ethnicity, sexual orientation and
learning disabilities.
Tajfel (with Turner, 1982) argues that people form identities by means of the membership of a social
group. They exaggerate differences with those in the ‘out-group’. They gain positive self-concept by
valuing the in-group and devaluing the outgroup (forming hostile expectations of them). Ward and
colleagues (2001) argue that in relation to ethnicity, this process takes place within both the majority
and minority group. However, the impact bears more heavily on the out-group since members will be
constantly negotiating with the power of the majority. Someone in an ethnic minority may face
stereotypes and discrimination throughout their life.
Phinney’s research about the experiences of African American and Native American teenagers (Phinney
and Rosenthal, 1992) led her to suggest a model of stages through which people form an identity as
a member of a minority ethnic grouping. The first stage is an unexamined ethnic identity, during which
they may unquestioningly take views which are explicit or implicit in surrounding culture – that black
men are sporty, UK politicians are white, and so on. To the degree that these are negative they may not
realise they are part of the negative group, or there may be disconnects between different aspects.
Issues come more into conscious focus with the greater cognitive ability in adolescence: ‘the young
African American may learn as a child that black is beautiful, but conclude as an adolescent that white
is powerful’ (Spencer and Dornbusch, 1990: 131, as quoted by Boyd and Bee, 2006: 325). The second
stage is of ethnic identity search, often triggered by some experience which makes thought about
perceived ethnicity unavoidable – perhaps an experience of abuse or discrimination. Finally, Phinney
identified that men and women in minority ethnic groups achieve secure ethnic identity. For some, this
is an ability to live in a bicultural way, which her research found to characterise high achievers with
high self-esteem and good interpersonal relations. For others it may be a strong single cultural
identification, even if this means some practical losses because the person excludes themselves from
the dominant culture and proclaims an identity which attracts discrimination and hostility.
‘Sexual identity’ and ‘sexual orientation’ are conceptualised in many different ways. No doubt both gay
?
and straight people are constantly adjusting their understanding. Morrow and Messinger (2006; 4), in
a volume intended for social workers, state that ‘sexual orientation’ is a preferred term to ‘sexual
preference’ because the latter implies too much of a willed choice in contrast to what is experienced
as the ‘innate essence of their affectional nature’. They describe that the choice available
is whether to embrace or reject their essential orientation. As discussed in Chapter 6, Lisa
Diamond’s ten-year longitudinal study (2008) led her to conclude that ‘sexual orientation’
does not feel ‘chosen’ by women, does not feel like a decision. But she also concluded that,
Chapter 6
in contrast to typical male experience, it does not determine specific sexual desire,
213
Maturity and some of its hazards
affectional impulses and sexual behaviour, which arise out of strongly situational and interpersonal
factors rather than gender categories. In a social world of ‘compulsory heterosexuality’, in a context
‘of cultural denial, distorted stereotypes, rejection, neglect, harassment, and sometimes outright
victimisation and abuse’ which complicates attachment relationships (Tharinger and Wells 2000: 159,
quoted in Morrow and Messinger, 2000), different people face different challenges in establishing a
sexual identity.
In forming an identity as a gay man or lesbian, a person confronts some people’s expectations of
‘normality’. What is experienced differs between individuals, between men and women, and between
decades and cultures. There are issues at the interpersonal level, issues to do with cultural views which
colour anonymous interactions and dealings with organisations, there are practical issues of finance
and personal safety, and there are issues which have mainly to be addressed at the political level.
Examples of the first include managing conversations with friends and family members. The second
includes dealing with false expectations, ignorance or prejudice in work encounters, or dealing with
other organisations such as health services. Practical issues include gaining adequate rights as ‘next
of kin’ to a partner or pensions, or issues of personal safety from attack. And all people who have a
classification which gives them a ‘spoiled identity’ are ultimately involved in problems which require
a political remedy, ensuring rights, protection and equality.
As you might expect, researchers have proposed stage models of how a gay, lesbian, bisexual or
transgender identity develops. Some regard the word ‘phase’ as more appropriate because it does not
suggest some universal sequence. In discussing the developmental pathways in becoming a lesbian
parent, Morningstar (1999) refers to ‘components’ of a process rather than ‘stages’. Morrow and
Messinger (2006: 86, 96) summarise a total of ten models in diagrammatic form. Morrow concentrates
particularly on Cass’ six-stage model which incorporates pressures from both outside society and
internal dynamics. She portrays a movement of identity change (starting with ‘identity confusion’)
in which a previously held view of sexual identity based on surrounding social messages about the
universal self has to be replaced by a clear homosexual identity. The model is based on the idea that
development occurs when the self-experience is made to be congruent with the environment. In this
view a homosexual identity has to involve ‘the presentation of a homosexual self-image to both
homosexual and heterosexual others’ (Morrow and Messinger, 2006: 24 quoting Cass, 1984). For Healy
(1999), the issue is less clearcut – she takes it as given that every day there are conscious or
unconscious decisions about concealment or self-disclosure.
Morrow and Messinger propose guidelines for good practice throughout their book, including work
related to the ongoing process of identity formation throughout the life course, and about work with
older gay and lesbian people. In relation to adults they list risk factors such as emotional distress,
isolation, depression, violence, suicide and family conflict (several studies found that one of the most
distressing experience of gay and lesbian identity formation is loss of parental affection and support).
Protective factors include positive and supportive family relationships, strong self-esteem, stable intel-
lectual functioning, special talent (for example, musical or athletic) and supportive friendships – in
adolescents, supportive school relationships.
214
Maturity and some of its hazards
Spoiled identity
Erving Goffman (1959/2008) studied how people’s identity can be defined by those with power over
them, and how the individual’s actions are then interpreted through this definition of their identity.
He paid particular attention to people in prison and mental hospitals, noting how the inmates acquired
a despised and low status identity in society. He used the phrase ‘spoilt identity’ to describe this. This
research tradition did not look at these aspects of life from the point of view of those in power who
ascribe the identity (the psychiatrist diagnosing the ‘mental illness’ or the court convicting a ‘criminal’)
but from the point of view of those who find themselves on the receiving end of the judgements.
To a degree, as Goffman points out (1963/1990: 152), social interaction always involves managing
aspects of spoilt identity. Everyone has to form an identity in the face of social disapproval from
significant cultural groups about some of their qualities and attributes. This section has singled out a
few examples for comment, because it is in the nature of social work that most users of service
experience these issues intensively – as children in care, parents of children in care, adults with learning
disability, older people, and so on.
Goffman described several ways in which people cope with the dangers that come with being allocated
a ‘spoilt identity’ in society. Some conceal the qualities which give rise to it. For example, during racist
segregation in the USA, a large number adults defined by the state as ‘negro’ achieved entry to legal
and other professions by ‘passing’ as white people, often claiming Mediterranean ancestry; gay and
lesbian young people may find it safer to pass as ‘straight’. Others respond with defiance and challenge,
and others with irony, as in the self-attribution of ‘nigger’ and ‘queer’. The chosen name of a cam-
paigning mental health group, ‘Mad Women’ (Ryan and Pritchard, 2004: 198), combines irony with
defiance. Tajfel and Turner (1982) reported a similar range of strategies individuals use to combat
threats to their identity, but also included ‘leaving the group’. They highlight the importance of group
strategies, where a group changes its social identity. These can be positive, as when it challenges the
negative valuation and asserts its members’ pride in themselves. On the other hand, they can have
further negative effects – as when the ‘out-group’ chooses a different, more disadvantaged group for
comparison, and gains positive valuation by disparaging this more vulnerable group. An example
occurs when people with milder learning disability gain status in an institution by devaluing those with
severe disability. Goffman highlights that a secure reference group is important in supporting identity
formation in the face of cultural condemnation.
Within each of the examples there are wide differences in social experiences depending on age, gender,
social context and generation – in most cases, there is no single pattern that applies across individuals,
or to both sexes, or in different decades, even though in an intellectual way we can abstract some
common features.
The next chapter looks at the experience of older people. One distinctive feature of the management
of spoilt identity in relation to older people is that the individual is entering an identity towards which
they themselves have been prejudiced in the past. The chapter will refer to research which shows that
215
Maturity and some of its hazards
this complicates the process of positive identity formation and has negative physical and psychological
implications.
In summary, for the purposes of understanding development through the life course, we can identify
four areas in which a social worker has to understand the implications of stigma and spoiled identity:
• identity development and identity change through the life course – a perspective on the evolution
and modification of a person’s social and psychological identity;
• the management of that identity in personal interactions – the constant pressures of what
Goffman describes as ‘information management’ and ‘image management’;
• the practical implications for finance, safety and freedom;
• the role of social and political action which is necessary to change the social value assigned to the
group.
The anti-oppressive value base of social work makes it a responsibility not only to understand these
issues, but also to be active in undermining the stigma and prejudice with which they are associated.
?
Reflective thinking
Consider yourself or someone to whom you provide service in order to discuss:
• In what ways has it been hard to form a positive social identity because of negative
attitudes, misinformation or negative views spread by opinion formers?
• In what ways is this ‘same’ prejudice different for people brought up in a different
decade or in a different culture/nationality?
• In what ways does this person have privilege compared with others? (What stigma do
they not have to cope with, and are there any ways in which they contribute to the
stigma of others?)
It is relevant to the subject itself, as well as being necessary for personal comfort, to note
that some aspects of what is relevant to this discussion have to remain private.
216
Maturity and some of its hazards
The final section of the chapter moves on from issues about personal identity and self-description to
a broader look at the social influences on the development of people with learning disabilities. The
subject illustrates how, in anti-discriminatory practice, the personal, the organisational and the political
are closely connected.
?
Lead-up questions
• What do you understand by the term ‘learning disability’?
• What words have you heard used to describe people with a learning disability?
• What is the source of those words as you understand it?
Use the government website Valuing People and/or other resources to view the current useage of
‘learning disability’.
217
Maturity and some of its hazards
? IT FURTHER
TAKING
Some social influences on the development of people with learning disabilities
Introduction
Learning disability refers to an impaired social ability linked to low intellectual ability. Since it
refers not just to ‘low IQ’, it is not surprising that the life of a person with learning disability is
profoundly affected by social factors. This section of the chapter reviews the impact of some of
these factors.
The experience and development of people with learning disability vary considerably depending
on social attitudes and arrangements about the label which is applied to them. In every area of
social care, staff will meet and have responsibilities for people with learning disability. What fol-
lows considers dimensions of development such as health, education, presence in the community
and self-determination.
Terminology
A discussion earlier in this chapter referred to the challenges to identity faced by people to whom
this description or label is applied. People First prefers the term ‘learning difficulties’, which
describes what its members experience in comparison with other people, and emphasises their
motivation to learn. Service organisations in the UK usually refer to ‘learning disability’. The World
Health Organisation recommends the use of the term ‘intellectual impairment’. The term often used
in the USA is ‘mental retardation’. This is regarded as a pejorative label in the UK, and the situation
is further complicated because American usage specifically separates this from ‘learning disability’,
which is used to mean a specific disability (such as dyslexia), usually manifest in people of other-
wise average intellectual ability or above.
Background
There is no accurate statement of the numbers of people described as having learning disability, and
this statistic would vary according to the definition. Emerson and Hatton (2008) estimated that about
?
3 per cent of children and 2 per cent of adults in the UK are described as having a learning disability
– they based their work on data from local councils, the Department of Health, the Department of
Education and the census. This indicates about 1.5 million people, of whom 1.2 million are described
as having mild disability and 210,000 as having severe or profound disability. About 5 per cent of
people with learning disability have Down’s syndrome, which is caused by a single genetic abnor-
mality (an extra chromosome 21 – see Corr, 2006: 57 and ‘Essential background’, section
1). A specific physiological cause is known for a further small proportion who suffered
brain trauma during birth, and there are a large number of very specific syndromes
Chapter which affect extremely small numbers of people. Race (2007), however, points out that
EB1 in total, physiological explanations are known only for a small minority of people.
The experience and development of people with learning disabilities in the UK is shaped by class,
income, ethnic community and urban or other geographic differences; it is obviously affected by
218
Maturity and some of its hazards
living in institutions. Michael (2008: 14) reports that severe and profound learning disabilities
occur independently of class and income, while moderate learning difficulties are related to poverty
and deprivation and occur more frequently in cities. Rates are also higher in people in prison.
Emerson and colleagues (1997) found that rates occurred in some south Asian communities at
about three times those of other families.
There are differences within the population of people who have learning disabilities. For example,
the major survey by Emerson and colleagues (2005) found that: ‘Men were more likely to have less
privacy, see friends who have learning difficulties less often, be a victim of crime and smoke.
Women were more likely to be unemployed, have been bullied at school, attend a day centre, not
exercise, feel sad or worried.’ Younger people were more likely to live in unsuitable accommo-
dation, have less privacy at home, be bullied, be poor, be a victim of crime, and to feel unhappy,
sad or worried, left out or unconfident.
219
Maturity and some of its hazards
inquiry by Sir Jonathan Michael (2008) into healthcare for people with learning disabilities. He
found widespread discrimination and poor practice in the medical treatment of people with
learning disabilities; his report details ‘convincing evidence’ that in general medical matters they
received less effective treatment than others. He gives many examples, including one woman who
had repeat prescriptions from her GP for twenty years without once being seen; and a systematic
pattern of inadequate provision of pain relief or palliative care. He recorded evidence (2008: 16)
from the Welsh Centre for Learning Disabilities that over half had an unmet medical need.
On the other hand, many changes in health care have had dramatic positive effects. These have
steeply increased the average life expectancy of people with Down’s syndrome to around 60 years
(only a few decades ago, parents might have expected their child to die before the age of 20). Even
here, however, the significance of social factors must be understood. Eyman and Call (1991) found
that the life expectancy of people with Down’s syndrome did not vary according to the severity of
their medical conditions, but according to the competence of their self-help skills.
Education
Education services have a major impact. For people with learning disabilities, they have varied
substantially over time and between social systems. The 1944 Education Act in the UK can be seen
as part of the founding of the welfare state. At the same time as it set up a ‘universal’ system of free
education, however, it enshrined in law that some children were ‘ineducable’. These children, with
what would now be called learning disabilities, were often left in medically run institutions.
If parents chose to raise them at home, there was no school for them to attend. Race (2007:
194–196) refers to the series of scandals about life (and abuse) in the institutions which have led
to new social policies and to the expectation of a more open life for people with learning disabilities
in the last forty years.
Emerson and colleagues (2005) found that 72 per cent of adults with learning disabilities in
England had attended a special school, 19 per cent a special unit in a mainstream school, and 18
per cent a mainstream school. In the period since 1981, the local authority must provide a child
with learning disability with the particular assistance defined in an ‘Assessment of Special
Educational Need’ and this could be either in a special school, a special unit or a mainstream school.
After the age of compulsory schooling, many will now go on to educational programmes in Colleges
of Further Education.
220
Maturity and some of its hazards
home, adaptations to accommodation, education, and leisure-time activities such as holiday play
schemes.
Having a son or daughter (as a child or an adult) who requires intensive support reduces the
parents’ earning capacity while creating additional financial costs (Emerson et al., 2005). It makes
an enormous difference to the quality of life of a family – disabled person, parents and siblings –
if the state expects to make allowance for this additional cost as part of its commitment to ensuring
equal citizens’ rights for all. Race (2007: 37) lists the non-educational services specified as
entitlements in Swedish law for people with intellectual or other impairments. He cautions that in
all systems, official policies do not necessarily describe the reality ‘on the ground’ but states that
the culture of Swedish society combines with the enforceable nature of these legal rights to ensure
that the policies are largely put into operation. A society that ensures that the relevant services are
provided for each individual – including finance and personal assistance – automatically allows the
life of that individual, and those of other family members, to be ‘normalised’.
In different societies, services may recognise the significance of family members more or less effec-
tively, both in childhood and in adulthood. The official inquiry Healthcare for All (Michael, 2008:
20) found many examples of good practice. However, it also heard many statements such as the
following: ‘My daughter needs 24/7 care and when she is in hospital I or another person who
knows her well have to stay with her . . . I often have to sleep in her wheelchair, or the seat by her
bed, or a mattress on the floor if I am lucky. I am not offered a drink or food, or access to a toilet
for myself.’ The report describes as totally unacceptable this situation in which relatives who were
‘reluctant to leave a vulnerable and possibly confused patient . . . sometimes spent long hours
without a drink or food on the ward; indeed, they were sometimes explicitly barred from access to
these basics.’
221
Maturity and some of its hazards
Food (5%)
Heating (4%)
(Emerson et al., 2005)
In general, people living in private households (with family, partner or alone), were most likely to
be unable to afford one or more of these items. Emerson classified someone who cannot afford two
or more items on the list as ‘poor’ and on that basis identified 23 per cent, nearly a quarter, as ‘poor’
(the impact of poverty is considered further earlier in this chapter).
However, control over money is as important as the amount of money people receive. Emerson and
colleagues (2005: 7) report that ‘just over half of the people we asked (54%) said someone else
decided how much money they could spend each week, and just over one in ten (12%) said that
someone else decided what they could spend their money on’. They summarise their findings under
the heading ‘People with learning difficulties often have little control over their lives’.
222
Maturity and some of its hazards
known that the child has learning disabilities, the family has access both to the Swedish universal
childcare arrangements and also to more specialist childcare. Swedish law provides entitlement to
a range of assistance, financial and personal (such as a ‘companion service’) for people with
impairments and their families. The philosophy behind this is broadly that the state must provide
facilities which enable all citizens to have equal access to the way of life considered normal for
people in the country. Thus, David Race describes a person, evidently with Down’s syndrome,
leaving her own flat in the apartment block where he was staying, every morning, off to her
business about town.
Current policy in Britain is also directed towards ensuring that people with learning disabilities
have the right to be ‘full members of the society in which they live, to choose where they live and
what they do and to be as independent as they wish to be’ (Department of Health, 2001: para
1.2). However, the Inquiries by the Parliamentary Committee into Human Rights of people with
learning difficulties (UK Parliament, 2008), and by Sir Jonathan Michael (2008) into their health-
care, found that despite much progress in the last thirty years, there are ‘appalling examples of
discrimination, abuse and neglect across the range of health services’, ‘discrimination is active in
access to and outcomes from services’ (Michael, 2008: 7, 21), and that ‘it is still necessary to empha-
sise that adults with learning disabilities have the same human rights as everyone else . . . stronger
leadership is urgently needed to create a more positive culture of respect for human rights in the
United Kingdom’ (UK Parliament, 2008: 6). The parliamentary inquiry found concerns about basic
human rights in relation to residential care, childcare services, and the criminal justice system.
The social changes that Michael recommends include those directed at the implementation of equal
healthcare for all. He concludes that staff often interpret the NHS standard of ‘equal healthcare’ to
mean ‘the same healthcare’, and points out that in fact it has different implications. In relation to
people with learning disabilities, it means that staff need to understand how the person com-
municates and absorbs information, and that signs, for example, should include easy-to-understand
pictorial information. Some staff tend to stereotype people with learning disabilities, not taking the
time to see their individuality, or to understand their specific form of communication. He analyses
‘diagnostic overshadowing’ as a significant problem. This is the term used when illness symptoms
and behaviour are wrongly attributed to the general learning disability. He found the problem to
be particularly acute in relation to pain relief: ‘One parent described vividly how symptoms of
severe pain that she could see in her daughter were denied by staff because they mistakenly
attributed them to her learning disability. There is also some evidence that staff believe people with
learning disabilities have higher pain thresholds’ (Michael, 2008: 17).
Mir and colleagues (2001) investigated the experience of people with learning disabilities from
minority ethnic communities. Compounding the factors just described, they and their families may
face stereotypes or prejudgements. There are likely to be failures of communication or understand-
ing about cultural factors that are relevant. As for all people with learning disabilities, access to
advocacy is important, but for British South Asian cultures the European emphasis on individuality
may be experienced as running ‘counter to the values of collectivism and close family relationships
that exist in some communities. The roles of family and community networks need to be taken
into account when planning services for individuals’ (Mir et al., 2001: 3). In their view, racism is
involved, based on power structures rather than just cultural difference, and these can be expressed
either through culturally specific services or generalist services (2001: 8, 9). Sensitive services
have to be able to tackle prejudice faced by people with learning disabilities from within family
and community structures.
223
Maturity and some of its hazards
The effect of the label ‘learning disability’ can be to stress what people can’t do, to conceal recog-
nition of their special gifts, to engender low expectations, to create an identity which is dominated
by the role of client, and often to restrict meaningful interaction to other people with the same
label. This likely to create a cycle in which low expectations are confirmed by the person’s behav-
iour and performance and go on to confirm the stereotype, fostering greater disability.
It is important to adopt a capability approach – recognising risks, but building on relationships,
social skills and competence. The current UK policy about people with learning disabilities states
(Department of Health, 2001: para 1.4):
Valuing People is based on the premise that people with learning disability are people first. We
focus throughout on what people can do, with support if necessary, rather than on what they
cannot do.
Healthcare for All recommends a fresh and enforced emphasis on making reasonable adjustments
for people with a learning disability (a requirement of the Disability Discrimination Act). The ‘core
standards’ of the NHS should be amended to make this specific, and clinical education should
include a ‘competence based’ requirement about service for people with learning disabilities. It
recommends that the Human Rights Commission should monitor compliance with human rights
and other equality law in relation to learning disability.
O’Brien (1989) summarises the attitudes which assist people with learning disabilities, along with
their families and friends, to discover and move towards a desirable personal future as part of
ordinary community life (see Figure 7.2):
Figure 7.2
Characteristics of positive social systems and services for people with learning disability (based on O’Brien, 1989)
congregating people with disabilities expressing visions of desirable personal futures, even
together for service purposes for people who have very limited experiences or great
difficulty communicating, and even if these involve
major changes in policy
a primary focus on people’s deficiencies; attention alliances with people with disabilities, their families,
to specific negative behaviours to change; and friends
emphasis on treatments; awareness of
professional–client interaction
human service owned and operated buildings better incorporating necessary, skilled help with health,
mobility, communication, learning, and self-control
into the routines of ordinary settings
organising support around agency administration; personalising support to match individual needs
around policies and procedures; around occupational
identities and boundaries
224
Maturity and some of its hazards
Conclusion
Race uses the framework of Social Role Valorisation to analyse experience in the seven countries
he compares. He quotes (2007: 31) Nirje’s formulation, which embeds principles in the standards
of a society: that society acts correctly, it states, when it makes available to all persons with
intellectual or other impairments:
1 A normal rhythm to the day
2 A normal rhythm of the week
3 A normal rhythm of the year
This section of the book has briefly reviewed a few of the social factors – including health,
education, family services, finance and social attitudes – which affect the development of people
with learning disabilities. Using Bronfenbrenner’s words (1979/2006), the paper has concentrated
on the systems beyond the microsystem. Other equally important dimensions to explore include
housing, sexual relationships, the justice system and parenting. Any consideration of this area of
disability which concentrates only on psychological, ‘learning’, or medical dimensions will neglect
major factors in development.
Summary
This chapter has focused initially on three painful and difficult aspects of development – poverty,
violence and mental health difficulties. It then turned your attention to the effect of stigma on
identity formation. The last of these involves an understanding of the effect of cultural forces on
individual psychology.
About 13 million people in the UK were defined as in poverty in 2006–7. Poverty affects all areas
of family life and well-being, from infant mortality to adult life expectancy. The effects of childhood
poverty last throughout life.
Social workers deal with violence or its after-effects in relation to children, adults in intimate
relationships, in street violence and in pubic violence such as war. They have responsibilities to
affect subsequent development: by intervening, for example to ensure that children are safe from
violence, by advocacy, by undoing the after effects of violence, and by working with the violent
person.
225
Maturity and some of its hazards
One in six adults will be diagnosed as having a mental illness during their lifetime. Causal factors
involved in mental health difficulties can include genetic makeup; stress factors in earlier life
including abuse; current stress; relational factors in childhood; and the after- effects of trauma or
victimisation. The chapter included brief accounts of the experience of mental health difficulties
and the distinction you are likely to encounter between ‘neurosis’ and ‘psychosis’. Different accounts
are given of the association between mental health difficulties and social adversity, including the
view that the core issue is the diagnosis and treatment of mental illness, a social process in which
certain problems in living are defined as medical problems. Mental illness in the form of anxiety or
depression can sometimes lessen spontaneously. After longer-standing mental illness, some people
experience a sense of renewed personal growth, as well as damaging effects. Some forms of organic
brain deterioration, like Alzheimer’s, are at the moment irreversible. For about 17 men and 5 women
in 100,000 the outcome of mental illness will be suicide; this figure has varied over recent decades,
and the government has targets to reduce it.
Social forces create difficulties for some people in forming a positive identity. This places a psy-
chological challenge for them in their development; it has to be managed in personal and business
interactions; and socially it leaves them disregarded and exposed to practical discrimination. The
chapter picked out three illustrations: in relation to ethnicity, Phinney’s model of identity
development; the example of gay and lesbian people have to form integrated identities in the face
of elements of social (even family) disapproval; and in relation to people with learning disabilities,
unlike members of other self-advocacy groups, some may not see the label as applying to them-
selves. Although there is a sociological logic in identifying ‘spoilt identity’ in different situations,
the operation of social forces are profoundly different for different people, different sexes, different
cultural settings and different periods of history.
The chapter concluded by analysing how the development of people with learning disabilities is
fundamentally affected by social factors and attitudes – in health, education, social presence, and
finance. To remove these barriers involves focusing on what people can do rather than on what they
cannot do.
Further reading
Poverty statistics; links with health and education:
The Poverty Site, www.poverty.org.uk.
Palmer et al. (2007) Monitoring Poverty and Social Exclusion 2007. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation. Available
online: www.jrf.org.uk/sites/files/jrf/2152-poverty-social-exclusion.pdf.
Crowley, A. and Vulliamy, C. (2007) Listen Up! Children and Young People Talk: About Poverty. London/Cardiff: Save
the Children Fund. Available online http://www.savethechildren.org.uk/en/docs/wales_lu_pov.pdf.
226
Maturity and some of its hazards
The Survivors Handbook: Available as a free download from www.womensaid.org.uk. Contains practical advice on
many topics about domestic violence, including finance, housing and legal.
227
Maturity and some of its hazards
?
Questions
1 What is meant by ‘relative poverty’? Discuss some of the short- and long-term effects of poverty.
2 What are some of the contexts in which social workers deal with problems of violence? What devel-
opmental knowledge is relevant to understanding the problems?
3 What are some of the insights given about problems of violence by (a) attachment theory, (b)
psychodynamic models, (c) Bandura’s social learning theory, (d) Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model of
development?
4 What are some of the contexts in which social workers need to understand problems of mental health?
What challenges might these situations present to a social worker?
?
228
In this chapter you will find:
8 Adulthood and
ageing
• Body and mind in later adulthood
This chapter will describe some aspects of the later phases of adult life. As throughout the book, it has
several aims:
• First, to encourage you in the flexibility of imagination which is required in order to enter into the
lives of people at different ages and to see the world from their point of view.
• Second, to highlight a few relevant biological aspects of human ageing.
• Third, to comment on some of the lifespan theories with which researchers have tried to make
sense of development in later life.
The topics affect people from those in their forties to those over 100. Since a common preconception
might be that somewhere there is a body of knowledge about ‘older people’, we need to start with clear
messages about diversity. Any treatment of late adulthood, ‘older age’, is covering a wide age span; a
moment’s thought will assure you that this group is no more likely to be uniform than any other group
whose ages cover a span of forty or fifty years. Furthermore, research about any particular subgroup
finds that the most striking feature is – diversity and difference.
This emphasis is important because people of all ages hold stereotypes about ‘old people’. As a social
worker, it is important that you are aware of these stereotypes, in order to understand the social
231
?
Adulthood and ageing
contexts in which older people are trying to create their lives, and that you have some facts to replace
the stereotypes.
In relation to older people, it can be as if a stooped frame and cautious gait, white,
thinning hair and fragile bones, somehow convey a ‘thinner’, less vigorous self. Referring
back for a moment to the discussion in Chapter 7 about ‘identity’, this social identity may
Chapter 7
be wildly at odds with the reality of a person whose sense of self is undiminished. Their
vitaI sense of identity may be felt just as strongly, and perhaps more so as they have more experience
to draw upon. The practice in hospitals of displaying photographs of their older patients which show
them in earlier adulthood may be a helpful pompt to younger staff members.
To be competent in understanding people in the later decades of their life (as in understanding anyone)
requires the social worker to have the imagination to enter into the current chapter of the ‘narrative’
of the person they are talking with.
Problems of terminology
Problems of terminology are more prominent here than in most of the other chapters. In part, this is
because ‘ageing’ means the process of growing older but has attached connotations of decline (the
difference between what some philosophers call the ‘denotation’ of a word and its connotations).
Specifically, however, the difficulty is that ‘ageing’ in relation to cell biology actually means (‘denotes’)
the process of deterioration. There is a constant danger that ‘ageing’ (the process of living during the
second half or so of life, the process of adding age to a younger organism) is taken to mean the process
of decline, damage or deterioration.
Gerontologists refer to the characteristic profile of the young old, old old and oldest old. In the early
stages, say about 60 to 75 years of age, the lives of many adults may include caring responsibilities for
grandchildren or for their own parents, and also the enjoyment of active leisure pursuits previously
squeezed out by paid employment. Adults who suffer from a chronic condition at 65 are less likely to
do well, cognitively and physically. The oldest old (say, older than 85) are more likely to suffer from
disability, but people who survive longer seem actually to be constitutionally and cognitively more
robust than younger people who die earlier (ONS, 2005; Boyd and Bee, 2006).
232
Adulthood and ageing
?
Reflective thinking
What does ‘old’ mean?
Imagine you are 88 and about to move into residential care, a move prompted in part by severe
problems with your short-term memory. You are having a chat with a granddaughter of whom
you are very fond, and you recognise her well. You have met some of the residents and share with
your granddaughter that some of them really are, sadly, pretty out of touch with reality.
In a somewhat sad and wistful mood, you talk with your granddaughter about what ‘old’ meant
to you at different ages in your life. Write down what you say.
Perhaps begin: ‘When I was two I didn’t know what “old” meant. Though I remember my mum
saying that it didn’t matter if that towel got dirty because it was old. When I was 5, “old” meant
anyone over the age of . . .’
Perhaps after going through views at different ages, end ‘and what does “old” mean to me now?
“Old” means . . .’
The mortality statistics quoted in the previous paragraph are based on rates of death at different ages
at the present time. In addition to this are changes brought about by improvements in future health
or social care, which cannot be statistically predicted (see ONS, 2007b). In the last hundred years these
have consistently operated to increase lifespan. So the chances are that the 40-year-old male will on
average live to be older than 78; projections suggest that by 2020 a woman aged 65 can expect to live
until she is 87 or so (ONS, 2004).
233
Adulthood and ageing
Bodily experience at different ages is affected by which decade you were born in. As Professor Tom
Kirkwood put it in his distinguished BBC Reith Lecture of 2001, people in their seventies today are in
the same physical condition as people in their sixties were when he started practising medicine
(Kirkwood, 2001). Heredity is an important influence in ageing, but so are environment, earlier disease
and so on.
Loss of function, as well as life expectancy, is related to social, cognitive and emotional factors in
interaction with physical change. The emotional and cognitive features may be out of awareness, as
illustrated an experiment by Hausdorff and colleagues (1999). Walking speed is a common indicator
of physical fitness, and has been found in American studies to correlate with nursing home admission
and with death. In the work of Hausdorff, forty-seven men and women aged between 60 and 85 played
a computer game about the interaction of their physical and mental skills. For one group negative
words about age, such as ‘senile’ and ‘dependent’, were flashed up for a few thousandths of a second.
This is long enough for the brain to decode the word-image, but not long enough for it to enter
conscious awareness (the images are ‘subliminal’). For the other group, the words used were positive,
such as ‘wise’ and ‘accomplished’. The researchers were themselves surprised to find that the
foot-speed of the ‘positive message’ group increased by just under 10 per cent, a substantial change
equivalent to gains found in older people after months of rigorous physical training. The researchers
went on in other experiments to find that unconscious positive stereotyping had a similar effect
?
on memory performance. It seems likely that positive self-image (taking it for granted that one is
competent, effective and has a functioning memory) has a direct effect on performance. Older
people who apologise for their memory and expect to be less sharp then their younger colleagues,
become so.
As you would expect, the processes involved are thought to be linked to the stereotyping
effects referred to in Chapter 7. In relation to ethnicity, Levy and Banaji (2006) refer to
research that showed that simply asking students to note their ethnicity was sufficient to
Chapter 7
diminish the cognitive scores of African American boys. They point out that in relation to
prejudice about ‘race’, gender or sexuality, there is a large difference between strong, consciously held
views and unconscious negative bias – intellectual beliefs and conscious anti-discriminatory attitudes
can override subconscious bias. By contrast, explicit attitudes about ‘old age’ appear to have less
potency to override negative bias. In implicit attitudes about ‘old people’, the negative bias is stronger
(than against other ‘out-groups’) and the difference between explicit and implicit attitudes is smaller
than in relation to other groups. In relation to ageing stereotypes, the negative implicit stereotypes do
not diminish as the person enters that group themselves. Unlike membership of other stereotyped
groups, all younger people eventually become part of this stereotyped group themselves, and both the
conscious and unconscious stereotypes remain unabated. In addition to other implications, this
ensures a feedback mechanism for the perpetuation of negative stereotypes. Levy and Banaji (2006),
in their survey of 660 adults, found that people with positive stereotypes of ageing (measured up to
twenty-three years before they died) lived on average 7.5 years longer than those with negative stereo-
types. This was the measure of the effect after allowance was made for functional health, gender and
234
Adulthood and ageing
other relevant socioeconomic factors were taken into account. As Levy and Banaji comment (2006:
60), ‘with friends like oneself, who needs enemies?’
In addition to biological and psychological factors, social arrangements clearly have an effect on
functional independence – family, friendships and community involvement as well as social policy play
a large part in determining whether a person with a degree of physical limitation has an engaged or
dependent life.
?
Reflective thinking
Social and psychological aspects of stereotyping
What are some of the different images of ageing that are presented in the mass media? Check
?
some television schedules, television programmes and newspapers and see if they bear out your
answer.
Looking closer
Explicit stereotypes and attitudes can be measured by self-report studies.
Implicit stereotypes and attitudes, as described by Levy and Banaji (2006: 51), ‘operate without
conscious awareness, intention or control’, so they cannot be measured by self-report. The
authors describe some of the methods used to assess the strength of implicit stereotypes and
attitudes (2006: 53–54).
They found that implicit negative attitudes towards older people are stronger than those of
other prejudices – ‘based on 64,000 tests, it remains one of the largest negative implicit
attitudes we have observed, consistently larger than the anti-black implicit attitudes amongst
white Americans’ (2006: 54). Explicit attitudes were closer to implicit attitudes than in other
prejudices.
Sexual system
The ovaries of most women stop producing eggs sometime between the ages of 45 and 55. ‘Menopause’
is technically defined as occurring after a year in which there has been no menstruation, and it occurs
235
Adulthood and ageing
at an average age of 51 (Boyd and Bee, 2006: 408; Powersurge, 2008). The impact of the associated
hormonal changes varies considerably. For some women, the transition is clear and brief; for others it
may be irregular and extended. It may be accompanied by physical symptoms such as hot flushes (the
rush of heat from chest to head), night sweats, vaginal dryness, sleep disturbance and mood swings or
heightened emotional variability which are experienced as intrusive and uncomfortable.
As in other ages of life, specific reliable information about physical sexual activity and its relation to
cultural and historic circumstances is unlikely to be established. Some gain a new energy in their sexual
lives, perhaps because of a general lack of concern about periods or the risk of getting pregnant (see,
for example, Purnine and Carey, 1998). Others, whether living on their own or in heterosexual or lesbian
relationships, end physical sexual activity before or during the menopause. Some of these nevertheless
experience their feminine sexuality as undiminished, not equating erogenous stimulation as central to
their sexuality; some derive great satisfaction from touching and other physical contact, and some are
less clear about their sexuality or consider it is no longer an important part of themselves. Bartlik and
Goldstein (2000, 2001) found that 70 per cent of heterosexual couples were sexually active at the age
of 70. For these couples, it seems to be usually an episode such as separation or illness which breaks
the pattern and leads to the discontinuation of physical sexual activity. The vagina is essentially a
(lubricated) muscle and although regular use maintains muscle tone and lubrication, hormonal
changes after the menopause precipitate loss of muscle tone (atrophy). This is reversible. For women,
these changes mark the beginning of the second half of life.
The decline in male hormones occurs more gradually. It is very variable in healthy men, and effects are
rarely evident in those under 60 (although levels have usually been changing since the age of about
45). When they occur, changes in testosterone levels may be associated with a decline in the
experienced level of sexual desire. Until this decline occurs, any loss of erectile function is unlikely to
be caused by hormonal changes. The loss of sexual desire or hunger will mean different things for
different men, reflecting the variety of earlier experience. By the age of 80, about half have low
testosterone, and many will still not notice the effects (Mayo Clinic, 2007; Sternbach, 1998). In men,
variations in sex hormones appear to be linked with cognitive functioning – Boyd and Bee (2006: 461)
quote findings that lower levels of testosterone caused by prostate cancer treatment had reduced
cognitive functioning and these returned to normal when hormone levels were restored. Sternbach
summarises that testosterone decline or deficiency in men is not analogous to the female menopause.
236
Adulthood and ageing
Strokes
A stroke (cerebrovascular accident) occurs when a blood vessel bleeds inside the brain. If this results
in damage, it can involve immediate loss of function, potentially followed by recovery as the brain
recreates new pathways for achieving the function previously carried out in the damaged part of the
brain. A stroke can vary in severity from something which is barely noticed, to being a cause of sudden
death. Strokes occur more frequently in later life. When Olsen and colleagues (2007) studied all 40,000
stroke suffers in Denmark from 2001 to 2007, they found that women had fewer but more severe
strokes than men, and were more likely to survive them.
In Alzheimer’s disease, areas of the brain become coated with a sticky ‘plaque’. Recently, for the first
time, brain scans are able to show these deposits, holding out the promise of making diagnosis more
reliable (at the moment, diagnosis is made through a subtle process of judging patterns of memory
loss). Its cause is not well understood. Both Parkinson’s and Huntington’s diseases, involve a loss of
nerve cells in (different) parts of the brain. Huntington’s disease is a hereditary condition caused by a
variation in one specific gene; Parkinson’s has a large hereditary component involving various genes
which have not been fully identified, but it is also the result of environmental factors. Multi-infarct
dementia is the result of blockages in the blood flow to parts of the brain (because of deposits in the
arteries) and is responsible for 40 per cent of cases of dementia (Sergo, 2008).
Cognitive functioning
Short-term memory and working memory decline with age. Working memory is a short-term memory
store which is deleted after use, so that its contents are not transferred to long-term memory. Working
memory is used when, for example, you hold the overall requirements of a ten-minute task in your
head while performing a sub-task that takes one minute. The decline is kept at bay by activities such
as crosswords, sudoku or general intellectual activities. There is, however, some evidence that these
only delay its onset, the rate of subsequent deterioration being faster for these people. There have been
similar discoveries in relation to education. Higher levels of education delay the onset of dementia, but
237
Adulthood and ageing
it appears that this factor only ‘holds it at bay’, perhaps by unconscious compensation for physical
deterioration. Once the symptoms show, the condition progresses more rapidly, one suggestion being
that this is because the brain decay is already at a more advanced state (Goudarzi, 2008). There is some
evidence that skills which depend on social intelligence improve with increasing age (Hakamies-
Blomqvist, 2006; Henry et al., 2004).
?
Reflective thinking
Interview someone over retirement age. Ask them what they find rewarding about growing older,
and what they dislike.
Theories of ageing
As with other areas of life, there are many lenses through which we can view full adult maturity. Each
brings into focus different aspects of life. The biggest gap in the literature is probably first-hand
articulated accounts by people who are themselves in late old age; reflective accounts which explore
the experience in its richness and ambiguity. ‘The young [the author means anyone who is not old]
know nothing directly about old age, and their enquiries of the subject must be done blind . . . the
condition for [most writers and researchers] must in a sense be a closed book to them . . . those who
have had actual experience of old age are likely to be dead or very tired or just reluctant to discuss the
matter with young interlocutors’. Thus wrote Frank Kermode at the age of 88, reviewing The Long Life
by Helen Small (Kermode, 2007; Small, 2007).
238
Adulthood and ageing
It has proved difficult to create theory which reliably combines all the different factors in later life.
Such a theory must recognise both similarity and variation in psychological development. It must
?
account accurately for biological influences and the effect of illness. It must not over-generalise cohort
effects which affect only people born in a particular time. It is required to identify and explain the
specific impacts of economics, culture, social policy and social stratification. The theory must give a
convincing account of change and continuity in ‘identity’, and in general must establish both the extent
of variability and the limits on variability. The lifespan approach, which will be discussed
in Chapter 10, has paid particular attention to these features in later life, but nevertheless,
Bass (2006) regards this synthesis as still ‘the holy grail’ of theory about old age – some-
Chapter 10 thing everyone searches for but no one has found.
Disengagement theory
The first comprehensive multidisciplinary theory of ageing was proposed by Cumming and Henry
(1961). They moved away from purely psychological, biological or sociological approaches and empha-
sised that ageing cannot be understood separately from the characteristics of the social system in
which it takes place. Based on a study of 279 men and women between the ages of 50 and 90, they
identified ‘social disengagement’ as a characteristic process of older people which benefited both the
individual and the social systems of which they are a part.
Disengagement was viewed as an adaptive behaviour because it allowed older people to maintain a
sense of self-worth while adjusting to the loss of earlier social roles. This process was seen to have
a positive function and positive qualities, in contrast to activity theory (outlined next) which assumed
older people needed to be ‘busy’ and ‘engaged’ to remain well-adjusted.
The implication (or assumption?) of disengagement theory is that because of inevitable biological and
intellectual changes, older people become decreasingly active within the external world and increas-
ingly preoccupied with their inner lives. Cumming and Henry understood disengagement to be a
functional aspect of social order and continuity because it allowed the systems in place in a society to
be handed on from one generation to the next, as older people withdraw and pass power and influence
to the younger.
In present-day wealthy industrial societies, for example, many people remain socially and economically
active well after they have retired from paid work roles. Research by Ashfar and colleagues (2002) finds
large differences in disengagement between different subcultures in the same area of Britain. Their
parenting role may change from its earlier physical manifestation and yet still be highly significant. It
239
Adulthood and ageing
may involve a closely coordinated sharing of the daytime (or overnight) care of grandchildren, and
involve a substantial financial element as well as continuing emotional interaction. The level of daily
physical activity may change, but the amount they travel may increase and their leisure activities, their
cultural and social engagement, may become more evident.
In general, the theory is criticised (Bengtson and Achenbaum, 1994) for failing to take account of:
• individual choice;
• individual personality dimensions;
Nevertheless, despite these criticisms, many people, old and young, recognise an element of realism in
the analysis which Henry and Cumming made from their data. For these theorists, ‘disengagement’ is
a natural feature of good adjustment in older age. This contrasts with the next theory to be discussed.
Activity theory
Activity theory was proposed after disengagement theory (Neugarten, 1977), and presents a con-
trasting picture. It is based on the hypothesis that active older people are more satisfied than those
who are not active. Self-concept is validated by participation in valued roles, and there are many
socially valued roles characteristic of middle life which should replace lost roles – productive roles in
voluntary associations, religious and leisure organisations.
According to this view, social services which are based on disengagement theory are likely to embody
(and may be an excuse for) ageism. They should instead take the responsibility to support community
structures which encourage and make possible the continuing of activities from middle age into older
age. In terms of societal well-being, older people should replace lost roles with new ones to maintain
their place in society.
Activity theory is criticised for treating the problems of ageing as individual problems and ignoring the
realities of older people’s bodies, energies and choices.
Ageing is characterised by a progressive loss of the ability to adapt to stress: ‘With progressing age,
this falls below the level required for daily living’ (Pendergast et al., 1993). If this is true, older adults
more often operate near the limits of their ability to adapt. Faced with this, a typical strategy is to adapt
to choose activities that leave them within their limits. Boyd and Bee (2006) suggest the example of
giving up mountain climbing but continuing to walk regularly.
Activity theory is supported by the finding in several studies that active older adults show slightly
higher levels of life satisfaction and morale than others (Boyd and Bee, 2006: 488, referencing
Adelman, 1994, George, 1990 and others). As Bee points out, however, the idea that disengagement is
240
?
Adulthood and ageing
unproblematic gains support from the findings mentioned in Chapter 5: that although
older people have fewer social contacts than younger people, it is younger people who
report more loneliness (Revenson, 1982).
Chapter 5
Both approaches have to be supplemented by a third set of research findings, about con-
tinuity. There may be significant continuity between earlier patterns and disengagement/activity in
later life. People who tend to be satisfied with their own company and personal enthusiasms earlier
in life tend to be so in later life, while those who gain satisfaction from social roles and physical activity
earlier will tend in later life to continue these or to replace earlier roles with equivalents. Stuart-
Hamilton (2006: 166) gives some detail about American studies which support this view, including that
of Reichard and colleagues (1962), who interviewed eighty-seven men aged 55 to 84. In a very long-
running project, Haan studied a core sample of 118 subjects over a period of fifty years. She reports
on dimensions both of continuity and of variability (Haan et al., 1986). Neugarten (1977) reports on a
sample of people in their seventies and found different personality types which would link with the
different approaches of disengagement or ‘activity’. He concludes that ‘older people adjust their
personalities but do not change them radically’.
This is put forward formally by theorists such as Atchley (1989), who argue that life satisfaction is
determined by how consistent current activities or lifestyles are with one’s lifetime experience. The
model is generally seen as an elaboration of activity theory and in opposition to disengagement theory.
‘Continuity’ is analysed in a subtle, rather than a static way. External continuity is distinguished from
internal continuity, which allows people to have a sense of ‘who they are’ even as they change
(Sugarman, 2001: 163). Continuity theory emphasises a constantly moving dynamic.
This is taken a stage further by narrative theorists who explore how this continuity is created. They
say that memory is essential in the process, involving constantly re-interpreting the past so as to create
a coherent story with the individual as its subject (Sugarman quotes Cohler (1982) and McAdams
(1997) as key authors).
Feminist approaches
Feminist approaches to understanding development in older age emphasise that here, as in other
aspects of life, gender must be a primary consideration. Feminist writers and researchers adopt dif-
ferent views on many issues, but some of their key beliefs are:
• The experiences of women are often ignored in attempts to understand human life unless a
specifically feminist perspective is taken.
• These experiences must be analysed critically, taking into account how they are structured by
women’s unequal access to power.
• Experiences of women in old age can only be understood in the context of the inequality they
experience in care-giving, health and poverty across the life course.
241
Adulthood and ageing
They emphasise that feminist researchers should be explicit about the framework of power and knowl-
edge from which they undertake research; they are likely to be middle class, financially privileged and
carrying out research with the prior beliefs outlined above, with the aim of benefiting women and
involving them in the research as active participants.
Other views adopted by some feminists are that women occupy an inferior status to men in older age
because of the structures of capitalist patriarchal society. Arber and Ginn, (1995:71; see also 1991),
for example, use this perspective to explain how the differential age of entitlement to state pension
(at the time, 60 for women and 65 for men) is a manifestation of male patriarchal power.
Until recently, the women’s movement has tended to focus its attention on the need for out-of-family
childcare to allow women to take on more paid employment, as well as the perceived injustice of the
amount of (unpaid) care for older people provided by women. This has been compounded by some
earlier perspectives which envisaged ‘the death of the family’ (Simon et al., 1968). It has paid less
attention to the necessity of finding arrangements for the care of ageing parents which allow women
to take an active role; arrangements which accommodate this as a ‘normative family stress’ (Brody,
1985), without neglecting the wishes of the parents or weighing excessively on the lives of these
women. Conceptualising the requirements in terms of victimhood or burden have not created solutions
to the potentially crippling problems of financial stress, depression and physical damage arising from
‘the 36-hour day’ (Mace and Rabins, 2007).
?
Reflective thinking
Can you briefly summarise disengagement theory, activity theory and feminist theories of ageing?
If you can, have a conversation with a retired person in which you explain these views and ask
for their observations.
242
Adulthood and ageing
The ‘oldest old’ at the present time experienced the economic depression and mass unemployment
of the 1930s, followed by the Second World War. They had to create a totally different picture of
‘old age’ and ‘welfare’ from their parents, many of whom regarded ‘welfare’ as a destructive ghost,
haunting them with the spectre of the workhouse and degradation. The ‘young old’ were born during
that war and just afterwards – they are the ‘baby boom generation’ who were brought up to take the
welfare state for granted, and saw massive changes in the position of women. The image of ‘old age’
presented to the older of these generations was that it was likely to last only a couple of years after
retirement from work, and public pensions at a realistic level were a new idea.
History and culture are intimately linked. Cultures change over time, and these changes are sometimes
the outcomes of specific historical events. Afshar and colleagues (2002), in their study in the ‘Growing
Older’ programme of the UK Economic and Social Research Council, found that:
There was a connection between life course events and quality of life in later years. The curtail-
ment of education, reduced employment opportunities, war time experiences and for Polish and
Caribbean women being unable to use their previous training were all mentioned as having an
effect. The African Caribbean women told of the horrific racism they had experienced on arrival
in Britain.
. . . There are ethnic differences in how the women perceived ageing, with some Pakistani and
Bangladeshi women reporting feeling older at a much earlier age than other groups.
(Afshar et al., 2002)
This study illustrates the different experiences of ageing which may arise in different cultural groups.
White women who were financially better off and had relocated on retirement were building new social
networks and forms of social support; minority ethnic women who had migrated into this country
during the course of their life and poorer white women who had lived in the same vicinity all their lives
tended to have families who lived nearby, and had a sense of purpose linked to kinship ties – ‘agreed
tasks, obligations and reciprocities which bind families together’. Childcare tasks and grandparenting
roles were important in their daily lives. Some, particularly Indian and Polish participants, made a
link between a sense of well-being and being respected and valued by others, especially in terms of
the status afforded to older people in their cultural circle. Only the white non-migrant women raised
the issue of feeling ignored or dismissed because of their perceived age. The researchers noted that
disadvantaged white non-migrant women can easily be excluded from consultation processes about
social policy. Some minority ethnic groups felt over-researched (and saw few tangible results from the
information they provided – Afshar et al., 2002: 4).
As has already been pointed out, people above pensionable age form only a small proportion of people
in poverty in the UK. The proportion of pensioners in poverty in 2007 (17 per cent) had halved over the
preceding decade. Those in the lowest income group were significantly more likely to suffer poor
health, but the risk varied with age. Overall, about a third of people over 50 reported a limiting
243
Adulthood and ageing
?
long-term illness, but this varied from 45 per cent of the poorest fifth to 19 per cent of those in the
richest fifth. For those over 85, the risk is 70 per cent and is less dependent on income (Evandrou et
al., 2002: 48).
Poverty after pension age for the oldest old and for single people – see Chaper 7.
Patterns of living vary substantially even between the countries of the European Union, among
different subcultures in the UK and across different decades. In Italy, only 36 per cent (just over a third)
of older women live alone, whereas in the UK 46 per cent (just under a half) do. In Sweden, the
proportion is 51 per cent, just over a half. This represents a general pattern of greater family living in
the Mediterranean countries which lessens progressively to the north. In each country the numbers
had increased significantly over the last thirty years. Older Asian people in the UK (admittedly from a
very small relevant population of 13,000) are much more likely than indigenous white people to live
in a household including other generations. Tomassini points out that, assuming families provide care,
these patterns have implications for the provision of care by the state (all these statistics are from
Tomassini, 2005).
The national statistics analysed by Evandrou (2005) show that compared with the general population
in any age group there was a much greater proportion of single, widowed and divorced people living
in communal establishments in 2003. She takes this to indicate the extent to which spouses normally
provide support for independent living. When care is provided by a partner, men are as likely to provide
it as women. When provided by a younger generation, more women than men provide it – daughters-
in-law as well as daughters. This group is sometimes called ‘the sandwich generation’ – ‘women in the
middle’ – caring for their adolescent children or their children’s children at the same time as providing
care for their own parents (for more detail about multiple roles in mid-life, see Evandrou et al., 2002,
and Brody and Saperstein, 2006).
UK governments have found it difficult to find acceptable and sustainable approaches to social care
for older people. There are ‘calls to clarify state and individual responsibilities’ and for a new financial
settlement (Commission for Social Care Inspection, 2008). In 2005, at a time when the population over
75 had increased by 3 per cent, the Commission for Social Care inspection found that the number of
people receiving social care had fallen, largely through local councils tightening their eligibility criteria.
It estimates that 6,000 people with high levels of need for support (and 275,000 with lower levels of
need) received no care. They disputed the common assumption in councils that services such as
shopping and cleaning can be privately arranged by vulnerable individuals, considering that this
approach leads to problems later. They found that one method through which this rationing took place
was by the use of global service-level directives about eligibility (rather than individualised professional
244
Adulthood and ageing
assessment of need), and consider that this is particularly likely to result in poor service when social
care managers or assessors are not qualified social workers.
The current arrangements in England and Wales are that older people live on their accrued assets
(including the value of their house) until they drop to a minimum, at which point they become eligible
for local authority services. These provisions are not comparable to healthcare (which is free), and,
because of the anomalies created, the policies are widely regarded as unsatisfactory, but the financial
implications of an increasing population mean that finding realistic and widely accepted alternatives
is not easy. In Scotland, social care is provided largely free, but there are concerns that the increased
costs in this system have led to even higher eligibility levels before service is provided.
In the UK at the present time, public policy emphasises personalised provision and control. The qualities
that people require in their care are:
• the retention and promotion of independence;
• the retention of choice and control;
• flexibility.
In home care, the services are provided to enable:
245
Adulthood and ageing
is a cause for shame. It could be a creative solution for the public services to support a residential
facility which is culturally and psychologically ‘owned’ by the community. This might avoid the sense
of ‘abandonment’ on the one hand and shame on the other.
As was mentioned earlier, the period after the age of 60 is as likely to be a time of providing care as of
receiving it. The Office of National Statistics (ONS, 2005a) reports that the majority of social care is
provided by family members. ‘In 2001, over three quarters (78 per cent) of all older people who reported
suffering from mobility problems were helped by their spouse or other household members.’ Some 1.2
million men over 50 and 1.6 million women reported supplying social care to spouses, family or
neighbours. About a quarter of these are providing more than fifty hours each week. Of the younger
old, more women provide care, but over 75, the proportion of men providing care is twice that of
women. The proportion of people who provide care decreases with age, but of these, the proportion
providing fifty hours each week increases. These are significant issues for any social care system to
take into account. Social workers and other care workers have to understand the experiences of both
the person who needs care and the person who provides it.
Whether the carer is young or old, this provision will have its own individual dynamics, containing a
varied mixture of satisfaction, hatred, obligation and duty, love and self-sacrifice. Many women find it
impossible to continue in paid employment and carry out necessary caring roles. The punishing
physical responsibilities that may be involved, the isolation caused by constant attendance, or the
depression brought about by the situation may damage the carer and their well-being (Brody and
Saperstein, 2006). The job of the social worker and other providers is to work sensitively with each
family or community network to offer as helpful a service as possible. This may involve tasks additional
to the simple assessment of an individual’s care needs, and is a subtle, sensitive process of fitting in
with carers’ development.
At the time of writing, there is a policy drive to put the service user ‘in control’ by allocating individual
budgets (to deploy as they think best) rather than providing services. This will still leave social workers
the task of sensitively working alongside carers in the existing network of the individual.
?
Reflective thinking
Do you know someone who has the role of carer in relation to an older person? Ask them if they
would mind discussing with you some good things about this role and some areas which are
negative or problematic.
246
Adulthood and ageing
?
Reflective thinking
The Chapter 7 commented on identity – how it is created by social context and is also a personal
‘sense of self’. This activity reflects on this subject in relation to ageing.
First, think about yourself. What aspects of your identity are common to others in your generation
(perhaps, specific to your generation)? What aspects are formed by social expectations of
‘someone in your position’? What aspects of your identity do you regard as specific to your
personal qualities and experience?
Thinking of an older person whom you know, what aspects of their ‘identity’ do you think are
common to others of the same generation (perhaps, distinctive to that generation)? Which are
generated by social expectations of ‘someone in their position’? Which aspects of their identity
are specific to their personal qualities and experience?
247
Adulthood and ageing
248
Adulthood and ageing
emotion (as well as articulating the way care should be managed) in a way that is not available to
others. A daughter or partner caring for her experiences the loss of freedom, perhaps the loss of a work
role, which only she is in a position to articulate. The most important lessons in social work come not
from courses but from the people with whom you work.
Many may have insights to communicate about spiritual matters. They may consider that it is
all-pervasive, even in secular life, and gives a fundamental meaning to what is done – or may now
believe that, despite all the sacrifices they made for religion and the way they allowed it to structure
their lives, it is in fact a social construct and cannot have the transcendence they once attributed
to it. ?
As in so many matters, they may be able to comment in a way not available to younger people on the
combination of decline and vitality they experience.
Erikson’s analysis of the later stages of life is that it is a time when the older person
reviews life. The two possible outcomes of the characteristic crisis of older age (see
‘Essential background’, section 6) are integration or despair. The positive outcome achieves
Chapter
EB6 a sense of integration of the experiences of life, valuing achievements and accepting
realistically goals of middle life which turn out to be unattainable. Although this chapter
has suggested that this is bound to oversimplify the long decades and diversity of the years grouped
together as ‘older age’, it is worth noting that Erikson himself (1902–1994) published his last work,
Vital Involvement in Old Age, at the age of 87. Feil (1989) suggested that a more realistic scheme would
be to add a final stage to Erikson’s, a stage in which the possible outcomes are to sink into a vegetative
state or to integrate the experiences of life.
This chapter has looked at a number of the dimensions to this mature experience of self, the ‘mature
imagination’. The self is forming and re-forming in the context of a changing experience of the capa-
bilities of body and mind, for many a changed experience of pain or discomfort. The chapter referred
to the gap which can open up between the sense of self and restrictive, dependency-creating social
structures. The research about social support indicated the importance of an individual feeling that he
or she is still of use in society. As a study undertaken by older people themselves put it, the purpose of
social support is to ensure ‘you feel as though you are still someone’ (CSAC and Age Concern Cumbria,
2006). Atchley’s work emphasises the importance of being able to create ‘continuity’ in selfhood
through the changes of life. Ashfar’s research indicated the difference between people in different life
settings, white indigenous women who had not moved from their locality being the most likely in their
study to feel ignored, marginalised and disregarded. Erikson’s view that the developmental pathways
arising in this particular life crisis can lead either to ego integration or to despair is a useful caution
against romanticising the mature experience of self (Boyd and Bee, 2006: 482, report studies which
found that ‘wisdom’ is higher in young people than older people). Statistics indicate the degree to
which older people at different ages are themselves carers for others, with all that this entails in terms
of health, experience, relationships, and self-identity. This active self-formation is structured by
economic realities – of poverty or comfort, good or poor accommodation, physical neglect or care.
249
Adulthood and ageing
Chapter 9 takes our thoughts to death – the approach of death; the brute fact of death itself, at
whatever age it arrives; the circumstances of death; and the impact on those bereaved.
Summary
‘Older age’ refers to a very wide age span and widely varying experiences. One of the most striking
features of any research into ageing is the extent of difference and diversity. A common scheme is
to distinguish ‘young old’ (about 55 to 75), old old (about 75–85) and the oldest old (85 and older).
The older people are, the longer they can expect to live. Life expectancy at 40 for men is 78; at the
age of 70 it is 83.
Loss of function in later life is related to social and psychological factors interacting with physical
changes. The menopause is an example of changes which take place in the sexual system; arthritis
is an example in joints and movement; strokes in the cerebrovascular system; dementia (through
Alzheimer’s disease or other causes) in cognitive functioning.
Disengagement theory views progressive social disengagement as a positive dynamic, for both the
individual and society. Activity theory by contrast views participation in valued social roles as an
important part of healthy ageing, and holds that active people are more satisfied than those who
are not. Feminist perspectives emphasise that development in older age can only be understood if
gender is taken into account. Exchange theory suggests that social life is structured by what people
can offer to each other, and that dependency is characteristic in older people because it is all they
have available to ‘exchange’. The political economy of ageing regards older age as a period of
enforced dependency caused by the lack of economic productivity.
Culture, social policy and history shape the ageing process. Social care should be flexible, designed
to promote independence and choice. It is crucial for the social worker to be able to respect,
appreciate and enter into the ‘mature imagination’ of each person.
Further reading
Johnson, M.L., Bengtson, V.L. Coleman, P.G. and Kirkwook B.L. (eds) (2005) The Cambridge Handbook of Age and
Ageing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Standard resource on all aspects of ageing.
Bond, J., Peace, S. Dittmann-Kohli, F. and Westerhof, G. (eds) (2007) Ageing in Society. London: Sage. Thorough
account of social aspects of ageing.
Brody, E. (2004) Women in the Middle. New York: Springer Publishing. This is a powerful account of the multiple
competing demands on the time and energy of women who are the caregivers to ageing parents. The central
focus is on the care-giving story from the perspective of women; its joys, demands and problems. It analyses the
social background and subjective experiences of parent care, never losing sight of the experience of the older
250
Adulthood and ageing
generation being cared for. The policy background is American, but that does not affect the universality of the
studies in this remarkable book.
Statistics about older people – population profile, life expectancy, income, work, caring, gender, ethnicity and
much more – are available from the Office of National Statistics, which holds a treasure house of information,
much of it readily available online. Focus on older age (www.statistics.gov.uk/focuson/olderpeople) contains
clearly written, accessible and relevant information. There is also a shorter summary version, providing the
‘headlines’, and there are many single webpage ‘nuggets’ such as www.statistics.gov.uk/cci/nugget.asp?id=949.
The ONS links census data with other researches such as the longitudinal study to create numerous relevant and
readable studies, like Focus on Gender or Focus on Families.
For a comprehensive and readable account of ageing, with detail, about many topics relating to older age, an
(American) source is Boyd and Bee (2006), chapters 17 and 18.
For topics related to social care and social policy, the Joseph Rowntree Foundation commissions and publishes
much relevant research. Most of their research is accompanied by two- or three-page online summaries, found
by using their search facility on www.jrf.org.uk/.
Stewart, S. and Vaitilingam, R. (eds) (2004). Seven Ages of Man and Woman. London: ESRC. Chapters 6 and 7,
pp. 28–35, set older age in the context of the rest of life.
?
Questions
Based on the text.
1 Write an essay titled: ‘Anti-oppressive practice in social work with older people’.
In suggesting this question, I make the assumption that you already have some knowledge of the
meaning of ‘anti-oppressive practice’.
On the website accompanying this book, you will find suggestions about a response to this question..
Note that the essay will not focus only on prejudice and discrimination about age, but will discuss how
ageing can be negatively structured because of other factors like gender and ethnicity.
continued
251
Adulthood and ageing
252
In this chapter you will find:
• Understanding self
Introduction
Nicola’s mother Ann is talking to the social worker. ‘It just seems so hard at the
moment. I was told about a death in the family at midnight last night. It’s the god-
mother of my sister’s children. So she’s only our age. It’s terrible news. It was quite quick,
not expected, though she’s been ill.
‘And then there’s my mother. She’s been looking out for her neighbour Doris, who’s got
dementia as well as cancer. But now Doris has died. Mum’s . . . quite getting on, you
know, she’s quite old now, so it’s a kind of relief. But it’s just so much.’
Freud described the experience of bereavement as ‘living psychologically beyond our means’ – that is,
trying to do something we do not have the resources to do. Whatever one’s personal history, one may
be apprehensive about exploring this experience.
Every bereavement, and its effect, is different. For one man, the death of his mother after a long illness
may be sad, an event which causes some simplification of his day-to-day life, an event which brings
home his own mortality. It may be an occasion which brings together family members, which brings
the comfort of shared grief and the unusual experience of acceptable shedding of tears in public. Then
and afterwards it allows his father, brothers and sisters to talk about his mother, her admirable and
her difficult qualities, to reflect in a new way about the totality of her life. He may feel able to relate
to her now in a less conflicted way, his affection released from the day-to-day difficulties she presented
for him. But in other circumstances, the emotional experience may be intense, and the social worker
who is involved may be faced with emotional challenges.
The doctors sought the consent of Robina and her husband to switch off Yasmin’s life
support machine. Yasmin was 10, and the hospital social worker had known the family
for several years now. Yasmin had apparently been progressing well, and had been living
at home for periods of up to six months. But when she came into hospital six weeks ago,
there were unexpected complications, and it was evident that things were seriously
wrong. In the last week, the doctors reported that her brain was not responding; on
Tuesday, after discussions with the clinical team (including the social worker), the
doctors held a meeting with the parents about switching off the equipment that
was keeping Yasmin’s body functioning. The social worker had met with different
combinations of parents, aunts, children, and cousins four times during the week. ‘Would
you stay with me when they switch off the machine?’ Robina had asked. In collaboration
255
Dying, grief and mourning
with the nurses, the social worker had asked whether the parents wanted some music
played during Yasmin’s last moments; whether they wanted incense burning; who
they wished to be in the room; what time of day the final moments should take place;
whether they wanted any photographs taken. Despite the comparative suddenness of
the deterioration, the parents had reached a resignation, and there was a moving
solemnity and calm about the final (technology-assisted) breaths taken by their
daughter.
By contrast, Beryl’s experience of the medical treatment and death of her child 40 years ago, followed
by her attempts to establish her life afterwards are remembered as a nightmare. In recollection, the
events are disjointed and fragmentary. They cause a blackness to descend on her experience and
responses, a blackness she can neither describe nor manage. Now aged 60, she leads a tightly organised
life. In a way that she herself can hardly put her finger on, she thinks of her child ‘most days’ or
‘most weeks’. When there is a ‘new arrival’ among acquaintances, she makes what she considers
to be appropriate social noises and finds her attention quickly turns to other matters. She and her
husband separated nine months after the death of their child, and she has not had the energy to
invest in another close relationship since. When the television news announced an enquiry into
child deaths at the hospital, they posted a phone number. She debated for days with herself and then
on an impulse rang the number. The social worker who took her phone call invited her to see her. At
the subsequent interview, it was explained that there were no indications of any links between the
news story and her child, but that sometimes people appreciated the time to talk about what had
happened. In her meeting with the social worker, she talked for the first time about the events that
seemed burned into her memory. The day after the birth, she was allowed to see her baby for two
periods of thirty minutes each. On the morning of the next day she was informed that her baby had
died during the night, and she didn’t see her body. ‘They tried to shield me,’ she said. ‘As if she was
something ugly. But they didn’t understand. To me she was beautiful. She was mine. And I never even
gave her name. I just call her “my angel”. I blame them. I wish I had given her a name. They just sent
me home. No one spoke to me.’ On the whole, she kept her composure during this meeting. But the
social worker sometimes felt her own eyes filling up, and felt unsure whether this was acceptable.
Occasionally there were long pauses, too, and the social worker didn’t know if she should say
something. In truth, she didn’t know what to say. At the end, she asked, ‘Shall we fix another meeting?’
There was no uncertainty in Beryl’s response as she nodded agreement and took a small diary from her
handbag.
256
Dying, grief and mourning
of 85 (note that because there are different numbers of people in each of these age bands, one can
compare rates, but not add two rates together). Life expectancy at birth in the UK in 2006 was 81 for
women and 77 for men. Mortality statistics can sometimes surprise people – a person has to be several
years over 100 before the chances are that half of the people in their age group will have died within
the year (all statistics from the Office of National Statistics – ONS, 2007c). Using UNICEF data on infant
mortality, Collison and colleagues (2007) quote the mortality rate for children under school age in the
UK as about five in 1,000, in a study that they say confirms the strong statistical link previously found
between relative inequality and child death in wealthy nations. To illustrate this, Sweden has half the
differential between the highest and lowest incomes compared with Britain and half the infant
mortality rate. There are a number of significant differences in the causes and numbers of infant
mortality between different ethnic minority groups in England – the rates for people who define
themselves as Asian or Black Caribbean are twice those for people who define themselves as white
British. The causes of death are also very different – twice as many babies died from congenital
abnormalities in Asian families, and 50 per cent more from low birth weight or early birth in Caribbean
families (ONS, 2007d; see also ONS, 2008c).
According to UK official statistics (ONS, 2007c: table 17) over three-quarters of all deaths take place
in a hospital or other institution away from home. The Department of Health (Department of Health,
2005: 40, table 64) quotes research that shows a large discrepancy between the wishes of the public
and current practice concerning where people die. Whilst there may be staff from other professions
paying attention to this, it is clearly an area of core social work responsibility, involving psychosocial
needs and practicalities. Munday and colleagues (2007) discuss the issues involved in paying greater
respect for the wishes of people in these last stages of their development – which include funding
issues, service availability, policy direction, and the competence and confidence of staff to hold (and
sometimes initiate) the appropriate conversation with older people.
Having choice and control over where death occurs is one of the requirements for a ‘good death’ as
identified by Age Concern – see box below.
257
Dying, grief and mourning
• To have choice and control over where death occurs (at home or elsewhere).
• To be able to leave when it is time to go, and not to have life prolonged pointlessly.
(Age Concern, 1999)
If you consider the features listed in the box above, it can be seen that some are purely medical. Most,
however, are psychosocial and are therefore matters a social worker may have the responsibility to
identify and implement. There is not the space here to discuss each in detail, but it is worth spending
a few minutes to consider the information people need. This will come up in form of questions, but
sometimes the social worker must create a conversational opening for these to be asked. The social
worker may or may not be the person to give the answers, but should always be alert to the
psychosocial issues involved.
258
Dying, grief and mourning
expect of their children after the bereavement, and how much to involve them in the death. The social
worker needs to understand, as Monroe puts it, that helping to provide this information is as much a
question of listening as of telling. What is certain is that asking these questions and assimilating the
resulting information is a part of development around the time of death.
?
Reflective thinking
The social worker felt tears in her eyes as she listened to Beryl.
• Is this under the social worker’s control?
• Is it wrong?
• What are some of the possible impacts on the person she is listening to?
• Is it something to be discussed in supervision?
• What are the requirements of supervision for this discussion to be possible?
Particularly when there is access to hospice care, many people experience a ‘good death’. However, for
whatever reason, death may be fundamentally unwelcome, and the idea of ‘a good death’ should not
be idealised. Social workers in all fields are likely to be involved with ‘the person-in-the-situation’ (with
the dying or the bereaved individual) precisely where there are complications, when the situation is
additionally disturbing.
Deaths may be particularly difficult for many reasons. Untimely deaths, where the child dies before the
parent for example, will seem to upset the natural order of things. There are insoluble aspects to the
situation where the person who is dying has a young child. There are similar complications when there
is an adult son or daughter with learning disabilities who has cognitive difficulties in grasping what
has happened, and for whom the bereavement has serious consequences for day-to-day living
arrangements. Deaths which occur suddenly are likely to cause additional emotional complications, as
are those where the manner of death is particularly distressing, as with road traffic accidents, murder
or suicide. And in every age there are people who suffer ‘disenfranchised grief’ – grief in a relationship
which is ignored, disapproved of or misunderstood by wider society. This may occur when the
relationship is illicit (with a person married to someone else, for example, or with a teacher or pastor),
or is an unacknowledged homosexual relationship, or when the person who experienced the loss is
convicted of having caused it by murder or neglect.
In learning disability services, mental health, family support or work with older people, the social
worker is often involved with the dying person and their family before, during and after the death.
Where there is continuing psychological support required, the social worker will often (but not
259
Dying, grief and mourning
necessarily) be the provider of choice for the bereaved, because of the relationship and understanding
already established. As in other aspects of social work, they may provide both practical and emotional
support, or may focus almost entirely on the interpersonal support to be offered.
But there are others who attempt the difficult task of identifying common patterns in this area of
?
human development. Becoming familiar with their work may sometimes give you words and ideas
which will be found helpful by those with whom you work. It will give you appropriate concepts to
consider in your writing, and enable you to build your understanding on the outcome of sensitive and
persistent research into the subject.
This part of the chapter first looks at the application of models introduced earlier in the
book – the humanistic model (Chapter 5 and ‘Essential background’, section 7), psy-
chodynamic theories (Chapter 2 and ‘Essential background’, section 4), and attachment
Chapters
2, 5, EB2, theory (Chapter 2 and ‘Essential background’, section 2). Following sections then analyse
EB4, EB7 the issues around death and grief using three models: Kubler-Ross’ model, which refers
to stages of dealing with death; the work of Stroeb and Schut, who emphasised how
people have two perspectives in their life after bereavement – one looking forward and the other
looking back; and the approach of William Worden, who presents a description of the tasks which face
bereaved people.
260
Dying, grief and mourning
?
Reflective thinking
The first paragraph of this section stated, ‘There can scarcely be any situation presented to social
workers which does not involve loss’.
Choose one example, perhaps from your own experience or that of one of your fellow students.
Write a paragraph which contains a sentence or two describing the loss (for example, ‘Every time
a child in care moves placement, he or she has to get used to a new bedroom, new carers, new
people to share the house’), a brief reference to the ways in which social workers and other
welfare staff can be helpful or unhelpful, and an explanation of why supportive supervision for
these staff members is necessary. The ‘loss’ may not be in the present – social workers repeatedly
?
provide service for people who have multiple or poorly handled losses in the past.
Bob
Bob has been in the hospice for three weeks now. It is increasingly difficult for him to
retain any food, and the doctors are clear that he is unlikely to live another fortnight.
His granddaughter Sharon is unaware of anything about him, let alone that he is dying.
Bella has not seen her father for twenty-five years, and has never spoken to her daughter
about him. The final rift came when Bella, aged 19, came home at half past midnight
from the cinema. In his blustering, imperious way, Bob let loose a tirade of vitriolic
criticism. As she tried to explain what had happened – ‘Don’t talk back to me, young lady,’
he said as he pushed her against the wall. ‘If you live in my house, you do as I say.’ He
blamed the lateness on her boyfriend, George, of whom he violently disapproved. He said
his daughter was becoming no better than a slut, and he had a respectable house to
261
Dying, grief and mourning
maintain. A girl’s place was to obey her father until she settled down with a responsible
man who would cherish her like his own flesh.
Bob was repeating out loud arguments he had been rehearsing in his mind for weeks, and
venting his fury at being unable to control his daughter. Using a modified version of
tactics which had kept him in line in the army, he was expressing the views which had
been rigidly instilled in him in his upbringing.
For Bella, this was the last thing she was prepared to tolerate. When she stormed out
this time, she went back to George, and she did not return. The only message which
reached her from her father was for her to stay away until she knew better, and she took
him at his word.
The hospice nurses were quite fond of the old-fashioned 70-year-old with his tales of
the army and his feisty spirit. Picking up his distress about his estrangement from his
daughter (but knowing nothing of the details), they had put him in touch with the social
worker.
As usual, the social worker was under pressure from a great volume of work. But she put
lots of energy into tracing Bob’s daughter, and after a number of frustrating attempts,
made a telephone call on Wednesday evening arranging to meet Bella at her home, 40
miles from the hospice, on Friday morning. At the end, Bella thanked her for taking the
trouble to find her and give her the news of her father, saying she would think about it
over the weekend and ring her on Monday.
On Monday Bella rang the social worker to explain that it had been painful for her to sort
out what had happened in her childhood. Having found a way to deal with how her father
had treated her, she could not face reopening the matter now. She had her own way of
coping with what had happened; she had no feelings for her father, and did not want to
meet him.
The social worker put the phone down and sat there. All the extra pressure she had put
herself under last week – the stress she had created for herself, the evening phone
calls – had come to this. She swore out loud, a single expletive. It crossed her mind that
if she said nothing, Bob would not know it had been resolved so quickly and, given his
condition, time would resolve the situation without her having to deliver this final
message of rejection.
That afternoon, she sat beside Bob’s bed. His drip had just been changed, and the
dressings that covered him from his neck to his abdomen were more bulky than before.
‘You’ve spoken to Bella.’ His words were a statement, not a question. His eyes focused
on hers and after a moment or two she put her hand over his, which was lying on top of
the sheet . . .
262
Dying, grief and mourning
During the conversation which followed, Bob said that he had always felt guilty about
how he had treated Bella. There was little talk in this ‘conversation’ and long pauses. ‘We
were brought up you . . . had to do what you were told,’ he said. ‘I didn’t know what to do
with her . . . It wasn’t right, though. In the end, it didn’t matter.’
He meant that in the end, there were more important things than whether a person in
authority could force another person to bend to their will. Relationships and care, human
connectedness, were more important.
?
‘You aren’t responsible for how you’re brought up,’ said the social worker.
‘I didn’t know any better,’ said Bob. ‘No one told me.’ There was a pause. ‘No one . . . told
me.’
The social worker had met Sharon’s mother (Bob’s daughter) and knew how destructive his
behaviour had been. Perhaps she thought of the cruelty and rejection he had displayed, but
accepted that it came from his view of the world at that time. Her ‘congruence’, in Rogers’
Chapter
EB7 terms (‘Essential background’, section 7), was to respond accurately to his experience. The
interview was potentially very difficult. She had been frustrated because she had wasted
time and effort; she had failed to deliver what he wanted (a meeting with his daughter), and did not
know how this additional upset would affect his precarious physical condition. If she had analysed it
?
later in supervision, she could have recognised the value and meaning in the simple fact of human
contact, the value of comfort which came from the physical contact of her hand on his; and the
recognition that, whilst only he could know the regret and guilt which belonged to his actions, he was
entitled not to blame himself for how he was brought up, for not knowing things about relationships
which he did not know. Specifically (see Chapter 3) we could analyse this as lessening
punitive self-criticism. As we shall discuss in Chapter 10, the social worker would not have
known in advance that this was what was needed, and is unlikely to have ‘theorised’ while
Chapters she was with him (although being able to put it into words after one occasion may well
3 and 10 strengthen her for the other ambiguous situations she would encounter in her work).
Does this have anything to do with human development which culminates in the high-sounding
humanistic phrases ‘self-actualisation’ or ‘fully functioning person’? Well, the social worker had failed
in the practical task Bob had requested, but had given him some time of value – in which he had said
out loud, to someone who understood, what really mattered to him, and spoken of his profound regret
at his part in the destruction of a relationship which was part of the true meaning of life. Perhaps in
a day or two she would phone Bella back and ask if she wanted to hear about her father’s last days
and have a word about what had happened with her dad.
263
?
Dying, grief and mourning
Psychodynamic approaches
There is little in the previous paragraphs (about humanistic approaches) which would
seem out of place in a psychodynamic perspective. However, there would be some
characteristic additions. Earlier states of mind (Chapter 2 and ‘Essential background’,
Chapters
2 and EB4 section 4) may be presented in the here and now. It is not infantilising to understand that
the comfort offered to a man may be essentially the same as that offered to a baby boy
(or whatever age-appropriate state of mind a client such as Bob may bring to the encounter). In dealing
with the last stages of life or bereavement, the themes present to a psychoanalytic perspective are not
new. Themes of death may in earlier states of mind be unconscious – present in the mind, but shut off
from consciousness. The teenager’s pursuit of thrills in driving recklessly, or the enjoyment of ‘cops and
robbers’ films, may be a denial, an avoidance, of the reality of death and bodily maiming in favour of
a fantasy of immortality; and, indeed, one criticism of humanistic psychology is that its resounding
affirmation of life and growth is a denial of death, decay and destructiveness, a firm shutting out of
this reality. From the beginning of life, and in all its stages, the developing self has to deal with the fear
(and sometimes the realistic proximity) of its own destruction. The approach of an individual’s own
death is linked to the way in which they have previously dealt with the death or other loss of those
close to them.
According to this approach, the loss of a loved object has been experienced since infancy, when the
baby cried for someone to come when it woke up alone at night. Relevant incidents thread through
life, inevitable, but different for each individual – the first day at school, the first weeks of a student
alone at college, the experience of being ‘dumped’ in a relationship. The external reality and degree of
finality is different, but the individual’s psyche has nevertheless been finding ways to deal with the
absence of someone who seems essential for survival.
In relation to the action of the social worker in placing her hand over her client’s – or in physically
comforting a bereaved relative by holding their hand or putting an arm around their shoulder – note
that different psychoanalytic practitioners take different views. Some (particularly, but not only, social
workers) find the physical demonstration of care in this situation as straightforward, the cue to its
presence or avoidance being in the state of the client. Other psychoanalytic thinkers would see such
contact as inappropriate, an unrealistic attempt by the worker to take away inevitable pain. They might
argue that it arises at some level from the worker’s own needs, and is not an appropriate part of the
relationship between professional and service user.
The standard psychoanalytic understanding is that in grief, the person who has lost the object of
their love must undertake the task of detaching the energy invested in that person. This frees them to
attach it elsewhere. Many psychoanalytic commentators would expect that avoiding the expression of
grief may prolong its effects and lead to complicated or pathological patterns later. It is true that this
can sometimes happen – a social worker may need to listen and be with someone who says, ‘I just wish
I hadn’t been so busy and unable to grieve at the time – I feel it’s just made it worse later; it’s
unfinished, not sorted out.’ And the person my gain relief from expressing the feelings they would have
264
Dying, grief and mourning
liked to have presented at the time of the loss. But there are many experienced practitioners who
believe it is profoundly misleading to think that the avoidance of grief expressions at the time of a
significant loss necessarily causes problems later on. Evidence supporting this view is cited by Boyd
?
and Bee (2006: 538).
?
Reflective thinking
Loss is a simple fact – the deprivation of something or the failure to get something.
Grief is a set of responses to loss – feelings and the expression of those feelings, a process people
go through.
Does all loss result in grief? What are some of the components of grief, feelings that may be
experienced or expressed?
265
Dying, grief and mourning
?
Reflective thinking
‘She helped them to listen to their patients and not act out of their own discomfort or guilt or
technical priorities’ (see paragraph above).
Thinking of the characteristics of a good death as identified by Age Concern (see box page 257),
in what way might social workers or medical staff act out of their own discomfort or guilt or
technical priorities, instead of attending to the needs of someone who is going to die?
Obviously, there are many possible answers to this. You could point out:
• A social worker may be unsure of how to talk about death with a person whose needs they
are assessing for discharge from hospital – so they don’t enquire where the person wishes
to die.
• Nurses may concentrate on their clinical tasks of medication and hygiene instead of taking
the time to listen.
• Doctors may treat the person as a medical challenge and keep them alive without asking
what the person wants.
• Social workers may keep rigidly to an assessment schedule about accommodation, activities
of daily living, finance and independence, and not take the time to listen to the person’s view
of their situation.
•
?
The topic may be avoided because the staff member is embarassed, or the worker may
think it is helpful to ‘cheer the patient up’ because it is too painful to stay with the real
feelings.
Kubler-Ross found that it is common for people to react initially with shock (you will recall
from Chapter 4 that Hopson also identified this as the first reaction to life transitions in
general – see Adams et al., 1976). In this phase they may find it hard to take the
Chapter 4
information in, may think there has been a mistake (perhaps the medical records have
266
Dying, grief and mourning
been mixed up), may find their ability to concentrate impaired, and have difficulty making decisions or
managing their affairs.
?
This is followed by a phase of anger – perhaps directed at the people who conveyed the diagnosis, at
people whom the individual blames for causing the illness, at friends and acquaintances, at ‘God’ or
‘fate’ for being so harsh.
The phases she identifies are set out in ‘Essential background’, section 9. If full progress is
made, the final phase is one of acceptance, in which the person is at ease with the reality
of life, and can deal appropriately with tasks and relationships. This includes making
Chapter
EB9 appropriate financial arrangements, particularly as regards any dependants. It also means
having a realistic sense of how to end close relationships – to say goodbye, exchange
mementoes, to make space for things which need to be said but were not said earlier in life. It involves
appreciating the needs of the other party in the relationship, who must face the loss. This too has
echoes in Hopson’s final phase of transitions, where he describes people coming to an understanding
of their life and its meaning.
Kubler-Ross also applied her model to the experience of loss and bereavement. The bereaved person
also goes through phases of shock, disbelief, guilt, sadness and so on – and if conditions are right, will
finally reach acceptance.
This view highlights the varied support needed by a social worker working with people who are dying
(or bereaved). If someone is in shock, and failing to cope appropriately with some of their affairs, it can
be difficult to decide how much to do for them (or encourage friends or relatives to do), or whether it
is more respectful of their independence to leave them to manage (that is, not manage) their own
business. If someone disbelieves reality, the worker must decide how much to confront them, and
implicitly dispute with them, about the situation; whereas when the person is accepting, it may be the
worker’s denial which is the challenge (‘Don’t give up hope,’ or, ‘Don’t say that,’ she may want to say
when the individual says they don’t need to get a card for their daughter’s birthday or no longer need
their pension book). Each phase presents different challenges for staff.
Having the words of Kubler-Ross available can help staff to respond appropriately. If incompetence (in
shock) or disbelief or unreasonable anger is understood as part of an expected stage, then responses
may be more realistic. Otherwise the behaviour may be experienced as a personality trait of the indi-
vidual; as undesirable behaviour to be managed. It is important to recognise that the dying person may
come to an ‘acceptance’ which is beyond the social worker’s own capacity; it may make them less likely
to respond with ‘cheering up’ or ‘denial’.
267
Dying, grief and mourning
A rigid scheme?
Kubler-Ross emphasised that she did not regard the phases as part of a fixed scheme. She said people
may move through them in a different order, or may move backwards and forwards, revisiting expe-
riences they thought had gone away. One advantage of this framework is that it helps others not to
overreact or pathologise the experiences of the bereaved and those facing the end of their life. They
can be reassured that depression or guilt, disbelief in the reality of what is happening, or anger at
people behaving in a genuinely helpful way, are natural responses. The model encourages staff not to
confront and psychologically struggle with the individual, but to stay with them through a difficult
and sometimes confusing journey. The difficulty for social workers is often that they are responsible
for different people whose needs and timescales conflict.
Twenty-eight years ago Bella was 17. Her mother, Gracie, was 43, and had just returned
to hospital two days before. The doctors were disappointed to find that her cancer had
metastasised and was spreading rapidly. They estimated that she would die soon.
Gracie, however, insisted (incorrectly) that the nurse had told her the most recent
treatments had been successful, and that she had come in for checks to be made on
her progress.
If anyone attempted to discuss her prognosis with her, she would say, ‘I’m very tired just
now,’ and bury herself beneath the bedclothes. Bella was a rebellious teenager who
resented and feared her father’s attitude. She took it out on her mum, blaming her for
doing so little around the house now that she claimed to be getting better, and for being
emotionally unavailable. Believing the words her mother told her, she came to the
conclusion that her mum was just being weak and lazy.
This would be a typical situation referred to a social worker because of ‘family issues’, although then, as
now, the pressure on the social worker would have been to give priority to discharge and so-called ‘bed-
blocking’ by other patients. The social worker would want to allow the teenager and mother to have a
conversation in which Bella can be told the facts, and mother and daughter can say some of the things
they need to say. But Gracie would not even speak about the end of her life – she was in a phase of denial.
A scenario in which the problems are even more acute is one in which the mother is a lone parent and
the children are only 11 or 12. As well as the emotional needs of mother and children, there may be
practical arrangements to make about care for the children after the mother has died.
There are a number of difficulties with this ‘stage’ scheme. Chaban (2000) alleges that Kubler-Ross
worked with patients only for a short time, and spent little time with them – directing her energies
instead towards her career as a writer and publicist of her work. Subsequent academic empirical
research has failed to validate the stages she identifies (Shneidman, 1995). It places much more
emphasis on individual variation, the effect of particular circumstances and the impact of social
context.
268
Dying, grief and mourning
Kubler-Ross’ work achieved great prominence, and has been taught widely on courses for nurses and
counsellors. Despite her view that the stages should not be regarded as ‘fixed’, bereaved people have
described how, if they didn’t go through the stages described, they were made to feel that there was
something wrong with them, that they hadn’t ‘grieved properly’. It is said that staff who receive a
briefing about the model in their training tend to regard it as a blueprint for what people ‘ought’ to
experience and try to fit people to the model. People who are deemed to be staying too long in one of
the phases or not ‘progressing’ may be described as experiencing ‘pathological grieving’.
Stage models are probably not the best way of understanding bereavement. In their work, Stroebe and
Schut (2001) describe the process by looking at the varying focus of the bereaved person’s emotional
energy – their ‘orientation’. William Worden (1992) presented an influential account which describes
the person as having to accomplish certain tasks.
LOSS RESTORATION
ORIENTATION ORIENTATION
Remembering Undertaking
the past new tasks
Pain/relief
Finding new
absence/
enjoyment
yearning
Figure 9.1
Dual process model of response to bereavement
269
Dying, grief and mourning
Bereavement, then, appears to be a pervasive part of growing up and for some it is not notably
problematic. The lasting pain or damage caused for others may only be truly evident several years later,
and the cross-cutting features of gender, social class, personal and family characteristics which are
relevant have not really been analysed adequately. Young people who experience multiple bereave-
ments or bereavements alongside other difficulties are most at risk of negative outcomes – in such
areas as education, depression, and risk-taking behaviour (Ribbens McCarthy and Jessop, 2005).
270
Dying, grief and mourning
Some studies suggest that many bereaved young people never talk to anyone about their experiences.
Friends and family can be either key sources of support or can contribute to additional subsequent
problems. There are a number of adults who may be in a position to pay appropriate attention, but
social workers may by virtue of their profession have specific day-to-day therapeutic relationship
responsibilities with many of the most vulnerable; they have ethical responsibilities and a professional
obligation to assess and provide for the continuing personal and social needs of the individual. Adults
in a routine and non-stigmatising role such as club leaders and teachers also have a particularly
important role to play. Some occupations involve only a specific and time-limited role (health staff
whose contact with the family ends with the death of a child, or a year-tutor in school). They may not
have a professional framework to take account of all the individuals in a social situation, or may be
providing the support as ‘befriending’, additional to their core role and socially recognised expertise.
?
This does not diminish the support received by the young person (indeed that may be enhanced), but
it can create a vulnerability or lack of confidence for the adult, and indicates the importance of
consultation and support for the professionals involved in these other roles about the important
emotional services they offer young people.
Many researchers from Piaget onwards (see Chapter 3 and ‘Essential background’, section
5) have studied the child’s concept of death. Orbach and colleagues (1988) concluded that
different aspects of ‘death’ (finality, causation and so on) are understood at different
Chapters
3 and EB5 stages, and that intelligence and anxiety as well as age are relevant factors. Current
research suggests children develop a concept of death about the age of 5, at the same
time as they begin to construct a biological model of how the human body functions to maintain ‘life’
(Slaughter, 2005). One drawback of the Piagetian approach is that it tends to assess children’s cog-
nitions as imperfect and immature versions of adult thinking. The ideological stance of children’s rights
advocates is much more that children’s views must be accepted as valid in their own right.
This has particular relevance to children facing their own death or developing wisdom and maturity
through their experience of the death of others. Valuing and listening to the voices of children can
convey a humbling wisdom and knowledge about the world and its values. This is not appreciated if
the adult is too aware of the ‘imperfections’ of childhood cognitions about death. That said, those
working with children must understand their cognitive framework in order to respond appropriately
and helpfully to their experience of death. This is equally true of work with adults who were bereaved
as children. What lodges in their mind may be their understanding at the age of bereavement, which
no one has thought to update in line with their age-developing powers of understanding.
‘My father died when I was 5,’ Anna told me. ‘I knew he was dead, but the last I saw him
was when he went to hospital, and somehow in my mind I thought he was still there. It
was only when I was 9 – I remember it vividly – that I realised he was dead; he was not in
the hospital, he had been buried at the funeral, and he could not come back.’
It may be the task of the social worker in the course of their conversations with the adult to allow them
to move on from their (now outdated) cognitions of the original event.
271
Dying, grief and mourning
Many people – parents themselves, social workers, counsellors, nurses and researchers – have com-
mented on the additional strain that the death of a child puts on the parental relationship. This has
sometimes been extended by implication to say that the bereavement causes an increased risk of
parental break-up. However, both Schwab (1998) and Murphy and colleagues (2003) found that
empirical study did not bear this out, the risk of divorce being no greater than in the general popu-
lation. The literature about bereavement increasingly identifies gender differences in patterns of
grieving, and it is thought that this is one component of increasing tension in the parental relationship.
One parent, for example, may respond to a death by looking for intimate physical comforting where
the other may feel this is inappropriate selfishness and a distraction from their grief. There is some
evidence using Stroeb and Schut’s scheme that women tend to focus on the loss orientation and men
on the restoration orientation, and this difference may lead one partner to feel that the other does not
really feel the loss or does not understand their grief.
Wider issues
Culture and context
Death, grief and their management are woven into a cultural fabric. One function of culture is that it
gives structure and meaning to the present. Rituals (whether at birth, adolescence, marriage or death)
can be understood as part of the mutual human resourcefulness to support the creation and maintenance
of meaning. Marris, an important researcher about death and bereavement, argued that death,
bereavement and loss are central to culture – in its structure and function, culture has to take account
of the constant passing away of the old, and has to be able to deal with the disruption when the ‘imagined
272
Dying, grief and mourning
future’ is suddenly torn away by bereavement (1974). This links with Seale’s view that ‘social and cultural
life can in the last analysis be understood as a social construction in the face of death’ (Seale, 1998: 211,
as quoted by Beckett, 2002). The converse is also true – Eisenbruch (1984a and b) identified the cultural
bereavement of refugees as taking away the framework which gave meaning to their lives and death.
Death and grieving, therefore, as part of human growth and development, cannot be understood
separately from their social and cultural context – dying is a social event.1 Biological death occurs when
the body dies; theorists distinguish this from social death, the point at which the person is treated like
a corpse and is recognised to have no living social presence (Boyd and Bee, 2006: 516). The culturally
competent social worker must have the sensitivity to tune in to the meaning of death for both the
individual and the social network that surrounds them.
Cultural ways of death are varied. Dominant Western practice regards the self as no longer present in
a corpse, but expects respect to be shown, even in private. It is satisfied that this can be accomplished
by strangers, and allows hospital and funeral staff to prepare the body. In some Islamic cultures, by
contrast, dressing the body after death is a family duty and privilege, and it would be a strange
abandonment for staff to be paid to do so. Contemporary Western culture takes the view expressed in
this book that people – whether the relatives or the person with a short time to live – may wish to talk
about their experiences, whereas a major Chinese cultural tradition regards doing so as impolite and
an invitation to bad fortune.
One part of understanding the cultural place of death is to understand the ontology of the culture or
religion – what is deemed to exist. In all the different Christian communions, each person has an
immortal soul, so that the death of the body does not mean the end of the person – indeed for
Catholics death may be seen as the end of a sojourn in ‘this vale of tears’. For Hindus, the person will
continue on their journey, to be manifested (‘reincarnated’) as a higher human or a repulsive animal,
partly depending on the manner in which the person is dealt with around the time of death. Many
bereaved people report seeing the dead person (Frankenburg, 1996: 3) and this has a different meaning
for a culture in which this is taken to be a real sighting of a person whose spirit (or self) lives on, from
a secular context in which it is treated as an hallucination or a psychological creation.
Only limited understanding, however, can be gleaned by reading about, or being told about another
culture, important though this general education is. In the first place, there is frequently no easy literal
translation of meanings between two cultures – every word and practice (‘relative’, ‘aunt’, ‘mourning’)
has meanings which can only be understood in relation to the lived practice of a host of other words.
Also, cultural and religious practices change over time and between different local communities. The
practices and expectations of an English Pakistani community, whose originators came in the 1970s,
may by 2009 be different from the practices current in their Pakistani village of origin. They may be
more conservative, Muslims and Hindus in the village being more intermarried than the expatriot
community would allow. They may be subtly different from related families who went to live in South
Wales, or Scotland. An Anglo-Indian community in the Punjab may have practices which are both
influenced by Punjabi life and more like the cultural habits of 1950s England.
273
Dying, grief and mourning
Finally, it is important to realise that the individual’s experience and beliefs in facing death or
bereavement may differ from those of their religion or culture. A lifelong Catholic may find that their
experiences leading up to death remove their belief in God and the afterlife; a man who has been
brought up to keep a ‘stiff upper lip’ and not to show emotion may feel at a sombre and quiet funeral
that he wants nothing so much as to give vent to his grief publicly and noisily. Conversely, a
West Indian woman at the death of her close friend may wish everyone would go away, stop all
the noisy celebrating and leave her in peace and privacy, to keep her emotions to herself. General
cultural statements and patterns, cultural expectations placed upon people, do not tell us about the
individual.
To explore this as a social worker, and to understand the journey of those in close proximity to death,
involves listening to clients, reflection, reading, thinking and discussion. The approach of the individ-
ual’s own death, or the loss of a relationship with a significant figure, can undermine the assumptions
which allow meaning to be created. Conversely, grief work can be understood as the route through
which meaning is re-established. As Worden put it, ‘Life can be meaningful once the cognitive and
emotional aspects of grief and love have been experienced’.
The young person may take someone’s life and vitality for granted. When this is taken away, the pro-
found pain and disturbance caused in the young person by the loss may well force them to think about
what they really value in life. The focus is on what really mattered in the person whose life was gone.
When my [great-grandfather] died I think it made me realise that I can’t waste time and seeing
the years were going by so quickly . . . cause of that it made me realise that I don’t have much
time to waste.
(Shirleen, quoted in McCarthy and Jessop, 2005)
The effect, as McCarthy and Jessop observe (2005: 3), can be either that young people create high ideals
for themselves, or that they become overwhelmed and demotivated. The effects, however, may have
significance over long periods of time and reinforce the need for continuing attentiveness on the part
of adults.
In the sixteenth century, Michel de Montaigne wrote a series of essays after the death of his father.
Perhaps it is fitting to end with words which have a very different sense depending on whether they
274
Dying, grief and mourning
are uttered by a young, vigorous person speaking to an elder or an older person imparting words of
wisdom: ‘Make way for others,’ advises Montaigne, ‘as others did for you. Imagine how much more
painful would be a life which lasts for ever’ (de Montaigne, Bk 1, essay 20, 1580/1993).
Understanding self
The subject of this chapter may be highly sensitive, for an experienced worker as much as for a student.
Its emotional impact is specific to the person, their sensitivities and their previous experience. The
subject matter may be particularly delicate because of current life events. As part of its review of the
subject matter of the whole book, the next chapter reflects on the need for staff (and students) to
receive competent and compassionate care for their feelings, and the need for them to understand
their own responses.
Summary
Experiences of death and grief are very diverse.
About ten in every 1,000 people die in the UK each year. Infant mortality varies between different
social groups, including between ethnic groups; this is related to relative inequality in wealth as
well as absolute levels.
There is a large difference between the expressed wishes of the public concerning where they would
like to die and the reality of where the majority of deaths take place.
Age Concern lists ten principles of a good death which include having control over pain, choice of
where to die, having questions answered and having access to spiritual or emotional support.
Humanistic models emphasise the importance of unconditional acceptance, empathy and con-
gruence. Psychodynamic models emphasise that ‘states of mind’ have been dealing with loss and
deprivation since birth, and originally conceptualised ‘grief work’ as detaching emotion invested in
the loved person. Attachment theory understands bereavement as a form of interruption to an
attachment relationship.
Kubler-Ross developed a stage model of the processes experienced by dying people, and has also
applied it to grieving. It describes five stages, which include shock, anger, denial and finally
acceptance. Kubler-Ross did not see these as a fixed sequence.
Stroeb and Schut do not describe stages – they analyse the person’s fluctuating emotional orientation
towards the past or towards the future. Worden identifies tasks which have to be accomplished –
cognitive tasks such as accepting the reality of the death, and emotional tasks such as accepting the
pain involved. When these tasks are accomplished, life can become meaningful again.
275
Dying, grief and mourning
Harrison and Harrington concluded that most young people are likely to experience a ‘significant’
bereavement before they are 16. They point out that this experience will be different for different
social groups, and that insufficient detail is known about it. For some it is not notably problematic,
but for others it may still cause difficulties years later, and attention should be paid to the needs of
young people. Factors such as intelligence and anxiety affect when the child develops an under-
standing of death, as well as the Piagetian idea of age-related cognition.
Dying and bereavement always have a cultural dimension to which social workers need to be sen-
sitive, whilst realising that an individual’s beliefs and experience will not necessarily follow cultural
norms.
Note
1 The isolated death of a socially unknown individual does not seem to be an exception to this, as it has an
important cultural meaning. The Beatles sang in 1964 of ‘Eleanor Rigby, died all alone and was buried along
with her name,’ and the city now has a bronze statue to her on one of its main streets; in 2008, the publicity
about the death of Olive Archer, who had been unvisited for many years in a nursing home, drew a national
response for fears that the funeral service would be attended only by the minister and the funeral director
(Salisbury Journal, 14 January 2008).
Further reading
About social work in palliative care:
Monroe, B. (2004) ‘Social work in palliative medicine’. In D. Doyle, G. Hanks, N. Cherny and K. Calman (eds), Oxford
Textbook of Palliative Medicine, 3rd edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 1007–1017.
276
Dying, grief and mourning
Turner, M. (1998) Talking with Children about Death and Dying – A Workbook. London: Jessica Kingsley.
We Connect People (Macmillan Cancer Support, 2007): free directory of support resources (including local groups)
for people with cancer and their families.
When Someone with Cancer is Dying (Macmillan Cancer Care, 2007) Also available as free download from
www.macmillan.org. Considers some commonly experienced anxieties about death and dying and lists sources
of further information and support. Covers issues such as communicating with family members, personal care,
last wishes and grief.
‘If you’re not going to recover’ (a website from www.cancerbackup.org.uk). Includes sections covering
‘preparation’ and ‘afterwards’, tackling questions facing people who are not going to recover. Framed about
cancer, but relevant for anyone facing their own death:
http://www.cancerbackup.org.uk/Resourcessupport/Relationshipscommunication/Talkingtochildren/Ifyouarenot
goingtorecover.
‘Bereavement’ – A leaflet from the mental health information site of the Royal College of Psychiatrists for ‘anyone
who has been bereaved, their family and friends, and anyone else who wants to learn more’:
http://www.rcpsych.ac.uk/mentalhealthinfoforall/problems/bereavement/bereavement.aspx.
Bereavement – leaflets and information available from Help the Aged: http://www.helptheaged.org.uk.
Cruse Bereavement Care (http://www.crusebereavementcare.org.uk): promotes the well-being of bereaved people.
Leaflets include ‘Advice for Older People’, ‘Helping children through Bereavement’.
SANDS (http://www.uk-sands.org/) is the Stillbirth and Neonatal Charity.
?
Questions
1 What are some of the words or phrases people use to avoid saying ‘dead’ or ‘dying’? What are some
of the reasons why they are used?
2 i. Use Bronfenbrenner’s model to describe briefly what you think may cause ‘a large discrepancy
between the wishes of the public and current practice concerning where people die’ (Dept of
Health, 2005).
ii. Expand briefly, in your own words, on what is meant by the following phrases from this chapter:
a. ‘people who are dying and those close to them may have many questions about death’.
b. ‘Stroebe and Schut considered that in the process of coming to terms with a loss, people find
they alternate between two contrasting orientations’.
c. ‘Worden analyses four tasks that face the bereaved person’.
d. ‘the need for staff (and students) to receive competent and compassionate care for their
feelings, and the need for them to understand their own responses’ (comment on why social
workers working with people who are dying (and their relatives) should have someone to talk
to about their emotions, and the sorts of matters that might be raised).
Choose one of the topics in question 2iia–e, and write a more extended dicussion, using your own further
reading and examples.
277
?
Dying, grief and mourning
278
In this chapter you will find:
10 Fitting the
pieces together
• Different kinds of knowledge about development
• concludes by outlining the ‘lifespan approach’ as a framework within which a number of other
approaches can be located.
How do you learn about human development? Social workers should learn from the people to whom
they provide service, through their own life experience, through reflective discussion and through pro-
fessional study. At its best, a course in human development in your professional training will introduce
you to the conversations, literature and concepts through which to tackle the subject effectively. A
moment’s reflection will indicate that these different methods can create different kinds of knowledge.
?
the results. For the moment we can call this ‘objective knowledge’ – the discipline treats people and
their development as outside objects of study. Understanding what goes into creating this ‘objective’
knowledge turns out not to be straightforward – it is the subject of the philosophy of science and also
of social science research methods.
Other knowledge does not arise in this way. A mother may know correctly that her baby
is troubled (or know what troubles it) when a developmental scientist does not. In the
example used in Chapter 9, the social worker had no ‘objective’ knowledge that it was right
Chapter 9
for Bob’s development at the end of his life for her to put her hand on his. Often, what
we can call this ‘subjective’ knowledge is something like ‘How would I feel if I were in the other person’s
situation? What would have led up to this, and what would be most helpful as a response?’ It is unlikely
that the worker would think explicitly like this in the interview, but it is an appropriate way to describe
the thought processes. It may be one relevant part of a discussion in supervision afterwards.
281
Fitting the pieces together
Clearly, unlike objective knowledge, this subjective knowledge is not based on observations that can
be reliably replicated by others. It arises from experiencing oneself and the other person as a ‘subject’.
What I have here called ‘objective’ and ‘subjective’ knowledge arise from different methods of human
enquiry, but I am not suggesting that the two are in totally separate categories. Researches based on
replicability and falsifiability (often called empirical methods) may well be extended by further enquiries
examining more subjective aspects of the topic. Qualitative research methods are explicitly designed
?
as reliable ways to explore subjective matters. As a professional person, the social worker’s ‘subjective’
judgement on a particular occasion may have been informed by supervision, discussions, seminars and
research findings. The philosophical examination of knowledge, what it is and how it is validated, is
known as epistemology.
There are a number of well-established criteria for establishing objective validity, which you will
examine in later study about research methods. Questions relevant to this appraisal are: ‘What are the
studies on which the theory is based, and have they been subsequently replicated?’ There are detailed
questions to ask about the studies, such as the method by which the study sample was chosen and
whether it was biased towards a particular age group, social class, gender or geographical area. Key
points are whether the method was appropriate to the research question, and whether the researcher’s
conclusion is justified by the findings. Different types of study – observation, experiment, cohort
studies – all have different characteristics, with different critical questions to be asked. There are then
related questions to ask of a theory or model which is claimed to be based on the studies. The
evaluation of qualitative studies, which might explore how people make sense of some life experience,
for example, is less clear-cut. There is a varied body of literature and much debate on what can be taken
to constitute ‘rigour’ for these studies (Guba and Lincoln, 2005). When you consult reference books
about the theories discussed in this book, any statements about their strengths and weaknesses are
likely to place much emphasis on these ways of evaluating studies and theories. In using research
findings in practice, the question is then, ‘Is the finding or theory relevant to the service users I am
considering?’
All this means that there are, in principle, readily understood ways of checking the analysis of ‘objective
knowledge’. The appropriate interpretation of what I have called ‘subjective knowledge’, on the other
282
Fitting the pieces together
hand, raises many questions. One key implication relates to the importance of self-knowledge, and this
is the subject of the next section.
Understanding self
In using ‘subjective’ knowledge, one’s own life experience is brought to bear on the life and
development of someone else. This requires a combination of caution and confidence. Confidence in
one’s perceptions and responses, but also caution that personal experience may be an unhelpful guide
to the experience of someone else.
In the immediate interaction, a social worker’s main focus of attention will be on the person with whom
they are working. However, in many situations social workers will also have powerful feelings of their
own, and it is much better that they should give free and accurate attention to these rather than allow
them to be an unrecognised influence on how they behave. Mature workers, indeed, may develop the
ability to hold an explicit awareness of themselves while they are paying attention to their client.
Sometimes the worker’s feelings may be important indicators of the emotional (as distinct from the
verbal) interaction.
Self-awareness may not be straightforward. Some feelings may be kept out of awareness because
they seem unacceptable. They may be suppressed as unsuitable for a man (or woman) or for a profes-
sional person; or they may be disowned, even to the self, because they conflict with the self-image.
Disentangling the relative effects of self-generated stressors and external pressures can be confusing.
It can sometimes be hard to know if particular experiences have a physical or psychological origin.
Current experience may be strongly coloured by influences earlier in development – once again, present
‘states of mind’ have specific echoes from earlier times. Everyone is capable of self-deception.
There are, no doubt, many routes to self-knowledge. One of them is for the worker to make use of help
for personal difficulties, especially for those which arise directly from work. In counselling and psy-
chotherapy, it is taken for granted that the provision of this help is an essential component of training.
Attitudes in social work are sometimes more mixed, even though social workers may have intimate
and continuing involvement with the people and situations which are the most difficult in society.
If this help is offered in a developmental way, the worker can learn much about themselves and the
process of helping. It is important to be clear that using personal assistance for difficulties is not a sign
of weakness or unsuitability. Mattinson (1975) explores how the very dilemmas faced by clients will
often be precisely those which the worker experiences – and taking it a stage further, how they will in
turn be the issues which confront the supervisor. The converse is also true, that when the supervisor
effectively tackles the problem experienced by the worker, that may be the very support which enables
the worker to be helpful with the person to whom they provide service.
283
Fitting the pieces together
Three examples
Like everyone else, social workers in their own development have needs:
• ?
for assistance about feelings of self-reproach or depression.
Here are three brief examples, referred to in the course of the book, which illustrate the link between
careful support and self-knowledge. They expand in turn on the above bullet points.
Chapter 9 referred to the work of a social worker in a children’s hospital. Parents of a 10-
year-old asked the social worker to be present when their daughter’s life-support machine
was switched off. The social worker helped with arranging the room, choice of time of day,
Chapter 9
whether they wanted incense burning, whether they wanted flowers or music, and so on.
During that day she may have been composed and supportive, but perhaps tears came to her eyes at
the most emotional moments. She had known the child and her parents for several years. It was a day
of intense emotion. It would not be surprising if afterwards, in supervision, she wept silently for a while
as she described what had happened. If the supervisor is competent, there would be nothing
inappropriate or out of place in this. The opportunity for expressing her feelings, along with the
response by the supervisor and perhaps some reflective discussion afterwards, contribute to her sense
?
of security about her emotions and their appropriateness. They are one part of what equips her not to
shield herself from her emotional responses at work, to understand what is going on for herself in
difficult situations, and indeed to allow her energies to be focused on the needs of her clients when
she is with them.
In Chapter 4, I quoted the words of ‘Emma’, who at the age of 21 had kept in touch with
Ann-Marie, her former foster carer – ‘I speak to her on the phone I guess every couple of
weeks.’ But when they had first met, Emma had been in the children’s home where Ann-
Chapter 4
Marie worked – ‘everyone was my enemy . . . me and Ann-Marie never used to get on, I used
to hate Ann-Marie. I have threatened Ann-Marie with a knife, I’ve bitten her and everything because I
didn’t like her.’ What emotions might Ann-Marie have reported in supervision? Perhaps her dislike of
the girl, her fury at Emma’s lack of gratitude and the insolent way she behaved; how, for all her
experience and training, she felt the urge to respond to the provocation, felt impulses of retaliation
and aggression towards Emma. And then perhaps she’d end by saying how much she enjoyed going
shopping with Emma, how much fun she could be, reflecting that she just wished she could understand
what troubled her. Perhaps this supervision would bring home in a vivid way that powerful feelings of
one sort can conceal their opposites. And it was with Ann-Marie that Emma started talking about the
abuse she had suffered when she lived with her father – abuse she said she had never talked about
with anyone. Perhaps she needed to know that she was talking with someone who knew how to
contain their own feelings of violence and hatred.
284
?
Fitting the pieces together
Some troubles and needs are not about external relationships. As discussed at the end of
Chapter 3, people doubt themselves, or are punitive towards themselves – they have a
relationship with themselves as well as with other people. External reassurance will not
Chapter 3
remove that doubt – ‘it doesn’t matter what you say, I will still feel responsible’. Social work
has a particular tendency to trigger self-doubt in staff, and, once again, appropriate support not only
tackles the immediate difficulty – if offered in a developmental way, it leaves staff with a greater
understanding of the related issues faced by people they work with.
A lack of self-awareness, on the other hand, can cause problems. The worker who is unclear about their
own needs may be intrusive, or controlling, or too vulnerable to self-doubt. If their own development
is unexamined, they may make clumsy and inaccurate assumptions about other people’s lives. The
influence of culture on development can be surprisingly invisible. To act in a culturally competent way,
a worker has to be reflective and self-aware of these influences, and we are all required to recognise
the extent to which the values and beliefs we hold may be culturally (or gender) specific.
Social workers are involved with intimate matters in people’s lives, and they inescapably draw on ‘sub-
jective’ knowledge. Whatever route they choose, they have a professional responsibility to pay as much
attention to this knowledge as to ‘objective’ and research-based knowledge.
An essential starting point in many academic subjects is a definition of the terms used. In this book, I
have taken a common-sense use of the word ‘development’ for granted. For further studies, however,
a closer look at the terms used may become essential. ‘Development’ can refer to a field of study (the
subject of this book) or it can refer to the progressive growth of an organism to maturity, a usage
common in biology. Similarly, ‘ageing’ can refer to a subject of study or to a process of decline after
peak maturity (again, the usage common in cell biology). There are a number of related concepts to be
examined, and they overlap with questions such as whether development proceeds by stages, and what
is implied by stage models.
‘Lifespan’ in itself refers to the period between conception and death. ‘Lifespan development’ is the
perspective developed in particular by Paul Baltes (1987) and now adopted in many reference and text
books, for example by Boyd and Bee (2006) and Sugarman (2001). It emphasises that development is
a constant process of change and adaptation to new contexts through the whole of life. This has a
number of implications:
285
Fitting the pieces together
Lifespan development focuses attention on the impact of different cultural and historical circum-
stances, highlighting the problems involved in searching for a ‘universal’ model of development.
Since it regards development as occurring throughout life, it contrasts with models which view
development as a process leading to a static (or declining) ‘maturity’.
It enlarges the range of academic disciplines which are concerned with ‘human development’.
Baltes was particularly concerned with development in older age. ‘Lifespan development’ draws
attention to the way changing life expectancy must change the discipline of ‘human growth and devel-
opment’. It makes subjects such as economics and anthropology (as well as biology, psychology and
sociology) relevant. It examines historical differences in development, and explores the two-way influ-
ences in such topics as brain development, culture and environment. Lifespan researchers, therefore:
In whatever field social workers are engaged, they have constantly to develop their knowledge. They
have to pay active attention both to new ‘objective’ knowledge developed by others, and to the
constantly changing subjective knowledge which is theirs alone.
?
Reflective thinking
The activity in Chapter 1 suggested that you draw a lifeline and think about biological, psy-
chological and sociological factors that are involved in development. Create your own lifeline
again, and this time label it with specific topics you have covered in your course.
Here is the outline using some section headings from this book. The left-hand labels are
specifically chronological, while the right-hand picks out concepts, theories and themes.
Use one of the topics as a prompt, and reflect on its application to your life (this is a private
exercise, and probably requires you to find a quiet, comfortable space and think reflectively, much
more than it requires you to write). Perhaps you will repeat the exercise choosing a different
theme.
A book such as this has to be selective, so many interesting themes and topics have been
neglected. Thinking about your life and your development, list some important events or themes
that, for you, are missing from the book – ethnicity? spirituality? increasing knowledge and
intellectual achievement through life? the impact of illness? drugs and alcohol? travel? Every
person you work with has their own special themes and influences.
286
Chapter 1: Family Attachment
circumstances about • Infancy, childhood
pregnancy and birth • Separations in adolescence,
in adult relationships;
Experience in the womb
bereavements
Chapter 2: A secure base Psychodynamic perspectives –
Birth
earlier states of mind may be
Brain development in infancy ............................
present in later experience;
– emotional factors conscious and unconscious factors;
Start
school internal conflict; self-attack
Infancy – before conscious
memories Erikson – psychosocial stages in life;
identity formation
Birth of a sibling
Living alone
.........................
Chapter 3: Early childhood Hopson’s theory of transitions
Things I didn’t Sex
Piaget and other ideas of
understand … Love
cognitive development,
schooling Children
Rocky patch
Gender differences
Chapter 4: Adolescence Death of children
Intended
Physical changes path Mental health
Class and income affect lifetime
Chapter 5: Living development
independently ............................. Violence and agression
Stigma
Chapter 6: Adulthood
Disengagement and activity theories
Chapter 7: Adulthood of ageing; continuity theory;
feminist perspectives
Chapter 8: Adulthood Understanding self
Lifespan theory – the impact of
Chapter 9: Death, dying and economics and history on lifetime
bereavement development
Fitting the pieces together
Further reading
Research methods in the social sciences:
Sarantakos, S. (2005) Social Research. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
Lifespan perspective:
Sugarman, L. (2001) Lifespan Development : Theories, Concepts and Interventions. Hove: Psychology Press.
Self-development and self-knowledge:
Howe, D. (2008) The Emotionally Intelligent Social Worker. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Wilson, K., et al. (2008) Social Work: An Introduction to Contemporary Practice. Harlow: Pearson. This textbook
emphasises that relationship-based practice requires knowledge that comes through self-awareness as well
as the knowledge that comes through ‘objective’ studies. Pages 8–12 introduce some ideas about ‘the use of
self’, and pages 95–106 discuss the nature of the knowledge which is used in social work.
Casement, P. (2008) Learning From Our Mistakes : Beyond Dogma in Psychoanalysis and Psychotherapy. New York:
Psychology Press. It is many years since Patrick Casement ended his work as a social worker and devoted his
time to psychoanalysis and psychotherapy. But his careful exploration of how it is that in professional
conversations our mistakes and misperceptions lead us back to a greater understanding of what we actually
need to understand is classic reading for all in professions concerned with human development.
?
Questions
1 Select a developmental theory or model. Outline its main features, and explain its strengths and
weaknesses. Use an example to illustrate its application in a social work situation.
Guidance: the theory or model may relate to the whole of life (such as Erikson’s psychosocial theory,
or attachment theory) or it may relate to one phase of life (such as William Worden’s approach to
bereavement).
2 ‘Continuity, change and diversity’: use this as an essay title to discuss an aspect of human
development.
Guidance: you may choose to discuss a model or theory, or to discuss a particular aspect or phase of
life. If you decide on the first, you might choose attachment theory, and discuss what its adherents
say about the continuity of attachment patterns over the life course, and about change and dis-
continuity; you could discuss diversity across different cultures and nationalities, including critiques
of attachment theory. On the other hand, if you choose to apply the title to a phase in life, you might
discuss adolescence, aspects of continuity in life before and afterwards, aspects of change, and areas
of diversity – say between males and females, or across different cultures or historical periods. Or you
could choose a concept such as ‘identity’ and look at areas of continuity, change and diversity.
3 ‘When and where you were born predicts more about your life course than do your genes.’ Discuss.
288
Fitting the pieces together
4 ‘Self-awareness is the first necessity on the part of those who seek to help others’ (Ferrard and
Hunnybun, The Caseworker’s Use of Relationship). Looking back to the example about the youth club
in Chapter 6, or the meeting with Bob in Chapter 9, or choosing an example of your own, discuss why
self-awareness is important and why it is not always straightforward.
5 Explicitly or implicitly, social workers are constantly assessing ‘human development’ (past, present and
potential future). Discuss how their assessment is influenced by:
i both conscious and unconscious factors in themselves;
ii both affective and cognitive factors in themselves;
iii their own social and cultural background.
?
289
Essential
Background
1 The principles of heredity 292
Size
If all of the DNA molecules in your body were uncoiled and laid end to end, it has been estimated that
they would reach to the moon and back 6,000 times.
There are more than 10 million million cells in your body – if all the oceans in the world were divided
into cups of water, there are more cells in one body than cups of water. The scale of the units we are
discussing is extremely small.
Babies combine ‘instructions’ from mother and father. Except for male sperm and female eggs, the cells
in the body always contain two of each of the twenty-three chromosomes making up the ‘instruction
set’. The sperm and egg contain only one set each, and when they combine to form the beginnings of
a new baby, this combined cell then has its double set – one from each parent. From then on, as this
cell divides and multiplies, the cells it produces all have chromosomes in pairs. The DNA in chromo-
somes is self-copying, so one of the chromosome pair contains the mother’s information and one
contains the father’s.
What happens when the instructions from the mother and father are different? The instructions from
the mother may be for brown eyes, but from the father for blue. What happens then? Well, simplifying
the situation, one version of the gene (signalling the creation of brown eyes) is ‘dominant’, whilst
another (for blue) is called ‘recessive’. If there are conflicting instructions, a dominant gene always
‘wins’. There will therefore be blue eyes only if the instructions from both mother and father are for
blue. In any other case, the brown-eyed signal will be put into effect. Another dominant gene is for
normal colour vision versus colour blindness, and another is for Huntington’s chorea (a distressing
neurological condition leading to early dementia and death). An example of a recessive gene is for
cystic fibrosis, which occurs in about one in 1,000 births.
Many genes display various types of ‘incomplete’ dominance and co-dominance. In these cases, one
form of the gene fails to mask all the effects of the other. In the central case, if the instructions are
additive, the final characteristics are half way between the two parental instructions – a white and red
292
1
carnation producing pink offspring, for example. Another option is that the instructions ‘multiply’ each
other, greatly intensifying the result of either signal on its own.
Most characteristics, particularly the psychosocial features at work in relationships, emotion and
behaviour, involve the operation of numerous genes in complex interactions with each other.
Human variation
It is random which of the pair of chromosomes (one from the mother, one from the father) is chosen
to be present on its own in the sperm or egg. With twenty-three pairs, this implies 223 or over 8 million
different combinations – each fertile person is capable of producing this number of different
eggs/sperm. Theoretically, therefore, there are 65 trillion different possible fertilised eggs combining
one of these from a father and one from a mother. In fact, because this is a simplified account, the
number of possibilities is even larger, and this is why each person has a unique genetic inheritance.
The one exception is when an existing fertilised egg splits identically into two. These form identical
twins, a feature of approximately one in 270 pregnancies.
293
1
Sex
Twenty-two of the chromosomal pairs are similar for males and females. Chromosome 23 is an excep-
tion, and it controls sex. It comes in two forms, labelled X or Y because of their shape. If the fertilised
egg has the combination XX, the individual is female; if it has the pair XY, the individual is male. The
form of the embryo is initially female for all offspring. If the Y chromosome is present, it initiates the
production of testosterone in the womb, and this heavy dose of testosterone causes the development
thereafter of male characteristics.
Some diseases are linked specifically to the X or Y chromosome.
294
1
?
differ on the strength of genetic influences in psychosocial behaviour, there can be no doubt that they
are important’ (Rutter, 2006: 6).
Genetics are referred to in: Chapter 1, where the final section is a discussion of the mode of interaction
of genes and environment; Chapter 7, in relation to mental health; Chapter 8, in relation to ageing.
295
2 Attachment theory
Core features of attachment theory
• Infants are pre-programmed to seek proximity to an attachment figure, usually a parent or sig-
nificant adult, who supplies comfort and protection from danger in times of distress, illness and
fatigue.
• In the absence of threat this attachment figure offers a secure base – from which the infant begins
to explore the (physical and emotional) environment.
• The hormone levels of the baby and its attachment figure co-regulate each other. Regulation of
infant affect (emotion) occurs through the attachment relationship(s).
• When parted from the attachment figure the infant responds with separation protest – expres-
sions of distress and aggression.
• The reciprocal relationship of the individual and the attachment figure is stored psychologically
in the form of an internal working model. This guides the child’s future assumptions about the
responsiveness, reliability, proximity and intimacy of those to whom the child feels close.
• The attachment dynamic does not end in infancy but continues throughout life in a latent form
and is particularly activated in adults at times of distress.
Attachment behaviours
Attachment behaviours are inborn behaviours which serve a function in establishing the attachment
relationship. They include smiling, crying, clinging and following, which act to keep adults within a
protective range. The ‘kewpie doll’ appearance – large forehead; chubby, protruding cheeks and so
on – also cause highly favourable reactions and make the infant appear cute or loveable. Adult
responses which are evoked and then reinforce these behaviours include touching, holding and
soothing.
After the attachment relationship has developed, separation from the attachment figure arouses the
child’s attachment system. Attachment behaviours then activated include loud protest, searching and
refusal to be comforted (consider the response of a child in a busy shopping centre, when it realises it
has inadvertently become separated from its mother).
296
2
Attachment relationships
An attachment relationship is defined by Bowlby as an emotional link, an ‘affectional bond’, between
two people which serves certain functions (Bowlby, 1979). Infant/caretaker attachments serve to
protect the young from prolonged discomfort and from predators. An attachment relationship for the
child only develops through experience with an appropriate adult, whereas the child is born with the
propensity to display attachment behaviours.
Howe and colleagues (1999: 19–21) draw on the work of Bowlby, Ainsworth and Schaffer to describe
four phases in the development of a child’s attachment, from early prosocial behaviour in newborns
to goal-corrected partnerships over the age of 3 years. In this later stage, different attachment figures
may serve different functions, and child and caregiver each consciously shape the nature of the other’s
attachment relationship (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Bowlby, 1979; Schaffer, 1996).
297
2
intended both to provoke the child’s motivation to explore and to arouse a degree of security-seeking.
The sequence begins with the child settled with its mother in an unfamiliar room. It includes a stranger
entering the room, the mother leaving and her return after a short absence.
There is broad agreement about the categories into which children’s responses fall, but the detail and
boundaries vary between researchers. A common current scheme describes:
• Secure attachment – these children show a balance between exploration and play; they notice
their mother leaving and their play activity is disrupted; they can be settled, but their routine again
changes as she re-enters.
• Ambivalent attachment – these children are ‘clingy’ to start with and affected by mother’s
leaving; their behaviour is again disrupted by her return, but they don’t settle. They constantly seek
her proximity, but are not settled by it. Howe describes this as the state in which the attachment
system is constantly aroused, but cannot be brought back to equilibrium.
• Disorganised attachment – the child shows a variety of confused and contradictory behaviours,
such as crying unexpectedly after having settled, or displaying a cold, frozen posture.
A procedure commonly used to identify internal working models in adolescence and adulthood is
the Adult Attachment Interview, developed by Mary Main and colleagues (1985). Researchers find that
even though there is some continuity, the internal working model is modified by actual experience in
attachment relationships.
Attachment behaviours tend to be activated particularly at times of illness or stress in adult life.
Response to separations
In examining the responses of children separated from their ordinary, ‘good-enough’ parents and not
allowed to have contact with them (for example, in some hospital regimes), and in examining the
responses of children in general to institutionalisation, Bowlby and his social work colleagues (see
Bowlby and Robertson, 1953) claimed to observe three phases.
298
2
First there is a phase of protest, which is healthy, but of course can be problematic for staff. There is a
phase of depression, when staff may mistakenly feel the child has ‘settled down’ and may prefer
parents to stay away ‘because he’s only more upset when you come’ – this is actually a sign that
attachment systems are still functioning, even though the child is deprived of the attachment figure
he needs. And if the child’s attachment distress is not attended to, there is a phase of detachment/
despair, when the attachment systems become deactivated. If this final stage is reached, the child may
again be more compliant with adults, but the child has learnt to manage relationships both in the
present and in the future without attachments. This is much harder to reverse.
?
There is a similar contrast with behavioural and learning theories. These conceptualise behaviour as
the result of rewards and punishments. Attachment theory, on the other hand, regards attachment
behaviours as inbuilt, not learned. Attachment relationships develop as particular adults respond to
these attachment behaviours. Bronfenbrenner (‘Essential background’, section 3 and Chapter 5)
regarded his ecological theory as contrasting with theories such as attachment, which
he saw as too individually based, placing too much emphasis on parent–child bonds and
too little emphasis on ecological systems. Most attachment theorists, however, would
Chapters have little difficulty in regarding the ecological model as a realistic way to describe the
5, EB3 multitude of influences on development, of which attachment bonds form a part.
Multiple attachments
One area of current interest is the exploration of children’s use of multiple attachments. For example,
it appears that in situations of shared caregiving, children form a hierarchy of attachments – looking
when distressed first for their chosen attachment figure and then working through the list of possible
attachments they have. Again, it appears that there may be patterns in the expectations and use
children have for different attachment figures (for example, mother, father and grandmother).
Criticisms
Attachment theory is widely used to make sense of development, some researchers using it more
systematically than others. Some psychologists have argued that it uses a single term to conflate a
range of social influences and systems on development (see Thompson, 2005). For example, attune-
ment is a measure of the degree to which a caregiver is openly responsive to the child’s changing
299
2
mindset and emotions, its inner world. Many attachment researchers refer to it as one component of
the attachment relationship. On the other hand, in her research Meins (2005; Meins et al., 2003) found
attunement to be an effective predictive factor in its own right, and separate from other variables
grouped into the concept ‘attachment’.
There have been critics of attachment theory who argue that it is Eurocentric, or that it is sexist (see,
for example, Burman, 2000). Feminists have argued that it is based on a particular orthodox ideology
about the role of children and women. There have been concerns that it is an instrument for ‘policing
motherhood’, specifically in the context of contemporary child protection activities.
From a different standpoint, Barth and colleagues (2005) argue that modern childcare social work
(particularly in relation to support of adoptive parents) is over-reliant on attachment thinking to the
detriment of other, more empirically based interventions. The prevalence of attributing the difficulties
which some adults face with children to postulated ‘attachment disorder’ has been a significant
concern among developmental psychologists. O’Connor and Nilson (2005), for example, describe the
?
consequent interventions described as ‘attachment therapy’ as based on a loose metaphor rather than
a proper understanding of attachments.
Attachment theory is referred to in: Chapter 1, implicitly on pages 11–12, about Mathew and about
Bella; Chapter 2, page 36, pages 37–39, describing brain development and describing the development
of ‘mind-mindedness’; Chapter 2, pages 54–61 – examining a variety of aspects of attachment theory
in more detail; and Chapter 9, referring to the way in which attachment theory has been used to shed
light on human bereavement and loss.
300
3 Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model
Uri Bronfenbrenner argued that development can only be understood by considering the developing
person and their surroundings together. A developmental relationship involving two people is a dyad,
and both members of the dyad influence each other – for example, a mother changes at the same time
as she has a baby and as her child grows. The change in her then affects the way her baby develops.
Bronfenbrenner contrasts this systems view with one in which the development of the individual is
considered on its own, as if it were independent of its environment. He considered that much research
about the development of children either does not specify the changing environment in which the
research took place, or takes place in a setting where the environment is in fact the psychologist’s
laboratory.
Theoretical ideas
Bronfenbrenner specified what should be counted as ‘development’ as distinct from ‘behaviour cur-
rently shown’. He describes his model in terms of ‘systems’. A ‘system’ is a collection of elements with
relationships which describe how one element affects the others. A system has a boundary, and its
behaviour may affect the behaviour of other systems through its outputs. It is in the nature of systems
that changing one element (in a system, or one subsystem of a larger system) may cause changes in
other elements or in the larger system as a whole. Understanding how systems behave is the subject
matter of the academic discipline called ‘systems theory’, which has been applied to subjects from plant
growth to economics and politics.
He regards the developing individual (say, a child) as part of various systems. For example, the child
may be a baby in a family comprised of two parents and an older brother. This same individual will be
part of other systems (for example, the class at school comprising a teacher, a teaching assistant and
thirty children). These are sometimes subsystems of a larger system – the sibling system of a younger
sister and older brother is a subsystem of the family; the class is a subsystem of the school. All the
systems mentioned in this paragraph are described by Bronfenbrenner as microsystems – the individual
is a part of these systems and is directly influenced by them.
301
3
Microsystems are themselves elements in broader systems, and Bronfenbrenner analyses the rela-
tionships between different microsystems as the next level of influence on the child. The child is
affected not just by the family and by the school, but also by how these microsystems relate to each
other. For one child, the school may understand the values and beliefs of the family and reinforces
them, while the family is in close contact with the school and works in tandem with the teachers. It
is a very different situation if the school and family are hostile to each other and hold different
beliefs and expectations. The system of relationships between the different microsystems is called by
Bronfenbrenner the mesosystem.
Next there are various systems of which the child is not even a part, but which nevertheless affect the
child’s development. An example would be a father or mother’s employment. Pressure or stress at work,
unemployment or promotion and expected hours of work will all affect the child. Bronfenbrenner
describes this system as the exosystem. How the children’s services are organised is an exosystem for
the child looked after by the local authority. Systems of parental support or religious community are
other examples of exosystems.
Mesosystems
Jane
Work Family
Large-scale systems of
society as a whole Macrosystem
Figure E3.1
Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model
302
3
Finally, Bronfenbrenner’s ecological system draws attention to the macrosystem of society as a whole
in which attitudes, events and interactions at the large-scale level eventually affect the individual.
The macrosystem includes cultural attitudes and the economy – how society views gender roles
will have an effect on the expectations, self-image and behaviour of girls and boys; a negative panic
about Islam may end up affecting the experience of a particular Muslim girl; national economic
prosperity or crisis will affect the individual child. Bronfenbrenner was responsible for a major national
project to support families with difficulties and regarded many attitudes and features of society as
destructive for parents and their children (Bronfenbrenner, 1990; US Department of Health and Human
Services, 2007).
These systems change over time, and the nature of that change has a significant effect on the indi-
vidual. Bronfenbrenner incorporated this attention to change over time by adding to his model the
idea of the chronosystem. When a family moves from Pakistan to Britain, there may be major changes
that have an important effect on the development of the child and, of course, the parent. The
chronosystem is the system of change over time.
303
3
newborn babies), and his model is deeply concerned with improving the care of children and support
for families.
?
Bronfenbrenner’s model could be used to summarise the underlying ideas of many parts of this book –
Chapter 1 in explaining the developmental differences for Nicola, Sharon and Naoko; Chapter 2 to 4 in
setting out influences on the child and adolescent; Chapter 6 in the account of sexual development as
involving biological, psychological and social influences; Chapter 7 in relation to mental health and to
‘spoiled identity’, both of which are presented as involving individual characteristics, face-to-face
interactions, and societal influences; the different theories of ageing in Chapter 8 place differential
emphases on aspects of the systems identified by Bronfenbrenner; and the experiences of dying and
bereavement are similarly affected by the different systems, from the micro- to the macro- and the
chronosystem. It is introduced explicitly in Chapter 5, at the end of which a more detailed example is
given of how it might be interpreted.
304
4 Psychoanalytic theories
Variety
There is no agreed definition of the terms ‘psychodynamic’ or ‘psychoanalytic’. Proponents argue about
different theories and how they should be described. The ideas have a ‘family resemblance’ to each
other, but no single set of core concepts is agreed by all. Many of the strands are named after the
person who was influential in putting forward the ideas – Freudian ideas after Sigmund Freud, Kleinian
theory after Melanie Klein, and so on. There are sometimes major disagreements in which one set of
thinkers regard another as fundamentally mistaken, even though both may appropriately be described
as ‘psychodynamic’.
To use the terms appropriately requires reading the literature and working alongside psychoanalytic
or psychodynamic practitioners. Until you are confident and authoritative, make it clear in your writing
whose terms you are using.
Unconscious motivations
All psychodynamic and psychoanalytic thinkers regard actions and experiences as strongly influenced
by forces which are not in conscious awareness. Some consider that these unconscious forces and
conflicts can be brought into consciousness, with consequent improvements for self-awareness and
emotional integration. Others regard ‘the unconscious’ as intrinsically and inevitably lying beyond
consciousness – there will be forces whose effects can be seen, and whose content can be deduced
but never directly brought into consciousness.
Unconscious forces can be dramatically contradictory – for example, at the unconscious level a person
may want to be totally self-determined and independent, but also may want to be totally dependent
and have someone else take all responsibility and make everything right. Unconscious motivations may
be quite contrary to conscious behaviour and demeanour – for example, a person may behave and be
aware of themselves as considerate and obliging, and yet their unconscious impulses may be ruthlessly
self-seeking and ruthlessly antagonistic to anyone who thwarts them.
Conflicting motives
So it will be no surprise that psychodynamic thinkers take the presence of conflicting motives for
granted. In this view, it is not out of the ordinary that a devoted mother should also potentially have
305
4
murderous impulses towards her children (or that an abusive father experiences fierce devotion to his
child). The conflicting impulses may be in consciousness, or may be between conscious and uncon-
scious motivation, or may be entirely unconscious.
Orthodox psychoanalysts (and many other psychoanalytic thinkers) would find the root of many
psychological symptoms and antisocial behaviour in problems caused by unconscious conflict. For
example, constant opting-out of developmental opportunities in life may result from a feeling that
approval of the father is essential, combined with a drive to rebel and attack the father; avoidance of
commitment in relationships may come from a strong drive to be united with a parent, combined with
a conviction that close love overwhelms and destroys the other person.
Id, ego, superego; other ideas about the structure of the mind
A characteristic of psychodynamic thinkers is that they regard the mind (or the self, or the personality)
as comprised of different parts which interact with each other. Two of Freud’s ideas about the struc-
ture of the mind are that it comprises a conscious and an unconscious part (see above), and that it
comprises three areas called the id, ego and superego. In summary, the id comprises animal drives and
306
4
totally self-seeking impulses, with no regard for others, morality or respect for the law; the superego
develops as the part of the person which holds that they must be good, moral and law-abiding, regards
punishment as a just response to wrongdoing and creates feelings of guilt if the individual does
something ‘bad’. These two parts, which may not be conscious, are at war with one another, and the
part of the person which tries to find a balance, is primarily conscious, and tries to decide on everyday
behaviour in a way that is rational, realistic and takes account both of legitimate need and of morality
is the ego.
These two ideas about structure do not necessarily fit together logically. Freud, for example, developed
the idea of conscious/unconscious and id/ego/superego at different times and there is no single view
which explains how they fit together. Similarly there is no single account of exactly what is regarded
as having this structure. In different contexts the ideas best refer to divisions of the ‘mind’, or of the
‘self’, of the ‘person’, or indeed of what Freud called the ‘ego’. These terms can be more or less similar
in different contexts. ‘Mind’, for example, nearly always means something which is not the ‘body’. The
‘self’ (‘I’, ‘you’ and so on) may or may not include bodily characteristics.
Woodmansey’s concept (1966, 1989) of ‘punitive superego’ is that it is like another self, or another ego,
which splits off from the original ego and develops with the aim of avoiding external punishment and
keeping high self-esteem. It has the single (and, if necessary, self-attacking) purpose of controlling any
natural impulses which tended to cause trouble when the person was young. It is like a conscience,
but is not based on true morality but on what was approved or disapproved of during development.
Defence mechanisms
In most psychodynamic views, the developing self is sometimes faced with overwhelming anxiety,
without the ‘mature’ resources to be able to cope. An example would be when the child wishes
to destroy the very person it most loves and whom it totally depends on for survival and comfort.
Psychoanalytic theory considers that the person copes by finding solutions which are not reality-based,
but remove the overwhelming anxiety. For example, in Melanie Klein’s view, the infant experiences the
‘good mother’ as a different person from the ‘depriving mother’. It can love the one and hate the other
307
4
without worry – this defence is described as ‘splitting’. Other defence mechanisms include ‘denial’ –
a physically abused child may be convinced its father loves it (better to believe that than that it is
living with someone who wishes to harm and kill it); and ‘identification with the aggressor’ – a boy
believes it is ‘right’ to become tough, bullying and domineering like his father, of whom he was
originally afraid.
Defences may be essential for physical or psychological survival in one phase of life, but they are not
realistic, and may be problematic later on. In a psychodynamic view, however, ‘defences’ had a reason
for arising, and in the present they are a response to a situation which is experienced as the same as
that which required the original response. There are also situations throughout life which require
psychological defence reactions. For many psychoanalytic thinkers (beginning with Freud) important
human achievements (Michelangelo’s art, Ghandi’s triumphant non-violence, for example) result from
the sublimation of unconscious conflicts.
States of mind
Each ‘state of mind’ emerges seamlessly from those before it, and is not an isolated entity. Some
psychoanalytic thinkers use this concept of ‘states of mind’ productively to describe how the present
somehow often contains or reawakens experiences from the past (as when a scene in a film strikes an
emotional chord with a viewer’s past experience). Waddell (2002) describes how this way of thinking
derives from the work of Melanie Klein (1975).
Since the helper is human, they too bring experiences from the past, and attribute qualities to their
client or service user which are not accurate. They may fear criticism, seek approval or anticipate anger
in ways which are not reality-based. This is called countertransference.
When Freud first realised people transferred to him qualities which were not realistic, he saw it as a
hindrance in his work. When he realised that actually in this transaction the client was presenting with
great clarity (but not in words) the difficulties they experienced in relating, he stated that ‘what had
at first seemed the analyst’s greatest enemy turned out to be his greatest ally’. As Paula Heimann put
308
4
it later, ‘On the stage of transference the original problem is brought up not just for re-enactment but
for revision’ (Heimann, 1959/1989).
The ideas about transference, developed originally in psychoanalysis, are applied by many people in
situations such as social work. Because of their past experiences, users of service may expect criticism,
become angry before anything has been said, or be trusting because of earlier experiences. The worker
brings their own attitudes to the encounter which are more or less realistic.
Like many psychoanalytic ideas, different writers have explored these concepts in different directions.
What is described as transference and countertransference differ between different writers.
Critiques
Although psychoanalytic thought is widely used in many areas of study, it is equally widely criticised,
ignored or ridiculed.
Many, but not by any means all, academic psychologists regard it as impossible to use in their discipline
because it is not based on the scientific method and on empirical evidence, observation and testing (it
tends to derive from individuals reflecting on their own life experience or their experience as therapists
with other people). Some are of the view that where it has been subjected to testing, it has been found
severely inaccurate. Some would see it more as ‘storytelling’ than science. Similar to this criticism is
the problem (for example, about unconscious ideas) that the ideas may seem impossible to disprove –
and one model of ‘scientific truth’ holds that scientific statements must always be expressed in such
a way that they are capable of being disproved.
Psychoanalytic ideas in general have been criticised for taking for granted (not questioning) a male-
centred society as if universal truths about ‘people’ were being uncovered. There have been similar
criticisms that it is based in very orthodox Western social and family arrangements at particular points
in time and yet presents findings as if they were about ‘all people’.
There have been numerous critiques and negative evaluations of the practice of psychoanalysis on
which the ideas are based.
Each different psychoanalytic view has had its own response to these criticisms. In general, it is prob-
ably accurate to point out that psychoanalytic thought is based on an understanding of the common
humanity of all people. It emphasises people as the subject of experiences, not the external object of
?
study. But it is hard to see it (despite Freud’s original intentions) as based on the ‘objective’ scientific
procedures in study which have resulted in the advances of physics, genetics and physical medicine.
The approach taken in all of the chapters, of encouraging your thought about ‘states of mind’ from
within the person’s frame of reference (the mother in Chapter 1, for example, or the infant and toddler
in Chapter 2, or the dying person in Chapter 9) draws on the psychodynamic tradition. A central theme
of the explanation about guilt and self-attack at the end of Chapter 3 is psychodynamic.
309
5 Piaget’s theory of cognitive development
In Piaget’s view, the individual is always trying to construct reality out of their experiences. We are
innately constructivists and we are innately scientists.
Table E5.1
Stages of cognitive development, after Piaget
Cognitive stages
Birth to 2 years Mental processing consists of sensory perceptions and simple Sensori-motor
bodily responses.
2–7 years Internal representation of the outside world gets more Pre-operational
developed, begins to become more differentiated. However,
many simple aspects of reality are misunderstood. The mental
symbolisation still does not allow for the manipulation of
thoughts and symbols.
7–11 years Child can reason about concrete objects, can perform mental Concrete operational
‘operations’ on them. They can mentally reverse procedures stage
which they observe and develop the idea of generating general
principles from their own experiences.
11–12 years Children develop the ability to reason about abstract ideas, Formal operational stage
and beyond understand the reason for distinguishing between valid and
invalid conclusions. Can approach problems in a rational,
thought-out manner using abstractions from reality.
The term used in Piaget’s theory for mental structures that represent, organise and interpret expe-
riences is schema.
When new experiences are assimilated to existing ways of understanding the world, no new cognitive
schemas are required. By contrast, when children create new models (schemas) to make sense of their
310
5
experience, they accommodate to the external reality of the world. For example, the description of any
four-legged animal as a dog occurs because a child assimilates their experience to an existing model;
but to understand that some people are born ambiguously male or female, the process of
accommodation requires modification of existing schemas about sex and gender.
In the course of adaptation to the world, existing cognitive structures may be modified
(assimilation) or the mind may have to create new cognitive structures for the new knowledge
(accommodation).
Sensori-motor stage
Piaget identifies six substages:
• Primary use of reflexes (0–1 month). Primitive reflexes such as sucking adapt through small
accommodations. No sense of persisting objects – just sense experiences.
• Primary circular reactions (1–4 months). Simple actions are repeated for their own sake.
Beginnings of coordination of different senses – baby sucks on what it can reach/looks towards
sound.
• Secondary circular reactions (4–8 months). Baby makes external events repeat by a kind of trial-
and-error process. Imitation of actions already in the child’s repertoire. Responds to visible objects,
and will look over edge for dropped objects but acts as if objects out of sight don’t exist.
• Coordination of secondary reactions (8–11 months). Child begins to solve problems – for example,
by moving cushion to get toy. Is surprised by ‘trick’ behaviour of objects. Can imitate unfamiliar
behaviour.
• Tertiary circular reactions (12–18 months). Child varies responses and tries out new ones. Active
and purposeful experimentation. Most infants grasp the fact of the continuing existence of
objects.
• Mental representation begins (from 18 months). Child able to solve some problems by thinking
about them – there is a mental representation separate from the object.
311
5
Pre-operational stage
Four significant qualities of pre-operational thinking in children are:
• The use of symbolism – the child washes a doll as if she were a mother giving a baby a bath, for
example.
• Egocentrism – the child interprets the world always from its own point of view (looking at a doll’s
house from the front, she is asked what the teddy bear in the back garden sees, and she picks out
a picture which is of the front of the house, not the back).
• Centration – the child focuses on only one significant aspect of an object at a time (so animals
walking on four legs are all dogs; a leaf that blows in the wind is alive).
• Conservation – during this period the child acquires the idea of conservation, that matter can
change in shape or appearance without changing in quantity.
312
5
Vygotsky’s work places more emphasis on the role of culture and society in cognitive development. He
used the term ‘zone of proximate development’ to refer to the area in which learning will be possible,
as long as adults (for example in school) provide the right ‘scaffolding’ to enable it to take place.
An alternative framework of ‘information processing research’ is more rigorously based in laboratory
experiment and statistical/biological study. Regarding the brain as analogous to a computer, this
approach analyses the different functional units and processes involved. It examines the development
of each, providing a much more analytic picture than Piaget’s more generalised approach. For example,
this approach studies the development of ‘working memory’. This is an area of memory which is used
just in the performance of immediate tasks and is deleted after use – when you look up a phone
number and hold the digits in mind while you concentrate on pressing the phone buttons, you use
working memory. Researchers find that the number of items which can be stored in working memory
grows as the brain develops. This is why children must be given only a number of instructions which
?
fit in their short-term memory – a child may appear to be disobedient because ‘close your books, stand
up quietly, go to your desk and sit down’ may contain too many items for their working memory.
313
?
6 Erikson’s psychosocial theory of personality
Erikson developed his eight-stage model from the following origins:
• His basic psychoanalytic outlook – he accepted Freud’s ideas of the conscious and
Chapter unconscious and the functioning of the self at three levels – id, ego and superego (see
EB2
‘Essential background’, section 2).
• But he placed much more emphasis on conscious processes – emphasising the importance of the
ego.
• And where Freud’s developmental ideas explored childhood, Erikson’s eight-stage framework
describes the whole of life.
• Erikson paid systematic attention to social and cultural influences. He derived his model from an
analysis of lives from a wide variety of cultural and ethnic backgrounds.
• He was particularly concerned with ‘identity’. Many ideas in everyday use today about one’s
‘identity’ use ideas developed by Erikson.
Main features
Erikson (1950/1995) analyses the lifespan as involving eight stages. For each stage, there are positive
or negative dimensions of outcome depending on experience, listed in Table E6.1. Each stage builds on
the outcome of the previous stages. His work examines case studies to show how the individual’s ego
development is affected by the social environment. Each stage is characterised by a particular task, a
problem in psychosocial development that is central to that life stage. Each therefore represents a
particular turning point – in Erikson’s terms, an unavoidable crisis arising from intrinsic physiological
development combined with external social demand.
314
6
Table E6.1
Stages in Erikson’s psychosocial model
• It is a psychosocial model that takes account both of psychological development and of the
influence of social and cultural factors.
• All research needs a framework in which questions can be explored and hypotheses tested, and
Erikson’s model has provided this. For example, he describes the processes involved in identity
formation in adolescence, and his concepts make a starting point for the research of Phinney and
others into ethnic identity. They allow comparisons of identity development between males and
females, and have informed research about young offenders.
Limitations
• Erikson’s model presents a highly abstracted presentation of a universal model. This contrasts with
approaches such as the Lifecourse model, which regards all development (even in theory) as
inseparable from the specifics of culture, history, geography and individual circumstances. The
question is whether Erikson’s model takes full account of the different routes which can be taken
through life.
• Even allowing for Erikson’s view that the model must not be interpreted too rigidly, it is a stage
theory, which thereby presents a linear picture. All elements in development are deemed to
315
6
?
progress coherently to produce this single set of stages, each time with a ‘crisis’ which has a single
summarising description.
• It has been criticised for reflecting a male perspective. Bingham and Striker (1995)
have presented models (sometimes a modification rather than an abandonment
of Erikson’s model) more reflective of women’s experience in life (see Chapters 4
Chapters
4 and 5 and 5).
• Erikson’s original scheme specified just one stage for ‘old age’. This has been seen as inadequate
for such a long and diverse period. Feil (1989) expanded his scheme to include a further stage after
Erikson’s eighth stage. Erikson himself (1985) expanded on the final stages of life in his book The
Lifecycle Completed.
•
?
Although Erikson based his work on his interest in people from different cultures (and quite
possibly on his own experience as having more than one cultural heritage), his work nevertheless
has the feel of being created from within a specific culture at a particular point in time.
Most of the chapters make at least a passing reference to Erikson’s work: Chapter 2 where the ideas are
introduced; Chapter 3 (introduction); Chapter 4 in relation to adolescence; Chapter 7 in relation to
models of the development of ethnic identity; Chapter 8 in relation to later life.
316
?
7 The humanistic models of Maslow and Rogers
Humanistic psychology
The humanistic movement in psychology reacted against two earlier ways of under-
standing people. Behaviourist psychology (Chapter 3 and ‘Essential background’, section
8) was regarded as too mechanistic to capture human psychology, and treated people as
Chapters
3, EB8, if their responses are essentially those seen in animals. Psychoanalytic theory (Chapter 2
2, EB4
and ‘Essential background’, section 4) was regarded as too ‘expert’-based (ignoring the
expertise people have about their own lives), and too concerned with destructive and self-
centred motivations. Humanistic psychologists emphasised that all people set out with the capacity to
become good people, to achieve their potential. If the conditions are right, this is what will happen; if
the conditions are wrong, then the growth will go in other directions. This potential is highly individual,
but distinctively human.
Both Maslow and Rogers regard people as having within them a ‘self-actualising tendency’.
The first four levels were described by Maslow as ‘deficit’ needs, because they operate to reduce a felt
sense of deficit or discomfort. They operate as ‘tension reduction systems’. If one is hungry, one eats
to reduce the tension and the motive to eat disappears. The final stage, in contrast, he described as a
‘growth’ need. Growth motives continue to act even when there are no deficiencies. The parent, writer
or artist continues to carry out their fulfilling activities even when there is no ‘deficit’ to be satisfied.
The ‘hierarchy’ is not regarded as absolute – the earlier needs do not need to be fully satisfied before
the higher ones become salient.
317
7
Self-actualisation
(Achieving individual potential)
Esteem
(self-esteem and esteem from others)
Belonging
(Love, affection, being part of a group)
Safety
(Shelter, removal from danger)
Physiological
(Health, food, sleep) Figure E7.1
Maslow’s hierarchy of needs
318
7
The inherent tendency of the organism to develop all its capacities in ways which serve to maintain
or enhance the person.
(Rogers, 1959: 196)
This is expressed through the organismic valuing process. Experiences that maintain the self and help
it towards actualisation are valued. Rogers considered that in infancy this is straightforward – the
infant experiences what is good or hurtful directly. The developing person, however, also has a
need for positive regard – approval from others. As children grow, their self-concept becomes a
combination of what is directly experienced as good and bad and what brings praise or rejection from
others.
Unconditional positive regard enables the child to remain in touch with their organismic valuing
process, so that they maintain, develop and enhance themselves according to their true potential.
Unconditional positive regard does not stop the expression of parental emotion or displeasure, but the
response is about the behaviour and not the person – the child remains secure that it is loved and
accepted. Rogers regarded conditional positive regard (‘I will love, accept and respect you only if you
are the sort of person I want you to be’) as damaging the child’s potential to develop themselves to
the full. It requires children to disown their feelings rather than inhibit their expression.
A simple example of the effects in later life might be a mother who has been taught that a good
mother always feels loving towards her child. She has to have some process to deal with the fact of
her ‘organismic’ experience of dislike, or of feeling angry or punitive. In Rogers’ view, this process must
be either denial of her true feelings (the defence mechanism of denial) or the distortion of reality –
since a good mother would be angry with a child only if it deserves punishment, the child must be
doing something which deserves punishment. In either case, the process has the negative effect of
creating more distance between reality and how it is experienced.
Other typical examples of values which are introjected, but are not in accord with organismic valuing
are:
• sexuality (or sexual fantasies, or masturbation) is bad;
319
7
Area 2: In Area 1: In
awareness, but awareness and
distorted accurate
Area 3: Actual
experiences, now
out of awareness
Figure E7.2
Rogers’ self-structure
Rogers’ self-structure
organismic experience that has had to be put out of awareness because it couldn’t be accommodated
(area 3) – for example, that her parents were wrong and hurtful to criticise and punish her.
Applications
Many social workers find that humanistic models are a natural way to describe the developmental
issues they face in their work with service users – children who have been neglected or abused, people
with mental health problems, people in the last stages of their life, and so on.
Rogers was a psychologist and counsellor whose first unqualified job was in residential work
with children. Aside from the fact that social work places a natural emphasis on practical as well as
‘talking’ help, and recognises the need people sometimes have for advice and guidance, the model of
person-centred (‘Rogerian’) counselling he developed is very in tune with the principles of social work.
Researchers working in the person-centred tradition found that the effectiveness of help depended
not on the theories used by helpers, but on their personal qualities. These qualities are that the helper
320
7
offers unconditional acceptance and empathy, and shows congruence. Unconditional acceptance is
referred to above. Empathy is a continuing process whereby the counsellor lays aside her own way of
experiencing and perceiving reality, preferring to sense and respond to the experiences and perceptions
of her ‘client’, combined with the accurate communication of this perception. In brief, congruence is
the ability to ensure that experience and awareness (the two circles in Figure E7.2) overlap as much as
possible.
With modification over the years, the models developed by the humanistic psychologists have been
widely adopted in other human services – education, conflict resolution, mental health and manage-
ment.
321
7
?
Despite this work about counselling and therapy based on humanistic psychology, the most funda-
mental criticism of the model is the difficulty of producing evidence which supports or disproves it. It
is hard to see how the characteristics of ‘self-actualising’ people (Maslow) or the ‘fully functioning
person’ (Rogers) can be put into objective measures, and the chances of relating these to a whole life
history which identifies scientifically whether the person received ‘unconditional positive
regard’ are even more remote. Chapter 10 explained that one criterion for a scientific
theory is that it must be based on falsifiable statements, and it is argued that it is hard to
Chapter 10
?
see what would count to disprove these theories.
It can also be argued that the theories are based on the idea of ‘human nature’ – and some have argued
that this is a concept which is at best misleading and at worst non-existent.
The final section of Chapter 4 outlines the arguments put forward by a different criticism,
one which applies to many general theories of human development. Some feminist
thinkers have argued that, until recently, all descriptions of ‘human’ need were actually
Chapter 4
covert accounts of ‘men’, not including women (see, for example, Kagan and Tyndall,
2003). Society has oppressed women, it is argued, and all men benefited from the oppression; the
meanings of all terms used in academic discourse have been necessarily implicated in this oppression.
?
Attempts to describe the ‘personal’ independently of the ‘political’ were necessarily flawed. When
Rogers, for example, states that he is explaining ‘the most profound truth about man’ the critics may
say that, unwittingly, he is indeed thinking about only half the population.
Humanistic theories are referred to in the last section of Chapter 4 (the discussion about general
theories of ‘human’ development), and in Chapter 10.
Further reading
Many psychology textbooks summarise the humanistic approach to psychology – for example, Eysenck, M. W.
(2000) Psychology: A Student’s Handbook. Hove, UK: Psychology Press, pp. 27, 713, 725.
Rogers, C. R. (1961/2004) On Becoming a Person : A Therapist’s View of Psychotherapy. London, Constable; Boston:
Houghton Mifflin.
An authoritative account of the theory’s application to counselling: Woolfe et al. (eds) (2003) Handbook of
Counselling Psychology. London: Sage, Chapter 6, ‘The humanistic paradigm’.
Gross, R. (2007) Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behaviour. London: Hodder Arnold.
Rogers, C. R. (1959) ‘A theory of therapy, personality and interpersonal relationships, as developed in the client-
centered framework’. In S. Koch (ed.) Psychology: A Study of Science. New York: McGraw Hill, pp. 184–256.
322
8 Learning and behavioural models
Behaviourism set out to be ‘a purely objective, experimental branch of natural science’ (Watson,
1913, quoted by Eysenck, 2000). Its goal was to explain, predict and control behaviour. Self-reports of
feelings, moods and thoughts are not independently verifiable, so reference to these, and the use
of introspection (thinking about oneself) was ruled out. Observations and experiments referred only
to factors that are objectively and independently verifiable. Current approaches, however, include
‘cognitive behavioural’ methods, and the disciplines of experimental and biological psychology do take
account of cognitions and emotions.
The main mechanisms of behavioural learning are classical conditioning, operant conditioning and
social learning theory.
?
Classical conditioning
Dogs and cats salivate when they approach food. If a bell is sounded every time food is put down for
them, they begin to salivate at the sound of the bell, even without the presence of food. This is classical
conditioning. An unconditioned response (salivating in the presence of food) has changed
into a conditioned response (salivating in the presence of a stimulus that has constantly
been associated with the food). Chapter 3 gave the example of an abused boy who has an
Chapter 3 anxiety response as soon as he hears the door slam when his father returns home.
The target behaviour is affected by what precedes or accompanies it.
Operant conditioning
Behaviour which is consistently rewarded is likely to persist. When a baby gets a round of applause,
smiles and laughter for his first few steps, he is encouraged to try again, and looks round for a similar
response. A girl who is praised for looking pretty pays attention to clothes, hair and make-up. The
responses which make the behaviour more likely are described as ‘reinforcements’.
In operant conditioning the behaviour is shaped by rewards or punishments which follow.
323
8
learning to be effective, Bandura (1977) found that the models should appear to the observer to have
high prestige; they should appear to get rewards for the action; the observer should focus attention on
the behaviour; they should be capable of reproducing it (a golfer can learn observationally from an
expert golfer, but will not learn brain surgery by watching a surgeon).
Behavioural interventions
Behavioural models have had extensive applications to control or influence behaviour. Some phobias,
for example, arise through classical conditioning, a neutral stimulus such as a dog becoming associated
with a response such as panic. The therapy might consist in reversing this association by systematically
associating dogs with relaxation. Operant conditioning has been used to systematically modify the
behaviour of children (or autistic people, or people with learning difficulties, or youth offenders) using
rewards and punishments.
As the name implies, the approach combines cognitive therapy with behavioural learning principles.
Faulty thought patterns (‘cognitions’) are seen to underlie many problems of behaviour and emotion,
including depression and anxiety. Therapy is directed at identifying and changing these faulty
cognitions, using the principles of learning – systematic reinforcement and stimulus-response. In
experimental studies, cognitive behavioural therapy has been found to show the most promising
results for many problems of mental health.
It is a didactic model of intervention, in which a number of different aspects of coaching and teaching
may be combined. The person directing the modification is the expert, always in the method and
sometimes in the behaviour that should be achieved. Often, the therapist works initially with the client
to identify the thought patterns which are causing the problem, and then works out a schedule which
will lead to reinforcement of accurate thought patterns. For example, the faulty cognitions may relate
to a conviction that the person always has to please others. The therapist may identify this as a prob-
lem, and enable the person to see theoretically for themselves that they are just as socially effective
when they don’t act on this belief. They may then work out a schedule which enables the person to
internalise the new convictions, to gain reinforcement from the resulting behaviour and emotions, and
to achieve social satisfaction by being less dependent on others’ good regard.
324
?
8
Chapter 3, about cognitive development in children and about learning; Chapter 7, about mental
health difficulties.
Further reading
An excellent overview for social workers: Ronen, R. (2008) ‘Cognitive behavioural therapy’. In Davies, M. (ed.) The
Blackwell Companion to Social Work. Oxford/Malden, MA: Blackwell.
There are many accessible instructional manuals about CBT in relation to specific fields, of which the
following are examples:
• Youth offending
• Alcohol and drug services
• Illness and chronic pain (Winterowd et al., 2003, New York: Springer).
325
9 Models of ageing: social disengagement theory,
activity theory, feminist perspectives and political
economy theory
As with many areas of development, there are different lenses through which development in later
life can be viewed. What follows is a quick guide to social disengagement theory, activity theory,
feminist perspectives and political economy theory. A reference is also made to Simon Biggs’ term
‘the mature imagination’. This is not usually presented as a single coherent theory, but it is an
attempt to capture and explore the developing sense of self and the creation of a mature identity in
adulthood.
?
responsibilities. This disengagement is therefore seen as positive, ‘normal’ and healthy. Elaine Cumming
(1975) suggests that with increasing age comes: decreasing life space – fewer interactions with others;
an active choice of disengagement; and a change in style of interaction, towards less a socially
controlled style.
As Chapter 8 above points out, the theory has been criticised for not looking sufficiently
at the details of different cultures and social settings; at the degree to which some
societies establish useful roles for older people and some exclude them. Hochschild (1975)
Chapter 8
criticised the research for not distinguishing ‘maturational’ effects of ageing from ‘cohort’
effects (‘cohort’ effects are those which arise simply because people are born into a particular society
in a particular decade). Bengston and Achenbaum (1994) summarise in addition that it fails to explain
or allow for individual choice and individual differences – for example, many people may resent or
dislike ‘disengagement’ or may be financially disadvantaged by it.
Cumming herself (1975: 187) considers that the dispute over activity is ‘an unenlightening controversy’
which is not relevant to the theory, but many others have seen a contrast between disengagement
theory and activity theory.
326
9
Activity theory
Activity theory (Neugarten, 1977) maintains that the healthiest approach to older age is to be active
and socially engaged. Roles from middle life which are no longer appropriate should be replaced by
relevant alternatives. Services which build on an implicit model of disengagement theory are likely in
this view to be overtly or covertly ageist. The role of services should be to support and maintain com-
munity structures which allow for the continuing engagement and activity of older people.
The evidence for activity theory is that research generally finds (small) positive advantages for older
people who are active and socially engaged. Politically oriented perspectives criticise it for being overly
individual and ignoring structural inequality.
Stuart-Hamilton (2006: 166) quotes research showing that there is often, but not invariably, continuity
between personality style in earlier and later ages in life – this may account for some of the controversy
between activity theory and disengagement theory, as people are more or less active and more or less
isolated in their earlier lives. Atchley (1989) argues for continuity theory. He describes how people create
both internal and external structures to their lives. The core of the theory is that people make efforts to
support and maintain these structures as they progress through maturity. Circumstances may change,
but they develop strategies and lifestyles in the new circumstances which provide continuity and links
to the past as they perceive it. This approach arises particularly from a life-course perspective.
Like other theories referred to so far, continuity theory is criticised for paying insufficient attention to
sociological and structural disadvantages that affect ageing. The final two theories summarised here,
on the other hand, are based on a model of society that assumes continual conflict, with vested
interests always oppressing and exploiting those with less power.
327
9
328
10 Three approaches to loss and grief
This section summarises three models which have been put forward to describe the human response
to bereavement. Kubler-Ross’ work (1989) considers the path experienced by the bereaved person as
a series of stages. Stroeb and Schut (1999, 2001) analyse the experience not as stages which may be
followed, but as involving two mindsets which can alternate in the bereaved person. Worden (2003)
sees the process as involving tasks which have to be accomplished by the bereaved person.
Elizabeth Kubler-Ross
Elizabeth Kubler-Ross identified stages people went through as they approached their death, but her
work has been widely used also as a framework for understanding the experience of bereavement. The
terms she uses are in the first column of Table E10.1 (the notes are mine). The stages do not have to
be followed in strict order, and the person may move forward and back between them.
Table E10.1
Kubler-Ross’ model: stages of grief
Stage Notes
Denial A typical reaction of people in relation to awareness of a terminal illness is to respond with the
statement, ‘No, not me. It cannot be true.’ A similar response is often the first reaction to the
news of the death of someone close.
Anger The bereaved person may be angry with the dead person for leaving them; with people, such as
doctors, associated with the circumstances of death; or aware in general of a rage within
themselves.
Bargaining Examples are the wish in anticipatory grieving that they be taken instead of the person who is
dying. Bargaining is also evident when a bereaved person copes by offering imaginary ‘deals’: if I
do this, adopt such and such a strategy, the pain and the disruption will diminish, things should
stabilise for me.
Depression Feelings of guilt, no purpose to life; a great sense of loss, as the person is aware of how much
they have lost.
Acceptance Realisation that life can and does go on, coping positively with feelings.
329
10
• a loss-orientation focused on the gap and the pain caused by the loss of the person who has gone;
William Worden
William Worden emphasises that bereavement is a process rather than a state, and analyses the
experiences of bereaved people as they work through their reactions. He understands them to be
tackling four overlapping tasks:
• to face the reality;
• to work through the emotional pain of their loss;
• to adjust to the new reality – changes in their circumstances, roles, status and identity;
• to reinvest in the future.
?
He would understand the tasks to be complete when the bereaved person has integrated the loss into
their life and let go of emotional attachments to the deceased.
These and other theories of bereavement are referred to in Chapter 9 (many writers about loss and grief
have made use of psychoanalytic theory and attachment theory). Chapter 2, explaining ‘states of mind’
refers also to the experience of loss, as does Chapter 3; in the context of guilt; Chapter 4 about
transitions; Chapter 5 about loneliness and alone-ness; Chapter 6 about the loss of children; Chapter 8
in relation to older people.
330
Glossary
accommodation the term in Piaget’s theory of cognitive development to indicate that an individual’s
scheme of understanding has changed to accommodate a new experience in the world – the opposite
of assimilation.
activity theory theory of ageing which holds that older people who remain active will have better
psychological well-being and be better adjusted than those who do not.
affect the experience of feeling or emotion (affective: relating to affect); affect may often be
discussed in relation to cognition or thought processes – affect is partly in contrast to cognition and
partly interlinked with it.
ambivalent holding opposite attitudes at the same time – as when a person both loves and hates
another.
assimilation the term in Piaget’s theory of cognitive development to indicate that a new experience
from the outside world is made to fit (assimilated into) existing schemes of understanding – the
opposite of accommodation.
attachment theory theory of development which states that individuals are born with a drive to seek
attachments. These are affectional bonds which provide comfort in times of threat and a secure base
from which to explore; separation from the attachment figure activates attachment behaviours.
attunement in attachment theory the process demonstrated by an attachment figure when their
responses are ‘in tune with’ the internal experiences of the individual (in contrast, for example, to self-
centred responses based on the impact of behaviour on the caregiver).
331
Glossary
the child shows signs of ignoring. The infant is unlikely to show emotional ‘sharing’ with the caregiver,
although they may turn to them for practical help and support.
bioregulation mechanisms by which the body brings itself back to equilibrium after disturbance.
Physically, for example – sweating to reduce the effects of heat; emotionally – mechanisms to return
to a steady state after panic or exhilaration.
chromosomes structures (large molecules) which contain genetic information about the individual.
Within the chromosome, this information is stored in units called genes. There are twenty-three pairs
of chromosomes in each cell of the body.
classical conditioning learning process in which an unconditioned response (such a dog salivating
in the presence of food) becomes associated with a conditioning stimulus (such as the bell which is
always rung when the food is presented). After conditioning, the response can be produced by the
conditioned stimulus.
cognitive behavioural therapy therapy which combines behavioural learning (using ideas of stimulus
and behavioural response) and cognitive therapy (emphasising the importance of thoughts).
congruence in humanistic psychology, the condition in which a person’s self-awareness matches their
actual experience; also used to mean that the therapist’s attitude and awareness matches the client’s
experience.
controlled (or independent) variable in an experiment, the feature under the researcher’s control,
which can be varied in order to find the change caused in the uncontrolled (or dependent) variable.
co-regulation process by which bioregulation occurs not within the individual but between two
people – the excited biological state of one person is brought back to equilibrium by the other.
corporate parent an organisation which has the responsibilities of a parent (used to describe a UK
local authority when it has parental responsibilities towards a child by virtue of the Children Act).
cosmopolitanism the situation in which it is increasingly common for individuals living in one
country to owe allegiance and identity to cultures in other countries, in contrast to strongly local or
nationalistic societies. This is also associated with the idea that all peoples are part of a single moral
community, and contrasts with Eurocentric, Afro-centric or radical Islamic philosophies.
332
Glossary
determinist theories in which clear rules or laws enable the present situation to have been exactly
predicted from a state existing in the past.
didactic based on teaching by an expert.
disorganised attachment in attachment theory, children’s attachment behaviour typically falls into
one of a limited number of patterns (see also secure, avoidant and ambivalent). In the disorganised
pattern, parents evoke contradictory behaviours in the child, of simultaneous avoidance and approach.
In the ‘strange situation’ observation, children display disorganised and disrupted behaviour – such
as wandering around whimpering, banging their head against a wall, or ‘freezing’. Co-regulation is
absent, and the child does not develop a coherent sense of self.
empathy the capacity to think and feel oneself into the inner life of another person; it also involves
the ability to communicate with the other person so that they feel this sense of being understood.
epigenetics inherited changes which are not caused by changes in genes. Variations in the effects
of genes which are not caused by changes in core genetic information.
epistemology the branch of philosophy dealing with knowledge – what it is, the different forms it
can take, how it is acquired, how its truth is established or disproved.
Erikson’s psychosocial model a model of development through the life course, it combines a psycho-
analytic approach about inner psychological development with a formulation about the influence of
social conditions and culture. It divides the life course into eight stages from infancy to old age.
eugenics ideology that if care is not taken, ‘poor quality’ breeding lines will be perpetuated into later
generations (and conversely, that careful breeding can improve the human stock). Associated with
racist ideologies and the sterilisation of people with learning disabilities.
existentialist a school of philosophy which placed emphasis on the importance of subjective expe-
rience; this could not form an ‘object’ of study (because that would again leave unexamined the subject
who was doing the study).
exosystem in Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model, a system which affects the growing individual
even though they themselves are not part of it. In particular, the exosystem involves other people in
the individual’s microsystem. The parent’s workplace is an exosystem for the child.
expressive role a role in which the personal qualities of an individual are relevant (as in the role of
lover or friend), in contrast to an instrumental role.
genes a tiny component of the body which contains information about the way the body should be
built. Each of the more than 100 trillion cells in a human body contains a complete set of the 22,000
genes. They are arranged on twenty-three chromosomes.
genetics the science studying the transmission of information from one generation to the next
through genes.
gerontology the study of ageing.
333
Glossary
hereditable able to be passed from one generation to the next by heredity. In a group of people, some
differences (such as eye colour) may be totally caused by heredity. These qualities are described as 100
per cent hereditable. Other qualities (which particular language is spoken) may be totally independent
of heredity and are 0 per cent hereditable. Many qualities lie between these extremes, with some of
the variation caused by heredity and some by environment.
hierarchy of needs in Maslow’s theory, the arrangement of different kinds of needs, such that some
are more prominent (salient) than others at different times in life (for example, food and shelter are
salient in infancy, whereas the need for self-esteem becomes prominent after other more basic needs
have already been met).
hormones chemicals released into the body that affect other aspects of functioning; hormones carry
signals or messages between different systems in the body.
humanistic a loose grouping of psychological models which emphasised the distinctive nature of
human experience to strive for personal and social improvement.
instrumentality in psychology, the degree to which individuals act as directive agents in their lives
as distinct from having matters determined by someone else.
instrumental role in social psychology or sociology, a role in which the personal quality of the person
is not relevant to performance (for example, street cleaner) – contrasts with expressive role.
internal working model in attachment theory, the individual’s mental representation of attachment
relationships. It contains a view of: (1) the individual themselves – whether they are basically likeable
or not; (2) the attachment figure – whether they are well disposed and competent when needed; (3)
the relationship – whether it is usually available and competent. The internal working model is par-
ticularly important in the regulation of negative emotions such as fear, panic and anger.
life course the course of life from conception to death.
lifespan development an approach which emphasises that development occurs throughout life and
must be understood in the light of social and historical factors; its study therefore involves a range of
academic disciplines.
lifespan theory theory which regards development as the system’s adaptation to changing circum-
stances and environment. It therefore occurs throughout life, and valid research about it has to explore
historical, economic, political and cultural dynamics as well as the biological and psychological.
Lifespan theorists have paid particular attention to development in older age.
life structure a concept in Levinson’s theory. It represents the character of an individual’s life at a
given time. In adulthood, this often focuses on family and work. In Levinson’s view, there are charac-
teristic periods of structure-building and structure-changing.
macrosystem in Bronfenbrenner’s ecological theory of development, the broadest system of social
and cultural influences.
334
Glossary
modernist a term with different meanings in different academic fields. In relation to the social
sciences, it is a fairly vague term referring to theories which seek to find general patterns of devel-
opment thought to have universal applicability. The theories assume that it is possible to identify truths
which are independent of the observer.
multifactorial arising from the combination of several causes – the cause of heart disease may be
multifactorial, involving genetic predisposition, diet in childhood and current lifestyle.
ontology branch of philosophy dealing with existence; within a particular view (such as a religion)
the beliefs about what exists.
operant conditioning in learning theory, the process by which actions become more (or less) frequent
because of rewards or punishments. The target behaviour is affected by its consequences.
paradigm a set of assumptions, procedures and values which inform a particular approach to an
academic discipline; the feminist paradigm, for example, assumes that if gender is not specifically
referred to in a social research, it is likely that differences between men and women are being over-
looked.
phenotype an observable characteristic of an individual, which can include shape, appearance and
behaviour.
play therapy work with troubled children which recognises that children normally communicate in
play (not necessarily using words), and that they work on problems in their play. For example, a child
who has been sexually abused and then lived with two different foster carers before being adopted
may have many questions and conflicts. The therapist may work with the child in painting, in play with
dolls, or with sand play.
political economy of ageing a model which views the ageing process as structured by the position
of older people in a given political and economic system.
political economy theory referred to particularly in connection with ageing – emphasises the
importance of social structure, power, hierarchy and distribution of resources, all of which can impact
negatively on older people.
335
Glossary
positivist research which insists that terms should be reducible (at least in principle) to verifiable
observational statements, that effects have deterministic causes, that the goal of (social) science is to
create hypotheses and laws which account for the observed diversity of the world. One of several
possible paradigms for social science; positivism is limited in its ability to explore and value the
subjective states of individuals.
postmodernist a vague term used in many different ways and deriving from the contrast with
modernist. In social science, postmodern theorists tend to regard knowledge as always presented from
particular points of view, and as having implicit connections with social power structures; often, the
distinction between ‘objective’ and ‘subjective’ is doubted, and reality is regarded as constructed by
society and language. This approach is sometimes criticised for further complicating ideas which are
already complex.
primary health care normally the first point of contact for people entering the healthcare system. In
the UK it is typically the family doctor, health centre or walk-in centre.
primary sex characteristics the sex organs of reproduction; in the female, uterus, ovaries, and vagina;
in the male, the penis.
privilege term used to indicate how different people have unearned advantages (including basic
human rights) over others in different social networks – typical examples may be males over females,
welfare professionals over welfare applicants, white Europeans over others, women over children, able-
bodied over disabled people – but there are many others depending on the attribution of power to
specific characteristics in particular social arrangements.
psychiatry the medical speciality concerned with ‘mental illness’.
psychoanalysis (psychoanalytic) a method of investigating the mind; a form of therapy; a theory
developed from these practices. The method places particular emphasis on free association (the subject
saying whatever comes into their mind), and self-exploration rather than expert direction; the theory
includes attention to conscious and unconscious components of the mind.
psychodynamic theories and practices which draw on psychoanalytic ideas, and emphasise that
different parts of the mind are in dynamic interaction with each other. ‘Psychodynamic’ is often a
broader term including, but not limited to, ‘psychoanalytic’. There is no authoritative distinction
between the two terms.
psychosis (plural: psychoses) in traditional psychiatric classification, a mental impairment in which
thought disorders are prominent. No longer used in the main official diagnostic manuals.
psychosocial paying attention both to external social factors and to internal psychological states.
Social work is a psychosocial activity.
qualitative studies research investigations which pay attention to factors which are not necessarily
countable – for example, to the way in which people make sense of their lives; there are many quali-
tative studies about reactions to a cancer diagnosis, or to the experience of belonging to a youth gang.
336
Glossary
resilience factors which distinguish people who are not permanently negatively affected by adverse
events from those who are.
salient prominent.
scaffolding term in Vygotsky’s model of cognitive development. It indicates the support given by
knowledgeable adults to enable the child to develop their cognitive skills; this support is gradually
withdrawn as the child acquires the skill.
schema term in Piaget’s description of cognitive development. Refers to the various types of mental
routines. For example, an infant’s schemas are largely bodily responses of the brain, nervous system
and muscles to experience; the sucking response is one such ‘schema’. But in the ‘formal operation’
stage, schemas may be complex mental operations based on abstractions – solving complex mathe-
matical problems using advanced algebra, for example.
secondary sex characteristics features that distinguish the sexes but are not reproductive organs –
for example, facial hair.
secure attachment in attachment theory, children’s attachment behaviour typically falls into one of
a limited number of patterns (see also avoidant, ambivalent and disorganised). Secure attachment
is built up by sensitive and attuned responses by a caregiver to the child’s attachment behaviour.
Arousal states are co-regulated, and the attachment figure provides a ‘secure base’ – for exploration
of the world, and for help in managing powerful emotions of fear, anxiety, pain or sadness.
serial monogamy monogamy is the practice of having only one sexual partner at a time. ‘Serial
monogamy’ is pair bonding which does not last for life but is replaced by another.
social constructionism the idea that groups construct reality out of their discourse and culture; often
used specifically to apply to concepts which are thought by others to be naturally arising and existing
in nature. There are many different degrees of social constructionism – many accept that ‘family’ is a
social construct; some would regard the existence of mountains as a social construct.
social disengagement theory in relation to ageing, the idea that the process of gradually
withdrawing from social roles benefits both the individual and society.
social exchange theory a model which views social life as structured by what participants exchange
with each other. Interactions continue as long as each feels they are profiting, and that there is some
reciprocity.
social learning theory learning theory associated with Albert Bandura – that behaviour is shaped not
just though stimuli and reinforcements, but also through social observation.
transference in psychodynamic thinking, the re-creation in the present of states of mind which
originate in earlier (particularly parent–child) relationships.
337
Glossary
without conditions being placed upon their behaviour. A concept of Rogers’ humanistic model of
psychology and (person-centred) helping.
uncontrolled (or dependent) variable in an experiment, the feature which changes because the
experimenter manipulates the controlled variable.
338
Bibliography
A National Voice (2009) A national voice. Retrieved 31 January 2009, from www.anationalvoice.org/index.htm.
Abbott, P., Wallace, C., and Tyler, M. (2005) An Introduction to Sociology: Feminist Perspectives. London, New York:
Routledge.
Adams, J. D., Hayes, J., and Hopson, B. (1976) Transition: Understanding and Managing Personal Change. Bath:
Pitman Press.
Adelman, P. (1994) Multiple roles and psychological well-being in a national sample of older adults. Journal of
Gerontology, 49: S277–S285.
Afshar, H., Maynard, M., and Franks, M. (2002) Women, Ethnicity and Empowerment in Later Life. Sheffield: ESRC
Growing Older Programme.
Age Concern (2000) The Debate of the Age. London: Age Concern England.
Agnew, L. (2004) The care programme approach. In T. Ryan and J. Pritchard (eds) Good Practice in Adult Mental
Health. London, Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Allott, P. (2004) What is mental health, illness, and recovery? In T. Ryan and J. Pritchard (eds) Good Practice in
Adult Mental Health. London, Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Ainsworth, M., Waters, E., Blehar, M., and Wall, S. (1978) Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the
Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Akiyama, H., Antonucci, T. C., Takahashi, K., and Langfahl, E. S. (2003) Negative interactions in close relationships
across the lifespan. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 58(2): 70–79.
American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Amnesty International (2008) Women’s human rights. Retrieved November 2008, from www.amnestyusa.org/
violence-against-women/female-genital-mutilation—fgm/page.do?id=1108439.
Andrews, G. (2001) Should depression be managed as a chronic disease? British Medical Journal, 322: 419–421.
Appiah, K. A. (1997) Europe upside down: fallacies of the new Afrocentrism. In R. R. Grinker and C. Steiner (eds),
Perspectives on Africa (pp. 728–731). London: Blackwell.
Arber, S., and Ginn, J. (1991) Gender and Later Life: A Sociological Analysis of Resources and Constraints. London:
Sage.
Arber, S., and Ginn, J. (1995) Connecting Gender and Ageing: A Sociological Approach. Buckingham, Philadelphia:
Open University Press.
Aries, P. (1996) Centuries of Childhood. London: Pimlico.
339
Bibliography
Association of Reproductive Health Professionals (2005) Women’s Sexual Health in Midlife and Beyond.
Washington, DC: Association of Reproductive Health Professionals.
Atchley, R. C. (1989) A continuity theory of normal aging. The Gerontologist, 29(2): 183–190.
Atkinson, J., and Braddick, O. (1982) Sensory and perceptual capacities of the neonate. In P. Stratton (ed.)
Psychobiology of the Human Newborn (pp. 191–220). Chichester, New York: Wiley.
Ayotte, W. (2000) Separated Children Coming to Western Europe: Why They Travel and How They Arrive. London:
Save the Children.
Baillargeon, R. (1987) Object permanence in 31⁄2- and 41⁄2-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 23,
655–664.
Baillargeon, R. (2008) Infant cognition lab. Retrieved November 2008 from www.psych.uiuc.edu/~rbaillar/ICL/
welcome.html.
Bales, K. L., and Carter, S. (2009) Neurobiology and hormonal aspects of romantic relationships. In M. De Haan
and M. R. Gunnar (eds) Handbook of Developmental Social Neuroscience. New York: Guilford Press.
Baltes, P. B. (1987) Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: on the dynamics between
growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23(5): 611–626.
Bamber, C. (2004) From grassroots to statute: the mental health user movement in England. In T. Ryan and J.
Pritchard (eds), Good Practice in Adult Mental Health. London, Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Bandura, A. (1977) Social Learning Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Barn, R., Andrew, L., and Mantovani, N. (2005 ) Life After Care: The Experiences of Young People from Different
Ethnic Groups. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation. Available online: www.jrf.org.uk/bookshop/eBooks/
1859351921.pdf.
Barth, R. P., Crea, T. M., John, K., Thoburn, J., and Quinton, D. (2005) Beyond attachment theory and therapy:
towards sensitive and evidence-based interventions with foster and adoptive families in distress. Child and
Family Social Work 10(4): 257–268.
Bartlik, B., and Goldstein, M. Z. (2000) Practical geriatrics: maintaining sexual health after menopause. Psychiatric
Services, 51:751–806.
Bartlik, B., and Goldstein, M. Z. (2001) Practical geriatrics: men’s sexual health after midlife. Psychiatric Services,
52: 291–302.
Bass, S. (2006) Gerontological theory: the search for the Holy Grail. Gerontologist, 46: 139–144.
Basson, R. (2005) Women’s sexual dysfunction: revised and expanded definitions. CMAJ Canadian Medical
Association Journal, 172(10): 1327–1333.
Bateson P. (2001) Where does our behaviour come from? Journal of Biosciences. 26(5): 561–70.
BBC (2007a) The Sex Lives of Us. Radio 4, 13 September. Information available online at www.bbc.co.uk/
programmes/b007ztwv (accessed 4 December 2008).
BBC (2007b) Woman’s Hour message board, 22 April 2007. From www.bbc.co.uk.
Beart, S. (2005) ‘I won’t think of meself as a learning disability. But I have’: social identity and self-advocacy. British
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(3): 128–131.
Beart, S., Hardy, G., and Buchan, L. (2004) Changing selves: a grounded theory account of belonging to a self-
advocacy group for people with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities,
17: 91–100.
Beck, A. T., and Weishaar, M. (2005) Cognitive Therapy. Belmont, CA: Thomson Brooks/Cole Publishing Co.
Beck, U. (2000) The cosmopolitan society and its enemies. Theory, Culture and Society, 19(2): 17–44.
Beckett, C. (2002) Human Growth and Development. London: Sage.
Bee, H., and Boyd, D. (2007) The Developing Child (11th edn). Boston, New York, London: Pearson.
Belsky, J. (1980) Child maltreatment: an ecological integration. The American Psychologist, 35(4): 320–335.
Belsky, J., Melhuish, E., and Barnes, J. (2007) Evaluating Sure Start: Does Area-Based Early Intervention Work?
Bristol: Policy Press.
340
Bibliography
Bengtson, V. L. (1996) Paradoxes of families and aging. In R. H. Binstock and L. K. George (eds), Handbook of Aging
and the Social Sciences (4th edn, pp. 253–282). San Diego: Academic Press.
Bengtson, V. L., and Achenbaum, W. A. (1994) The changing contract across generations. Gerontologist, 34(4): 564.
Bennett, A. (2005) Untold Stories. London: Faber and Faber.
Bentovim, A. (2002) Preventing sexually abused young people from becoming abusers, and treating the
victimization experiences of young people who offend sexually. Child Abuse and Neglect, 26(6–7): 661–678.
Beresford, P., and CPAG (1999) Poverty First Hand: Poor People Speak For Themselves. London: Child Poverty Action
Group (CPAG).
Biddle, B. J. (1979) Role Theory: Expectations, Identities, and Behaviors. New York: Academic Press.
Biehal, N., Clayden, J., Stein, M., and Wade, J. (1992) Prepared for Living? A Survey of Young People Leaving the
Care of Three Authorities. London: National Children’s Bureau.
Biehal, N., Clayden, M., Stein, M., and Wade, J. (1995) Moving On – Young People and Leaving Care Schemes.
London: HMSO.
Bilton, T. (2002) Introductory Sociology. Basingstoke: Palgrave.
Biggs, S. (1999) The Mature Imagination: Dynamics of Identity in Midlife and Beyond. Buckingham: Open University
Press.
Bingham, M., and Stryker, S. (1995) Things Will Be Different for My Daughter: A Practical Guide to Building her Self-
Esteem and Self-Reliance. New York: Penguin Books.
Blackwell, D. (1997) Psychotherapy, politics and trauma: working with survivors of torture and organized violence.
Journal of Social Work Practice, 11(2): 81–90.
Blakemore, S.-J., Ouden, H., Choudhury, S., and Frith, C. (2007) Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for
thinking about intentions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2: 130–139
Blum, R. W., Resnick, M. D., Nelson, R., and St Germaine, A. (1991) Family and peer issues among adolescents with
spina bifida and cerebral palsy. Pediatrics, 88(2): 280–285.
Bohannan, P. (1970) Divorce and After: An Analysis of the Emotional and Social Problems of Divorce. Garden City,
NY: Anchor.
Botcherby, S., and Hurrell, K. (2004) Ethnic Minority Women and Men. Manchester: Equal Opportunities
Commission.
Bowlby, J. (1979) The Making and Breaking of Affectional Bonds. London: Tavistock Publications.
Bowlby, J., and Robertson, J. (1953) A two-year old goes to hospital. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine,
46(6): 425–427.
Bowlby, J. (1951) Maternal Care and Mental Health: A Report Prepared on behalf of the World Health Organization
as a Contribution to the United Nations Programme for the Welfare of Homeless Children. Geneva: WHO.
Boyd, C., and Bromfield, L. (2006) Young People Who Sexually Abuse: Key Issues. Melbourne: Australian Institute
of Family Studies. http://www.aifs.gov.au/acssa/pubs/wrap/acssa_wrap3.pdf.
Boyd, D. R., and Bee, H. L. (2006) Lifespan Development. Boston: Pearson/Allyn and Bacon.
British Red Cross (2009) Our Services for Refugees. Online: www.redcross.org.uk. From this page, use search term
‘refugees’ or drop-down menu ‘In the UK’. Accessed 21 April 2009.
Brody, E. M. (1985) Parent care as a normative family stress. Gerontologist, 25(1): 19–29.
Brody, E. M., and Saperstein, A. R. (2006) Women in the Middle: Their Parent Care Years. New York: Springer.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979/2006) The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature and Design.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1990) Discovering what families do. In D. Blankenhorn, S. Bayme and J. B. Elshtain (eds),
Rebuilding the Nest: A New Commitment to the American Family. Milwaukee, WI: Family Service America.
Summary available online: www.montana.edu/www4h/process.html. Accessed 23 April 2009.
Brown, S. (2005) Understanding Youth and Crime: Listening to Youth? Maidenhead, New York: Open University Press.
Burman, E. (2000) Deconstructing Developmental Psychology. London and New York: Routledge.
341
Bibliography
Burton, J. M., and Marshall, L. A. (2005) Protective factors for youth considered at risk of criminal behaviour: does
participation in extracurricular activities help? Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health 15(1): 46–64.
Buss, D. M., Abbott, M., Angleitner, A., Asherian, A., et al. (1990) International preferences in selecting mates:
a study of 37 cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 21(1): 5–47.
Butler Sloss, E. (1988) Report of the Inquiry into Child Abuse in Cleveland, 1987: Presented to Parliament by the
Secretary of State for Social Services by Command of Her Majesty, July 1988. London: HMSO.
Butler, I., and Roberts, G. (2004) Social Work With Children and Families: Getting into Practice. London, New York:
Jessica Kingsley.
Butler, J. (1990) Gender Trouble: Feminism and the Subversion of Identity. New York: Routledge.
Butler, J. (1993) Bodies That Matter: On the Discursive Limits of ‘Sex’. New York: Routledge.
Bynner, J. (2002) Young People’s Changing Routes to Independence. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation/YPS.
Online: www.jrf.org.uk/knowledge/findings/socialpolicy/n92.asp.
Cann, J., Falshaw, L., Nugent, F., and Friendship, C. (2003) Understanding What Works: Accredited Cognitive Skills
Programmes For Adult Men and Young Offenders. London: Home Office.
CSAC and Age Concern Cumbria (2006) You Feel As Though You Are Still Someone. Report presented at CSAC
conference, Rheged, June 2006. Cumbria, UK: CSAC.
Casement, P. (2008) Learning From Our Mistakes: Beyond Dogma in Psychoanalysis and Psychotherapy. New York:
Psychology Press.
Cass, V. C. (1984) Homosexual identity: a concept in need of definition. Journal of Homosexuality, 9(2–3): 105–126.
Cassidy, J., and Shaver, P. (eds) (1999) Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Applications. New
York, London: The Guilford Press.
Ceci, S., and Williams, W. (1999) The Nature-Nurture Debate. Oxford: Blackwell.
Centrepoint (1999) Taking Risks. London: Centrepoint.
Centrepoint (2008) Facts About Young People. Retrieved 30 January 2009, from www.centrepoint.org.uk/be-
informed/facts-about-young-people.
Chaban, M. (2000) The Life Work of Dr. Elisabeth Kübler-Ross and Its Impact on the Death Awareness Movement.
New York: Mellen Press.
Chamberlain, D. B. (1992, revised 2008) Babies are not what we thought: call for a new paradigm. International
Journal of Prenatal and Perinatal Studies, 4(3), 1–17.
Chapman, M. G. T., and Woodmansey, A. C. (1985) Policy on child abuse. British Journal of Clinical and Social
Psychiatry, 3 (supplement).
Charlton, L., Crank, M., Kansara, K., and Oliver, C. (1998) Still Screaming. Manchester: After Adoption.
Chisholm, J., Quinlivan, J., Petersen, R., and Coall, D. (2005) Early stress predicts age at menarche and first birth,
adult attachment and expected lifespan. Human Nature, 16(3): 233–265.
Chivers, M. L., Seto, M. C., and Blanchard, R. (2007) Gender and sexual orientation differences in sexual response
to sexual activities versus gender of actors in sexual films. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(6):
1108–1121.
Chodorow, N. (1999) The Reproduction of Mothering: Psychoanalysis and the Sociology of Gender. With a New
Preface. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Cicchetti, D., and Rogosch, F. A. (1997) The role of self-organization in the promotion of resilience in maltreated
children. Development and Psychopathology 9(4): 797–815.
Citizens as Trainers Group, Young Independent People Presenting Educational Entertainment (YIPPEE), Rimmer,
A., and Harwood, K. (2004) Citizen participation in the education and training of social workers. Social Work
Education, 23: 309–323.
Clough, R., Manthorpe, J., and Older People Researching Social Issues (OPRSI) (2007) The Support Older People Want
and the Services They Need. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation. Online: www.jrf.org.uk/sites/files/jrf/1954-
older-people-services.pdf. Accessed February 2009.
342
Bibliography
CLS (2008) National Child Development Study. Online: www.cls.ioe.ac.uk. Accessed 23 April 2009.
Cobb, N. J. (1995) Adolescence: Continuity, Change, and Diversity. Mountain View, CA: Mayfield.
Cohler, B. (1982) Personal narrative and the life course. In P. B. Baltes and O. G. Brim (eds) Life-Span Development
and Behavior. New York: Academic Press.
Colborn, T., and Clement, C. (1992) Chemically-Induced Alterations in Sexual and Functional Development: The
Wildlife/Human Connection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Scientific Publishing Co.
Collison, D., Dey, C., Hannah, G., and Stevenson, L. (2007) Income inequality and child mortality in wealthy nations.
Journal of Public Health, 29(2): 114–117.
Commission for Social Care Inspection (2006) Real Choices – The Qualities People Expect in Home Care Services.
London: CSCI.
Commission for Social Care Inspection (2008) The State of Social Care in England. London: CSCI.
Cooley, C. H. (1902) Human Nature and the Social Order. New York, Chicago, Boston: Scribner.
Cooper, A., and Lousada, J. (2005) Borderline Welfare: Feeling and Fear of Feeling in Modern Welfare. London: Karnac.
Corr, P. J. (2006) Understanding Biological Psychology. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Crawford, C., Dearden, L., and Meghir, C. (2007) When You Are Born Matters: The Impact of Date of Birth On Child
Cognitive Outcomes in England. London: The Institute for Fiscal Studies.
Crispin, T., Milliken, D., and Bews, K. (2005) Armbands in Deep Water: A Summary of Research Into Home Start’s
Home-Visiting Volunteers. Leicester: Home Start. Online: www.home-start.org.uk/about/publications.
Crowley, A., and Vulliamy, C. (2007) Listen Up! Children and Young People Talk: About Poverty. London/Cardiff:
Save the Children Fund. Online: www.savethechildren.org.uk/en/docs/wales_lu_pov.pdf.
Cumming, E. (1975) Engagement with an old theory. International Journal of Aging and Human Development,
6(3): 187–191.
Cumming, E., and Henry, W. (1961) Growing Old: The Process of Disengagement. New York: Basic Books.
Damasio, A. R. (1997) Emotion in the Perspective of An Integrated Nervous System. Paper presented at the
Nobel Symposium: Towards an Understanding of Integrative Brain Functions – Analysis at Multiple Levels,
Stockholm, Sweden, June 1997.
Davies, C., and Jenkins, R. (1997) She has different fits to me; how people with learning difficulties see themselves.
Disability and Society, 12: 95–109.
Davoren, E. (1974) The role of the social worker. In R. E. Helfer and C. H. Kempe (eds) The Battered Child (2nd edn,
p. 262). Chicago, London: University of Chicago Press.
DCFS (2006) Working Together to Safeguard Children. London: The Stationery Office. Online: www.
everychildmatters.gov.uk/_files/AE53C8F9D7AEB1B23E403514A6C1B17D.pdf.
DCSF (2007b) National Curriculum Assessment, GCSE and Equivalent Attainment and Post-16 Attainment by Pupil
Characteristics, in England 2006/07. London: Department for Children, Schools and Families. Online: www.dcsf.
gov.uk/rsgateway/DB/SFR/s000759/SFR38–2007.pdf.
DCSF (2008) Children Looked After in England (Including Adoption and Care Leavers) Year Ending 31 March 2008
(No. SFR 23/2008) London: Department for Children, Schools and Families.
DCSF/Children and Family Court Advisory and Support Service (2006) Putting Your Children First: A Guide For
Separating Parents. Nottingham: Department for Education and Skills.
DCFS/DfES (2004) Parental Separation: Children’s Needs and Parents’ Responsibilities. London: The Stationery
office. Online: www.standards.dfes.gov.uk/eyfs/resources/downloads/dfeschildrensneeds.pdf.
DCSF/DfES (2005) Statutory Guidance on the Duty on Local Authorities to Promote the Educational Achievement
of Looked After Children Under Section 52 of the Children Act 2004. Nottingham: Department for Education
and Skills.
DCFS/DfES (2006) Widening Participation in Higher Education. Nottingham: Department for Education and Skills.
DCSF/DfES (2007) Care Matters: Time for Change. Norwich: Stationery office. Online: www.official-documents.
gov.uk. Retrieved 30 January 2009.
343
Bibliography
DCSF/DfES (2007) Time for Change. Nottingham: Department for Children, Schools and Families/DfES. Online:
www.dcsf.gov.uk/publications/timeforchange/.
Dean, J., and Goodlad, R. (1998) Supporting Community Participation? The Role and Impact of Befriending.
Brighton: Pavilion Publishing in association with the Joseph Rowntree Foundation. Research summary avail-
able online: www.jrf.org.uk/sites/files/jrf/scr038.pdf. Accessed 23 April 2009.
de Bernieres, L. (1994) Captain Corelli’s Mandolin. London: Secker & Warburg.
de Montaigne, M. (1580/1993) The Essays (M. A. Screech, trans.). London: Allen Lane.
Department of Health (2000) Framework for the Assessment of Children in Need and Their Families. London: The
Stationery Office.
Department of Health (2001) Valuing People: A New Strategy For Learning Disability For the 21st Century. London:
Stationery Office.
Department of Health (2005) Tackling Cancer: Improving The Patient Journey. London: Stationery Office. Online:
www.nao.org.uk/publications/NAO_reports/04-05/0405288.pdf..
Department of Health (2006) Tackling Cancer: Improving the Patient Journey: Nineteenth Report of Session
2005–06. London: Stationery Office.
Department of Health (2008) Refocusing the Care Programme Approach. London: Department of Health.
Diamond, L. M. (2008) Sexual Fluidity: Understanding Women’s Love and Desire. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Dimigen, G., Del Priore, C., Butler, S., Evans, S., Ferguson, L., and Swan, M. (1999) Psychiatric disorder among
children at time of entering local authority care: questionnaire survey. British Medical Journal 319: 675. Online:
www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov. Accessed 13 April 2009.
Dirie, W., Milborn, C., and Alabaster, S. (2005) Desert Children. London: Virago.
Dirie, W., and Miller, C. (2006) Desert Flower: The Extraordinary Journey of a Desert Nomad. London: Virago.
Dowd, J. J. (1980) Stratification Among the Aged. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole Pub. Co.
Drobnic, S., Blossfeld, H.-P., and Rohwer, G. (1999) Dynamics of women’s employment patterns over the family life
course: a comparison of the United States and Germany. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61(1): 133–146.
Dunn, J., and Layard, P. R. G. (2009) A Good Childhood: Searching For Values in a Competitive Age. London: Penguin.
DWP (2008) Households Below Average Income; An Analysis of the Income Distribution 1994/95–2006/07. London:
Department for Work and Pensions. Online: www.dwp.gov.uk/asd/hbai/hbai2007/pdf_files/full_hbai08.pdf.
Eapen, V., O’Neill, J., Gurling, H. M., and Robertson, M. M. (1997) Sex of parent transmission effect in Tourette’s
syndrome: evidence for earlier age at onset in maternally transmitted cases suggests a genomic imprinting
effect. Neurology, 48(4): 934–937.
Eisenbruch, M. (1984a) Cross cultural aspects of bereavement 1: A conceptual framework for comparative
analysis. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 8(4): 283–309.
Eisenbruch, M. (1984b) Cross cultural aspects of bereavement 2. Ethnic and cultural variations in the development
of bereavement practices. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 8(4): 315–347.
Elliott, E., and Kiel, L. (2001) Chaos theory in the social sciences: foundations and applications. Ann Arbor,
Michigan: University of Michigan Press.
Emerson, E., Azmi, S., Hatton, C., Caine, A., Parrott, R., and Wolstenholme, J. (1997) Is there an increased prevalence
of severe learning disabilities among British Asians? Ethnicity and Health, 2(4): 317–321.
Emerson, E., and Hatton, C. (2005) The Socio-Economic Circumstances of Families Supporting a Child at Risk of
Disability in Britain in 2002. Lancaster: Institute for Health Research, Lancaster University.
Emerson, E., and Hatton, C. (2008) Research Report 2008(1): People with Learning Disabilities in England. Lancaster:
CEDR: Lancaster University.
Emerson, E., Malam, S., Davies, I., and Spencer, K. (2005) Adults with Learning Disabilities in England, 2003/4. Leeds:
NHS Health and Social Care Information Centre.
Erikson, E. H. (1950/1995) Childhood and Society. London: Vintage.
344
Bibliography
Erikson, E. H. (1985) The Life Cycle Completed: A Review. New York, London: Norton.
Estes, C. L., Biggs, S., and Phillipson, C. (2003) Social Theory, Social Policy and Ageing. Maidenhead: Open University
Press.
Evandrou, M., Glaser, K., and Henz, U. (2002) Caregiving – multiple role occupancy in midlife: balancing work and
family life in Britain. The Gerontologist, 42(6): 781.
Eyman, R. K., and Call, T. L. (1991) Life expectancy of persons with Down’s syndrome. American Journal of Mental
Retardation, 95(6): 603–612.
Eysenck, H. J. (1968) The Scientific Study of Personality. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Eysenck, M. W. (2000) Psychology: A Student’s Handbook. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Fahlberg, V. (1994) A Child’s Journey Through Placement. London: BAAF.
Feil, N. (1989) Validation: The Feil Method: How to Help Disoriented Old-Old. Cleveland, OH: Edward Feil
Productions.
Ferri, E., Bynner, J. and Wadsworth, M. (2003) Changing Britain, Changing Lives: Three Generations at the Turn of
the Century. London: Institute of Education, University of London.
Firth, M., Dwyer, M., Marsden, H., Savage, D., and Mohamad, H. (2004) Non-statutory mental health social work
in primary care: a chance for renewal? British Journal of Social Work, 34, 145–163.
Fitzpatrick, S., Kemp, P., and Klinker, S. (2000) Single Homelessness: An Overview of Research in Britain. Bristol:
Policy Press.
Flavell, J. H., Miller, P. H. and Miller, S. A. (2002) Cognitive Development. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Fonagy, P. (1999) Psychoanalytic theory from the viewpoint of attachment theory and research. In J. Cassidy and
P. Shaver (eds), Handbook of Attachment Theory and Research (pp. 595–624). New York: Guilford Press.
Forde, L. (1999) Report of the Commission of Inquiry into Abuse of Children in Queensland Institutions. Brisbane:
Queensland Government.
Frampton, P. (2004) The Golly in the Cupboard. Manchester: Tamic.
Frankenberg, R. (1996) Cogito ergo doleo. Mortality, 1(2): 213–217.
Frankham, J., Edwards-Kerr, D., Humphrey, N., and Roberts, L. (2007) School Exclusions: Learning Partnerships
Outside Mainstream Education. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation.
Freud, S. (1905/1991) On Sexuality: Three Essays On the Theory of Sexuality and Other Works (J. Strachey and A.
Richards, trans.) London: Penguin.
Friendship, C., Blud, L., and Erikson, M. (2002) An Evaluation of Cognitive Behavioural Treatment For Prisoners.
London: Home Office, Research, Development and Statistics Directorate.
Galinsky, E. (1982) Between Generations: The Six Stages of Parenthood. New York: Berkley Books. Online:
www.dushkin.com/connectext/psy/ch03/parenthood.mhtml.
Garfield, S. (2005) Our Hidden Lives: The Remarkable Diaries of Postwar Britain. London: Ebury.
GBHGIS (Great Britain Historical Geographical Information System) (2008) General Report of the 1921 Census,
With Appendices, 1927. Online: www.visionofbritain.org.uk/census/report_page.jsp?rpt_id=EW1921GENand
show=ALL. Accessed February 2009.
Gelles, R. (1998) The youngest victims: violence towards children. In R. K. Bergen (ed.), Issues in Intimate Violence.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
George, L. K. (1990) Social structure, social processes, and social-psychological states. In R. H. Binstock and L. K.
George (eds), Handbook of Aging and the Social Sciences (pp. 186–204). San Diego: Academic Press.
Gerhardt, S. (2004) Why Love Matters: How Affection Shapes a Baby’s Brain. Hove: Brunner-Routledge.
Giddens, A. (2009) Sociology. Cambridge: Polity.
Gilligan, C. (1982/1993) In a Different Voice: Psychological Theory and Women’s Development. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Glasser, M., Kolvin, I., Campbell, D., Glasser, A., Leitch, I. and Farrelly, S. (2001) Cycle of child sexual abuse: links
between being a victim and becoming a perpetrator. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 482–494.
345
Bibliography
Goffman, E. (1959/2008) The Presentation of Self In Everyday Life. New York: Anchor Books.
Goffman, E. (1963/1990) Stigma: Notes On the Management of Spoiled Identity. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books.
Gordon, D., Adelman, L., Ashworth, K., Bradshaw, J., Levitas, R., Middleton, S. et al. (2000) Poverty and Social
Exclusion In Britain. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation.
Goudarzi, S. (2008) Double-edged sword: education delays dementia, but memory declines faster once it hits.
Scientific American- Mind, 19(1).
Greenberg, L. S., Elliott, R. K., and Lietaer, G. (1994) Research On Experiential Psychotherapies. Oxford: John Wiley
and Sons.
Greenlaw, L. (2007) The Importance of Music to Girls. London: Faber and Faber.
Guba, E. G., and Lincoln, Y. S. (2005) Paradigmatic controversies, contradictions, and emerging influences. In N.
K. Denzin and Y. S. Lincoln (eds), The Sage Handbook of Qualitative Research (3rd edn), pp. 191–215. London:
Sage.
Guse, L., and Masesar, M. (1999) Quality of life and successful aging in long-term care. Perceptions of residents.
Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 20: 527–539.
Haan, N., Millsap, R., and Hartka, E. (1986) As time goes by: change and stability in personality over fifty years.
Psychology and Aging, 1(3): 220–232.
Hakamies-Blomqvist, L. (2006) Are there safe and unsafe drivers? Transportation Research, Part F: Traffic
Psychology and Behaviour, 9(5): 347–352.
Halsey, A. H., Heath, A. F., and Ridge, J. M. (1980) Origins and Destinations: Family, Class, and Education in Modern
Britain. Oxford, New York: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press.
Hanisch, C. (1968/2006) The Personal is Political. Notes from the Second Year: Women’s Liberation. New York:
Radical Feminism. Online: http://scholar.alexanderstreet.com/download/attachments/2259/Personal+Is+Pol.
pdf?version=1.
Hansard (2007) House of Commons Written Answers, 9 January 2007. Online: www.parliament.the-stationery-
office.co.uk.
Hansard (2008a) Hansard 14 Jan 2008 Column GC412 (Children and Young Persons Bill) UK Parliament. Online:
www.parliament.the-stationery-office.co.uk/pa/ld200708/ldhansrd/text/80114-gc0004.htm.
Hansard (2008b) Hansard 3 July 2008 Column 259 (Children and Young Persons Bill) London: UK Parliament.
Online: www.publications.parliament.uk/pa/cm200708/cmpublic/children/080703/pm/80703s08.htm.
Harris, J. R. (2009) The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do. New York: Free Press.
Harrison, L., and Harrington, R. (2001) Adolescents’ bereavement experiences. Prevalence, association with depres-
sive symptoms, and use of services. Journal of Adolescence, 24: 159–170.
Hatloy, I., and Stewart, G. (2007) Suicide Rates, Risks and Prevention Strategies. London: Mind.
Hausdorff, J. M., Levy, B. R., and Wei, J. Y. (1999) The power of ageism on physical function of older persons: rever-
sibility of age-related gait changes. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 47(11): 1346–1349.
Hazan, C., and Zeifman, D. (1999) Pair bonds as attachments: evaluating the evidence. In J. Cassidy and P. Shaver
(eds), Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Applications. New York, London: The Guilford
Press.
Healy, T. (1999) A struggle for language: patterns of self-disclosure in lesbian couples. In J. Laird (ed.), Lesbians
and Lesbian Families: Reflections On Theory and Practice. New York: Columbia University Press.
Heath, A., and McMahon, D. (2001) Ethnic differences in the labour market: the role of education and social class
origins. In H. Goulbourne (ed.) Race and Ethnicity: Critical Concepts in Sociology (pp. 35–59). London, New York:
Routledge.
Hecht, D. T., and Baum, S. K. (1984) Loneliness and attachment patterns in young adults. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 40(1): 193–197.
Heimann, P. (1959/1989) Countertransference. In P. Heimann and M. Tonnesmann (eds) About Children and
Children-no-longer: The Work of Paula Heimann (pp. 151–160). London: Routledge.
346
Bibliography
Hendricks, J. (2004) Public policies and old age identity. Journal of Aging Studies, 18(3): 245–260.
Henry, J. D., MacLeod, M. S., Phillips, L. H., and Crawford, J. R. (2004) A meta-analytic review of prospective memory
and aging. Psychology and Aging, 19(1): 27.
Hersch, J., and Stratton, L. (2002) Housework and wages. Journal of Human Resources, 37(1): 217–229.
Hetherington, R., Cooper, A., Smith, P., and Wilford, G. (1997) Protecting Children: Messages from Europe. Lyme
Regis: Russell House.
Hicks, S. (2000) ‘Good lesbian, bad lesbian. . .’: regulating heterosexuality in fostering and adoption assessments.
Child and Family Social Work, 5(2):157–168.
Higgins, J., and Hirsch, J. (2007) The pleasure deficit: revisiting the “sexuality connection” in reproductive health.
International Family Planning Perspectives, 33(3): 133–139.
Hill, H. (2008) Articles and Research. Online: www.helen-hill.com/ArticlesResearch.htm. Accessed November 2008.
Hirsch, D. (2007) Experiences of Poverty and Educational Disadvantage. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation.
Online: www.jrf.org.uk (use publication ref no 2123).
Hirst, M., and Baldwin, S. (1994) Unequal Opportunities: Growing Up Disabled. London: HMSO.
Hobbs, G., Hobbs, C., and Wynne, J. (1999) Abuse of children in foster and residential care. Child Abuse & Neglect.
23 (12): 1239–1252.
Hobson, C., Cox, J. and Sagovsky, N. (2008) Deserving Dignity: The Independent Asylum Commission’s Third Report
of Conclusions and Recommendations. London: Independent Asylum Commission in association with the
Citizen Organising Foundation.
Hobson, R. (1974) Loneliness. Journal of Analytical Psychology, 19(1): 71–89.
Hochschild, A. R. (1975) Disengagement theory: a critique and proposal. American Sociological Review, 40(5):
553–569.
Hokanson, J. E., Megargee, E. I., O’Hagan, S. E., and Perry, A. M. (1976) Behavioral, emotional, and autonomic reac-
tions to stress among incarcerated, youthful offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 3(3): 203–234.
Hollander, N. C. (2000) Exiles: paradoxes of loss and creativity. In U. McCluskey and C.-A. Hooper (eds)
Psychodynamic Perspectives on Abuse: The Cost of Fear. London: Jessica Kingsley.
Holmes, J. (2000) Attachment theory and abuse: a developmental perspective. In U. McCluskey and C. A. Hooper
(eds), Psychodynamic Perspectives on Abuse: The Cost of Fear. London: Jessica Kingsley.
Holmes, T. H., and Rahe, R. H. (1967) The social readjustment rating scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
11(2): 213–218.
Home Office (2008) Domestic Violence Mini-site. Online: www.crimereduction.homeoffice.gov.uk/dv/dv01.htm.
Accessed 2 February 2009.
Horrobin, D. F. (2002) The Madness of Adam and Eve: How Schizophrenia Shaped Humanity. London: Corgi.
Howe, D. (1995) Attachment Theory For Social Work Practice. Basingstoke: Macmillan.
Howe, D. (2005) Child Abuse and Neglect: Attachment, Development and Intervention. Basingstoke: Palgrave
Macmillan.
Howe, D. (2008) The Emotionally Intelligent Social Worker. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Howe, D., Brandon, M., Hinings, D., and Schofield, G. (1999) Attachment Theory, Child Maltreatment and Family
Support. Basingstoke, New York: Palgrave.
Howe, D., Sawbridge, P., and Hinings, D. (1992) Half a Million Women: Mothers who Lose their Children by
Adoption. London: Penguin.
Howes, C. (1999) Attachment relationships in the context of multiple caregivers. In J. Cassidy and P. Shaver (eds),
Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Applications. New York, London: The Guilford Press.
Huber, D., Veinant, P., and Stoop, R. (2005) Vasopressin and oxytocin excite distinct neuronal populations in the
central amygdala. Science, 308: 245–248.
Humm, M. (1992) Feminisms: A Reader. New York, London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Hutchison, E. D. (2003a) Dimensions of Human Behaviour: The Changing Life Course (2nd edn). London: Sage.
347
Bibliography
Hutchison, E. D. (2003b) Dimensions of Human Behaviour: Person and the Environment (2nd edn). London: Sage.
Iacovou, M. (2004a) Life Chances: the impact of family origins and early childhood experiences on adult outcomes.
London: ESRC. Retrieved 1 July 2009 from www.esrc.ac.uk.
Iacovou, M. (2004b) ‘Life chances: the impact of family origins and early childhood experiences on adult
outcomes’. In Stewart, S. and Vaitilingam, R. (eds) Seven Ages of Man and Woman (pp. 12–15). London: ESRC.
Online: www.esrc.ac.uk.
Jackson, S., Ajayi, S. and Quigley, M. (2005) Going To University From Care. London: Institute of Education.
Jackson, S., and Scott, S. (2002) Gender: A Sociological Reader. London: Routledge.
Jahoda, M. (1982) Employment and Unemployment: A Social-Psychological Analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Jencks, C., and Phillips, M. (1998 ) The black-white test score gap: An introduction. In C. Jencks and M. Phillips
(eds) The Black-White Test Score Gap (pp. 1–51). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution.
Jordan, B. (1991) Social Work in an Unjust Society. Hemel Hempsted: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Joyce, A. (2005) The first six months. In E. Rayner, A. Joyce, J. Rose, M. Twyman and C. Clulow (eds) Human
Development: An Introduction to the Psychodynamics of Growth, Maturity and Ageing (4th edn, pp. 47–70).
London, New York: Routledge.
Kaati, G., Bygren, L. O., and Edvinsson, S. (2002) Cardiovascular and diabetes mortality determined by nutrition
during parents’ and grandparents’ slow growth period. European Journal of Human Genetics, 10(11): 682–688.
Kagan, C., and Tindall, C. (2003) Feminist approaches to counselling. In R. Woolfe, W. Dryden and S. Strawbridge
(eds) Handbook of Counselling Psychology. London, Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Kaplan, H. S. (1979) Disorders of Sexual Desire and Other New Concepts and Techniques in Sex Therapy. New York:
Simon and Schuster.
Kawaguchi, M. C., Welsh, D. P., Powers, S. I., and Rostosky, S. S. (1998) Mothers, fathers, sons, and daughters:
Temperament, gender, and adolescent-parent relationships. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44(1): 77–96.
Keizera, P., Dykstra, A., and Jansen, M. (2008) Pathways into childlessness: evidence of gendered life course
dynamics. Journal of Biosocial Science, 40: 863–878.
Kennedy, A. (2008) Eugenics, ‘degenerate girls’, and social workers during the progressive era. Affilia, 23(1):
22–37.
Kermode, F. (2007) The long life. The London Review of Books, 29(24): 17.
Kershaw, C., Nicholas, S., and Walke, A. (2008) Crime in England and Wales 2007/08; Findings from the British Crime
Survey and Police Recorded Crime. London: Home Office. Online: www.homeoffice.gov.uk/rds/pdfs08/
hosb0708.pdf. Accessed January 2009.
King, G. A., Shultz, I. Z., Steel, K., Gilpin, M., and Cathers, T. (1993) Self-evaluation and self-concept of adolescents
with physical disabilities. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 47(2): 132–140.
Kirkwood, T. B. L. (2001) The End of Age. London: Profile Books.
Klaus, M. H., Kennell, J. H., Plumb, N., and Zuehlke, S. (1970) Human maternal behavior at the first contact with
her young. Pediatrics, 46(2): 187–192.
Klaus, M. H., and Klaus, P. H. (1998) Your Amazing Newborn. Reading, MA: Perseus Books.
Klein, M. (1975) Envy and Gratitude and Other Works. London: Hogarth.
Klentrou, P., and Plyley, M. (2003) Onset of puberty, menstrual frequency, and body fat in elite rhythmic gymnasts
compared with normal controls. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 37(6): 490–494.
Klusmann, D. (2002) Sexual motivation and the duration of partnership. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 31(3):
275–287.
Klusmann, D. (2006) Sperm competition and female procurement of male resources as explanations for a gender-
specific time dependent course in the sexual motivation of couples. Human Nature, 17(3): 283–300.
Koenig, A. L., Cicchetti, D., and Rogosch, F. A. (2004) Moral development: the association between maltreatment
and young children’s prosocial behaviors and moral transgressions. Social Development, 13(1): 97–106.
348
Bibliography
Kohlberg, L., Levine, C., and Hewer, A. (1994) Moral Stages: A Current Formulation and a Response to Critics: 3.
Synopses of Criticisms and a Reply; 4. Summary and Conclusion. New York: Garland Publishing.
Kohli, R. (2007) Working With Unaccompanied Asylum Seeking Children: Issues For Policy and Practice.
Basingstoke, New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
Kohner, N. (2007) Birth to Five: Your Complete Guide to Parenthood and the First Five Years of Your Child’s Life.
London: COI for the Department of Health.
Kristensen Whittaker, M., Brown, J., Beckett, R., and Gerhold, C. (2006) Sexual knowledge and empathy: a com-
parison of adolescent child molesters and non-offending adolescents. Journal of Sexual Aggression, 12(2):
143–154.
Kubler-Ross, E. (1989) On Death and Dying. London: Routledge,.
Lamb, M. E. (2004) The Role of the Father in Child Development. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Online: www.abc.net.au/
rn/talks/lm/stories/s1099987.htm.
Leahy, R. L., and Dowd, E. T. (2002) Clinical Advances in Cognitive Psychotherapy: Theory and Application. New
York: Springer Pub. Co.
Levinson, D. J. (1978) The Seasons of a Man’s Life. New York: Ballantyne.
Levinson, D. J. (1986) A conception of adult development. Amercian Psychologist, 41: 3–13.
Levy, B., and Banaji, M. (2006) Implicit ageism. In T. Nelson (ed.) Ageism: Stereotyping and Prejudice Against Older
Persons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Lewin, K. (1935) A Dynamic Theory of Personality. New York: McGraw Hill Custom Publishing.
Li, J., Precht, D. H., Mortensen, P. B., and Olsen, J. (2003) Mortality in parents after death of a child in Denmark: a
nationwide follow-up study. Lancet, 361: 363–367.
Local Authority Workforce Intelligence Group (2006) Adult, Children and Young People Local Authority Social Care
Workforce Survey 2005. London: Local Authority Workforce Intelligence Group.
Lunney, J. R., Lynn, J., Foley, D. J., Lipson, S., and Guralnik, J. M. (2003) Patterns of functional decline at the end of
life. Journal of the American Medical Association, 289(18): 2387.
Mace, N. L., and Rabins, P. V. (2007) The 36-Hour Day. New York, London: Warner.
Main, M., Kaplan, N., and Cassidy, J. (1985) Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: a move to the level of
representation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1–2): 66–104.
Malina, R. (1990) Physical growth and performance during the transition years. In R. Montemayor, G. R. Adams
and T. Gullotta (eds) From Childhood to Adolescence: A Transitional Period? Newbury Park: Sage.
Mallon, G. P. (1998) We Don’t Exactly Get the Welcome Wagon: The Experiences of Gay and Lesbian Adolescents in
Child Welfare Systems. New York: Columbia University Press.
Marris, P. (1974) Loss and Change. New York: Pantheon Books.
Maslow, A. H., and Frager, R. (1954/2003) Motivation and Personality. New York: Longman.
Masters, W. H., and Johnson, V. E. (1966) Human Sexual Response. London: Churchill.
Mattinson, J. (1975) The Reflection Process in Casework Supervision. London: Institute of Marital Studies, the
Tavistock Institute of Human Relations.
Mayo Clinic (2007) Male Menopause: Myth Or Reality? Minnesota: Mayo Foundation for Medical Education and
Research. Online: www.mayoclinic.com/health/male-menopause/MC00058#.
McAdams, D. P. (1997) The Stories We Live By: Personal Myths and the Making of the Self. New York: Guilford Press.
McAuliffe, D., and Sudbery, J. (2005) ‘Who do I tell?’ Support and consultation in cases of ethical conflict. Journal
of Social Work, 5(1): 21–43.
McCarthy, M. (1999) Sexuality and Women With Learning Disabilities. London, Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley.
McCluskey, U., and Hooper, C.-A. (2000) Psychodynamic Perpectives on Abuse: The Cost of Fear. London,
Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley.
McGuire, J. (ed.) (1995) What Works: Reducing Reoffending – Guidelines from Research and Practice. Chichester:
Wiley.
349
Bibliography
McKittrick, D. (2007) Life expectancy of Irish travellers still at 1940s levels despite economic boom. Independent
(world edition), 28 June.
McLeod, J. (2003) The humanistic paradigm. In R. Woolfe and W. Dryden (eds) Handbook of Counselling
Psychology (pp. 140–160). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
McMurray, I., Connolly, H., Preston-Shoot, M., and Wigley, V. (2008) Constructing resilience: social workers’
understandings and practice. Health and Social Care in the Community, 16(3): 299–309.
Meins, E. (2005) Developmental Outcomes of Joint Attention and Maternal Mind-Mindedness. Swindon:
ESRC.
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Clark-Carter, D., Gupta, M. D., Fradley, E., Tuckey, M. (2003) Pathways
to understanding mind: construct validity and predictive validity of maternal mind-mindedness. Child
Development, 74(4): 1194–1211.
Meltzer, H. (2003) The Mental Health of Young People Looked After By Local Authorities in England. London: Office
for National Statistics.
Mencap (2007) Death By Indifference. London: Mencap.
Mental Health Foundation (2007) Depression and Suicide in Later Life. London: Mental Health Foundation. Online:
www.mentalhealth.org.uk/information/mental-health-a-z/depression-and-suicide-in-later-life/.
Mental Health Foundation (undated) My Daddy Wasn’t Scared of Anything Except Feeling Sad. London: Mental
Health Foundation.
Michael, J. (2008) Healthcare for All: Independent Inquiry Into Access to Healthcare For People With Learning
Disabilities. London: Independent Inquiry into Access to Healthcare for People with Learning Disabilities. Online
in two versions: (easy read version) http://valuingpeople.gov.uk/echo/filedownload.jsp?action=dFileandkey=
2102 (full version) www.iahpld.org.uk/Healthcare_final.pdf.
Michaels, K. (2008) Eight facts about Mills and Boon books. The Times, 25 January.
Mind (undated) Factsheet: Learning Disabilities and Mental Health Problems. Online: www.mind.org.uk/
Information/Factsheets/Learning+disabilities/. Accessed 10 April 2009.
Minsky, R. (1996) Psychoanalysis and Gender. London: Routledge.
Mir, G., Nocon, A., Ahmad, W., and Jones, L. (2001) Learning Difficulties and Ethnicity: Report to the Department
of Health. London: Department of Health.
Monroe, B. (2004) Social work in palliative medicine. In D. Doyle, G. Hanks, N. Cherny and K. Calman (eds) Oxford
Textbook of Palliative Medicine (3rd edn, pp. 1007–1017). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Moore, K. A., and Glei, D. (1995) Taking the plunge: an examination of positive youth development. Journal of
Adolescent Research, 10(1): 15–40.
Morgan, R., and Lindsay, M. (2006) Young People’s Views On Leaving Care. What Young People In, and Formerly In
Residential and Foster Care Think About Leaving Care: A Children’s Rights Director Report. Great Britain:
Commission for Social Care Inspection. Online: www.rights4me.org/content/beheardreports/6/leaving_care_
report.pdf. Accessed 26 April 2009.
Morningstar, B. (1999) Lesbian parents: understanding developmental pathways. In J. Laird (ed.) Lesbians and
Lesbian Families: Reflections On Theory and Practice. New York: Columbia University Press.
Morris, J. (2008) Down’s Syndrome Live Births in England and Wales (198–2006). Online: www.wolfson.qmul.ac.uk/
ndscr/update/livebirths.html. Accessed 10 April 2009.
Morrison, L. L., and L’Heureux, J. (2001) Suicide and gay/lesbian/bisexual youth: implications for clinicians. Journal
of Adolescence, 24(1): 39–49.
Morrow, D., and Messinger, L. (2006) Sexual Orientation and Gender Expression in Social Work Practice Working
With Gay, Lesbian, Bisexual, and Transgender People. New York: Columbia University Press.
Morrow, S. L., and Hawxhurst, D. M. (1998) Feminist therapy: integrating political analysis in counseling and
psychotherapy. Women and Therapy 21(2), 37–50.
Morrow, V., and Richards, M. (1996) Transitions to Adulthood: A Family Matter? York: York Publishing Services.
350
Bibliography
Mughal, A. (2008) Torture: Truths and Consequences Annual Review 2007–2008. London: The Medical Foundation
for the Care of Victims of Torture.
Munday, D., Dale, J., and Murray, S. (2007) Choice and place of death: individual preferences, uncertainty, and the
availability of care. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 100(5): 211–215.
Murphy, S. A., Johnson, L. C., and Lohan, J. (2003) Challenging the myths about parents’ adjustment after the
sudden, violent death of a child. Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 35(4): 359–364.
Myers, J. E., Madathil, J., and Tingle, L. R. (2005) Marriage satisfaction and wellness in India and the United States:
a preliminary comparison of arranged marriages and marriages of choice. Journal of Counseling and
Development, 83(2): 183–190.
Myerscough, G. (1981) I’m Not A Bloody Label! Southsea: Issness.
NACRO (2000) The Forgotten Majority: The Resettlement of Short Term Prisoners. London: National Association for
the Care and Resettlement of Offenders
National Institutes of Health (2005) Tourette Syndrome Fact Sheet. Bethesda: NINDS.
Nelson, H. W. (2000) Injustice and conflict in nursing homes: toward advocacy and exchange. Journal of Aging
Studies, 14(1): 39–61.
Neugarten, B. (1977) Personality and ageing. In J. E. Birren and K. W. Schaie (eds) Handbook of the Psychology of
Aging. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Newman, T. (2002) Promoting Resilience: A Review of Effective Strategies for Child Care Services. Exeter: Centre
for Evidence-Based Social Services. Online: www.barnardos.org.uk/resources/researchpublications/documents/
RESILSUM.PDF.
Nirje, B. (1999) How I came to formulate the normalization principle. In R. J. Flynn and R. A. Lemay (eds) A Quarter-
Century of Normalization and Social Role Valorization: Evolution and Impact. Ottawa: University of Ottawa
Press.
O’Brien, J. (1989) What’s Worth Working For?: Leadership For Better Quality Human Services. Syracuse, NY: Center
on Human Policy, Syracuse University.
O’Connor, T. G., and Nilson, W. (2005) Models versus metaphors in translating attachment theory to the clinic and
community. In L. J. Berlin, Y. Ziv, L. Amaya-Jackons and M. T. Greenberg (eds) Enhancing Early Attachments:
Theory, Research, Intervention and Policy (pp. 313–326). New York and London: Guilford Press.
O’Neill, J., and O’Neill, D. (2005) What Do Wage Differentials Tell Us About Labor Market Discrimination? New York:
National Bureau for Economic Research.
Obholzer, A., and Roberts, V. Z. (2005) The Unconscious at Work: Individual and Organizational Stress in The Human
Services. London, New York: Routledge.
OECD (2007) Education at a Glance 2007. Organisation For Economic Co-Operation and Development.
Office for National Statistics (ONS) (2004) Life Expectancy – More aged 70 and 80 than ever before. London: ONS.
Office for National Statistics (2005a) Focus on Older People. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan. Online: www.
statistics.gov.uk/focuson/olderpeople/.
Office for National Statistics (ONS) (2006) Cancer Survival: England and Wales, 1991–2001. www.statistics.gov.uk/
StatBase/Product.asp?vlnk=10821andMore=Y. Accessed 11 April 2009.
Office for National Statistics (ONS) (2007a) Interim Life Tables. Online: www.statistics.gov.uk, using search term
‘Interim life tables’. Accessed 11 April 2008.
Office for National Statistics (ONS) (2007b) Calculating Expectations of Life. Online: www.statistics.gov.uk/cci/
nugget.asp?id=1898. Accessed 11 April 2009.
Office for National Statistics (ONS) (2007c) Mortality Statistics General Review of the Registrar General on Deaths
in England and Wales, 2005. London: ONS.
Office for National Statistics (ONS) (2007d) Infant and perinatal mortality by social and biological factors, 2006.
Health Statistics Quarterly 36: 84–88.
Office for National Statistics (ONS) (2008a) Focus On Gender. Newport: Office for National Statistics. Online:
351
Bibliography
www.statistics.gov.uk. For the specific detail referenced in Chapter 8, a separated ‘nugget’ is available: www.
statistics.gov.uk/cci/nugget.asp?id=1657.
Office for National Statistics (ONS) (2008b) Ageing: More Pensioners Than Under-16’s For First Time Ever.
www.statistics.gov.uk/cci/nugget.asp?ID=949. Accessed February 2009.
Office for National Statistics (ONS) (2008c) Large Differences in Infant Mortality By Ethnic Group. www.statistics.
gov.uk/pdfdir/imeth0608.pdf. Accessed 9 February 2009.
Office for National Statistics (ONS) (2008d) Deaths: Age and Sex, Numbers and Rates, 1976 Onwards (England and
Wales): Population Trends 132 Dataset Name: PT13061. www.statistics.gov.uk/STATBASE/ssdataset.asp?vlnk=
9552. Accessed 1 Jan 2009.
Office for National Statistics (ONS) (2009a) Suicides. Online: www.statistics.gov.uk.
Olsen, T. S., Dehlendorff, C. and Andersen, K. K. (2007) Sex-related time-dependent variations in post-stroke
survival — evidence of a female stroke survival advantage. Neuroepidemiology, 29(3–4): 218–225.
Orbach, I., Gross, Y., Glaubman, H., and Berman, D. (1986) Children’s perception of various determinants of the death
concept as a function of intelligence, age, and anxiety. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 15(2): 120–126.
Palmer, G., MacInnes, T., Kenway, P., and Joseph Rowntree Foundation (2007) Monitoring Poverty and Social
Exclusion 2007. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation. Online: www.jrf.org.uk/sites/files/jrf/2152-poverty-social-
exclusion. pdf.
Parkes, C. M., Stevenson-Hynde, J., and Marris, M. (1991) Attachment Across the Life Cycle. London: Routledge.
Parkes, C. M., Laungani, P., and Young, B. (1996) Death and Bereavement Across Cultures. London: Routledge.
Parliament of Australia (2001) Child Migrants from the United Kingdom. Online: www.aph.gov.au/library/intguide/
sp/childmigrantuk.htm. Accessed 11 April 2009.
Parry, G., Cleemput, P. V., Peters, J., Walters, S., Thomas, K., and Cooper, C. (2004) The Health Status of Gypsies and
Travellers: Report to Department of Health. University of Sheffield.
Paul, S. N., Kato, B. S., Hunkin, J. L., Vivekanandan, S., and Spector, T. D. (2006) The big finger: the second to fourth
digit ratio is a predictor of sporting ability in women. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 40(12): 981–983.
Paus, T., Zijdenbos, A., Worsley, K., Collins, D. L., Blumenthal, J., Giedd, J. N., et al. (1999) Structural maturation of
neural pathways in children and adolescents: in vivo study. Science, 283: 1908–1911.
Payne, H., and Butler, I. (1998) Improving the health care process and determining health outcomes for children
looked after by the local authority. Ambulatory Child Health, 4: 165–172.
Payne, M. (2005) The Origins of Social Work. Basingstoke, New York: Palgrave.
Pendergast, D. R., Fisher, N. M., and Calkins, E. (1993) Cardiovascular, neuromuscular, and metabolic alterations
with age leading to frailty. Journal of Gerontology, 48 Spec No, 61–67.
Perlman, H. H. (1957) Social Casework, A Problem-Solving Process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Persaud, R., and Royal College of Psychiatrists (2007) The Mind: A User’s Guide. London: Bantam.
Phillipson, C. (1982) Capitalism and The Construction of Old Age. London: Macmillan.
Phinney, J. S., and Rosenthal, D. A. (1992) Ethnic identity in adolescence: process, context, and outcome. In G. R.
Adams, T. P. Gullotta and R. Montemayor (eds) Adolescent Identity Formation. Newbury Park: Sage.
Piaget, J. (1950/1997) The Origin of Intelligence inThe Child. London, New York: Routledge.
Piaget, J., Gruber, H. E., and Vonèche, J. J. (1995) The Essential Piaget. Northvale, NJ: J. Aronson.
Pillemer, K., and Suitor, J. J. (1998) Violence and violent feelings: what causes them among family caregivers? In
R. K. Bergen (ed.) Issues inIntimate Violence (pp. 255–266). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Pinker, S. (2008) The Sexual Paradox: Men, Women and the Real Gender Gap. New York: Scribner.
Piontelli, A. (2002) Twins: From Fetus to Child. London, New York: Routledge.
Pollert, A. (1996) Gender and class revisited: or the poverty of patriarchy. Sociology, 30(4): 639–659.
Potts, T. (2006) Quest Digest. London: Quest. Online: www.questgaycatholic.org.uk/publications/digest/digest05.
pdf. Accessed May 2008.
352
Bibliography
353
Bibliography
Roker, D. (1998) Worth More Than This: Young People Growing Up in Family Poverty. London: Children’s
Society.
Rothamsted Research (2008) Molecular-biology-notebook. Online: www.rothamsted.ac.uk/notebook/courses/
guide/dnast.htm. Accessed 17 November 2008,.
Rutter, J. (2001) Supporting Refugee Children in 21st Century Britain. Staffordshire: Trentham Books.
Rutter, M. (2006) Genes and Behavior: Nature-Nurture Interplay Explained. Malden, MA, Oxford: Blackwell
Rutter, M., O Connor, T., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Dunn, J., Groothues, C. (2000) Recovery and deficit following early
profound deprivation. In P. Selman (ed.) Intercountry Adoption: Developments, Trends and Perspectives.
London: BAAF.
Ryan, T. and Pritchard, J. (eds) (2004) Good Practice in Adult Mental Health. London, Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley
Publishers.
Sarantakos, S. (2005) Social Research. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
Scarr, S., and McCartney, K. (1983) How people make their own environments: a theory of genotype-environment
effects. Child Development, 54(2): 424–435.
Schaffer, H. (1996) Social Development. Oxford: Blackwell.
Schonert-Reichl, K. A. (1999) Relations of peer acceptance, friendship adjustment, and social behavior to moral
reasoning during early adolescence. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 19(2): 249–279.
Schore, A. (1994) Affect Regulation and the Origin of the Self: The Neurobiology of Emotional Development.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schore, A. (1999) Foreword. In Bowlby, J., Attachment and Loss. Vol.1, Attachment. London: Hogarth Press.
Schore, A. (2001a) The effects of a secure attachment relationship on right brain development, affect regulation
and infant mental health. Development and Psychopathology, 8: 59–87. Online: www.trauma-pages.com/
articles.php#Schore. Accessed 12 April 2009.
Schore, A. (2001b) The effects of early relational trauma on right brain development, affect regulation, and infant
mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22: 201–269. Online: www.trauma-pages.com/articles.php#
Schore. Accessed 12 April 2009.
Schore, A. N. (2003) Affect Dysregulation and Disorders of the Self. London: Norton.
Schwab, R. (1998) A child’s death and divorce: dispelling the myth. Death Studies, 22(5): 445–468.
Scott, M. J., and Dryden, W. (2003) The cognitive-behavioural paradigm. In R. Woolfe, W. Dryden and S.
Strawbridge (eds) Handbook of Counselling Psychology. London, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Seale, C. (1998) Constructing Death: The Sociology of Dying and Bereavement. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Sergo, P. (2008) Predicting Alzheimer’s: a new technique may give years of advanced warning. Scientific American
Mind, 19(1).
Shachar, R. (1991) His and her marital satisfaction. Sex Roles, 21: 451–467.
Shelter (2008) What is Homelessness? Online: http://england.shelter.org.uk/get_advice/advice_topics/
homelessness/what_is_homelessness. Accessed 30 January 2009.
Shneidman, E. S. (1995) Voices of Death. New York: Kodansha International.
Sidebotham, P., Perwez, S., Horwath, J., Powell, C., and Fox, J. (2007) Responding to childhood deaths. Exchange,
(4): 9. Original and extended version available in NSPCC (2007) Inform (July 2007). Online: www.nspcc.org.uk/
Inform/publications/Serials/Exchange/respondingtochildhooddeaths_wda47953.html.
Siegenthaler, A. L., and Bigner, J. J. (2000) The value of children to lesbian and non-lesbian mothers. Journal of
Homosexuality, 39(2): 73–91.
Silverberg, S. B., and Steinberg, L. (1987) Adolescent autonomy, parent-adolescent conflict, and parental well-
being. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16: 293–312.
Simon, A., Epstein, L. J., and Association., A. P. (1968) Aging in Modern Society. Washington: American Psychiatric
Association Committee on Research.
354
Bibliography
Singh, S., Wulf, D., Samara, R., and Cuca, Y. P. (2000) Gender differences in the timing of first intercourse: data
from 14 countries. International Family Planning Perspectives, 26(1): 21–28, 43.
Singleton, N., Bumpstead, R., O’Brien, M., Lee, A., and Meltzer, H. (2001) Psychiatric Morbidity Among Adults Living
in Private Households, 2000: The Report of a Survey Carried Out By Social Survey Division of the Office For
National Statistics On Behalf of the Department of Health, the Scottish Executive, and the National Assembly
For Wales. London: Stationery Office.
Skinner, B. F. (1971) Beyond Freedom and Dignity. New York: Knopf.
Slaughter, V. (2005) Young children’s understanding of death. Australian Psychologist 40(3): 179–186.
Sloper, T., and Beresford, B. (2006) Families with disabled children. British Medical Journal 333: 928–929.
Small, H. (2007) The Long Life. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Smallwood, S., and Wilson, B. (2007) Focus on Families. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan/ Office of National
Statistics. Online: www.statistics.gov.uk/downloads/theme_compendia/fof2007/FO_Families_2007.pdf.
Smith, M., Edgar, G., and Groom, G. (2008) Health expectancies in the United Kingdom, 2004–06. Health Statistics
Quarterly, 40. Online: www.statistics.gov.uk.
Smith, P. K., Blades, M., and Cowie, H. (2005) Understanding Children’s Development. Malden: Blackwell.
Smitsman, A. W. (2001) Action in infancy: perspectives, concepts and challenges, development of reaching and
grasping. In G. Bremner and F. Fogel (eds) Blackwell Handbook of Infant Development (pp. 71–99). Oxford:
Blackwell.
Sneed, J. R., Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Gilligan, C., Chen, H., Crawford, T. N., et al. (2006) Gender differences in the
age-changing relationship between instrumentality and family contact in emerging adulthood. Developmental
Psychology, 42(5): 787–797.
Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Holmes, C. J., Jernigan, T. L., and Toga, A. W. (1999) In vivo evidence for post-
adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions. Nature Neuroscience, 2(10): 859–861.
Spencer, M. B., and Dornbusch, S. (1990) Challenges in studying minority youth. In S. S. Feldman and G. R. Elliott
(eds) At the Threshold: The Developing Adolescent. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Sroufe, L. (1988) The role of infant-caregiver attachment in development. In J. Belsky and T. Nezworski (eds)
Clinical Implications of Attachment Theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Steele, M., Hodges, J., Kaniuk, J., Hillman, S., and Henderson, K. (2003) Attachment representations and adoption:
Associations between maternal states of mind and emotion narratives in previously maltreated children.
Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 29(2): 187–205.
Stein, M. (2004) What Works For Young People Leaving Care? Ilford: Barnardo’s.
Sternbach, H. (1998) Age-associated testosterone decline in men: clinical issues for psychiatry. The American
Journal of Psychiatry, 155(10): 1310–1318.
Stewart, S., and Vaitilingam, R. (eds) (2004) Seven Ages of Man and Woman. London: ESRC. Online: www. esrc.ac.uk.
Straus, M. A. (1999) The controversy over domestic violence by women: a methodological, theoretical, and
sociology of science analysis. In X. B. Arriaga and S. Oskamp (eds) Violence inIntimate Relationships. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Stroebe, M. S. (2008) Handbook of Bereavement Research and Practice: Advances in Theory and Intervention.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. See Does ‘Grief Work’ Work, online: www.mabf.org.uk/
newsletter_archive.asp?archive=27 November 2008.
Stroebe, M. and Schut, H. (1999) The dual process model of coping with bereavement. Death Studies, 23: 197–224.
Stuart-Hamilton, I. (2006) The Psychology of Ageing: An Introduction. London: Jessica Kingsley.
Sudbery, J. (2002) Key features of therapeutic social work: the use of relationship. Journal of Social Work Practice,
16(2): 149–162.1
Sudbery, J., Hicks, S., Thompson, S. McLaughlin, H. and Bramley, C., with Wilson, K. (2005) A Bibliography of Family
Placement Literature: A Guide to Publications On Children, Parents and Carers. London: British Association for
Adoption and Fostering.
355
Bibliography
Sudbery, J., and Blenkinship, A. (2005) ‘Acting as a good parent would’? Psychosocial support for parents in a
children’s hospital. Journal of Social Work Practice, 19(1): 43–57.
Sudbery, J., and Bradley, J. (1996) Staff support in organisations providing therapeutic care. Journal of Social Work
Practice, 10(1): 51–62.
Sudbery, P., and Sudbery, I. (2009) Human Molecular Genetics. Harlow: Pearson Prentice Hall.
Sugarman, L. (2001) Lifespan Development: Theories, Concepts and Interventions. Hove: Psychology Press.
Sullivan, A. (2002) Alone again, naturally: the Catholic Church and the homosexual. In E. F. Rogers (ed.) Theology
and Sexuality: Classic and Contemporary Readings. Oxford, Malden: Blackwell Publishers.
Summerfield, C. and Babb, P. (2003) Social Trends. No. 33, 2003. London: Stationery Office. Online: www.statistics.
gov.uk/downloads/theme_social/Social_Trends33/Social_Trends_33.pdf.
Sutherland, N. S. (1998) Breakdown: A Personal Crisis and a Medical Dilemma. Oxford, New York: Oxford University
Press.
Svanberg, P. O. G. (1998) Attachment, resilience and prevention. Journal of Mental Health, 7(6): 543–578.
Tajfel, H., and Turner, J. (1982) Social identity and intergroup relations. In W. G. Austin and S. Worchel (eds) The
Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Tanner, J. M. (1992) Growth as a measure of the nutritional and hygienic status of a population. Hormone
Research, 38(Suppl. 1): 106–115.
Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Gurung, R. A. R., and Updegraff, J. A. (2000) Biobehavioral
responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review, 107(3): 411–429.
Tharinger, D., and Wells, G. (2000) An attachment perspective on the developmental challenges of gay and lesbian
adolescents: the need for continuity of caregiving from family and schools. School Psychology Review, 29:
158–172.
Thompson, R. (2005) Multiple relationship multiply considered. Human Development, 48, 102–107.
Thompson, S. (1990) Putting a big thing into a little hole: Teenage girls’ accounts of sexual initiation. Journal of
Sex Research, 27(3):341–361.
Tomassini, C. (2005) Demographic profile. In A. Soule, P. Babb, M. Evandrou, S. Balchin and L. Zealey (eds) Focus
On Older People (pp. 1–10). Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan. Online: www.statistics.gov.uk/downloads/theme
_compendia/foop05/Olderpeople2005.pdf.
Townsend, P. (1979) Poverty in the United Kingdom: A Survey of Household Resources and Standards of Living.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
Truax, C. B., and Carkhuff, R. R. (1967) Toward Effective Counseling and Psychotherapy: Training and Practice.
Chicago: Aldine Pub. Co.
Truckle, S. (2000) Treatment or torture? Working with issues of abuse and torture in the transference. In U. McCluskey
and C.-A. Hooper (eds) Psychodynamic Perpectives on Abuse: The Cost of Fear. London: Jessica Kingsley.
UK Parliament (2007) Joint Committee on the Human Tissue and Embryo (Draft) Bill. Session 2006–2007. Volume
1: Report. London: The Stationery Office.
UK Parliament (2008) A Life Like Any Other? Human Rights of Adults with Learning Disabilities. London
Parliamentary Joint Committee on Human Rights. Online: www.publications.parliament.uk/pa/jt200708/
jtselect/jtrights/40/40i.pdf.
UNHCR (2007) 2007 Global Trends: Refugees, Asylum-seekers, Returnees, Internally Displaced and Stateless
Persons. Geneva: UNHCR.
US Department of Health and Human Services (2007) Re-authorization of Headstart Act. Washington.
van Anders, S. M., Hamilton, L. D., Schmidt, N., and Watson, N. V. (2007a) Associations between testosterone
secretion and sexual activity in women. Hormones and Behavior, 51(4): 477–482.
van Anders, S. M., Hamilton, L. D., and Watson, N. V. (2007b) Multiple partners are associated with higher
testosterone in North American men and women. Hormones and Behavior, 51(3): 454–459.
356
Bibliography
van Anders, S. M., and Watson, N. V. (2007c) Testosterone levels in women and men who are single, in long-
distance relationships, or same-city relationships. Hormones and Behavior, 51(2): 286–291.
van den Akker, O. andre, J., and Murphy, T. (1999) Adolescent sexual behaviour and knowledge. British Journal of
Midwifery, 7(12): 765–769.
Vincent, J. A. (2003) Old Age. London, New York: Routledge.
Vincent, N. (2006) Self-Made Man: My Year Disguised As a Man. London: Atlantic.
Von Klitzing, K., Simoni, H., and Bürgin, D. (1999) Child development and early triadic relationships. International
Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 80: 71–89.
Vygotskij, L. S., and Cole, M. (1978) L.S. Vygotsky: Mind in Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Waddell, M. (2002) Inside Lives. London: Duckworth.
Wade, J., and Dixon, J. (2006) Making a home, finding a job: Investigating early housing and employment out-
comes for young people leaving care. Child and Family Social Work, 11(3):199–208. Research report online:
www.york.ac.uk/inst/spru/research/pdf/leaving.pdf.
Walby, S., and Allen, J. (2004) Domestic Violence, Sexual Assault and Stalking: Findings From the British Crime
Survey. London: Home Office. Online: www.homeoffice.gov.uk/rds/pdfs04/hors276.pdf.
Waldfogel, J. (1998) Understanding the gender gap in pay for women with children. Journal of Economic
Perspectives, 12(1): 137–156.
Walsh, L., Turner, S., Lines, S., Hussey, L., Chen, Y., and Agius, R. (2005) The incidence of work-related illness in the
UK health and social work sector: The Health and Occupation Reporting network 2002–2003. Occupational
Medicine, 55(4): 262–267.
Ward, C. A., Bochner, S., and Furnham, A. (2001) The Psychology of Culture Shock. Hove, Philadelphia: Routledge
Warr, P. B., and Warr, P. (2002) Psychology At Work (5th edn). London: Penguin.
Watson, J. B. (1930) Behaviorism. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Weiner, G. (1994) Feminisms in Education: An Introduction. Buckingham: Open University Press.
Weiss, R. S. (1980) Loneliness: The Experience of Emotional and Social Isolation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wellings, K., Field, J., Johnson, A. M., and Wadsworth, J. (eds) (1994) Sexual Behavior in Britain: The National Survey
of Sexual Attitudes and Lifestyles. London: Penguin Books.
Wellings, K., Nanchahal, K., Macdowall, W., McManus, S., Erens, B., Mercer, C., et al. (2001) Sexual behaviour in
Britain: early heterosexual experience. Lancet, 358: 1843–1850.
What Makes the Difference/National Leaving Care Advisory Service (2008) Written evidence from What Makes the
Difference and the National Leaving Care Advisory Service 10 March 2008. Online: www.parliament.the-
stationery-office.com/pa/cm200708/cmselect/cmchilsch/359/359we01.htm. Accessed 11 April 2009.
White, S. (undated) Death of a Child. Online: www.childbereavement.org.uk/files/Death_of_a_Child_6pp.pdf.
Accessed 11 April 2009.
Whitten, P. L. (1992) Chemical revolution to sexual revolution: historical changes in human reproductive
development. In T. Colborn and C. Clement (eds) Chemically-Induced Alterations in Sexual and Functional
Development: The Wildlife/Human Connection (pp. 311–334). Princeton, NJ: Princeton Scientific Publishing Co.
WHO (World Health Organisation) (2006) A factual overview of female genital mutilation. Progress in Sexual and
Reproductive Health Research, 72: 2–7. Online: www.who.int/reproductive-health/hrp/progress/72.pdf.
Wikeley, F., Bullock, K., Muschamp, Y., and Ridge, T. (2007) Educational Relationships Outside School: Why Access
Is Important. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation.
Wilson, B., and Smallwood, S. (2008) The proportion of marriages ending in divorce. Population Trends, (131):
8–36. Online: http:statistics.gov.uk.
Wilson, D., Sharp, C., and Patterson, A. (2006) Young People and Crime: Findings from the 2005 Offending, Crime
and Justice Survey (OCJS). London: The Home Office. Online: www.homeoffice.gov.uk/rds/pdfs06/hosb1706.
pdf.
357
Bibliography
Wilson, K., Ruch, G., Lymbery, M., and Cooper, A. (2008) Social Work: An Introduction to Contemporary Practice.
Harlow: Pearson.
Wine, J. D. (1989/2007) Gynocentric values and feminist psychology. In A. Miles and G. Finn (eds) Feminism: From
Pressure to Politics. Montreal: Black Rose Books.
Winnicott, D. W. (1952/2007) Through Paediatrics to Psycho-Analysis. London: The Hogarth Press and the Institute
of Psycho-Analysis. Available through online subscription: www.pep-web.org/document.php?id=ipl.100.0001a.
Winnicott, D. W. (1979) The Maturational Processes and the Facilitating Environment: Studies in the Theory of
Emotional Development. London: Hogarth Press.
Winterowd, C., Beck, A. T., and Gruener, D. (2003) Cognitive Therapy With Chronic Pain Patients. New York:
Springer.
Women and Work Commission. (2006) Shaping a Fairer Future. London: Department of Trade and Industry. Online:
www.equalities.gov.uk/pdf/Shaping%20a%20Fairer%20Future%20report.pdf.
Woodmansey, A. C. (1966) The internalization of external conflict. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, XLVII:
349–355.
Woodmansey, C. (1972) The Unity of Casework. Social Work Today, 2(19): 10–12.
Woodmansey, C. (1989) Internal Conflict. British Journal of Psychotherapy, 6(1): 26–49.
Woods, M., and Hollis, F. (1968/2000) Casework: A Psychosocial Therapy (5th edn). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Worden, J. W. (2003) Grief Counselling and Grief Therapy: A Handbook For the Mental Health Practitioner.
Philadelphia, Hove: Brunner-Routledge.
Xu, X., and Whyte, M. (1990) Love matches and arranged marriages: a Chinese replication. Journal of Marriage
and the Family, 52(8): 709–722.
Yelsma, P., and Athappilly, K. (1988) Marital satisfaction and communication practices: Comparisons among
Indian and American couples. Journal of Comparative Family Studies 19(1): 37–54.
Yu, M., and Stiffman, A. R. (2007) Culture and environment as predictors of alcohol abuse/dependence symptoms
in American Indian youths. Addictive Behaviors 32(10): 2253–2259.
Zelt, D. (1981) First person account: the Messiah quest. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 7(3): 527–531.
358
Index
Abbott, P. 80, 121 243, 244; Erikson’s psychosocial theory 44, 249,
Achenbaum, W.A. 240, 326 316; feminist theories of 241–2, 328; and genetic
active gene influences 25 disease 237; healthcare philosophy 238; and
activity theory 15, 240–1, 327 identity 215–16, 232, 248–9, 327; and life
Adams, J.D. 116, 266 expectancy 233, 242; lifespan development model
adolescence 99–126; antisocial behaviour 91; and 286; and long-term illness 244; loss of
attachment needs in 106, 107–8, 112, 123; function and emotional and cognitive factors 234;
biological changes 102–4; children in care and the mature imagination 248–9, 326, 327;
111–13; and culture 104–6, 123; death rate in political economy of 15, 248, 328; and poverty
103; and disability 113–15; effects of poverty in 195, 198, 243–4; residential care 245–6, 247;
196; Erikson’s psychosocial model 44, 111, 124, social care and support 244–6, 249; social change
189, 315; experiences of bereavement 270–1; gay 242–3; social disengagement model 15, 239–40,
and lesbian young people 109–10; and identity 326, 327; social exchange theory 247–8, 326; and
110–11, 112, 124, 315; psychodynamics of 106–8; stereotyping 234–5; terminology problems 232;
and sex 108–10, 160, 165–6; states of mind theories of 15, 238–42; violence towards older
106–7; transitions in 104–5, 115–16, 124 people 202–3
adoption 27, 58, 184, 294 Ainsworth, M. 56, 297
Adult Attachment Interview 298 alcohol 141, 206
adulthood: development stages 188–9; Erikson’s Alzheimer’s disease 204, 210, 237
stages of development 189; Levinson’s life American Psychiatric Association 24, 205
structures 189; lifespan theory 189 Amnesty International 131
Adults with Learning Difficulties in England 2003/4 antisocial behaviour: 24,106; cognitive behavioural
221 approach 88, 91, 96; disrupted attachments a risk
age: of consent 105; of criminal responsibility 105; of factor for 60; linked to lower levels of moral
majority 105 reasoning 91
Age Concern 257–8 Arber, S. 242, 328
ageing 229–52; at 100 years 233; activity theory 15, arousal–relaxation cycle 56–7
240–1, 327; bodily change 235–8; and carers 244, arthritis 236
246, 249; cognitive functioning 237–8; continuity Ashfar 239, 243, 249
theory 241, 327; cultural factors in experiences of Assessment Framework for Children In Need 78
359
Index
Association of Reproductive Health Professionals 166, rates 198–9, 256; interaction with the mother 36,
168 37–8, 139–40; prenatal development 9; rage in 48,
asylum seekers: see refugees and asylum seekers 56; social nature of young children 36; states of
Atchley, R.C. 241, 249, 327 mind 48
attachment theory 14, 52–61, 296–300; and abused Baldwin, S. 114, 115
children 84; adolescent needs 106, 107–8, 112, Baltes, P. 285, 286
123; and adult pair bonds 162–3, 173; and animal Bamber, C. 208, 209
behaviour 54; arousal–relaxation cycle 56–7; Bandura, A. 75, 88, 92, 324
attachment behaviour 52, 296, 298; attachment Barth, R.P. 58, 300
relationships 52–3, 59–60, 297; attachment Basson, R. 166, 168
systems 53, 54, 56, 58, 67–8; babies’ attachments Beart, S. 217
38, 53, 55, 59–60, 162, 296; background to 52–3; Beck, A.T. 75, 136
and bereavement 58, 265, 298; and biology 55; Beckett, C. 108, 120, 273
caution in applying 58,61, 300; characteristics of Bee, H.L. 9, 37, 73, 77, 90, 91, 102, 103, 104, 106, 120,
early attachment 162; children deprived of 178, 181, 187, 188, 189, 213, 236, 238, 240, 245,
attachments 57, 60, 107, 204–5; children in care 249, 273, 285
57–8, 60, 107–8, 112, 284, 299; classification of behaviour: gene–environmental antecedents of 23–4;
styles of attachment (secure, etc) 56–8, 60, 297–8; hormonal regulation of 37–8; and learning 73–7;
co-regulation 38, 43, 55, 227, 297; criticisms of negative reinforcement of 77; rewards and
58–9, 299–300; disrupted and failed attachments sanctions to shape 91; of young children 37
38, 55, 60, 84, 107, 204–5; and exploratory system behavioural learning theories 15, 76–7, 88, 323–5;
36,53, 54, 56, 298; and educational achievements and behavioural interventions 324; classical
106; father–child relationship 181; frequently conditioning 73, 74, 77, 95, 323, 324; early
asked questions 59–61; influence of parents’ conditioning and risk factors for mental health
attachment style on children 58, 60; internal problems 204–5; epistemological framework 282;
working models 53, 56–7, 84, 297, 298; loneliness operant conditioning 73, 74, 95, 323, 324;
and disrupted attachments 134; multiple perspective on moral behaviour 91–2; relation to
attachments 60, 299; psychological experience attachment theory 299; social learning theory 15,
56–7; reactions to the loss of a partner 169–70; 73, 75, 88, 92, 95, 323–4; ; see also cognitive
and reduced risks of early pregnancy 106; relation behavioural’being held in mind’ 49
to other models of human development 299; and Belsky, J. 79, 303
resilience 55–6; risks for mental health problems Bengtson, V.L. 168, 240, 326
60, 204; secure base 36, 52–3, 56, 106 162, Bennett, A. 160–1
296separation from the attachment figure 57, bereavement 58, 255–6; and attachment theory 58,
265, 296–9; as a social construct 58–9; through 265, 298; cultural 273; dual process model in
the life course 53, 58, 107, 298 response to 269, 275; Freud on 255; Kubler-Ross
autonomy: building identity through 42, 43, 50; 267–9, 275, 329; Stroeb and Schut 269,272, 275,
and building resilience 86; male as opposed to 277, 330; Worden’ 270, 275, 330; young people’s
female concepts of 110, 132; in the workplace experiences of 270–2; see also grief
186 Beresford, P. 114, 195, 196, 197
Biggs, S. 248, 326, 327
babies: attachments 38, 53, 55, 59–60, 162, 296; birth Bilton, T. 81, 82, 122, 212
experience 35; brain development 37–8; Bingham, M. 110, 111, 316
breastfeeding 38; co-regulation 38, 55, 297; bio-psycho-social knowledge 13–17; biological
cognitive development 69, 70; development of the variation 23
mind 38–40; Erikson’s psychosocial model 43, 44; biology: the ageing process 235–8; and attachment
exploratory behaviour 36, 41, 53; infant death theory 55; and factors for mental health problems
360
Index
204; puberty 102–4; relevance to cognitive relationships 84; impact on the development of
development 68; of sex 165–6 the mind 39–40; loneliness of 113, 145; neglect
Bion, W. 45, 49 39, 40, 84; physical 84, 178, 200; prevention and
Borderline Welfare: Feeling and Fear of Feeling in protection 46–7, 83; rates of 112; reduced
Modern Welfare 48 understanding of moral issues 91; resilience to
Bowlby, J. 52, 54, 57, 59, 297, 298, 299 55–6, 86, 96; risk factor for adult mental health
Boyd, C. 86 problems 200, 204; risk factor for homelessness
Boyd, D. 9, 37, 73, 77, 90, 91, 102, 103, 104, 106, 120, 141; and risk of becoming an abusive parent
178, 181, 187, 188, 189, 213, 236, 238, 240, 245, 85–6; school experience for abused children 84–5;
249, 273, 285 sexual abuse 40, 84, 85, 113; social work with a
brain development 37–8, 55; in adolescence 102–3 victim and perpetrator of 143–4; social work with
breastfeeding 38 abused children 46–7, 84; varying cultural and
British Association for Adoption and Fostering 112 societal definitions 83
The British Cohort Study 154 Child Migrant Trust 139
British Red Cross 130 child poverty 60, 195, 196, 198–9
Brody, E.M. 185, 242, 244, 250 Child Poverty Action Group (CPAG) 195, 196, 197
Bronfenbrenner, U. 67, 72, 78, 149, 151, 152, 154, child protection in the UK and Europe 83–4
247, 299, 301, 303, 304 childlessness 173, 181, 188
Bronfenbrenner, U: ecological model16, 72, 78, Children Act 1989 83, 111, 220
145–54, 301–4; application to asylum seekers Children Act 2004 82
151–2; application to young people leaving care children in care 10, 12, 23, 40, 63, 71, 84–6, 94–5,
150–2; applying the 146–7, 150–4, 303–4; 109, 115, 125, 137, 139, 284, 288–99; adolescents
chronosystems 147, 153, 303; ecological systems 111–13; asylum seekers 136, 139; attachment
148; exosystems 147, 150, 152, 302; limitations relationships 57–8, 60, 107–8, 112, 284; corporate
154, 304; macrosystems 147, 150, 152–3, 302; parents 134, 135; East European orphanages 58,
mesosystems 147, 149–50, 152, 302; 60; education 82, 112, 134; ethnic origin and
microsystems 146, 147, 149, 150, 151–2, 180, 195, experiences of leaving care 136 ; forming
301–2; relation to attachment theory 299; identities 112, 215; increased risk of abuse 112;
strengths 303; in UK public policy 154 lack of state ‘parental’ care after 18 105, 133, 134;
Burman, E. 58, 300 leaving to live independently 116, 133, 134–6,
Bynner, J. 133, 150 137, 142–3, 150–2, 155; ‘maternal deprivation’ 52,
60; resilience 55–6, 113; risks of negative
Captain Corelli’s Mandolin 161 outcomes 112, 113, 139, 141; supporting 84, 113
Care Leavers Act (2003) 134 Chodorow, N. 180
Care Matters 134 Christie, L. 26
Care Programme Approach (CPA) 208 chromosomes 20, 23, 24, 292, 294
carers 244, 246, 249 chronosystems 147, 153, 303
Casement, P. 145 Citizens as Trainers Group 221
Cass, V.C. 214 classical conditioning 73, 74, 77, 95, 323, 324
Cassidy, J. 58, 59 co-regulation 38, 43, 55, 297
Ceci, S. 26, 27 cognitive behavioural theory/ therapy (CBT) 15, 75–6,
Centre for Longitudinal Studies 154 208, 324; and understanding ‘prosocial’ and
Centrepoint 140 ‘antisocial’ behaviour 88, 91, 96; see also
cerebral palsy 115 behavioural learning
child abuse and protection: application of the cognitive development 67–73, 153; applying to social
ecological model 303; emotional abuse 84; future work 71; in babies 69, 70; in childhood 68–71,
effects of 82–6, 96; impact on attachment 153; concrete operational stage 70, 312; formal
361
Index
operational stage 70, 312; the growth of Department of Health 78, 154, 208, 223, 224, 257,
understanding 310; information processing 303
research 313; later developments and alternative Depression 27, 75, 117, 169, 176, 205, 208–9, 214,
views to Piaget’s 70, 72, 312–13; Piaget’s theory 226, 284; and adolescents in care 112; and carers
of 68–73, 310–12; pre-operational stage 69, 312; 242,246; and cognitive behavioural therpay 75,
schema 310; sensori-motor stage 69–70, 310; 234; and gender 176; multiple gene–environment
strengths and weaknesses 72–3; Vygotsky’s model pathways to 24; and older people 210; postnatal
72, 95, 313 depression 142, 180, 192; and poverty 196; and
cognitive therapy 75, 76 separation/loss 205, 268, 278, 299, 329;
Commission for Social Care Inspection 244, 245 consequent upon violence 202, 226
communal violence 203–4 Diamond, L. 167, 213
concrete operational stage 70, 312 disability: in adolescence 113–15; and links with
conditional positive regard 319 poverty 197; measures of self-esteem 115
congruence 263, 321 Disability Discrimination Act 2005 221
conscience and guilt 92–5 discrete categories 21, 23–4
continuity theory 241, 327 divorce 81, 162, 169, 170
continuous variables 21, 23–4 Dixon, J. 135, 150
Cooper, A. 48, 94 Down’s syndrome 24, 114, 126, 218; different
corporate parents 134, 135 societies’ attitudes and support for 219, 220,
Corr, P.J. 26, 38, 204, 205, 218 222–3, 225; life expectancy 220; resources 227
cosmopolitanism 136–7 Down’s Syndrome Association 219
counter-transference 164, 308–9, 321 drugs 206
Cumming, E. 239, 326 dual process model 269, 275, 330
cystic fibrosis 292
The Ecology of Human Development 154
de Bernieres, L. 161 Economic and Social Research Council 243
death: 253–77; biological 273; of a child 139–40, education 77–82; of children in care 82, 112, 134; for
183–4, 255–6, 272, 284; children’s concepts of children with learning difficulties 220; correlation
270; cultural factors in 272–4; a ‘good’ 257–9; with good attachments 106; and delaying the
humanistic model and understanding of 261–3, onset of dementia 237–8; ethnic group
275; Kubler-Ross’ theory on approaching 266–9, educational achievements 80–1; girls and boys
275, 329; and the meaning of life 274–5; place achievements 79–80, 121; parental influences on
of 257; psychodynamic approaches to 264–5, educational achievements 81; poverty and links
275; questions about 258–60; religious meanings with educational achievements 134, 198; and
273; social 273; a social worker dealing with loss relative age effect 26, 78; role of school in social
and 261–3; social workers’ involvement with and emotional development 78–9, 96; social class
255–6, 258–60, 284; young people’s experiences and educational achievements 81, 133–4;;
of 270–1 university 80, 82, 112, 132; Vygotsky on 95
death rates 256–7; in adolescents 103; in children ego 92, 306, 307
103, 256–7; in infants 198–9, 256 Eleanor Rigby 276
debt 140 Eliot, T.S. 45
defence mechanisms 48–9, 307–8, 319 Emerson, E. 114, 218, 220, 221, 222
dementia 204, 210, 237–8 emotional abuse see child abuse
Department for Children Schools and Families 80, 81, emotional labour 187
82, 134, 135, 136 empathy 145, 263, 321
Department for Education and Skills (DfES) 82, 134, environment: and genes 22–8, 294–5;
135 gene–environment correlations 24–6;
362
Index
363
Index
364
Index
internal working model 53, 56–7, 84, 297, 298 changing patterns of 132, 133; and ‘empty nest
syndrome’ 140, 181; gender differences and
Jahoda, M. 185 family contact after 132–3; globalisation and
prisoner of war survivors 139, 145 cosmopolitanism 136–7; and links with poverty
Jessop, J. 270, 274 198; and loneliness 132, 137, 142, 143, 241;
Joseph Rowntree Foundation 81, 82, 105, 133 young people leaving care 133, 134–6, 137, 142,
Joyce, A. 35, 36 143, 150–2
lesbians: see also sexual identity, gender expression,
Kagan, C. 102, 121 sex, etc; in adolescence 109–10; desire for
Kermode, F. 238 children 165, 171; families 171, 181, 214; forming
Kindertransport 139 an identity 214, 226; intimate relationships
Klaus, M.H. 36, 54, 303 166–8; religious attitudes to 171–2; violence in
Klaus, P.H. 36 intimate relationships 178, 201
Klein, M. 45, 48–9, 51, 52, 94, 95, 305, 307, 308 Levinson, D. 189
Klusmann, D. 168 Levy, B. 234, 235
knowledge: bio-psycho-social knowledge 13–17; libido 50, 108, 307
objective and subjective knowledge 281–3, 285; life expectancy 233; at birth in 2006 257; changing
self-knowledge 17–18, 46, 94, 144, 161,164, 216, 242, 286; for people with learning difficulties
233, 247, 259, 264, 275, 283–5, 286–7 219–20
Kohlberg, L. 89–90, 91, 96 life structure 189
Krammzorg 183 The Lifecycle Completed 316
Kubler-Ross, E. 266–9, 275, 329–30 lifelines 17, 286–7
lifespan model 189, 239, 285–6
Lamb, M. 181 Lindsay, M. 134, 137, 142, 143, 150
learning and behaviour 73–7, 323–5; observational living independently: see leaving home; loneliness
learning 92; perspective on moral behaviour 91–2; loneliness: and challenges for social workers 143–5;
see also behavioural learning theory, cognitive in children moved from Britain without their
behvioural theory/therapy parents 139; after death or other loss of a child
learning disabilities, people with: adopting a 12, 137, 139–40, 183–4, 256, 269 ; correlation
capability approach 224; different societies’ with disrupted attachments 134; and
attitudes and support for 219, 220, 222–3, 225; homelessness 140; and inadequacy of the
“Donald” (an example) 143–4; education 220; mesosystem 152; for parents when children leave
family support and involvement 220–1; finances home 140, 180; practical responses 142; in prison
222; having an independent voice 221; healthcare 137–8; for refugees, exiles and torture victims
219–20, 220, 221, 223, 224; identity formation 138–9; relationship responses 142–4; social
216–17, 226; life expectancy 219–20; links to worker responses to 141–3, 154–5; two forms of
poverty 219, 222; and mental health 206–7; in 170; in young people leaving care 137, 142, 143;
minority ethnic communities 223; problems over in young people leaving home 132, 241
pain relief 223; sexual information and activity The Long Life 238
110, 114, 143–4, 164; social attitudes towards Lousada, J. 48, 94
221–4; social influences on the development of
218–25; in South Asian communities 219, 223; macrosystems 147, 150, 152–3, 302
statistics 218; Swedish policy 219, 220, 222–3; Main, M. 298
terminology 218 marriage 157–92
On Learning from Our Mistakes 145 Maslow, A. 118, 120, 122, 212, 317–18, 321
leaving home 132–7; applying the ecological model ‘maternal deprivation’ 52, 60
to 149–53; and becoming homeless 140–1; the mature imagination 248–9, 326, 327
365
Index
366
Index
367
Index
368
Index
369
Index
activity in older 236; sexual desire and work 185–9, 190; autonomy at 186; earning power
intimate relationships 165, 166–8; violence 187, 188; emotions in 187–8; as an expressive or
towards 177–9, 200–2; see also gender, lesbians, instrumental activity 186–7; functions of 185–6;
mothers ten-factor model of 186; women in 187–8
Women and Work Commission 188 World Health Organisation 59, 218
Woodmansey, C. 47, 92, 93, 307
Worden, W. 269, 270, 274, 275, 330 Zeifman, D. 162, 169, 173, 265
370