Download
Download
ABSTRACT KEYWORDS
Attachment theory may be considered controversial given that some Adoption; attachment;
of its foundational principles are contested. Not only this, it is cur- children; education;
rently being developed by insights from neuroscience, another per- fostering; social work
spective that academics have subjected to critique. Nevertheless, at
the beginning of the twenty-first century in England and the United
Kingdom in general, there has been a renewed interest in its expla-
nation of child development, as well as its application in schools,
social care settings and the practice of professionals such as social
workers and teachers. This paper outlines the core principles of
attachment theory, acknowledges some of the criticisms, then traces
the ways in which the theory has been developed over time. The
theory is then illustrated with a description of the ways in which it is
being applied in the training of foster carers, the provision of support
to adoptive parents and in the school environment.
Introduction
In England in 2015 the National Institute of Health and Care Excellence provided guidance on
the identification, assessment and treatment of attachment difficulties of children in the care
system or at risk of entering the care system (National Institute of Health and Care Excellence
[NICE], 2015). In the same year, the Department for Education provided advice on attach-
ment disorder and the mental health of children in the school (Department for Education
[DfE], 2015). Whilst the principles of human attachment are informing policies and practice,
the theory itself has been subjected to critique. This paper outlines the theoretical principles
first proposed by Bowlby, responds to some of the critiques, but also draws attention to the
various ways in which the original theory has been built upon. Examples of the ways in which
the theory is being taken up in social work, social care and education are then provided.
Attachment is the strong, affectionate tie we have with special people in our lives that leads
us to experience pleasure and joy when we interact with them and to be comforted by their
nearness in times of stress. By the second half of the first year, infants have become attached
to familiar people who have responded to their needs (Berk, 2013, p. 428).
The origins of attachment theory are attributed to John Bowlby, who was a psychoanalyst,
and a member of the British group of object relations theorists. These theorists were
developing an explanation of the way in which the relationship between the primary
carer and the child created the structure and processes of the child’s mind. Bowlby,
however, shifted away from a focusing on psychoanalytic theory only, to linking it with
knowledge gained from studying animals, and the practicality of care received by the
human infant.
According to Bowlby, the infant’s relationship with the parent begins as a set of innate
signals that call the adult to the baby’s side. Over time, a true affectionate bond forms,
supported by new emotional and cognitive capacities as well as a history of warm, sensitive
care (Berk, 2013, p. 429).
From his research, Bowlby concluded that the primary caregiver has a particular role in
providing the infant and child with a sense of safety: a secure base from which to thrive
and flourish. According to the theory that was developed, there are four stages in the
development of attachment:
have given rise to critiques, departures and altered emphases. An extensive body of
literature has been created reflecting competing views and perspectives. Before illustrating
the application of attachment theory in practice, it is to this recent theoretical literature that
attention is turned.
Summarising the literature, Fonagy and Allison (2014) argue that the sensitivity of the
carer will allow the child to experience being recognised as an ‘intentional agent’ as well as
82 E. HARLOW
the point that there are about ’ . . . 10 billion neurons in the cortex of the brain alone, which
are interconnected in manifold ways. In addition, there are countless chemical interactions
involving neurotransmitters, hormones and neuropeptides . . . ’ (Glassman & Hadad, 2013,
p. 88). Critics such as Rose and Rose (2016) argue that fMRI scans do not give rise to
causal scientific truths (as some might claim), but interpretations that suggest correla-
tions. Unfortunately, the way in which some scientists have reduced behaviour to
nothing more than the biology of the brain, together with the oversimplification and
popularisation of neuroscience in the media, some authors (such as Gabriel, 2017) and
professionals (such as psychoanalysts) have shunned it as a serious contributor to our
understanding of human development (Fotopoulou, 2012).
In constructing a theory of human development that involves attachment between the
infant and primary carer as key, all of the three theorists discussed above (Bowlby,
Fonagy and Porges), have emphasised the biological component of human attachment
to a greater or lesser extent: put another way, all three have offered a biopsychosocial
account. However, it might be argued that for Bowlby and Fonagy the emphasis is on the
psychosocial construction of, and interpretative function of the ‘mind’, whilst for Porges
the biological materiality of the body and neural process is ascendant. Underpinning
these theoretical contributions lie the philosophical debates concerning the mind-body
dualism. Debates that have been taken up by Fotopoulou (2012).
Fotopoulou (2012) has articulated the projects of neuropsychoanalysis and neurophiloso-
phy which aim to overcome the mind-body dualism: for the mind-body dualism to be
understood, the ‘mind’ has to be seen to be as real as the brain and that both subjective
experiences as well as brain states have to be appreciated. Theoretical innovations which
attempt to overcome the mind-body dualism are applauded by academics such as Mackenzie
and Roberts (2017). These debates suggest that theories relating to human attachment are the
result, not only of technological innovation (such as fMRI), but philosophical considerations
also. In sum, taking into account the expansion of theoretical insights (not all of which could
be covered here), Schore and Schore (2008) suggest that there has emerged a new emphasis
on the affect regulation or the regulation of the emotions:
Irrespective of the preferred explanation or point of emphasis, human attachments and the
associated implications, are seen as crucial to the practice of professionals tasked with
ensuring and enhancing the developmental wellbeing of children. It is to some of the
practical applications of attachment and corresponding theories that attention is now turned.
Attachment theory and its application in social work, social care and
education
Although there has, to some extent, been a departure from a simplistic and rigid categor-
isation of attachment types, an extreme version of an insecure attachment has now been
84 E. HARLOW
recognised as a disorder by the National Institute of Health and Care Excellence (see
NICE, 2015). However, NICE has made clear that the term attachment disorder should
only be used when a young person has been formally diagnosed by qualified professionals
in accordance with the established criteria. Furthermore, social workers have been advised
to use the term ‘attachment’ with caution and that in most instances, the more general
term ‘relationship’ might suffice (Shemmings, 2018). Nevertheless, since Bowlby’s initial
contribution, the principles of human attachment have been important components of
social work. Along with associated psychodynamic theory, it is relevant to relationship-
based practice with all service users (see Ruch, 2018). Here, however, attention will be
given to work with children, young people and their families. As indicated above, children
and young people who have experienced phenomena such as loss and grief, abuse and
neglect, are less likely to have benefitted from sustained secure attachments: although not
inevitably so, they are more likely to be insecure and anxious. For fostered and adopted
children and young people, continuity, an understanding environment, and one or more
relationships that provide a secure base become essential if previous difficulties are to be
surmounted. These principles are important in their home, but also in their school. What
follows is an attempt to illustrate the way in which these principles are being promoted
through the training and/or support of foster carers, adopters and school staff members.
By means of longitudinal research on children (in middle childhood or early adoles-
cence) growing up in foster care, Schofield and Beek (2005) have theorised the day-to-day
construction of an emotionally secure base. The researchers identified five intercon-
nected and mutually reinforcing dimensions of the care provided by the foster carers.
These dimensions, together with examples of parenting and their children’s behaviours,
are indicated in brief.
Promoting self-esteem
Positive parenting conveys to children their unconditional acceptance. This occurs by means
of loving gestures, kind words and praise. Children learn to feel valued for who they are.
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE 85
Hence, failures can be tolerated and help sought when required. The parents in Schofield and
Beek’s study spoke with pride about the children’s achievements and the characteristics with
warmth. A growing sense of self-esteem was demonstrated by the children as they took part
in classroom activities, became more outgoing and acknowledged their strengths.
Promoting autonomy/self-efficacy
Infants express their needs and sensitive parents react appropriately, hence there develops
the idea that a behaviour leads to a positive response, but also that there is the capacity for
control over the environment. Parents who are available, allow the child to explore
confident in the knowledge that they will still be available on return. However, as the
child matures, there is recognition of the parent’s needs, so develops co-operation and
negotiation. Foster children may have had to be too independent in the past and been too
powerful. Alternatively, if birth parents had been too controlling, they may not have had the
opportunity to exercise independence. Foster carers’ in the Schofield and Beek study
encouraged children to be independent in ways that related to the child’s age and ability,
for example, encouraging a child to choose which clothes to wear or facilitating in the
achievement of homework rather than undertaking the homework on the child’s behalf. The
children demonstrated independence by taking up hobbies, either solitary hobbies or those
that were shared with others. Most of the behaviours involved negotiation and compromise.
Perhaps re-mapping those mundane or shocking events – he spat in my face; she deliberately
smashed the bowl – through ethological or biochemical processes might bring parents to
engage differently to the child or between the child and themselves at that moment, and
thereby help both the child and themselves to remain anchored in the present (which is less
frightening or dangerous in objective terms) (Mackenzie & Roberts, 2017, p. 137).
Recognising the development away from the reliance on Bowlby’s original formulation only
and the recent emphasis on ‘brain-based parenting’ (which is more aligned with the work of
Porges, as summarised above), Mackenzie and Roberts noted the ad hoc approach that was
taken to theory: that is, with the intention of offering a positive intervention professionals did
not refer to original research, clinical papers or promote theoretical coherence, but articulated
explanations of attachment, child development and behaviour on a pragmatic basis. Such an
approach is replicated in the popular ‘how-to’ books that were recommended as forms of
assistance for the adoptive parents (Mackenzie & Roberts, 2017).
A Centre for Adoption Support (CfAS) was established in the northwest of England in
2013. An evaluation of its construction and performance showed that attachment theory
influenced the form and content of the services delivered (see Author, 2018; Harlow,
Mitchell, Doherty, & Moran, 2015). In particular, the work of Hughes (1999, 2004) had
been embraced. Hughes, a psychotherapist, has made a particular contribution to knowledge
and practice by applying the principles of attachment theory to the needs of adoptive families.
He aims to address the emotional pain that an adopted child may have experienced. Such
experience inhibits the building of the child’s affect regulation with the consequence of
behavioural and relational problems. According to Rushton and Dance (2002), adoptive
parents reported that children’s ‘weak return of affection or rejection, persistent non-
compliance, violence and aggression’ was particularly difficult to deal with. Hughes emphasises
verbal and non-verbal attunement in the building of the new relationship between the child
and his/her adoptive parents. The CfAS social workers had attended Hughes’ training on
Dyadic Developmental Psychotherapy (see Hughes, Golding, & Hudson, 2015). This training
appeared to inform and enhance the relationship-based social work that was being under-
taken. When talking about the service she had received from CfAS, an adoptive mother (with
the pseudonym of Mrs Black) said that she would have welcomed such support from the start
of the time her children had been placed with her:
Mrs Black: Well, it doesn’t have to be a therapist. [It] could be a therapeutic social worker.
Somebody like [names social worker from CfAS] who understands attachment, who is
emotionally in tune, that can be a listening ear and say, ‘Have you tried strategies?’. Some
practical advice as well as that sort of listening ear, so you don’t feel so isolated (T1:6)
(Harlow et al., 2015, p. 31).
From the CfAS research, it appeared that many adoptive parents needed support in
managing daily life at home, but they felt this was impacted by their child’s experience in
school. They felt that improvements could be made if the emotional needs of their
children were appreciated by school staff. This sentiment was echoed in research con-
ducted by Adoption UK:
Many respondents felt that staff and education professionals should be trained in issues of
trauma and attachment, and that there needs to be a better understanding of the needs of
adopted children (Pennington, n.d., p. 35)
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE 87
Social workers at the CfAS were responding to the concerns of adopted parents by
working directly with school staff members to create an environment that reduced the
anxieties of individual children: i.e. tailored packages of support were introduced. In
addition, however, training on attachment theory was made available for the whole
school (see also Adoption UK, n.d.).
Attachment disorder is now acknowledged in the Department for Education’s advice on
mental health and behaviour in school (DfE, 2015). However, interest in human attach-
ments and their significance in the school environment is broader that the topic of health
(see for example, Clements, 2013; Geddes, 2006; Marshall, 2014; Phillips, 2007; Ubha &
Cahill, 2014; Webber, 2017). As with social work, as indicated above, the practice literature
relating to education is often eclectic in approach. For example, Rose and Gilbert (2017)
refer to the work of Bowlby as well as Porges. Irrespective of this eclecticism, however,
contributors in general argue that the implementation of attachment principles in school
can improve the general wellbeing of students and in consequence their performance (see
Bergin & Bergin, 2009). Also, it is argued that by helping school staff understand and
respond to disruptive behaviour as evidence of emotional insecurity, the rate of exclusions
can be reduced. There are three main approaches to the implementation of attachment
principles in school. Firstly, children who are struggling to regulate their emotions may be
offered emotion coaching (see Bombѐr & Hughes, 2013; Gus & Wood, 2017). Secondly,
efforts are made to develop the awareness of all school staff members in order that they
might reflect on the meanings of behaviour (for example, in the light of insights concerning
fear and anxiety) with the ambition that their relationships with all students are enhanced.
This relates closely to the third approach which emphasises the functioning of the whole
school (see Bergin & Bergin, 2009). Importantly, it is the responsibility of school leaders to
encourage a warm socioemotional climate and a culture of respect. In order to stimulate
a sense of security, continuity of people and place is beneficial, and advantage may be
gained when essential transitions (across years and schools) are facilitated. Small schools
that are embedded within their communities are more likely to encourage student bonding,
and inclusive extra-curricular activities are helpful with this. In short, the school itself
should become a secure base.
Conclusion
Decades have elapsed since Bowlby developed his theory of attachment. Its popularity
has fluctuated and over recent times it has been reinvigorated. However, from the
discussion above, it can be seen that it has been developed and integrated with a range
of new theoretical and philosophical insights. It might be argued that there is no longer
one ‘attachment theory’, but that over time there have evolved a number of theoretical
explanations of human attachment. Whilst each of these theoretical explanations may
take into account biology, psychology, and social relationships – making the explanation
biopsychosocial – each explanation may emphasise different elements. However, when
psychologists, social workers and associated professionals inform their work with the-
ories of attachment (which may or may not refer to neuroscience), like cultural and social
theorists, they may do so in an eclectic manner (see Mackenzie and Roberts (2017).
Whilst there is a risk that an emphasis on the biology of ‘brain science’ becomes collapsed
with ‘brain damage’, respondents in the CfAS study as well as Mackenzie and Roberts
88 E. HARLOW
found that as adoptive parents, their experience of training and support provided by
professionals to be helpful. Being provided with an explanation of their child’s challen-
ging behaviour, reassurance that change could occur, together with suggestions for
making this happen, brought welcome relief. Nevertheless, theories of attachment do
not constitute a panacea and Scott (2018) warned against their over-use as an explanatory
tool: that is, it would be possible to misdiagnose the needs of a child if there was an
automatic reliance on the principles of attachment. Similarly, some therapies associated
with attachment and/or ‘brain science’ may not be evidentially legitimate: it is not
possible to ‘fix’ specific components of the brain and it is better to pursue conventional
therapies that have been thoroughly evaluated and tested over time.
Finally, it may be argued that focusing on attachment highlights the child and parent/carer
relationship with the consequence that essential considerations concerning the wider com-
munity are cast into shadow. However, Schofield and Beek (2018) embed their emphasis on
relationships within Bronfenbrenner’s (1979) ecological model of growth and human devel-
opment. Furthermore, the social workers who are working with the staff members of schools
are acting within the terms of this model. Importantly, the emphasis on whole school change
shifts attention away from specific parents, carers or children, and encourages systemic
organisational change that aims to benefit all concerned.
Acknowledgments
Gratitude is extended to all those who supported the writing of this paper. These include: Alan
Wood; Kate Fillingham; Karen Izod; Hilary Rose, Steven Rose, Fiona Sheikh; and the Alex
Timpson Trust.
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author.
Funding
This work was supported by the Alex Timpson Trust.
Notes on contributor
Elizabeth Harlow PhD, BA, CQSW, FHEA is a Professor of Social Work at the University of
Chester. She worked as a practitioner before beginning her academic career in 1988. Focussing
primarily on the organisation, management and delivery of services to children and their families,
she has led research projects and evaluations and published her work over the last three decades.
[email: [email protected]]
ORCID
Elizabeth Harlow http://orcid.org/0000-0001-5031-7485
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE 89
References
Adoption UK. (n.d.). Bridging the gap: Giving adopted children and equal chance in school.
Banbury: Author. Retrieved from www.adoptionuk.org/Handlers/Download.ashx?IDMF=
e460b99a-4ebb-4348-bd23-64a50d747901
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological
study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Author. (2018). Defining the problem and sourcing the solution: A reflection on some of the
organisational, professional and emotional complexities of accessing post-adoption support.
Journal of Social Work Practice, 33(3), 269–280.
Bergin, C., & Bergin, D. (2009). Attachment in the classroom. Educational Psychology Review, 21,
141–170.
Berk, L. E. (2013). Child development (9th ed.). London: Pearson.
Bombѐr, L. M., & Hughes, D. A. (2013). Settling to learn. Settling troubled pupils to learn: Why
relationships matter in school. London: Worth Publishing.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1: Attachment. London: Howgarth Press.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2: Separation: Anger and anxiety. London: Howgarth
Press.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3: Loss: Sadness and depression. London: Howgarth
Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design.
Cambridge MA: Harvard University press.
Clements, J. (2013). A good practice guide for schools. Understanding and meeting the needs of
children who are looked after, fostered or adopted or otherwise permanently placed. London: Pac.
Crittenden, P. M. (1985). Social networks, quality of parenting, and child development. Child
Development, 56, 1299–1313.
Crittenden, P. M. (2000a). A dynamic-maturational model of the function, development and
organization of human relationships. In R. S. L. Mills, & S. Duck (Eds.), Developmental
psychology of personal relationships (pp. 199–218). Chichester: Wiley.
Crittenden, P. M. (2000b). A dynamic-maturational exploration of the meaning of security and
adaptation: Empirical, cultural and theoretical considerations. In P. M. Crittenden &
A. A. Claussen (Eds.), The organization of attachment relationships: Maturation, culture and
context (pp. 343–357). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. (1996). Emotional security as a regulatory process in normal
development and the development of psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8,
123–139.
DfE (Department for Education). (2015). Mental health and behaviour in schools. Department
advice for school staff. Retrieved from www.gov.uk/government/publications.
Duschinsky, R., Greco, M., & Soloman, J. (2015). The politics of attachment: Lines of flight with
Bowlby, Deleuze and Guattari. Theory Culture and Society, 32(7–8), 173–195.
Fonagy, P. (1998). An attachment theory approach to treatment of the difficult patient. Bulletin of
the Menninger Clinic, 62, 147–169.
Fonagy, P., & Allison, E. (2014). The role of mentalizing and epistemic trust in the therapeutic
relationship. Psychotherapy, 51(3), 372–380.
Fonagy, P., & Campbell, C. (2015). Attachment and mentalizing. In A. Elliot & J. Prager (Eds.), The
routledge handbook of psychoanalysis in the social sciences and humanities (pp. 343–
357). London: Routledge.
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., & Target, M. (2004). Affect regulation, mentalization, and the
development of the self. London: Karnac.
Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (1997). Attachment and reflective function: Their role in
self-organisation. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 679–700.
Fotopoulou, A. K. (2012). The history of progress of neuropsychoanalysis. In A. Fotopoulou,
D. Pfaff, & M. A. Conway (Eds.), From the couch to the lab: Trends in psychodynamic
neuroscience (pp. 12–24). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
90 E. HARLOW
Schofield, G., & Beek, M. (2018). Attachment handbook for foster care and adoption (2nd ed.).
London: CoramBAAF.
Schore, J. R., & Schore, A. N. (2008). Modern attachment theory: The central role of affect
regulation in development and treatment. Clinical Social Work Journal, 36(1), 9–20.
Scott, S. (2018, June 6). Presentation made at the conference ‘Realising our vision for adoption in the
UK: Working in partnership to deliver excellent services nationwide’, a public policy exchange
Symposium held at the Strand Palace Hotel, London, UK.
Shemmings, D. (2018, June 28). Why social workers shouldn’t use ‘attachment’ in their records
and reports. Community Care. Retrieved from www.communitycare.co.uk/2018/06/28/social-
workers-shouldnt-use-attachment-records-reports/
Smith, M., Cameron, C., & Reimer, D. (2017). From attachment to recognition for children in care.
British Journal of Social Work, 47(6), 1606–1623.
Ubha, N., & Cahill, S. (2014). Building secure attachments for primary school children: A mixed
methods study. Educational Psychology in Practice, 30(3), 272–292.
Waters, E., & Cummings, E. M. (2000). A secure base from which to explore close relationships.
Child Development, 21(1), 164–172.
Webber, L. (2017). A school’s journey in creating a relational environment which supports
attachment and emotional security. Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties, 22(4), 317–331.
Winnicott, D. (1965). The maturational process and the facilitative environment. New York:
International Universities Press.
Copyright of Journal of Social Work Practice is the property of Routledge and its content may
not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.