0% found this document useful (0 votes)
295 views14 pages

Download

Uploaded by

Bárbara Araújo
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
295 views14 pages

Download

Uploaded by

Bárbara Araújo
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 14

JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE

2021, VOL. 35, NO. 1, 79–91


https://doi.org/10.1080/02650533.2019.1700493

Attachment theory: developments, debates and recent


applications in social work, social care and education
Elizabeth Harlow
University of Chester, UK

ABSTRACT KEYWORDS
Attachment theory may be considered controversial given that some Adoption; attachment;
of its foundational principles are contested. Not only this, it is cur- children; education;
rently being developed by insights from neuroscience, another per- fostering; social work
spective that academics have subjected to critique. Nevertheless, at
the beginning of the twenty-first century in England and the United
Kingdom in general, there has been a renewed interest in its expla-
nation of child development, as well as its application in schools,
social care settings and the practice of professionals such as social
workers and teachers. This paper outlines the core principles of
attachment theory, acknowledges some of the criticisms, then traces
the ways in which the theory has been developed over time. The
theory is then illustrated with a description of the ways in which it is
being applied in the training of foster carers, the provision of support
to adoptive parents and in the school environment.

Introduction
In England in 2015 the National Institute of Health and Care Excellence provided guidance on
the identification, assessment and treatment of attachment difficulties of children in the care
system or at risk of entering the care system (National Institute of Health and Care Excellence
[NICE], 2015). In the same year, the Department for Education provided advice on attach-
ment disorder and the mental health of children in the school (Department for Education
[DfE], 2015). Whilst the principles of human attachment are informing policies and practice,
the theory itself has been subjected to critique. This paper outlines the theoretical principles
first proposed by Bowlby, responds to some of the critiques, but also draws attention to the
various ways in which the original theory has been built upon. Examples of the ways in which
the theory is being taken up in social work, social care and education are then provided.

Attachment theory: definition and foundational principles

Attachment is the strong, affectionate tie we have with special people in our lives that leads
us to experience pleasure and joy when we interact with them and to be comforted by their
nearness in times of stress. By the second half of the first year, infants have become attached
to familiar people who have responded to their needs (Berk, 2013, p. 428).

CONTACT Elizabeth Harlow [email protected]


© 2019 GAPS
80 E. HARLOW

The origins of attachment theory are attributed to John Bowlby, who was a psychoanalyst,
and a member of the British group of object relations theorists. These theorists were
developing an explanation of the way in which the relationship between the primary
carer and the child created the structure and processes of the child’s mind. Bowlby,
however, shifted away from a focusing on psychoanalytic theory only, to linking it with
knowledge gained from studying animals, and the practicality of care received by the
human infant.

According to Bowlby, the infant’s relationship with the parent begins as a set of innate
signals that call the adult to the baby’s side. Over time, a true affectionate bond forms,
supported by new emotional and cognitive capacities as well as a history of warm, sensitive
care (Berk, 2013, p. 429).

From his research, Bowlby concluded that the primary caregiver has a particular role in
providing the infant and child with a sense of safety: a secure base from which to thrive
and flourish. According to the theory that was developed, there are four stages in the
development of attachment:

● Preattachment phase (birth to 6 weeks)


● ‘Attachment-in-the-making’ phase (6 weeks to 6 to 8 months)
● ‘Clear cut’ attachment phase (6 to 8 months to 18 months to 2 years)
● Formation of a reciprocal relationship (18 months to 2 years and on).

(Berk, 2013, p. 429).


Bowlby explored, not only the building of this secure attachment, but also the loss and
grief that is experienced by the infant when s/he is separated from the mother (see
Bowlby, 1969, 1973, 1980). Indeed, it was the implications of the mother-infant separa-
tion that initially mobilised Bowlby’s interest. Bowlby went on to argue that the quality of
attachment between the primary carer and infant created an ‘internal working model’ i.e.
the mental model from which the child (and later adult) engages with the world. The
suggestion being that, the quality of the earliest relationship(s) will inform the way in
which the child (and later the adult) not only relates to others, but also explores and
engages with life’s opportunities. Howe (2011) elaborates on this when he traces attach-
ment across the life course.
Whilst a secure attachment between the primary carer and child will provide a positive
foundation for life, secure attachments are not possible for all. Mary Ainsworth developed
a technique for analysing the quality of an attachment and concluded with a categorisation
of three types: secure; insecure avoidant; and insecure resistant or ambivalent (Ainsworth,
Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978). This work was advanced by Main and Solomon (1986), who
added a fourth category – insecure disorganised attachment, and Crittenden who refined
the attachment categories still further (see Crittenden, 1985). According to Howe (2011),
Crittenden developed a lifespan developmental model of attachment (the Dynamic
Maturational Model) which suggests that fear (for one’s safety) does not give rise to
disorganisation, but rather adaptation: that is, the child might oscillate between avoidant
and ambivalent insecure patterns in an attempt to seek the greatest degree of security
(Crittenden, 2000a, 2000b cited in Howe, 2011, p. 159). Whilst researchers have refined the
foundations of attachment theory, recent empirical findings and new theoretical insights
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE 81

have given rise to critiques, departures and altered emphases. An extensive body of
literature has been created reflecting competing views and perspectives. Before illustrating
the application of attachment theory in practice, it is to this recent theoretical literature that
attention is turned.

Attachment theory: developments and debates


Critics of attachment theory have challenged the way in which the theory appears to ‘fix’
human development: firstly, in terms of the early years of life and secondly, in terms of the
categories of attachment. Firstly, Smith, Cameron, and Reimer (2017), by drawing on the
empirical work of Rutter et al. (2007) contest the argument that a child’s development is
fixed by two years of age. Citing Rutter et al. they argue that the effect of insecure
attachments early life can be reversed by positive later life experiences. However,
Schofield and Beek (2018) insist that Bowlby did not stipulate irreversibility: instead, the
‘internal working model’ is indeed a ‘working model’ that is available for revision. The
construction of an internal working model is linked to the primary carer as secure base.
Whilst essential for survival in emergency situations in early childhood, the secure base
allows for human development beyond this: it enables children to explore, engage in free
play, and in consequence further their development (see Schofield & Beek, 2005). Waters
and Cummings (2000) reiterate that after infancy, social learning and cognitive develop-
ment have influence also, which in turn impacts upon secure base expectations. All of this
means that, not only is the internal working model available to change, but the potential for
change lies in relationships beyond the initial dyad: multiple family relationships and events
can be significant (Cummings & Davies, 1996 cited in Waters & Cummings, 2000).
Secondly, Smith et al. (2017) have criticised the fixity and pathologizing implications
of Ainsworth’s categorisation of attachment. However, whilst Ainsworth’s original work
still has currency, it has been built upon and refined, not only by Main and Solomon
(1986) and Crittenden (1985, 2000a, and 2000b) (as identified above), but also
Duschinsky, Greco, and Soloman (2015) who have shunned the imposition of rigid
classification, and suggested that it may be more appropriate to think of attachment
theory as ‘a psychology of the interplay of dynamic forces.’ Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist, and
Target (2004) have reinterpreted attachment as relatively secure and relatively insecure.
Furthermore, they argue that attachment has been re-envisioned ’ . . . from an emphasis
on templates of relationships, fixed in early infancy, to a model that views attachment as
the context provided by evolution for the development of interpersonal understanding’
(Fonagy et al., 2004, pp. 16–17).
Fonagy, along with a number of colleagues, has developed the theory of mentalisation
as a means of explaining the construction of selfhood and attachment. According to
Fonagy, a sensitive carer will:
….respond with contingent and marked affective displays of their own experience in
response to the infants subjective experience, thus enabling the child to develop second-
order representations of his own subjective experiences (Fonagy, 1998 cited in Fonagy &
Allison, 2014, p. 372).

Summarising the literature, Fonagy and Allison (2014) argue that the sensitivity of the
carer will allow the child to experience being recognised as an ‘intentional agent’ as well as
82 E. HARLOW

facilitate his/her building of ‘cognitive, social-cognitive and emotion-regulating capacity’


(p. 373). Some of the relational behaviours that signal sensitivity include appropriate eye
contact, communicational turn-taking and tone. Within this conducive environment the
child develops epistemic trust in the carer. As the child grows and his/her social inter-
actions extend beyond the carer, this enables the generalising of epistemic trust to others
(where appropriate), which facilitates learning from the social environment.
By means of the concept of mentalisation, Fonagy et al. (2004) have developed
attachment theory in ways that that resolve the tensions between Bowlby’s contribution
and psychoanalysis: mentalisation allows the mind to mediate our experience of the
external world. As indicated above, as well as being crucial to our sense of self and agency,
mentalisation is linked to affect regulation, that is, our ability to regulate our affect states
(Fonagy & Campbell, 2015). It is proposed by these theorists that ‘self-regulation as well
as the ability to reflect on other minds are constructed capacities that have evolved (or not)
out of the earliest relationships’ (Fonagy et al., 2004, p. 5).
Whilst relational interaction in the social domain is the main focus of the account of child
development provided by Fonagy and the colleagues with whom he writes, they also acknowl-
edge that evolution, genes, hormones and neural functioning make a contribution to the way
in which this occurs. In contrast to these theorists, Porges (2011) prioritises an evolutionary
perspective and the neurophysiological components of child and parent interaction that
correspond with attachment theory. Given this contrast, and in order to provide another
example of the theoretical developments that have occurred, Porges’ contribution is sum-
marised below. Porges emphasises the significance of risk to human survival and the way in
which it is assessed (which he calls neuroception). His polyvagal theory concerns the
individual’s neurobiological defensive response to risk (such as fight, flight or freeze) and
technological innovation, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), has enabled
researchers to explore this physiologically. According to this theory, if a secure base is not
communicated by the primary carer (by means of facial interaction, for example), then the
infant’s neuroception may be impaired: that is, he or she may not develop the required
sophistication to assess risk in the social environment, which may lead to a heightened sense of
fear and inappropriate defensive reactions. In extreme cases this may lead to psychpathology.
For Porges, the consequence of the theory means that individuals with an insecure attachment
requires intervention in the social domain (externally), but also individually (internally): that
is, a secure base should be provided (in foster care or the school environment, as discussed
below, for example), but in addition he suggests ‘We can also intervene directly with children,
exercising the neural regulation of the social engagement system and encourage social behaviour’
(Porges, 2011, p. 19). Intervention may include forms of music therapy.
Porges’ theory is complex and outlined here in only the simplest of terms. There are
many criticisms, however. For example, there is no clear consensus on the value of
evolutionary psychology, the perspective which informed Bowlby’s work, but which is
now revived and emphasised in Porges’ thesis. This is because theory from this perspec-
tive can only ever be post-hoc and claims cannot be proved (Glassman & Hadad, 2013).
Furthermore, evolutionary psychology depends on biology and the biological intricacies
of brain functioning are not well understood. Rose and Rose (2016), critical of the
overstatement of claims for neuroscience point out that there is still no one theory of
the brain, and despite the advances in technology, it is still inadequate to the enormity of
the task that is at issue. In general support of this principle, Glassman and Hadad make
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE 83

the point that there are about ’ . . . 10 billion neurons in the cortex of the brain alone, which
are interconnected in manifold ways. In addition, there are countless chemical interactions
involving neurotransmitters, hormones and neuropeptides . . . ’ (Glassman & Hadad, 2013,
p. 88). Critics such as Rose and Rose (2016) argue that fMRI scans do not give rise to
causal scientific truths (as some might claim), but interpretations that suggest correla-
tions. Unfortunately, the way in which some scientists have reduced behaviour to
nothing more than the biology of the brain, together with the oversimplification and
popularisation of neuroscience in the media, some authors (such as Gabriel, 2017) and
professionals (such as psychoanalysts) have shunned it as a serious contributor to our
understanding of human development (Fotopoulou, 2012).
In constructing a theory of human development that involves attachment between the
infant and primary carer as key, all of the three theorists discussed above (Bowlby,
Fonagy and Porges), have emphasised the biological component of human attachment
to a greater or lesser extent: put another way, all three have offered a biopsychosocial
account. However, it might be argued that for Bowlby and Fonagy the emphasis is on the
psychosocial construction of, and interpretative function of the ‘mind’, whilst for Porges
the biological materiality of the body and neural process is ascendant. Underpinning
these theoretical contributions lie the philosophical debates concerning the mind-body
dualism. Debates that have been taken up by Fotopoulou (2012).
Fotopoulou (2012) has articulated the projects of neuropsychoanalysis and neurophiloso-
phy which aim to overcome the mind-body dualism: for the mind-body dualism to be
understood, the ‘mind’ has to be seen to be as real as the brain and that both subjective
experiences as well as brain states have to be appreciated. Theoretical innovations which
attempt to overcome the mind-body dualism are applauded by academics such as Mackenzie
and Roberts (2017). These debates suggest that theories relating to human attachment are the
result, not only of technological innovation (such as fMRI), but philosophical considerations
also. In sum, taking into account the expansion of theoretical insights (not all of which could
be covered here), Schore and Schore (2008) suggest that there has emerged a new emphasis
on the affect regulation or the regulation of the emotions:

. . . the concept of regulation theory as an amalgam of Bowlby’s attachment theory, updated


internal object relations theories, self-psychology and contemporary relational theory all
informed by neuroscience and infant research. This is a profoundly developmental
approach. We understand any individual’s personal trajectory of emotional growth, includ-
ing his/her unconscious, to be facilitated or inhibited by his/her family and culture (Schore
& Schore, 2008, p. 17).

Irrespective of the preferred explanation or point of emphasis, human attachments and the
associated implications, are seen as crucial to the practice of professionals tasked with
ensuring and enhancing the developmental wellbeing of children. It is to some of the
practical applications of attachment and corresponding theories that attention is now turned.

Attachment theory and its application in social work, social care and
education
Although there has, to some extent, been a departure from a simplistic and rigid categor-
isation of attachment types, an extreme version of an insecure attachment has now been
84 E. HARLOW

recognised as a disorder by the National Institute of Health and Care Excellence (see
NICE, 2015). However, NICE has made clear that the term attachment disorder should
only be used when a young person has been formally diagnosed by qualified professionals
in accordance with the established criteria. Furthermore, social workers have been advised
to use the term ‘attachment’ with caution and that in most instances, the more general
term ‘relationship’ might suffice (Shemmings, 2018). Nevertheless, since Bowlby’s initial
contribution, the principles of human attachment have been important components of
social work. Along with associated psychodynamic theory, it is relevant to relationship-
based practice with all service users (see Ruch, 2018). Here, however, attention will be
given to work with children, young people and their families. As indicated above, children
and young people who have experienced phenomena such as loss and grief, abuse and
neglect, are less likely to have benefitted from sustained secure attachments: although not
inevitably so, they are more likely to be insecure and anxious. For fostered and adopted
children and young people, continuity, an understanding environment, and one or more
relationships that provide a secure base become essential if previous difficulties are to be
surmounted. These principles are important in their home, but also in their school. What
follows is an attempt to illustrate the way in which these principles are being promoted
through the training and/or support of foster carers, adopters and school staff members.
By means of longitudinal research on children (in middle childhood or early adoles-
cence) growing up in foster care, Schofield and Beek (2005) have theorised the day-to-day
construction of an emotionally secure base. The researchers identified five intercon-
nected and mutually reinforcing dimensions of the care provided by the foster carers.
These dimensions, together with examples of parenting and their children’s behaviours,
are indicated in brief.

Promoting trust in availability


The carers in the project demonstrated a focus on the child’s needs, even in his/her
absence, that was seen as akin to maternal preoccupation (Winnicott, 1965). In addition
to thinking about the ways in which the child’s problems might be solved, thought was
given to positive contribution the child was making to the family. In this way, the carer
was holding the child in mind. Physical care was also provided. Even adolescent children
enjoyed practical care such as hair washing or affectionate hand holding.

Promoting reflective function


The carer’s ability to think flexibly assists the child to do the same and carers can help
children to make sense of their own minds as well as the minds of others (Fonagy &
Target, 1997). This is important in developing resilience as well as social and interperso-
nal competence. Ongoing conversations about day-to-day emotions took place and the
children benefitted from feeling understood and accepted.

Promoting self-esteem
Positive parenting conveys to children their unconditional acceptance. This occurs by means
of loving gestures, kind words and praise. Children learn to feel valued for who they are.
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE 85

Hence, failures can be tolerated and help sought when required. The parents in Schofield and
Beek’s study spoke with pride about the children’s achievements and the characteristics with
warmth. A growing sense of self-esteem was demonstrated by the children as they took part
in classroom activities, became more outgoing and acknowledged their strengths.

Promoting autonomy/self-efficacy
Infants express their needs and sensitive parents react appropriately, hence there develops
the idea that a behaviour leads to a positive response, but also that there is the capacity for
control over the environment. Parents who are available, allow the child to explore
confident in the knowledge that they will still be available on return. However, as the
child matures, there is recognition of the parent’s needs, so develops co-operation and
negotiation. Foster children may have had to be too independent in the past and been too
powerful. Alternatively, if birth parents had been too controlling, they may not have had the
opportunity to exercise independence. Foster carers’ in the Schofield and Beek study
encouraged children to be independent in ways that related to the child’s age and ability,
for example, encouraging a child to choose which clothes to wear or facilitating in the
achievement of homework rather than undertaking the homework on the child’s behalf. The
children demonstrated independence by taking up hobbies, either solitary hobbies or those
that were shared with others. Most of the behaviours involved negotiation and compromise.

Promoting family membership


Becoming a member of a family means complying with expectations and embracing the
norms and values. Being part of a family provides an anchorage and emotional solidarity
over the life course. The norms and values were played out by means of rituals, such as
the giving and receiving of cards and gifts. Carers and their extended family included the
fostered children in these rituals and over time the children talked about their home
rather than their placement.
This model, which acknowledges the biopsychosocial foundations of attachment
theory, emphasises the psychosocial content and has been used widely to train prospec-
tive foster carers for their future role (see Schofield & Beek, 2016). The model is relevant
in preparing adoptive parents for their new role. In order to help facilitate the success of
the adoption, training that helps adoptive parents understand that, despite their efforts to
build their relationship, the children may still exhibit troubled behaviour (McNeish &
Scott, 2013, p. 29). However, from the autoethnographic account provided by Mackenzie
and Roberts (2017), in providing training for adoptive parents, social workers and
clinical psychologists are also emphasising the importance of affect regulation and
drawing on theories that are influenced by evolutionary theory and neuroscience in
general. As a result of being immersed in training, support delivered from this perspec-
tive, and the recommended reading materials, Mackenzie and Roberts reflected on
parenting in the following way:
Encountering difficult behaviour and overwhelming feelings, we learnt to ask: is there some
visual trigger, olfactory memory or metabolic state (blood sugar, heart rate, cortisol) pointing
to other more dangerous places or times – something like a body memory – at play here?
86 E. HARLOW

Perhaps re-mapping those mundane or shocking events – he spat in my face; she deliberately
smashed the bowl – through ethological or biochemical processes might bring parents to
engage differently to the child or between the child and themselves at that moment, and
thereby help both the child and themselves to remain anchored in the present (which is less
frightening or dangerous in objective terms) (Mackenzie & Roberts, 2017, p. 137).

Recognising the development away from the reliance on Bowlby’s original formulation only
and the recent emphasis on ‘brain-based parenting’ (which is more aligned with the work of
Porges, as summarised above), Mackenzie and Roberts noted the ad hoc approach that was
taken to theory: that is, with the intention of offering a positive intervention professionals did
not refer to original research, clinical papers or promote theoretical coherence, but articulated
explanations of attachment, child development and behaviour on a pragmatic basis. Such an
approach is replicated in the popular ‘how-to’ books that were recommended as forms of
assistance for the adoptive parents (Mackenzie & Roberts, 2017).
A Centre for Adoption Support (CfAS) was established in the northwest of England in
2013. An evaluation of its construction and performance showed that attachment theory
influenced the form and content of the services delivered (see Author, 2018; Harlow,
Mitchell, Doherty, & Moran, 2015). In particular, the work of Hughes (1999, 2004) had
been embraced. Hughes, a psychotherapist, has made a particular contribution to knowledge
and practice by applying the principles of attachment theory to the needs of adoptive families.
He aims to address the emotional pain that an adopted child may have experienced. Such
experience inhibits the building of the child’s affect regulation with the consequence of
behavioural and relational problems. According to Rushton and Dance (2002), adoptive
parents reported that children’s ‘weak return of affection or rejection, persistent non-
compliance, violence and aggression’ was particularly difficult to deal with. Hughes emphasises
verbal and non-verbal attunement in the building of the new relationship between the child
and his/her adoptive parents. The CfAS social workers had attended Hughes’ training on
Dyadic Developmental Psychotherapy (see Hughes, Golding, & Hudson, 2015). This training
appeared to inform and enhance the relationship-based social work that was being under-
taken. When talking about the service she had received from CfAS, an adoptive mother (with
the pseudonym of Mrs Black) said that she would have welcomed such support from the start
of the time her children had been placed with her:

Mrs Black: Well, it doesn’t have to be a therapist. [It] could be a therapeutic social worker.
Somebody like [names social worker from CfAS] who understands attachment, who is
emotionally in tune, that can be a listening ear and say, ‘Have you tried strategies?’. Some
practical advice as well as that sort of listening ear, so you don’t feel so isolated (T1:6)
(Harlow et al., 2015, p. 31).

From the CfAS research, it appeared that many adoptive parents needed support in
managing daily life at home, but they felt this was impacted by their child’s experience in
school. They felt that improvements could be made if the emotional needs of their
children were appreciated by school staff. This sentiment was echoed in research con-
ducted by Adoption UK:

Many respondents felt that staff and education professionals should be trained in issues of
trauma and attachment, and that there needs to be a better understanding of the needs of
adopted children (Pennington, n.d., p. 35)
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE 87

Social workers at the CfAS were responding to the concerns of adopted parents by
working directly with school staff members to create an environment that reduced the
anxieties of individual children: i.e. tailored packages of support were introduced. In
addition, however, training on attachment theory was made available for the whole
school (see also Adoption UK, n.d.).
Attachment disorder is now acknowledged in the Department for Education’s advice on
mental health and behaviour in school (DfE, 2015). However, interest in human attach-
ments and their significance in the school environment is broader that the topic of health
(see for example, Clements, 2013; Geddes, 2006; Marshall, 2014; Phillips, 2007; Ubha &
Cahill, 2014; Webber, 2017). As with social work, as indicated above, the practice literature
relating to education is often eclectic in approach. For example, Rose and Gilbert (2017)
refer to the work of Bowlby as well as Porges. Irrespective of this eclecticism, however,
contributors in general argue that the implementation of attachment principles in school
can improve the general wellbeing of students and in consequence their performance (see
Bergin & Bergin, 2009). Also, it is argued that by helping school staff understand and
respond to disruptive behaviour as evidence of emotional insecurity, the rate of exclusions
can be reduced. There are three main approaches to the implementation of attachment
principles in school. Firstly, children who are struggling to regulate their emotions may be
offered emotion coaching (see Bombѐr & Hughes, 2013; Gus & Wood, 2017). Secondly,
efforts are made to develop the awareness of all school staff members in order that they
might reflect on the meanings of behaviour (for example, in the light of insights concerning
fear and anxiety) with the ambition that their relationships with all students are enhanced.
This relates closely to the third approach which emphasises the functioning of the whole
school (see Bergin & Bergin, 2009). Importantly, it is the responsibility of school leaders to
encourage a warm socioemotional climate and a culture of respect. In order to stimulate
a sense of security, continuity of people and place is beneficial, and advantage may be
gained when essential transitions (across years and schools) are facilitated. Small schools
that are embedded within their communities are more likely to encourage student bonding,
and inclusive extra-curricular activities are helpful with this. In short, the school itself
should become a secure base.

Conclusion
Decades have elapsed since Bowlby developed his theory of attachment. Its popularity
has fluctuated and over recent times it has been reinvigorated. However, from the
discussion above, it can be seen that it has been developed and integrated with a range
of new theoretical and philosophical insights. It might be argued that there is no longer
one ‘attachment theory’, but that over time there have evolved a number of theoretical
explanations of human attachment. Whilst each of these theoretical explanations may
take into account biology, psychology, and social relationships – making the explanation
biopsychosocial – each explanation may emphasise different elements. However, when
psychologists, social workers and associated professionals inform their work with the-
ories of attachment (which may or may not refer to neuroscience), like cultural and social
theorists, they may do so in an eclectic manner (see Mackenzie and Roberts (2017).
Whilst there is a risk that an emphasis on the biology of ‘brain science’ becomes collapsed
with ‘brain damage’, respondents in the CfAS study as well as Mackenzie and Roberts
88 E. HARLOW

found that as adoptive parents, their experience of training and support provided by
professionals to be helpful. Being provided with an explanation of their child’s challen-
ging behaviour, reassurance that change could occur, together with suggestions for
making this happen, brought welcome relief. Nevertheless, theories of attachment do
not constitute a panacea and Scott (2018) warned against their over-use as an explanatory
tool: that is, it would be possible to misdiagnose the needs of a child if there was an
automatic reliance on the principles of attachment. Similarly, some therapies associated
with attachment and/or ‘brain science’ may not be evidentially legitimate: it is not
possible to ‘fix’ specific components of the brain and it is better to pursue conventional
therapies that have been thoroughly evaluated and tested over time.
Finally, it may be argued that focusing on attachment highlights the child and parent/carer
relationship with the consequence that essential considerations concerning the wider com-
munity are cast into shadow. However, Schofield and Beek (2018) embed their emphasis on
relationships within Bronfenbrenner’s (1979) ecological model of growth and human devel-
opment. Furthermore, the social workers who are working with the staff members of schools
are acting within the terms of this model. Importantly, the emphasis on whole school change
shifts attention away from specific parents, carers or children, and encourages systemic
organisational change that aims to benefit all concerned.

Acknowledgments
Gratitude is extended to all those who supported the writing of this paper. These include: Alan
Wood; Kate Fillingham; Karen Izod; Hilary Rose, Steven Rose, Fiona Sheikh; and the Alex
Timpson Trust.

Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author.

Funding
This work was supported by the Alex Timpson Trust.

Notes on contributor
Elizabeth Harlow PhD, BA, CQSW, FHEA is a Professor of Social Work at the University of
Chester. She worked as a practitioner before beginning her academic career in 1988. Focussing
primarily on the organisation, management and delivery of services to children and their families,
she has led research projects and evaluations and published her work over the last three decades.
[email: [email protected]]

ORCID
Elizabeth Harlow http://orcid.org/0000-0001-5031-7485
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE 89

References
Adoption UK. (n.d.). Bridging the gap: Giving adopted children and equal chance in school.
Banbury: Author. Retrieved from www.adoptionuk.org/Handlers/Download.ashx?IDMF=
e460b99a-4ebb-4348-bd23-64a50d747901
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological
study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Author. (2018). Defining the problem and sourcing the solution: A reflection on some of the
organisational, professional and emotional complexities of accessing post-adoption support.
Journal of Social Work Practice, 33(3), 269–280.
Bergin, C., & Bergin, D. (2009). Attachment in the classroom. Educational Psychology Review, 21,
141–170.
Berk, L. E. (2013). Child development (9th ed.). London: Pearson.
Bombѐr, L. M., & Hughes, D. A. (2013). Settling to learn. Settling troubled pupils to learn: Why
relationships matter in school. London: Worth Publishing.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1: Attachment. London: Howgarth Press.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2: Separation: Anger and anxiety. London: Howgarth
Press.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3: Loss: Sadness and depression. London: Howgarth
Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design.
Cambridge MA: Harvard University press.
Clements, J. (2013). A good practice guide for schools. Understanding and meeting the needs of
children who are looked after, fostered or adopted or otherwise permanently placed. London: Pac.
Crittenden, P. M. (1985). Social networks, quality of parenting, and child development. Child
Development, 56, 1299–1313.
Crittenden, P. M. (2000a). A dynamic-maturational model of the function, development and
organization of human relationships. In R. S. L. Mills, & S. Duck (Eds.), Developmental
psychology of personal relationships (pp. 199–218). Chichester: Wiley.
Crittenden, P. M. (2000b). A dynamic-maturational exploration of the meaning of security and
adaptation: Empirical, cultural and theoretical considerations. In P. M. Crittenden &
A. A. Claussen (Eds.), The organization of attachment relationships: Maturation, culture and
context (pp. 343–357). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. (1996). Emotional security as a regulatory process in normal
development and the development of psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8,
123–139.
DfE (Department for Education). (2015). Mental health and behaviour in schools. Department
advice for school staff. Retrieved from www.gov.uk/government/publications.
Duschinsky, R., Greco, M., & Soloman, J. (2015). The politics of attachment: Lines of flight with
Bowlby, Deleuze and Guattari. Theory Culture and Society, 32(7–8), 173–195.
Fonagy, P. (1998). An attachment theory approach to treatment of the difficult patient. Bulletin of
the Menninger Clinic, 62, 147–169.
Fonagy, P., & Allison, E. (2014). The role of mentalizing and epistemic trust in the therapeutic
relationship. Psychotherapy, 51(3), 372–380.
Fonagy, P., & Campbell, C. (2015). Attachment and mentalizing. In A. Elliot & J. Prager (Eds.), The
routledge handbook of psychoanalysis in the social sciences and humanities (pp. 343–
357). London: Routledge.
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., & Target, M. (2004). Affect regulation, mentalization, and the
development of the self. London: Karnac.
Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (1997). Attachment and reflective function: Their role in
self-organisation. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 679–700.
Fotopoulou, A. K. (2012). The history of progress of neuropsychoanalysis. In A. Fotopoulou,
D. Pfaff, & M. A. Conway (Eds.), From the couch to the lab: Trends in psychodynamic
neuroscience (pp. 12–24). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
90 E. HARLOW

Gabriel, M. (2017). I am not a brain. Cambridge: Polity Press.


Geddes, H. (2006). Attachment in the classroom. The links between children’s early experience,
emotional well-being and performance in the classroom. London: Worth Publishing.
Glassman, W. E., & Hadad, M. (2013). Approaches to psychology (6th ed.). London: McGraw-Hill.
Gus, L., & Wood, F. (2017). Emotion coaching. In D. Colley & P. Cooper (Eds.), Attachment and
emotional development in the classroom. Theory and practice (pp. 83–99). London: Jessica
Kingsley Publishing.
Harlow, E., Mitchell, A., Doherty, P., & Moran, P. (2015). Constructing and delivering services of
support: An evaluation of the Northwest post-placement adoption support service (2). Chester:
University of Chester.
Howe, D. (2011). Attachment across the lifecourse. A brief introduction. Basingstoke: Palgrave
Macmillan.
Hughes, D. (2004). An attachment-based treatment of maltreated and young people. Attachment
and Human Development, 6(3), 263–278.
Hughes, D., Golding, K., & Hudson, J. (2015). Dyadic developmental psychotherapy: The devel-
opment of the theory, practice and research base. Adoption and Fostering, 39(4), 356–365.
Hughes, D. A. (1999). Adopting children with attachment problems. Child Welfare, LXXVIII(5),
541–560.
Mackenzie, A., & Roberts, C. (2017). Adopting neuroscience: Parenting and affective
indeterminacy. Body and Society, 23(3), 130–155.
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1986). Discovery of a new, insecure-disorganized/disoriented attach-
ment pattern. In M. Yogman & T. B. Brazelton (Eds.), Affective development in infancy (pp.
95–124). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Marshall, N. (2014). The teachers introduction to attachment. Practical essentials for teachers, carers
and school support staff. London: Jessica Kingsley.
McNeish, D., & Scott, S. (2013). What works in achieving adoption for looked after children: An
overview of evidence for Coram/Barnardo partnership. Retrieved from www.coram.org.uk/sites/
default/files/resource_files/Evidence_review_on_adoption_NCEP.pdf
NICE (National Institute of Health and Care Excellence). (2015). Children’s attachment:
Attachment in children and young people who are adopted from care, in care or at high risk
of going into care. Retrieved from www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng26
Pennington, E. (n.d.). It takes a village to raise a child: Adoption UK survey on adoption support.
Banbury: Adoption UK.
Phillips, R. (2007). The need for information on how attachment difficulties of adopted and looked
after children affect their Schooling. Adoption and Fostering, 31(3), 28–38.
Porges, S. W. (2011). The polyvagal theory: New physiological foundations of emotions, attachment,
communication and self-regulation. London: W.W.Norton and Company.
Rose, H., & Rose, S. (2016). Can neuroscience change our minds? Cambridge: Polity Press.
Rose, J., & Gilbert, L. (2017). Attachment aware schools. In D. Colley & P. Cooper (Eds.),
Attachment and development in the classroom: Theory and practice (pp. 65–81). London:
Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Ruch, G. (2018). The contemporary context of relationship-based practice. In G. Ruch, D. Turney,
& A. Ward (Eds.), Relationship-based social work (2nd ed., pp. 19–35). London: Jessica Kingsley
Publishing.
Rushton, A., & Dance, C. (2002). Adoption support services for families in difficulty. A literature
review and UK survey. London: British Association of Adoption and Fostering.
Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Colvert, E., Kreppner, J., Mehta, M., . . . Sonuga-Barke, E. (2007).
Effects of profound early institutional deprivation: An overview of findings from a UK long-
itudinal study of Romanian adoptees. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(3),
332–350.
Schofield, G., & Beek, M. (2005). Providing a secure base: Parenting children in long-term foster
family care. Attachment and Human Development, 7(1), 3–26.
Schofield, G., & Beek, M. (2016). The secure base model. Promoting attachment and resilience in
foster care and adoption. London: CoramBAAF.
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE 91

Schofield, G., & Beek, M. (2018). Attachment handbook for foster care and adoption (2nd ed.).
London: CoramBAAF.
Schore, J. R., & Schore, A. N. (2008). Modern attachment theory: The central role of affect
regulation in development and treatment. Clinical Social Work Journal, 36(1), 9–20.
Scott, S. (2018, June 6). Presentation made at the conference ‘Realising our vision for adoption in the
UK: Working in partnership to deliver excellent services nationwide’, a public policy exchange
Symposium held at the Strand Palace Hotel, London, UK.
Shemmings, D. (2018, June 28). Why social workers shouldn’t use ‘attachment’ in their records
and reports. Community Care. Retrieved from www.communitycare.co.uk/2018/06/28/social-
workers-shouldnt-use-attachment-records-reports/
Smith, M., Cameron, C., & Reimer, D. (2017). From attachment to recognition for children in care.
British Journal of Social Work, 47(6), 1606–1623.
Ubha, N., & Cahill, S. (2014). Building secure attachments for primary school children: A mixed
methods study. Educational Psychology in Practice, 30(3), 272–292.
Waters, E., & Cummings, E. M. (2000). A secure base from which to explore close relationships.
Child Development, 21(1), 164–172.
Webber, L. (2017). A school’s journey in creating a relational environment which supports
attachment and emotional security. Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties, 22(4), 317–331.
Winnicott, D. (1965). The maturational process and the facilitative environment. New York:
International Universities Press.
Copyright of Journal of Social Work Practice is the property of Routledge and its content may
not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.

You might also like