Blank - Et - Al-1990-A Test of The Situational Leadership Theory
Blank - Et - Al-1990-A Test of The Situational Leadership Theory
1990.43
WARREN BLANK
The Leadership Group
Maharishi International University
JOHN R. WEITZEL
Syracuse University
STEPHEN G. GREEN
Purdue University
Correspondence and requests for reprints should be addressed to Warren Blank, Ph.D.,
The Leadership Group, 3463 State Street, Suite 157, Santa Barbara, CA 93105.
579
580 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
SLT focuses on two primary types of leader behavior: task and rela-
tionship behavior (Hersey & Blanchard, 1982). Hersey and Blanchard
(1969, 1982) suggest that these behaviors are very similar to consider-
ation and initiation of structure which are well grounded in leadership
literature (e.g., Cartwright & Zander, 1960; Halpin, 1959; Katz, Mac-
coby & Morse, 1950). Moreover, Hersey and Blanchard (1982) suggest
that the task and relationship behaviors be operationalized in a manner
that closely parallels existing operationalizations of consideration and
initiation of structure (see Leader Behavior Descriptive Questionnaire
[LBDQ-XII], Stogdill & Coons, 1957). Vecchio (1987) used the LBDQ-
XI1 to measure task and relationship behavior, arguing it is a more widely
accepted index of leader behavior than the Hersey and Blanchard LEAD
instrument. Therefore, the present study measures leader task and rela-
tionship behaviors in terms of consideration and initiation of structure.
SLT aldo focuses on subordinate “maturity” as the key situational
characteristic that is said to moderate the relationship between leader
behavior (task and relationship) and leader effectiveness (Hersey &
Blanchard, 1969, 1982). Maturity is defined as the “ability and willing-
ness of people to take responsibility for directing their own behavior”
(Hershey & Blanchard, 1982, p. 151). Hersey and Blanchard (1982) ar-
gue that subordinate maturity consists of two dimensions: psychological
maturity and job maturity. Psychological maturity is somewhat ambigu-
ously defined in Hersey and Blanchard’s (1982) recent work. It is char-
acterized as a “willingness or motivation to do something” and as hav-
ing “to do with confidence and commitment” (p. 157). Examples of its
WARREN BLANK ET AL. 581
S
0
.-b
>
S
Task Behavior
Relationship
Behavior
MI M2 M3 M4
Follower Maturity
this study, our first step was to examine the prescribed linear relationship
between task behavior and maturity and the prescribed curvilinear rela-
tionship between relationship behavior and maturity. This might shed
more light on the predictions of SLT and provide a comparison to exist-
ing, more complex, published assessments of the model.
Our second step in examining SLT was to test the more complex
formulation of SLT. Concerns with conducting this analysis are noted
above; however, a complete understanding of SLT requires taking this
step in addition to focusing on the underlying assumptions. Analyzing
the more complex formulation of SLT requires combining task and rela-
tionship behavior to create four “leader styles” suggested by the model
(see Figure 2). SLT argues that each style is most appropriate for one of
-
I
! I 1
-P
I
Hiah HiahTask
tP
K
sI
W
m
4
u)
Z
E9
W
1
(LO’ -TASK BEHAVIOR
’
I’ LowandTask‘ IRelationship!
Low
(1
four levels of maturity. For this analysis, psychological and job maturity
are combined since SLT suggests this. Again, we have noted above that
there are limitations implied by this approach. However, in faithfulness
to SLT, we conducted the analysis as SLT specifies.
Hersey and Blanchard (1982) suggest a difference in “effectiveness”
when there is a leader styleflevel of maturity “fit” as opposed to when
there is “no fit” between leader style/maturity. SLT suggests leader style
S1 fits maturity level M1, S2 fits M2, S3 fits M3, and S4 fits M4. We
examined this fit/no fit relationship in relation to criteria of effectiveness.
Figure 2 depicts the prescribed relationships between leader style and
subordinate maturity. The curve represents the change of leader style
required to achieve leader effectiveness for each level of maturity.
Hersey and Blanchard (1982) define effectiveness in terms of goal
accomplishment and the internal states or predispositions of the subor-
dinate. An effective leadership “style” influences subordinates to “do a
certain job” (p. 109), suggesting that subordinate performance should
be affected. Effective leadership style also creates a climate where the
subordinate “respects (the leader) and is willing to cooperate with (the
leader)” and finds the job rewarding (p. 109), suggesting that subordi-
nate affect also should be affected. Thus, this research focuses on sub-
ordinate performance and satisfaction with the supervisor and work as
indicants of leader effectiveness for both steps in the analysis (i.e., ex-
amination of underlying assumptions and the complex formulation of
the model). Use of multiple indicants of leader effectiveness provides a
more complete test of SLT.
In summary, the present study first examines the basic assumptions
of SLT. This is a reasonable step, not taken by previous research, to as-
sess the strength of SLT. In an effort to more fully examine SLT, our
second step was to analyze the relationship between combined task and
relationship behavior (leader style) and subordinate maturity. For both
analyses, pqrticular attention is given to the use of measures with ade-
quate psychometric properties since previous research on SLT, discussed
above, uses measures that strain standards of validity and reliability.
Hypotheses
Method
Sample
Measures
TABLE 1
Zero Order Correlation
Variables Mean SD Range 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
RA staff size was 4-35 with a median size of 9). This “peer rating”
of psychological maturity was used because it avoided the bias of self-
reports. It also provided a measure of maturity that was independent of
the leader behavior descriptions (RAs) and the performance judgments
(HDs) thus minimizing method variance problems.
The psychological maturity measure was also completed by each RA
for himherself (a self rating) and by each HD for all hisher RAs (a
leader rating). These additional rating sources were used to assess the
validity and reliability of the psychological maturity measure (see below).
Data from each of these three rating sources (peer, self, and leader)
were factor analyzed (VARIMAX rotation). The three factor solutions
indicated that the same 11items as in the manager sample had adequate
(Le., > .40)loadings. Therefore, the 11 retained items were summed
to create a score for the self (Cronbach alpha = .95) and leader rating
(Cronbach alpha = .93). To derive a peer maturity rating, a mean score
for each item was computed for the ratings provided by all peers. These
mean scores were summed for the peer rating (Cronbach alpha = .95).
The 11items are: To what extent does : (1)follow through on
job tasks, (2) act conscientiously on the job, (3) know what to do on the
job without being told, (4)work hard on the job, (5) try hard to improve
hidher performance on the job, (6) strive to do hisher best on the job,
(7) make job related decisions on hisher own, (8) do extra on the job,
(9) take care to do the job right, (10) do a thorough job on any tasks
undertaken, (11) set hisher own job goals.
The peer rating of psychological maturity, which is used for all hy-
pothesis testing, demonstrated adequate reliability and validity. The
measure yielded substantially the same factor structure over five inde-
pendent samples and repeatedly demonstrated strong internal consis-
tency, suggesting good reliability. Its validity is supported by its strong,
repeatable internal consistency (Runkel & McGrath, 1972, argue that
588 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Data Collection
site liaison during staff meetings within each hall. The liaison was a top-
ranking administrator within the residence hall system and was some-
one with whom the research team had close contact and a high level of
confidence. The liaison followed the research team’s carefully prepared
instructions. Participants at both sites were informed that the instru-
ments they were completing were to be used in a research study regard-
ing various behaviors of themselves and those with whom they worked.
Anonymity of their responses was assured and it was explained that only
aggregated results would be analyzed. Participants did not indicate any
concerns to the members of the research team at site 1 or to the liaison
at site 2 about their participation in the study. Follow-up data collection
sessions were provided at both schools. An overall participation rate of
88% was achieved across both schools.
Results
Since the first two hypotheses address the relationship between leader
behavior and leader effectiveness contingent on subordinate maturity,
moderated regression was used to test for a significant interaction term
for leader behavior and subordinate maturity in predicting subordinate
performance and satisfaction. Separate tests of the hypotheses were con-
ducted for psychological and job maturity.
For the hypotheses related to task behavior ( l a and b) both maturity
measures were dichotomized at the median. Low maturity was coded
as one and high maturity was coded as zero. The same analyses were
also conducted splitting both maturity measures into thirds and coding
the upper and lower thirds as one and zero. Since the results were
almost identical to the median split analyses, the median split findings
are reported here.
Results show little support for the hypotheses related to task behav-
ior (see Tables 2A and 2B). Only in the case of psychological maturity
and work satisfaction do we find a significant task behavior by maturity
interaction term. The form of the interaction does support the hypothe-
sis 2b (see Figure 3). Task behavior alone makes significant, unique con-
tributions to both types of satisfaction but not performance. Subordinate
psychological maturity alone makes a significant, unique contribution to
work satisfaction.
For the hypotheses related to relationship behavior (2a and b), both
maturity measures were divided into four quartiles. The middle two
quartiles were considered as the middle range or moderate group (i.e.,
the group which needed more relationship behavior) and coded as a one.
The lowest and highest quartiles were put into the extremes group (i.e.,
the group which needed less relationship behavior) and coded as zero.
590 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
TABLE 2A
Regression Models for Task Behavior and Psychological Maturity
Task Psychological Task x Psychological
Dependent behavior maturity maturity
variable R-Square p< P< P<
Performance .09*** .18 .10 .58
Supervisor satisfaction .17*** .O1 .45 .42
Work satisfaction .06** * .01 .O1 .03
TABLE 2B
Regression Models for Task Behavior and Job Maturity
Task Job Task x Job
Dependent behavior maturity maturity
variable R-Square P< P< P<
Performance .O1 - - -
Supervisor satisfaction .17*** .O1 .29 .41
Work satisfaction .03** .O1 .98 .98
Note: p values are for the partial F value for the terms and indicate unique contribution
to the model.
* *p<.o1; ***p<.oo1
high maturity
Work
Satisfaction
‘s
low maturity
Task Behavior
TABLE 3A
Regression Models-Relationship Behavior and Psychological Maturity
Performance .01 - - -
Supervisor satisfaction .29*** .01 .54 .62
Work satisfaction .04*** .01 .48 .68
TABLE 3B
Regression Models-Relationship Behavior and Job Maturity
Again, alternate splits were examined (i.e., upper and lower sixths coded
as extremes group, and third and fourth sixths coded as moderate group).
Almost identical results were obtained. The findings using quartile splits
are reported here.
Results (see Tables 3A and 3B) indicate no support for the hypothe-
ses. In no case was there a significant contribution by an interaction term.
In addition, neither maturity measure alone made significant unique
contributions for any of the criteria. The leader’s use of relationship
behavior, however, did make significant, unique contributions to both
types of subordinate satisfaction.
To examine the complex formulation of the model, four “leader
styles” and four “maturity levels” were created. Leader styles were cre-
ated by dividing both task and relationship behavior at the median into
a high and low levels. Each respondent was then assigned one of four
leader style scores: S1 = high task, low relationship; S2 = high task,
high relationship; S3 = low task, high relationship; and S4 = low task,
low relationship.
Maturity levels were created by dividing both psychological and job
maturity into four quartiles and then combining each respondent’s score
to create hisher maturity level. As noted above, there are ambiguities
inherent in this approach. The quartile splits of the job and psychological
maturity scores might not place a respondent in the same maturity level.
For example, the respondent might be in the second quartile for job
592 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
TABLE 4
MANOVA-Fit Between Leader Style and Subordinate Maturity
Fit No fit
Subordinate maturity (n = 128) (n =221) F df P
Multivariate - - 4.96 3,345 .02
Performance 3.83 3.80 .23 1,347 .63
Work satisfaction 36.51 33.95 9.46 1,347 .002
Supervisor satisfaction 44.81 43.80 .28 1,347 .26
maturity and in the third quartile for psychological maturity. Hersey and
Blanchard (1982) provide no guidelines on what to do with respondents
in such “ambiguous” maturity group. In the absence of clear guidelines,
we established a procedure for hypothesis testing that provides the most
liberal test of SLT
To test the fithot fit hypothesis, a fit grouping variable was created.
If the leader style exactly matched the prescribed style for both the sub-
ordinate’s job and psychological maturity level, the fit grouping variable
was set to indicate “fit” (e.g., if leader style was S2 and both job and
psychological maturity placed the respondent in level two, fit was indi-
cated). For respondents in an ambiguous maturity level, the fit grouping
variable was set to indicate “fit” when leader style showed a prescribed
match with either of the maturity variable levels (e.g., for S1 leader style,
a fit was indicated if either job or psychological maturity were in maturity
level one). Otherwise the grouping variable was set to indicate “no fit.”
This provided a liberal test of SLT.
One-way MANOVA was used with performance, supervisor and work
satisfaction as the dependent variable and fithot fit as the grouping vari-
able. To test whether or not fit added to variance contributed by task
behavior, relationship behavior, and subordinate maturity, analysis of
covariance was run with fit as the grouping variable and task behavior,
relationshiq behavior, and subordinate maturity as covariates. All com-
binations of fit were tested against all combinations of no fit. Results
indicate a multivariate result for fit between leader style and subordi-
nate maturity (see Table 4). An examination of the one-way analysis
of variance results for each criterion variable indicates the significant
MANOVA is primarily due to work satisfaction ( F = 9.46, 1,347 df,p
= .002). Although all means are in the expected direction, this result
provides not much more support for SLT. As a further test, the same
analyses were conducted using the self- and leader-ratings of maturity.
There were no significant relationships using the self-rating. A signifi-
cant multivariate result was found using the leader-rating with none of
the one-way analyses significant.
WARREN BLANK ET AL. 593
Discussion
and maturity apply only when the leader's performance and maturity as-
sessments of subordinates are used). Obviously more research is needed
on SLT to help us understand it.
At this point, given the results of the small line of research to which
this study adds, it is difficult to be very optimistic about SLT. The model's
underlying assumptions were not supported and the complex formula-
tion of the model received mixed support. Of course, one must always be
concerned that a reasonable examination was provided. In the present
case, we had reason to believe the sample and the setting employed
would be pertinent to the processes described by SLT. Moreover, the
SLT authors invoked similar settings in presenting their theory, and Vec-
chi0 used an educational setting for his work. Future research could only
benefit by examining SLT across different samples (Vecchio, 1987), but
we have no reason to believe that the present sample was inappropriate.
Similarly, the measures employed attempted to faithfully represent
the constructs described by SLT. The psychological maturity measure
was developed with care and appears to demonstrate reasonable levels
of reliability and validity. The measure of job maturity can be criticized
for being narrow in its scope, for example, focusing on job experience
alone; but, this aspect of job maturity is central to the concept as defined
by Hersey and Blanchard (1982) and thus provided at least a partial test
of this aspect of SLT. Use of the Hambleton, Blanchard, and Hersey
(1977) measure cannot be viewed as a more rigorous approach since that
measure has yet to demonstrate validity and reliability comparable to the
measures used here.
The measures of leader behavior and leader effectivenessalso appear
to be fair extrapolations of the theory as it is stated and were the same
as those used by Vecchio (1987). In addition, different respondents
provided the leader behavior, maturity, satisfaction, and performance
data which minimized method variance problems. Finally, the analyses
used to tes SLT attempted to give the hypotheses every chance to be
1
confirmed. Therefore, this study is considered a reasonable, though
certainly not definitive, test of the underlying assumptions of SLT.
In a larger sense, the conduct of this study revealed a number of
things about SLT that may be useful to future work in the area. The idea
of leader behavior and subordinate maturity interacting to predict leader
effectivenesscannot be abandoned prematurely. It was supported in one
instance here. This study also suggests that both managers and peers
could recognize psychological maturity in subordinates, and Hersey and
Blanchard (1982) present persuasive arguments for the potential impor-
tance of this concept. Given the few main effects for subordinate matu-
rity found in this study, it still may well be the case that the role of sub-
ordinate maturity is best understood in terms of interactions with leader
WARREN BLANK ET AL. 595
REFERENCES
Argyris C. (1957). Personality and organization. New York: Harper and Row.
Blank W, Weitzel J, Blau G, Green SG. (1988). A measure of psychological maturity.
Group and Oqganization Studies. 13,225-238.
Cartwright D, Zander A (Eds.). (1960). Group dynamics: Research and theory (2nd ed.).
Evanston, I L Harper & Row.
Fiedler F. (1964). A contingency model of leadership effectiveness. In Berkowitz L (Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press.
Fiedler F. (1967). A theory of leadership effectiveness. New York McGraw-Hill.
Graeff C. (1983). The situational leadership theory: A critical review. Academy ofMan-
agement Review, 8,285-291.
Halpin A. (1959). The leadership behavioral school superintendents. Chicago: Midwest
Administration Center.
Hambleton R, Blanchard K, Hersey P.(1977). Maturity scale-selfratingform. San Diego:
Learning Resources Corporation.
Hambleton RK, Gumpert R. (1982). The validity of Hersey and Blanchard’s theory of
leader effectiveness. Group and Organization Studies, 7,225-242.
WARREN BLANK ET AL. 597
Hersey P, Blanchard K. (1969). Life cycle theory of leadership. Training and Development
Journal, 2 , 6 3 4 .
Hersey P, Blanchard K. (1982). Management of organizational behavior (4th ed.). Engle-
wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Hoelting E (1980). Resident student development. Normal, IL: National Entertainment
and Campus Activities Association, Illinois State University.
House RJ. (1971). A path-goal theory of leader effectiveness. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 16,321-339.
Katz D, Maccoby N, Morse N. (1950). Productivity supervision, and morale in an ofice
situation. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan, Institute for Social Research.
Kauffman J (Ed.). (1968). The student in higher education. New Haven, C T The Hazen
Foundation.
Kerr S, Schriesheim C. (1974). Consideration, initiating structure, and organizational
criteria: An update of Korman’s 1966 review. PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY, 27, 555-
568.
Korman A. (1966). Consideration, initiating structure, and organizational criteria: A
review. PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY, 66,394-361.
McClelland D, Atkinson J, Clark R, Lowell E. (1953). The achievement motive. New York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Nickerson D, Harrington J. (1971). The college student as counselor. Norvic, NY: Chronicle
Guidance.
Nunnally J. (1978). Psychometric theory (2nd ed.). New York McGraw Hill.
Runkel P, McGrath J. (1972). Research on human behavior: A systematic guide to method.
New York Holt, Rinehart & Winston, Inc.
Smith F’, Kendall L, Hulin C. (1969). The measurement ofsatisfactionin workand retirement.
Chicago: Rand McNally.
Stogdill R, Coons A (Eds.). (1957). Leader behavior: Its description and measurement
(Research Monograph No. 88). Columbus, OH: Ohio State University, Bureau of
Business Research.
Vecchio R. (1987). Situational leadership theory: An examination of a prescriptive theory.
Journal ofApplied Psychology, 72,444-451.
Yukl G. (1981). Leadership in organizations. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.