0 ratings0% found this document useful (0 votes) 73 views83 pages4 Analyzing English - Howard Jackson
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content,
claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF or read online on Scribd
Forthcoming in this series
F. AARTS and J. ARTS
English Syniactic Structures
‘An introduction to units and structures
in contemporary written English
IVAN POLDAUF
English Word Stress
ANALYZING ENGLISH
An introduction to descriptive linguistics
HOWARD JACKSON
City of Birmingham Polytechnic
SECOND EDITION
©
PERGAMON PRESS
‘Oxford - New York » Toronto « Sydney + Paris » Frankfurtux.
usa,
CANADA
AUSTRALIA
FRANCE
FEDERAL REPUBLIC
OF GERMANY
Pereamon Press Lt
(OXS OW, Fagland
Pergamon Pres ne, Maxwell House, Faivew Pas
imford, Now York 10523, U.S.A.
Pergamon Press Canada Lid, Suite 104, 180 Consumers
Road, Willoweele, Ontario MAY 1P9, Canada
Pergamon Press (Aust) Pi. Lid, P.O. Box SH, Pts
Point, N°.W. 2011; Austin
Peigamon Press SARL, 24 rue des Beoes, 75240 Pats,
(Codex 05, France
Pergamon Press GmbH, 6242. Kronberp-Taunus,
ammerwet 6, Federal Republic of Germany
Headington Hit Hall, Oxford
‘Copyright © 1982 Pergnon Press Lid
AIL righ eserved No part ofthis publication may be
dnl edn feral eetinor aed
‘ny form or by any meant: electron, leona
imac wpe, chanical phoning, reading or
‘herwse, wou permission in hing ro the pub
her
Fit eiton 1960
Reprine1982
Sezond eon 1982
LUbrary of Congres Cataloging in Publication Data
Jackson, Howard,
‘Analying English,
(Language courses)
Tes inde
1 English anguape-Grammar-1950 1. Tle, Series.
PELIGIE RD 2)B1-23834 AACE
rks Library Cataloguing in Publieation Data
Jackson, Howard
nalyzing English; an introduction to descriptive
igus 2nd ed (Language courses)
1 Engi rege
Le I
fn PEL
ISBN 0-08-025567-4
Printed nGreat Briain by A. Wheaton & Co. Lit, Exeter
for HilaryContents
Acknowledgements
Introduction
Part One: Sounds
Making sounds
English consonants
English vowels
Sounds and their variants
Sounds in connected speech
Combining sounds
intonation
Part Two: Structures
8, Grammar and sentences,
9. Word classes
10. Phrases 1—noun phrases
1. Phrases 2—verb phrases
12. Phrases 3—adjective phrases, adverb phrases, prepositional phrases
13, Clauses 1—strueture and types
Jd. Clauses 2—dependent clauses
\Fext 1—rearranging items
fext 2—cohesion
Part Three: Words
17. Morphemes
18. English morphology
19. Dictionary information
20. ‘Grammar’ of words
21. Relations between words
Conclusion and further reading
Key to exercises
International Phonetic Alphabet
Index
2
2
2
48
35
n
8
83
9
102
109
4
19
2s
136
138
Ist
153Acknowledgements
This book came into being over a number of years in the course of teaching the
analysis of the modern English language (o students on the English degree at
Birmingham Polytechnic, Like many students of English coming to study for a
dogree which includes a fair proportion of linguistic work, after the purely literature
courses they have pursued at. school they find the ways of thinking and the
analytical approach required for this work at first often new and strange. I am
{grateful to all those students who bore with my attempts to find the most
satisfactory way of introducing the modern linguistic study of English, 1 would also
like to thank Vaughan James of the Pergamon Institute of English for his help and
advice in the preparation of the manuseript for publication. And owe a debt, asin
many other things, to my wife
Hy
Introduction
Many people associate the term ‘linguistics’ with the philosophy of language
(Wittgenstcin and the like), with learning foreign languages, or with abstract,
‘mathematical symbolism supposedly charting the language that we speak and write.
‘Now linguisties does have something to do with all these three activities, and the
third of them in particular occupies the attention and energies of many scholars
‘working in the field of linguistics. Perhaps the most well-known, but by no means
only, scholar falling into this category is Noam Chomsky, who initiated the
“transformational generative’ school of linguistics.
Such scholars are often referred to as “theoretical linguists, Their aim is to
construct a unified model or theory to ‘explain’ the nature of individual languages
and of human language in general. Like all theoretical sciences, theoretical
linguistics aims to make as explicit as possible its axioms and procedures, so that its
hypotheses and theories can be adequately tested. The explicitness required
demands a measure of formalism, and for this theoretical linguistics looks, like
fther sciences, to mathematics and symbolic logic. This makes many linguisties
books daunting to the uninitiated and difficult for the layman to gain access to,
‘Much modern linguistics over the past 25 years has been of this kind, often
concentrating on individual problems of description at a quite abstract level,
However, not all linguistics is like that. More important, in the opinion of this
author, and complementary to the kind of approach outlined in the previous
paragraph is a branch of linguistics referred to as ‘descriptive’ linguistics. This
‘approach starts with a language as it is spoken and written, withthe data of actually
occurring utterances and sentences, and seeks to analyze, catalogue and describe
‘that data and that language. Descriptive linguistics is about describing languages
rather than about constructing theories and models. It must, of course, use
categories of description, but these arise inductively from a consideration of the
language data rather than deductively from the axioms of a theory.
Jn many ways, descriptive linguistics isthe successor to ‘traditional grammas’, as it
used to be taught in schools. And it is ironic that, while in recent years great
advances have been made in the description of contemporary English, the
education system in Britain no longer demands that pupils should be acquainted
with the nature and structure of their mother tongue. In the seventies a number of
significant reference works have been published, distilling the latest knowledge
about the English language, among them the Grammar of contemporary English by
R Quitk, § Greenbaum, G Leech and J Svartvik, published by Longman in 1972.
‘This present book is aimed at those wishing to discover the techniques and insights
Of linguistics as applied to the description of the contemporary English language.
‘Such persons might be students in further or higher education undertaking a coursexX Introduetion
in linguistics oF English language, without any school background in language
analysis. Or they might be teachers or advanced learners of English as a Foreign
Language, secking the beginnings of a systematic linguistic description of modern
English. Or they might be interested lay people wishing to know what this still
relatively new science of linguistics has to say about the nature of the language they
speak. ‘i
‘The book is divided into three sections, dealing with Sound (ie pronunciation),
‘Structures (je grammar) and Words (ie vocabulary and meaning). The terminology
used is, much of it, derived from traditional grammar, and it relates directly to that
found in A Grammar of contemporary English and similar works. Each chapter
presents a framework of analysis for the point being discussed, and is provided with
fn exercise, to give practice in applying the analytical techniques presented in the
chapter. Solutions to the exercises are given in a Key at the end. The aim of the
ook is 10 show how linguistic analysis and description is done and to lead the
reader to make his own analyses. It is intended that the book may be used
seltinstructionally, or i could be used by a teacher. And it is hoped that after
working through the book the reader will then be able to make practical analyses of
any English sentence or text himself—with the aid of the reference works cited in
the Conclusion,
Part One: Sounds|
|
1. Making sounds
Speaking and writing
‘We can transmit our language in two ways: either by speaking or by writing. We are
all conscious of what writing invalves: the use of a pen or pencil in our hands, the
‘making of special marks (letters) on paper. But we are not at all conscious of what
speaking involves, It involves sound coming out of our mouths. But how is that
sound produced? And how does it relate to the letters that we write? Afterall itis
the same language, whether we speak it or write it
‘The reason we are more conscious of writing is because we had to be taught how to
write, how to hold the pencil correctly, how to shape the letters so that they could
be recognized hy someone else, But we learned to speak on our mother’s knee.
[No one told us how to make the right sounds, what to do with our tongues or our
lips. All natural languages have been transmitted by speech, but not all by writing.
‘And all normal healthy people have learned to speak their language in childhood,
but not all have Ieamed to write, In this sense speech is prior to writing and
characteristic of us as human beings.
When we speak we use over half of our bodies to do so, from the diaphragm,
situated below the lungs, to the mouth and nose in our faces. Speech is quite simply
‘column of air, that originates in the lungs, and is modified in various ways before
its passage through the lips, and s0 out of the mouth and into the air. Like any other
sound, speech is the vibration of the the air to make sound waves. In this case the,
vibration, and the characteristics of the sound waves are determined by the human
vocal organs.
‘What we have just described is the most usual basis of speech, ie egressive lung
‘ir—air originating in the lungs and passing outwards. It is also possible to speak
while breathing in, with ingressive lung air. We sometimes speak like this in
‘moments of tension, but it is not a normal way to produce speech sounds. However,
ingressive mouth air sounds, produced when drawing air into the mouth but not
into the lungs, do occur. For English speakers the tut-tut sound is made like this,
and in some languages such sounds are regularly used. Egressive mouth air sounds
also occur; for English speakers blowing a raspberry or giving a kiss are sounds of,
this kind, But for articulating the sounds of English words egressive lung air is
virtually always used in normal English speech.
‘Vocal cords
‘As air is expelled from the lungs, it passes up the windpipe (trachea) and into the
larynx (see Figure 1). In the larynx is found the glottis, the passage between the
‘vocal cords (oF vocal folds). Here the air coming from the lungs receives its first4 Analyzing English
major modification. The vocal cords, despite their name, are not to be imagined
like strings of a guitar or piano. They are a pair of fleshy, lip-like membranes, that
are hinged at the front and may be moved together or apart in order to impede or
allow the passage of air through the glottis
The action of the vocal cords (ie the state of the glottis) determines whether a
sound being produced is voiced or voicelesss.When the vocal cords are brought
close together, but not shut tightly, the air escaping from the lungs eauses them to
Figure 1: The vocal organs
vibrate: this is called voicing, and a sound produced while the vocal cords are
Vibrating is a voiced sound, When the voeal cords are apart, no vibration occurs,
and sounds produced with the glottis in this state are voiceless sounds. This is also
the state of the glottis for normal breathing, The vibratory action of the vocal cords
‘may be easily demonstrated by blocking up the ears and producing alternately an s
And ax sound. The s isa voiceless sound and the z a voiced sound, The articulation
of the latter should induce a buzzing sensation
There are two further possible states of the glottis, which are both of marginal
interest to the production of speech. One is when the vocal cords are brought
tightly together and the glottis is completely closed. We do this when we are lifting,
heavy objects, and it occurs in speech as a momentary action, in the production of
the ‘glottal stop’, familiar from the Cockney pronunciation of the t in butter. The
other state of the glottis is intermediate between vibrating and completely open:
the vocal cords are brought together, but not sufficiently for vibration to occur. This
is the state of the glottis associated with whispering,
‘Summarizing, there are four possible states of the glottis:
1, Open and relaxed, wsed for normal breathing and making voiceless sounds;
2. Vibrating, used for making voiced sounds;
3. Completely shut, used for making the glottal stop;
Making sounds 5
4. Close together but not vibrating, used for whispering.
(Only the first two are of importance in the description of speech sounds, giving us
the categories of voiceless and voiced sounds. A speech sound must belong to one or
the other of these categories, and this is determined by the state ofthe glottis at the
time of its production.
(Oral and nasal sounds
As the column of air passes beyond the glottis, through the pharynx, there are two
possible ways of escape from the face: either through the mouth, or through the
nose. At the back of the mouth, as an extension of the root, isthe soft palate, oF
volum. This is under muscular control and may at any time be in one of two
positions: either raised, or lowered, Ifthe soft palate is raised, the passage through
the nose is blocked and air may escape only through the mouth. Ifthe soft palate is.
lowered, the passage through the nose is free, and air may escape both through the
nose and through the mouth, Sounds produced with the soft palate lowered are
nnasal or nasalized sounds. Sounds produced with the soft palate raised and air
escaping only through the mouth are called oral sounds. Indeed we assume that
sounds are oral unless we use the term nasal or nasalized. The difference between
oral and nasal sounds may be illustrated from the word sudden. In a normal, fairly
rapid pronunciation of this word, the final two sounds are da. The only respect in
Which these sounds differ from each other is that d is an oral sound and n is a nasal
sound. If you monitor your pronunciation carefully you should be able to feel the
action of the soft palate in the transition from d to n, i from a raised to a lowered
position
‘The function of the soft palate is, then, either to allow or not to allow the passage of
air through the nose. When it is lowered it does not restrict the escape of air
through the mouth. However the eseape of air through the mouth may be restricted
in other ways, and air may be escaping only through the nose. For example, in the
pronunciation of m, air cannot escape from the mouth because the lips are tightly
shut, so that the escape of air is only through the nasal cavity.
The most complex modifications of the column of air that originated in the lungs
take place in the mouth. Here we are concemed with two aspects of articulation:
firstly, the manner of articulation; and secondly the place of articulation, The
‘manner of articulation refers to the way in which a sound is made, and the place
refers tothe position in the mouth at which the sound is made. A number of sounds
made in different ways may be made at the same position,
‘Manner of articulation
With manner of articulation we frst of all make a broad distinction between sounds
that are produced without any obstruction in the mouth, and those that are made
with some kind of obstruction, The former we call vowels and the latter consonants.
‘The differing quality of vowel sounds is determined by the openness of the mouth,
the configuration of the tongue and the shape of the lips. The quality of consonant6 Analyzing English
sounds is determined by the kind of obstruction to the passage of air (or elasure), 8
well as by the place of articulation.
igure 2: Near closure fors
Complete closure
‘A umber of kinds of closure may be identified. First there is the complete closure,
when a total obstruction is made to the flow of air. When the soft palate is raised
and air is prevented from escaping through the nose, air-pressure will build up
behind the obstruction, and may then be released with an explosion. Sounds
produced in this way are called plosive sounds (or sometimes stops). Such a sound
in English isthe intial b in bin, Here the complete closure is made by the two lips.
Obviously the closure, the build up of air-pressure, and the plosion are momentary
activities; but they are all three stages in the articulation of plosive sounds. If, with a
complete closure, the soft palate is lowered, then air may escape through the nose.
Sounds made in this way are called nasal sounds, eg m in English man, where the
closure is again made by the lips.
Figure 3: Complete closure for g
Near closure
‘A second kind is a near-closure. Here the air is allowed to escape, but not freely.
‘The escaping air causes friction at the point of near-closure, and the sounds
produced in this way are called ftieative sounds. Such a sound in English is the
initial v of van, Here the near-closure is between the bottom lip and the top front
teeth, with friction resulting.
Laterals
‘A third kind is made with the front of the tongue forming a complete closure, but
With the air being allowed to escape over the sides of the tongue, Sounds produced
in this way are called lateral sounds, since the air escapes laterally. Now, the air
Making sounds 7
escape may be completely free, as in the case of the initial 1 of English lid, or the
sides of the tongue may be raised, so that air escapes only with friction, as in the
initial 11 of Welsh Lian-, This latter sound is called a lateral fricative.
Intermittent closure
A fourth kind is an intermittent closure, In making sounds with an intermittent
closure, « complete closure is made very quickly and repeated several times. Such a
sound isthe rolled r, sometimes heard with an emphatic (or Scottish) pronunciation
‘of F in, for example, red. Here the intermittent closure is between the tip of the
tongue and the back of the upper front teeth. An intermittent closure may,
however, involve only one such closure, in which case itis called a Napped sound,
sometimes heard when F occurs between vowels eg in very.
Near closures without friction
‘A fifth and final kind is a near-closure, like the second kind, but without friction.
The articulators come close together, but not close enough for friction to occur.
‘These sounds are called frietionless continuant sounds. Such a sound isthe intial w
in wer. Here the near-closure occurs between the two lips. These sounds could be
classed as vowels, since they are made without any obstruction to the air-flow
through the mouth, but they are usually regarded as consonants, at least as far as
English is concerned, because they act like consonants in all other respects,
especially in syllable and word structure.
These, then, are the categories that we use to describe the manner of articulation of
sounds. There is first ofall the broad distinction between vowels and consonants,
‘and then among consonants we distinguish between plosives, nasals, fricatives,
laterals, rolls and flaps, and frictionless continuants.
Place of articulation
Now let us consider the place of articulation of sounds. From what has been said
about the distinction between vowels and consonants, it will be clear that
‘consonants are easier to describe for place of articulation than vowels, since it is
possible to feel where the obstruction is taking place in the mouth. Nevertheless,
‘vowel sounds may be described in terms of their place of articulation
Vowels
‘wo sets of categories are relevant to this description. The first involves the
‘openness of the mouth and height of the tongue, for which the categories are close,
half-close, half-open, open (an alternative corresponding set of categories is also
sometimes used: high, mid-high, mid-low, low). The second involves the general
configuration the tongue and the area of the mouth in which the sound is made,
the categories here are front, eentral, back. A third set of categories i relevant to
the description of vowel sounds, involving the shape ofthe lips; the categories are8 Analyzing English
‘rounded and spread (also referred to as unrounded). Ilustrating from English, the
vowel sound in beat, represented by the letters ea, isa close front vowel with spread
lips; the vowel sound in boot, represented by the letters 09, is a close back vowel
with rounded lips
Figure 4 Vowel Chart showing and 00
close ST Ty .
WG
|
5, are involved,
In making consonant sounds, two parts of the mouth, or articulators,
For the majority, one of these articulator isthe tongue, oF atleast some part of the
tongue. Since this is so, the label for tongue, (from Latin lingua), is not usually
included in the description of the place of articulation of the sound.
Consonants
We will begin our review of places of articulation at the front of the mouth. The
Figure 6: Labio.dental f
Jace involves the two lips as articulators, and sounds made here are called
bilabial sounds (from Latin labium). They may be illustrated trom the initial b in
bit, which is bilabial plosive, or the initial m in men, which is a bilabial nasal. At
the next place of articulation the bottom lip makes a closure with the top front
teeth; these sounds are called tabio-dental sounds. In English the inital fof fun isa
labio-dental fricaive
‘The other consonant sounds all involve the tongue as one of the articulator. First,
consonants are made by the articulation of the tip of the tongue and the back of the
"upper front teeth; these are called dental sounds (from Latin dens). In English the
final sound in teeth, represented by the letters th, isa dental fricative, Next, a sound
‘Making sounds 9
is made by the articulation of the tongue and the bony ridge just behind the upper
front teeth. The part of the tongue used is that just behind the tip, called the blade,
Figure 7: Dental Figure &: Alveolar d
and the bony ridge is called the alveolar ridge, which
‘made in this position —alveolar sounds. In English the initial d of din is an alveolar
plosive. The next area of the roof of the mouth, behind the alveolar ridge, is called
the hard palate. An articulation occurs between the hard palate and the part of the
Figure 9: Palatal y
tongue behind the blade, called the front of the tongue. These sounds are called
palatal sounds; in English the intial sound of yes, represented by the letter y, is a
palatal frictionless continua.
If you run your tongue over the roof of your mouth you will feel the hard palate
being replaced by a soft area, called the soft palate or velum. There is an
articulation between this area and the back of the tongue, making velar sounds: in
English the intial g of ge isa velar plosive. The velum extends into the uvula, the
piece of flesh that you can see dangling atthe back of your mouth when you look in,
mirror. The uvula articulates with the back of the tongue to produce uvular
sounds. In French the sound is often uvular roll, with the uvula making an
intermittent elosure against the back of the tongue. Moving further back in the
‘mouth we come to the pharynx; itis possible for an articulation to occur between
the back or root of the tongue and the pharynx, giving a pharyngeal sound. No such10 Analyzing English
sounds ovcur in English or the European languages; they are typical, however, of
‘Arabic, Proceeding beyond the pharynx we come to the glottis, where the position
Figure 10: Velarg
of the vocal cords may produce glottal sounds, We have mentioned the production
of the glottal stop (the Cockney pronunciation of the tt in butter) by means of a
complete closure in the glottis.
From this description of the possible places of articulation for speech sounds it will
bbe clear that for purposes of description, the tongue is divided into a number of
identifiable areas as is also the roof of the mouth. Beginning at the front end, the
Figure
arts ofthe tongue and root of the mouth
Team 7
parts of the tongue that are distinguished are: tip, blade, front, back, root. The
areas of the roof of the mouth are alveolar ridge, (hard) palate, yelum (ie soft
palate), uvula,
Deseribing speech sounds
In deseribing speech sounds there is a convention well worth following. When
labelling a vowel sound, give first of all the category for height, then the one for the
general area of the mouth, and then the one for the position of the lips. Thus, the
Vowel of heat is described as a close front spread vowel, Vowels are assumed to be
Making sounds 11
voiced, ie articulated with the voeal cords vibrating, since this is predominantly the
‘case. And in English, at any rate there is no regular distinction to be made between
voiced and voiceless vowels. In labelling consonant sounds however, voicing is
portant and is given frst, then the place of articulation, and finally the manner of
articulation. Thus, the b of beat is described asa voiced bilabial plosive, and the fof
fun as a voiceless labio-dental fricative.
International Phonetic Alphabet
It will have become clear that the Roman alphabet, which we use for writing
English, is by no means adequate for representing the diversity of speech sounds
that we use for speaking English. The inadequacy becomes evident if we reflec that
thete ate over 40 distinguishable speech sounds in English, while our alphabet
contains only 26 symbols. To make the point more concrete, consider that we have
{0 use a pair of letters (th) to represent the single voiceless dental fricative sound at
the end of teesh, Moreover, this same pair of letters also serves to represent the
voiced dental fricative sound, as found at the beginning of shen. So in representing
speech we make use of the International Phonetic Alphabet, developed by the
International Phonetic Association. This alphabet is based on the Roman alphabet,
but with the addition of symbols from other sources. We shall be introducing the
symbols that are necessary for representing English speech sounds in the following
chapters, and the complete alphabet is given on page 151.
Exercise 1
‘Make a, description of the following speech sounds in the way recommended eg the b of
beat—voiced bilabial plosive; the en of bea—close front spread vowel
1 the € of beat
2. tho v of van
3. the k of kiss
4 the th of thin
5. the mof now
6
7
8
the a of bath
the p of pin
the €e of seed
the 0 of bole
10. the Lof like
11, the g of go
12, the # of 002. English consonants
In the previous chapter we defined consonants as those speech sounds which are
produced with some kind of closure in the mouth, restricting the escape of air. And
‘we distinguished among consonants according to the place in the mouth that the
closure occurs (place of articulation) and according to the kind of closure made
(manner of articulation). These factors, along with the state ofthe glottis (voicing),
determine the kind of consonant sound that is produced. We shall now take each of
these classes of consonants in turn, discover which sounds are used in English, and
introduce the phonetic symbol for each sound from the International Phonetic
Alphabet.
Fricatives
The largest group of consonants in English comprises the fricatives, sounds that
involve a near-closure with friction resulting between the articulators, In most cases
there is a voiced and a voiceless fricative occurring at each place of articulation.
Bilabial fricatives, where the friction occurs between the two lips, have symbols
taken from the Greek alphabet: for the voiceless bilabial fricative the symbol is,
igure 12: Bilabialiative
and for the voiced bilabial fricative 8, These sounds do not regularly oceur in
hglish, except in certain contexts as variant sounds for labio-dental fricatives (see
Chapter 5).
‘The labio-dental fricatives have the familiar symbols f and v for the voiceless and
voiced variety respectively. Friction occurs between the lower lip and the upper
front teeth. These sounds occur regularly in English words, in all positions, eg
initially in feignivain; medially in referee(reverie; finally in lifelive. When the
phonetic symbol is a familiar one, as in this ease, it should not be assumed that
English consonants 18
when the corresponding letter occurs it always represents the sound that the
phonetic symbol stands for. So, in the case of the letter f for example, the word
‘written of is in fact pronounced with a voiced labio-dental fricative as the final
sound, ie represented by the phonetic symbol .
Figure 13: Dental fricative
In producing dental friatives the friction occurs between the tongue tip and the
back of the front teeth. For some speakers the tongue tip actually protrudes
between top and bottom teeth, and these sounds are sometimes called interdental
fricatives. One symbol is taken from the Greek alphabet: for the voiceless dental
fricative the symbol is; and for the voiced dental fricative—8, which is a specially
invented symbol. These sounds occur in all positions in English words, eg thiefthis;
lethaltleather; clothiclothe.
Iveolar frgative
‘The alveolar fricatives have familiar symbols, s for the voiceless alveolar fricative,
and z for the voiced alveolar fricative. Again, it should be noted that not all
‘occurrences of the letter $ are voiceless alveolar fricaives, ie symbolized phoneti-
cally by s; eg rise has a voiced alveolar fricative as its final sound, represented
phonetically by 7. With alveolar fricatives the friction is between the tongue tip or
blade and the alveolar ridge. But they differ from dental fricatives not only in place
Of articulation, but also in the shape that the tongue takes up in the articulation of
the sound. With alveolar fricatives in English the tongue is shaped so that there is a
groove down the centre line along which the air passes, whereas with dental
fricatives the tongue is relatively flat with only a narrow slit. So s and 2 are groove
fricatives in English, while @ andO are slit ficatives. The alveolar fricatives occur in14 Analyzing English
all positions in English words, although x is not common initially: seal/zeal,
racerirazor, lacellaze.
Figure 15: Palao-alveolar fictive
Further back in the mouth we come to the pair of palato-alveolar frieatives. Friction
occurs between the tongue front and the hard palate, but at the same time the
tongue tip is raised towards the alveolar ridge. These sounds are symbolized by J
for the voiceless palato-alveolar fricative, and 3 for the voiced palato-alveolar
fricative. They occur in all postions in English words, eg, ship/gigolo,fssion!vision,
rushirouge. But the voiced palato-alveola fricaive is of very restricted occurrence
initially and finally, found only in words borrowed from French; and there is a
tendency in some people's speech to replace the voiced palato-alveotar frieative in
these positions with the voiced affricate (see below).
Palatal fricatives do not occur in English. Of the velar frieatives only the voiceless
‘member occurs, and then in only a few words borrowed from other languages. The
velar fricative is produced with friction between the tongue back and the soft
palate, and the voiceless member is symbolized by x. It occurs in the Scottish
pronunciation of lack, and in some German names, eg of the composer Bach,
‘The only other fricative to note in English is the glottal fricative, which occurs only
as-a voiceless sound, It is represented by the symbol h. This consonant occurs
mainly initially, although it is also sometimes found medially, eg headlahead,
huiltuphill. [is sometimes discounted as a consonant by phoneticians of English. In
articulating the h the mouth takes up a configuration for the following vowel sound,
‘and for this reason it is considered to be a voiceless onset for the vowel which
follows. But for practical recognition and transcription purposes it is useful to
regard it as @ separate sound, and we shall call it a voiceless glottal fricative,
symbolized by h.
Figure 16: Velar fricative
English consonants 15
Plosives
Prosive consonants in English involve a complete closure in the mouth, a raised
velum preventing escape of sir through the nasal cavity, and plosion after the
release of the closure.
Figure 17: Bilabial plosive
Bilabial plosives occur commonly in English and are represented by the familiar
symbols p and b, for the voiceless bilabial plosive and the voiced bilabial plosive
respectively. They are found in all positions in English words, eg pearibear,
chapelicable, ropeirobe.
Figure 18 Alveolar plosive Figure 19: Vela plosive
‘The alveolar plosives are made with a closure between the tongue blade, or
sometimes tongue tip, and the alveolar ridge. They are represented by the familiar
symbols t and d, for voiceless alveolar plosive and voiced alveolar plosive
respectively, They occur frequently in all positions in English words, eg trainidrain,
writeririder, matlmad.
‘A final pair of plosives in English is made at the velar place of articulation,
involving a closure between the tongue back and the solt palate. The voiceless velar
plosive is symbolized by k and the voiced velar plosive by g. Like the other plosives
they are found commonly in all positions in English words, eg cane/gain, pack-
agelbaggage, rickirig.16 Analyzing English
In many cases the voiceless plosives in English are articulated with aspiration
‘accompanying the release phase of pronunciation. This means that when plosion
‘occurs, a puff of air accompanies it, So we talk about aspirated and unaspirated
‘voiceless plosives, and aspirated voiceless plosives are symbolized pt, th, KS. The
aspiration of voiceless plosives in English isa more-or-less phenomenon rather than
an either-or one; that isto say, there are a numbar of degrees of aspiration rather
than aspiration versus non-aspiration. Strong aspiration is typical when voiceless
plosives occur initially, and a complete absence of aspiration typically occurs when
voiceless plosives follow s, cfpin/spin,un(sin,Rilliskill. A lighter aspiration is often
present when voiceless plosives occur medially or finally, eg happerthope, latter!
le, packeripack.
There is one further plosive that we should include—the glottal plosive, or glottal
stop, that we mentioned in Chapter 1. [tis not really a plosiv, in that no observable
plosion occurs; hence ‘stop’, the alternative name for plosive, seems more
“appropriate inthis instance. Only a voiceless glottal plosive occurs; its represented
by the symbol 9. Apart from its widespread use in Cockney and some other accents,
its oceurrence in English speech is restricted to special uses. It may be heard in the
pronunciation of words where two vowels follow each other but belong to separate
syllables, eg co-operate, re-action. Here a glottal stop is often used to separate the
two vowels, It may also be heard on occasions as a reinforcement of the articulation
of a final voiceless plosive, eg in leap ot leak.
ASfricates|
‘One pair of sounds in English is related to both plosives and fricatives: the
palato-alveolar affricates. The voiceless palato-alveolar aifricate may be illustrated
by the inital and final sounds in church, and the voiced by the initial and final
sounds in judge. Atfricates involve a complete closure, as for plosives, but the
release phase is not with plosion but with friction, In the case of the palato-alveolar
affricates the closure is made with the tongue blade and front at the alveolar ridge
and hard palate area, and the release is by means of a palato-alveolar fricative.
‘These sounds are symbolized by t{ for the voiceless palato-alveolaraffricate, and by
Figure 20; Palato-alveolar afreate
4 for the voiced palato-alveolar allricate. They occur in all positions in English
words, eg chinigin, richesiridges, lunchilunge.
English consonants 17
Nasal
Like plosives, nasals involve a complete closure in the mouth; but unlike for
plosives, the soft palate is lowered so that air may escape through the nose. Nasals,
Unlike plosives, but like fricatives, are continuant sounds. This means that their
articulation may continue for as long as the person speaking can find breath:
compare mmm, ff, p. In English, nasal consonants are normally voiced; voiceless
nasals do sometimes occur in some contexts, but they are then variants of normal
voiced nasals, and may be ignored for our purposes.
[English has a bilabial nasal, represented by the familiar symbol m, which occurs in
all positions in English words, eg mind, limit, rhyme, ‘The labio-dental nasal,
involving @ closure between bottom lip and upper teeth, is represented by the
‘symbol 1). This isnot a regular sound in English, occurring only as a variant of the
bilabial m or the alveolar nasal n before labio-dental fricatives, eg in symphony and
in infant. And then it occurs really only in fairly rapid speech, It will be mentioned
again in Chapter 5.
|As already implied, English has an alveolar nasal, represented by the familiar
symbol n, which occurs in all positions in English words, eg nigh, lantern, line.
Finally, English has a velar nasal, involving a closure between the back of the
tongue and the soft palate, It is reprosented by the symbol n, and is of restricted
occurrence, never being found initially. It occurs frequently before a velar plosive,
and in some accents of English, eg in the Midlands, itis always followed by a velar
plosive. In other accents it may occur finally as well as medially, eg linger, fongue,
singer.
Laterals
Like nasals, the lateral consonants are also continuant sounds: there is a complete
closure, but air escapes over the side of the tongue. In English laterals are normally
voiced, although there is a voiceless variant that regularly occurs in a particular
context and which will be discussed later (Chapter 4), The only lateral occurring in
English isthe alveolar lateral, represented by the familiar symbol |. Itis found in all
positions in English words, eg lav, filler, rail. In fact, there is a marked difference in
{quality between the alveolar lateral in inital position and that in final position. The
difference arises from the configuration of the body of the tongue in the mouth: for
the alveolar lateral in final position, the back of the tongue is raised towards the soft
palate, and it is said to be velarized, The velarized alveolar lateralis symbolized by
‘igure 21: Alveolar letral ~ non-velarized and velarized18 Analyzing English
compare leafifel, lateiil. The details of the contexts in which the two varieties
‘occur will be discussed in Chapter 4.
Frictionless continuants
Last in our review of English consonants we.come to the class of frictionless
‘continuants, the sounds involving a near-closure in the mouth but without friction,
‘The normal English r sound is a post-alveolar fritionless continuant: a near-closure
is made between the tongue blade and the area just behind the alveolar ridge. It is
represented in the Intemational Phonetic Alphabet by the symbol J, but for
purposes of broad transcription (see Chapter 4) it may be more convenient to use
the usual orthographic symbol. It is of restricted occurrence in English, not
normally being found in final position eg run, free, furrow. The orthographic
symbol appears frequently in final position in English spelling, but careful
‘observation will reveal that in British English itis not normally pronounced in that
position, eg car, more, bear, farm, although it is pronounced here in many
‘American aecents. Other kinds ofr sound are also found in English speech and will
be discussed later (Chapter 4). The postalveolar frictionless continuant is reckoned
to be the normal English sound. It is usually voiced, although a voiceless variant
does occur in some contexts (see Chapter 4),
‘Two further sounds are included among the frictionless continuants, although they
are sometimes also called semi-vowels. They have similarity with particular vowel
sounds of English, and were it not for the fact that they pattern like consonants in
the structure of English words and syllables, they would be counted as these vowels,
One is the bilabial frictionless continuant, represented by the familiar symbol ,
which is similar to the elose back rounded vowel, as in boot. This occurs in initial
and medial positions, eg wet, cower. But in final positions its occurrence in spelling
is usually considered to be a close back rounded vowel.
‘The other vowel-like consonant sound is the palatal frictionless continuant, where
the near-closure is between the tongue front and the hard palate. It is represented
by the symbol j, and it is similar to a close front spread vowel as in beat. The
‘occurrence of the palatal frictionless continuant is restricted: it is found in initial
position, and in initial consonant combinations after certain consonants, ¢& ye,
beauty (bj), queve (kj). Both these frictionless continuants are normally voiced,
By way of summary the consonants of English are set out in a chart below. Place of
articulation is indicated across the top of the chart, and manner of articulation
down the left-hand side. If a particular kind of consonant has a voiceless and a
voiced variant, then two symbols appear in the relevant box in the order voiceless
followed by voiced. Consonants that occur only as variants of other regularly
‘occurring sounds have been omitted from the chart
English consonants 19
‘igure 22: English consonants
] a) 3 ay |
al ails atl y A
| 3 | 2 se) s]a}é
als eli] alii
fv | @6 | sz ss | b
pe ta ke
Afriate v4
Nal = [el] 3
Tatra [a
Frionles | wy 1 fi
Continuant | _ _
Exercise?
‘Transcribe the following English words in the symbols of the International Phonetic
Alphabet charted in Figure 22 to represent their pronunciation. Since every word has a
‘owel sound, # number of vowel symbols will b needed. The following vowels have been
{sed in the words listed: | as peep, asin pit a8 in bet, anda asin the educated southern
[English pronunciation of pat
cf then Sen; jar dg; lick hk
1, shed 2 teeth 3. chart
4. detest 5. jet 6. guard
7. these 8 barge 9. vase
10, chet 11 peach 12, thieve
1B. yeast 14, green 15, Mars
16. wealth 17. beet 18, wrench
19. crease 20. charge 21, shriek
2. fling 23. stink 24. yield3. English vowels
In Chapter 1 we defined vowels as sounds which are made without any kind of
closure or impediment to the escape of air through the mouth. Because there is no
contact oF near-contact of articulators, vowel sounds are more difficult to deseribe
than are consonants. But, as with many consonants, the tongue is crucial in
determining the quality of vowel sounds, and it i the differing configurations of the
tongue in the mouth that cause the varying qualities,
As we mentioned in Chapter 1, there are three parameters by which we deseribe
vowel sounds: firstly, the height of the tongue or openness of the mouth; secondly,
the area of the mouth having the highest part of the tongue or the general area of
the mouth in which the vowel is made; and thirdly, the shape of the lips. The frst of
these gives us the categories lose, half-close, half-open and open; the second the
‘categories front, central, and back; and the third the categories rounded and
spread. Vowel sounds are, of course, normally voiced, so that voicing is not relevant
for their description.
Before we look at the English vowels in detail we should mention a broad
distinction to be made among vowel sounds between pure vowels and diphthongs.
‘This is particularly relevant for English, since the proportion of diphthongs is
unusually large. Pure vowels are made with the mouth taking up a single position
during the articulation. In the ease of diphthongs the configuration of the mouth
changes in the course of the articulation of the vowel sound. As we shall see,
«diphthongs can be deseribed in terms of the pure vowel from which the articulation
starts, and the pure vowel in whose direction the articulation moves.
Pure vowels
First of all, we shall consider the pure vowels. In English, pure vowels are made in
all three areas of the mouth—front, central and back
Front vowels
Inthe front area English has four vowels; all are made with spread lips. Two of the
front vowels are in the close area. One is the lose front spread vowel found in beat
or sheep and symbolized by i, so these words would be transeribed phonetically as
‘biv and sip. The other is a more open and a more central close front vowel,
usually described as a lowered and centralized close front spread vowel. It is
represented by the symbol 1, and it occurs in bir /bit) and ship IS
English has no vowel at precisely either the half-close or the halfopen position, but
‘one mid way between the two. This is usually described as a mid front spread vowel
It is sometimes symbolized by ¢, which is the International Phonetic Alphabet
English vowels 21
symbol for a half-close front spread vowel, but itis more usually symbolized by €,
the IPA symbol for a half-open front spread vowel, and this is the symbol we shall
be using. This vowel occurs in ber /bet! and jeich ite
‘The other English front vowel isin the open area, but it is not completely open: itis
‘somewhere between half-open and open, although itis usually described as an open
front spread vowel. However, the IPA symbol for an open front spread vowel (a) is,
not used, but rather the symbol z. It occurs in bat bet! and catch Jka}
Back vowels
In the back area of the mouth we can recognize five vowels in English. Four of them
‘are made with rounded lips and one with spread lips, There are also two vowels in
English that fallin the close back area, One is the elose back rounded vowel, as
found in boot and too! and symbolized by u, so these words will be transcribed
phonetically as /but/ and /tul. The other is a more open and more central variety,
usually described as a lowered and centralized close back rounded vowel. It is
represented by the symbol v and occurs in (Southern British pronunciations of)
‘book Sook! and put (pov.
{AS in the case of front vowels, English has no vowels at precisely half-lose and.
hhalf-open back positions. There is one vowel somewhere between the two,
probably nearer to half-open thn hall-close. The IPA symbol for a half-close back
rounded vowel (0) is sometimes used to symbolize it, but more usually the IPA.
symbol for a half-open back rounded vowel is used, 9, It may be described asa mid
back rounded vowel and it occurs in boughe lot and law ia!
In the open back area two vowels are found in English, one made with rounded lips,
the other with spread lips. The open back rounded vowel is, like its front
counterpart, not completely open, but somewhere between half-open and open.
Nevertheless, the IPA symbol for an open back rounded vowel is used to represent
it, p. This vowel occurs in hor /hoV and moss /mos’. The open back spread vowel is
completely open but not completely back: it tends towards the central position, The
IPA symbol for an open back spread vowel is used to represent it, and it oceurs in
farm Vom and cart Skat.
Central vowels
‘We come now to the final group of pure vowels, those made in the central area of
the mouth. There are three vowels produced in this area in English, all with spread
lips. One of these central vowels isin the open area,but like z and o in fact mid-way
between half-open and open, although it is described as an open central spread
vowel. It is represented by the IPA symbol for a half-open back spread vowel,
rnamely a, and it occurs in (the southern British pronunciation of) bur /bat! and
some isam,
The other two,central yowels in English are both mid central spread vowels, that is
mid-way between half-lose and half-open. These sounds may be illustrated by the
vowel in bird, represented by the symbol 3, and the final vowel in father,22 Analyzing English
Fepresented by the symbol 9. The latter sound is often referred to as the schwa
vowel; it occurs in English only in unstressed syllables and has a generally lax
articulation. By contrast the 2 vowel occurs in stressed syllables, and is generally
{longer in duration than the schwa vowel. They may be fusther illustrated by loser
Nusa, ahead fabed/, git igeW, first Nast.
This completes our description ofthe tel’ English pure vowels. f you do not
speak with a southern Brilsh accent, you may have found that some. the
ilstatons given didnot match your ptonenction ofthe word, Indeed sone
Midland and Northern speakers nay fad dilfleltt dcovera word Ih ey
speech that contains an open central spread vowel (a). The ellerenoos in Eghat
Accents ae due tn considerable extent to the vowel sounds, and fn two tee
Vowel sounds ifr among accent in their quality, for example, In wnne aceon
‘he mid back rounded vowel is considerably more open atkulation tna
described here. in caught ka! And vcondly, vowel sun difersnane accor,
in the words in which they occur; for exampley most Northemer sad some
Midlanders wil wea lowered and centrlzed clos back rounded vovel Seon,
rather than an open central spread vowel (1). Itis impossible to do justice to the
variety of British English accents in a work of this kind, not to mention American,
Australian and other accents. So, transcriptions will be given as representing
Southern British pronunciation, and the reader will be left to determine what the
transcription will be for his own accent
‘The vowel chart
So far we have characterized English vowel sounds by means of a descriptive label
such as ‘close front spread vowel’, ete. Phoneticians also use another means of
characterization: the vowel sounds are plotted on a vowel chart. This is a
fepresentation of the human mouth (in a squared out form!) and the peripheral
Points constitute a set of ideal sounds (called cardinal vowels) against which the
sounds actually occurring in a language are compared and their points plotted on
the chart. The TPA symbols are those given to the cardinal vowels. The chart of the
pure vowels of southern British English is as shown below:
Figure
nglsh vowels
_ V_\
Ha-close
iain \2]
Open
ea
English vowols 23
Diphthongs
Let us now consider the diphthongs of English, of which cight are usually
recognized. They may be divided into three groups according to the vowel towards
‘which the articulation of the diphthong moves. In the case of three diphthongs the
articulation moves towards a lowered and centralized close front spread vowel (1);
in the case of two it moves towards a lowered and centralized close back rounded
‘vowel (v); and in the case of the remaining three it moves towards the unstressed
‘mid central spread vowel (9).
1 diphthongs
‘Taking each of these groups in turn, we begin with that in which the diphthong
tends towards 1
One of the diphthongs in this group begins with a half-close front spread vowel, s0
it is represented by the symbol e1. The beginning point of this diphthong is then
more close than the articulation of the mid front spread pure vowel of English (),
‘This diphthong is found in late leit! and blame (ler.
AA second diphthong in this group begins with an open front spread vowel and is
‘represented by the symbol a1. The beginning point is more open than the
articulation of the English pure vowel described as open front spread (2). It occurs
in sight Isatt) and mice may
‘The third diphthong in this group begins with a half-open back rounded vowel; it is
sometimes represented by the symbol 21 and sometimes by the symbol ni, since the
half-open position is mid way between the English pure vowel symbolized and the
vowel symbolized 0, the former being mid way between half-close and half-open,
and the latter mid way between halfopen and open. We shall be using the latter
symbol, 01. This diphthong occurs in soil soil and boy Show,
v diphthongs
‘The second group of diphthongs comprises those tending towards v
‘The first in this group starts with the unstressed mid central spread vowel and is
represented by the symbol 9v. This diphthong occurs in home /hovmy/ and in boat
Ibavu.
‘The other in this group starts with an open central spread vowel, a vowel which
slightly more central than the English pure vowel described as open back spread;
and in fact the symbol used for this pure vowel is also used in the symbolization of
the diphthong, av. This occurs in house /havs! and ow! fav).
{It will be noticéd that in making both the diphthongs of this group, not only is a
cchange in the shape and height of the tongue involved, but also a change in the
shape of the lips, moving from a spread vowel to a rounded one.24 Analyzing English
2 diphthongs
‘The third group of diphthongs tends towards the mid central spread vowel 0
First there is a diphthong beginning with the lowered and centralized close front
spread vowel and represented by the symbol 19. This occurs in fierce ifs! and clear
kl. "
‘A second in this group begins with a half-open front spread vowel, that is with a
‘vowel more open than the pure vowel described as mid front spread. The symbol of
this pure vowel is used in the diphthong and is the LPA symbol for a half-open front
spread vowel. So the diphthong is represented by the symbole and occurs in scarce
Iskeos! and fair (feo.
‘The remaining diphthong of this group begins with a lowered and centralized close
back rounded vowel and is represented by the symbol v2. It occurs in cruel /krval/
and pure Ij.
‘The eight diphthongs of English may be plotted on a vowel chart:
Figure 24: English diphthongs
Front Central Back
se
Hale-close
Hattopen
Open
xereise 3
“Make a transcription ofthe pronunciation ofthe following English words, using the symbols
iscussed. The Key will give # representation of a southern British pronunciation; you may
ike t0 also consider what the transcription should be in your own accent
tg humble tambo, lathe ile, badge Soa!
1. caught 2. another 3. faint
4. sock 5. bag © cover
7 tam 8 fuel 9. count
410. look 11. catching 12. te
413. large 14. bee 415. dozen
16. dirty 17. share 18. goal
19. food 20. flesh 21, fear
22, fall 23, morning 24, pleasee
25. confess 26, moist 27. plain
28. salt 29. lurking 30, climb
| oGtnctine Gotures 2
(oo Demers 4 Farmer)
English vowels 25
‘Summary
‘Asa summary of the last chapter and the present one, here is list ofthe 44 sounds
‘of English that we-have. identified and described. Each one is given with its symbol
followed by its label.
Consonants
voiceless labio-dental fricative
voiced lahio-dental fricative
voiceless dental fricative
voiced dental fricative
voiceless alveolar fricative
voiced alveolar fricative
voiceless palato-alveolar fricative
voiced palato-alveolarfricative
voiceless glottal fricative
voiceless bilabial plosive
voiced bilabial plosive
voiceless alveolar plosive
voiced alveolar plosive
voiceless velar plosive
voiced velar plosive
voiceless palato-alveolar affricate
voiced palato-alveolar aflricate
bilabial nasal
alveolar nasal
velar nasal
alveolar lateral
bilabial frictionless continuant
post alveolar frictionless continuant
palatal frictionless continuant.
Seg -segganrateu raSN aces
Vowels
close front spread vowel
lowered and centralised close front spread vowel
mid front spread vowel
‘open front spread vowel
close back rounded vowel
lowered and centralized close back rounded vowel
‘mid back rounded vowel
‘open back rounded vowel
‘open back spread vowel
id certral spread vowel
‘unstressed mid central spread vowel
‘open central spread vowel26 Analyzing English
1 diphthong beginning with half-lose front spread vowel, moving towards
lowered and centralized close front spread vowel
1 diphthong beginning with open front spread vowel, moving towards lowered
and centralized close front spread vowel
DI diphthong beginning with half-open back rounded vowel, moving towards
lowered and centralized close front sproad vowel
90 diphthong beginning with unstressed mid central spread vowel, moving.
towards lowered and centralized close back rounded vowel
‘90 diphthong beginning with open central spread vowel, moving towards lowered
and centralized close back rounded vowel
19 diphthong beginning with lowered and centralized close front spread vowel,
moving towards unstressed mid central spread vowel
69 dipthong beginning with half-open front spread vowel, moving towards
unstressed mid central spread vowel
'v9 diphthong beginning with lowered and centralized close back rounded vowel,
‘moving towards unstressed mid contral spread vowel.
xercise 4
Make transcription of the pronunciation ofthe following English words, using the symbols
sven,
1. playground 2. Mexible 3. Chinese
4. Snudgery 5: insinuation 6 pleasurable
7. blinkers 8. understate 9. Search-warrant
10. blackmail 11. migration 12 frequency
13, robust 14. forceps 15. paranola
16. birthday 17. catedstor 18. writhing
19, cures 20. overestimate
4, Sounds and their variants —
In the preceding chapters we have in general assumed that each sound that we
perceive as a separate sound in English is transcribed by just one symbol: p sounds
are transcribed by /py, | sounds by /V, and so on, At the same time we have also
noticed that some such perceptually different sounds may, in different contexts,
have different phonetic values. We have distinguished, for example, between a
non-velarized and a velarized alveolar lateral (I and 4); and we have noticed
aspirated and unaspirated varieties of voiceless plosives (eg p and p). But in
‘transcription we have not taken account of these finer differences, although we
could have done. If we had done so, we should have produced a narrow
transcription instead of the broad one we have been making,
Broad and narrow transcription
A broad transcription is one that only takes account of the sound differences that
are important to distinguish words from each other in a language. The distinction
between [p4] and [p] does not make a difference between words in English, If we
substitute [p] for [p"] in [pln] we produce a peculiar pronunciation of pin but not a
new word; similarly, if we substitute [p] for [p] in [spin]. But the substitution of p
for t does make a difference of word: pin /piw/ and tin /tn/ are different words in
English. A narrow transcription attempts to represent more of less accurately the
‘way in which a particular speaker pronounces his words. Obviously the amount of
Phonetic detail that may be represented can vary enormously; that is to say, there
are degrees of narrowness of transcription, and a narrow transcription may be made
for the pronunciation of a group of speakers, with a particular dialect or accent.
‘This distinction is, in fact, not merely a distinction between different kinds of
transcription; its also.a distinction between different ways of looking at the sounds
of a language. When the sound system of a language is viewed as a system of units
for maintaining distinctions between units on a higher level of linguistic organiza
tion (ie words), we call the sounds phonemes. The sounds that people actually make
‘when they speak are called phones. Its part of the task of phonology (or phonetics)
to establish and describe the relationship between phones and phonemes in a
language,
Phonemes and allophones
‘The phonologist starts with phones, the accurate phonetic transcription of the
sounds that hé hears speakers making, But it soon becomes clear that not all the
distinctions made at this level are relevant for distinguishing words from each other.
Or, to put it another way, native speakers do not perceive as distinct sounds all the28 Analyzing Engi
phones that the phonologist has recorded. Several phones may, therefore, belong to
the same phoneme; or a phoneme may have a number of variants, called
allophones. In grouping phones together into phonemes, three criteria are used by
the phonologist: complementary distribution, free variation, and phonetic similar-
ity,
Complementary distribution
‘The criterion of complementary distribution states that if two or more phones occur
in non-identical environments, then they may be members of the same phoneme.
So, we find that for English the non-velarized alveolar lateral (I) occurs before
vowels, while the velarized alveolar lateral [#] occurs before consonants and at the
cends of words. Their distribution in English words is complementary; they do not
occur in the same environment, Hence these two I-phones belong to the same
phoneme. Or, we can say thatthe /V phoneme has wo allophones: (1) [1] occurring
before vowels; and (2) *] occurring before consonants and word-tinaly. From this
statement you will notice the following convention: a broad transcription ie in
terms of phonemes, is enclosed between slashed brackets / /; and a narrow
transcription, ie in terms of phones, is enclosed between square brackets }. Taking
‘contrary example, we find that the phone [p] and the phone [0] both oceur in a
umber of identical environments eg onipun, shopishor, sippinglsiuing. Their
distribution is, therefore, not complementary and they cannot be considered as
belonging to the same phoneme.
Free variation
‘The criterion of free variation states that if two or more phones occur in the same
environment, but without changing the word in which they occur, then they may
belong to the same phoneme, So, in English, a word like bid is sometimes
articulated with @ fully voiced final alveolar consonant [4]. and sometimes with @
devoiced (voiceless) final consonant, represented by [d]. But whichever phone is
used, the word is the same. These two phones, then, belong tothe same phoneme.
‘Or we can say that the phoneme /d! has two allophones when it oceurs word-finaly
() [al and (2) {J}, which are in free variation. Iti also the case that a devoiced
velar plosive [g] occurs word-finally; so that we eould hypothesize that {d) and (g]
were in free variation, But we soon realize that the substitution of one forthe other
makes a difference in the word: bid [bid] and big [big] are diferent words.
Phonetic similarity
‘These two criteria—complementary distribution and free variation—by themselves
Teave a loophole, which has to be filled by the criterion of phonetic similarity. In
English, on the basis of the criterion of complementary distribution, the voiceless
alottalfricative (h) and the voiced velar nasal (9) would be members of the same
phoneme, since [hi] occurs only word-initially and [9] occurs only word-medially
and word-finally. But, quite obviously, it is ridiculous to suppose that these two
sounds have anything in common: a glance at their descriptive labels reveals that
i
Ve
|
|
Sounds and their variants 29
they share nothing—not in terms of voicing, nor place of articulation, nor manner
of articulation. This insight is formalized in the criterion of phonetic similarity,
which states that if two or more phones are.to be members of the same phoneme
they must be phonetically similar. In practice, this means that the allophones of a
phoneme usually share at least two of voicing, place of articulation and manner of
articulation in common.
English phonemes and allophones
‘A phoneme may be defined, then, as a class of phonetically similar phones in
complementary distribution or in free variation or in both. Remembering that
transcription in terms of phones is a narrow transcription and is enclosed in square
brackets, and a transcription in terms of phonemes is @ broad transcription and is
enclosed in slashed brackets, we shall now turn to a detailed consideration of
English sounds (phonemes) and their variants (allophones)
Plosives
We begin with plosive sounds, which asa class show more variation than any other.
‘We have already noted, in Chapter 2, that voiceless plosives in English (pt, k) may:
be either aspirated (accompanied by a puff of air on release) of unaspirated. ’That is
to say, the phonemes /p,/¥ and /ki each have an aspirated allophone [p(t] and
{ki}, and an unaspirated allophone [p}, {t] and [k]. And these allophones are in
‘complementary distribution: the unaspirated allophone occurs after /s/ and the
aspirated allophone occurs in all other positions. As we pointed out in Chapter 2,
though, the degree of aspiration varies from environment to environment, There is
strong aspiration when the plosive occurs initially in a stressed syllable eg in pin, tn,
kin, Between vowels the aspiration tends to be rather weak, and may indeed be
absent altogether eg in upper, utter, sucker: we could say that we have @ ease of
allophones in free variation in this instance. And in final position the amount of
aspiration is variable, depending whether the plosive is released fully or not eg as in
sip, sit, sick.
The voiced plosives in English, d, g/ may be considered to have two allophones.
‘The main allophone, and the one which occurs in all environments, is the fully
voiced [b], [4], [f]. The other allophone occurs only in worde-final position and is &
devoiced variant (b), [0 [QI isin free variation with the fully voiced allophone,
that is, either of them may occur in this position with no particular factor
conditioning the occurrence of one rather than the other, excep forthe speakers
whim atthe time eg asin ib, id ig.
‘The difference between /p,t,k/ and /b, d,g/ in English is not merely one of voicing,
or even unequivocally one of voicing; that is, the distinetion between voiced and
voiceless. It is also a difference of aspiration. The one environment in which
unaspirated voiceless plosives regularly occur, ie after /, is the one in which voiced
plosives do not occur: there are no English words beginning with sb, /sd/ oF /s
‘There is, therefore, no contrast between voiceless and voiced plosives in English in
this environment. In all other environments (initially, finally, intervocalicall) /p,30 Analyzing English
W/ and /b, d, g/ are distinguished both by voicing and usually also by aspiration. In
fact, aspiration might be said to be more important in distinguishing /p, tk! (or
words containing them) from /b, d, g/ (or words containing them), ef pipibib, suid,
ickigig.
For this and other reasons, the series /p, ,k/ are often referred to not as ‘voiceless’
plosives, but as fortis plosives; and the /b, d, series not as ‘voiced plosives, but as
lenis plosives. The terms fortis and lenis refer to the muscular energy used in
articulation: fortis consonants are more energetically articulated than lenis conson-
ants. These terms are used not only for plosives, but also for fricatives and affricates
in English,
‘The variants of plosive that we have dealt with so far involve the whole class of,
plosives in English. We now consider variants of individual members of the class.
Besides the allophones already mentioned, the alveolar plosives each have a further
allophone. In words like widih /wd8/ and eighth fut the articulation ofthe plosive
is not alveolar, but dental: the tongue tip makes @ closure with the back of the
upper front teeth, rather than with the alveolar ridge. In other words, the
articulation of the plosive is the same here as that of the following dental fricative,
and this allophone occurs only in this environment. So /d! has an allophone [d]
before a dental fricative, and /V has an allophone [}] before a dental fricatve.
Velar plosives also have variants in addition to those already mentioned. Compare
the articulation ofthe velar plosives in the following words: keepigeese, cupleum,
ooplgoose. By careful observation you will feel thatthe contact of the tongue with
the roof of the mouth is made in slighty different position for each of the three
Pairs of words. The closure ofthe velar in the frst pair is more forward than that of
{he second pair, and the closure of the velar in the lat pair is further back than that
in the middle pai. The conditioning factor is the nature ofthe vowel that follows the
inital velar plosive: a front vowel, asin keep/kip/ or geese /eis! tends to pull the
articulation forward, and a back vowel, as in coop /Kup’ and goose igus, tends to
push the articulation back. So if we regard the articulation of the velar before
central vowels, asin eup /kapy and gum /gatn, as genuinely velar, we can call the
articulation before front yowels ‘pre-velar" and the articulation before back vowels
‘post-velar’, So the phoneme /k/ has an allophone {k] or [kt] before front vowels,
and an allophone [k] or [k#] before back vowels; and the phoneme /g/ has an
allophone [8] before front vowels and an allophone [g) before back vowels.
One further variant needs to be mentioned for plosives, When a voiceless (or fortis)
alveolar plosive /¥’ occurs in final position in a syllable, it may be articulated as a
glottal stop cg in suit So the phoneme // has allophone (?] occuring in syllable final
Position, and which isin free variation with [t)
Larerals
We have already mentioned the fact that the alveolar lateral /V in English has two
allophones, a non-velarized allophone [1], occurring before vowels and /j as in
leave, early, illuminate, and a yelarized allophone [H], occurring before consonants,
Iw! and word-finally (ie before pause), as in altogether, always, pool. These
‘Sounds and their variants 91
allophones are, then, in complementary distribution. There is, in addition, a third
allophone which isin complementary distribution with these two: after a forts (ie
voiceless) plosive /p, k/ the alveolar lateral in English is devoiced (ie voiceless), eg
in play, clean. So the phoneme /V has the allophone {I} after /p, W/. There is yet one
more allophone of /V/ in complementary distribution with the others: like the
alveolar plosives, the alveolar lateral has dental articulation when it occurs
immediately before a dental fricative, as in filth, stealth, although. Since this
‘occurrence is before a consonant, the allophone is also velarized: (I)
P sounds
In (Chapter 2) we said that a number of ‘sounds occur in English speech. Itis
possible to recognize three regularly occurring allophones of the /t/ phoneme in
English, The allophone with the widest distribution isthe post-alveolar frictionless.
‘continuant [1]. After fortis consonants pt, k, f, 9, J ete a devoiced (voiceless)
allophone occurs [J] og in tain, crane, free, shrug. These two allophones are in
complementary distribution. The third possible allophone is a flapped consonant
Ir], which may occur intervocalically eg in very, marry. This allophone is in free
varition with the voiced post-alveolar frictionless continuant. While other ‘r=
sounds do occur in the speech of English native speakers eg a rolled consonant // or
‘ uvular roll /R/, they do not occur regularly enough for them to be included in a
general description of English sounds.
Nasals
‘Among the nasals we have to consider two variants. Like its alveolar counterparts,
among the plosives and laterals, the alveolar nasal has a dental articulation before
ental fricatives as in plinth, ninth, So the phoneme /n/ has an allophone (9)
occurring before dentals, and an allophone [1] occurring elsewhere; they are in
complementary distribution, The other variant occurs as an allophone of both the
bilabial nasal and the aveolar nasel. When these occur before a labio-dental
{ricative if, v! 9s in symphony, convent they are often articulated as a labio~dental
‘nasal (m)]. This allophone is probably in free variation with the normal allophone
[m] and [n]; its occurrence is more likely, the more rapidly a person speaks. So the
phoneme /mm/ has an allophone [ny] before labio-dental fricatives, which is in free
variation with the normal allophone [m]; and the phoneme /n/ has an allophone [79]
before labio-dental fricatives, which is in free variation with the normal allophone
(nl.
Fricatives
Lastly among the consonants we come to the fricatives. Like voiced plosives, voiced
fricatives have a devoiced allophone occurring word-finally (before a pause), which
is in free variation with the normal fully voiced allophone eg rise [2], live [y], seethe
[B]. The palato-alveolar fricatives /f, 5/ present at an interesting case for the
application of phoneme theory to English. They could almost be regarded as
allophones of the same phoneme. The voiced palato-alveolar fricative (5/ occurs32 Analyzing English
only intervocalically, except for a few loan-words from French, where it may occur
initially and finally: although, as pointed out earlier, many speakers substitute a
palato-alveolar affricate in these positions. Moreover, there is hardly & pair of
words in which /f/ and i5/ contrast; the nearest is perhaps fission and vision.
However, the fact that /f/ does occur intervocalically eg also in mission, cushion,
‘bushel, means that the two sounds cannot be eonsidered to be in complementary
distribution or in free variation, and so must be separate phonemes,
Vowels
‘The English vowels can be divided into two groups: a number of the pure vowels
and all the diphthongs are in general longer in duration than the remainder of the
ure vowels, A comparison of seat and st wll make it plain that the vowel of seat i!
hhas a longer duration than the vowel of sit /i/. The set of short vowels is /,¢, 4,5
, a/ and the set of fong vowels /:,u:, a:,9:,2:/ together with the diphthongs. The
symbol : denotes length. Some of these vowels may be matched into pairs of
complementary long and short vowels eg i,t, /u:, 8 93, D, /:,a/ but it is
important to note that the distinguishing factor between these vowels is primarily’
one of quality determined by difference of articulation, and not one of length.
‘The short vowels have no variants. The long vowels and diphthongs, however, vary
in Iength according to the environment in which they occur. They ean be considered
te have two allophones: (1) a short allophone before a fortis consonant ie /p, t,k, tf,
4,0, 15 (2) along allophone in all other environments. Compare, for example, the
length of the vowels in bead/bear, rude/root,cardlcart,cause/course, curdicurt. The
{ong allophone may be marked with a length mark eg [i] and the short allophone
without eg [i]. In broad (phonemic) transcription, there will be no need to indicate
the length of long vowels at all, since it is not a phonemically distinguishing
characteristic. But in a narrow (phonetic) transcription the long vowels will need to
be marked for length when they occur in environments where they are relatively
long, ie not before fortis consonants.
Neutralization
One further topic remains to be dealt with under the heading of sounds and their
variants: neutralization, We have referred to phonemes as contrastive units of
sound: the substitution of one phoneme for another will result in & different word
being formed, ef pinibintinisinithin, butbetlbatbootlbaitiboatlbutlbitetboughtlbeat
etc. Sometimes, however, in a particular environment, the contrast between
phonemes in a particular set will be neutralized: the normal contrast will not
Operate. We have already mentioned an instance of this in English, though without
using the term ‘neutralization’, ‘This was the neutralization of the contrast between
fortis (voiceless) and lenis (voiced) plosives after /s/. The plosive after Is) is
‘unaspirated and voiceless: the firs feature it shares with /b, d,g/ the second with /p,
1, W/. The contrast between /p, t, k/ and /b, 4, g/ is neutralized in this environment,
since there are no words in English that differ from each other by the fact that one
begins with /sp/ and the other with /sb/ (or /st, /sd/ and /sk/, sg/). In fact, the phone
|
‘Sounds and their variants 38
is ne of either the fortis
‘occurring in this environment could be considered an allophone of either t
set of the lenis set. It is usually allocated to the fortis set, however, and this is
reflected in the orthography.
‘Another case of neutralization occurs in Englishi among the nasals /m, n, y/ when
they occur before fortis plosives Jp, t, K/. This is a slightly different case from the
preceding one. Here the neutralization of contrast between fm, /n/ and /p) arises
from the fact that which of these nasals occurs is totally predictable from the
following plosive: a bilabial nasal occurs only before a bilabial plosive, an alveolar
nasal before an alveolar plosive, and a velar nasal only before a velar plosive, as in
limp, lint, link, That is to say, only the combinations mp, /nt/ and /gk/ occur, and
not the combinations /mU, /mk/,/np!,/nk/, pl,
From what has been said in this chapter it will be clear that the list ofthe 44 sounds
fof English at the end of Chapter 3 represents an inventory of the phonemes of
English, those units which are regarded as being contrastive, making a difference in
‘words if one is substituted for another. By way of summary for this chapter, there
now follows the same list, together with the allophones of each phoneme:
Consonants
‘pv [p] after /, [pH] elsewhere
‘bv [p] word-fnally (free variation), [b] normally
iy [t) after i, [0] betore 1, 01, (41 word-fnaly (free variation) [1] ekewhere
‘a [g) before 8, 6, [d] word-fnally (fee variation), [) elsewhere
‘ki [before front vowels, (k] before back vowels, [k] after I, {k¥] elsewhere
ig! f] before front vowels, [g} before back vowels, [g) word-finally (Free
variation), [z] elsewhere
wt
iv! [y] wordefinally (free variation), [v] normally
(0)
8! (] word-finally (free variation), [6] normally
iw fs)
‘rd [z] word-fnally (free variation), [2] normally
aw
3)
tw [h]
im [m] before /f, v/ (free variation), {m] elsewhere
In! [p] before /®, 6/, [mp] before /f, w/ (free variation), [n] elsewhere
‘a fo)
1 [HJafter/p, Ws {1} before 1, 0, [t] before consonant, pause, [I] elsewhere
‘1! ({)after fortis consonant, [| intervocalially (fee variation) (1] elsewhere
iw! [w]
wv fil
Vowels
[i] before fortis consonant, [i:] elsewhere94 Analyzing English
wt
a! Ie}
‘a! [e)
‘ai [a] before fortis consonant, a: eewhere
‘oo
‘3! [3] before fortis consonant, [>] elewher
a [Ol 7 a
‘[u] before forts consonant, u:] elsewhere
a) bl
‘a) [3] before fortis consonant, [:] elsewhere
tl ‘ aoe
‘feu! [et] before fortis consonant, [et] elsewhere
‘au’ [at] before fortis consonant, [ats] elsewhere
You! [o1) before fortis consonant, [o1:) elsewhere
‘av! [90] before fortis consonant, [2v:] elsewhere
‘avi [av] before fortis consonant, [av] elsewhere
‘io! [19] before fortis consonant, [9:) elsewhere
‘ea/_ [va] before fortis consonant, [e9:] elsewhere
Jv2/_ [v9] before fortis consonant, [v9:] elsewhere,
Exercise §
Make a broad (phonemic) and narow (phonetic) transcription ofthe following English
pret fre (bed), pune ope [Pap shaving wick SermnscW ex :wpsh)
freak ‘wealth 5. kee
& plinth 5. Thursday 6 achive
7, forgeous 8 comfortable 5: pereusion
10. fre 11 aleahot 12, thirty
reject 14 15, especialy
16. mis 0 1 paramount
9. conversion 20. penne 21 favourite
22, recive tosure 24, ablaze
25. sreech
5. Sounds in connected speech
All the variations in pronunciation that we considered in the previous chapter were
inthe pronunciation of words a units in isolation, Changes such as /n! to [n] before
10) we accounted for by word-internal factors, or by the assumption of pause at the
‘end of the word, as for example in the conditions for the occurrence of a velarized
alveolar lateral [¥], But we rarely speak in single word utterances. And in studying
the phoneties of a language we have to take account of the kinds of changes in
pronunciation that take place as a result of words being strung together. Most of
these changes occur at word boundaries and affect the final end inital sounds of
words.
Assimilation
Some of the allophonic variation which occurs within words, that we discussed in
the previous chapter, occurs also at word boundaries. Alveolar consonants at the
end of a word have a dental articulation if the intial sound of the following word is
4 dental fricative eg the dental allophone [t] of occurs in no hin (nol Orn). Here
there isin fet no aspiration of the alveolar plosive, because itis unreleased, the
release occurring through the following fricative. In ten thumps, the dental
allopbone [a] of’ occurs: ['e9 Onmpts]}. And in well thought, the dental allophone
fH] of /1/ occurs: wet Gat, A second case of allophonic vatiation occurring at a
‘word boundary is the substitution of alabio-dental nasal fora bilabial or alveolar
nasal before a labio-dental fictive. In the sequence fen forks, the alveolar nasal
may have the labio-dental variant {r) in a fairly rapid pronunciation; and in the
Sequence come Jor me the bilabial nasal at the end of come may have the
Jabio-dental variant in a rapid delivery. A third case of allophonie variation
dceurring at word boundaries as well a8 within words is the incidence of the
voiceless (devoiced) allophones of /V and /r ie [I] and J], when these phonemes
Cccur after a fortis (voiceless) consonant. So in the sequence af last, the initial
alveolar lateral of last is devoiced after the forts alveolar plosive:[3tJast]. And in
the sequence a rest, the post-lveolar frictionless continuant is likewise devoiced:
ot Jest)
‘There isa further case of variation at word boundaries which could be considered
as allophonie variation, but this isnot a kind which occurs within words, This is the
case of word-final lubio-dental fricatives having a bilabial articulation when the
initial sound ofthe folowing word isa bilabial plosive. This probably only occurs in
a fainly rapid delivery. For example, in the sequence rough paich, the final
labio-dontal tricative of rough may become [®] in rapid pronunciation: [ra
phe]. And in the sequence live bird, the final voiced labio-denalfricative of live
‘may become (Bin apid pronunciation: [Ian b96 Analyzing English
‘The conditions that we gave for the occurrence of one allophone of « phoneme
rather than another applied to words spoken in isolation. It happens in a couple of
cases that these conditions no Tonger apply ina sequence of words, particularly in
relation to wordefinal positon. One of the conditions for the occurrence of
velarized alveolar lateral is word-final position; but if an alveolar lateral word-
finally is followed by a vowel in intial postion in the subsequent word, then its
non-velarized eg in the sequence flit compared with fil: [fi 1, [68]. We said that
lenis (voiced) plosives and frcatives tend wo be devoieed in word-fnal position. But
it ina sequence, the word following begins witha vowel or voiced consonant, then
they will not be devoiced, though they wil be if the inital sound ofthe following
ude vote mona Compr ood das [pe et wih ood es
iz
‘The influence of one sound on another to become more like itself is called
assimilation. Assimilation may be to the place of articulation of a neighbouring
sound, as in the dentalizaton of alveolars before dental fricatives, or the substitu-
tion of a labio-dental nasal for a bilabial oF alveolar nasal before a labio-dental
fricative. Or assimilation may be in voicing, as when voiced plosives and fricatives
become devoiced before voiceless sounds or silence (pause), or when /V and iti
become voiceless after fortis (voiceless) consonants.
All the cases of assimilation that we have considered so far have been cases of
allophonic variation: the substitutions have been regarded as allophones of the
same phoneme, with the criterion of phonetic similarity being of particular
relevance here eg in counting [}] and [Bas allophones of // and !v/ respectively.
But there are many cases of assimilation where the substitution is not allophonic but
phonemic, the substitution of one phoneme for another.
Alveolar sounds in word-final postion are particularly able 10 assimilate to the
place of articulation ofthe initial consonant ofa following word; that is, the sounds
1, 5,2. Final is ikely to become pl before bilabial sounds /p,b, mv eg in the
sequences shat pen [Dep pPen}, sat boy [Omp bo, that man [Owp msen]. And Final
‘becomes /by before the same sounds eg in good pen [gob plea] ete, Final is
likely to become /k/ before velar plosives, g/g, in the sequences that cup [Bzxk
i shat gir Ozek go 3]. And likewise ia! becomes before the same sounds eg
in good cup [avg k*npH) ete
‘A word-fina alveolar nasal /n/ becomes bilabial /n before bilabial sounds /p,b,
and velar iy before velar plosives /k, gl. Examples: en pens [em prenz ten boys
{tem bor:2], ten men [Nem menl; ten cups [te keaphs, en girs (te ga Ha). Ira
Sequence of alveolar nasal and alveolar plsive occurs worde-finall, then both may
assimilate to the place of articulation ofa following consonant, Examples: int in
don’ assimilates to the place of articulation of the following iy in don’t be late
[ov :mp s lett;/n/in won’ assimilates to/k/ in he won's come {hu wav: kb ms
‘nin ound assimilates tothe bilabial articulation of following ib in he found both
{hu fav:mb bv); /nd in kind assimilates othe velar articulation of ina kind gift
fo kearing git.
Alveolar fricatives /s, 2/ assimilate to a following palatal sound, either palato-
‘Sounds in connected speech 97
alveolar fricative ot palatal frictionless continuant J, j/ and become palato-alveolar
fricatives,3/- Examples: this shop [O15 fop) this year [rf jt J; has she [has fi],
those young men [80:3 jay men],
Assimilations of the kind we have been discussing could, in theory, give rise 0
ambiguous utterances, since in substituting one phoneme for another the new
phoneme may already make an acceptable sequence in English. For example, the
sequence [lenk*wikt}} could be taken as representing either rang quickly or ran
quickly with assimilation of fw to following ‘ki. Or the sequence (woif jo : wert]
Could represent either watch your weight or what's your weight with assimilation of
Js! to following palatal /j
‘When wordefinal alveolar plosives and fricatives/t, d,s, 2/ are followed by an initial
palatal frictionless continuant //, then assimilation is to palato-alveolar place of
articulation and the initial /! usually disappears, although for /s, x this is not always
the case, as indicated above. The alveolar plosives become palato-alveolar affri-
cates tf, d5/ in this instance, and the alveolar fricatives become palato-alveolar
fricatives f, 3, as discussed earlier. Examples: woud you [wod3 u: ], what you want
[woufu: wont); as yer [eget], in case you need it [iq Kerfus nid 10
Now we come to assimilations involving nasalization. A voiced alveolar plosive /d!
‘may become a nasal when followed by a nasal. Ifthe following nasal is alveolar, /d/
becomes /n, eg in the sequence he wouldn't do it, the Jd! of would becomes an
alveolar nasal: [hi wonn(t) du: 10]. There is a tendency for the /V of not to
disappear. Ifthe following nasal is velar, then /d! becomes /; eg in the sequence he
wouldn't go, the ‘nl assimilate to the following velar /g/, and the /d/ of would then
assimilates to the velar nasal following, while the /k/ from /t/ probably disappears
{iv wonn(k) gov :]. IF the following nasal is bilabial, then Jd becomes /m/,
assimilating again both in place of articulation and in nasalty eg good morning
{[gom mo nin]. It is not just word-final alveolar consonants that may be involved in
assimilation; for example, the voiced labio-dental fricative /W/ may assimilate in
place and nasality to a following bilabial nasal /mi, as in you cam have mine [ju ko
hhasm mat in}. In this sequence also the vowel of ean probably disappears (see further
below on reduction) and the final alveolar nasal /n/ assimilates towards the glottal
articulation offv and becomes velar.
Finally we turn to assimilations involving voice. Word-final lenis (voiced) fricatives
‘may become fortis (voiceless) when they are followed by an initial forts consonant,
especially ifthe two words are part ofa closely knit group. Examples: / becomes
10) in with thanks [0 Oaey}s), breathe slowly [bliB stv sr; /2! becomes is in these
socks [is sok*s], he was sent {In was sent]; 1 becomes /t/ in of course [ot k*9s],
‘we've Jound it (wil fav nd 11}; fg! becomes (tf] in bridge score [bUrtf ska
‘Assimilation isa widespread phenomenon in speech, The instances that we have
discussed represent the main ones in English. Quite obviowsly they do not
necessarily occur all the time; but statement about assimilation implios that when
people speak, and the more rapidly they do so, then these kinds of alteration may
{ake place. Since this is so, these variations will need to be taken account of in a
harrow transcription of speech, Indeed, it may be argued that the phonemic38 Analyzing English
variation resulting from ass should even be accounted for in a broad
transcription. But for practical purposes we shall assume that a broad transcription
represents words as if spoken in isolation.
By way of further illustration, here is an imagined short conversation between a
policeman and a motorist in narrow transcription including assimilations (under-
lined). .
Good morning, sit {om mo:nig 9
Good morning, officer. m ma :niy ofis
Your stop lights not working jp stop [ais nop wat
teas when left he house. tp woz wen a1: ke f.bo havs
—That’s not quite now. Oss nok Kwart nao
Which one might tbe? with vam malt 1p bi
—The left light doen fate
LU have to get the thing fixed. aH hak to get 60 Oxy fist}
Buercse 6
Make marow transition of the following sequences of wort, ineuding any poste
assimilation,
8 Shut the yard gate behind you [fa D2 jag getp tuhat:ndsus)
A. They train cows to jump
2. A badd picture of rose tree
3. Who do you want?
4 He was shipwrecked
5. He's found seven fake pitures
Elision
Besides assimilation, there are two further processes that cause variation in the
pronunciation of sounds at word boundaries: elison and liaison. The process of
élision involves the complete disappearance of a sound and takes place only in
fairly rapid speech. In such speech it is very common for word-initial fh’ to
disappear—and this isa characteristic by no means restricted to cockney speakers!
Example: up the hill (4p 01 1],
In word-fnal postion it isthe alveolar plosives that are most likely to undergo the
process of elision, particularly if they are preceded by another consonant and the
following word has a consonant in initial position. The pairs of word-final
consonants for which this isthe case can be divided into two groups. The frst of
‘these comprises continuant consonants followed by /t or a: st, ft, nd, 1d, 2d, 8,
val, Examples: nest day [nek's det:), lef turn [lef than}, mashed potatoes [maf
Potertov:z], bend back [bem bak), hold tight (hav t tart], refused both
{ufju 12 bov0], loathed beer [lov :6 bx9 +], moved back {mu : v bk], The second
group comprises plosives and affricates followed by /V oF /d: ip, kt, tft, bd, gd,
5d. Examples: kept quiet [k'p! K>wat :a1) liked jam [laik* dseem), reached Paris
[ph red bor Lob boo) lagged behind (eg brat: changed colour
ter sods kb)
Sounds in connected speach 99,
\Word-final/U may undergo elision in two further circumstances. I he initial sound
of the following word is iV or /d), then the resulting cluster of /t/ of /t! is often
simplified to or (respectively. Examples: 've got 1 go (at:v gp to gov: |, what
do you want [wo «gu : wont]. The second case involves the negator not when itis
reduced to nt, and the /U/ undergoes elision before another consonant. Examples:
‘you mustn't Tose itv masn lu:z.1t, doesn’t she know [dazn Ji:nav:)- This similar
to the continuant +/t, d/ cases discussed above.
Liaison
Liaison, the other process occurring at word boundaries, involves the insertion of a
sound that is by and large unmotivated from the phonetic context. The sound is.
‘And it is by and large unmotivated, because, although an /r/ occurs in the written
form of some of the words involved, reflecting an earlier pronunciation, the use of
sking /rf has been extended to contexts where there is no r in the orthographic
form. In the sequences here and there (ial an Sea), father and son {fa :69.1 20 san),
fand the far east [Ba fa: 1 ist] an r occurs in the written form, although not if the
words concerned are pronounced in isolation: [ha], (fa:60), [fa:}. But in the
sequences law and order [lo: 1 an ada} andche idea of i [1 a1:419: J av it) there is
no in the orthography and yet liaison takes place
Reduction
‘There is one further process that occurs as the result of rapidly articulated speech,
but itis not one that occurs at word boundaries. It is called reduction and involves
the substitution ofa ‘weak’ central vowel, especially // ori, for a peripheral vowel
fe front or back. Reduction occurs particularly in unstressed syllables of words with
‘mote than one syllable, and in single syllable words that have a grammatical (rather
than a lexical) function, eg words like is, are, have, of, his, the, does, and, a, an
(Syllables are discussed in Chapter 6 and the distinction between grammatical and
lexical words in Chapter 9.)
Ic is impossible to give a detailed list of all the possible reductions, but the following
examples will provide a fair idea of the kinds of cases that may occur. Take the
‘word of, pronounced in isolation as /ov. In connected speech this would commonly
be pronounced [av], and may even be reduced further to [v] or [3] eg in wo of mine
{tu: o(v) main}. In isolation have would be pronounced /hiev/, but in connected
speech the /hy will undergo elision and the front vowel /a/ will be reduced, giving
[ov], eg in shey have eaten [Set : ov iton]. The word the, pronounced in isolation as
‘ix/, may in connected speech be either [O1], if it occurs before a word beginning
with'a vowel, or [6a] before an initial consonant. In isolation and is pronounced
‘endl, but in connected speech as [ond], or more frequently as [an] or even [n].
In unstressed syllables that end in lor the vowel is normally the unstressed mid
‘central spread vowel /9/ ep in boule fbotot or buuon Vaan’. But in connected
speech a further reduction often occurs which causes the vowel to be dropped
completely, giving (bot*4] and [btn]. Normally a syllable has to contain a vowel
(Gee Chapter 6), but in this case the /V and /n/ are doing service for the vowel and40 Analyzing English
‘are called syllabic A and syllabic ini respectively. The symbol for a syllabic
consonant is the small stroke bencath the consonant symbol: fj) [2]
As in the case of assimilation, a narrow transcription of a connected sequence of
speech will have to take account of the processes of elision, liaison and reduction
By way of summary, hete is list of the processes described in this chapter that
‘occur when words are put together in connected specch.
Assimilation
Word-final alveolars become dental before dental fricatives,
Bilabial and alveolar nasals become labio-dental before labio-dentalfricatives.
Word-initial /W and /r! becomes voiceless after fortis consonants.
nal labio-dental fricatives may become bilabial before bilabial plosives.
inal /V is non-velarised if followed by an initial vowel
inal lenis plosives and Iricatives are not devoiced if followed by a vowel or
voiced consonant.
inal /, d/ become bilabial before bilabial consonants.
inal /t, d/ become velar before velar plosives
inal /n/ becomes bilabial before bilabial consonants.
inal /n/ becomes velar before velar plosives.
inal /nt, nd/ both become bilabial before bilabials and velar before velars.
Word-final /s, 2/ become palato-alveolar before palato-alveolar fricatives and the
palatal frictionless continuant.
Word-final /t,d, s, / become palato-alveolar affricates (It, dl) or fricatives (Js, z/)
before / and /j/ disappears
Word-final /d) becomes a nasal before a nasal, at the place of articulation of the
nasal.
Word-final /v/ becomes a nasal before a nasal
Word-final lenis fricatives become fortis before an initial fortis consonant
Elision
Word-inital Jh frequently undergoes elsion,
Word-final /t, d’ preceded by another consonant and followed by an initial
consonant undergo elision.
Word-final t’ followed by /t, d/ undergoes elision.
Liaison
Word-final vowel followed by an initial vowel are often linked by insertion of /!.
Reduction
Vowels in unstressed syllables, or in monosyllabic grammatical words are often
reduced to // of i
‘Complete reduction, ie elision, may oceur in unstressed syllables ending in or /n/,
giving a syllabic consonant,
‘Sounds in connected speech 41
Exercise 7
Make s narrow transcription ofthe following uiterences, taking account of any asain,
stision ltion and reduction, * an
fg He'oe the of fof ta: greengrocer
i019 oe tra of Sk pteloes)
1. What cid you sy his name
2. We discusted the idea of Twas ast Tuesday
5 He dnt come tote lecture or he would have known
a
5
‘They camped by the river on the other side
‘The masked gunman held the hostage at gunpoint6. Combining sounds
Phonemes themselves have no meaning: their only function is to combine together
to form higher-level mesningful units; that is, words. But between phonemes and
words we have to recognize an intermediate level of phonological organization: the
syllable. Many words in English have only one syllable; they are ‘monosyllabic’. But
‘many have more than one syllable; they are ‘polysyllabic’. And yet it is clear that
the same principles operate in the combining of phonemes into syllables, whether
the syllable itself constitutes a word, or whether the syllable in turn combines with
another or several other syllables to form a word. Like phonemes, syllables are in
principle meaningless eg sim /sum/ and ple /pal/ of simple, even though in some cases
they do eaincide with a meaningful unit, eg in hard—ship, un—do or, of course, in
monosyllabic words,
Syllabie structure
A syllable has a basic three-part structure, composed of a central part and a
peripheral part before and after the central one. The central part is almost always a
vowel sound and the peripheral parts are consonants. Each syllable has only one
vowel, either a pure vowel or a dipthong, at its centre; but the number of
consonants in the peripheries may vary. It is possible that one or other or both of,
the peripheral parts will not be realised ie there may be no consonants either before
or after the vowel, as for example in out favt, foe /tav/ /at!. In English there may,
in fact, be up to three consonants before the Vowel and up (0 four consonants after
it eg in sirengihs /strepk0s!. Phonemes, then, combine to form syllables, and
syllables combine to form words. The maximum number of syllables possible in an
English word seems to be about seven or eight eg in in—ter—natio-—na-lizar
fon.
We have said that a vowel is almost always at the central part of a syllable. The
exceptions to this statement are the so-called syllabic consonants, discussed in
Chapter 5, which arise from the reduction of a weak vowel to extinction, The
consonants which most often become central io the syllable for this reason are il/
‘nd /n, asin the examples given in Chapter 5: bottle bo, button ib ty In some
other languages syllabic consonants are normal central elements, eg a form of rin
Czech, Vowels in English never function at the peripheries of syllables, and this
provides the reason for treating the ‘semi-vowels’/w/ and /j as consonants: inspite
of their vowel-like quality they pattern in syllable structure like consonants, ie they
‘occur in the peripheries of syllables and not in the central part.
One practical reason for recognizing syllables as intermediate units between
phonemes and words is that they represent the most convenient level at which to
describe the way in which phonemes combine together and the restrictions that
Combining sounds 43,
‘ccur on combinations of phonemes. The possibilities of phoneme combination are
not unlimited: any phoneme may not combine with just any other. There isthe
obvious restriction imposed by the nature of syllable structure Consonant-Vowel-
Consonant (CVC); a syllable could not be composed of just consonants, nor of
more than one vowel or diphthong. But even taking ths restriction into account,
some phoneme combinations would be impossible to pronounce eg /pgnust, except
perhaps with extreme difficulty. Other combinations, although in principle quite
pronounceable, just do not occur in English, eg nufR/. English does not use all the
possible pronounceable combinations of phonemes to make syllables and words
‘That isto say, there are certain rules of phoneme combination for English which
can be deduced from the syllables and words actually occurring
Initial periphery
Since the nature of the syllable itself specifies that only one vowel phoneme may
‘occur in each syllable, we can describe the rules of phoneme combination in terms
(of the consonants and their combinations that may occur in peripheral positions in
syllables. We shall look first at the consonants in initial position in the syllable. Tt is
possible that no consonant at all will occur before the vowel (as in oud), and that is
the case in English before all the vowel phonemes except two: / and /oa/ never
‘occur without a consonant preceding. All the consonant phonemes of English may
coccur singly in syllable initial position except two: / and iq/ never occur by
themselves, nor for that matter in combination wit another consonant, in the
initial periphery of an English syllable. The permissible combinations of two
consonants ia syllable initial position may be expressed by the following diagram:
Figure 28: Sllable-inital ewo-consonant combinations
als eens f
According © this diagram the following syllable initial two consonant combinations
are possible: /smv, sn, /st, ski, sp sf, fw, Ow/, lt, fr, er, hw, k,l,
pr, Hl, fot, axl, ipl, MU, POV, IgV, Ife, ISw/, VS, making a total of 26. This
obviously represents only a very small proportion of the arithmetically possible
combinations of the 24 consonants in English, which would amount to 576,
assuming that each consonant would be allowed to occur with itself. The restric-
tions are even more remarkable when we turn to combinations of three consonants
in syllable initial position. Here the arithmetical possibilities would be 13,824, but
the actually occurring combinatiqns number six, of which one is found in only one44 Analyzing English
‘word. The following diagram represents the permissible three consonant combina
tions:
Figure 26: Syllable-initiatthree-consonant combinations
The six three-consonant combinations are, then: /spV,/spr,/st, st! /kr/,/skw
‘The combination /sfr/ occurs only in the word sphragistics, the study of engraved
seals
xercie 8
Find English words to illustrate exch of the permissible two-and three-consomant combi
tions in syllable initial position in English
Final periphery
‘We turn now to the rules of phoneme combination that apply to the final periphery
of the syllable, Here also itis possible for no consonant to occur: this is the case
after any vowel phoneme in English, except the following five, which are always
followed by a consonant: fe; fa, /o/, ial. All consonant phonemes may occur
singly in syllable final position in English, except the following four: i, wl
11 is more difficult to find precise formulas to express the permissible clusters in
syllable final consonant combinations than in syllable initial combinations, As
stated earlier, there may be up to four consonants in the final periphery; and as in
the case of syllable initial clusters, the fower are the permissible combinations, the
larger the clusters become. Although not expressing the restrictions in the greatest
possible detail or with complete precision, the following chart provides a neat
representation of permissible combinations in syllable final position:
Figure 27: Sllable-inal consonant combinations
Position A_ Position B _ Position € Position D__|
T ‘Any consonant @ t
mao except ta 5
3 hewj se
eeesae sees
‘Two consonant combinations may comprise either a consonant from Position A
combined with a consonant from Position B, or from Position B combined with
Position C. The 48 permissible three-consonant syllable final combinations are
either Position A + Position B+ Position C, or Position B + Position C + Posi
tion D. The seven permissible four consonant combinations are as follows: /Ikts,
Impstl, Impts/, Aksts/, MDs), Os, /ntOs!, as in mudets, glimpsed, tempts, texts,
‘welfihs, sicths, thousandihs. Obviously, these sound clusters are likely to be
Combining sounds 45,
reduced under the pressures of connected speech. The reader is let withthe task of
discovering examples for the two-and three-consonant combinations, and indeed of
discovering precisely which combinations are found in contemporary English.
"The phoneme combination rules that we have been discussing are established on
the basis of monosyllabic words. In polysyllabie words itis not always clear, when a
number of consonants ovcurs at syllable boundaries, to which syllable they should
be assigned. For example, in the word exira fekstro/ there are obviously (wo
syllables, as two vowel phonemes occur, but itis not clear how the syllables should
be divided. Three possible divisions may be made, all of which would give
permissible consonant combinations: k-str, ks-tr and kst-r.The method proposed to
solve this kind of problem is a statistical one, A statistical survey of syllable initial
and syllable final positions has produced an order of “favoured” syllable beginnings
and endings, and the break between syllables that conforms to this most favoured
principle isthe preferred one. At the beginning of a syllable the order of favour is:
CV, VC, CC (C= consonant, V = vowel); at the end of a syllable the order is: VC,
CV, CC. So in a word like aster /esta/ (VCCV) the preferred break would be as-7.
For extra one would presumably have to choose between VC-CCCV and
VCCC-CY, ie ek-stra and ekstr.
Stress
Another good reason for recognizing syllables is that there isa further phenomenon
that scems to be associated with them, that of stress placement. When we hear
polysyllabie words spoken we are often aware that some part of the word sounds
louder or seems to be uttered with more force than another part. Or, to put it
another way, one syllable is more prominent than another. This is achieved by the
fact that syllables have different degrees of stress, and this is one of the phonetic
factors that ensures that speech does not sound completely monotonous and level
‘Stress is perceived as difference in loudness, but from the articulatory point of view
stress variation arses from force of articulation and more especially from difference
in pte
In English words we usually recognize three degrees of stress: primary stress,
indicated by a stroke above the line at the beginning of the syllable so stressed (');
secondary stress, indicated by a stroke on or below the line atthe beginning of the
syllable $0 stressed; and unstressed syllables, which are not marked. Each
English word, spoken in isolation, has one and only one primary stress, So
monosyllables have just a primary stress. In connected speech some monosyllabic
‘words may become unstressed; this applies particularly to ‘grammatical’ words,
those that are also subject to vowel reduction (see Chapter 5). Polysyllabie words
may have, though not necessarily, one secondary stress, and in some fairly rare
‘cases Iwo secondary stresses. All remaining syllables will be unstressed, Each
English word, then, has @ characteristic stress pattern which normally remains
unchanged in connected speech.
Unlike in some languages, primary stress in English does not fall on the same
syllable in every word (eg last sylable or penultimate); it may fall on just about any48 Analyzing English
syllable. However, for any particular word primary stress usually fall on the same
syllable whenever the word occurs. So in every occurrence of reserve, primary stress
always falls on the second syllable. In English, then, stress placement is free in the
sense that there is no uniform pattern for every word, and itis fixed in that the
stress patterns of individual words do not change according to context,
Here now are some examples of stress patterns i English polysyllabic words:
secondary + primary jun known
unstressed + primary llone
primary + secondary ‘profile
primary + unstressed 'ta ble
Secondary + unstressed + primary un derstand
primary + unstressed + unstressed "ba che for
Primary + unstressed + secondary ‘pho tograph
unstressed + primary + unstressed im'por tant
secondary + primary + unstressed jun'cer tain
unstressed + primary + two unstressed n'po ssi ble
secondary + primary + two unstressed ,un'for tu nate
unstressed + primary +-unstressed + secondary eli maize
primary + three unstressed ‘me fan cho ly
Primary + unstressed + secondary + unstressed ‘educa ted
unstressed + secondary + unstressed + primary misjun der'stand
unstressed + secondary + unstressed + primary + unstressed comsi de'ra tion
secondary + unstressed + primary + unstressed + secondary in ca'pa ciate
unstressed + primary + three unstressed ad’mi nis tra tive
unstressed + secondary + unstressed + primary + two unstressed con,ver sbi lity
secondary + three unstressed + primary + unstressed yaa tio na lea tion
secondary + unstressed + primary + theee unstressed yn dis'tn gui sha ble
secondary + unstressed + Secondary + unstressed + primary + two unstressed
vu rei abi lity
Uunstressod + secondary + two unstressed & nrimary + two unstressed impe ne
tra bi lity
‘A mere difference in stress pattern does not usually serve to distinguish one word
from another. Stress is not contrastive in the sense that phonemes are: the
substitution of one phoneme for another results in the creation of a different word
eg ban'panitanican etc. But for stress this is not the case: stress is contrastive in the
sense that it contrasts one syllable with another in the linear sequence of a word,
However, there is small number of words in English where the substitution of one
stress pattern for another does result in the creation of a different word. Very often,
though, the difference in stress pattern is uecompanied by a difference in vowel
quality, since unstressed syllables tend to be associated with vowel reduction and
centralized vowels. Compare the following pairs of words:
‘trans fer trans fer
‘digest di'gest
‘con duet con'duct
‘protest pro'test
Combining sounds 47
‘The words in the first column, with primary stress on the first syllable, are nouns;
while those in the second column, with primary stress on the second syllable, are
verbs.
We have said that each word in English has a characteristic stress pattern. These
stress patterns may, however, change in the course of time. For example, the word
‘momentary has the stress pattern in modern English of a primary stress on the first
syllable, with all the others unstressed, In the seventeenth century the primary
stress was on the penultimate syllable, with a secondary stress on the first
syllable—mo men'ta ry. This older stress pattern is retained in present-day
American-English pronunciation. In modern English there are a few words whose
stress pattern is variable, even within the speech of an individual, and it could be
that these are words whose stress pattern is currently in the course of changing, eg
“im te graliin'te gral; ‘con tro ver sy,con'tro ver sy
Rxercse 9
Work out the stress patterns for the following English words:
eg control, fant a s},,sup po 's ton.
1, machine 2. female 3, magazine
4. quantity 5 telephone 3. thinoceros
7. Gowardliness 8. satisfactory 9, inferiority
10, personification 11, unilateralism
a7. Intonation
When we speak, we do not do s0 on a monotone, Indeed, we should all sound very
boring if we did. Speech does not just consist of a sequence of phonemes combined
into syllables and words. Accompanying these combinations of phonemes, occur-
ring simultaneously with their articulation, are two features of speech that have
linguistic importance. One of these is stress; the importance of which we sav in the
previous chapter in contrasting one syllable with another in the phonological
structure of words. Stress has a similar {unction in longer stretches of utterance, in
according prominence to one syllable (and so to the word of which itis a part) as
‘against another. For example, the utterance The cat chased the mouse could be
stressed in one of three ways:
The CAT chased the mouse
The cat CIASed the mouse
The cat chased the MOUSE.
As in the case of words, stress is produced in utterances not especially by greater
force of articulation on one syllable rather than another, but more particularly by a
change in pitch on a syllable,
‘Tone groups
‘The other linguistically important feature accompanying the phoneme combina
tions is intonation, This isthe musical or melodie accompaniment of speech. On the
level of intonation each utterance is composed of a number of tone-groups, Each
tone-group represents a unit of information, which isto say that intonation has the
function of dividing an utterance up into information units. Viewed phonetically,
tone-groups consist ofa series of rises and falls in the pitch of the voice. Since, as we
have said, pitch movement is probably the most important contributing factor to
stress, intonation and stress can be seen to be closely interconnected in this area of
speech production. Besides structuring the utterance in terms of information units,
intonation is often important grammatically in distinguishing one type of sentence
from another; and it is also important in signalling the attitude of the speaker in
what he is saying.
Nucleus
Every tone-group has a nucleus, which i its most prominent part. The nucleus is, in
other words, the most heavily stressed syllable in a tone-group, and the one which
has associated with ita pitch movement. In a tone-group without contrastive or
‘emphatic stress the nucleus falls on the primary stressed syllable of the last lexical
Word (noun, verb, adjective or adverb, as against pronoun, preposition, ete which
48
Intonation 49
are ‘grammatical’ words—see Chapter 9) eg The cat chased the MOUSE. Where
there is contrastive or emphatic stress, the primary stressed syllable with such stress
is the locus of the nucleus, eg The cat CHASed the mouse, The CAT chased the
Intonation is a notoriously difficult area of phonetic investigation, and phoneticians
vary considerably inthe way that they analyze it. Among other things, they disagree
‘on the number and types of nucleuses that should be recognized for English. We
will restrict ourselves to four types of nucleus, which seems adequate, They are:
falling, ising, flling-rising ond rising falling. We can illustrate these, together with
the notation that is frequently used, on the uterance No:
Falling ‘No (a matter-of-fact statement)
Rising ,No (questioning)
Fallngzrising ‘No (expressing incredulity)
Rising-falling "No (emphatic probbition, solding).
“The notes in brackets are an attempt to provide a meaning for each of the nucleus
tones, and to help the reader identify and produce them for himsel.
Tail
[Any lables in a tone-group coming after the nucleus are called the tall The pitch
‘on these syllables continues the direction of the nucleus. So if the nucieus has a
rising or @ fallng-rising pte, that on the tail will be ising; and it will be falling i
{he nucleus is falling oF rising-falling. Compare the following utterances:
He wants some “ICE-cream on top off (statement)
He wants some ,ICE-cream on top of i (qiestioning).
Head
“The part of the tone-group coming directly before the nucleus is called the head.
This extends from the primary stressed syllable of the first lexical word in the
tone-group up to the nucleus. The tones of the head are level, rather than having.
pitch movement, 50 that two kinds of effect may be found. The head may all be
‘tered on the sume pitch, usually low, or a stepping effect may be produced, either
from relatively high to relatively low pitch, or from relatively low to relatively high
pitch, Compare the following utterances:
Pat it on the “TABle (nonchalant instruction)
Put it on the “TABle (emphatic, impatient command).
Pre-head
‘Any syllables coming before the head are called the preshead. They willbe part of
“lexical” words of “grammatical” words, and will be relatively unstressed. The
pre-head may normally consist of high level tones or of low level tones. Compare
the following, utterances:
He dest kinow Row io 'SAY 3 (matter-of-fact statement)
Fe doesn't kilaw how fo “SAY it (mocking tone).50 Analyzing English
‘The foregoing isa sketch of one possible way to analyze and describe the intonation
of English. When one tries to observe intonation, it is often difficult to recognize
‘what isa falling tone and what isa rising one, but it soems clear that in conversation
‘we do perceive and react to these differences, and that they play an important part
in signalling meaning. 1t is partly from the intonation of an utterance that we
Understand what a speaker intends by what he is gaying. That means his intention
both in respect of linguistic meaning, and in respect of his attitude to what he is
saying, We will now consider these two aspects of the function of intonation in
English.
Intonation and meaning
The contrast between a falling intonation pattern and a rising intonation pattern
often correlates with a distinction between a statement and a question. Compare,
{for example, the intonation patterns that might normally accompany the following:
Cats chase mice.—statement;
Do cats chase mice?—question expecting the answer yes of no;
Where did the ball go?—question seeking information; itis possible for this kind of,
question to be spoken with either rising or falling intonation, Indeed, with
information seeking questions falling intonation is probably the norm, In any case,
for either kind of question it is not just the intonation that indicates whether itis &
{question or a statement: the order of words (do before eats, id before the ball) and
the presence of a question word (where) in information seeking questions are
‘enough (o make the distinction clear. Where the intonation invariably carries the
contrast between statement and question is if the utterance has a statement form
but a rising intonation pattern eg Cats chase mice?
Another meaning associated with a rising intonation pattern is that of incomplete~
ness. A rising intonation at the end of a tone-group may indicate that what is being
said is not finished, that there is more to come eg
First she went to the Cupboard, then she tried the Sideboard, and finally she found itin
the table draver
Or, in offering a list of items—Would you lke tea, o coffee or milk?—a falling
intonation on the last item implies that the choice is limited to those items
mentioned, whereas a rising intonation on the las item implies that the fist is not
closed and that the speaker is open to further suggestions
‘The meaning of an utterance may also be affected by where the nucleus of a
tone-group is placed: this may be erueial in deciding which itoms in an utterance
bbelong together grammatically. The by now famous example to illustrate this point
is the utterance He fed her dog biscuits. Ifthe nucleus falls on BiScuifs, then itis the
ddog that he is feeding, If, on the other hand, the nucleus falls on DOG, then the
lady participant is being fed with dog biscuit.
In a similar way, how the utterance is divided into tone-groups, ie where the
‘one-group boundaries are placed, may also affect the sense of what is being said. A.
traditional example is the utterance He washed and brushed his hair, If this
ltterance is spoken as one tone-group, ie with only one nucleus, then it means that
Intonation 51
the actions referred to applied only to the hair. If, on the other hand, itis spoken as
two tone-groups, with a tone-group boundary between washed and and, then the
washing may be taken to refer to other parts of his body and the brushing alone
applied to the hair.
Intonation may, then, be & contributory factor in determining which words belong
together grammatically in an utterance, by tone-group boundary placement, by the
placement of the nucleus within a tone-group, or by the kind of nucleus and general
intonation pattern that is used,
Intonation and attitude
Perhaps more important than the use of intonation to make distinctions in grammar
is the function of intonation to signal how a speaker intends his utterance to be
taken or what his attitude is as he speaks the utterance. But this aspect of intonation
is probably the most difficult of all to characterize, not least because we scem not to
have a handy or agreed list of labels to apply to different attitudes. For example,
how could we characterize the differences between the following, utterances:
‘That's fine (falling)—? matter of fact
That's sine (rising) —2 encouraging
That's “fine (falling-rising)—? what have you been worrying about.
Part of the difficulty is that the same intonation pattern may have different
‘meanings or signal different attitudes in different contexts. It seems to depend on
the meaning of the lexical items as much as on the meaning of the intonation.
Compare the following utterances:
No (rising-faling)—indignant
‘Really (rsing-falling)—ineredulous, sarcastic
He "always is (cising-talling)—impatient
and the following:
No (falling-rising)—doubtful but encouraging
If you want to (falling-rising)—grudging acceptance.
It is not just the type of nucleus that may be important in conveying the attitude of
the speaker, butalsothe kind ofhead, For example, Good Morning isnormally spoken
with a falling nucleus on MORNing. But the head good may be characterized by
cither a high level pitch or a low level pitch: the high pitch signals the attitude
‘cheerful’, while the low pitch signals the attitude ‘routine’
Bxercise 10
For normal rendering of the following utterances, consider where the nucleus would fall,
land what kind of nucleus it might be:
eg thar's a STUpid way to do i
Hell telephone 19’ MORrow.
1, Don't do it lke that.
2. He came home yesterday
3. What did hd say after that.
4. His suggestion ix simply ridiculous,
5, You ean” go out,82 Analyzing English
(6. He seems alright to me.
7. Is that al he talked about?
8, He won't find me there,
9. Lan understand it
10. You're never here on time.
Notes ”
"The etttude labels are taken from A C Gimson’s An introduction to the pronunciation of
English Edward Amold, 1970
# Labels trom Gimson, op cit
For a dettiled investigation ofthe attitudes conveyed by different kinds of tone-group and
intonation patetn, the reader is refered to O'Connor, J D, and GF Arnold Intonation of
colloquial English Longmen, 1961.
| Part Two: Structures8. Grammar and sentences
‘A linguistic description, and consequently a language, is often regarded as being
‘composed of three parts: phonetics/phonology, grammar and semantics. And there
is a sense in which grammar links phonology and semantics: phonemes combine
into words (phonology), words combine into sentences (grammar), and sentences
refer to events, actions and states in the world (semantics). However, phonology
‘and grammar are different from semantics in that they deal exclusively with the
internal structure of language (form), while semantics is concerned largely with the
external relationships of language (meaning).
Phonology and grammar
The relationship between phonology and grammar is not as simple as has been
implied. There is no automatic progression from phoneme to word to sentence
There are, in fat, two parallel sets of hierarchical relationships: phoneme—syll
able—word (—tone-sroup); morpheme—word—. sentence. It so happens that
the unit ‘word occurs in both, and not without reason. “Word” i the level at which
the greatest amount of congruence is found between units in phonology and units in
grammar. For the majority of phonological words there are equivalent grammatical
words in # one-to-one relationship. But there are several interesting cases that
demonstrate the separateness of the two sets of hierarchical relationships. These
are cases where there is no one-to-one match between phonological and grammati-
cal word.
The uneven match between phonology and grammar is welt illustrated by the
so-called homophones, where different grammatical words have the same phonemic
structure or pronunciation, Take the phonological word /bav!. It represents a
‘number of lexical items (grammatical words): bough, bow (of a boat), bow (verb,
“to incline the head or body"), bow (noun, ‘inclination of the head or body’).
Besides being homophones, the last three items mentioned are also homographs, ie
they are written the same. Or take the phonological words /stend! and /marnd/
teach of them represents two grammatical words, one @ noun and the other a verb
Uneven matching between phonology and grammar is also found the other way
round, though more rarely. The past tense of the verb dream may be represented
phonologically as either Jdremv or /drimd/. And whether integral has the phonolog.
ical structure /ntagraV or /in'tegral, itis till the same item as far as the grammar is
concerned.
‘The separateness of the phonological and grammatical sets of relationships is
sometimes referred to as the ‘double articulation’ of language. This implies that
language is patterned simultaneously on two levels: sound and syntax. Phonemes
pattern into words, words pattern into sentences, and the link between the two