TAT Practical Guide
TAT Practical Guide
Copyrighted Material
Copyrighted Material
A PRACTICAL GUIDE
TO THE THEMATIC
APPERCEPTION TEST
Copyrighted Material
USA Publishing Office: BRUNNER-ROUTLEDGE
A member oj the Taylor & Francis Group
325 Chestn ut Street
Philadelphia, PA 19106
Tel: (2 15) 625-8900
Fax: (2 15) 625-2940
Distribution Center: BRUNNER-ROUTLEDGE
A member oj the Taylor & Francis Group
7625 Empire Drive
Florence, KY 4 1042
Tel: 1-800-634-7064
Fax: 1-800-248 -4724
UK BRUNNER-ROUTLEDGE
A member oj the Taylor & Francis Group
27 Church Road
Hove
E. Sussex, BN3 2FA
Tel: +44 (0) 1273 2074 11
Fax: +44 (0) 12732056 12
A CIP ca talog record for thi s book is avai lable from the British Library.
@ The paper in thi s publication meets the requirements of the ANSI Standard
Z39.48 -1 984 (Perma nence of Paper).
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Aronow, Edwa rd.
A practical gu id e to the thematic appercep ti on test: th e T.A.T. in clinical practice /
Edwa rd Aronow, Kim A ltm an Weiss, Marvin Reznikofr.
p. cm.
Includes ind ex.
ISBN 0-87630-944-9 (a lk. pape r )
l. Thematic Apperception Test. I. Weiss, Kim Altman. !T. Reznikoff, Marvin. III.
Title.
I SBN 0-87630-944-9
Copyrighted Material
To Drs. Lon Gieser and Wes Morgan-
two dedicated TAT historians.
Copyrighted Material
Copyrighted Material
CONTENTS
Introduction ix
Test Administration 5
Interpretation 13
Th eo ry 13
Scori ng Systems 14
Main Themes Technique 15
Nomothetic and Idiograp hi c Interpretat io n 16
Areas of Interpretati o n 17
Rules and Ca ution s in Interp retat ion 25
vii
Copyrighted Material
viii Contents
Research Findings 51
Pmtocol # 1 76
Protocol # 2 80
Sample Protocols 95
Protocol # 1 95
Protocol # 2 98
Protocol # 3 102
Protoco l # 4 104
References 107
Index 115
Copyrighted Material
INTRODUCTION
The purpose of the present text, as its name implies, is to provide the
student with a short, manageable, but also reasonably comprehensive guide
to the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) and other apperceptive tech-
niques.
As the present authors have stated elsewhere (Aronow, Reznikoff, &
Moreland, 1994), the TAT, like the Rorschach, provides essentially idio-
graphic rather than nomothetic information. Thus, it is quite effective in tell-
ing the clinician how the subject views the self and the world in his or her
own unique way. It is less effective as a psychometric procedure, provid-
ing a nomothetic measure of traits. For this reason, we prefer the term
technique when dealing with projective devices, reserving the term test for
those instruments that are primarily nomothetic (e.g., Millon Adolescent
Personality Inventory-2, MMPI-2). Rossini and Moretti (1997) have cor-
respondingly remarked on the resulting tendency of TAT applications in
clinical practice to abandon both any psychometric approach or scoring
system.
The present text will cover both the well-known TAT and the Children's
Apperception Test (CAT), and will also foray into the somewhat lesser
known cousins of these techniques that have come into the testing arena
in recent years. Subject populations that will be considered include chil-
dren, adolescents, and adults. Some scoring systems that have been put
forward will be discussed but, as we will state, we do not view scoring
systems for apperceptive techniques as time-effective. Their usefulness is,
thus, principally limited to research on apperceptive techniques. This book
will also give special attention to cross-cultural issues and the application
of apperceptive techniques to minority populations.
The text is organized as follows: Chapter 1 will discuss the history of
apperception techniques, with Chapter 2 devoted to test administration.
ix
Copyrighted Material
x Introd ucti on
Copyrighted Material
A History of Apperception
Techniques
Th e pre hi story of projective techniqu es goes back quite far, including works
of th e ancient Greeks on st imulu s a mbiguit y, comments by Oa Vin ci on
the a rti sti c u se fuln ess of di sce rning obj ects in th e Illud or the e mbers of J
fire, and Shakespeare's co mm e nts on "cloud int e rpre ta tion ."
For in sta n ce, in th e 15rh ce nlUry, Leonardo Oa Vin ci quoted Botlicelli as
stat in g that when a spo nge full of various colors is thrown aga in st a wall, a
blot is produ ced in which figures of people, various animals, a nd so on ,
may be pe rce ive d. Oa Vinci sugges te d th e use of pe rce ption s such as th ese
lor a rti sti c in spira tion (cited in Zubin , Emn , & Schumer, 1965) . He sta te d :
Don't take Ill y a d vice li g htl y w h e n 1 adv ise yo u , eve n though it m ay a ppear
bo rin g to stop a nd gaze a t wa ll spo ts, or a t th e ashes in th e fire , in the cloud s,
or in th e Illud a nd a t similar things; yo u will, if yo u consider it caref ull y,
discove r in it Jllany wo nd e rful thin gs. For th e pa inter's spirit is a rou sed to
n ew thin gs b y it, be it in compositioll of batt les, of animals a nd m e n , or in
th e va ri o u s co mpo sition s of la nd scapes a nd of unu sual thin gs su ch as d ev il s,
a nd t h e ir lik e, w hi ch are ca lcul a te d to bring yo u hon or. Through th e ind e-
scribab le a nd indefinit e thin gs, the spirit becomes awa ken e d to new discov-
eri es. (Da Vinci, quo ted in Zubin et a I. , 1965, p. 167)
Copyrighted Material
2 A Prac ti cal Guid e to the TAT
Binet aJl(] Henri, the founders of modern intelligence testing, are usu-
ally given credit as the originators of the scielllific approach to projective
te chniques, using inkblots in the study of visual imagination (Binet & Henri,
1896). Binet and Henri also lI sed children's reaction s to pi ctures as m ea -
sures of int e ll ect.
The TAT was first de veloped by Murray and hi s covvorkers at the Harvard
Ps yc hological Clin ic, having first been described by Morgan and Murray
(1935). The term apperception was chosen in view of th e fact that subjects
don't just perceive, rather, the y construct stories about the cards in accor-
dance with their persona lit y characteristics and th e ir experiences (Ander-
son, 1999). As contrasted with the Rorschach, the TAT ha s usually been
regarded as providing Illore structure to the subject. As Murra y noted in
th e test m a nual, the original procedure required two on e- hour sessions
with 10 ca rd s used in eac h session. Those cards used in the second session
were chosen to be mort' unusual, with subjects asked to give free pla y to
t hei r imagi nat ions.
Th e re ha s been controversy about the eMIl' history of the TAT in that
Christiana Morgan was, at the initial stages, given the first authorship on
the TAT. Th e controversy \vas magnified beca use of the long, conlliClual
relation s hip between Morgatl and Murray. Douglas (199 3 ) has suggested
that the downgra ding of Christiana Morgan in the authorship of til l' TAT is
anexampic of male chau v inislll, though ll10st current authors view Murray
as lhe prim <lry lorce behind the TAT (c .g., M()rgall , 1995). There is gCllcra l
agreem e n r that the id e il lor t he TAT l'merged from a n in -class quest ion
asked b y on e of Murray's undergra duatt' students. The stud e nt reported
th a t h e r son, when ill, had spent the d ,lY making up stories about pi ctures
in maga zincs. This mother w()nliered if pictures might be used in a clini ca l
se lling to re lease fama sy material (Douglas, 1993).
The TAT quickly becam e one ()f the most popular a nd so m et imt's tile
most popular proj ective technique in the clilli cian's armamentariulll. Obrzut
and Bol ick (1986) describe thematic picture techniques as "lhe 1llost wide-
spread projectiv e technique s use d with children and adolescents" (p. 176).
Similarly, Piotrowski and Keller (1984) lound that clini ca l progrilill direc-
tors Illost often cited [he TAT as the projective test with which tra ined
psychologists should be familiar. Lubin , Larsen, and Matara zzo (1984) and
Watkin s, Ca mbell, Ni e berding, and Hallm a rk ( 1995) have likewise re ported
the instrument's continuing popularit y among projecti ve rec hniqu es in a
variety of situations and populations. The TAT and simi lar themaric in-
struillents have been round to be frequent ly used for ilssessmenl in cross-
cu ltura l resea rch (Ret ieL 1987) . A good grounding in the interpretarion of
a pperce ption techniques thu s appears highly desirable .
Th eTAT a ppea rs to ha Vt' rccei vell be t t er a ccep ta nce in t h e scient i fic com-
Illunit y than, lor example, the Rorsc hach. It s hould a lso be noted lhat the
Copyrighted Material
A Hi story ot Apperceptioll Techniques 3
Copyrighted Material
Copyrighted Material
Test Administration
D Materials
The advantages of the TAT include the relative simplicit y of admin istra -
tion and the ease with which the materials are able to be transported. The
exam in er needs a set of TAT cards, an 8W' x II" pad of paper, a writing
Copyrighted Material
6 A Practical Gu ide to th e TA T
in strum e nt to record responses, and poss ibl y a n audio recorder (if desired).
A clirboa rd may be usdul to the exa miner for holdin g and discreetly re-
cmd i n g res po n ses.
Setting
Rapport
Befo re beginning any test adm inis tra tion proced ures, it is s uggested that
the examiner ask th e s ubj ect if he or s h e knows the purpose of the tes tin g,
in order to better understand the Illeaning th e test procedure h as for the
indi v idu a l. The exami n er sho uld clear up mi sco n ce pti o n s regarding ho w
th e test results wi ll be used and a n y confidentiality issues relevant to the
situ at ion , and endeavor to esta bli sh proper rapport through presenting a
reasonably friendly atti tud e . The eXa llli l1 t'r cou ld poss ibl y begin by talking
brielly about some non-threatening topi cs.
Copyrighted Material
Test Administration 7
Som e resea rch e rs and clinicians ordinarily use th e male se t of TAT cards
with men, and th e fe male set of TAT cards with women, because it is
beli eved that on the TAT examinees identify more with stimulus charac-
te rs of the sa me sex as themse lves, and therefore produce Illore projective
mate rial wh e n the stimulus figures match th e ir own sex. Kat z, Ru ss, and
Overholser (1993) conducted a study to test this assumption, however,
a nd their results do not support it. A Significant e tfect of gender wa s not
found for amount of fanta sy, amount of affect, intensity of affect, or le ngth
of story for any of the cards administered. Thi s study does not recommend
th e use of separate sets of TAT cards for men and women .
Clini cians often choose to administer cards tha t are re lated to their h y-
potheses about the subject's probl e ms. Howe ver, it is inadvisabl e to ad-
mini ste r only cards that th e exa min e r be lieves will have particular "pull"
for the individual subjecT, ba sed on s uspected areas of conflict (Rosenwald,
1968). Thi s practice results in a n unre prese n tat ive or bia se d view of the
subject. Rapaport , Gill, and Schafer (1968) suggest that the ca rds that do
not have as mu ch personal m ea nin g for the subject ma y provide a baseline
perform a nce lor eva luating th e more personally relevant cards.
Hartman (1970) asked 90 psychologists to rank th e ir choice of cards,
wh ich resulted in th e following recommendation for a STa nda rd set of eigh t
TAT cards: 1, 2, 3B M , 4 , 6BM, 7BM, 8BM, and 13MF. Thi s se t ha s been
critici zed (Bellak & Abrams, 1997) beca use all of th ese cards a re m ea nt lor
l11 e n, a nd none a re mea n t pa rt icula rly lor women. Bella k suggested s ub-
stituting card 7GF for card 7BM, and adding cards 9GF and 10 to create a
more bal a nced ba sic se t. In our experience card 12F may be far more use-
ful and provocative than card 10, the lalte r ofte n eliciting stereotyped,
unrevea ling th e me s. Other cards may be added if the examiner believes
that they ma y yield fruitful mate rial specific to the subject's conflicts.
The cards are usu a lly a dmini ste red in numerical order. However, a study
by Terry (I (52) indicated that th e ord e r of administration does not have
an impact on the e motional tone, outcome, or level of response of th e
s tori es .
Although it is not one of th e 1110re cOlllmonly used ca rd s (Hartlllan,
1(70), Ka h n (1984) suggested that the bla n k ca I'd, # 16, is a very use-
ful card to use beca u se it is completely neutral, simple, and culture-free.
Other cards have been criti cized as being overly n ega tiv e, too compl ex for
children, or oriented toward the middle cla ss . Th e blank card is not ame-
nabl e to such criticism, and on the contrary, is the ideal unstructured
projective stimulus. Because of th e absolute lack of external stimulus, one
ca n be assured that the re sponse is based completely on internal factors,
and therefore is lik e ly to be meaningful.
Problems with th e u se of the blank card can be attributed large ly to poor
instructions for its use . Kahn (1984) suggested the following instructions
Copyrighted Material
8 A Prac ti ca l Guide to th e TAT
as the blank card is give n to the subj e ct: "This card is diffe re nt from th e
others. This time you get to mak e up th e picture, too. Te ll m e a complete
st ory based on your picture ju st lik e in the other cards, and te ll me what is
h a ppening, what le d up to it. and what will happe n in th e e nd ." These
clear and simple instructions emphasize the similarit y be twee n th e blank
card and th e other cards, and remind th e subject to produ ce a stor y rather
than a d esc riptiv e scene. It is reco lllm e nd ed th a t th e blank card be admin-
istere d last. which highlight s th e speci a l nature of the task. " Whit e
respo n ses," such as "a po lar bear in th e snow," a re considere d rej ection s
of th e ca rd , and may re fl e ct hostility toward th e examiner and the pro-
ced u re.
,~,
Instructions
The a bove in structions a re simplifi ed sli ghtl y for childre n and adu lt s of
limited ed ucation.
We suggest that th e exam in e r eliminate, "This is a tes t of imagination,
one form of inte llige n ce," and "Your tas k wi ll be to make up as dramatic a
sto ry as you can." Th e fiv e -minut e tim e limi t instruction ca n a lso be re -
moved. It is fe lt that these a re unn ecessa ry and m ay be di stractin g. If the
su bj ect ta lks too lon g, then it would be accep table for the exa min e r to ask
him or her to fini sh up and go on to the n ex t card.
Th e in structions shou ld be re peated on request. Parts of the instruction s
may have to be repea ted late r on, su ch as reminding the subject to te ll you
th e characters' thoughts and feelings, or to create an ending for th e stor y.
After repeating a spec ific in struction on severa l cards, the exa miner ma y
choose to cease to remind the subj ect 01 missing pa rts 01 th e story. How-
ever, it should be note d which pans of th e in structions mu st be repeated
pe rsistently, as thi s gives information regard ing what is difficult for th e
subj ect and can be quite m ea nin gful.
Copyrighted Material
Test Administration 9
Record the stories and any other utterances verbatim, including pauses,
questions, off-the-record remarks, and additiona l verbali za tions that may
not be meant to be parr of the story. If the subject speaks too quickly, then
it is acceptable to ask him or her to speak more slowl y so that you are ab le
to record the stories word for word.
Rosenwald (1968) suggested that the responses be recorded by hand,
and as part of th e instructions the subject shou ld be told to speak slow ly so
that the examine r is able to write it all down. Rosenwald asserts that the
subject's compliance with this request will yie ld useful information about
his o[ her empathy and sell-res traint. This information would be lost by
the use of a recording device, or by allowing the subject to write down his
or her own stories.
Asking the subj ect to speak slowly so that you can reco[d ve rbatim is
preferable to asking the subject to repeat his or her story when you fall
behind. because it is common that the subject will alter stories on repeti-
tion (Rapaport et al.. 1968). If a subject asks what you are writing down,
or if you are writing a ll their remarks, respond truthfully that you are
recording the slOries and other remarks as they say them as part of the
standardized administration procedure.
Baty and Dreger (1975) conducted a study to compare three common
methods of recording TAT protocols: machine recording, subjects record-
ing their own stories, and the examiner recording the stories. The stories
yie ld ed were contrasted in the degree to which they revea led personality.
Seventy-two undergraduate subjects were administered the TAT on three
occasions, each time utilizing a different recording method. While th ere
were no significant differences in mean number of content categories u s-
ing the three methods, it appeared that a great deal of material was lost
when the examiner recorded the stories by hand. It was concluded that
recording by machine and by the subject are the most efficient methods.
Machine record in g. of cou rse, requires lengthy transcription and may miss
Copyrighted Material
lO A Practi ca l G uide to th e TAT
Be h a vioral obse rva tion s should b e re cord e d. Bod y la ngua ge, fa cia l ex -
press ioll s, posture, hand IllOVe ll1 e nt s, tone of voi ce, pa u ses, att e ntion , and
motiv a tion a ll provid e inform a tion u se lul to int er pre ta r ion of th e
stori es .
D urin g TAT admini stra tion , s ubjects will oft e n as k th e exa min e r qu e s-
tion s to re duce the a mbiguit y of th e situ a tion. Th e ex amin e r s hould ta ke
care th a t hi s or h e r a n sw e rs do not lead th e s ubject and ruin th e int e nd e d
a mbi g uit y import a nt to th e purpose of th e tes t. Wh e n as ke d for g uid a n ce
in crea tin g respon se s, tlw e xaminer s hould a n swer a mbi g uou sly, usin g
sta tem e nt s s u ch a s "as you wi s h ," "as you think bes t," a nd "th e re arc no
ri ght or wrong a n sw e rs." Ove ra ll. it is bes t lor the exa miner to say nothin g
t hrou ghout th e tes tin g unle ss J bsolutel y n ece ssa ry, in ord e r to a v o id le Jd-
in g the subj ect.
II a s ubj ect's ve rba li za tion s a re simpl y d escripti ve of th e pi cture, re mind
th e s ubj ect 0] th e qu es tion s to he a nswe red b y th e stor y: whJt is h a ppen-
in g, chclfacte rs' thoughts a nd Ieelin gs, w hat le d up to it , how it J II turn s
OUI. It lll ay a lso help to remind th e s ubj ect th a t h e or s h e is to make up a
stor y th a t move s be yond the pi cture per se . Descripti o n s ma y be vi e w ed JS
refu sa ls (R a paport e t J I., 1968 ). The s ubj e ct might be gentl y remind e d th a t
th e ex amin e r is int e res te d in a stor y plot. not ju st a description o f th e
picr u re .
If th e s ubj e ct goes on too long b y be coming o ve rl y d e ta iled, th e exa m-
in e r m ay as k th e subj ect to m a k e up a n e nding lor th e story, a nd s ugges t
th a t th ey go on to th e n ex t one. Th e exa miner m ay te ll th e s ubject t hat th e
stori es n eed not be so long.
If a s ubj ect te ll s onl y short con s tri cted s tori es , th e exa min e r mi ght Js k
him or h e r to tell a lon ge r stor y, or to " let go " more (Be ll a k & Abra m s,
1997, p . 58 ). A h esita nt subj ect mi ght be prompted b y say ing " So?," "W e i!? ,"
Copyrighted Material
Test Administration 11
Inquiry
Copyrighted Material
12 A Prac ti ca l Guide to th e TAT
Testing Children
When admin istering the TAT to a ch i Id, t here a re some specia I consider-
ations to keep in mind. The examiner should be more concerned with
es tablishing sufficient rapport with a child than with an older individual,
beca u se children may be slower to warm up to a stranger. Becoming overly
authoritarian is also to be especia ll y avoided with children. The exam in er
should take great care to gear hi s or her language to the cognitive leve l of
th e child, in order to enhance rapport and to help the child understand the
instructions for the task at hand. The examiner should be sensitive to the
fact that children have a lower aHe ntion span than Jllan y adults, and may
need a break in th e middle of administration .
On the other h a nd, the same instructions m ay be use d with chi ldren
and adults. If an inquiry is used, illlmediat e rather than delaye d inquiry is
reco mm e nd e d with ch ild ren. We suggest that the TAT shou Id not be ad-
mini ste re d to subjects under the age 0\ 5 (though a very mature 4 -year-
old Illight still be a test subject).
There has been concern in contemporary te sting that the race or the racial
match of the examinee and exa min e r h as an e ffect on test results. A l-
thou g h thi s issue has not bee n thoroughl y investigated , we have found no
compe llin g evidence to support this concern.
One study found that race of s ubject. race of exa miner, racial match
between subject and examiner, and race of stimulus figures do not have
an impact on assessment of need for achievement and power using the
TAT (Lefkowitz & Fraser, 1980) . This study failed to support the common
assumption th a t racially congruent stimuli or examiners increase id e ntifi-
cation, and thu s impact TAT responses, influencing productivity.
Copyrighted Material
Interpretation
D Theory
13
Copyrighted Material
14 A Practi ca l Guide to the TAT
common factors underlying the var ious stories (both their content and
form) and behavior observed during testing, and translating these themes
into meaningful exp lan ations of the subject' s personality .
Scori ng Systems
Copyrighted Material
In terp retation 15
Copyrighted Material
16 A Practical Guide to th e TAT
Copyrighted Material
In te rpretati on 17
a dul t m ales and s ubsequ e ntl y for a dult fem ales (Eron , 1953 ). Norm s for
th e m es a re a lso not e d accordin g \() a ch eck li st o f more t han 100 th e m es.
Th ese norm s a re qui te de ta il e d . Howeve r. usin g n o m o th e ti c d a ta a lon e
lea ves th e ri sk of losin g a ll m ea nin g ful da ta a bout a reas not addressed in
th e norm s. Anoth er limit a ti o n is th a t re ly in g on nomoth e ti c da ta m ay e n-
courage a n overe mphas is o n di scre te e le m e nts ra th e r th a n a n int eg ra te d
pe rson a lit y pi cture, a nd on e m ay " lose th e forest lor th e trees."
Th e idiographic a pproac h in vol ves exa minin g th e individu a l s ubj ect 's
record a nd d isce rnin g m ea n in g from th e pa rti cul a I' respon ses he or she
chose . Durin g thi s kind o f a n a lys is, th e exa miner asks him- or h erse lf,
"Wh y w ould a hum a n be in g say th a I. out o f a ll th e possibiliti es that e x ist?"
(Ka ron , 198 1). Und e rsta ndin g t he person a li zed story is th e cr u x of int e r-
pre ta tion. This t ype of m e thod a llo ws th e exa miner to m ax illl a ll y pe rson-
a li ze th e writt e n re port, doin g ju sti ce to th e indi v idu a lit y of res pon ses .
Howeve r, w ithout the g roundin g pro vid e d by good norms, so me illle rpre-
ta tion s m ay be of I-tra cK .
Both nOllloth e ti c a nd idiogra phi c m et hod s should be u sed in TAT inter-
pre ta tion. Th e d eg ree of unu s ualn ess of th e res pon se a s we ll as th e idio-
sy ncrclti c m ea n ing to th e s ubj ect are both importa nt pi eces contributin g to
a thorough int erpre tat ion . Compa ri son to norm s is u seful in puttin g th e
re spon se in broa d er pe rs pec ti ve, pa rti cul a rl y for th e beginnin g exa min e r.
Howeve r, eve n w h e n th e ge n e ra l plot Ill ay be common , th e d e ta il s a re
o ft e n di stin ct ive, per ha ps by th e choi ce of ve rb s a nd a dj ecti ves, or by usc
of proper na m es a nd d a tes (Ro se n wa ld , J 96 8). Co m m on th e m e s ca n be
u sed in idiog ra phi c ways. Th e compl ex it y a nd indi v idu a lit y of idiog ra phi c
int e rpre ta tion , a long w ith reg ul ar comparison to norm a ti ve d a ta, yields
th e most compre hen sive a nd va lid TAT inte rpret a tion s.
Areas of Interpretation
Copyrighted Material
18 A Practica l G uide to th e TAT
ing a reas should , howe ve r, be a t lea st considered during int e rpre tation.
Th e followin g are sugges tion s a s to wh e re e xa min e rs should fo c us th e ir
atte ntion as th e y are con Sid e ring respon ses to TAT assess m e nt. Th e areas
of inte rpre tation are brok e n into three ca tegori es : story cont e nt, story stru c-
ture, a nd be havior obse rvation s.
Story Content
Exa miners should interpret th e actu a l content of tile stories subje cts te ll in
res pon se to th e TAT ca rd s. Th e mo st fruitful mate ri a l for interpre tation
u suall y comeS from fo cusing atte ntion on th e hero or m a in characte r of
t h e stori es, th e rel a tion ships be twee n ch a racte rs in th e stori es, th e m a nn e r
in whi ch th e e nvironm e nt is de pi cte d , a nd th e outcomes of th e stori es .
The Hero
Th e e xa min e r can disce rn much inform a tion a bout th e subj ect by exa min-
in g th e h e ro of th e stor y. Th e h e ro ca n be identifi ed by lookin g for th e
ch a ra cte r most spok e n or. th e on e who se feelin gs a nd s ubj e ctive notion s
a re m os t di scu ssed, th e on e w ho initi a tes importa nt a cti viti es, or th e fi g ure
w ith w hom th e subj ect se e m s to id e ntify. [fit is uncl ea r, th e hero is usu-
a ll y th e fi gure most rese mbling th e subject in a ge, se x, or other ch a racter-
istics (Be ll a k & Abra m s, 199 7) .
Th e n eeds and drives of th e he ro are lik e ly to be re la ted to th e subj ect' s
n ee ds a nd dri ve s. For exa mpl e, a h e ro w ho is a mbitious a nd work s pe rsis-
te ntl y m a y indi ca te th e subj ec t's n ee d for a chi e ve m e nt. A hero who see ks
th e compa n y of oth e rs Jll ay re vea l th e s ubj ect's nee d for a ffili a tion ; on e
who destro ys prope rt y or hurt s peopl e demon stra tes a stron g aggress ive
dri ve . Th e direction of a gg ress iv e dri ves (out or in) is a useful pi ece of
information . For exa mpl e, a hero who sma shes a sculpture would be as -
sum e d to direct hi s or he r agg ress ion out wa rd , w he reas o ne who spee ds
hi s or h e r ca r into a tree would be a ss utll e d to ha ve inn er- directed agg res -
sion.
Th e be ha viors o f th e he ro a re usua ll y a mi xture of be ha v iora l n eeds a nd
w ishe s or fa ntas ies of th e subj ect. It is import a nt but some tim e s q uite d if-
fi cult to tr y to di stin g ui sh betwee n la te nt n eeds a nd ove rt be havior. Th e
ps yc holog ist m ay Li se biog ra phi ca l or ba ckg round inform a tion a nd be h av -
iora l ob servatio n s to help di stin g ui sh be twe en th e m. Th e exa min e r ca n
a lso look to cha ra cte ri stics o f th e h e ro to di sce rn how acce ptable the subj ect 's
needs a nd drives a re to th e se lf. More obj ec tion a ble ch a racteristi cs a re a lso
Copyrighted Material
Interpretation 19
Copyrighted Material
20 A Prac ti ca l Guide to th e TAT
Object Relations
The figure s in the stories represe nt the subject (the hero or id e ntification
figure) and oth e r important figure s in his or her life , such as parents , chil-
dren, sa me-sex peers, love objects, siblings, and so m e times oth e rs. Atti-
tudes toward , and relationships with, parental figures are us ually see n in
ca rds that show older and younger figure s together. such as 2, 5, 6BM,
7BM, 7GF. 12L and sometimes 8BM, 6GF, and 12M (Rapaport et aI., 1968).
Subjects us ually identify with th e younger-looking s ubj ects on these cards
because parental figure s remain so significant in the su bject 's psychologi-
cal world (Rapaport e t al.. 1968). Relationships with sex ual partners are
u suall y revealed in cards 4, 10, 13MF, and sOllletimes 5, 15, 17GF, 3GF.
and 6GF (Rapaport et aI., 1968).
The exa miner should look at the rl'lation ships in the stories as depic-
tion s of the subject's own tend e ncies in relation ships. To what ex te nr are
the relationships free of maladaptive elemcnts more appropriate to child-
hood situations t ha n presl'lll ones ? Are relat ions cha ract e ri ze d by u nre -
solved conflicts, hostility, imIllaturit y, or sadomasochism? Are rclation s
gratifying to adult sex ual. aggressive, and ego needs? Does the hero gener-
a ll y react to others with submission, autonomy, empathy, discomfort,
e nthusi as m, conflict. or noncomplian ce' What are th e hero's emotional
n.:'actions to others ' How does th e hero anticipate others will treat him '
The most pathological situation is a protocol t hat depicts a complete ab-
se n ce of social relations.
Through the attitudes and e ncounters 01 the hero, the exam iner can look
at th e s ubject's view of the world. This is becau se th e subject's memori es
of the past inlluence his or h e r lutulT e x pectation s and color his or h e r
interpre tation of card stillluli. One should keep in mind such questions as
the following: Does the out sid e world foster rhe hero's development or
hinder his Of her growth? Does th e environmelll act as an obstacle or
facilitator of th e he ro' s goals? [s it fri e ndly or hostile ? [s the h ero comfort-
able in his or her e nvironm e nt or in conflict with it ? Look for what forms
environmelll'al obstacl es take (such as other pe ople), their freq ue nc y, and
their strength.
How the subject's stories are resolved revea ls pot e ntially important intor-
mation a bout th e s ubj ect. Degree of optimism or pess imi sm is demol1-
Copyrighted Material
Interpretati o n 21
stra ted b y th e d egree to whi ch rh e storie s have happ y or unh a ppy e ndin gs .
Rea lit y tes tin g a nd fa ntasy livin g a re revea led b y th e a ppropri a te n ess of
th e e ndin gs that, not a typi ca ll y, have wish - fulfillin g compon e nts. Se n se of
compe te n ce is sho w n b y how s u ccessful th e h e ro is in te rm s of reachin g
hi s or her goa ls, fulfillin g hi s or h e r need s, a n d resol v in g confli ct.
Wh e n int e rpre tin g co nt e n t o f the TA T, th e exa min e r should we igh
hea vil y th e d eg ree to wh ich th e stor y is con siste nt w ith th e ca rd stimuli.
Thi s ca n be don e b y compa ri son to norm s, a nd to a lesse r d egree by re ly in g
on th e exa min e r's o w n ex perie n ce a nd a ppe rce ption . Beca use there a re
thought to be thre e contributors to th e response-th e ca rd stimulu s, the
tes tin g e n v ironm e nt, a nd th e s ubj e ct 's inn er world a nd ex pe ri e n ces-
th e less th e stimulu s see m s to contribute, th e more th e oth e r tw o compo-
n e nt s a re likel y a ddin g. Ka ron ( 198 1) h as suggeste d th a t th e less the re -
spon se "fit s" th e card , th e more m ea nin g ful it proba bl y is; th a t is, th e more
idiog raphi c inform a tion it pro vid es . Th e refore, t he more proj ection th a t
see lll s to be taking pl ace, th e more th e respon se should be we ig h ed during
in t erpre ta t ion .
In th e precedin g secti o n , interpre ta tion of story collle n t w as di sc ussed
prim a ril y from th e sta ndpoint of idiogra phi c inte rpre ta ti o n , but nonn a ti ve
compa ri son for th ese a reas is a lso a pprop ri a te a nd d esira ble to put respon ses
in conte xt. Th e conte nt of res pon ses ra n ges from ste re ot y pi ca l to ori gin a l,
a nd is u s ua ll y a mi x, som e wh e re in betwee n . A record th a t sho w s the
a bilit y to see thin gs as oth ers co lllmonl y see th e m a nd th e a bilit y to be
c rea ti ve delllon stra tes go od ego-fun ctionin g of th e s ubj ect. In ge n e ra l,
st imuli to whi c h s ubj ects react with bl a ta ntl y di scord a n t res pon ses a nd
infre qu e nt th e m es a re li ke ly to h ave person a l m ea nin g for the s ubj ect a nd
dese rve a ddition a l a tt e ntion in int erpre ta tion .
Story Structure
Copyrighted Material
22 A Practi ca l Guid e to the TAT
Copyrighted Material
Int er[)retiltion 23
ilnother's arms? Or does the hero in the next story ex perien ce an even
more terrible e nco unter? Looking at the TAT stori es in rei a tionlO th e ones
before and alter th em is cal led seqllt'l1ce analysis. This gives a n indi ca tion of
th e sub ject's defenses. Of reaction s to stress .
TIll' sllbj ect's defenses Ciln also be re vea led in unusual types 01 story
st ructure; for example, responding with short descriptions 01 the pictures
ra ther than storil's, cOlll lll c nt s on pictures themselvcs (e.g., that they a re
out of date), or excessively detailed stori es . The examiner should a ttempt
to discern the type of defense used, and the adequacy 01 th e defenses in
wa rding ofl anxiety or depression, possibly by lookin g at the respo nse to
the following card elnd by observing the subject's behelvior and nonverbal
cO llllllunica tion. Dcil'nse:; that interfere with perfo rilling th e TAT assess-
Illent lllel Y be vie we d as in co nsistent with adaptive fun ctioning. For
cxample, avoidance (possibly a defe nse aga in st ex press ion of a nge r) th a t
pre ve nt s th e subject Iroillm a kin g up stories on th e TAT may a lso be illter-
Icring with the subject's co mmuni ca tion in personal rel a tion ships in li fe
out side th e test ing situation.
The icngth elnd timing ()f storie s should be considered indi ca tors of sub-
ject conflict or prcoccupat ion. The average sto ry is 100 words told in elbout
3 minllt es, a nd is begun aftt'!' the stimlilus card is preselHl'd for a ppro xi-
Ill a tel y 20 seconds (Rapelporl et a I. , 1968) . Significa nt de via tion s from these
averages Illa y point the exam iner to potential areas or con ce rn. The exa m-
iner should look lor inter- elnd intra- indi vidual incon sistencies (Rapaport
et a l. ). Int e r-individu a l in co nsisten cies (nomothetic) include pallerns that
consistl'lltly diner frolll average. These may revea l stable characteristics of
the s ubjecl. For example, conSistently quick res pon se tim es Ill ay indi ca te
impul sive ne ss, w hile long pau ses may dClllonstrate dLicnsiveness or slow
cog niti ve pro cess ing . Intra-indi vidu a l incon siste ncies refe r to differe nces
in res ponse cO lllparcd to o thn sto rie s by the sa llle suhje ct. For example, a
particul ar ly IOl1 g story lllay reveal el ll a rea of preocc upa tion. When th e
psyc hologist notes 10 which cclI'c1s th ese deviations occur, interpretatiol1s
ca n take th ese e xce pt ions into accou nl.
The subject's beila vim durin g tes ting is ass um ed to rdle et hi s or ill'!'
thought s, k clin gs, a nd allx ieties evo ked in response to the testing situa -
tion a nd vclI' iou s card stimuli. Accordingl y, by ca relull y obst'rv ing the
subject's 110n vcrba i and verba l behavior, va luable information lll ay be
obtained regarding the subj ect's rea ct ion s to th e vclI'ious situ a tion s depicted
in the cards, and th t' testing si tu a tion it se ll.
Copyrighted Material
24 A PrZlctical Guide to th e TAT
The qUJlity of the subject's interZiction with the eXZlllliner can be re-
gMded as a sJmple of his or her illlerpersoncli relations in general. The
attitude toward the exallliner, the JlllOunt of eye contJct, friendlin ess,
Jnd JppropriJlt'lless of the interaction should JII be noted Jnd used as
corroborJtive eviclence lor pJttcrns noticed in the story contcnt.
COlllmcnts outsidc of thc TAT stories shollid be consicieredl1latcrial for
interprctation. Statelllents of sell-denigriltion or self-doubt, such as "Oh, I
won't be good at this, l'lll not creiltivl'," refil'ct the subject's view of his or
her own cOlllpetence. Subjects of tell vl'l"bali/e their reactiolls 10 panicular
cards . For exampil', thcy lllil)' stilte whether thc y like or dislike the scenes,
whether they are relevallt or irrcicvilnt to their own li ves, ilnd if they
consider till' piClures dated. Sllch COllllllcnts Ciln bc cOllsidercd defensive
statelllcnts, becallse the subject is basically avoiding creating a story. In
addition, declilring that the pictures Me irrelevant or old-fashioned is a
WelY of distancing oneself frolll the stories. This is an indi cation thilt the
particulilr stil1lulus causes ilnx iety ill th e subjcct, or thilt unilcceptabil' lcel-
ings or drives arc threatening to surface in response to that cMd.
Subjccts l1lay illso dCl1l0nstrilte their anxiety in respollse to the stillluills
through nonverbal be hilvior. The examiner should tJke cMelu l note 01
when thc subject blushcs, hites his or her nilils, seems restil'ss, smiles,
frowns, exhibits mannerisllls, illtl'l"s fluenc y of speech, c hanges affect, and
so on because th ese often indi ca te Mt'ilS of conllict lor thc subject. It should
be kept in lllilld th at words llla y helVl' difkl"l'llt meanings depending Oil
the tone with which the y MC delivl'l"eti. Of particular note is when a subject's
words and the cmotionillit y behind tile words seelll to convcy confli cting
l1lessages. For exalllple , "Hc really loves his parents!" spoken w ith a cyni-
ca l ring lll elY inforlll the eXillllinn th a t SOllle confl ict exists. Perhaps the
subject hates his parents, perhaps the SUbjl'ct harbors guilt about his lccl-
ings toward hi s parents; the particlliar interpretation shou ld be based on
th e context of the statel1lent in tl'l"lllS or hilckground inforlllation and other
test responses.
Perhaps one of the clearest \-VelyS to ohtain information about the subject
is by observing th e degree to w hich the subject complics with thc given
instruction s to tell a story, including what is happening, what led up to it,
what the characters are thinking and lceling, ilnd how it all turned OUI.
The ll10st COIllll10n olllissions are thoughts and feelings of the still1ulus
characters (Rapaport et ill., 19(8). Compliance with in structions rellects
th e personality organization and general adequilcy of functioning of the
subject (Dana, ( 985). The examiner shou ld note the cards for which
the subject gave an incomplete respon se, and which questions (e.g.,
what the characters are thinking, leeling, the outcoll1e) are omitted . It also
should be recorded if cOlllpliance was obtained through prompting by the
exa miner.
Copyrighted Material
Interpretation 25
Exam iners should keep the following guide lines in mind when interpret-
ing TAT stor ies. They will a id exam iners in making interpretations that
come va lidl y from the data. These guidelines are based on ones presented
in a previous book by the present authors concerning the Rorschach Tech-
nique (Aronow et a I. , 1994).
Be Conservative In Interpretation
Exam in ers should he cautious rather than hold when interpreting TAT
stories. When in doubt about an interpretation, the examinc r sho uld pre-
fer to err on the sid e of conservatism, rather than going out on a limh. It is
better not to say anything than to say sOllleth in g th a t is in va lid about the
subject. Related to this guideline abo ut being conservative in interpreta-
tion is the preference to err on the sid e of health rather than to err on the
side of pathology. Essent iall y, be carefu l not to go beyond the data when
making statelllents about an indi vidual.
Copyrighted Material
26 A Practi ca l Gui de to the TAT
Copyrighted Material
InterpretJtion 27
Rather th an attach in g too much sign ifi cance ro a n yone response, exam-
in ers shou ld look lor co rro bo ratio n among stories, and among different
areas or int erpretatio n (con tent, structure, a nd behavior observations).
In terpretat ion s made with this gu id e lin e in mind are lik e ly to be more
rel iable and va lid . Occas io n a ll y, howeve r, there a re except ion s where a
sin g le response or behavior is so st rikin g that it warrants in terpretat ion
based on it a lon e. In th ese cases, it is appropriate to do so, qualifying the
tenta ti veness or the interpretat io n and m e ntioning th e iso lated n at ure 01
th e response.
The rule 01 u sing a confluCIlce of data for interpreta tion can optimally be
served by admini sterin g the TAT as a part of a test battery, rather than in
isolation. II a battery is lI sed, then hypotheses based on TAT data can a lso
be supported or negated by responses coming from other tests that, to-
gether, may create a more comp lete and accurate port rait of the subj ect.
Periodically, a n exam in er shoul d ask other profess ionals for feedback abo ut
the acc uracy of h is or her int erpretat ion o f test material. Fo r examp le, th e
exam iner might approach th e psychotherapist of a subject (if this was agreed
upon durin g inform ed conse nt) to see if interpretations fit with what th e
therapist sees. The examiner co uld a lso ask any o th er professional expe ri-
enced in TAT int e rpretation for reliability checks. Obtaining feedback ca n
help the exam in er avoid misinterpretations and h e lp the psycho log ist be -
come aware 01 his or he r own blindspots.
Copyrighted Material
28 A Practica l Gu ide to th e TAT
Psycho logists should take into accollnt the con text in w hi ch th e teSTing
took p lace. What e n viro nll1 enta l f,l Clors Illay h,lVl' influ enced the responseQ
Was th e assessment cond u cted in a hospital? Was it done by a stranger?
Was the room comfortable,) The potential illlpact of th ese factors shou ld
be considered.
The degree of ge n t'r,lii /ation inllul'nces till' valid it y 01 your conclu sion s.
The more specific th e co neiu s ion , the ll1ort' dillicult it is to ('onlirm and the
easier it is to in val id a te. The Illore genera l the coneius ion , the more li ke ly
it is to be va lid (Piotrowsk i, 1950). For examp le, il a s ubj ect tells a story in
wh ich a ho y smashes the v io lin his parents make ilim pl ay, the interpreta-
tion would be more lik ely to be correct il it sai d , "The subject Illay have
aggress ive tendencies," than il it sa id , "The subject breaks objects w h e n his
pMenTs act con trollin g."
Beg inners tend to interpret stories too litl'i"lil y (Ste in , 1981). For ex-
aill ple, on cclrd 7GF, th e subject tells J story in w hi ch the litt le gir l does not
WJnt to stay ins id e and li sten to thl' hook heing read, but wishes she were
outside taking a wa lk in th e s u nshine. One shou ld not a utom at ically as -
sUllle the subject prl'lcrs walking to rl'<ld illg, unle ss ev id ence Imlll o th er
stories supports thi s. In stl'cld, one Illa y in tcrpret th at th e s ubject is dissatis-
lied with her c ur rent activ iti es ami wislws fo r change. The feelings and
,lt tilllci es expressed by th e subject Me ty pil\l ll y more vcl lid than th e par-
ticular stimulu s objects mentioned lll'cause th e kl'iings are more lik e ly
project ion s, as op posed to the stimulus objects that Me more obj ectivel y
presented in the pictures.
Psyc hologi sts s hould take (Me not to unduly weigh t the first Icw stories
read. It is poor practi ce to form hypotheses Irom the first lew cards and u se
laTer stories to gat her confirmin g evideIlce w hil e ig n ori n g d isconfirill in g
evidence. All cards shou ld be read for possible n ew hypOTheses and a ll
data s hould be taken in to considera ti oIl (lub in c t a I. , 1965). Simi lar ly,
p syc hologists s hould not be undul y swayed b y the iniTial appearance and
behavior of the subject.
Copyrighted Material
inte rpre t,ltion 29
When int e rpre ting TAT st ori es told b y c hiILin.' n , on e IllUst co nsider the
c hild 's ilge, developillent a l status, and verbal abi lit y . Younger age is gener-
a ll y ilssociated w ith lo wer norillilli ve illlpui se co ntrol. less sophisti cate d
a nd proper la nguilge, and less Illilturity in pee r relations. Filillasy li v ing is
more ilcceptilble in c hildren than in il dults. Overilii. the range of what is
con s idered "n orlllal " is somewhilt greater in c hildre n. Exa miners more
fa miliar with assessing ildults s hould be careful not to ovcrpatllOlogize the
resp on ses of children and should remember lh a t ildult norm s are inilppro -
priate for assessing th e commonality of respon ses in c hildre n. With c hil-
dren, it is eve n more impo rt a nt to consider the tes tin g context a nd behav-
iora lob servat ion s.
Method of Interpretation
Th e following is our reco illmend ed proced ure for interpreting TAT tes t
Illat e ri a l. III sUlllmary, illt e lVITt a lioll is ba sed on ver bil tilll reco rdin g, s um-
m aril ing the storie s, d e termining which aspects of till' stori es a re Illo st
sig nifi ca nt. organizing the IllJteri a l by a rea s of s ubj ect pe rsonillit y, co m-
pJri so n to norlll S, Ill a king h ypoth eses. and discerning m ea ning abo ut til e
indi v idu a l s ubje ct.
Copyrighted Material
30 A Prilcti ca l Guide to th e TAT
2. Writ e sUlllm a ry sta tl' lllent s 0 1 rill' Ill <l in th e lll es lor eac h stor y. NOl es
m ay be m ade ill th e Ill Mgins legMcii ng tenwti ve interpre tive h ypoth -
eses .
4 . Loo k o ve r a li st of CO lll1l1OJl a reas to int er pret . such as the Oll l'S pre-
se nted ill thi s cha pte r. 10 ,]Vo id Ie,w in g (l ut import a nt indi ca tors 01
pe rson a lit y.
S. Compare t() Ilorm s a nd not e UIlU SlI ,l l ()CC UITe n Cl'S o r lac k of th e m .
6 . Lookin g a t hi g hlight ed ma tL'lial. lll <lin the1l1 es. story stru cture. a lld
lll' ha v iora l obse rva tio 1lS. \lot l' re pl' titioll S o f th e me s. confli cts. a nd
react ion s.
8 . Illl eg ra tl' ciille rl'nt as pects o f th e indi vidu a l's pn son ,l lit y into a m ea n-
in gful pi cture th a t C,l n be d es nib l'll in a re port .
Copyrighted Material
Stimulus Characteristics
of the TAT Cards
31
Copyrighted Material
32 A Pra cti ca l Guide to th e TAT
a critical point. Fewer individuals in the hospilJli zed groups gave extreme
outcomes as well.
Eron (1950) also found that sex misidentifications of the ligures were
common, with no particular differences evident betwee n the normal and
the cl ini ca I groups. The overinterpretat ion of sex misiden ti fications is, thus ,
something to be avoided. Howcver, Eron found that misidentifications in-
volving variables other than sex werc Il10rc COIl1mon in the genera l hospi-
tal group. Other differcnces that were found include thc normal subjects
offering Illore thellles of belongingncss and also of rUlllin ation .
In a table, Eron (1950) report cd on the "stiIl1u latory va lli e" of TAT cards
(i .e., the number of themcs e li cited by thc cards). The rank ordering was
as follows (from most thcmes to least thellles elicited): I 3MF, 20, 188M,
68M, 38M, 4, 12M, 15, 7BM, 10, 178M, 8BM, 9BM, 14, 5, 2, L 19 , I L
and 16. However, even the least stimulatory card, card 16 (the blank card),
elicited fully 199 thellles.
In 1953, Eron published a study utilizing fcmale subjects via college stu-
dents and a group of pregnant women. Eron again reponed the lllost com-
mon themes for TAT ca rds.
[n a 1972 study, Murstcin investigated thcstillluluspull of TAT cards for
a group of introductory psycholog y college students. In this study, Murstein
found that the stud ents' stories tcnded to be prcdominantly sad in terms of
what is happening and why it is happcning, but that the stories tended to
havt: happy e ndings. Considt:rablt' variatioll was fOlilld among the cards.
Thu s, cards 2, 8GF, [L and 14 were found to be predominantly neutral for
th e "what" and "why" categories, whereas 38M, 3GF, 13MF, and 18BM
were highly negative in these respects. Cards 8BM, 10, 12M , and 18GF
were predominantly positive.
GolcHried and Zax (1965) used semantic differential ratings of male and
female co ll ege students to d es cribe the characteristics of the TAT cards
(with the exception or the blank card).
In a 1962 study, Weisskopf-Joclson and Foster teste d whether having
humans or anima ls as the protagonists in pictures produced greater emo-
tional productivity in stories, and a lso whether colored or black and white
cards produced more productive stories . In this study, kindergarten Stli-
dents were the subjects. Modifications of four CAT cards were made to
produce the human and color conditions.
The authors found that neither the human or a nim al variable nor the
color variable affected the emotional productivity of th e cards. However,
there was a tendency for subjects with low transcendence scores to tell their
most productive stories to anima l and black and white pictures, with high
transcendence subjects tending to tell their most productive stories to chro-
matic pictures. Transcendence refers to going beyond the obvious stimu-
lu s qualities of the cards.
Copyrighted Material
Stimulu s C haracteri sti cs oi the TAT Cards 33
In a 1950 stud y, Weisskopf found th at co ll ege -age s ubj ects are sig nifi-
ca ntl y more produ ct ive when responding to pi ct ures about pa rent- child
relationships a nd a bout h e te ro sexua l re lat ion sh ips among contemporaries
th a n to pi ctures abo ut other lOpics.
The following is a sum ma ry of th e general a ppearance of th e TAT ca rd s,
what in our e x pe ri e n ce a re common them es, and wh a t lh e resea rch lit e ra -
ture see m s to indi ca te about them.
Copyrighted Material
34 A Practical G uide to th e TAT
Card 1
Card 2
This card presellts a rural sce ne w ith a young girl in th e foregro und hold -
in g a book, a preg na nt wOlllan watching, and a Illan laborin g in a field in
the bac kground. According to Morgan ( 1995), thi s ca rd had it s origin in a
pailltillg don e by the a rti se Kroll.
In our expe ri e nce , th e m os t CO IllIllOIl th e lll e is one of the young
girl desiring to continu e her education a nd leave th e farm , cont ra ry to
th e desin:,s of th e family. Sometimes there is an allian ce desc ribed be -
twee n ce rtain membe rs of th e fa mil y aga in st other members th a t ca n be
rev ea lin g.
Ero n 's 1950 stud y found th at for males th e I110St frequ e nt story is mod-
e rat ely h appy and th e most freq ue nt outcome is ha ppy. More subjects
shift to happy rather th a n sa d co nclu sion s. The three most frequent th e mes
are occu pation a I concern, as pi rat ion, a nd eco nomi c press Li re.
Eron 's (1953) stud y with fe males found th a t th e three most co mmon
themes are asp ira tion, occupation al co n ce rn , and pa rental press ure. The
se mant ic diflerential stud y found thi s ca rd to be lack in g in stron g stimulu s
quality.
Copyrighted Material
Stimulus Chara cteri stics of th e TAT Ca rd s 35
Card 3BM
Card 3GF
This CMel portrays a Iemale figure standing next to In open cloor. One
hJnd is clutching her face, which is clowncJst. Morgan (1995) has lillie 10
say about the provenan ce of this card. In our experience this ca rd, like
38M, strongly elicit s depressive themes. Eron's (195 3 ) study with femJIe
subjects [ound th e most common th e m es for this ca rd involved th e deJth
or illness of a child, pressures from J partner. or In unre quited femJIe
pMtner.
The selllJntic differential studies with 3GF come out similar to those
with 38M, with sadness, hope lessn ess, a nd unplea sJ ntn ess prominent. As
with 3B M, the eXJminer should be carelul not to overint e rpre t negatively
tinged stories to this cani.
Copyrighted Material
36 A Practi ca l Guide to th e TAT
Card 4
This card portrays a woman grabbing the shoulders of a man while the
Illan is turning away from her. There is a distant picture of another woman
undressing in th e background. Morgan (1995) indicat ed that thi s card comes
from a color illustration.
In our experience, the woman is usually perceived as trying to restrain
the Illan from hostile action or tl1l're is a love triangle involving th e figure
in the background. Eron's (1950) study with male subjects found that emo-
tional ton e ratings are neutral to mildly negative. The most fr e qu e nt
outcome is moderately happy. Only rare ly will subjects shift to sadder con-
clusions. The three most fre quent thellles are pre ss ure from a pa rtner,
succorance from a partner, and competition.
The Eron (1953) st udy with females found the most common themes to
be succorance from a partner, departure from partner, and nurturance to
partn e r. The se mantic difterential ratings of this card paint st rong and defi-
nite reactions to it, involving the card being described as impulsive, ag-
gress ive, and dangerous.
The nature of how male and Iemale relations are see n can be portrayed
very graphically on this card.
Card 5
Thi s card portrays a n older woman looking into a room from the doorway
with a look (of surprise?) on her face. Morgan (1995) reports that this
drawing was originally done by Christiana Morgan.
Feelings involving the mother are often elicited by this card. Wh e re she
catches the child mi sbehaving, it is inte restin g to note how she handles
it-I s she und e rstanding? Does she invoke guilt?
Er()Jl'S (1950) study found that th e most frequ e nt story to this card is
n e utral to mod e rately nega tive, with the most frequ e nt outcome neutral.
The three most frequ e nt th e mes involved curiosity, pressure from parents,
and illicit sex.
Eron's (1953) study with temale subjects found the three most fre quent
themes to be curiosity, parental press ure, and concern on th e part of the
parent. Semantic diffe re ntial stud y of thi s card yields ambiguous results.
Copyrighted Material
St imulu s Character ist ics of the TAT Cards 37
Card 6BM
This card portrays an older woman in int eraction with a somewhat yo unge r
mak. Morgan (1995) reports that this p ict ure lik ew ise had it s origin in a
drawing by Christi ana Morgan.
In our ex pe ri ence, common themes in vo lve brin g in g the mother bad
news or informing the mother that he (the son) is leaving or moving out.
Ero n' s ( 1950) st ud y found that most stories to this picture are sad, with
n e utra l stories rare. More than h alf the s ubjects sh ift to h app ier conclu-
sions. The three most frequent them es are pressure from parents, depar-
ture from parents, and marriage of a ch ild. Bo th male and female subjects
in the semantic differential study viewed this card as sad and unpl easant.
This card is often referred to as the mother card for male subj ects. It is
thus int erest ing to note the complexity of feelings between th e male and
female figures, whether she is designated as his mother or not.
Card 6GF
Copyrighted Material
38 A PrJ cticJI Guide to the TA T
Card 7BM
This card fea tures an older man looking at a younger man who is not
returning the look. Morgan (1995) hJS little to S,lY of the origin of thi s
ca rd. In our experience, this card typically elicits stori es of an older man
giving advice to a younge r man. The 1950 Eron stlldy found that th e most
fr equent story to this card is n e lltralw moderatel y sad. The most frequent
outcome is moderately happy. Then:, are many shilts to happi e r conciu-
sic)!1 s. Th e three most frequent themes seen were succorance from a par-
ent, pressure twm pare nt s, and OccupJtional concern.
Semantic dilfere ntial study or this card suggested an ambiguous appear-
ance. Thi s card has bee n des crib ed as the lather card for male s ubj ects and
in our ex perience is sliccessflli in eliCiting feelings toward the father and
other mall' authority figures.
Card 7GF
Copyrighted Material
Stimulus Characteristics of the TAT Cards 39
Card 8BM
This card depicts a young boy in the foreground with an operation l<lking
place behind him. The barrel of a rifle is visible at one sid e. According to
Morgan (1995), this card had it s origin in an illustration that accompanied
a short story and pictured an emergency appendectomy.
In our experience, common s\"Ories to this card center arollnd amb ition
or an operation scene. In Emn's (1950) study, it was reported that about
ha lf of a ll stories were moderately sad, with about half of all outcomes
moderately happy. There is a frequent shift to the happy direction. The
thre e most common themes seen were aspiration, war, and death or ill-
ness of a parent. The semant ic differential study suggested a somewhat
amb iguous stimulus.
It is not unusual for Oedipal conflicts to be seen on this card, with the
s ubj ect identifying with the adolescent boy. How these confl icts arc played
out can be quite interesting.
Card 8GF
A woman is seated and stJring off into space. According to Morgan (1995),
this card originated as an oil painting currently at the Metropolitan Mu-
seulll of An in New York City.
In our expe ri ence, this is a vague card th at does not yield particularly
i11l erest ing stor ies. The three most common themes reported in Eron's
(1953) study of female subjects involved happy reminiscence, aspirat ion ,
and occupational concern. The semantic differential stud y genera ll y found
a positive tone for this card.
Copyrighted Material
40 A Practical Guide to the TAT
Card 9BM
This card depicts four men lying down against each other in a field.
According to Morgan (1995), this card originated as a photograph with
Western-style clothing more prominent in the photograph.
In our experience, this card typically elicits stories involving a hobo theme.
Eron (1950) found that the most frequent story is neutral in tone. The
three most frequent themes were found to be retirement, exhaustion, and
vacillation. The semantic differential study suggested a lack of specific stimu-
Ius qualities.
Card 9GF
This card features two female figures. One of them is standing behind a
tree, watching the other run along a beach. According to Morgan (1995),
this card originated as an illustration to accompany a serialized novel.
In our experience, this card usually elicits a story involving conflict be-
tween the two female figures, usually over a man. Eron's (1953) study
with female subjects found that the three most common themes involved
escape from a perilous environment, curiosity, and jealousy. The semantic
differential study found both male and female subjects viewing this as an
unhappy card. Themes of female peer competition are often seen on this
card. The card may also tend to elicit paranoid themes because of the ele-
ment of the one woman being observed.
Copyrighted Material
Stimulus Characteristics of the TAT Cards 41
Card 10
Secn on this card is a young woman's head leaning against a man's shoul-
der. Morgan indicates that this card originated as a photograph. In our
experience, this card usually involves a story about a heterosexual relation-
ship. Eron ( 1950) found that the most frequent outcome lor this story was
moderately happy. The three most frequent themes seen were content-
ment with a partn e r, nurturance to a partn e r, and departure from a
pa n ner.
The Eron (1953) study with female subjects found that the three most
comlllon themes were contentment, death or illness of a child, and
nurturance to a partner. The se mantic differe ntial study found this to be
rated as one of the happi es t cards.
The quality and fabric of heterosexual relationships are often cl ea rly
seen in stories to thi s card. If both panies are indicated to be male, this
Illay be indicative of hOlllosexual orientation in mal e subjects.
Card 11
Copyrighted Material
42 A Practical Guide to th e TAT
Card 12M
This card depicts a m a n w ith hi s h a nd raised and a boy lyin g down with hi s
eyes closed. Morgan (1995) indi cated onl y th at thi s card a ppeared with
so m e variations in ea rli e r sce n es or t h e TAT.
Emn (1950) found that th e most frequent s\()ry lor thi s card is Illod e r-
a te ly sa d , with stori es rare ly h a pp y . The three most freq u e nt th e m es were
h ypnoti s m, religion, a nd illn ess or death of th e ce ntral ch a ract e r. Th e se -
m a nti c differential study sugges ted few strong stimulu s chara cte ri stics for
thi s card.
In our ex perien ce, this card ca n be parti cul a rl y useful in elucidating a
patie nt' s possible reaction to psychotherapy, th a t is, th e nature o lth e re la -
tion s hip th at h e or s h e is lik e ly to es tablish in thera p y.
Card 12F
Copyrighted Material
Stimulu s Charac teri sti cs o f th e TAT Ca rd s 43
Card 12BG
Card 13MF
Copyrighted Material
44 A Pra cti ca l G uide to th e TAT
Card 138
Thi s ca rd di spl ays a youn g boy silling on th e doorstep o f a log ca bin. Ac-
co rdi ng to Morga n ( 1995), thi s ca rd has it s origin in a photogra ph e nt it led,
" Mr. Abe Lin co ln , Jr. " The cro pped print of it b y Murray focuses more
a tt e ntion on th e child .
In our ex peri e n ce, how childhood is viewe d is often revea le d in stories
to thi s card a nd , so m e tim es, th e m es of loneli ness a re see n . The sema ntic
differential stud y of thi s ca rd indi ca tes th a t th e stimulu s ch a ra cte ri sti cs a re
not stron g or pronoun ce d.
Card 13G
This ca rd depict s a littl e girl climbin g a w inding fli g ht of ste ps. Morgan
(1995) indi ca ted that thi s ca rd ori g in a ted as a photo of the ph o tograp h e r' s
wife, who wa s pe tit e .
Like th e prev iou s ca ret th e m es of c hildhood a nd lon e lin ess are [ound in
stori es to thi s card . The se manti c diffe re ntial st ud y indi cates no stron g stimu-
lu s prope rti e s for thi s ca rd.
Copyrighted Material
Stimulus Characteristics of the TAT Cards 45
Card 14
Card 15
This card portrays a gaunt man with clenched hands standing among grave
stones. Morgan (1995) indicated that this card originated in a woodcut
print by an American illustrator.
Eron (1950) reported that the most frequent story to this card was ex-
tremely sad, with the most frequent outcome sad. The three most com-
mon themes found were death or illness of a partner, religion, and death
or illness of a peer.
Eron (1953) found that among female subjects, the three most common
themes were intra-aggression, religion , and death or illness of a female
partner. A very sad quality was indicated in the semantic differential study
of this card.
In our experience, this is not one of the more useful cards. It is so struc-
tured that it almost always produces death themes.
Copyrighted Material
46 A Practical Guide to the TAT
Card 16
This is the blank card in th e series. For best results, it should be adminis-
tered as the last in the series. Stories are highly variable-some are quite
revealing, with others revealing lillI e or nothing about the subject.
Emn (1950) found that the most frequent story is ncutral in tone. The
three most common themes werc ioundto be favorable environment, war,
and contcntment with a partner.
Emn (1953) found that among female subjects, th e three most com 111 on
themes were pa ren tal contentlllen t, asp i rat ion, a nd aggression from the
environment.
Card 17BM
This card shows a (naked?) lllan climb in g a rope. This card is described by
Morgan (1995) as a drawing by Tha i based on an unfinished skctch by
Daumier.
III our experience, frequent themes in volve at hl et ic events or escape
from a dangerous situ at ion. Because of the Iludity, themes in vo lvin g
homosexual idcation may be obtained from latent homosexual male
Sli bjects.
Eron (1950) found that th e most frequent story to this card was neutral
to moderately happy. The thrce most frequcnt" themes were self-esteem,
exhibit ion, and compe ti tion with a peer. A neutra l to moderately happy
view of this card was suggested by the seillant ic difierential study.
Copyrighted Material
Stimulus Characteristi cs ot the TAT Carcls 47
Card 17GF
This card shows a fe male figure leaning over a bridge's railing with tall
buildings and small figures of mt'n in the background. Morgan (199'5) in-
(licated th ell this card originat e d as a woodcut.
In our experience, this is not on e 01 the Illore useful cards. An exception
is that individuals may see the ligure as contemplating or preparing to
cOll1lllit suicide by jumping off the bridge .
EnlIl (19'53) lound that among female subjects the three ll10st com III on
themes were suicide, ordinary activity, and curiosity. The scmantic dillcr-
ential study lound this card to be low in stimulus propertics.
Card 18BM
This card shows a Illan being clutched fwm behind by three hands. Mor-
gan (199'5) indicated tha t this card originat e d in a drawing by Christiana
Morgall.
E mn (19'50) found that the most frequcnt story to this card is neutral to
moderately happy. The three Illost frequellt themes were found to be sclf-
estcem, exilibition, and co mpetition with a peer. A Ilcutralto moderatcly
happy tont' lor this card was suggcstcd by the semantic diflerential study.
In our expcricncc, drunkenness and attitudcs toward it is a cOlllmon
thcmc lor this card. Paranoid individuals Illay also rca c t to it as rcpresent-
ing an "attack (rom behind."
Copyrighted Material
48 A Pra cti cal Guide to th e TAT
Card 18GF
This card displa ys a WOlllall with her hands aroulld the throat of another
WOl11all, with a flight of stairs seen. Morgan (1995) indicates that this im-
age was redrawn by ThaI. possibly frolll all carlin photogrclph.
Emn (1953) lound that aillollg !elllak subjects, the three most COIlll110n
themes for this card were found to be succorance from a parellt. parental
pressure, and death or illness of a child. A Ilegative valence for this card
was suggested by the semantic diflel'Cnti,ll stud y.
In our experience, hostile interactions with other feillale figures tend to
be highlighted in stories to this carll. whether Oed ipa l in nature or not.
Card 19
This card presellts a "weird" piC! ure of cluuds o\'n a slluw-covered cabin.
Morgan (1995) reported that this card is based on a watercolor painting
that was dOlle by a painter who, at the tillle, was suffering frulll severe
depression and ha Ilucinat ions.
[n our experience, this is not olle of the Illorl' usdul cards. The Emn
(1950) study found that the most frequent story to this card is neutral to
Illoderatel y sad. The Ihrel' 11I()SI lreljucill lilcilles seen were aggressioll
frolll an impersonal source, contclltlllcnt ell 11Onll', and vacillation.
[n the ErOIl (1953) study with Icmak subjects, the tlllTe most COllllllon
Ihemes were aggression frolll the ellvironlllClll, contentmellt with a par-
ent. and the supernatural. In the seillantic diflcrenti ,ll study, !elllaks had
a sOlllewhat more negative illlpressioll of this card than did males.
Copyrighted Material
Stimulus Characte ristics of the TAT Cards 49
Card 20
Copyrighted Material
Copyrighted Material
Research Findings
Research stud ies that have utili zed th e TAT are q uite numerous. The present
ch ap ter w ill co n cern it se ll w ith areas that have been researched in some
depth and have sign ifi cant clini ca l re levance.
David McCl e lla nd 's need for ach ieve ment resea rch has bee n one of the
more famou s and creat ive efforts in TAT resea rch. McClelland a nd his co -
workers somew hat narrowe d Murray's definition of th e nAch to, essen-
ti a ll y, des ire to do a good job; that is, to atta in a standard of exce ll ence
(McC le ll and eta\., 1953; McCle lland, 196 1a, 196 I b).
McCiella nd used seve ra I TAT pi ctures, bu t a Iso included stimuli specia ll y
designed to e li cit nAch o In struct ion s were a lso often modified to enhance
nAcho It should be noted that McCl e ll and assumed that nAch is learned
rat h er than inn a te. Further, McClelland found th at nAch , as he measured
it. did not co rre late wit h subj ects' own se lf-d escripti ons as to the stre n gt h
of such a Illotive in themse lves (McCl e ll and, Koestner, & Weinberger, 1989;
Spang ler. 1992) .
NAch ha s a lso been found 10 have biological corre lates. Th u s, th e arousa l
51
Copyrighted Material
52 A Practic:ti Guide to th e TAT
Copyrighted Material
Researc h Fi nrl ings 53
Ih l' la rgl'1 in a rin g -lOss ga m e (M cCle ll a nd , 1958), a nd coll ege s ubj ecls
choosin g geom e tri ca l p uzzll's w ilh diffl're n t deg rees of dilli cull Y (W e in er,
1970) .
Siudi es of n Ac h h ave of len bee n int e rt w in ed w ilh a differe nt, th o u g h
rel a le d , moti ve-w h a I has bee n ca ll ed th e fear offailure (FF) . Joh n A Ik in son
h as bl'e n m os l id e nlifi e d wi Th Ihi s bo d y o f resea rc h . Atk in so n a nd co -
w ork e rs (e .g., At kin son a nd Fea lh e r, 1966) h ave act ua ll y d l've lope d m a Th -
e m a li ca l lormul al' Ih a l cO lllbin l' n Ac h, FF, a n d th e in ce ntivl' va lu l' of Ih e
silU J li o n in pre diCi in g be h av ior . Alkin son h as fo und Ih a l s ubj ecls wilh
hi g h Il Ac h J nd lo w FF w ill g ra vira te IO w Jrd "easy" lasks. Thosl' w ilh lo w
n Ac h J nd hi g h FF w ill prde r diffi cull tas k s (pres um a bl y, th e hi gh FF s ub -
jecls ca n bl a m e fa ilure o n a diffi cult task o n th e las k il se lf).
I I
1 .. .1 Hosti I ity and Aggression
Copyrighted Material
S4 A Pra ctical Guid e to th e TAT
ing out. alld high-school bo)'s with a history of acting out. The number of
aggressive TAT stories told was found to be a good predictor of acting-out
behavior, as was the appearance of aggressive stories \0 Ilonaggressive cards.
The best predictor oj ,lggrcssioll was foulld to be the reaction time oj the
su bjects.
Reznikoll and Oollin (1961) studied the relatiollship between clggressive
stories to the TAT and the EdwMds SociClI Dcsirability Scale. Thcse authors
found tha t high social desirahility (S.D.) subjccts werc more likel y to usc
covert than overt hostility .
.James and Mosher (1967) studied aggression in TAT stories with relcr-
e ilCL' to the stimulus pull llf the cards. Working with Bo)' Scouts, the)'
found that thematic aggression to cards high in such stimulus pull was
related to overt aggressive beh,wior, but this W,lS not the case lor cards lovv
in stilllulus puilloraggressioll. Addition cllly, thcscauthors Jound that there
was a signiJicant ncgative relation ship betwcen hostilit), guilt and aggres-
sive stories given to CMelS with low aggressivc pull. This study is important
ill that it suggests that. at least lor the Illeasurelllcnt oj hostility and
aggression potelltial. one would walll to use cards of high relevance to
aggression .
Hainer and Kaplan (1960) CtJlllparL'(i till' 'lbilit)" oj hoth the Rorschach
Test and the TAT to 1l"Il',lSUre ()\Trt ,lIld Ul\l'rt Illlstility. For IIH:' TAT, overt
alld covert hoslility wCl"e lound to hc Ilcg ,llively related to each ollH:'r. Ir
\Va, cOllcluded that the TAT might be more sellsitive thall the Rorschach
to the ove rt versus covert llature llf hostility.
In ,111 eMly study, Fishn ,1Ild l-lilHl\ (1951) dc,llt with psychiatric P,l-
ticilts ,1ml a normal control grllup . In tlli s study. o Ill' of si x cOlllparisons
reached significallcc, Ilamel y, suicidal sclli/ophrellics werc found to dis-
pia " Illore outward hostility thMI pMalloid subjeL"\s.
In a 1957 illvestigati()ll, Scodel alld Lipetz contrasted three groups 01
schil.ophrcnics on TAT hostilitv. TIll' thrce groups werc cOlllposed of non-
violent subjects, subjecls who had displa)Td external violence, and sui-
cidal subjects. No difkrcnces ill TAT h{)stility wne found.
Purcell (1956), in a very interesting study, investigated difkrt'nces ill
\'arious hostile ,1SI)l'cts of T;\ T stories of threc groups that differed in de-
gree of antisocial behavior. The ver)' antisocial subjects wert' f{)und to dis-
play Illore dircct and ulldisgui sed TAT hostility thall the least antisocial
subjccts.
Haskin (1958) studicd collcge studellts alld ps ychiatric patiellts ill tcrlllS
oft h ei r TAT realistic ilH,,,ressilJII clll d 1111 n:alist ic Ll,<I.qrcs.livlI. Rcal ist ic aggression
was defillcd elS socially ,lcccptahlc, ,lnd elll clppropriate Illcans of expres-
sion; unrc,llistic aggression W,lS dclincd as illclppropriatc or punitive ag-
gression, or aggrcssion turned ag,lin st the sell. This author l(lund Ihat the
normal studcnts showcd signilicalltly 1ll000e realistic aggressioIl, while the
Copyrighted Material
Resea rc h Findings 55
Copyrighted Material
56 A Pract ical Guide to th e TAT
Copyrighted Material
Research Findings 57
Copyrighted Material
Copyrighted Material
Diversity Issues in TAT Use
and Alternatives to the TAT
59
Copyrighted Material
60 A Prac ti ca l Guide to th e TAT
for th e unfamiliarity of the materials to the s ubj ects in their inte rpre ta-
tions and did not tak e into account the subjects' cu ltural fram e of refer-
e nce. Th e refore, th e int e rpre tations likel y over-pa thologized the s ubjects.
Today it is wide ly recognized that cultural issue s must be considered in
order to maximi ze validity of te st results <lIld avoid mi slabel in g individual s
who are tes ted . The American Psychologi ca l Associ at ion 's Ethics Code
(American Psychological Association, 1992) emphasizes that examiners
are obligated to become knowledgeable about and be responsibl e about
test usage and interpretation with rega rd to individual dilferen ces. Th e
following two examp les of e thical codes make thi s responsibi lit y clear:
Copyrighted Material
Diversity Issues in TAT Use and Alte rn atives to the TAT 61
Age Diversity
Ea rl y in the hi sTory of the TAT, before iss ues of cross -c ultLlrJ lu sage were
commonly a ddressed , it WJS recogni zed that different versions of the tec h-
nique could be useful lor diflere nt population s, increas ing th e pot e ntial
for subjects' id e ntification with th e c hJracte rs. The diversity iss ue first ad-
dresse d was di ve rs it y in terms of age. Several of the original TAT cards
depi ct children (ca rd s 1, 7GF, 8BM) and severa l d e pict old e r a dult s (cards
613M. 713M). Alternatives were d eve loped 10 appeal more a nd eli cit more
re le va nt inlormJtion fwm individuals be longin g \0 these age groups. More
rece ntl y, alternative s tillluli have bee n d e veloped for use with different
c ultura l groups.
The idea that it is import a nt for s ubject s to be J ble to id e ntify with the
characters depicted in TAT picrures is not a n ew id eJ . Th e CAT is J tec h-
nique creJted b y Bellak a nd Bellak (1948) as a n alternJtive to th e TAT for
u se with c hildre n . Th e cha racte rs used in the pi ctures arc animals. JS it
WJS belie ve d that children could idelllify more readily with a nimals th a n
wit h people (BeliJ k & Abra illS , 1997). The CAT ca rds depict situ a t ions like ly
to elicit stories reveJ ling d y nami cs central for c hildren, such as keding
isslil's. sibling riva lry, a nd relation ships with parellts.
The CAT is eas ier to u se cross-culturally than th e TAT becau se the ani-
m a l charaCll'l's are re la ti ve ly ambiguous with rega rd to sex and c ulture .
SOllle of the furnitur e and objects, howe ver, are more c ulturall y specific.
Thl'l'e alT Indian (Chowdhury, 1960a ) and Indon es ian adaptations of the
CAT in which the furniture and some of th e animals arc changed to be
more culturally familiar.
Although thl'l'e is litera t ure to support th e idea th a t children more rea dil y
identify with animals th a n with pe ople , it has been lound that mall Y chil-
dren do res polld bell e r to human c haracter stimuli. Therefore, the CAT-H
(Be llak & Bellak, 1965) was d eveloped, a versioll of the CAT with human
figures depicting th e same scenes as th e CAT animals. For a rev iew of
studies comparing th e u se of animal and human c h ar,lc te r stimuli , sec
Bellak and Hurvich (\966); res ult s h ave bee n mix e d.
Copyrighted Material
62 A Practi ca l G uicie to th e TAT
The RATe
Th e RATC (McArthur & Roberts. j <)82) i ~ ,1 Illorc rece ntl y deve lopcd p ro-
jecti ve storyte lling technique lor ch ildrcll ililli ildoiesCCllts aged 6 to 15. It
has several aclva illilges o ver the CAT. il lc luciill g a sta nd ard ize d scor in g sys-
te m for quant ifyin g ildaptivc alld clini cal pl'lso ll a lit y dilllellsions and morc
mod e m pi ctur es. Validit y s tudic s h avc had quitc po si ti ve filldin gs
(Palomares. Crowley. Worchel. Olsoll, Fr Rae, I ()9 1).
Cultural Diversity
It has bccn ,lrgllcd that !'V \lIIT,lY 'S T f\T l,lrtIS il lT IWt ap propri ate luI' usc
with nO ll -W hite 1)()PlIl ,ltiollS. bl' C,lllSl' SOlliC stlldi es have reported th a t Illi-
Ilorit y g rollps res polld Ill'gcltivcly [() th e TAT (rIW lllpso ll b Bac hrach, 1(5 1).
ScvL'rill studies have showll th,lt Sllbjclls rc,lct n]()r,' positivel y to stimuli
show ing eharactns 01 thcir OWll r,lCl'. 13,liil' y ,lll<l Grce ll ( 1977) fOllnd thell
Alrical l Allll'ri cclll subjccts ril ln! ,1 Sl't 01 TAT ca n! s th at dcpictcd I3 lack
charanerc, 1l11 )JT positi vl'i\' th elll till' ()Iigillill TAT c,lrds. Alri cilll Allll' ri can
iclll,lie colil'ge stud cll ts gave longer sto rie'S a lld scorce! hi g her on Ilccd lor
,lcilicvclllell t w hc ll rl'sp()nding \I) nl,lCk . rclt lll'r till'll White, stimui lis char-
ilctl'r S (Cowa ll b- G() ldbe rg . 19(,/).
TholllPSOIl ( 1949) LTC,ltn! a set (11 picture stimlili s ubsti1l1ting HI,lCk l(lr
White l i g ur c~, o thl'J' w isc keeping till' St illiltii as Silllil M ,1S possible to
Mu rray's TAT lMds, ,1IH! lou IIII that till' ,l\ n,lgl' stm ), icllgth of the adul t
mall' Alri can Aill cric,l ll subjects IV,lS Sigililic(ll lll y grl' il ter th il ll ill response
to the origillill ulrds. Hc ill lcr p rl't l'd thi s a,> l' vid l' ll cl' that il1 e lllbn,> 01 mi-
IlOr it y grou ps illT bcttLT ahi<: 10 idl'lltily ilild c ilipa thi ze wit ll l llMilctl'rS
wilo MC ril l icl il y cOllg rlll'll! with till'lll ,>c lvl'S. HOlI'l'VtT, olhers h ,lVC criti -
cized 1hi s illtLTj1ITtiltioll ,1 Ill! hel\'(' Ilot bl'ell ,lbk to rl'pl iGlll' Tilo mpson's
lilldillgs (Korc hill , Mitc hell. h ,\ .lclt/olJ. 1') ')0) .
Dc plll a lld Kimbrough ( 198 2) lOll Ill! th ,lt Alric,l ll Aillcrican ch ildrl'l l I'l'-
Copyrighted Material
Diversity Issu es in TAT Use and Alternatives to th e TAT 63
sponded with more positive themes to the Themes Concerning Blacks (TCB)
cards, which show Black figures, than to TAT cards. The children selected
mostly TCB cards as the cards they liked best, and were more likely to
choose TAT cards as ones they liked less. The TCB responses had mostly a
positive feeling-tone, but were not longer than the TAT responses (Triplell
(J Brunson, 1982). However, it should be noted that because there was no
comparison group of White children in either of these studies, it is un-
known whether the positive themes and card prefere nces are due to racial
cOllgruence of the subjects and stimuli, or are Illore silllply attributable to
something e lse about th e TCB stimuli as compared to the TAT.
Alternative techniqu es have bee n developed ill respon se to the argu-
mClll that Murray's TAT stimuli ma y not be optimal for lise with minority
subjects. Wherea s Murray's pictures present White characters, the new
stillluli typically represent more diverse populations, or populations of the
specific culture or race to be assessed using that test, and often are se t in
situations more familiar to non-White individuals. The main argument for
the development of new lest stimuli is that subjects respond more readily
and more validly when the y are maximall y able to identify with the stimulus
characters and the situational contexl.
It should be rell1emtwred, however. that the assumption that increased
similarity between subject and stimulus characters results in increased iden-
tification has not been empirically proven. Neither ha s the assumption
that greater identilication results in beller assessment results. One of the
few studies specifically in ves tigating the relationship betwee n similarity of
subjects cll1d card charact e rs and degree of projection did Ilot support the
utility of iIl cn.'a sed similarity. TATstories of 52 college students were scored
lor projection. defined as amount ur ra nt asy, allluunt or arfect, intensit y or
arlec!, and length. No Significant increase in projection as a result 01 match-
ing the sex of subjL'ct to the sex of stimulus figures was found (Katz. Russ.
5· OVL'l"holser. 1993). Although gender is a different variable than ract' or
culture. OIlL' should not simply aSSUll1e that similarity betwet'Il subjects
and stimulus characters a utomaticall y increases test utility.
It also rem a ins unproven whether more identification with stimulus char-
acters necessaril y facilitate s the usdulness and validity of the test result s.
Takcn to the extrcme, this logic would lead to the suggestion that the best
techniqu e would be to ask subjects to create a story with a lllirmr as cl
stimulus (Korchin. Mitchell, & Meltwfl 1950). It could also be argued
that if the charactL'l"s and settings are too familiar. the face validity of the
test will elicit more defenses and be less revealing of illlrapsychic conllic!.
However. accepting the assllmptions discussed above, alternative st imuli
to ,1'v1urray's TAT hav e been dcveloped that are intended to be of increased
usc with a variety of cultural groups. Several of the mort' promising ones
arc presented below.
Copyrighted Material
64 A Practi ca l G ui de to th e TAT
Cho w dhur y ( 196 0b ) develop ed TAT stillluli lor use w ith Indi a n popul a -
tion s. The set is compose d o f 14 ca rd s: 12 adapt ed fro lll th e ori gin a l TAT
pi ctures, and 2 additional on es re pre sentin g th e ille s import a nt to Indi a n
c ulture not re prese nt ed in the ori gin a l TAT, th a t is, jo int fa mil y a nd reli-
giou s fa nt asy. Th e stillluli de pict Indi a n chara ct ers wea ring tradition a l In-
di a n appa re l in similar situ a tion s to the ori gin a l cards . For exa illple, in card
I a n Indi a n strin ged instruill e nt, a Ta npura, is substitut ed lor a violin.
Cho w dhm y admini ste red thl' 14 ca rd s to 260 indi vidu a ls, includin g Uppe r
Cas te Hindus , Lowe r Cas tl' Hindu s, and Mu slim s. Result s indi ca te th a t IT -
SPOIlS l'S we re revea lin g o i the subje cts' inner drives, w ishes, stresses, a nd
a n x ietie s, a nd th e res pon ses w ere found to Ill' con g ru e nt w ith Rorsc hac h
res ult s of th e subj ect s.
TEMAS
TEM AS (C osta Iltino e t a i. , 198 1) was de ve loped as a Illulli cult ura l apper-
cepti ve proj ective techni q ue lor usc w ith minorit y a lld IloIllllinorit y chil-
cir e n . It is a n ac ron ym lo r "Tc ll -M e -A-S tor y" in En gli s h , a nd m ea n s
"tlw m es" in Spa ni sh. Th e re a re tw o pa ra llel version s, each con sistin g of 23
ca rei s, includin g 9-ca rd short forill s. Th e minorit y versioll de pi cts urba n
e thni c minorit y ligures, c ultura l tll c ill es a nd sy mbol s, an d urba n scttings;
th e nonminorit y vers ion show s p re domin a llll y Whit e cha racte rs w ith id e n -
ti ca l th e m es a nd settings. Other dilfere ncl's be tw ee n tlw TE M AS and th e
TAT a rc th a t TE M AS has color pi ctures zll1d th e pi cture s a re less a mbi g u-
ou s.
A de tailed scorin g syste m was developed for TEMAS . Stori es ca n be scored
lor 18 Cogn iti ve Fun ct ion s (s uch as Reaction Tim e, Ima gin a tion , Eve nt
Omi ss ion s ), 9 Perso na lit y Fun ct ion s (in cl ud in g Aggrcss ioll, Self- Con ce pt.
a nd Rea lil Y Tes tin g), ,l nd 7 AIIl'c ti ve Fun ctioll s (e .g., Ha pp y, Fea rful ,
An g ry) . TEMA S was sta nd a rdi zed 011 64 2 Blac k, Whit e , a nd Hi spa ni c
childre n frolll Ncw York Cit y publi c sc hool s. Illtcrra ter reli a bilit y lor th e
Per son a Ii I Y Fun c t ion s ra n g e d I r 0 III 7 5 (Yo - 95 01<, in a sa m p Ie 0 I 2 0
nOllminorit y protocol s (C osta ntino, Ma lgad y, Cas ullo, & Cas tillo , 199 1).
Int c rn a l con siste ncy (lo r th e lon g form ) a mon g cards for spccili c scorin g
va ri abl es h ad a m edi a n va lu c oi .73 lor a Hi spa ni c sa mpl e a nd .62 for a
Black sa mp le of childre n.
Thi s tes t's usduln ess has bl' e n c mpi rica ll y supported . Costa ntino c t 21 1.
( 198 1) reported th a t bilin gua l exam in ers admini ste red six TAT ca rd s a nd
th e n six TEMA S ca rds to 76 Hi spa ni c childre n aged 0- 12 . It was fo und
th a t, OIl ave rage, child re n de mon stra ted grea ter verbal producti vit y (grea te r
Copyrighted Material
D ive rsity Iss ues in TAT Use and A ltern ati ves to the TAT 65
APT
Th e Appe rce pti ve Pe rso na lit y Test (APT; Holm stro m , Silbe r. & Ka rp, 1990)
is a re la ti vel y n ew proj ecti ve story tech ni q u e th a t w as d e ve loped lor u se
with a dol esce llt a nd a dult subj eCTS of a n y racia l bac kg round. Th e APT is
compri sed of a n e ig h t-ca rd se t th a t is a dmini ste re d to a ll subj ects, in con-
trast to th e TAT a n d oth e r techniqu es in which the exa min e r chooses whi ch
Copyrighted Material
66 A Practical Guide to the TAT
cards are to be used. The benefits oj having a constant set of cards admin-
istered are that test results are mOlT t'asily compared with each other,
research is facilitated, and examiner bias is reduced. An effort was made to
create male and female APT characters oj differing ages and ethnicities.
They are depicted in comlllon, modern settings involving a range of po-
tential social and interpe rsonal themes.
A strong advantage of the APT is that it inclucies a single standard scor-
ing procedure. First. the eight cards are administered, with subjects writ-
ing down their stories. Following this administration, subjects refer to th e ir
stories while completing the APT questionnaire, which asks subjects to
answer six questions about each story. The questions ask about the rela-
tionships between characters, fee lin gs and act ion s of the characters, who
the hero is, the outcome of the story and adjectives describing the charac -
ters . The inform ation is coded by subjects from their own stories, thus
there is no interpretation on the part of the examiner (of co urse, tradi-
tional interpretation can also be done with the stories as well). Responses
are th e n compared to norms.
Overall, mean tes t-rete st reliability for APT scores was found to be .75 .
Evidence supporting the validit y of the APT includes relationships between
APT responses and MMP[ results, such as significant correlations between
APT indices of hostility and ps yc hopathology and hostility on the MMPL
and the MMPI depreSSion scail' and the APT score for unhappiness
(Holmstrom et al., [990) . Whl'll compared with the TAT, the APT stories
showed less aggression and less achievl'mellt imagery, but not significant ly
different story outcome ratings. To sUlllmcuize, the APT stimuli are more
re prese ntative and contemporary than those of many other apperceptive
techniques, and can be used in th e traditional manner as wel l as with a
reasonabl y psychometrica II y sou nd sco ri ng system. Th ese prelim ina ry re-
su lt s are encouraging.
Oraw-A-Person as Stimulus
Copyrighted Material
Diversity Issues in TAT Use and Alternatives to th e TAT 67
children in the Southwest and states that this approach minimizes dis-
comfort to the child and requires less interpretation by the examiner, lim-
iting the opportunity for misinterpretation. However, the high face valid-
ity of the technique may increase the impact of social desirability on
responses.
The influcnce of the test stimuli should not be underestimated. When story
responses to the Family of Man photo essay collection, published by the
Museum of Modern An, were compared with responses to Murray's TAT
pictures, it was found that the Family of Man pictures yielded responses
balanced between positive- and negative-roned stories, and higher energy
stories; whereas the TAT yielded more negatively toned stories, and lower
energy stories (Ritzier et a!., 1980). This study demonstrates the im-
portance of the role of stimuli lIsed in projecrive techniques, under-
scores the importance of conscientious test development, and reminds
examiners to consider the realistic aspects of the stimuli perception during
in terpreta t ion.
The common theme in the development of new stimuli for storytelling
assessment techniques is that the characters and the contexts should be
relevant to the subjects. For this reason, it could be argued that there should
be different sets of stimuli for different populations. If stimuli are tailored
to particular racial or cultural groups, great care should be taken to avoid
prejudicial intentions and building harmful stereotypes into the pictures.
For example, th e TAT-Z (Erasmus, 1975), an early adaptation of the TAT
for lise with African Americans, is criticized by Retief (1987) becallse it
was intended to measure attitudes toward White authority. The cards de-
pict situations that would likely provoke angry feelings in most minority
subjects. Interpretation oj the resulting stories could easily be biased.
Retief ( 1987) suggested that new tests should be developed with more
structured stimuli, design ed to assess specific areas of personality makeup,
such as aggression and need [or achievement. Constructs should be cho-
sen that have meaning across differe nt cultures and can be operationalized
in terms of observable behavior. Retief argued that tests that select focus
on specific areas, rather than include as many areas for interpretation as
possible wiliminilllize errors in interpretation. Formal scoring systems could
also reduce misinterpretation.
Sherwood (1957) offered detailed, specific guidelines for designing a set
of TAT cards for cross-cultural use. First. he has several suggestions to
maximize the lIsefulness of the pictures as projective stimuli:
Copyrighted Material
68 A Practical Guide to th e TA T
Copyrighted Material
Diversity Issu es in TAT Use and Alternatives to th e TAT 69
part of the larger society in which the subject's group is emlwdded. Physi-
cal environments that are familiar to the subjects and implied int erper-
sona l relationships that are patterned after relationships commonly l(lund
in the cu ltun~ are also important in creating pictures that are cul tura ll y
appropriate.
It is proposed that the dlecriveness of the pictures as appropriate projec-
tive stimu li can be evaluated by lookin g at the length of e li cited responses;
the relevance of responses to till:' content areas the pictures arc intended
to tap; and the degree of fantasy the pictures generate, measured by the
number oj introduced material, varieties of responses in plot, and t he imagi-
native, creative quality of the responses (Sherwood, 19 57).
It should be mentioned that despite the soundness of Sherwood's (1957)
recommendations, unfortunately there seems to have been minimal ap-
plication of his principles. His paper has only been ralTly cited (Dana, \ 999).
The question arises as to whether one should choose to use Murray's TAT
or to usc one of the alternative techniques, and if so, which olle' The
clearest advantages of using the original TAT is the examiner's prior expe-
rience with the technique, and the wea lth of published data on the tech-
nique. Norms published by Eron ( 1950, 1953) provide examiners with
information w ith which to compare subjects' responses. Examiners' own
exper iences, fol\owi ng many TAT ad mi n istrations, a Iso a !lord much da ta
about the conventiona lit y or originalit y of responses. This sum of informa-
tion is highl y va lu ab le, because va lid TAT interpretation relies on illter-
preting that which is uniquel y reflective of the indi vidua l being assessed.
For examp le, if the majority of subjects attribute viole nt behavior to a
character stimu lus , one would be less lik e ly to interpret such a response as
nl<:'aning somethi ng about an individual subj ect. In order to recognize popu-
la r and original TAT responses, an ongoing acculllulation of knowledge
about the asseSSlllent technique is needed. On the other h and, exploring
prolllising new techniques is th e only way to acculllu late a knowledge
base about them. Research must clearly be done for appropriate popula-
tion norms before the newer tests can be used with any confidence.
Whenever a test is selected for use , the bod y of research evaluating the
test's val idit y shou ld be considered. Allot"her less formal way to evaluate
the uses of a test is to use Sherwood's (1957) criteria, as discussed ill the
previous section, Oil a sample of subjects, looking in depth at the qualita-
tive and qualltitative features of responses by th e subjects.
Copyrighted Material
70 A Practica l Guide to the TAT
Copyrighted Material
Divers ity Iss ues in TAT Use and Alternatives to the TAT 71
tance to the culture of the individual being tested, preferably derived from
the structure and content of stories from a particular culture, rather than
automatically coming from Anglo-American sources (Dana, 1999).
Using culturally sensitive or culturally appropriate tests is not a suffi-
cient alternative to a culturally sensitive, knowledgeable examiner.
Administering the test with social etiquette appropriate to the subject's
culture is recommended (Dana, 1996) and may facilitat e rapport and valid
results . Relevant aspects of etiquette may include appropriate greetings,
eye contact, and body language . To furth e r avoid ove rpathologizing, it is
recommended that d e tails of the circumstances under which the test was
given be recorded and addressed, including instructions given and interac-
tions be tween the subject and the examiner. The influence of language
and translation problems should also be determined. In general, one would
rather "err on the side of health" when interpreting test responses rather
than overpathologi zing or mislabeling a subject. Always consider possible
alternative explanations for unusual respon ses. Cultural diffe rences should
be considered as a salient alternative explanation.
Copyrighted Material
Copyrighted Material
The TAT in Psychotherapy
The TAT has a long asso cia tion with t h e psychotherapeutic endeavor. Combs
(1946) wrote o f the abundance of a utobiogra phi ca l m a teria l th a t is ty pi-
ca ll y co nt a in e d in TAT stori es, thu s providing th e therapist with importa nt
backg round inform a tion a bout test subj ects. In a later swdy , Hoffm an and
Kupe rm a n ( 1990) not ed how discllssioll of re pet iti ve TAT th e ml's in psy-
chotherapy m ay help in sin glin g out a hi stor y of trauma.
Ullmann (1957) found sign ifi ca nt corre la tion s be tween TAT scores of
psychi a tri c patient s on th e o ne ha nd , a nd hospita l statu s a nd gro up therapy
sca le pred ictions on the ot h er. In a 1960 st ud y, Fairweather et a l. u sed th e
TAT to assess th e effectiveness of psyc hoth e ra py modalities. These a uthors
found a signifi ca nt int eraction effect in that group th erapy a nd co ntrol
groups showed more positive TAT ch a nge for non psychotic th an for ps y-
choti c psych ia tri c subj ects while individu a l th erapy produced more posi-
tive TAT ch a nge for lon g-te rm psychotic indi vidu a ls.
The TAT ha s a lso been used fairly extensive ly in outcome studi es in
co nn ect ion with psychotherapy resea rch (e.g., Coch e & Sillitti, 1983;
Dymond, 19 54; Fran k & Gund e rson, 199 0; Goldman & Gree nbl a tt , 19 55).
One line of invest iga tion with relevance to psychoth era p y is th e stud y
of what the TAT m ay assess in terms of th e "leve ls" or depth of personality
reac hed by different measures. Thus, Stone a nd Dellis (1960) found th a t
the TAT reached lowe r levels than an int e lli ge n ce test a nd a se nt ence
comp let ion test, but not as low as the Rorschach or the Draw-A-Person
73
Copyrighted Material
74 A Prac ti ca l G uide to the TAT
I. A roa d m a p 01 d y na mi cs, confli cts, ego stre ngth s a nd wea kn esses, so-
cia l as pects of functionin g, a nd so Oil prior to th e on se t o f thera p y. Thi s
is, or course, the more tra dition a l use to w hi ch th e TAT a nd o th e r pro-
jecti ve techniqu es have bee n put in conn ecti o n w ith p sychoth e ra p y .
Copyrighted Material
The TAT ill Psychotherapy 75
that occurs when th e therapi st sha res Th e int erpre ta tion of responses
with th e subj ect. Thi s last use of projective Tec hniqu es is, of co urse, th e
most demanding in te rm s of clini ca l skill a nd timing .
Copyrighted Material
76 A Practical Guide to the TAT
test data and interpretations with patients. These studies have included
the works of Appelbaum (1990); Aronow and Reznikojf (1971); Craddick
(1972, 1975); Fischer (1970, 1972); Gass and Brown (1992); Roth, Wolford,
and Meisel (1980); and Stein, Furedy, Simonton, and Neuller (1979).
Finn and Tonsager (1992) published a rather unique study in this re-
spect in that it constituted a strictly empirical study of the eflects of MMPI
test feedback. These authors based their feedback on their own "co ll abora-
tive model" of the process they developed. It was found in this study that
subjects given feedback reported a significant decrease in stress and an
increase in self -esteem. Interestingly, these positive effects of the feedback
were found to have increased two weeks aller the feedback was rendered.
[J Protocol # 1
The following are before a nd after psychot hera py TAT protocols from a
19- year-old college freshman, which two o[ the current authors had pre-
viously published (Aronow & Reznikoll 1971).
The client had presented at the university counseling center with symp-
toms of radical personality change, alienation from people, and growing
feelings of unreality. The cli ent came from a large family in which both
parents were alcoholics, with the fatller quite verbally and physically abu-
sive toward him. He was tested at the onset of therapy, and again after
seven months of once-per-week psychotherapy with the first author. Only
selected TAT stories are presented.
First Testing
Card 38M
When the police arrived they had to arrest the poor soul. He was to be
tried for suicide. The D.A. was running lor governor and needed a co nvi c-
tion so they were going to hold this guy for murder in the first degree. The
scene was in the courtroom: "They've just put the corpse on the stand. He
refused to talk and didn't request an attorney. He's been on a hunger
strike, but you bleeding hearts beller not feel sorry for him. He's a mur-
derer. We knew that he was planning to kill himself for SOIlle time now.
So it was contemplated and premeditated. (Pointing to the body) "Deny
that! I dare you! Silence proves your guil1." The D.A. smiles at the jury:
"Sure, he was depressed, but it' s our right to be depressed. Would any oj
Copyrighted Material
The TAT in Psychotherapy 77
you have denied George Washington at Valley Forge his right 10 be de-
pressed? O[ course not! Did he shool himself? No. Can we lei this murder-
ous villain go out of here a free corpse 10 corrupt o ur soil? No." It was a ll
very effective and the jury brought in the verdict of "Guilty." The man was
sentenced to life in the state pen, but they let him go for good behavior.
Summary: A man who comm itted su icide is tried for murder and con-
v icted.
Card BBM
The linl e prince of Eston ia stood by watching the doctors operate to save
the life of a royal guardsman. "Certainly screams a lot, doesn't he'?" the
boy sa id. "He's not made of tough st uff, I guess." The doctor, having lis-
tened to this for the last hour, gripped the knife. He thought to himself:
"Let's have a look to see what yo u're made of. fresh punk." Then he sighed
and conti nu ed his work. The man holding the light was sharing in the
thoughts of the doctor. He too was a guard and knew it cou ld easi ly have
been him on the table. Being a guard to this cruel and unpopular prince
was no easy task. The man on the table asked the doctor: "My chances, my
chances ... " "Poor," came the slow reply. The little prince sneered: "You
don't deserve to speak, I a lmost was killed because of you." The dying
guard mustered hi s la st strength and rose from the table. "Give me that
knik. doctor." "What are you go ing to do?" Before the answer came, the
prince la y dead.
Summary: A dying guardsman murders an insensitive prince.
Card 9BM
In a depression, few men have jobs and these fe llows are no except ion.
Their spiri l to work had been robbed, so they returned to nature. They left
their homes and families in search of something. Who knows? They would
find it down th e road. They quickly spent their "fortunes" and found them-
se lve s hungr y. Five hungry men can be a problem. They knew they had to
eat or starve to death. They finally arrived at a so lution and if you look at
how many men there are in the picture you' ll know. Snake eyes don't
a lwa ys come up, but they sure did this time.
Summary: Hungry men during a depression eat one of their fellows.
Card 13MF
He knew sh e loved him. For over a year they had been planning to get
married. They had never slept together before, but this rime they would.
Copyrighted Material
78 A Practi ca l Guide to th e TAT
As he lay there in bed with h er. sh e said: "I lo ve yo u ." And h e knew it was
tr u e . He thou ght for a lon g mOl11ent. He t urn e d to her a nd slowly h e choked
her. As she died she sa id , "I lo ve yo u. " " I know, and you a lways wil l. " He
ca ll ed th e police and now stands over his love . He had h er love now and
cou ld never lose it. ("Kind of maudlin.")
Summary: Lovers have sex for the first time. and he murders her so h e
ca n n e ver lose her love.
Second Testi ng
Card 38M
Toy guns, a ll in fun, th e poor bo y shot him se lf. His head li es on th e bed, hi s
mother bore him to the bed. For hi s twelfth birthd ay, h e wanted a g un . His
mother bought him one, it wasn't real. "I w ish th is was a real gun," h e' d
often say. One day, h e got his wish and shO! hi mselL Moral: Don't wish
too hard for what yo u want, yo u might get it.
Summary: A boy w ishes for a gun, gets it. and is shot by it.
Card 88M
"Th is barn wasn't a lways an oJlera ting room," th e boy wh ispe red. "Once
there were cows and horses in here. Do you remember, fath e r?" The bo y's
father li es un the table. Moment s ago he had be e n shot accidenta ll y while
hunting. Doctor Sc hre id er. a veterinarian, was fortunately next door and
had come to h e lp . Now illook ed bad. The boy bit hi s lip nervous ly. "Father
w ill li ve, won't h e? I'm s ure the cows don't mind the noise." Hi s father
di e d ju st then. "I saw an old hoot owl in here. We'll have to chase hi m out,
he' s frightening the calves." The doctor t ried to te ll the boy, "Son, yo ur
father is d ea d." Th e boy w inced for a mom e nt, just a fl eeting second, and
contin u ed, "r don't think we' ll have rats aga in th is year. do you, fat h er?"
(I was kind to him thi s time.)
Summary: A father d ies in surgery by a ve terin arian after an accident.
Card 98M
He had just come upon th e scelle. Three bodi es lay in the clearing. He took
ou t hi s pad. "T hat makes seven, e ight a nd nin e. Nin e more we've found
today." He had an easy job as Offic ia l Calcu lato r of Corp i (plural of corpses)
for th e Un ion of Friend ly States. He enjoyed fo llowin g th e army to count
the dead, somehow it remind ed him of go ldfi sh in a bowl. He went on
Copyrighted Material
The TAT in Psychotherapy 79
aways. Suddenly h e sme lled the familiar gas. "Over here," someone ye ll ed.
He became di zzy and co ll a psed. An officia l walked up to him. "A nd th at
makes 10."
Summary: A "body counter" in a war becomes one of th e casua lties .
Card 13MF
Interpretation
In th e first test ing (before therapy) a number of themes are appa re nt. First.
there is a grand iose and removed-From-reality quality to some of the sto -
ries (card 38M, ca rd 88M). Further, there appears to be a strong conflict
with and hostility toward male a uthorit y (same cards). It is a lso unclear
with whom t h e client identifies in the story to S8M, not a healthy sign .
Also o n card SEM, there is grandios ity ("a prin ce") and the fee lin gs of low
se lf- wort h the gra ndiosity covers up (prince of what was then a non-
ex istent co untry). Th e sto ri es to all the cards are creative, but bizarre. Fin-
ishing it otl the story to card 13MF demonstrates an almost total lack of
trllst or abi lity to handl e intimate re lationships .
In th e second test in g (after seven months of psychot h erapy) unhealthy
trends are sti ll present. Thus, a ll stories st ill have an unhappy resolution.
The story to card 98M is sti ll somewhat bizarre (though perhaps not so bad
as canniba li sm). There is st ill anger at male a uthority (card SEM-fathe r
being trea ted by ve terinarian , fat h er'S death, comment of "I don't think
we'll have rats aga in " after fathe r'S death).
Some im provements, though, are worth noting. As ju st ment ion ed,
the stories have a less extreme and less bizarre quality. In addition, some
tenderness toward a father fi gure is expressed (card S8M). Perhaps most
impon ant, story th emes a re tess distanced from rea lity and gra ndiose,
now involvin g dramas between parents and a son (card 38M, ca rd 88M).
Copyrighted Material
80 A Practical Guide to the TAT
The client's clinical picture is consistent with the TAT changes, involv-
in g improvement in symptomatology but a continued need for psycho-
therapy.
Protocol # 2
Card 1
Mr. M.: 11 look s lik e the boy is trying to decide whether he wants to play
the violin.
Mrs, M,: His parents probably told him to practice, but h e wants to go out
to play with his friends. 1 think he'l l decide to practice.
Mr. M.: Yeah-he decides to play the violin, but he's not happy.
Card 38M
Copyrighted Material
The TAT in Psychotherapy 81
Mr. M.: It look s lik e a guy who doesn't know what to do. Should he kill
hilllsell? He's 10m. Maybe his girlfriend just broke up with him.
Mrs. M.: l think that this person, with time, recovers. Nothing drastic hap-
pens-it will a ll end up okay.
Card 4
Mr. M.: I don't see that. I think that so m ething is bothering him and he
JUSt wants to be left alone.
Mrs. M.: Maybe we could just say that she has something sh e wants to
talk to him abo ut and he doesn't want to li sten .
Card 6BM
Mr. M.: It's a guy and his mother-he just told her some bad news. Maybe
the father ju st had a h eart attack and died. They both look very
sad.
Mrs. M.: They both look stunned . There's a lot to be sa id , and st uff to be
done-but right now th ey' re both paralyzed. But they'll get over
it with the passage of time .
Card 7BM
Mr. M.: (After a lon g pause) . I don't see that. It looks more lik e the older
guy is just selfish and thi s kid is go ing to suffer. He hates him. He
JU St wants him to go away.
Mrs. M.: I see it differently-they look like a father and son talking .
Copyrighted Material
82 A Practical Guide to the TAT
Mr. M.: Well, I do. (Mr. M. seems ag it ated and the y do not reach consen-
s us. )
Interpretation
Certa in themes a re evident in this TAT protocol. First, Mrs. M. seems most
interested in their reaching a consensus and in stories reaching a "happy"
resolution. Her desire to reach co nsensus w ith the husband is most evi-
dent Oil card 4, and sh e eve n seeks to do so on ca rd 78M, thou gh here
w ithou t sllccess. Other th emes evideIH in clude Mr. M.'s unhappiness (card
I. card 4, card 78M), the wife's expression of the cur rent paralysis in th e
relat ion ship (card 38M), Mrs. M.'s desire to cOJllmunicate and the hu sba nd 's
reluctance to do so. Overall, an interest in g record, butl10thing really new.
Following this sess iol1 , however, the husband reques ted an individu a l
sessiol1. In this sess ion , he revealed something that he had not spoken 01
when hi s individu a l hi story was taken-namely, that as a littl e boy he had
been sexua ll y abused on two occasions by two different men. He h ad never
spoken of t hi s to anyone. In s ubsequeIH individual sessions, he was ab le to
relate this to doubts about hi s masculine identity, distrust of others, a nd
after some time h e a lso spoke of a fear that because this had happened to
him he might in turn sexua ll y ab use his own chi ldren. While these issues
might h ave come out later in the therapy, th e TAT procedure clearly facili-
tated the broaching of this major area w ithout wh ic h the counse lin g lik e ly
would have stalled and wou ld have ended unsuccessfully. Individual psy -
chotherapy for the hu sba nd was then added as an adjun ct to the marital
sess ioll s with faster progress thell being made.
Copyrighted Material
I ntegrati ng the TAT into a
Test Report
83
Copyrighted Material
84 A Practical Gu ide to the TAT
------,
~
I
Outline of a Psychological Report
I. Demographic Data
List a ll the tests adm ini stered in the current assessme nt battery. If app li-
cable, cite the reference 0/ the scoring system(s) emp loyed.
State who referred the subject lor eva lu ation, and the reason for referral.
Often directly quoting the referral source or the questio n to be answered is
most effective. Also include, without going int o extensive detail, a state-
men t regarding the su bject 's ed ueat ion, occu pa tion, 111 a rita I status, ll1edi-
cations and dosages, treatment history, past traumatic events, and any
other special circulllstances th at warrant further exp loration and may im-
pact on test interpretation.
Copyrighted Material
Integrating the TAT into a Test Report 85
This section 11<.'lps the subject of the report "come to life" for the
reader, as well as providing a context for interpretation of test material.
Generally, this section should focus on demeanor, ve rbalizations outside
of formal test responses, and general behavior. Interpretation of these ob-
servations should be made in conjunction with test results in the next
section.
This is the main section of the report. Here the writer will present the
results of the psychological assessment and attempt to flesh out hypoth-
eses about their meaning. The interpretation section is best organized by
areas of the subject's functioning, as opposed to being organized by test.
For example, all information pertaining to the subject's achievement mo-
t ivation shou ld be presellled together, combining fi ndings from t he va ri-
(Jus tests given. This adds to the presentation of a meaningful, coherent
picture of the subject. The writer is encouraged to integrate material from
different tests and make conclusions about each area of functioning. An-
other significant advantage of this type of organization is that it empha-
sizes the point that the report is about a person, not about assessment
instruments. The report should focus on the individual tested, rather than
the test material. When mentioned, the test materia l should only be used
as supporting evidence. For example, it is more effective to state, "Mr. D.
appears to have angry feelings, as reflected in the arguments in his TAT
stories," than, "the TAT stories included many arguments, showing a lot of
anger." The former is person-focused, the latter test-focused.
The number of areas one could potentially discuss in this part of the
report is infinite. The following provide examples of several areas to ad-
dress; however, the referral question shou ld ideally determine what needs
to be included and what is emphasized in any psychological report.
A. Intellect/Cognitive Functioning
What is the examiner's best estimate of the subject's ability level? If formal
cognitive assessment was performed, results shou ld be presented here. A
profil e of strengths and weaknesses should be described, if available. If no
tests with normative comparisons were administered, intelligence can be
roughly estimated as below average, average range, or above average based
on vocabulary used during assessment and educationa l attainment.
What is the subject's level of ambition or asp iration? Information re-
garding this area can often be obtained, for examp le, by looking at re-
Copyrighted Material
86 A Practical Gu ide to th e TAT
sponses to TAT cards I and 2, th e perce nt age of whole respon ses on the
Rorsch ach , em ph as is on h eads on fi gure drawings, and observations of the
subj ect's diligence in working on the assessment tasks in gene ral. The de-
gree of cons istency among the subj ect's a pparent int e lli ge n ce, amb iti o n
le ve l, and occupation m ay be noteworthy.
In this sect ion , th e exam iner may a lso com m ent on th e s ubj ect's
cogn it ive sty le, or strategy lor approac hin g tasks. Th is is prim ar il y revealed
by the process by which the subj ect creates th e sto ri es, rathe r than the
content. Fo r examp le, a re the stori es logicaJ7 Are th ey particularly con -
cre te or abst ract? Are th ey we ll organized or h aphazard? Does the subject
loc u s on details or the total picture? Inform at ion rega rdin g cogn it ive style
sho uld be integrated w ith evidence from other tes ts, s uch as the locat ions
u sed most often on the Rorschach, planning ab ilit y on the Bend er Visual-
Motor Gesta lt Test, and sty le of a ttemptin g tasks on formal in te lli gence
tes ts.
B. Affect
C. Self-Regard
How the subj ect feels about him- or herself is an area th a t ha s ramifica-
tions [o r diagnosis and treatment planning. Looking at how the cent ra l
characters in the TAT stories are depicted will often a llow the examiner to
make a statement abo ut the subject's se lf- es teem an d degree of se lf-
criticism. For exa mpl e, a re the c haracters competent? How do they react
to making mistakes? Do thin gs work out for th e m ? How do o th e r charac -
ters view t h em? To corrobora te this informat ion the exam in er can, for
examp le, a lso look to the size a nd pl aceme nt of fi gure drawings and, per-
Copyrighted Material
Integratin g th e TAT into a Test Report 87
h a ps most importa ntl y, th e subj ect's comm e nts about hi s or her own p e r-
fonn a n ce durin g testin g.
D. Object Relationships
E. Diagnostic Aspects
Copyrighted Material
88 A Pra cti ca l Guide to the TAT
ties to rea lity, based on how realistic the stories are a nd how accurately
the visual stimuli are perceived.
Additional evidence from th e Rorschach is very helpful h e re. What is
the subject's degree of control o ver hi s or her impulses! What is the subject's
leve l of maturity ? How mllch insight does the subject have into his or her
own personality and conflicts? MMPI-2 scores on Scales 1 (Hypochondria-
sis), 2 (Depression), ) (Hysteria), 4 (Psychopathic Dev iate), 6 (Paranoia), 7
(Psychasthenia), and 8 (SchilOphrenia) arc also va lu able sources of cor-
roborative evidence, panicularl y when interpreted as a prolile (see Butcher,
19 9 0, for an introduction to profile interpretatioll).
Even more thall ill other Me<lS of the psychological report, one should
take great care ill the d iagllOstic section not to make statements that go
beyond the purposes of the tests used or e xcee d the inlorrnatioll obt,lined
Irolll the elata. All diagnostic inlormJtion should be back e d lip b y other
sources of data including other tests, behavior observation s, and bJckground
informatioll, Jnd shou ld be consistent with the subject's clJ y -to-d ay level
ollunction i ng.
Depending on the rclelTJI question and the setting in which th e report
is written, the exa miner mJl' choose to oller a diagno stic impression
or outline a dilTere ntial diJgnosis. For e X(l mpic, this mJ Y be more appro-
priate in J hospital settillg, where the ps yc hological assessment will be
directl y used in treatment plJnning, thJn in J school setting, where confi-
dellliJ lit y of the report will likel y be sOll1ewhJt limited.
F. Strengths
Every psychologica I report shou ld spea k to the su bject '5 srrt'ngt hs. The
eXJllliner should in corporate pos itive JSI1l'cts of test findings throughout
the report, but mJY also c hoose to briefl y sUll1marize such cJpacities Jt the
cOlllpletion 01 the interpretation section. This helps to temper the COIll-
Illonplace andunfortunJte tendency to patllOlogi ze th e subject. Exa mples
of strengths to highlight here includ e intelligen ct' level, aspiration level,
leve l of contro l over impulses, personalit y chMacteristics, in sight, and tie s
to reality. The eXJminer call J lso idelltily circumstances und e r which the
subject's lunctioning seelllS to be mJ x imized . C larifying the s ubject's
strengths is generally helpful in treatl11ent plJnning.
VI. Summary
Copyrighted Material
Integrating the TAT into a Test Report 89
tion, the summary should focus on the indi vidual tested, not the tests
themselves. New material should clearly not be introduced at this point;
only information disclosed earlier shou ld be included. In the summary,
the referral question, index scores from standardized tests, and th e most
salient psychodynamic findings shou ld be restated. The writer should keep
in mind that this may be the only section read by a busy professional;
therefore, it should outline a ll of the most important points and offer a
balanced view of the subject.
VII. Recommendations
The examiner shou ld carefu ll y consider th e test findings and oller recom-
mendJtions regJrding the refelTJI question. Addit ion a ll y, the examiner
lllay suggest further JSSeSslllent of problem areas. Illlpli cations for treJt-
lllent plJnning can be of/ered, including what type of therapy may be
most appropriate for the s ubj ect, or how the subject's individual strengths
CJn be used to work on his or her areas oj weakness. Specifi c. prJcticJI
suggestions are lllOst VJl uJblc.
The following JIT sugge~l ion s Illal will enllance till' efIeClivt'nl'ss of till'
psychological report. They primarily reflect general issues to keep in mind
throughout the process oj int erpretJt ion oj test datJ, orgJnization of J
report, and actua l report wri tin g. Many of these points have been raised
e lsewhere (Aronow, Reznikoll &. MorelJnd, 1994; Tallent, 1993; Teglasi,
1992). Addressing the following issues of organization, VJ li d it y. and com-
l1lunicJtion sJtisfacror il y will lik ely increase the usefulness of th e report
Jnd rcnder it eJs ier to digest by the reader.
Organization
Take the time to organize the material for the report before writing it. A
detailed outline is a tremendous asset in that it helps the writer to sec
relationships among the different domains of the subject's functioning.
Contradictions in the data may emerge, and can be reconciled at this point.
"Exp lanatory hypotheses" shou ld be formulated based on the interpretive
Copyrighted Material
90 A Prac ti ca l Guide to th e TAT
analyses. Th ese ex plain the reasons for the subject's feelings, re lationships,
and conllicts, and foster understanding of the plTse nting problems (Teglasi.
1992). One should try to clarify the findings and create an integrate d ,
complete picture of the individual subject. A string of statements that are
not re lated to each other should be avoided. While organizing the report ,
the writer decides which interpretations to include and exclude; evaluates
how much e mphasis to place on th e m, ba sed on th e ir relative impor-
tance; and synthesizes the findings into a coherent summary of the person
tes ted .
Maximizing Validity
There are seve ral aspects of report writing that inlluence the va lidit y of the
end product. It is important to remembe r to integrat e findings with the
s ubject' s history, background information, and life evenls. Th e te st report
should not appear to have been written in a vacuum, but rath e r in th e
context of an individual's life. Consider a lt ernat ive exp lanations for th e
s ubject's responses and behavior. In cluding the subject's perspective helps
in crease understanding of th e indi vidua l (Teg la si, (992). Rem e mbe r to
includ e the subject's areas of strength, not only patho logy .
A difficult aspect of report writing that pertains to the va lidit y of the
re port is th e confidence with which interpretive statements, diagnostic
impressions, and recom mendat ions a re made. La nguage chosen shou ld
appropriate ly rerI ect th e level of certainty of till' exa miner in making state -
ments about the s ubject. For ex ample , the writer should choose carefu ll y
among phrases such as, "it lllay be thaI," "it appears thac" "ev id ence sug-
gests thaI," "it is quite certain thaI," or "there is strong indication that."
The choice should be base d on the ava il abil it y of supporting data, princi-
pally tes t material, but other information JS we ll. Ove rinterpretation of
data and unwarrant ed authoritative statemenTS reflect irresponsibl e use of
the test materi a ls and the eXJm in er's role. It can be appropriate to offer
speculat iolls, but they should a lways be labeled as such.
Caution shou ld a lso be taken not to err on the opposite ext reme, ex-
press ing too littl e confidence ill one's findings. Writers who hedge too Illuch
undermine their own work by making it appeJr that th e report is purely
spec ulative and baSically of no usc at all. Evaluat in g on e's own level of
c e rtainty when pr ese nting interpretation s and opinions is one
of th e most difficult aspects of report writing for th e novice, and even for
th e more experienced psycho logist. Feedback from supervisors should
be helpful in s triking an Jppropriatc balance betw ee n over- and
u nderconfidence .
Copyrighted Material
Integrating the TAT into a Test Report 91
Optimize Communication
A psychological test report that is logically presented and easily read and
undersrood, will obviously have the greatest impact. One of the issues
affecting the readability of the report is whether to, and, if so, how to
include raw data and technical terms. The following guidelines will help
the report flow with minimal interruption. It is recommended that the
individual tested should always be the major focus with raw data from the
test material. such as quotes and examples, primarily used to support in-
terpretations and hypotheses. This material should not be the central topic
of discussion. If tests were formally scored, scores can be reported in a
separate section, either before the interpretation of the results, or in an
appendix. Technical terms should usually be avoided.
The writer should keep in mind the setting in which the report is writ-
ten and the audience for whom the report is wrilten. Are the potential
readers familiar with psychological assessment instruments and psycho-
logical terms? Will the report be available only to professionals, or to the
subject. or to the family of the subject? These questions will guide word
choice in many instances. Specific technical data, such as scores and ratios,
should only be cited when the report is written in a learning context.
Another common issue that arises in report writing has to do with the
appropriate length for psychological reports. Obviously, there is no hard
and fast rule regarding report length. Acceptable length commonly varies
with the working culture of the setting for which the report is written.
More important than length are good organization, focus, and relevance.
However, complaints by readers that reports are too long clearly outnulll-
ber resentments about brevity (Tallent, 1993). Reports that are too long
run the risk of not being read, only having the summary section read, or,
at minimulll, not being read carefully. Important findings can be diluted
by being immersed in too much relatively trivial or tangential material.
This risk can be reduced by carefully tailoring the report to the referral
question, and only adding other findings that are particularly striking.
Avoiding wordiness is advised (see Williams, 1989, for excellent instruc-
tion in clear. concise writing).
Finally, reports should always be carefully proofread for errors in spell-
ing, syntax, and gramma r. These kinds of mistakes take away from the
professionalism of the report and can make the content seem unreliable as
well. One should proofread beyond using spell-check and grammar-check
programs of word processing programs.
Copyrighted Material
92 A Practi ca l Guide to th e TA T
Computer-Generated Reports
Copyrighted Material
Integratin g th e TAT into a Test Report 93
Copyrighted Material
Copyrighted Material
Sample Protocols
r.-: Protocol # 1
Th e firs t s ubjeCl is a 15- yca r-old w hite m a le (B ill J .) w ho was re icrred lor
psyc holog ica l testin g in conn ection w ith d ecision s t h a t n ee de d to be m a d e
a bout hi s dia g nosis a nd Il1 cd iC:llion. He h a d bee n in p syc h o thera py off a nd
o n for Ill a n y ycars, be in g trea te d fo r d e press ion . Rece ntl y th e re ha d bee n
sOlll e ac tin g out of a n ger in a violent way in hi s fa mil y a nd a lso w ith fri e nd s.
Hi s school g ra d es h a d a lso de teriora te d in th e pas t yea r. Hi s prese nt thera-
pi s t s u spe ct e d a po ss ible u nd e rl y in g psyc hoti c d iso rd er in a ddition to d e -
pre ss ion , w hi c h was th e precipit at in g fac tor for th e prese nt tes tin g. Th l'
p,l ti e nl was a dmini s tered the TAT as pa rt o f a full ball ery of tes ts, inc/ud -
i n g t h e Rorsch ac h Tec h n iq u e, t h e Fig ure Dra w i ng Tes t. a nd ot h ers .
Card 1
95
Copyrighted Material
96 A Practi cal Guide to th e TAT
He's afraid that he'll drop it again. He had a great talent, but now he'll
never play it again.
Summary: A very talented boy drops his violin, wants to torture himself
for this, and never plays again.
Card 2
This looks like a young wom a n and her mother. It looks li ke the mother is
getting in the way of the young woman. The young woman does n't want
the mother to get into her life anymore. There's a se rvant in the back-
ground. The mother think s that the girl and him will fall in love. The
mother doesn't want it to happen. The young girl hates the mother, but
she doesn't want to show it. She's going somewhere with he r books. Sh e's
going off to do her own thing, not what th e mother told her to do. The
mother knows what she is doing, she enjoys making her daughter mad.
She's sitting back and smiling. (How does it work out?) They'll always
hate each other. When she moves out. she'll neve r like her mother or see
much of h e r mother.
Summary: A mot her enjoys blocking her da ughter's asp iration, the da ugh-
ter moves out, and th ey always hate eac h other.
Card 3BM
Thi s looks lik e a woman crying on her bed. It look s lik e the woman is
married to a guy who is a busing he r. She ca n't divorce him and she does n 't
know what to do . She's afra id she'll be with him the rest of her life. which
she probably will.
Summary: A crying woman married to an abusive Illan is afraid to leave
him.
Card 6BM
This guy looks lik e he's angered-he knows that he did something wrong
to his mother-he 's depressed that he did it a nd sorry but he does n ' t want
to say anything . The woman never saw h e r son act this way-it's a sur-
prise to her. He feels bad but he won't apologize. He doesn't want to give
in. He won ' t apologi ze to his mom but she knows that h e's apo logetic at
the moment.
Copyrighted Material
Sample Protocols 97
Summary: A man did somet hin g wrong to his mother but doesn't want
to a pologize .
Card 7BM
These two guys are scu lll-th ey're involved in some illegal act. They're
ju st whispering something, to do somet hing-"We' ll do somet hin g illegal,
and get a lot of money." The guy at the bottom doesn ' t wa nt to do it, he
looks rebellious but he knows that they'll kill him if he doesn't. The old
guy looks lik e he's been doin g it a ll hi s life . He knows th e yo un ge r gu y will
get in trou ble, but he doesn't care.
Summary: Two crimina ls are planning a crim e, and the older one doesn ' t
ca re if the yo un ger one gets in troubl e .
Card 13B
This kid li ves in a poor hom e-the lam ily doesn't want him in th e ho use.
He wants to go some place where he can think things over. When he gets
money, hi s pare nts take it away fwm him. He'll be rich and famous as an
adu lt and they'll want help and he won 't help th em . He ha s friends so
that's how he gets by thi s period of hi s life.
Summary: Parents don ' t want to help a poor child and w he n he becomes
success ful he doesn't help them.
Card 13MF
Card16
There are two yo ung kids arguing, it looks like they' re about to get into a
hu ge light. The kids will beat the crap out of each other. Probably o n e will
Copyrighted Material
98 A Practi ca l Guide to th e TAT
die, and his family w ill get rl'vcnge a nd kill the other onl' and thcn evc ry -
thing w ill be even.
Summary: Two chi ldren fight and onc d ies, th c n the family gets reve nge.
Interpretation
In this pati e nt' s TAT sllnil's, ce rt ain themes repea t themselves. The stories
depict sit u a ti ons o f fai lure (not ab ly card I ); conflict w ith a lmost everyone,
but particu la rl y with au thorit y figures; a nd a de press ive, pessimistic, hope-
less out look regarding the future.
At one po in t anger is see n as d irected in wa rd (card I) and at anothe r
point th e patie nt is able to change a story in which the male protagonist is
vio lent to a story in which he is not (I3MF). In one ca rd , some tenderne ss
toward the mother fig ure is ex pre sse d (card 6BM). Remarks on card I
("torturin g himself") suggest a directing of a nger inward. Lack of confi-
dence in th e self is a lso pronoun ce d (card I ). The very strong and some-
what perseverative v iew of others as highly nega tive toward him suggests
that proj ection is being used. A genera l view olthe parents as malevolent
is apparent (cards 2, 138). Peers a re seen in more positive term s (ca rd
138) .
It should be note d tha t in other tests (notably the Rorsch ac h ) admi ni s-
tered to thi s pat ie n t extens ive use of projection is see n as well as genera ll y
poor ego -function ing consistent w ith psychosis . The gcnera l diagnostic
impress ion is one of Schizophrenia, Paranoid Type .
Ii
L-: Protocol # 21
The following test record was obtained Iroll1 a 76 -year-o ld woman who
volun teered as a tes t subject, thu s g ivin g us a test reco rd of a sen ior to
eva lu ate. She expressed appreciatio n for the company o f the exa miner. It
was necessa ry on seve ral occa sio ns to remind her of the basic qu es tion s to
be considered in constru ct in g a story and reassure h e r that there were no
cor rect answers. She appeared to have some minor difficulty with visua l
activi ty that may have in[\u e nced th e detail of her responses.
I Man), thank s are exp ressed to Lisa Tischler, M.A .. ()r F() rdhalll Un ive rsi ty lo r providing
protocol It 2 .
Copyrighted Material
Samp le Protocols 99
Card 1
I don't know what this object is. He's concentrating on this object. He
seems to be concentrating very hard on it, maybe figuring out what it is.
He has free time on hi s ha nds. He's ju st sittin g am usin g himself. He's ab-
sorbed with it. He may see somet hin g that I don't see. He's relaxed, he's
thinking. He may figure out what it is, but I don't know what it is. But he
look s like h e 's looki ng at somet hin g h e und erstands. If h e's bright he'll
fi gure it out.
(Add itiona l reading of directions at this point.)
Summary: Someone is lookin g at someth in g, patiently trying to figure it
out.
Card 2
This takes place on a farm. Farmer with a horse, seed in g the ground. This
must be the you n g daughter with sch ool books. His wife is watching him.
Horse to h e lp with seed in g. She may be goin g to schoo l because sh e has
books. No t 12 o r 13, 19 or upper teens. The more mature woman cou ld be
mom relaxing up aga inst a tree. Fie ld looks lik e a hard days work, it loo ks
well done. He did the field and accompli shed what h e set o ut to do in
making th e ground [ully p lowed.
Summary: A farm scene with a family go in g abo ut their business.
Card 3BM
Copyrighted Material
100 A Practi ca l Guide to th e TAT
Card 3GF
The male or female look s very unhappy. Som e thing awful must have hap-
pened to her. She's weeping and stooped over. Not abu sed, ju st an un-
happy individual. Ma ybe sh e had an argument but she's not beat e n.
Whether going back into the hou se to smoot h over what happened ....
Decid ed she's go ing back in. Don't know if go ing in or out. Goes in and
sllloothes it over. They mak e up and have a good even ing .
Summary: An unh appy woman is weeping th e n goes back in and makes
up.
Card 4
These two are a male and a female in an argument. He's angry at h er and
she's trying to pacify him . He won 't give an inch . She may wind up win-
ning him ove r because sh e's lookin g at him e nd earingly. Sh e wants har-
mony in the relationship and is sorry about what happened. Sh e will try
harder to win him back . [ think sh e' ll ge t him back. He isn ' t that angry,
and sh e's look in g at hi m endearin gly. She'll get him back .
Summary: A man and woman have an argument and she wins him over
w ith end earing look s.
Card 6BM
This looks lik e it could be mom and son in some deep conve rsa tion . Both
look ve ry unhappy though h e's lookin g out th e w indow. He's unhappy,
hi s eyes are downca st. They cou ld be discu ss in g li fes tyles. Maybe she doesn't
lik e hi s friends, or hi s job, or his wile . Anything. He look s unhappy . We
hope that they w ill work it out, but h e's very unhapp y right now. He'll
m ake up with hi s mom a nd it will be lo ve a nd bliss again.
Summary: A mother doesn't lik e her son's li lestyle, but th ey mak e up.
Card 7BM
Two men, don't represe nt any thin g. Not dad a nd son, maybe e mployee
and e mployer-very absorbed and di sc uss in g somet hin g of great impor-
tance. Very se riou s. Will win the case. Co uld be attorneys.
Summary: Two m e n discuss som e thin g important.
Copyrighted Material
Sample Protocols 101
Card 88M
Thi s looks like war. Gun here and surgeon and operation, and assistant,
and this dressed fellow. So maybe it's not a war scene. Operation on some-
one. Th e re 's a gun in the picture . Thi s is m aybe a bullet wound. They are
working ea rnestly. Th e gentleman on the table is half covered, and the
guy is with a knife culting into him, but the gentleman here is mislea ding.
If the g uy h e re wasn't here, it would be a war sce ne . You see the gun here.
I don't know, th e pati e nt will live.
Summary: Someone is being op e ra ted on, and survives .
Card 13MF
He's got cloth es on, so it 's not a sex ual thing. She's half dressed in bed.
Ma ybe he abused he r, h e's holding hi s eyes. Don't know if h e will change
his mind , be a ni ce g uy and make love to h e r. He looks sorry. He's covcring
hi s eyes and fa ce . So h e could have abused her, or hurt h e r. No indication s
of what he will do . Ma ybe sh e's a prostitute. He see ms ashamed. He looks
like he's going to leave.
Summary: A man hUrl S a woman and is ashamed and sorry.
Interpretation
The ge neral impress ion is oll e of ell1otional functioning that is within the
normal range. Thi s cli e nt does approJch environmental situations with
caution and un ce rtainty, fee lin g that she mu st ex pend considerable effort
a nd con ce ntration on inte rpre ting them objectively a nd correctl y (card I) .
She characteristically appears to sea rc h for incon siste nci es and illogi ca li-
ties in her perceptions to be confid e nt that she has judge d social interac-
tions accurately (cards 88 M, 13MF). She st rongly identifies with the role
of an effective peacemaker in he r deJ lings with mal es, whom she ex pe ri-
ences as typically st ubborn ; at tim es, irrationally a ngry; and as havin g ra th e r
conflicted a busive procli vities (cards 38 M, 3GF, 13MF).
As re fl ected in the cli e nt 's tend ency to assign h a ppy e ndings to many of
he r stories, deni a l and avoidance a re central defen ses in dealing with her
occasionally overwhelming dysphoric emotions (card 38M). She feels th a t
seek in g h e lp for h e r negative feelings might be appropriate and that "some-
one might mak e h e r fee l good."
He r conception of the famil y is a ba Sically traditional one (card 2). Mother-
child contacts are seen as sometimes friction lad e n , resulting from a critical
Copyrighted Material
102 A Practical Gu ide to th e TAT
Protocol # 3
Card lOB
(Boy w ith mOlle y an d a pigg" bank, im ag illillg a bi cycle ill a shop window
a nd buying ict' cream) .
Thl' boy lound th l' a nilllal (pig, w hi ch in the card is a piggy bank). He
wa nt s to sel l it. He feels sad becallse hl' lound Ih e a nimal. Here he is bu y-
ill g a n ice crearn w ith 11ll' JIloll ey. Hc lik es ice crl'a lll. Loo k, he re he is
I()okin g a t a bi cycle c1nd he is sa d because he lVa nt s 10 buy it. but he can' l.
That's it !
Summary: A boy lind s ,1n allilllai. \Vclilts to huy things, clnd is sad w ilcn
he can' t.
Copyrighted Material
Samp le Protoco ls 103
Card 11
Card 17B
(A boy w ho is stud yin g drea ms about gett in g an "A" fro m the teac her, or
ge ttin g an "F").
He is looking at a book (speak ing about th e boy siltin g a t a des k) and h e
is a lso lookin g he re (po inting to the boy who h ad a n F on th e pape r). He is
very sad becau se of the man. This one h ere (the boy who had an A on th e
paper) is a lso sa d. [don 't know why . The boy is sad becau se he h as a lot of
home work . [ have a lot of hom ework in sc hoo l. That's it.
Summary: A boy is sad (chaotic story).
Card 19B
Card 20
(A kid in bed dream s of a scene w ith a river, a horse, and a cas tl e).
[ don't want to do anymore! Th e kid took off hi s clothes. The horse is
jumping and wa n ts to go up to th e house. The boy is thinkin g about the
Copyrighted Material
104 A Practical Guide to the TAT
horse because he wants to ride it. I don't k now what he is feeling. This is
his horse and he wants to rid e ir. Look at th e Illountains.
Summary: A boy takes of! hi s clot h es and he wams to ride his horse.
Card 21
Interpretation
iJ Protocol # 4
The following is not a comp lete TAT test protocol. b u t only the response to
card 14 by a female co ll ege student who was a lso a talented writer and
who, tragically, comlllitted su icid e subsequent to th e test adm inistration.
Copyrighted Material
Sall1p le Protocols 105
Card 14
Copyrighted Material
Copyrighted Material
REFERENCES
AMOIl, N. S. ( 1967). Some pl'rso ll alit y dilkrcllcl's betwe en asth ill ati cs, allergic all d Ilormal
children. JOll rl/al oI Clillical Psyclw l ogy, 23, 336-340.
Allll'J'icall Psyc hi at ri c Associa tion. ( 1994). Di,l.'llI<Jsfic alld Sf,lfisficalll1£1l1l1 al of llimfal dis<Jrders
(4 1h cd.) . Washillgtoll, DC: Au tl wr.
Americall Psyc h o log ica l Associatioll. ( 19661. Interim stalldard s for autom atc d test scorillg
alld illlerplTtatioll practi ccs . Alllericall P.'),cll ol<J.'I isf, 22, I 141.
Allll'ricall Ps yc hol og ica l Associatioll. (1992). Et hi cal principles lor psyc hologists alld COliL- 01
collduct. A lllt'I'icall PSYc//(J/" ,'lisf, 47, I 597- 16 1 I .
A lld ersoll,.I. W. ( 1999). Hem y A. Murray alllit he creatioll of th e Thl'lllati c A pperceptioll
Test. III L. Ceiser 0 M. I. Ste ill (Eds . ), E I'OC,lfil '~ illla!/I'S (PP. 21-38). Was h illgtoll, DC:
AlllCr icJI1 Psycho logici'd AssociJliol1 .
A lldor, L. E. ( 1983). Psycllological alld soci,,/t'.'Jic,11 sflldics of fil l' Black p<'ople of Aji-ica, sO llfll oI fll e
5, llIara /960-/975 - AII (/III1Vf,lf<,d selecf biblio.'lrapln '. Johallllcsb urg, So ut h Af rica: Na-
tiollal Ill sti tutl' for Perso lill c i Research.
ApPl'l baullI, S. A . ( 1990). The re latiollship hl'tWl'l'll aSSeSSll\l' llt all d ps yc hot herapy. JOllmal
oI Persollalify A.lwsslllmf, 54, 791-80 I .
AraoJ, I). L. ( 1972) . The TIll'III.ltic A pperCepli()ll Te SI ill m ari tal th erapy. JOIIl'llal ofCollfml -
pvrary Ps\'cll vf ll cral'.I', 5, 4 1-48.
Arollo w, E., o· Re7.niko fL M. ( 197 1). Applicalioll of projecti ve tests to psych otherapy: A case
stud y. )0111'11<11 of Pas<lIhllif)' Assesslllmf, 35,379- 393 .
A roIlO W, E., ROllikoff, M., 5· Morl'lalld, 1<. ( 1994). Til e Rorsclhlcll feclllliqlle . Nel'dhalll Hei ghts,
MA: AIIY Il &. Bacoll.
Atk ill SOIl, .I. W., E,. Feat her, N. T. (Eds.). (196(, ). A fll <"'I)' ofacllin 'clllcllf lI/ ofil 'llfioll. New York:
Wiley.
Bailey, B. E., FJ Crl'CIl, J. ( 1977). Black thl' llIatic apperccptioll test st imulu s Ill al er iai. ),"11'-
11£11 of' PC/'wllalify AsscsSlllmf, 41, 25-30.
B.lty, M. A. c' D re ger, R. M. ( 1975). A colnparisoll of three llIet hods to rccord TAT protocol s.
) Ollnhll ,'I Ciillical Psycllv/t'.'IV, 3 1, 348.
Bl' llak, L., c Abrams, D. M. ( 1997). Til l' TAT, CAT, alld 5AT ill cli llicaillsc (6t h cd. ). Bostoll:
A lI YII &. BaCll Il.
Ikllak L., Abra m s, D. M., & Ackl'l'llIallll -Ellgel, R. ( 1992) . Halldbook oI illffllSil'l' brief a ll d
elll er'Jf'lIc,l' psycl/(Jfllera/,y. Larc 11l1l O1l1, NY: c. p.s.
Bcll"k, L., & I3c ll ak , S. S. ( 1948). Tile CA T. LcrlilillOlll, N Y: c. p.s.
Ikllak, L., & Ik ll ak , S. S. ( 1965). Til e CA T- H: A 1IIIIIIall lIIodificafio ll . Larc ltl1101l1, NY: c. PS
Bellak, L., & Bc ll ak , S. S. (1973). Tile5AT. Larclll ll ollt, NY: c.p.s.
Bdl.lk, L., o· i-lurv iclt , M . ( 1966). A hUlllall IllodiJ'i ca tioll o f t ill' Childn' Il 's AppcrcL')\ioll
Tcst. ) oll mal of ProjeCfil'e Tecilll iLlll es, 30, 228- 242.
107
Copyrighted Material
108 Refe re n ces
Berends, A., Wes ten , D., Leigh , J.. &. Si lber!. D. ( 1990). Ass l'ssin g allec t-t ol1l' 01 rel,1tion ship
pJrJd ig lll s frolll TAT J nd i tller vil'l'v dJ tJ. Ps),cllo"'.'1ic<l1 AssesslllclIl , 2, 329-332.
Ikrllste in, E., FJ' Pe rr y, J. C. ( 1995). A TAT <lll<llysis tl/"'/>i<'C I rl'lali,l/ls ill Bordalille, Alllisoci<ll,
Se/lizvlypal, alld Bipolar 1/ diso rdas. Unpublishl'd 1ll,1nu snipt , Bos ton U ni versit y .
Bine!. A. & Henri. V. ( 1896) . L1 psycho logie individuelk. Allllee Psycllolo.'1iqlle, 2,4 11 - 465.
Birney, R. c., ( 1968) . Rl'sea r ch Oil the ,1chil'Vl' llll'nt Illoti v e. III E. F. Borgalla h· W. W.
Laillbert (E d s.), Halldbook ofpcr.'Olidlil.\' Illc"'T <llld rcsc<lrc/I. Chi cago: Rand McNally.
Blulll, G. S. ( 1950). Tile Black.\' piClllres : M,'ll ll al ""im/l'llcliollS. New Yo rk : Psycho logical (01'-
p()rJt i()Jl .
Butcher, J . N . ( 1990). MMPI-2 ill pS),.-ilo"','lic"lll'Ctllll/('III . Nl'W Yo rk: Oxford U ll ivL'rsity Press .
Butcher, J. N ., Keller, L. S., E,. Baco n , S. F. ( 1985). Cu rre nt de velopillent s alHI futulT direc-
tions in cOlllp uteri zl'd person a li ty ,1SSl'SSlllL'll!. ./,l/mlill oIColIsllllill,'1 ,Hid Clillical Ps)'cll"l-
"9-", 53, 803-8 15.
Butche r, .I . N., h Panciwri, P. ( 1976). A 1'<1l1d/>,,,,k oflT,)Ss' lI ,lIi,l/lal MMPI resedrcll . Mi lllll'apO-
lis, MN: U ni ve rsit y of Mi n neso ta Pre ss .
Canter, M. 13., Bc nn et!. 13. E.. JOI1l'S, S E, F,. Nag\" T. F. ( 1994). Etllics Ivr psre/I"/v.'!isl':
A collllllelll<1r), 0111111' APA elllies c,,,le. Wash ington , DC: A illeri ca n Psyc hol ogical Associa-
tioll.
Chowdhury, U. ( 1960a). All 11I,liall ,ul<1I'lali,)fJ o(llie Cilildrl'II ',\ AI'I'e!'ccplioll Tesl. De l hi, I ndia:
Ma nasaya n.
CholVdhury, U. ( 1960 b ). A n Indi an nltldificatioll of the Theilla ti c A ppc rc eption Tl'st. Ti,e
)"111'11<110( Social PsycliolojY, 51. 245- 263.
(oche, E., 5 Sillilli , J. A. ( 1983) . The Theillatic Ap perce ption Tes t ill all Out CO llll' Ill easure in
psychot h erapy resea rc h. Psrcllolllaap)" Tlleor)" Researcll alld Praclice, 20,4 1-46 .
COlllbs, A. ( 1946). The use 0 1 personal experil'ncl' in Thl'nlatic Apperception Test story
plots. )olll'llal oIClillical Psycllolo,".!', 2,357-363.
(ost,1nti lltl, G., Ma lga d y, R. G., C1sl illo, M. M., h· Cas tillo, A. ( 199 1). C ro ss-c ultur,1 1 stan-
dardiza ti o n 01 T EMAS in three l-ii sp,1nic suh culture s. Hisl'allic )"lIl'IIal ""Bell<1l'iural Sci-
m ces, 13, 48-62.
Costdntin o , G., Ma lgady, R. G., Rogk r, L. H .. h- Tsui, E. C. (I (88). Discrilllin ant analysis of
clinical ou tpati e nt s and public school children hI' T EMAS : A Theill ati c A ppncep tion
Test lor Hispa ni cs and B lack s. )ollrllal tl/'Pl'rS,l/ I"IiI,l' Assessillml, 52, 670-678.
(os tantin o, G., Ma lga d y, R. G., b Va/.<]IIl'I., C. ( 198 1). A coillpa ri son or th e Mu rray-TAT ,l nd
a Il ew T h clllat i c A ppncept io n Te st lor u rha n II ispa n ic c h ildren. Hisp,lIIic )ullrll"l oj
Behm'ivra I Scielices, 3, 29 1-300.
Cowan, G .. 5 · Goldberg, F . .I. ( 1967). Nl'l'd achil'\Tl llL'nt as ,1 function of th e race and sex of
figures o f se lect ed T AT cdrds. ) ollrllalt1{Pers,'"alilr Lllld S"cial Psyclwlu,'l.1', 5, 245-249.
CrJdd ick, R. A . ( 1972). HUlllani sti c ,1SSl'SSnlL'llt: A r ep l y to Brown. i'sychulhcrapy: Th eory,
Resmrcll alld Practice, 9, 107- 1 10.
Cra dd ick, R. A . ( 1975) . Sharin g onesl' ll ill the assessille nt procedure . Prt1{essiolhll Psrello"'-'/)',
6, 279- 282 .
(rallll'r, P. ( 1987). The de ve lo pllll'nt 0 1 dl'icnse IllL' chani sllls. )"lIrllLlI v( Persollalil)" 55, 597-
614.
Cralll l'l', P. ( 1990). The dCl'eioplllL'llI "j dermse lIlee/ILIllisIIIS: TileolY, researcll, alld assessillml. N ew
York: Sp ring er · Ve rl ag.
Cr anlt'r, P., B lall , S. J., & Ford , R. Q. ( 1988) D l'ien sl' Ill ec hani sill s in the an acl itic and
introject i vl' person alit y configurat io n. )"lInll1l l'j COlisl/llill,q Lllld Clillical Psycho"'.'I.\', 56,
6 10-6 16.
Cralllel', P., 5· Block, J. ( 1998). Preschool antl'ce dl'nt s of ekfense llleCil dnislll u se in you ng
ad ults: A lon gi tudin al stud y . )ol/rllal vI Persollolil.l' "lid Svcial Psyclwlof/Y, 74, 159- 169.
Dana, R. H. ( 1985). Th ema ti c Apperception Test In C. S. Newilla rk (Ed,), Major ps),cll vlv,qical
asseSSllleli1 illslrllll1cl1ls (pp. 89- 134). New ton , MA: A ll y n 5· BaClll1.
Copyrighted Material
Refere nces 109
Dana, R. H. (1996). The Tlll'lllatic Apperception Test (TAT) . In C. S. NewlllJJ"k (Ed.1. Maj,"-
psvc/IO/O,'lica/asscsslllt'lI/ illSlnllllellls (2nd ed., pp. 166-205) . Needhalll Heights, MA: Allyn
& Bacon.
Dan,l, R. H. ( 1999). Cross-c ultural / Illulticultural use of the Thelll ati c A pperception Test. In
M. Geiser & M. Stein (Eds.), EI'ocalil't' illlages. Washington, DC: Aml'rica n Psychological
Assoc iJliuIl.
Davi ds, A .. & Rosenblatt. D. (1958). Use of the TAT in assess mcnt of ,llicnation. JOllma/ 0{
Proja/il'" Ta/lll ill llCS, 22, 14 5- 152.
Dcplu, 0 .. & Killlbrough, C. ( 1982). Feeling-tolll' and ca rd prderences of Black clellll'ntJrY
childrcn for the TCB and TAT. JOllma/ oINoll-Whilt' COllcems, 10, 50- 56.
Doug la s, C. ( 1993). TrallS/att' the darkllt'ss: Tht' Ii(e oIChristialla Mor"all. New York: Simon iT
Sc hust er.
Dymond, R. F. ( 1954). A djustlllent changes ovcr therapy frolll Thclllatic Apperccption Test
ratings. In C. R. Rogers & R. F. DYlllond (ELis.), Psychotherapyal1d persolla/itye/wll,"C (Pl'.
76- 84). Chi cago: University of Ch icago Press.
Er,lSlIllIS, P. F. (1975). TAT- Z cala/O.'lllC llO. 1676. Pretoria, Human Sciences Research Council.
Eron, L. D. ( 1950). A norlll ati ve stud y of the Thelllatic Apperce ption Test. Psye/lO/o,qiUl/ MOllO-
,,,mphs, 64(9, WIHlk No . ., 15).
[ron, L. D. (195.,). Responses of wOlllcn to the Theillatic Apperccpt ion Test. JOllma/ or
COllSll/lill,q Psych%,")', 17, 269-282.
Ex ncr, .1. E.. .lr. (1974). The Rorschach: A cOlllprehellSil'c systelll (Vol. I). Ncw York: Wiky.
F,lirwe,lther, G .. Simon , R .. Gc hhard , M., Weingarten, E .. Holland , .1., Sande rs, R., Stone,
G .. 5· Reahl. .1. ( 1960) . Relative effectiveness or psychotherapeutic prograllls: A
lIlulticriteria cOlllpJrison of four prograills lor three dillerent patient groups. PS.l'cho/O.'IY
MOllo.'lraphs, 74( 5).
Finn, S. E., CI Tonsager, M. E. ( 1992) . Thnapeutic dfcCls of providing MMPI-2 te st Iced back
to colkge student s awaiting therap y . PSYc/IO/o,qical Assesslllellt, 4,278- 287.
Fis chn, C. T. ( 1970). The testee as co-eva luator. JOllma/ 0{ COllllse/ing PSYc!IO/O.'/y, 17, 70- 76.
Fischer, C. T. ( 1972). Paradigm ch ange s which allow sharing of res ult s. Professiolla/ Psycho/-
o.'/y, 3, 365-369.
Fishn, S. P, Hind s, E. (1951). The org,lnilation of hostility controls in variou s pnsonalit y
structurcs. GClldie P;),e/w/t',qiml MOllo.'1raplis, 44, 3-68.
Fowln, R. D. ( 198 5). L'llHhllMks in cOlllputer-assisted psychological aSSCSS llll'llt. ./oll/"lla/ "'"
COllSll/lill,q lIlld Ciilliw/ PSYc!I(,h'.'IY, 53, 748- 759.
FrailI-;, A. F., <,. Gunderson, .J. G. (1990). The role of the therapeutic alliance in the treat-
ment of sc ili zo phrelli a: Rl'lationship to course and outcoille. Archivesv/Gcllaa/ Psycliia -
11'.1', 47, 228- 236.
Frcn ch, L. A. ( 199 3). Adapting projecti ve tests for Illinority children. Psycli%giw//?eporls, 72,
15- 18.
Gass, C. S., &- B ro w n, M. C. (1992). Ne urops ycho log ica l test Iced back to patients with brain
dys function. Psyc/w/o!1ica/ AssessllIml, 4, 272-277.
Gol dfried , M., & Zax, M. ( 196 5). The stilllulus va lu e of the TAT. ./oll/"lla/ oI Projectil'c Tce/I-
llii/lles, 29, 46-57.
Goldm,ln, R .. &. G reenbl att. M. ( 19 55). Changes in Thcmatic Apperce ption Test stories par -
alleli ng changes in clinical status of sch izophreni c patients. JOllrna/ of Nervolls ami Mm-
la/ Diseases, 121,243-2 49.
Hafnl'r, A. J. , & Kapl an, A. M. (1960). Ho stilit y COlllcnt anal ys is of th e Rorschach ,lnd TAT.
JOllmll/ v/Projeclil'e Teclilliqlles, 24, 137- 14 3.
Harrison, H. (1965). Thematic apperceptive methods. In B. Wolman (Ed.), Halldbook oI clilli-
cal psych% ,qy (PP, 562-620), New York: McGraw Hill.
Harrower, M. ( 1960). Projective co ull se ling-A psychotherapeutic t ec hniqu e. In M. I-Iar-
roWl'J" (Ed,), Crealive varia/iom ill Ihe projective leclilliqlles. Springfield, Il: Thomas.
Copyrighted Material
11 0 References
I-I a rtlllan, A. H. ( 1970). A basic TAT SCI . ."'llmal 4 Persollalily AssesSl/ICf{I, 34, ,91 - 396.
Haskell. R . .J.. Jr. (1961). RclaliollShip bCl we cll ,1ggrcssivc bch,wior a nd ps yc hoiogicalicsis .
.Jollr/wl of Projeclil'f' TecilII il{lIes, 25, 411 - 440.
H ,l S ~ill, P. R. ( 1958). A sllIdy of Iil e rci,TliollSllip belll'eCf{ realislic ,lIld IIl1realislic a.'l.qITss ioll , reti-
allCt' all calegorica l allillldes, ,lIId COlIslrllctin:lless '" adjllslillell/. Unpublishcd doctoral di s-
scrtalioll, Wcslcrn Rescrvc Un i vl'rs il),.
Hibba rd, S., Farmcr, L., Wclls, c., DiFillipo, E., BM r y . W., Korman, R., &. Simll, P. ( 1994).
Valid,ll ion 01 Crallln's deicn sc IIll'chanislll IIl,lI\ual lor Ihe TAT . .JO II rIIal 4 Pasoll,IIiIV
Assfsslllml, 63, 197- 210.
HibbMd, S., !-iilscnrolh, IV \'.I., Hibbard, .I. K., fi N,lS h, M. R. ( 199 5) A va lidil Y s\lld y 01 IWO
projeClivc objeCl ITPITscllialions Illl'aSllrl'S. PS.\'CII,,/".'lical Asses) lllelll , 7, 432- 439.
Hokr, 1' . .J.. & G reen , 13. F. ( 1985). TIll' cil,llknge 01 COllllll'll'ncc illHlne,llivil y in COIllIHIl-
crizt'd ps yc hologi ca l I('sl ing . .J"llrllal t>f CtlllSllllill.'1 alld Clilli(al Psycilo/'J.'1Y, 53, 826- 838.
1-lolfln ,111 , S., I'r KupermJn, N. (1990). Indircci lI'Calnll'lll 01 Ir,1LIlllal ic psyc hol og ical expni-
t'nccs: The use 01 TAT c,l rd s. Alllericall.Jollrllal <'f Psytilolilt.'rtlpJ'. 44. /07- /1 5.
!-iolnlS! rolli , R. W., Silber, D. L b Karl' , S. A . ( 199 0 ). DcvcloplllClli 01 thc Ap percc ptivl'
Pnsonalit y Test. Jtlllrll'll of Persollalil.\' Assessillml, 54, 2')2- 264.
Hol zberg, J. (1963). ProjeClivl' techniques and rcsisl ,ln lT 10 change in ps yc hoilH:'rap y ,1S
viewed I h rough a cOlllnllllli ca l ions Illodcl. ."'llrl/al tlr Pn>j,YliI 't' Tedllli'IIIt'S, 2 7, 430- 4') 5.
Hutt , M. L. ( 1980). Tile Mii'lii.'Jall Piclllre Tesl - R,'I'iseti: Mallllal. Ne w Yor~: Grulll' &. Stratton .
.I alll l' s, P. Ii.. Er Mosher, D. L. ( 1967). Thclllalic ilggressio n , hostilil y-g uilt. and ilggressi ve
bchavior . .Jollnlal tlj'Projet'lil'e Tecillliqlles alld I'crs,'lIalit\' AssCsSlllt'Ill, 3 1( 1),61 - 67 .
.lung, C. G. (1961) Melllories, dredills, dlld nj7t'diolls (A . .Ialie, Ed.). Nc w York: Pantheon.
I< ailn , M. (1984). Thc usefliiness 01 till' TAT blan~ c,l rd ill clinical practice. Ps.1'clltllll erilp.1' ill
Primlc Praclice, 2, 43- 50.
I<aron , 13. P. ( 19811. The Tilcillatic A ppnception Tesl III A. I. Rabin (E d.), AssesslllfIIl lI 'ilil
I'r,>jeclil'c Icelillil/llt's (PI'. i-\')- 120) Nell' York: Sp ri nger.
1<,1t/, H. E., Russ , S. W., &. Ovcr hol ser, .1. C. ! 1991) . Sl'X dilll' ITnCl's, Sl'X roics, and projcction
on Ihe TAT: M ,l l ciling qilllUILIs 10 C',lll1inl'l,' gcnder. JOllmdl of Pastlllillil.l' Assess illelll,
60. 18 6- 19 I.
I<lopier, B., b· Kcllc y, D. M. ( 194(, ). Til, ' I?,lrsclw(/I Icci/lli'IIIC. YOI1~crs, N Y: World gook Con l-
pJn \, .
I<orchin, S. J., Mi lCilcll, H. E., ii· Me llwl!, .I. (I ')501. i\ critic,ll l'\',lll1,ltio n 01 Ihc TiltJllllNlil
Thclllalic Apperccp lioll Tcs\. J,lllmdi "f Prtl}ectil'l' T('Clllliqlles, 14,445-45 1.
Lello w it7., .I. fr Frasl'r. A. W. ( 1980). ASs l' sslll elli 01 ,1chic'n'lllclll Jnd po w n nW l i vatioll tJI
BI,KKs alld WiliICS, lI Si llg a fllac~ ,1 11(1 Wilill' TAT, lVi lil Black ,1I1d Whi lc adlllini stra-
tors. Jtlllnlal of Applied P.'.I'(iltlit,.'].\'. 6 5. 685-6 t)6.
Lubin, B., Larsen, R. M., &. Mal<lr<l!.Io, .I. D. ( 1984). P,ll\lTn S 01 psychological Icst lIsagc ill
Ihc Uni ted Stalc,;: 19,5- 1982. Allleric-all PSI't1It,I".'1isl, 3'1,45 1- 4')4.
i'vl alg,ldy, R. C ., Cost,ln lillo, C .. fJ RtJgln, L. H. ( 1984). Dl'\Tlopllll'llt 01 a Thcllhllic Apper-
ccption Tc st (TE lv lAS) lor urba ll Hi ' p,lnic childrcn. Jilllmal tlI C"ll5 l1llill.'J alld Clillical
Psycilulv.'IY, 52, 986-99 6.
MeAn hllr. D. S., b Rohcns, C. E. ( 19i-\2). R"i>als Apl'erct'pli"1l TeSI I"l'
Cliildrell: A 1I1,llIlIal. Los
Angeles: Wcstern Psychological Sc nicc s.
McCIcIl,lnd, D. C. (1958). Ri s ~ 1,1 king in childrcn w ilh high and low Ill'l'd lor ,lChic vCllll'lI\.
In.l. W. A I~in son (Ed.), Mtllil'es in/'llI/tlSI', ,IClitlll, alld .",(icl.\'. PrinCl'ton, NJ: viln N osl 1',1 nd.
McClelialld, D. C. (196 1,1) . Tile tl cliit'I'inq ., ,'cicl\'. Ncw Yor~: V,l ll Nostra nd.
McCIeIlJI1(1, D. C. ( 1961 b) Ellcourilgillg l'XCClielln'. DtlcddlllS, '10, 711 -724.
McCle ll and, D. C. (1965). Achicvclllcni ,l l1d 1'lllrCl'l'l'lll'urship: A longilllciill al si lid y . Jtlllr-
11111 ur
Persolla lily illld Stlciill I'S.\'clltl it l .'JI', I , 38 9- , 9 I.
McCll'lIand, D. C. ( 1995). Achievl' IIIL'lll IlHliiv,llioll il l rl'l,llioll to ilchil' Vl'llIl'nl-l'clatcd rc-
c,lll, pl'rIOl'lllallCC a nd Ul'illl' 11m\', ,1 IllJl'kcr associ,lted lVi lh rclc',lsC 01 vasopressin.
Copyrighted Material
Reie rences 111
Copyrighted Material
11 2 Refe re n ces
m ent in children, adoiescents, and latc ad olesce nt s. J,'l/mal of Persollalily ASSCSSIIICIII. 7/,
4 11 -420.
PurcclL K. (1956). The T A T and J nti social beh av ior J"l/mal ofCvllSl/lling Psycholo.'/}'. 20, 449-
456.
Rapaport, D. , G ill. M. M., & Sc ha fe r. R. (1961'). Dia;llIoslic I'sychoiogicaill'slillg . New York:
Inter nation al U ni vers ities Press .
Retief. A. l. ( 191'7). Theill ati c apperccp tion tc stin g acro ss cultures: Tes ts of se ieu ion vers us
tcst s 01 inclu sion. SOIlIIt Afri((l/1 J"llmal of PSVd IOlo.'lY, 17,47-55.
RCZllikoll M. , &. Dollin, A. ( 196 1). SOCi,ll dcsir,lbilit y a lllithc t y pe of hostilit y expresscd on
th e TAT. JOl/mal of Ciillical Psyc1ltl/o.'i)'. 17, 3 I 5- 3 I 7 .
Ritzier, B. A .. Shark ey, K. J.. 5· Chud y, .1. F. (1980). A C!llllprchcnsive proj cc ti ve al ternative
10 th e TAT. JOllr1wl of PerSOli alii)' ASSCSSIIICIII, 44,351'- 362.
ROlllc. H. P.. Ma t aya, P., Pcarson, J. S.. Swcn so n , A. Z .. & B rannick , T. ( 1965). A utolll atic
personality assess ill ent. In R. W. St,KY fl· B. Waxillan (E cl s.), COlllpulers ill biollledical
research (Vol. I , pp. 505-524). Nc'w York: AC,llielll ic Press.
Rors cha ch. H . ( 1942). PsyclltJdia.'lIl,'slics (5 th ed. , P. LClll).; ,lU & 13. Kron c nberg, Trans.). Berlll',
Sw it zerland: Verlag Hans Huber. (Original w ork publi sh cd 192 1).
Rosenwald, G. C. ( 1968) . Thc Thematic Ap percep tion Tes t. In A. l. Rabin (E d. ). Prvjeclive
leclr11iques ill persol1alily assesS IIl I'I1I : A lIIodem ilTlrodl/clitlll (pp . 172-22 1). New Yor).;:
Sp rin ge r.
Rose n zwe ig, S. H. ( 194 8). The Theill ati c A ppncept i'"l Test in di,lgnosis and therapy . .},lllr-
lral vf Persollalily, 16,437--444.
Rossini. E. D., & Morelli, R . .J. ( 1997). Them atic A ppcrcC') lIion Test (T AT) interpretat ion :
Pract ice recolllillend atio ns lrom a su r vey 01 clinical psychology doctora l prog r,l m s ac -
lTed it ed b y lhe Ame ri ca n Psychological Assoc iation . Professiollal Psycirolo.'1.V: Researclr
arId PraClicl', 28, 393- 398 .
Rot h , L. 1-1., Wo lford , .J.. & MciseL A ( 191'0). Pat ient ,Kress to rccord s: Tonic or to x in ?
Alllaictlll JOl/mal of Psydlialrl', 137, 592-59(,.
Sa n lord , R. N ., et al. ( 1943). Pirysi'/lIe, pawllalily al/(I sc/ltl/arslrip . Washington, DC: Soci et y
lor Rescarch in Child Developmcnt
Schaler, R. ( 1954). Psyciwal1alylicilllerpre/alivlI ill/?,'rsc/wclr leslillg. New York: G nllll' f,. Strallon .
Scodcl. A. &. Lipetz, M. E. ( 1957). TAT host ilit y a nd psychopatholog y . JOllmal or Projeclil'e
Tecillliqucs, 2 1, 16 1- 165.
Sherwood, E. T. (1957). On the designing 01 T AT picturc s, w ith special refe ren ce to a sc t fo r
,I n A fri ca n Pl'Opie assilllil ,lti ng Wcstcrn culture Tire J"ll/'lwl or Social Psyclrolo,/y, 45,
16 1- 190 .
Shncid ma n , E. S. ( 1952). Tire Ma ke-A- Pic/llrc Slory Tesi. New Yor ).;: Psychologica l Corpora-
tion.
So lolllon, l. L.. &. Starr, B. D . (1968). School Al'pcrcepliclll Ml'llrod (SAM). New York: Sprin ge r.
Span gler, W. D. (1992). Va lidit y of qucstionnaire and TAT mca surcs of nccd for ac hie vc-
m ent : T wo Ill eta-a nal yses . Psydwlv,qical Bllllelill , 11 2, 140- 154.
Stein , E. J , Furc d y, R. L., SinlOnton , M . .J.. & Nc uller. C. H. ( 1979). Patient access to mcdical
r ccords on a psychiatric inpatient unit. A11Ierimll JVl/ rTlal vI' Psychialry, 136, 327-332.
Stein , M. l. ( 198 1). Tire Themalic AppI'I'Ceplioll Tesl (2 nd cd.). Sprin g li eld, IL: Thomas.
SlO n e, 1-1., & De lli s, N . (1960). A n exploratory in ves ti ga tion illlo th e leve ls h ypothesis. JVI/I'-
11£11 of Projeclive Tcci1niqlles, 24, 333- 340.
Tallent. N. (1993). PsycilOlv.'Iiml reporl wrilil1,q (2 nd cd.) Eng le wood Clills, NJ: Prentice-Ha ll.
Teg la si, H. ( 1992). Clinical use vfslory/elling. Bosto n : A ll y n & Bocon .
Ter r y, D. ( 1952) . T hc use 01 a rat in g sca le 01 levcl of re spon se in TAT stories. JOllrl1al of
Abl10rmal Social Psychology, 47, 507-5 1 I.
TheineI', E. ( 1962). Expe riment al n eed s arc cxprcsscd by proj ec ti ve tcchniqu es. JVllrTlal vf
Projeclive Techniques, 26, 354-363.
Copyrighted Material
References 11 3
Weste n , D .. Ludolph, p, Block, M . .J.. Wixolll, J .. & Wiss, F. C. ( 199 1). Developmcntal
hi sto r y and objccl r elalion s in psychi atrica ll y dislurbed adolesccn l gi rl s. A111eriCt111 JOllr-
Iwl <1 Psycllialry, 148, 14 19- 1420.
W illi allls, J. M. ( I 989) SIyl(': Te/llessolls il/ clarily al/d g/'{/ce I I Ol h ed.) . BOSlon: Sco t!. FOI'(.'snlan .
Wintcrbotto m , M. R. ( 1958). The relation of need for achi eve illent lo it'arning expe ricn ccs
in independence and IllJ stery . In .I. W. A l).;in so n (Ed.), MvtiVt's ill jillltasy, actiol/ , (/Ild
-',)CidV. Prin c(,lOll, N.I: Van Nostrand.
Wolk, R. I .. & Wolk, R. 13. ( 197 1). Malllla/: Gaolliolo.'limi Appcrcepliol/ 7esi. New York: HUlllan
Sc ien cc Press.
Worchel, F. F .. &. Duprce, .I. L. ( 1990). Projective slOrYle llin g techniqucs. In C. R. Rcy nold s
&. R. W. Kaillpha us (E d s. ), Hal/dbook <1 psychological alld t'dllcational asseSsl11C11t oI clrildrerr:
Pasollality, behavior, <11ld cOll lexl (PI'. 70-88). Ncw York: Gu ilford .
Zubin, .I. ( 1949). Personality re sea rch and psychopatllology as relatcd In clinical prJctice .
./c'llr1wl 4 Abnormal alld S()cial Psydwlogy, 44, 14-2 1.
Zubin, J .. Emn, L. 0 .. &. Schu m er, F. (1965). An t'xperilllClltal appmadl 10 projeclivl' techniq1les.
New York: Wiley.
Copyrighted Material
Copyrighted Material
INDEX
115
Copyrighted Material
116 Index
Capacil y lor Elllolionalln vl'Sl lll e nl se,lles, ')7 ])ra w-A- I'nson lesl, 66- 67
Cards Hispanic. 64-65, 67- 68
blanK, 7-8. 46 IlL'Cd lor Jcllievelllell!, 52-53
cMd I , 34, 80, 86, 95- 96, 98- 99 lesli ll g w illl TAT, 12
cMd 2, 34, 86-87, 96, 99 ex,ll11pic 01 TEMAS li se, 102- 104
cal'd 313M, 35,76- 78,80- 81. 96 , 99 Ciliidren's APPl'rCq llioll Tes1. ix, 3,6 1
ca rd ,GF, 35,99-100 ( ho w dllm\" U., 61, 64
eard4, 15- 16, 36,8 1,87, 100 Chlldy, J. F., 3, 67
eMd 5, 3(, elMk. R. A, 14,5 1
card 6BM, 37,8 1,87,96, 100 CochL', [ ., 73
card 6GF, ,7, 87 Cogall, R., 55, 57
cal'll 713M, 38. 8 1-82, 87, 97, I ()() Colors, 43
card 7GF, ,8, 87 COlllbs. A., 73
card 813M, 39,77-78, 100- 101 COllllllllll icalion ,9 1
card 8GF, 1') C()lllpeli lioll IhcIllL'S, 36,40,46-47
card 9BM, 40, 77-79, 87 C()lllpUIL'r -gL'neraled rcports, 92- 93
c,lrd 9G F, 40, 87 COIlSI,l llliIlO, G., 3, 64-65
card 10.4 1 COlllelllplalion IhL'lllL', 45
card 1013, 102, 104 COlllex!, 28
cMd I I , 41. 102- 104 Co()k, P. E., 55
card 12BG, 43 C()W,lll. C., 62
cMd 12F, 42 Craddick. R. A., 76
cMd 12M, 42 Crallll'l', P., 55-56
card 1313,44, 97 Cr()\\'icy, S. J..., 62
card I 3G, 44 ( ttilllra l d ivers il Y, 59-7 1, 62-67
card 13MF, 43,77-79,87,97, 101 Alrican-A lll e rica ll s, 62-65
card 14,45, 10 5 Ap perccpl i ve Persollalil y Tes!, 6(,
card 15,45 L' x,llll pk ()I TEMAS, 102- 104
cMd 16, 4() , 97 l-ii SP,lll ic A ml'ri ca ll s, 64-68
card 1713, 10 3-104 Illdiall p()plllali()ns, 64
cMd 17BM, 46 SL' ll Sili vi l ), I(), 29
card 17GF, 47 TAT ,1 1Il'1'n,1Ii vcs, 6 1-69
card 18BM, 47 TEN1AS, 64-65
ca rd 18GF,48 (mim il ), 111 L'IlIl'S. 36,40,45,47
ca rd 19,48
card 1913, 10 3- 104 Da VillC i, I..l'o I hHci 0, I
cMd 20, 49 ])all,l, R.!-I., 14, 19,24,29,69,7 1
ca rd 21, 104 Dalillg Ihellle s, 49
clloosing, 6-8 D,lt llllil'l', 1-1.,46
minoril y li se, 67-69 ])avids, A., 14
1ll0s1 u selul, 34 Dealilihellles, 42, 45, 102
psycllologisls ranK, 7 child, 35, 48
slilllulu s CilaraCl l'ri sli cs, 3 1- 49 ex,ll 11pics, 77-79
Tilellles Co nccrnin g Blacks, 63 pMell!, 39
Cas lillo, A., 64 partner, 43, 45 , 97
Cas ullo, M. M., 64 Ddense Illecha niSill s
Childllood Ihellles, 44 scor in g, 55- 56
Children Dellis, N ., 73
Alrica n- AIlll'r ica n , 65 Den ia I, 55- 56
CATICAT-H lise, 61 Dcplu, 0., 63
developmcillal si al US, 29 Depressive Iheille s, 35, 45, 99- 100
Copyrighted Material
Index 117
Copyrighted Material
118 Index
Copyrighted Material
In dex 119
Copyrighted Material
120 Ind ex
Copyrighted Material
Index 121
Copyrighted Material
Copyrighted Material